You are on page 1of 11

This article was originally published in a journal published by

Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the


author’s benefit and for the benefit of the author’s institution, for
non-commercial research and educational use including without
limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific
colleagues that you know, and providing a copy to your institution’s
administrator.
All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without
limitation commercial reprints, selling or licensing copies or access,
or posting on open internet sites, your personal or institution’s
website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission
may be sought for such use through Elsevier’s permissions site at:

http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial
Accident Analysis and Prevention 39 (2007) 843–852

Effects of major-road vehicle speed and driver age and gender

py
on left-turn gap acceptance
Xuedong Yan a,∗ , Essam Radwan a,1 , Dahai Guo b,2

co
aCenter for Advanced Transportation Systems Simulation, Department of Civil & Environmental Engineering,
University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL 32816-2450, United States
b Computer Science Program, College of Business, Florida Gulf Coast University, 10501 FGCU Boulevard South,

Fort Myers, FL 33965-6565, United States


Received 28 August 2006; received in revised form 1 November 2006; accepted 12 December 2006

al
Abstract
Because the driver’s gap-acceptance maneuver is a complex and risky driving behavior, it is a highly concerned topic for traffic safety and
operation. Previous studies have mainly focused on the driver’s gap acceptance decision itself but did not pay attention to the maneuver process
on
and driving behaviors. Using a driving simulator experiment for left-turn gap acceptance at a stop-controlled intersection, this study evaluated
the effects of major traffic speed and driver age and gender on gap acceptance behaviors. The experiment results illustrate relationships among
drivers’ left-turn gap decision, driver’s acceleration rate, steering action, and the influence of the gap-acceptance maneuver on the vehicles in the
major traffic stream. The experiment results identified an association between high crash risk and high traffic speed at stop-controlled intersections.
rs
The older drivers, especially older female drivers, displayed a conservative driving attitude as a compensation for reduced driving ability, but also
showed to be the most vulnerable group for the relatively complex driving maneuvers.
© 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
pe

Keywords: Driving simulator; Gap acceptance; Driving behavior; Stop-controlled intersection; Driver age difference; Driver gender difference

1. Introduction accidents occurring at stop-controlled intersections (Laberge


et al., 2006). Previous research has identified gap acceptance
Two-way stop-controlled (TWSC) intersections are the most problems as a significant contributor to the stop-controlled inter-
r's

prevalent intersection type in the United States (Gattis and Low, section accidents and indicated that incorrect gap acceptance
1999). At stop-controlled intersections, drivers on the minor might cause around 30% of left-turn accidents (Chovan et al.,
road need to make use of proper gaps among the traffic to 1994). As an essential driving performance at stop-controlled
cross or merge into the major road. Because the driver’s gap- intersections, the gap acceptance decision has been used as an
o

acceptance maneuver is a complex and risky driving behavior, important measurement to analyze and predict traffic conflicts
it is a highly concerned topic for traffic safety and operation. and accident rates at intersections (Spek et al., 2006; Alexander
th

Retting et al. (2003) reported that nearly 700,000 motor vehicle et al., 2002).
crashes occurred annually at stop signs in US and approxi- Furthermore, in the current AASHTO Manual (2001), the
mately one-third of these crashes involved injuries in US. In gap acceptance methodology is applied to determine inter-
Minnesota, there were 34,175 reported crashes on rural two- section sight distances, which is based on previous research
Au

lane roads between 2000 and 2002. Over 32% (11,069) of these works (Harwood et al., 2000). In the Highway Capacity Man-
crashes were intersection related, with 22% of the fatal rural ual (HCM, 2000), the critical gap accepted by drivers is a
key parameter to calculate the minor traffic capacity. How-
ever, both important highway design manuals ignored the
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 407 823 5810; fax: +1 407 823 4676. effects of important factors such as driver age and gender and
E-mail addresses: yxd22222@yahoo.com (X. Yan),
aeradwan@mail.ucf.edu (E. Radwan), dguo@fgcu.edu (D. Guo).
major-road vehicle speed on the gap acceptance, which has
1 Tel.: +1 407 823 2945; fax: +1 407 823 4676. generally been paid attention to by researchers in traffic safety
2 Tel.: +1 239 590 7583; fax: +1 239 590 7330. aspects.

0001-4575/$ – see front matter © 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.aap.2006.12.006
844 X. Yan et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 39 (2007) 843–852

1.1. Effect of traffic speed on gap acceptances same sex. Through both simulator and field measures, Staplin
(1995) indicated that older drivers show relative insensitivity
In the AASHTO and HCM manuals, it is assumed that the to vehicle approach speed in left-turn maneuvers across the
critical gap accepted by drivers does not vary with major-road major road traffic when compared with younger drivers. This
vehicle speed, which is based on the previous analysis by Kyte may increase the risk of accidents if there is a lone speeder in
et al. (1996). However, in the simulator experiment for turn- the traffic scheme. Abdel-Aty et al. (1999) pointed out that at
ing left from the major road into the minor road (Alexander et intersections, elderly drivers are over-represented in right and
al., 2002), the velocity of the on-coming traffic was the vari- left turns as well as angle collisions. Scialfa et al. (1991) con-
able that had the greatest effect on the median accepted gap cluded that older drivers generally over-estimate the speed of

