You are on page 1of 11

International Journal of Educational Research Open 3 (2022) 100124

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

International Journal of Educational Research Open


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijedro

Building self-efficacy without letting stress knock it down: Stress and


academic self-efficacy of university students
Elizabeth Hitches a, Stuart Woodcock b,∗, John Ehrich c
a
Institute for Social Science Research, University of Queensland, Australia
b
School of Education and Professional Studies, Griffith University, Australia
c
School of Education, Macquarie University, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Keywords: In recent decades the number of university students who in addition to their education, balance other substantial
Stress commitments has increased, leading to greater stress and pressure to succeed in their studies. Furthermore, when
Self-efficacy students are confident, they can succeed. This study examined the stress and academic self-efficacy levels of
Teacher education
305 Australian Teacher Education students via a self-reported survey. Results suggest that females and younger
University student
students may experience higher levels of stress and lower levels of academic self-efficacy than their peers. For
Higher education
students in higher education, support or intervention to improve their belief in their capabilities regarding aca-
demic tasks need to consider targeting specific groups of students. Implications and recommendations for future
research and practice will be discussed.

Introduction demic self-efficacy levels of TE students and to determine whether dif-


ferences exist across gender, age, their stage within their degree (begin-
Higher education benefits the individual, as well as the commu- ning, middle, or end), and their teaching specialisation.
nity and society to which they contribute (Ma et al., 2016), how-
ever, the educational journey is not without its challenges. Internation- Academic stress and academic self-efficacy in higher education
ally, students report high stress related to their education, which can
have adverse effects upon their health, quality of life and academic Stress
achievement (Pascoe et al., 2020). However, when students are confi- Individuals face demands or stressors as part of their normal daily
dent academically, they experience less stress, adjust more successfully lives, however, according to Lazarus and Folkman (1986) when these
to university, and are considered ‘healthier’ and ‘happier’ individuals threaten or overwhelm the individual’s resources, or place their well-
(Chemers et al., 2001, p. 62). Success in higher education encompasses being ‘at risk’, the individual often experiences stress. Whilst low lev-
not only students’ achievement but life satisfaction, which academic els of stress can aid performance, ongoing or high levels of stress can
confidence (self-efficacy) and stress may predict, respectively (Krumrei- be detrimental to an individual’s wellbeing (Varghese et al., 2015). In-
Mancuso et al., 2013). As such, examining the latter factors may provide deed, there may be an increased risk of poorer mental and physical
insight into how best to support university students to reach their full health outcomes, and even premature death, for individuals experienc-
potential, and which individuals may be in greatest need of such sup- ing high stress who perceive it negatively impacts their health (Keller
port. et al., 2012).
This study focuses specifically upon Teacher Education (TE) students
(i.e., pre-service/trainee teachers) for the following reasons. For TE stu- Students’ experiences of stress in higher education
dents, unmanaged stress during university-level study can increase their University students encounter diverse demands, and the stress expe-
‘vulnerability to burnout’ in their future teaching career (Deasy et al., rienced in response has implications for their wellbeing and academic
2014, p. 85). Furthermore, whilst research has examined TE students’ performance (see Hitches, Woodcock, & Ehrich, 2021; Pascoe et al.,
belief in their capabilities as developing teachers (teacher self-efficacy), 2020). Students’ concerns regarding their achievement, pressure to suc-
their belief in their academic capabilities as university students (aca- ceed, and finances have been correlated with stress, anxiety and depres-
demic self-efficacy) is significantly under-researched. Adding to this lim- sion (Beiter et al., 2015). Stress related to students’ studies is reported
ited research, this study aims to examine the academic stress and aca- in high levels by students internationally, and can negatively impact


Corresponding author.
E-mail address: s.woodcock@griffith.edu.au (S. Woodcock).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijedro.2022.100124
Received 1 December 2021; Received in revised form 5 January 2022; Accepted 5 January 2022
2666-3740/© 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)
E. Hitches, S. Woodcock and J. Ehrich International Journal of Educational Research Open 3 (2022) 100124

their learning and performance, as well as physical and mental health in the measures utilised, and the internal consistency and construct va-
(Pascoe et al., 2020; Powell et al., 2018). Stress can also predict stu- lidity of academic self-efficacy scales.
dents’ life satisfaction (Krumrei-Mancuso et al., 2013), and quality of
life (Ribeiro et al., 2018). Furthermore, ongoing stress can be a precur- Academic self-efficacy in higher education
sor of poor mental health, which may further the negative effects on In higher education, students’ belief in their academic capabilities
students’ educational experiences (Pascoe et al., 2020). has been associated with their academic achievement and function-
University students’ reports of stress may vary according to gender, ing. For example, students’ academic self-efficacy has been related to
age, and how far students have progressed in their degree. For exam- their overall academic performance (Cassidy, 2012), potentially me-
ple, female students have reported higher levels of overall stress than diated by students’ regulation of effort expenditure (see Honicke &
male students (Brougham et al., 2009). Whilst males may be reluctant Broadbent, 2016). Additionally, academic self-efficacy has been asso-
to self-report mental health experiences, such findings may result from ciated with academic resilience, with efficacious students more likely
female students’ more frequent use of coping-related behaviors such as to achieve when faced with challenging academic experiences (Ayala
ruminating (see Smith et al., 2018). Limited research has examined the & Manzano, 2018; Cassidy, 2015). Studies have also found that with
stress of students identifying as nonbinary, however, the campus climate intervention, students’ self-efficacy levels can improve (see Bartimote-
and a sense of belonging may play a role in students’ stress experiences Aufflick et al., 2016).
(Budge et al., 2020). Additionally, students who are ‘older’ may be more As with stress, university students’ academic self-efficacy may vary
resilient to stress than their peers, potentially explained by their life ex- across gender, age and how far students have progressed in their course.
perience or coping skills (Stallman, 2010). However, undergraduate stu- For example, a study by Huang (2013) found that male students reported
dents in the latter stages of their degree reported higher levels of stress higher academic self-efficacy than females, except within language arts
than in earlier stages, perhaps resulting from academic demands in the domains, and the gap between genders increased with age. Furthermore,
final year (Beiter et al., 2015). Beiter et al. (2015) emphasise the need during adolescence and adulthood self-esteem may increase with age
to investigate stressors related to different stages of study in order for (Orth et al., 2018), suggesting trends in academic self-efficacy be inves-
universities to effectively cater for students’ needs. tigated also. Additionally, students’ academic self-efficacy levels may be
higher in the latter stages of their course, compared with earlier stages,
Stress and teacher education students and their approach to learning also improved (Cassidy, 2012). The cor-
Teacher Education students may experience high stress, particularly relation between academic self-efficacy and students’ performance may
in their first year, with many reporting the difficulty of managing study, also be affected by students’ progression through their studies, however,
work and family responsibilities, and having a limited understanding of further research is required (see Honicke & Broadbent, 2016).
the teaching profession (Geng & Midford, 2015). Exams and assessments
contribute to stress (Deasy et al., 2016), as do the demands of main-
taining study during professional experience placements (Deasy et al., Academic self-efficacy and teacher education students
2016; Geng & Midford, 2015). Such stress has been negatively associated Self-efficacy research has examined TE students’ belief in their ca-
with TE students’ academic performance (Gustems-Carnicer, Calderón, pabilities as developing teachers or their teacher self-efficacy (e.g.,
& Calderón-Garrido, 2019). In regard to gender, female TE students Kokkinos & Stavropoulos, 2016; Tuchman & Isaacs, 2011), however,
may experience higher stress than males (Deasy et al., 2016). How- regarding self-efficacy for their studies and for performing as univer-
ever, the effect of stress on TE students appears to decrease as age in- sity students, research is significantly limited. Turkish studies predomi-
creases (Gustems-Carnicer, Calderón, & Calderón-Garrido, 2019), and nate the literature, with findings that TE students with higher academic
time management and self-regulation of learning may lower stress and self-efficacy levels procrastinate less (Malkoc & Mutlu, 2018; Ozer &
anxiety (Heikkila, Lonka, Nieminen, & Niemivirta, 2012). For the many Yetkin, 2018), that academic self-efficacy may be impacted by teacher
TE students who experience stress, there may be reluctance to seek as- self-efficacy (Yesilyurt, Ulas, & Akan, 2016), and that early childhood
sistance, and some are unaware of how to access support (Deasy et al., TE students’ academic self-efficacy was related to their levels of moti-
2016; Geng & Midford, 2015). This is significant considering stress vation in their studies (Bedel, 2016). Additionally, in Pakistan, TE stu-
which goes unmanaged during students’ TE years can increase their like- dents’ academic self-efficacy levels were associated with their academic
lihood for burnout during their teaching career (Deasy et al., 2014). TE performance (Nasir & Iqbal, 2019). These findings illustrate that studies
courses may be beneficial sites for intervention, with implications be- should be conducted examining the academic self-efficacy of TE students
yond students’ studies (see Deasy et al., 2014), and therefore, further in other countries, such as Australia.
insight is needed into TE students’ experiences of stress to understand
their needs. The relationship between stress and self-efficacy in academic domains
Whilst a holistic view of student success includes life satisfaction, of
which students’ stress can be an important indicator, it also traditionally Stress and self-efficacy are both social cognitive factors situated
includes academic achievement, which may be predicted by students’ within Bandura’s (1978) reciprocal determinism, and within academic
academic self-efficacy (Krumrei-Mancuso et al., 2013). domains, research has illustrated that students’ levels of stress and aca-
demic self-efficacy can impact each other. For example, academic self-
Self-efficacy efficacy may predict students’ adaptive coping with stress (Freire et al.,
When individuals feel confident they can complete a particular 2019), or moderate the impact of stressors (see Zajacova et al., 2005),
task, they approach it more calmly and with greater thoughtfulness with students who are confident in their academic capabilities likely
(Chemers et al., 2001). An individual’s belief in their capabilities ‘to perceiving stressors as challenging rather than threatening, lessening
organize and execute the courses of action required to produce given their stress and seeing them more adjusted to university (Chemers et al.,
attainments’, is termed self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997, p. 3). Self-efficacy 2001). Students with low self-efficacy, however, may consider tasks to
may vary between, and within, ‘activity domains’ (Bandura, 1997, be more demanding than they are, generating stress and limiting their
p. 42), and therefore, within academic domains it has often opera- problem-solving abilities (Pajares, 1996). Complementing such find-
tionalised under the term academic self-efficacy (see Honicke & Broad- ings, physiological experiences associated with stress can influence self-
bent, 2016), defined as a student’s belief in their capability to ‘success- efficacy, providing information on the level of threat of a task and the
fully attain educational goals’ (Elias & McDonald, 2007, p. 2520). It is individual’s capabilities to meet the demands (see Bandura, 1997). Fur-
typically measured at a task-specific level (Ferla et al., 2009), however, thermore, academic self-efficacy and stress have been found to be neg-
as Honicke and Broadbent (2016) discuss, there is significant variance atively correlated, such that the more confident the student is in their