py
size. This result corresponds to those of other previous studies vehicles traveling at low speeds, while under-estimating the
involving gap acceptance, which had shown that drivers accept a speed of those traveling much faster, which could explain the
smaller gap at higher approach velocities (Darzentas et al., 1980; over-involvement of elderly drivers in accidents at junctions.
Staplin, 1995). Another field study indicated that for a given

co
time gap, the probability that a driver accepts the gap increases 1.3. Objective of the study
as the speed of the opposing vehicle increases, which implies
that the distance is the primary determinant of gap acceptance Although numerous previous studies have been done for
(Davis and Swenson, 2004). Based on the speed dependency the gap-acceptance research, all of them only focused on the
of gap acceptance decision, Spek et al. (2006) developed a the- gap acceptance decision itself but did not pay attention to
oretical crash prediction model and found that the probability the maneuver process and related driving behavior. Using a

al
that a crossing vehicle collides with the major stream vehi- driving simulator experiment for left-turn gap acceptance, this
cle can be expected to increase when the major traffic speed study evaluated the effects of major traffic speed and driver
increases. age and gender on gap acceptance behaviors, such as minimum
on
accepted gap, driver’s acceleration rate, steering action, and
1.2. Age and gender differences in gap acceptances influence of the gap-acceptance maneuver on the vehicles in
the major traffic stream. This research aimed at analyzing the
It was reported that the process that driving performance interactive effect between gap-acceptance drivers and vehicles
rs

becomes progressively poorer with age accelerates somewhere on the major road, and identifying the driving behavior differ-
in the fifth decade of life (National Highway Traffic Safety ences associated with major traffic speed and driver age and
Administration, 1993). Older drivers have problems to ade- gender.
quately detect, perceive and accurately judge the safety of a gap
pe

(Laberge et al., 2006). Therefore, older drivers may experience 2. Methodology


greater difficulties at nonsignalized intersections as the result of
diminished visual capabilities, such as depth and motion per- 2.1. Participants
ception. Prior results indicated that judgments about whether
a potential collision would occur were less accurate for older The experiment was a 3 (age) × 2 (gender) × 2 (major traf-
drivers (40–64 years) compared with younger drivers (18–29 fic speed) within-subject repeated measures design. Sixty-three
r's

years) (DeLucia et al., 2003). Lyles and Staplin (1991) exam- paid participants in three age groups, ranging in age from 20 to 83
ined police-reported accidents in Michigan and Pennsylvania years, were recruited for this research. Table 1 lists the descrip-
and found that when these were ordered according to older- tive statistics of subjects by age and gender. The young group
driver involvement rate, turning left across on-coming traffic ranged from 20 to 30 years old, the middle-age group ranged
and crossing or turning into a traffic stream were found to be
o

from 31 to 55 years old, and the old group ranged from 56 to


the most dangerous maneuvers. Moreover, it was indicated that 83 years old. Every participant held a valid Florida’s driver’s
older drivers are over-represented in severe injury crashes at license with at least 2 years of driving experience. The experi-
th

intersections due in part to increases in frailty and functional


disabilities that occur with age (Oxley et al., 2006). Table 1
Lerner et al. (1995) concluded that older drivers are partic- Descriptive statistics of subjects by age and gender
Au

ularly overrepresented in multiple-vehicle, intersection-related Age Gender Mean Standard N


accidents, which could be interpreted to reflect slow detection deviation
of and reaction to other vehicles, or slow decision time for Young (20–30) Female 24.00 2.42 14
intersection-related maneuvers. Lerner et al. (1995) collected Male 23.57 2.85 14
gap acceptance data as a function of driver age for left turn, Middle (31–55) Female 39.10 9.27 10
right turn, and through movements at stop-controlled intersec- Male 33.64 3.29 11
tions. The findings indicated that younger drivers (20–40 years)
Old (56–83) Female 69.75 8.66 4
accept shorter gaps than older drivers (over 65 years). Alexander Male 73.30 8.86 10
et al. (2002) found that females require a larger median gap
Total Female 35.93 16.93 28
than males, irrespective of age, and older subjects (65–79 years) Male 40.94 21.83 35
require a larger gap than younger subjects (under 60 years) of the
X. Yan et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 39 (2007) 843–852 845

ment lasted for about 20 min in total and the participants were of the experiment, the driver first drives the simulator for 400 m
compensated 20 dollars for their participation. along the minor road (stage 1). When the simulator approaches
the intersection, all vehicles on the major road begin to enter
2.2. Apparatus/equipment the scene. Then, according to the voice reminder and indica-
tion sign (stop sign and left-turn sign), the driver stops at the
This study used the UCF (University of Central Florida) driv- intersection and waits for the appropriate gap in the traffic on
ing simulator as a tool for data collection. The driving simulator the major road to make his/her left turn maneuver (stage 2).
is an I-Sim Mark-II system with a motion base capable of oper- When the simulated vehicle enters the major road and its speed
ation with 6 degrees of freedom. It includes five channels (one is caught up with the major road traffic speed (stage 3), the

py
forward, two side views and two rear view mirrors) of image experiment is completed. All subjects were tested in two sce-
generation, an audio and vibration system, and steering wheel narios, 40.2 km/h (25 mph) major road speed (scenario A) and
feedback. The simulated environment is projected at 180◦ of 88.5 km/h (55 mph) major road speed (scenario B), which is
field view and at a resolution of 1280 × 1024 pixels. The driving shown in Fig. 1.

co
simulation system is composed of the following components:
2.4. Traffic design on the major road
• Simulator Cab: Saturn Sedan, automatic transmission, air
conditioning, the left back mirror and the back mirror inside A challenge in designing this experiment was how to make
the cab. the drivers perform their minimum left-turn gap-acceptance
• Simview: The software provides the graphical display based maneuver in the same way accomplished in the real world. For

al
on the computation. obtaining that goal, the oncoming traffic on the major road from
• Motion base: It provides motion when driving. the right was composed of two classes of intermingled gaps to
• Scenario Editor: The software helps researchers to edit a make the traffic appear random. One gap classification had very
on
tested traffic scenario. small gaps (less than 3 s) that were unlikely to be accepted by
• APIs for reading real-time data: APIs (Application Program- the participants. The other class consisted of increasing gaps
mer Interface) can read the real-time data from Simview. The in which the subsequent gap was 1 s larger than the previous
sampling frequency is 60 Hz. one. This design assured that the selected gap would be close to
rs

the driver’s minimum acceptable gap and the major road traf-
2.3. Description of traffic scenarios fic looked realistic. This concept is illustrated in Fig. 2. The
uniformly increasing gaps ranged in duration from 1 to 16 s,
In the gap acceptance experiments, a long straight undivided a large enough variation to accommodate all drivers. A previ-
pe

two-lane highway was used as the major road. Its length is ous gap acceptance study suggested that there is no meaningful
around 3000 m with a lane width of 3.66 m. A driving scenario information regarding driver gap acceptance behavior when the
consists of three stages as shown in Fig. 1. During the course accepted gap size is above 12 s (Gattis and Low, 1999).
o r's
th
Au

Fig. 1. Traffic scenario design for left-turn gap acceptance.

Fig. 2. Traffic design on the major road.


846 X. Yan et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 39 (2007) 843–852

Whenever a car on the major road approached the inter- The orders of scenarios were presented as A–B or B–A randomly
section, it was automatically changed from “record vehicle” for subject so as to eliminate the time order effect. For security
to the “normal vehicle”. The “record vehicles” move along a and liability reasons, each subject was escorted to the simulator
fixed path with the unchangeable speed from the start point cabin to commence the experiment and he/she was allowed at
to the end point, whereas the “normal vehicles” intelligently least 2 min to rest before running the next scenario.
move along the given route. While the normal vehicles fol-
low a designed speed, they can decelerate, accelerate, and pass 2.6. Dependent measures
the slow impeding car according to the traffic situation. There-
fore, if the simulator is turning left into the major road very In the simulator experiment, the measured parameters related

py
slowly, the coming car on the major road will decrease its speed to gap acceptance maneuver include minimum time gap
to avoid a crash. Unless some participants drive the simula- accepted by left turners (GAP), average steering angle velocity
tor too aggressively or disobey the rationale, there will be no during the left turn (SAV), average acceleration of the simula-
crashes. tor during the left-turn maneuver (ACC), minimum separation

co
between the minor-road vehicle and the following major-road
2.5. Experiment procedure vehicle (MCD), speed reduction rate of the following vehicle on
the major road (SRR), and the deceleration rate of the follow-
Upon arrival, the subjects were asked to fill out and sign ing vehicle (DEC). These dependent measures are explained as
an informed consent form (per IRB). The subjects were then follows:
advised to drive and behave as they normally would and to adhere
• GAP (in s): the time headway between two vehicles on the

al
to traffic laws as in real life situations. The subjects were also
notified that they could quit the experiment at any time in case major road into which a left-turn driver may choose to turn.
of motion sickness or any kind of discomfort. Prior to the formal • SAV (in rad/s): total rotation angles are divided by the total
on time during which the simulator steer turn left and turn back
experiment, drivers were trained for at least 5 min to familiar-
ize with the driving simulator operation. During the course of when subjects complete the left turn maneuver.
the practice, subjects exercised selected maneuvers including • ACC (in m/s2 ): average acceleration of simulator vehicle dur-
straight driving, acceleration, deceleration, left/right turn, and ing the period of making left turn maneuver.
• MCD (in m): minimum clearance distance between the simu-
rs

other basic driving behaviors.


After completing the familiarity course, the formal experi- lator and the following major-road vehicle before the left-turn
ments began during which all subjects faced the same set of two maneuver is completed.
driving scenarios, denoted A for the lower major traffic speed • SRR: the speed reduction rate of the following vehicle on the
pe

(40.2 km/h) and B for the higher major traffic speed (88.5 km/h). major road to accommodate the turning vehicle.

Table 2
Descriptive statistical results for dependent variables
Factors Parameter GAP (s) SAV (rad/s) ACC (m/s2 ) MCD (m) SRR DEC (m/s2 )

Speed
r's

40.2 km/h (N = 63) Mean 7.44 3.06 1.82 54.16 0.02 0.11
S.D. 2.08 0.79 0.61 17.75 0.05 0.21
88.5 km/h (N = 63) Mean 5.82 2.99 1.89 24.53 0.24 1.20
S.D. 1.46 0.82 0.68 13.38 0.13 1.25
o

Age
Young (N = 56) Mean 6.29 3.08 2.04 37.82 0.12 0.58
th

S.D. 1.79 0.69 0.60 18.68 0.14 0.81


Middle (N = 42) Mean 6.20 3.16 1.86 34.71 0.12 0.85
S.D. 1.50 0.82 0.61 18.58 0.14 1.51
Au