2
E. Hitches, S. Woodcock and J. Ehrich International Journal of Educational Research Open 3 (2022) 100124

academic capabilities, the less stressed, and vice versa (Chee et al., 2019; the final stage of their degree. Regarding teaching specialisation, 53.1%
Zajacova et al., 2005). were training in primary (elementary) teaching, 42.0% were training in
Research concurrently investigating university students’ secondary teaching, and 4.9% were undertaking study in another field
levels of academic self-efficacy and stress is limited, and as of education.
Zajacova et al. (2005) state, prior to their study it appeared these
constructs had been examined in relation to differing tasks, compro-
mising comparisons between academic self-efficacy and stress in higher Instrument
education environments. However, in examining both constructs in
relation to identical university-related tasks, such as preparing for This study employed the college Academic Self-Efficacy and Stress
exams, Zajacova et al. (2005) found that academic self-efficacy was a scales developed by Zajacova et al. (2005). This instrument has been
stronger predictor than stress for academic achievement, whilst stress employed in a number of studies across various higher education popu-
was a stronger predictor for students’ continued enrolment in their lations. The academic self-efficacy scale asks students to rate their con-
studies. fidence in their ability to successfully complete 27 tasks related to stu-
Success in higher education can be indicated by academic perfor- dents’ academic performance inside and outside of class, their interac-
mance and life satisfaction, which students’ academic self-efficacy and tions at college, and managing work, family and college. Scale items in-
stress levels may predict, respectively (Krumrei-Mancuso et al., 2013). clude tasks such as ‘doing well in my toughest class’ and ‘preparing for
Although research has observed differences in the stress and academic exams’. Students rate their confidence on an 11-point Likert-type scale
self-efficacy levels of university students across gender, age, and course from ‘not at all confident = 0′ to ‘extremely confident = 10′. The stress
progression, there are suggestions that instruments need to be investi- scale includes identical tasks to enable comparison with academic self-
gated to ensure differences are not the result of measurement invariance efficacy, with students reporting how stressful the tasks are for them on
or bias in academic self-efficacy scales (see Nielsen et al., 2018). Further- an 11-point Likert-type scale from ‘not at all stressful = 0′ to ‘extremely
more, research involving TE students has tended to focus upon stress and stressful = 10′. For use in this current study, minor changes were made
self-efficacy related to their role as developing teachers, and there has to context-specific terminology by the authors (such as using ‘univer-
not been the same consideration given to their role and experiences as sity’ in place of ‘college’). The stress and academic self-efficacy subscales
students of higher education, managing various educational challenges were found to have acceptable to high-reliability indices (i.e., subscales
and pressures to succeed. To effectively support them through their stud- ranged from 0.72 to 0.90; see Zajacova et al., 2005).
ies, such an understanding is essential. Currently, studies investigating
TE students’ academic self-efficacy are significantly limited and Turk-
Procedure
ish studies predominate, whilst further investigation is also required to
understand TE students’ study-related stressors. The aim of this study,
After approval was granted from the relevant Ethics Committee and
therefore, was to examine the stress and academic self-efficacy levels
participating university program directors, students were provided with
of TE students, and to determine whether differences exist across gen-
information about the study by the researchers during class and invited
der, age, and their stage of their degree (beginning, middle, or end)
to participate. Surveys were distributed in class and students deposited
using psychometrically sound measurement instruments. The following
their survey responses into a locked post-box in the following session
research questions were investigated:
for the researchers to collect. The survey consisted of two parts, the
(1) Are the academic stress and academic self-efficacy scales used in this first containing questions relating to student demographics, and the sec-
study psychometrically sound? ond containing the academic self-efficacy and stress scales developed by
(2) Do TE students’ academic stress levels for academic performance, Zajacova et al. (2005).
study skills, and interaction at university, differ across gender, teach-
ing specialisation, and stage of their degree (beginning, middle, or
Data analyses
end)?
(3) Do TE students’ academic self-efficacy levels for academic perfor-
Principal components analyses (PCA) were run on the Stress and Aca-
mance, study skills, interaction at university, and navigating univer-
demic Self-efficacy scales to determine their factor structures. This was
sity, differ across gender, teaching specialisation, and stage of their
considered appropriate by the researchers as although a number of stud-
degree?
ies have employed these scales, there is little psychometric information
(4) Are TE students’ overall academic stress levels related to age or stage
available on them (Shehadeh et al., 2020). The factors (i.e., subscales)
of their degree?
were then tested (and optimized) for their linear measurement qualities
(5) Are TE students’ overall academic self-efficacy levels related to age
using Rasch analyses, answering research question 1. Analyses of vari-
or stage of their degree?
ances (ANOVAs) were then run to determine significant differences in
(6) Are TE students’ overall academic stress and academic self-efficacy
stress and self-efficacy levels across person factors (e.g., gender, teach-
levels related?
ing speciality, and stage of their degree), answering research questions
2 and 3. A series of bivariate correlations were conducted to determine
Method the relationship between students’ age, stage of their degree, and over-
all academic stress and academic self-efficacy, answering research ques-
Participants tions 4, 5, and 6.