Old (N = 28) Mean 7.94 2.71 1.47 49.35 0.16 0.52


S.D. 2.38 0.92 0.63 27.96 0.17 0.47

Gender
Female (N = 56) Mean 6.93 3.03 1.81 40.88 0.14 0.65
S.D. 2.02 0.80 0.66 22.90 0.17 0.86
Male (N = 70) Mean 6.38 3.02 1.89 38.12 0.12 0.66
S.D. 1.90 0.81 0.64 20.58 0.12 1.18

Total
(N = 126) Mean 6.63 3.03 1.85 39.34 0.13 0.65
S.D. 1.97 0.80 0.65 21.60 0.14 1.05
X. Yan et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 39 (2007) 843–852 847

• DEC (in m/s2 ): the deceleration rate used by the following Table 5
Mean values of GAP for speed × gender × age
vehicle on the major road to accommodate the turning vehicle.
Category Young Middle Old
3. Experiment results 40.2 km/h
Female 7.56 6.97 10.99
The basic statistical descriptions of the experiment results Male 6.35 6.60 8.76
are listed in Table 2. In the subsequent statistical analyses, an 88.5 km/h
ANOVA was used to investigate differences between factors (see Female 6.00 5.63 7.11
Male 5.26 5.61 6.23

py
Table 3). The hypothesis testing in the following analysis was
based on a 0.05 significance level.

3.1. Correlation analyses for dependent variables is lowest and its speed reduction is greatest. An example for
this analysis is illustrated in Fig. 3. When time unit is equal to

co
Table 4 shows the correlation analysis among the dependent 114.45, the clearance distance is minimum and equal to 19.53 m,
variables. Clearly, the GAP is correlated with all the other param- and the speed of the following car is 63.46 km/h, which is close
eters. Intuitively, if drivers accept smaller gaps, they tend to to that (63.14 km/h) of the simulator vehicle. It can be concluded
make left-turn maneuvers with a faster steering velocity and that the minimum clearance distance and the maximum speed
higher acceleration rate, and result in shorter minimum clear- reduction always happen at the same experiment sampling time.
ance distances to the following vehicles, and larger percentage After minimum clearance distance occurs, the left turn maneuver

al
of speed reduction and higher deceleration rates of the following can be considered to be successfully completed by the subject.
vehicles. The smaller MCD and the larger SRR and DEC indicate that the
Furthermore, it was found that MCD, SRR, and DEC are left-turn vehicles have more effects on the major road traffic.
on
highly correlated with each other. When the left-turn vehicle
enters onto the major road at a slow speed, the following car 3.2. Minimum gap acceptance
on the major road decreases its speed to avoid a crash. As the
following car on the major road is approaching the turning-into Table 5 lists mean values of GAP for each speed and for
rs

vehicle whose speed keeps increasing, the distance between the each combination of gender × age. The ANOVA results show the
turning-into vehicle and the following major-road vehicle gets significant effects of age (p < 0.001), gender (p = 0.004), speed
smaller, until the minimum clearance distance between the two (p < 0.001), and two-way interaction between age and speed
pe

vehicles occurs. At that same time, the following car’s speed (p = 0.035) on driver’s gap acceptance (see Table 3).

Table 3
Analysis of variance table for dependent measures
Source d.f. F-ratio

GAP SAV ACC MCD SRR DEC


r's

Gender 1 8.514** .112 2.601 3.660 6.690* .024


Age 2 14.798** 3.587* 9.587** 9.749** 4.447* 1.325
Speed 1 37.044** .752 .159 111.678** 155.082** 36.781**
Gender × age 2 1.468 2.943 1.557 .337 4.053* .138
Gender × speed 1 1.314 1.129 1.003 .009 2.012 .019
o

Age × speed 2 3.462* .388 .018 1.824 .812 .545


Gender × age × speed 2 .204 .029 .171 .063 1.194 .302
Mean square error 114 2.560 .621 .382 219.173 .009 .837
th

** Significant at the 0.01 level.


* Significant at the 0.05 level.
Au

Table 4
Correlation among dependent variables
GAP SAV ACC MCD SRR DEC

GAP 1 −.280** −.302** .591** −.218* −.298**


SAV −.280** 1 .425** −.074 −.224* −.081
ACC −.302** .425** 1 .324** −.455** −.234**
MCD .591** −.074 .324** 1 −.557** −.448**
SRR −.218* −.224* −.455** −.557** 1 .593**
DEC −.298** −.081 −.234** −.448** .593** 1
** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
* Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
848 X. Yan et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 39 (2007) 843–852

py
co
Fig. 4. Minimum GAP vs. driver’s actual age by gender.

accepted by male drivers tend to gradually increase as the driver


age increases while those for female drivers show a U-shape
pattern, which indicates that middle age groups accept smaller

al
gaps and the younger and older groups have relatively larger
gaps. Furthermore, it shows that the gender difference in GAP
on
is not significant for drivers from 35 to 55 years; however, for
drivers younger than 35 years or older than 55 years, females
appear to accept larger gaps than males.
The results show that the oncoming vehicle speed on the
major road plays an important role in the gap size accepted
rs

by drivers. Drivers would accept smaller gaps (M = 5.82 s,


S.D. = 1.46 s) for the higher major-road traffic speed scenario
compared to those (M = 7.44 s, S.D. = 2.08 s) for the lower
pe

major-road traffic speed scenario. It implies that drivers rely


on both space and time information to perform gap-acceptance
judgments, which is corresponding to the conclusion of prior
studies (Alexander et al., 2002; Davis and Swenson, 2004).
Furthermore, the interaction effect between speed and driver
age shows that for the lower major-road traffic speed scenario,
there is a strong gap acceptance difference in driver age [F(2,
r's

60) = 34.756, p < 0.001], and the old drivers apparently accept
larger gaps (M = 9.40 s, S.D. = 2.24 s) compared to the younger
drivers (M = 6.96 s, S.D. = 1.82 s) and the middle-age drivers
Fig. 3. Relationship between clearance distance and speeds of the left-turning
(M = 6.77 s, S.D. = 1.49 s); however, for the higher major-road
o

vehicle (simulator) and the following car.


traffic speed scenario, the minimum gaps accepted by old drivers
(M = 6.48 s, S.D. = 1.48 s) are not significantly larger than the
th

Old drivers tend to accept larger gaps (M = 7.94 s, younger drivers (M = 5.63 s, S.D. = 1.51 s) and the middle-age
S.D. = 2.38 s), compared to younger drivers (M = 6.29 s, drivers (M = 5.62 s, S.D. = 1.31 s) [F(2, 60) = 1.926, p = 0.155].
S.D. = 1.79 s) and middle-age drivers (M = 6.20 s, S.D. = 1.50 s). The interaction effect between speed and driver age is illustrated
Au

There is no significant difference between young drivers and in Fig. 5.


middle-age drivers [t(96) = 0.283, p = 0.778]. For the gender fac-
tor, it appears that male drivers accept smaller gaps (M = 6.38 s, 3.3. Steering angle velocity during left turning
S.D. = 1.90 s) than female drivers (M = 6.93 s, S.D. = 2.02 s). The
findings suggest that older drivers and female drivers are more In the output file of driving simulator experiment, steering
conservative than the other groups. Although the interaction control data display the angle of the selected simulator’s steer-
effect between age and gender was not found to be signifi- ing wheel. According to the ANOVA results, only age (p = 0.031)
cant by ANOVA, a plot of the minimum gap versus driver’s has a significant effect on SAV. During the process of turning
actual age shows different trends of gap acceptance between left, old drivers (M = 2.71 rad/s, S.D. = 0.92 rad/s) tend to turn
males and females (see Fig. 4). The comparison of the 2nd- the simulator steering wheel more slowly than younger drivers
order polynomial regression lines shows that the minimum gaps (M = 3.08 rad/s, S.D. = 0.69 rad/s) [t(82) = 2.116, p = 0.037]
X. Yan et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 39 (2007) 843–852 849

py
co
al
Fig. 5. Interaction effect between age and speed on gap acceptance.
Fig. 6. Speed effect on minimum clearance distance.
on
and middle-age drivers (M = 3.16 rad/s, S.D. = 0.82 rad/s)
MCD (M = 49.35 m, S.D. = 27.96 m), compared to those for
[t(68) = 2.152, p = 0.035]. Between young and middle-age
young drivers (M = 37.82 m, S.D. = 18.68 m) and middle-age
groups, there is no statistical difference in SAV [t(96) = 0.488,
drivers (M = 34.71 m, S.D. = 18.58 m). Another interesting find-
p = 0.627]. This finding indicates the trend of decreasing driving
rs

ing shows that drivers have much smaller MCD for the higher
ability for older drivers. Experiment results show that the mean
major-road traffic speed (88.5 km/h) compared to that for the
of SAV is 3.06 and 2.99 rad/s for scenarios A and B, respec-
lower major-road speed (40.2 km/h), as shown in Fig. 6.
tively, and there is no statistical difference in the effect of the
Furthermore, the ANOVA results show that speed (p < 0.001),
pe

major road design speed [F(1, 124) = 0.299, p = 0.585].


age (p = 0.014), gender (p = 0.011), and two-way interaction
between age and gender (p = 0.020) have significant effects on
3.4. Left-turning acceleration the speed reductions of the following vehicles on the major-
road (SRR) (see Table 3). When drivers turn into the higher
The ANOVA result shows that only driver age (p < 0.001) speed major-road traffic, they cause the following vehicle to
has significant effect on driver’s acceleration rate during the reduce speed by a higher rate (M = 0.24, S.D. = 0.13), compared
period of the left turn at the intersection. Older drivers are
r's

to turning into the lower speed traffic (M = 0.02, S.D. = 0.05).


more likely to use smaller acceleration rates (M = 1.47 m/s2 , As aforementioned before, the speed reduction rate of the
S.D. = 0.63 m/s2 ) to achieve the major road traffic speed major-road vehicle is highly correlated to its deceleration rate.
compared to the younger drivers (M = 2.04, S.D. = 0.60) and The ANOVA shows that the deceleration rate (M = 1.20 m/s2 ,
middle-age drivers (M = 1.86 m/s2 , S.D. = 0.61 m/s2 ). Between S.D. = 1.25 m/s2 ) of the following vehicle in the higher major-
o

young and middle-age groups, there is no statistical difference road traffic speed scenario is also much higher than that
in ACC [t(96) = 1.473, p = 0.144]. Generally, the larger gaps that (M = 0.11 m/s2 , S.D. = 0.21 m/s2 ) in the lower major-road traf-
th