Participants included 305 Teacher Education students at a Sydney


metropolitan university in Australia. Of the 305 students, 78.4% identi- Results
fied as female, 19.7% identified as male, and 1.9% identified as being of
another gender, reflecting the gender ratio of in-service teachers in New Principal components analysis (PCA)
South Wales, Australia (NSW Department of Education, 2018). Further-
more, 23.0% were under the age of 20, 68.2% were aged 20 to 29, 4.6% In order to determine the internal structure of the Stress and Aca-
were aged 30 to 39, 3.3% were aged 40 to 49, and 0.9% were aged 50 demic Self-efficacy scales two separate PCAs were conducted. Maximum
and above. In terms of students’ stage of their degree, 32.1% were in the likelihood estimation and oblique (direct oblimin) factor rotation was
beginning of their undergraduate degree, 33.8% in the middle, 34.1% in used in both analyses.

3
E. Hitches, S. Woodcock and J. Ehrich International Journal of Educational Research Open 3 (2022) 100124

Table 1 Table 2
Principal component loadings for the Stress subscales. Principal component loadings for the Self-efficacy subscales.

# Item Component loadings # Item Component loadings

Component 1 (Academic Performance) Component 1 (Academic Performance)


10 Having more tests in the same week .774 13 Preparing for exams .822
8 Doing well on exams .757 8 Doing well on exams .784
18 Getting the marks I want .703 24 Finding time to study .779
13 Preparing for exams .652 18 Getting the marks I want .769
22 Doing well in my toughest class .649 14 Managing time effectively .752
3 Keeping up with the required readings .592 12 Managing both study and work .752
14 Managing time effectively .498 22 Doing well in my toughest class .716
9 Getting assessments done on time .488 9 Getting assessments done on time .700
5 Writing my assignments .437 10 Having more tests in the same week .646
1 Studying .435 5 Writing my assignments .597
Component 2 (Interaction at University) 19 Having enough money .509
2 Asking questions in class .833 Component 2 (Interaction at University)
26 Participating in class discussions .811 2 Asking questions in class .937
20 Talking to my lecturers .598 26 Participating in class discussions .837
23 Talking to university staff .571 20 Talking to my lecturers .598
21 Getting help and information at university .447 7 Making friends at university .534
Component 3 (Study Skills) Component 3 (Study Skills)
4 Understanding my lecturers -0.642 16 Improving my reading and writing skills .747
16 Improving my reading and writing skills -0.621 17 Researching for my assignments .682
27 Understanding university regulations -0.582 25 Understanding my unit readings .610
25 Understanding my unit readings -0.576 11 Taking good class notes .508
11 Taking good class notes -0.534 3 Keeping up with the required readings .495
17 Researching for my assignments -0.470 1 Studying .430
Component 4 (Managing Time and Money) Component 4 (Navigating University)
12 Managing both study and work .664 21 Getting help and information at university -0.814
19 Having enough money .613 23 Talking to university staff -0.658
24 Finding time to study .499 27 Understanding university regulations -0.586
Component 5 (Family and Friends)
15 Getting along with family members .738
6 My parents’ expectations of my grades .609
7 Making friends at university .439 Rasch analysis

In order to test the Stress and Academic Self-efficacy subscales for


their linear measurement properties we conducted Rasch analyses (see
Stress scale Tennant & Conaghan, 2007). The Rasch model (Rasch, 1960) is a prob-
The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) index (0.919) indicated acceptable abilistic mathematical model which is used to affirm the measurement
sampling with all values for individual variables above >0.5. Bartlett’s properties of tests, questionnaires, and scales in the social and medical
test of sphericity indicated that item correlations were large enough sciences. The strength of the model is that it adheres to the same mea-
for the purposes of PCA, X2 (351) = 3651, p < .001. Five eigenvalues surement principles found in the physical sciences (see Wright, 1997).
greater than 1 were identified explaining 61.3% of the variance. The In a Rasch analysis, the linearity of a measurement scale is not assumed
scree plot and component loadings also indicated a five-component so- a priori. Instead, Likert scale scores (observed data) are matched against
lution. As can be seen in Table 1, the five component solution included the theoretical expectations of the Rasch model whereby close proximity
Component 1 (Academic Performance) with items (1, 3, 5, 8, 9, 10, 13, (i.e., fit of the observed data to the Rasch model) provides evidence that
14, 18, 22); Component 2 (Interaction at University) with items (2, 20, the measurement scale is linear with an interval level structure (Tennant
21, 23, 26); Component 3 (Study Skills) with items (4, 11, 16, 17, 25, & Conaghan, 2007). Once a functioning interval level scale is affirmed
27); Component 4 (Managing Time and Money) with items (12, 19, 24); it is safe to conduct parametric testing (e.g., analysis of variance) on the
and Component 5 (Family and Friends) with items (6, 7, 15). All items data.
loaded well (standardised component loadings > 0.3). Cronbach alpha Rasch analyses were conducted on each of the Stress and Academic
tests indicated marginal to very good reliability (ranging from 0.61 to Self-efficacy subscales (identified by PCA) using the polytomous Rasch
0.87). model (PRM) with rating scale parametrization. Rasch Unidimensional
Measurement Modeling (RUMM) 2030 software (Andrich et al., 2010)
was used for all subsequent analyses. Person parameter estimates were
obtained by the Weighted Maximum Likelihood method. All subscales
Academic self-efficacy scale were tested for (1) model fit, (2) individual item fit, (3) differential item
PCA on the Academic Self-efficacy scale indicated good sampling functioning (DIF), (4) response dependency, and (5) unidimensionality.
(KMO = 0.934) with acceptable values for individual variables (all >
0.5) as noticed in Table 2. Item correlations were sufficiently large for
PCA, X2 (351) = 4748, p < .001. PCA results indicated five eigenval- Stress scale results
ues greater than 1 accounting for 67.3% of the variance. The scree plot Model fit: Overall model fit is determined by the item-trait inter-
indicated a five component solution. However, one component was dis- action, given as a chi-square statistic which indicates that the hier-
carded due to an insufficient number of loadings (items 6 and 15 only). archical ordering of items does not vary across the trait (Tennant &
Hence, a four component solution was derived. Component 1 (Academic Conaghan, 2007). A non-significant (p > .05) value indicates good fit
Performance) with items (5, 8, 9, 10, 12, 13, 14, 18, 19, 22, 24); Compo- to the Rasch model. Acceptable model fit was found in the subscales -
nent 2 (Interaction at University) with items (2, 7, 20, 26); Component Academic Performance, X2 (24) = 32.5, p = .11 (after removal of items
3 (Study Skills) with items (1, 3, 11, 16, 17, 25); and Component 4 (Nav- 1, 3, 8, and 22); Study Skills, X2 (12) = 14.7, p = .26 (after the removal
igating University) with items (21, 23, 27) (all standardised component of items 16, 25, and 26); and Interaction at University, X2 (16) = 18.6,
loadings >0.3). All subscales had very good reliability (>0.81). p = .29 (after removal of item 20). The subscales Managing Time and

4
E. Hitches, S. Woodcock and J. Ehrich International Journal of Educational Research Open 3 (2022) 100124

Table 3 Table 4
Fit of the Stress and Academic Self-Efficacy subscales to the Rasch model. Rasch fit statistics of the Stress subscales.