drivers accepted, the smaller accelerations that drivers used to fic speed scenario [F(1, 124) = 45.863, p < 0.001]. The results
turn into the major road. Therefore, the driver age effect on imply a general trend that left-turn vehicles have more effects
acceleration rate is corresponding to the trend that older drivers on higher speed major-road traffic and drivers take more risks
Au

accept larger gaps, and reflect older drivers’ conservative driving for gap acceptance maneuvers at intersections with higher design
attitude. speeds.
It was also found that the left turns of older drivers con-
3.5. Minimum clearance distance and speed reduction rate tribute to more SRR (M = 0.16, S.D. = 0.17), compared to
and deceleration rate of the following vehicle on the major those for young drivers (M = 0.12, S.D. = 0.14) and middle-age
road drivers (M = 0.12, S.D. = 0.14); and female drivers result in a
higher SRR (M = 0.14, S.D. = 0.17) than male drivers (M = 0.12,
Based on the ANOVA, it was found that both age (p < 0.001) S.D. = 0.12). Furthermore, the interaction effect between age and
and speed (p < 0.001) are significantly associated with the gender illustrates that the old female drivers cause the highest
minimum clear distance (MCD) between the left-turning sim- SRR (see Fig. 7) but other age-gender groups show similar SRR.
ulator and the following vehicle. Older drivers have larger Therefore, older female drivers have a main contribution to gen-
850 X. Yan et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 39 (2007) 843–852

pening at stop-controlled intersections. Also, it is suggested that


a further analysis of the quantitative relationship between inter-
section operation speed and gap-acceptance crash rate based on
crash databases be conducted.
The experiment results showed that there is an apparent age
difference in the left-turn driving behavior, as well as gen-
der difference. Compared to younger drivers and middle-age
drivers, the older subjects tend to select larger gaps to make
left turns, turn the steering wheel more slowly, and keep a fur-

py
ther clearance distance from the following car. The conservative
driving attitude of older drivers would compensate driving abil-
ity decrease. In the previous research, there is also evidence that
elderly drivers perceive potential risk situations at intersections

co
and change their driving habits to avoid them. It was demon-
strated that drivers 55 years and older, particularly those who
had a previous history of crashes, avoided driving in rain, during
rush hour and making left turns across traffic (Ball et al., 1998).
However, when making left turns in the higher major-road traffic
speed scenario, the older subjects did not show age difference in

al
gap acceptance. The results confirmed the conclusion that older
drivers rely primarily or exclusively on perceived distance to
Fig. 7. Interaction effect between age and gender on speed reduction rate.
perform gap-acceptance judgments, reflecting a reduced ability
on
to integrate time based on speed and distance information with
der and age difference in the following vehicle speed reduction increasing age (Staplin et al., 1993). Spek et al. (2006) indicated
rate. that the effect that drivers tend to accept smaller time gaps as
the approach speed increases appears to be stronger for older
rs

4. Discussions drivers than for younger drivers, suggesting that the older driver
is more prone to collide with speeding vehicles.
The experiment results illustrated that the major road For the gender factor, the results showed that male drivers
traffic speed contributes to an interactive effect between gap- accept smaller gaps than female drivers, which reflect a gen-
pe

acceptance drivers and vehicles on the major road. First, drivers eral conservative driving attitude of female drivers. By assessing
are more likely to accept smaller gaps in the higher major-road both general perceptual-motor performance and safety concerns,
traffic speed scenario than the lower major-road traffic speed it was found that male drivers consistently overestimate their
scenario. It implied that drivers show relatively less sensitivity perceptual-motor skills, whereas safety skills are more promi-
to vehicle approach speed but more sensitivity to on-coming nent among female drivers (Lajunen et al., 1998; Lajunen and
vehicle’s distance or position. The perceptual task of drivers for Summala, 1995). However, the result also showed that the older
r's

gap acceptance requires integrating speed and distance informa- female drivers contribute to the most significant influence in the
tion of a longitudinally coming-through vehicle moving in depth major road vehicle speed. The finding implied that older females
without significant change in visual direction. Olson (1996) sug- could be the most vulnerable group for the relatively complex
gests that the speed of an approaching vehicle may not easily driving maneuvers because of more reductions in their driving
be perceived until it is very near. This finding corresponds to ability. It is worthwhile to mention that the older female group
o

the previous study results of gap acceptances for left-crossing in this study only includes four observations, which led to larger
maneuver (Cooper et al., 1977; Hancock et al., 1991; Alexander variations of the related experiment results. Nevertheless, the
th

et al., 2002). observed trend in this study implies that a further safety concern
On the other hand, due to accepting smaller gaps for the is needed for this high risk driver group.
higher traffic speed, drivers have shorter separation from the fol- Driving simulators provide a safe and controlled virtual envi-
Au

lowing vehicle, result in more speed reduction and deceleration ronment to test high risk driving behaviors. The convenient
rate of the following vehicles to accommodate the turning vehi- traffic design in the driving simulator allowed researchers to
cle, and therefore contribute to more effects on major road traffic observe the minimum gap selected by subjects, while it is
operation. These findings implied a connection between high very difficult in a field study since drivers’ gap acceptances
crash risk and high traffic speed at stop-controlled intersections. are influenced by the traffic volume distribution on the major
A previous study reported that stop-controlled intersections road. According to the experience of this simulator experiment,
where high-speed, high-volume roads are intersected by lower- the scenario design method can be used to analyze other gap-
speed, lower-volume roads controlled by a stop sign are a major acceptance behaviors at priority intersections, such as right turn
problem for traffic safety, particularly in rural areas (Laberge et from the minor road and crossing maneuver from the minor road
al., 2006). Therefore, effective speed control countermeasures to or major road. Furthermore, the driving simulator enabled the
reduce speeding behaviors may lead to decrease in crashes hap- traffic parameters, such as SAV, ACC, MCD, SRR, and DEC
X. Yan et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 39 (2007) 843–852 851