Item trait Interaction PSI Unidimensionality Academic Performance


Subscale X2 (df) P Item Number Fit Residual X2 P F P

Stress 1 1.926 26.341 .000 7.318 .000


Academic Performance 32.5 (24) .11 .74 4.33% 3 3.021 10.768 .029 2.164 .073
Interaction at University 18.6 (16) .29 .73 2.33% 5 0.705 1.261 .868 0.155 .960
Study Skills 14.7 (12) .26 .66 1.67% 8 -1.934 18.105 .001 6.555 .000
Managing Time & Money 38.6 (12) .00∗ .52 0.00% 9 0.182 8.235 .083 1.781 .132
Family & Friends 28.5 (12) .00∗ .39 0.00% 10 1.125 12.772 .012 3.042 .017
Academic Self-Efficacy 13 -1.107 5.634 .251 1.813 .126
Academic Performance 37.3 (28) .11 .87 7.53%∗ 14 -0.418 3.338 .502 0.889 .471
Interaction at University 17.2 (12) .14 .63 0.69% 18 -1.377 1.559 .816 0.356 .839
Study Skills 38.3 (20) .10 .80 3.44% 22 -0.709 19.431 .000 6.065 .000
Navigating University 17.8 (12) .12 .70 3.09% Interaction at University
2 1.130 4.035 .401 0.804 .524
Note. Fit statistics were attained after the removal of misfitting items. ∗ ps 20 -2.052 13.969 .007 7.199 .000
< .05 are significant. Unidimensionality results follow Smith’s (2002) t-test 21 0.065 2.651 .618 1.224 .301
method, <5% of cases is considered evidence of a unidimensional structure. 23 -1.846 6.436 .169 2.834 .025
26 -0.384 5.450 .244 2.330 .056
Study Skills
4 -0.288 2.704 .608 0.859 .488
11 -0.194 11.158 .024 5.629 .000
Money and Family and Friends did not fit the model, X2 (12) = 38.6, 16 1.249 21.769 .000 6.685 .000
p < .001 and X2 (12) = 28.5, p < .005, respectively (see Table 3). 17 1.482 0.794 .939 0.157 .959
25 -3.273 19.099 .000 9.726 .000
The Person Separation Index (PSI), which is a measure of scale reli- 27 4.111 4.739 .357 0.946 .437
ability similar to a Cronbach’s alpha, indicated acceptable PSIs (> 0.7) Managing Time and Money
(Tennant & Conaghan, 2007) in Academic Performance (0.74) and In- 12 -1.188 15.232 .004 6.555 .000
teraction at University (0.73); with marginal reliability in Study Skills 19 1.627 18.134 .001 5.846 .000
24 -1.211 5.282 .259 2.130 .077
(0.66). The subscales Managing Time and Money (0.52) and Family and
Family and Friends
Friends subscales (0.39) had very low reliability. 6 0.132 8.332 .080 2.545 .040
Item fit: There are four main approaches to affirming item fit in the 7 -0.870 9.362 .052 3.930 .004
Rasch model. Individual fit residuals which fall within the acceptable 15 0.959 10.868 .028 3.199 .013
ranges (-2.5 and +2.5) are considered to fit the model well. Residu- Note. All misfitting statistics are depicted in bold print. Items with
als constitute the difference between the observed values and the the- fit residuals <-2.5 and > 2.5 are considered misfitting. Bonferroni ad-
oretical Rasch estimates. For each item, chi-square and F-test statistics justed p values are significant at (.05/10 = .005, Academic Perfor-
(ANOVAs) are also given, where an insignificant p value (>.05) indi- mance); (.05/4 = .012, Interaction at University); (.05/6 = .008, Study
cates good fit. Finally, Item Characteristic Curves (ICCs) can determine Skills); (.05/3 = .016, Managing Time and Money); and (.05/3 = .016,
item fit. An ICC consists of a graph which plots the observed values Family and Friends).
against those of the Rasch estimates (i.e., expected values represented
on a sigmoid curve). Hence, distance between the observed values and
the theoretical curve can indicate misfit. Misfitting items were found in stress than secondary specialisation respondents, F = 10.8, p < .002 at
the subscales Academic Performance (items 1, 3, 8, 22) and Interaction Bonferroni adjusted alpha of 0.005 (see Fig. 1).
at University (item 20). In the Study Skills subscale misfit was found in However, uniform DIF was resolved after the item was split. Split-
items 11, 16, 25, and 27. However, item 11 had an acceptable mean fit ting is the process of creating individual items per grouping factor. In
residual, chi-square statistic, and an ICC which indicated good fit hence this case, item 13 was split by teaching speciality into two independent
the item was retained in the subscale. Misfit was found in all items in items consisting of an item with sole primary specialisation responses
the Managing Time and Money subscale (i.e., 12, 19, & 24) while Family and one with sole secondary specialisation responses. The secondary
and Friends had two misfitting items (7 & 15) (see Table 4). specialisation responses are treated as missing data in the item consist-
DIF: Differential item functioning detects item bias, that is, when ing of primary specialisation responses and vice versa. When item 13
groups of respondents (e.g., based on gender, culture, etc.) have the was split the reliability index (PSI) did not change (remained at 0.74).
same level of the latent trait or attribute but respond significantly dif- Hence, retaining item 13 in the subscale was justified. No other DIF was
ferently on specific items. Significant DIF indicates anomalies in the data found.
and misfit to the Rasch model. DIF is detected by two-way ANOVAs run Response dependency: Response dependency occurs when a response
on the standardised residuals for persons and items and can be observed on one item impacts on the response of another or others. Response de-
in two main forms – uniform and non-uniform. Uniform DIF is when pendency can be detected by running a PCA on the standardised resid-
two or more groups of persons with the same levels of the latent trait uals in the RUMM2030 programme and through close inspection of the
respond differently in a consistent manner across class intervals (i.e., residual correlation matrix. Items found to be highly correlated (>0.3) is
levels of the latent trait) whereas non-uniform DIF indicates inconsis- evidence of response dependency. Examination of the residual correla-
tent differences between groups across class intervals. DIF can also be tion matrix found no evidence of response dependency in the Academic
detected by inspection of each item’s ICC graph whereby distance be- Performance, Interaction at University, and Study Skills subscales.
tween comparison groups’ curves (i.e., plotted observed means by class Unidimensionality: A scale should measure only one latent trait or
interval) can indicate misfit to the model. be unidimensional, which is an important measurement requirement
DIF was run on the person factors of gender (male vs female), teach- in Rasch analysis. Unidimensionality assumptions are tested in the
ing speciality (primary vs secondary) and stage of their degree (begin- RUMM2030 programme by conducting a PCA on the person-item resid-
ning vs middle vs end) per subscale. In the Academic Performance sub- uals. In theory, the person-item residuals (the distance between the
scale uniform DIF was found by teaching speciality in item 13 (To what observed data and the theoretically derived Rasch scores) constitute
extent are the following tasks stressful for you?: Preparing for exams), random noise and consequently should behave as such (Tennant &
with primary specialisation respondents indicating higher level of exam Conaghan, 2007). Hence, any detection of a pattern in the residuals can

5
E. Hitches, S. Woodcock and J. Ehrich International Journal of Educational Research Open 3 (2022) 100124

Fig. 1. ICC for question 13 (To what extent are


the following tasks stressful for you?: Preparing for
exams) indicating significant DIF. Primary spe-
cialisation responses are plotted in blue against
secondary specialisation responses in red. As
can be seen from the graph, groups of primary
and secondary specialisation persons with the
same levels of the trait (depicted as logits on
the x axis) respond differently (i.e., have dif-
ferent expected values of responses as depicted
on the y axis). The primary specialisation re-
spondents indicate higher expected values of
affirming this item than secondary specialisa-
tion respondents.

indicate the presence of additional dimensions in the data. Patterns in Table 5


the residuals can be detected by following Smith’s (2002) t-test method. Rasch fit statistics of the Academic Self-Efficacy subscales.
Following this procedure, t-tests are conducted on all cases of person Academic Performance
measures estimated from the positively and negatively loaded items Item Number Fit Residual ChiSq P F P
from the first principal component loading of the PCA (Smith, 2002).
5 1.256 5.509 .239 1.420 .227
A large percentage of cases with significantly different person locations 8 0.263 3.864 .425 0.905 .461
(> 5%) at the 5% level are considered to breach unidimensionality as- 9 0.933 12.505 .014 3.354 .011
sumptions. Post hoc analyses on the subscales of Academic Performance 10 1.179 6.117 .191 1.680 .154
12 0.223 4.268 .371 1.243 .293
(4.33%), Interaction at University (2.33%), and Study Skills (1.67%) in-
13 -5.172 7.325 .119 4.163 .003
dicted evidence of unidimensional structures (all cases <5%). 14 -0.408 2.315 .678 0.713 .584
18 -3.090 3.013 .555 1.106 .399
Academic self-efficacy scale results 19 7.478 14.010 .007 2.295 .059
22 -2.936 6.492 .139 2.833 .025
24 -1.948 2.769 .597 0.859 .489
Overall model fit was found for Academic Performance,
Interaction at University
X2 (28) = 37.3, p = .11, (after the removal of misfitting items 13, 2 -0.194 9.726 .055 3.192 .014
18, 19, and 22); Interaction at University, X2 (12) = 17.2, p = .14, (after 7 0.589 1.119 .891 0.173 .952
the removal of misfitting item, 26); Study Skills, X2 (20) = 38.3, p = .10, 20 -0.852 6.816 .154 2.769 .028
(after the removal of misfitting item 3); and Navigating University, 26 -1.069 23.558 .000 10.818 .000
Study Skills
X2 (12) = 17.8, p = .12. Item misfit was found in the Navigating
1 1.163 8.576 .072 2.203 .069
University subscale in one item (21) (see Table 5). However, the item 3 3.008 6.723 .151 1.845 .120
was retained because the ICC indicated good functioning. 11 1.302 0.944 .918 0.192 .942
PSIs for all subscales ranged from marginal to good (0.63–0.87). All 16 -0.036 8.549 .073 2.606 .036
17 -2.060 8.077 .088 2.892 .022
subscales indicated no evidence of DIF (all ns), nor response dependency
25 -0.274 2.093 .718 0.682 .604
(all ps < .3). Post hoc unidimensionality t-tests indicated an accept- Navigating University
able (small <5%) number of cases with significantly different person 21 -2.753 6.130 .189 3.728 .005
locations for all subscales except for Academic Performance which indi- 23 -0.821 2.317 .677 0.641 .633
cated evidence of violation of unidimensionality assumptions (7.53%) 27 2.470 9.314 .053 1.803 .128

(see Table 3). Note. All misfitting statistics are depicted in bold print. Items
In summary, Rasch analyses indicated three well functioning sub- with fit residuals ⟨-2.5 and ⟩ 2.5 are considered misfitting. Bon-
scales for the Stress subscales - Academic Performance (with items 5, ferroni adjusted p values are significant at (.05/11 = .004, Aca-
9, 10, 13, 14, and 18); Interaction at University (with items 2, 21, 23, demic Performance); (.05/4 = .012, Interaction at University);
and 26); and Study Skills (with items 4, 11, and 17) and four well func- (.05/6 = .008, Study Skills); (.05/3 = .016, Navigating Univer-
tioning subscales for the Academic Self-efficacy subscales - Academic sity).
Performance (with items 5, 8, 9, 10, 12, 14, and 24); Interaction at Uni-
versity (with items 2, 7, and 20); Study Skills (with items 1, 11, 16, 17,
and 25); and Navigating University (with items 21, 23, and 27). Now
that these subscales meet the assumption of functioning linear measure- in persons and the degree of difficulty (or degree to which the latent
ment (i.e., function to the specifications of the Rasch model) it is safe to trait can be affirmed) in items. Logits increase in order of magnitude
conduct parametric testing (i.e., ANOVA comparisons). with a usual range from -4 to +4 (Ludlow & Haley, 1995).
A series of one-way ANOVAs on the person locations (logits) on the
ANOVA results Stress subscales indicated a significant effect of gender, with females
indicating higher levels of stress than males on all three subscales - Aca-
One-way ANOVAs were run on the person locations (logits) investi- demic Performance, Interaction at University, and Study Skills, (all ps
gating three between groups factors - gender (male vs female), teaching < .05) (see Table 6). The spread of person locations by gender for these
speciality (primary vs secondary) and stage of their degree (beginning vs subscales can be seen in the respective graphs in Figs. 2–4. As partici-
middle vs end). A logit, (i.e., the log of an odds ratio) is the interval-level pants in this sample who identified as of a gender other than male or
measurement unit derived from probabilistic Rasch analysis on Likert female were so few, unfortunately it was not possible to include this
scale data. Logit units describe the estimated quantity of the latent trait subgroup within the statistical tests.

6
E. Hitches, S. Woodcock and J. Ehrich International Journal of Educational Research Open 3 (2022) 100124

Fig. 2. Person-item location graph for the Stress subscale Academic Performance. Male locations represented in blue are overall further to the left of the distribution
than females represented in red indicating less of the trait of stress.

Fig. 3. Person-item location graph for the Stress subscale - Interaction at University. This graph clearly depicts female locations (in red) as further right than male
locations (in blue) indicating higher levels of the trait of stress.

Fig. 4. Person-item location graph for the Stress subscale - Study Skills. This graph shows that female locations (in red) are further to the right than male locations
(in blue) indicating overall higher levels of the trait of stress.

7
E. Hitches, S. Woodcock and J. Ehrich International Journal of Educational Research Open 3 (2022) 100124

Fig. 5. Person-item location graph for the Stress subscale – Study Skills. This graph shows that primary specialisation locations (in blue) are slightly further to the
right than secondary specialisation locations (in red) indicating higher stress levels.

Table 6 Correlational analysis


ANOVA results on mean locations by gender (male vs female).

Male Female df F P A series of bivariate correlations were conducted in order to deter-


M (SD) M (SD) mine the relationship between students’ age, stage of their degree, and
their overall levels of academic stress and academic self-efficacy (see
Stress
Academic Performance 0.35 (0.6) 0.66 (0.7) (1 - 298) 9.829 .001∗ Table 9). A weak inverse relationship between age and stress (r = -0.142,
Interaction at University -0.27(0.5) -0.06(0.7) (1 - 298) 5.148 .024∗ p < .03) and a weak positive relationship between age and academic
Study Skills -0.16(0.6) 0.03 (0.6) (1 - 298) 4.371 .037∗ self-efficacy (r = .184, p < .004) was attained.
Academic Self-Efficacy
These results indicated that as students aged they became more con-
Academic Performance 0.55 (0.8) 0.28 (0.9) (1 - 292) 4.62 .032∗
Interaction at University 0.50 (0.7) 0.34 (0.7) (1 - 293) 2.77 .097
fident and experienced less stress. A very strong inverse relationship was
Study Skills 0.74 (1.0) 0.60 (0.9) (1 - 293) 1.35 .245 attained between academic self-efficacy and stress (r = -0.604, p < .001),
Navigating University 0.60 (0.7) 0.50 (0.9) (1 - 288) 0.51 .475 indicating that the more confidence a student felt the less stress he/she
felt and vice versa.
Note. ∗ ps < .05 are significant.