related to drivers’ gap decisions, to be efficiently collected. directly to practice in absolute terms. However, the observed
These parameters were generally omitted by previous gap accep- trends and patterns associated with the gap-acceptance behav-
tance studies. In this study, it was found that older drivers tended iors should be valid and contribute to the further research or
to accept larger gaps to compensate the reducing driving ability, practical work.
but they still showed more influence on the major road traffic
than younger and middle-age drivers. Thus, the results implied 5. Conclusions
that the gap acceptance accident risk may not be accurately pre-
dicted only based on the probability that drivers accept small In summary, this driving simulator experiment illustrated
gaps.

py
relationships among drivers’ left-turn gap decision, correspond-
However, it should be mentioned that for simulator exper- ing driving behaviors, and consequent influence on the vehicles
iments, there are some general limitations and validation in the major traffic stream. The results indicated that major
considerations. A problem that occurs frequently with all driving road traffic speed and driver age and gender have significant
simulators is simulator sickness. Based on the authors’ experi-

co
effects on the gap-acceptance maneuver. The findings identified
ences in previous driving simulator experiments, the simulator an association between high crash risk and high traffic speed
sickness is particularly correlated with turning maneuvers and at stop-controlled intersections. The older drivers displayed a
braking behaviors. Without a real safety risk as field test, sick- conservative driving attitude as a compensation for the reducing
ness in a simulator experiment may significantly harm drivers’ driving ability. However, due to the aging effect on cognitive,
gap acceptance decisions because drivers would try to complete perceptual, and motor abilities, they may still be the most vul-
the experiment as soon as possible to reduce the discomfort level. nerable group for the relatively complex driving maneuvers,

al
Therefore, this study attempted to avoid the sickness effect on the especially for older female drivers.
experiment results through encouraging participants with sick-
ness to quit the experiment (they still obtained payment for their
Acknowledgments
on
involvements even if not completing the experiment). Another
purpose of this policy is to protect older participants since the
The authors would like to acknowledge the Florida Depart-
oldest subject is 83 years old. In this study, the older female sub-
ment of Transportation (FDOT) for its sponsorship of this
jects were most sensitive to simulator sickness and only 4 of 10
research project. Appreciation is also extended to CATSS staff
rs

participants fully completed the experiments without suffering


and students for providing assistance with the execution of the
any sickness.
simulation of the experiment. The recommendations of this
Previous driving simulator validity studies related driving
study are those of the authors and they do not represent views
behaviors are measured using two types of validity: absolute
pe

of FDOT.
validity (when the numerical values between the two systems
are the same) and relative validity (when differences found
between experimental conditions are in the same direction, and References
have a similar or identical magnitude on both systems) (Blaauw,
American Association of State Highway and Transportation Officials
1982). For the absolute validity, the conclusions of the sim- (AASHTO), 2001. A Policy on Geometric Design of Highways and Streets
ulator validity varied in different types of driving simulators Washington, DC.
r's

and test scenarios. However, from the perspective of the relative Abdel-Aty, M., Chen, C., Radwan, E., 1999. Using conditional probability to
validity, most previous studies concluded that driving simulators find the driver age effect in crashes, J. Transp. Eng., 1999, ASCE, vol. 125,
can reflect a similar trend of the driving performance in driving No. 6.
Alexander, J., Barham, P., Black, I., 2002. Factors influencing the probability
simulators to that in the real world. Lee (2002) compared driv- of an incident at a junction: results from an interactive driving simulator.
ing performance of older adult drivers (60–90 years) between a
o

Accid. Anal. Prev. 34, 779–792.


PC-based simulator and subjects’ own cars. He found a covari- Ball, K., Owsley, C., Stalvey, B., Roenker, D.L., Sloane, M.E., Graves, M., 1998.
ance (r2 = 0.66) between the two measures and concluded that Driving avoidance and functional impairment in older drivers. Accid. Anal.
th

Prev. 30, 313–322.


simulator usage was a safer and more economical method than
Bella, F., 2005. Validation of a driving simulator for work zone design. Transp.
the on-road testing to assess the driving performance of older Res. Rec. 1937, 136–144.
adult drivers. For the speed and lane position control studies, Blaauw, G.J., 1982. Driving experience and task demands in simulator and
Au