Discussion
Table 7
ANOVA results on mean locations by Teaching Speciality (primary vs sec- This study examined the stress and academic self-efficacy levels of
ondary).
teacher education students. A particular strength is that the scales used
primary secondary df F P were tested for their linear measurement properties using Rasch analy-
M (SD) M (SD) sis. After the scales were optimized for linear measurement, significant
Stress differences in students’ stress and academic self-efficacy levels were at-
Academic Performance 0.59 (0.7) 0.61 (0.7) (1 - 283) 0.04 .850 tained. As such, conclusions have been drawn with the confidence that
Interaction at University -0.06(0.7) -0.15 (0.7) (1 - 283) 1.34 .248 observed differences between groups of students are not the result of
Study Skills 0.05 (0.6) -0.11 (0.6) (1 - 283) 4.89 .027∗ bias originating from the instruments used, nor potentially confound-
Academic Self-Efficacy
Academic Performance 0.30 (0.8) 0.37 (0.9) (1 - 277) 0.51 .475
ing measurement error, which were concerns raised in the literature
Interaction at University 0.40 (0.6) 0.36 (0.7) (1 - 278) 0.31 .581 (see Nielsen et al., 2018). Therefore, the findings of this study illustrate
Study Skills 0.59 (0.8) 0.70 (0.9) (1 - 278) 1.19 .276 significant differences in relation to Teacher Education students’ levels
Navigating University 0.49 (0.9) 0.57 (0.9) (1 - 273) 0.62 .431 of academic stress and academic self-efficacy across age, gender, and
Note. ∗ ps < .05 are significant. teaching specialisation, adding greatly to this under-researched area. In
addition, students’ academic self-efficacy and stress levels were nega-
tively correlated.
In relation to differences across gender, females reported higher
An effect of Teaching Speciality was found with primary speciali- stress than males regarding academic performance, study skills, and in-
sation students experiencing more stress than secondary specialisation teraction at university. That is, they experienced higher levels of stress
students on the Study Skills subscale (p < .05) (see Table 7). The spread overall in terms of tasks such as achieving the marks desired, participat-
of person locations by teaching speciality is given for this subscale in ing in discussions in class, researching for their assignments, and getting
Fig. 5. assistance and information at university. As very few participants in this
ANOVAs indicated no effects of Stage of their degree (all ns; see study identified as a gender other than male or female, unfortunately,
Table 8). On the Academic Self-efficacy subscales, only a gender effect statistical analysis was unable to be conducted on this subgroup. Other
was found, with males significantly more confident than females on the studies have also observed gender differences in stress (Brougham et al.,
Academic Performance subscale (p < .05) (see Table 6). The spread of 2009; Stallman, 2010). In considering the origins of gender-related dif-
person locations by gender on this subscale is given in Fig. 6. No other ferences, it is possible males were less likely than females to disclose
comparisons were significant (all ns). symptoms related to their mental health, as they may have perceived

8
E. Hitches, S. Woodcock and J. Ehrich International Journal of Educational Research Open 3 (2022) 100124

Table 8
ANOVA results on mean locations by Stage of their Degree (Beginning vs Middle vs End of their course).

Beginning Middle End df F P


M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

Stress
Academic Performance 0.635 (0.82) 0.502 (0.58) 0.505 (0.71) (3 - 244) 0.649 .585
Interaction at University -0.046 (0.72) -0.065 (0.56) -0.113 (0.72) (3 - 244) 0.130 .942
Study Skills 0.097 (0.60) -0.039 (0.59) 0.014 (0.63) (3 - 244) 0.779 .507
Self-EfficacyAcademic Performance 0.247 (0.79) 0.334 (0.70) 0.334 (0.94) (3 - 239) 0.219 .883
Interaction at University 0.332 (0.69) 0.407 (0.65) 0.358 (0.82) (3 - 239) 0.170 .917
Study Skills 0.520 (0.86) 0.633 (0.83) 0.745 (1.11) (3 - 239) 0.735 .532
Navigating University 0.514 (0.78) 0.438 (0.78) 0.249 (0.93) (3 - 239) 0.872 .456

Note. ∗ ps < .05 are significant.

Fig. 6. Person-item location graph for the Academic Self-Efficacy subscale – Academic Performance. Male locations (in blue) are (overall) more skewed toward the
right of the graph than female locations (in red) indicating higher levels of academic self-efficacy.

Table 9
Correlations between age of students, stage of their degree, total levels of academic
stress, and total levels of academic self-efficacy.

Measure 1 2 3 4
∗∗∗ ∗
1. age – .396 (0.075) -0.142 (0.061) .184∗ ∗ (0.068)
2. stage of their degree – -0.030 (0.071) .042 (0.071)
3. academic stress – -0.604∗ ∗ ∗ (0.058)
4. academic self-efficacy –

p < .05. ∗ ∗ p < .01. ∗ ∗ ∗ p < .001. Standard errors in parentheses.

it to be socially undesirable (see Smith et al., 2018). However, research ing when met with challenging academic experiences (Cassidy, 2015),
has indicated that females may experience greater stress due to more fre- female students’ lower academic self-efficacy in comparison to males
quent use of coping-related behaviors such as rumination (Smith et al., may negatively impact their academic success.
2018). The cause of females’ higher stress warrants further investiga- A weak relationship was observed between stress and age, such that
tion, as it can have negative implications for their academic performance as students’ age increased, students became less stressed and more con-
(see Pascoe et al., 2020), quality of life (see Ribeiro et al., 2018), and fident in their capabilities to successfully complete various university-
for those in teacher education courses, increase the likelihood that they related tasks. These results are consistent with findings that ‘older’ uni-
will experience burnout during their career (see Deasy et al., 2014). versity students may be more resilient to stress than their peers, po-
Additionally, female students reported lower academic self-efficacy tentially explained by life experience and more effective coping strate-
than males in relation to academic performance, having less confi- gies (Stallman, 2010). This suggests additional support may be re-
dence overall in their capabilities to successfully complete tasks such quired to enable younger students the skills to manage their stress, and
as writing assignments and achieving in exams. Females also expressed buffer against the potential negative effects it can have upon wellbe-
lower academic self-efficacy than males in terms of managing study ing and academic performance (see Pascoe et al., 2020, Powell & Gra-
and work, finding time to study and meeting assessment deadlines. ham, 2017). Regarding academic self-efficacy increasing with age, for
Huang’s (2013) meta-analysis illustrated that overall, females typically the related construct of self-esteem (Judge et al., 2002), in a meta-
had lower self-efficacy than males in education settings, and this cur- analysis of longitudinal studies, similar effects were observed during
rent study suggests a similar phenomenon may be occurring in the field adolescence and adulthood, with this suggested to be ‘normative’ for
of teacher education. As academic self-efficacy is associated with aca- these life stages (Orth et al., 2018). Future research may determine
demic performance (e.g., Cassidy, 2012) and the probability of achiev- whether the observed trend for academic self-efficacy is also normative.

9
E. Hitches, S. Woodcock and J. Ehrich International Journal of Educational Research Open 3 (2022) 100124

Although age-related differences were found, there were no signifi- Studies employing qualitative methodology may provide additional in-
cant effects observed for students’ stage of their degree upon their level sights which could enable educational institutions to better understand
of stress or academic self-efficacy, unlike some previous studies (see and cater for students’ differing needs, and effectively support them in
Beiter et al., 2015; Cassidy, 2012). It is possible in this current study, their studies. In addition, future studies could investigate the reasoning
students encountered different, yet relatively equal stressors through- behind the differing factor configurations of the stress and self-efficacy
out their course, as teacher education students undertake professional scales used in this study.
experience placements after the beginning stage of their studies, requir-
ing students to simultaneously manage their study and professional de- Limitations
velopment. The beginning stage of students’ degrees has been noted by
Geng and Midford (2015), and Ayala and Manzano (2018) as particu- Whilst the findings of this study contribute significantly to our un-
larly stressful as students manage competing responsibilities and their derstanding of Teacher Education students’ stress and academic self-
limited understanding of the teaching profession they are aiming to en- efficacy, there are limitations which should be considered. As students
ter. Similarly, their academic self-efficacy levels may be similar across were self-reporting their stress and academic self-efficacy, social desir-
the various stages of their course as they encounter new and different ability factors may have contributed to their responses (see Smith et al.,
stressors which must be managed. 2018). Qualitative or mixed-methods studies may provide additional in-
Additionally, this study found that those undertaking a primary formation to understand the reasoning behind students’ responses. As
teaching specialisation reported higher levels of stress relating to their this current study examined stress and academic self-efficacy within a
study skills than those undertaking a secondary teaching specialisation. single discipline, teacher education, a more comprehensive understand-
That is, they experienced higher stress overall in relation to tasks such ing of the relationship between stress, academic self-efficacy and de-
as understanding lecturers and class readings, conducting research for mographic variables may develop through examining other higher ed-
their assignments, and improving their skills in reading and writing. This ucation disciplines, with the potential of informing discipline-specific
result may be explained by the higher percentage of female teachers stress and self-efficacy interventions. Furthermore, whilst gender ratios
in the primary education sector compared to the secondary education in this sample reflected the teaching workforce in New South Wales, fu-
sector (see NSW Department of Education, 2018), with females in this ture studies may seek to investigate the role of gender with more equal
current study experiencing higher stress than males in relation to their sample sizes.
study skills. However, the specific causes of primary teacher education
students’ stress should be further examined. Conclusion
Furthermore, it was seen that students in this study who had higher
self-efficacy in academic-related tasks, reported lower levels of stress in Students’ success at university is indicated not only by their aca-
relation to these tasks. This aligns with Zajacova et al. (2005) findings, demic achievement, but also by their feelings of life satisfaction, of
adding to the limited volume of research concurrently examining stress which academic self-efficacy and stress have been predictive, respec-
and academic self-efficacy in higher education. It is suggested that when tively (Krumrei-Mancuso et al., 2013). Differences in students’ stress and
students are confident in their capabilities, they may consider stressors academic-self-efficacy levels across gender and age suggest that those
or demands as challenges, instead of threats, decreasing the stress expe- who are female and/or a young student may require additional support
rienced in response (Chemers et al., 2001). Complementing this, stu- to ensure they have the belief in their capabilities and the stress man-
dents’ self-efficacy beliefs can be influenced by the physiological ex- agement skills, which would aid them to reach their academic goals and
periences which accompany stress, which provide information regard- maintain their wellbeing throughout their studies. Higher education can
ing the threat of a task and their capabilities to meet the demands (see be both challenging and rewarding, and it is in the best interests of the
Bandura, 1997). As such, experiences or interventions which support student, the education system, and society more broadly, that our uni-
improvements in students’ academic self-efficacy beliefs may potentially versity students, and in this case, our future teachers, are supported and
improve their feelings of stress, and vice versa. enabled to reach their full potential.

Implications Declaration of Competing Interest

Higher education can present students with a number of challenges We have no known conflict of interest to disclose
as they learn to manage the demands of their course, the pressure to
succeed, and for many, the balancing of their study commitments with References
work and family responsibilities (Beiter et al., 2015). Students’ belief
Andrich, D., Sheridan, B., & Luo, G. (2010). RUMM2030: A windows program for the rasch
in their ability to meet these academic challenges can see students as unidimensional measurement model. [Computer software]. RUMM Laboratory.
‘healthier’ and ‘happier’ individuals (Chemers et al., 2001), with more Ayala, J., & Manzano, G. (2018). Academic performance of first-year university students:
successful academic performance (see Cassidy, 2012). However, when The influence of resilience and engagement. Higher Education Research & Development,
37(7), 1321–1335.
they experience high levels of stress, their quality of life and academic Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism. American Psychologist,
achievement may suffer (Pascoe et al., 2020). Regarding university stu- 33(4), 344–358. 10.1037/0003-066X.33.4.344.
dents in this current study, the findings indicate that support or inter- Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. W. H. Freeman and Company.
Bartimote-Aufflick, K., Bridgeman, A., Walker, R., Sharma, M., & Smith, L. (2016). The
vention to improve students’ belief in their academic capabilities may be study, evaluation, and improvement of university student self-efficacy. Studies in
beneficial for young and/or female students. Additionally, students who Higher Education, 41(11), 1918–1942. 10.1080/03075079.2014.999319.
are female, a young student or training in primary teaching, may benefit Bedel, E. F. (2016). Exploring academic motivation, academic self-efficacy and attitudes
toward teaching in pre-service early childhood education teachers. Journal of Educa-
from additional support with stress management and coping strategies,
tion and Training Studies, 4(1), 142–149.
and as suggested by Deasy et al. (2014), educational institutions could Beiter, R., Nash, R., McCrady, M., Rhoades, D., Linscomb, M., Clarahan, M., et al. (2015).
include such interventions in teacher education programs. Higher edu- The prevalence and correlates of depression, anxiety, and stress in a sample of col-
lege students. Journal of Affective Disorders, 173, 90–96. 10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.054
cation can be a time where individuals develop skills to manage stress
0165-0327.
throughout their lives (Brougham et al., 2009), which for teacher ed- Brougham, R. R., Zail, C. M., Mendoza, C. M., & Miller, J. R. (2009). Stress, sex differ-
ucation students, may help reduce their likelihood for burnout later in ences, and coping strategies among college students. Current Psychology, 28, 85–97.
their career (Deasy et al., 2014). 10.1007/s12144-009-9047-0.
Budge, S. L., Dominguez, S., & Goldberg, A. E. (2020). Minority stress in nonbinary stu-
Further investigation should be conducted to examine why differ- dents in higher education: The role of campus climate and belongingness. Psychology
ences across gender, age and teaching specialisation have occurred. of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 7(2), 222–229. 10.1037/sgd0000360.