Godley et al. (2002) and Törnros’s (1998) reported that par- instrumented car: a validation study. Hum. Factors 24, 473–486, 2004.
ticipants generally drove faster in the instrumented car than Chovan, J.D., Tijerian, L., Everson, J.H., Pierowicz, J.A., Hendricks, D.L.,
1994. Examination of intersection left turn across path crashes and IVHS
the simulator, whereas relative validity was achieved for both
countermeasures (DOT HS 808 154). National Highway Traffic Safety
speed and lateral position. More recently, a speed validation Administration.
study focused on the effectiveness of temporary traffic signs Cooper, D.F., Storr, P.A., Wennell, J., 1977. Traffic studies at T-junctions: the
on highways (Bella, 2005). The results showed that differences effect of speed on gap acceptance and conflict rate. Traffic Eng. Control 18,
between the speeds observed in the real situation and those mea- 110–112.
Darzentas, M.R., McDowell, C., Cooper, D., 1980. Minimum acceptable gaps
sured with the simulator were not statistically significant and
and conflict involvement in a simple crossing maneuver. Traffic Eng. Control
validated the driving simulator in absolute terms. In this study, 21 (2), 58–61.
since the authors did not conduct a field validation for the simu- Davis, G.A., Swenson, T., 2004. Field study of gap acceptance by left-turning
lator data, caution should be taken in transferring these findings drivers. Transp. Res. Rec. 1899, 71–75.
852 X. Yan et al. / Accident Analysis and Prevention 39 (2007) 843–852

DeLucia, P.R., Bleckley, M., Meyer, L.E., Bush, J.M., 2003. Judgments about Transport Research (CAITR 2002) University of New South Wales, Sydney,
collision in younger and older drivers. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Psychol. Australia, December 4–6 (2002).
Behav. 6, 63–80. Lerner, N.D., Huey, R.W., McGee, H.W., Sullivan, A., 1995. Older Driver
Gattis, J.L., Low, S.T., 1999. GAP acceptance at atypical stop-controlled inter- Perception-Reaction Time for Intersection Sight Distance and Object Detec-
sections. J. Transp. Eng. 125, 201–207. tion: Report No. FHWA-RD-93-168. Federal Highway Administration:
Godley, S.T., Triggs, J.T., Fildes, B.N., 2002. Driving simulator validation for Washington, DC.
speed research. Accid. Anal. Prev. 34, 589–600. Lyles, R.W., Staplin, L., 1991. Age differences in motion perception and specific
Hancock, P.A., Caird, J.K., Shekhar, S., Vercruyssen, M., 1991. Factors Influenc- traffic maneuver problems. Transp. Res. Rec. 1325, 23–33.
ing Drivers’ Left Turn Decisions Proceedings of the Human Factors Society National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1993. Addressing the
35th Annual Meeting. Safety Issues Related to Younger and Older Drivers. DOT HS 807

py
Harwood, D., Mason, J., Brydia, R., 2000. Sight distance for stop-controlled 957 NTS-31. US Department of Transportation. http://www.nhtsa.dot.gov/
intersections based on gap acceptance. Transp. Res. Rec. 1701, 32–41. people/injury/olddrive/pub/yorept.pdf. Access date, August 2006.
Highway Capacity Manual (HCM), 2000. TRB, National Research Council, Olson, P., 1996. Forensic Aspects of Driver Perception and Response. Lawyers
Washington, DC. & Judges Publishing Co., Tuscon, AZ.
Kyte, M., Tian, Z., Mir, Z., Hameedmansoor, Z., Kittelson, W., Vandehey, M., Oxley, J., Fildes, B., Corben, B., Langford, J., 2006. Intersection design for older

co
Robinson, B., Brilon, W., Bondzio L., Wu, N., Troutbeck, R., 1996. Capacity drivers. Transp. Res. Part F: Traff. Psychol. Behav. 9, 335–346.
and Level of Service at Unsignalized Intersections, Final Report: Volume Retting, R.A., Weinstein, H.B., Solomon, M.G., 2003. Analysis of motor-vehicle
1-Two-Way Stop-Controlled Intersections. National Cooperative Highway crashes at stop signs in four US cities. J. Saf. Res. 34, 485–489.
Research Program, pp. 3–46. Scialfa, C.T., Gucy, L.T., Leibowitz, H.W., Garvey, P.M., Tyrrell, R.A., 1991.
Laberge, J.C., Creaser, J.I., Rakauskas, M.E., Ward, N.J., 2006. Design of an Age differences in estimating vehicle velocity. Psychol. Aging 6, 60–66.
intersection decision support (IDS) interface to reduce crashes at rural stop- Spek, A.C.E., Wieringa, P.A., Janssen, W.H., 2006. Intersection approach speed
controlled intersections. Transp. Res. Part C: Emerg. Technol. 14, 39–56. and accident probability. Transp. Res. Part F: Traffic Psychol. Behav. 9,
Lajunen, T., Summala, H., 1995. Driver experience, personality, and skill and 155–171.

al
safety motive dimensions in drivers’ self-assessments. Personal. Individual Staplin, L., 1995. Simulator and field measures of driver age differences in
Differ. 19, 307–318. left-turn gap judgments. Transp. Res. Rec. 1485, 49–55.
Lajunen, T., Corry, A., Summala, H., Hartley, L., 1998. Cross-cultural differ- Staplin, L., Lococo, K., Sim, J., 1993. Traffic maneuver problems of older
ences in drivers’ self-assessments of their perceptual-motor and safety skills:
on drivers. Report No. FHWA-RD-92-092. McLean, VA: Federal Highway
Australians and Finns. Personal. Individual Differ. 24, 539–550. Administration.
Lee, H., 2002. The validity of driving simulator to measure on-road driving Törnros, J., 1998. Driving behaviour in a real and a simulated road tunnel—a
performance of older drivers, The 24th Conference of Australian Institutes of validation study. Accid. Anal. Prev. 30, 497–503.
rs
pe
o r's
th
Au

You might also like