10
E. Hitches, S. Woodcock and J. Ehrich International Journal of Educational Research Open 3 (2022) 100124

Cassidy, S. (2012). Exploring individual differences as determining factors in student aca- Ma, J., Pender, M., & Welch, M. (2016). Education pays 2016: The benefits of higher education
demic achievement in higher education. Studies in Higher Education, 37(7), 793–810. for individuals and society. The College Board.
10.1080/03075079.2010.545948. Malkoc, A., & Mutlu, A. K. (2018). Academic self-efficacy and academic procrastination:
Cassidy, S. (2015). Resilience building in students: The role of academic self-efficacy. exploring the mediating role of academic motivation in Turkish university students.
Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–14. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01781. Universal Journal of Educational Research, 6(10), 2087–2093.
Chee, C. L., Shorty, G., & Kurpius, S. E. R. (2019). Academic stress of Native American un- Nasir, M., & Iqbal, S. (2019). Academic self-efficacy as a predictor of academic achieve-
dergraduates: The role of ethnic identity, cultural congruity, and self-beliefs. Journal ment of students in pre service teacher training programs. Bulletin of Education and
of Diversity in Higher Education, 12(1), 65–73. 10.1037/dhe0000094. Research, 41(1), 33-42
Chemers, M. M., Hu, L., & Garcia, B. F. (2001). Academic self-efficacy and first-year college Nielsen, T., Dammeyer, J., Vang, M. L., & Makransky, G. (2018). Gender fair-
student performance and adjustment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(1), 55–64. ness in self-efficacy? A rasch-based validity study of the general academic self-
10.1037//0022-0663.93.1.5. efficacy scale (GASE). Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 62(5), 664–681.
Deasy, C., Coughlan, B., Pironom, J., Jourdan, D., & Mannix-McNamara, P. (2014). Psy- 10.1080/00313831.2017.1306796.
chological distress and lifestyle of students: Implications for health promotion. Health NSW Department of Education. (2018). Gender analysis of school teach-
Promotion International, 30(1), 77–87. 10.1093/heapro/dau086. ers. New South Wales Department of Education https://data.cese.nsw.
Deasy, C., Coughlan, B., Pironom, J., Jourdan, D., & Mannix-McNamara, P. (2016). gov.au/data/dataset/c07a2a26-cff7-3b89-93ab-7e852e9a77e0/resource/9fb0a1cf-
Psychological distress and help seeking amongst higher education students: e5a4-415b-91e1-00ac06dd0fc0/download/2017 genderanalysisschoolteachers.pdf.
Findings from a mixed method study of undergraduate nursing/midwifery and Orth, U., Erol, R. Y., & Luciano, E. C. (2018). Development of self-esteem from age 4
teacher education students in Ireland. Irish Educational Studies, 35(2), 175–194. to 94 years: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 144(10),
10.1080/03323315.2016.1146157. 1045–1080. 10.1037/bul0000161.
Elias, S. M., & MacDonald, S. (2007). Using past performance, proxy efficacy, and aca- Pascoe, M. C., Hetrick, S. E., & Parker, A. G. (2020). The impact of stress on students in
demic self-efficacy to predict college performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, secondary school and higher education. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth,
37(11), 2518–2531. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00268.x. 25(1), 104–112. 10.1080/02673843.2019.1596823.
Ferla, J., Valcke, M., & Cai, Y. (2009). Academic self-efficacy and academic self-concept: Ozer, Z., & Yetkin, R. (2018). Walking through different paths: Academic self-efficacy and
Reconsidering structural relationships. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 499– academic procrastination behaviours of pre-service teachers. Journal of Language and
505. 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.05.004. Linguistic Studies, 14(2), 89–99.
Freire, C., Ferradás, M. D. M., Núñez, J. C., Valle, A., & Vallejo, G. (2019). Well-being Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational Research,
and coping with stress in university students: The mediating/moderating role of self- 66(4), 543–578.
efficacy. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(1) Article Powell, M., & Graham, A. (2017). Wellbeing in schools: Examining the policy–practice
48. 10.3390/ijerph16010048. nexus. Australian Educational Researcher, 44, 213–231.
Geng, G., & Midford, R. (2015). Investigating first year education students’ stress level. Powell, M., Graham, A., Fitzgerald, R., Thomas, N., & White, N. (2018). Wellbeing in
Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 40(6), 1–12. 10.14221/ajte.2015v40n6.1. schools: What do students tell us? Australian Educational Researcher, 45(5), 515–531.
Gustems-Carnicer, J., Calderón, C., & Calderón-Garrido, D. (2019). Stress, coping strate- Rasch, G. (1960). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. University
gies and academic achievement in teacher education students. European Journal of of Chicago.
Teacher Education, 42(3), 375–390. Ribeiro, I. J. S., Pereira, R., Freire, I. V., de Oliveira, B. G., Casotti, C. A.,
Heikkila, A., Lonka, K., Nieminen, J., & Niemivirta, M. (2012). Relations between teacher & Boery, E. N. (2018). Stress and quality of life among university stu-
students’ approaches to learning, cognitive and attributional strategies, well-being, dents: A systematic literature review. Health Professions Education, 4(2), 70–77.
and study success. Higher Education, 64(4), 455–471. 10.1016/j.hpe.2017.03.002.
Hitches, E., Woodcock, S., & Ehrich, J. (2021). Shedding light on students with support Shehadeh, J. H., Hamdan-Mansour, A. M., Halasa, S. N., Hani, M. H. B., Nabolsi, M. M.,
needs: Comparisons of stress, self-efficacy, and disclosure. Journal of Diversity in Higher Thultheen, I., et al. (2020). Academic stress and self-efficacy as predictors of academic
Education. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dhe0000328 satisfaction among nursing students. The Open Nursing Journal, 15, 92–99.
Honicke, T., & Broadbent, J. (2016). The influence of academic self-efficacy on aca- Smith, D. T., Mouzon, D. M., & Elliot, M. (2018). Reviewing the assumptions about men’s
demic performance: A systematic review. Educational Research Review, 17, 63–84. mental health: An exploration of the gender binary. American Journal of Men’s Health,
10.1016/j.edurev.2015.11.002. 12(1), 78–89. 10.1177/1557988316630953.
Huang, C. (2013). Gender differences in academic self-efficacy: A meta-analysis. Smith, E. (2002). Detecting and evaluating the impact of multidimensionality using item
European Journal of Psychology of Education, 21, 1–5. 10.1007/s10212-011- fit statistics and principal component analysis of residuals. Journal of Applied Measure-
0097-y. ment, 3, 205–231.
Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J. E., & Thoresen, C. J. (2002). Are measures of self- Stallman, H. M. (2010). Psychological distress in university students: A com-
esteem, neuroticism, locus of control, and generalized self-efficacy indicators of a parison with general population data. Australian Psychologist, 45(4), 249–257.
common core construct? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(3), 693–710. 10.1080/00050067.2010.48210.
10.1037//0022-3514.83.3.693. Tennant, A., & Conaghan, P. G. (2007). The Rasch measurement model in rheumatology:
Keller, A., Litzelman, K., Wisk, L. E., Maddox, T., Cheng, E. R., Creswell, P. D., What is it and why use it? when should it be applied, and what should one look for
& Witt, W. P. (2012). Does the perception that stress affects health matter? in a Rasch paper? Arthritis Care and Research, 57(8), 1358–1362.
The association with health and mortality. Health psychology, 31(5), 677–684. Tuchman, E., & Isaacs, J. (2011). The influence of formal and informal formative pre-ser-
10.1037/a0026743. vice experiences on teacher self-efficacy. Educational Psychology, 31(4), 413–433.
Kokkinos, C. M., & Stavropoulos, G. (2016). Burning out during the practicum: the case Varghese, R., Norman, T. S., & Thavaraj, S. (2015). Perceived stress and self-efficacy
of teacher trainees. Educational Psychology, 36(3), 548–568. among college students: A global review. International Journal of Human Resource Man-
Krumrei-Mancuso, E. J., Newton, F. B., Kim, E., & Wilcox, D. (2013). Psychosocial factors agement and Research, 5(3), 15–24.
predicting first-year college student success. Journal of College Student Development, Wright, B. W. (1997). A history of social science measurement. Educational Measurement:
54(3), 247–266. 10.1353/csd.2013.0034. Issues and Practice, 16(4), 33–45.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1986). Cognitive theories of stress and the issue of circular- Yesilyurt, E., Ulas, A. H., & Akan, D. (2016). Teacher self-efficacy, academic self-efficacy,
ity. In M. H. Appley, & R. Trumbull (Eds.), Dynamics of stress. physiological, psycholog- and computer self-efficacy as predictors of attitude toward applying computer-sup-
ical, and social perspectives (pp. 63–80). Plenum. ported education. Computers in Human Behaviour, 64, 591–601.
Ludlow, L. H., & Haley, S. M. (1995). Rasch model logits: Interpretation, use, Zajacova, A., Lynch, S. M., & Espenshade, T. J. (2005). Self-efficacy, stress, and
and transformation. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55(6), 967–975. academic success in college. Research in Higher Education, 46(6), 677–706.
10.1177/0013164495055006005. 10.1007/s11162-004-4139-z.

11

You might also like