You are on page 1of 18

Child Development, January/February 2005, Volume 76, Number 1, Pages 196 – 211

Single Mothers’ Employment Dynamics and Adolescent Well-Being


Ariel Kalil and Kathleen M. Ziol-Guest
University of Chicago

The links between single mothers’ employment patterns and change over time in the well-being of the mothers’
adolescent children were investigated using the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. Adolescents were ages
14 to 16 at baseline, and they and their mothers were followed for 2 years. Relative to being continuously
employed in a good job, findings suggest that adolescents whose mothers lose a job without regaining em-
ployment show declines in mastery and self-esteem, those whose mothers are continuously employed in a bad
job show an increased likelihood of grade repetition, and those whose mothers are either persistently unem-
ployed or lose more than one job show an increased likelihood of school dropout. These effects are not explained
by concomitant changes in family income.

The booming economy of the middle to late 1990s, Pavetti, 1993). Keeping a job and staying off welfare
along with sweeping social policy changes, helped are more problematic; Harris (1996) reported that
single mothers reach unprecedented employment 25% to 40% of women leaving welfare via work re-
levels (Blank, 2002). Indeed, the tight labor markets turned to welfare within 1 year. Even in tight labor
of the latter half of the 1990s were most beneficial to markets, however, there is reason to be concerned
those with the fewest labor market advantages: with the quality of jobs that low-income single
younger families, minority families, and families mothers can obtain. In the 1980s, the wages and
headed by single mothers (Mishel, Berstein, & benefits associated with entry-level, low-skilled jobs
Boushey, 2003). The 1996 welfare reforms, along with declined, and it became increasingly difficult for
expansions of the Earned Income Tax Credit and low-skilled workers to earn enough to support a
Medicaid and increases in child care and job train- family at or above the poverty line (Blank, 1997;
ing, helped push single mothers’ employment Danziger & Gottschalk, 1995; Holzer, 1996; Wilson,
rates from about 57% in 1994 to almost 75% in 2000 1987).
(Blank, 2002; Meyer & Rosenbaum, 2000; Mishel et Psychologists and sociologists studying single
al., 2003). mothers’ (and, more recently, welfare recipients’)
Economists studying the labor market experiences work experiences have been concerned with how
of single mothers in the past decade have been women’s work experiences affect child well-being.
concerned both with whether single mothers who Questions of job stability and quality are also salient
enter the workforce will be able to earn a living wage in this literature. Taken together, recent studies sug-
and with whether women’s jobs are stable over time gest that low quality and instability in employment
(Johnson & Corcoran, 2003). National longitudinal may present risk factors for child well-being when
data show that before welfare reform, about half of single mothers’ work experiences are considered.
all mothers worked at some point while receiving What factors might account for the linkages between
welfare, with work accounting for about one half to poorly remunerated or unstable employment and
two thirds of all welfare exits (Harris, 1993, 1996; diminished child well-being? Two theories, drawn
from economics and psychology, respectively, are
prominent. The investments perspective (see Becker
This work was supported in part by a National Academy of & Thomes, 1986) posits that unstable or low-wage
Education/Spencer Foundation Postdoctoral Fellowship and a work limits families’ economic resources, namely,
William T. Grant Faculty Scholars Award to the first author. We are
also grateful to the McCormick-Tribune Foundation for research the income necessary to purchase the resources and
support and to the Center for Human Potential and Public Policy, goods (e.g., schools, housing, food, and safe and
which helped support the second author. We thank Greg Duncan cognitively enriched learning environments) that are
and seminar participants at Duke University, the University of critical for successful development. The family stress
Michigan, the University of Chicago, and Northwestern Univer- perspective posits that unstable or low-wage work is
sity for helpful comments.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
psychologically stressful for parents (see Conger &
Ariel Kalil, Harris Graduate School of Public Policy Studies, Uni-
versity of Chicago, 1155 East 60th St., Chicago, IL 60637. Electronic r 2005 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
mail may be sent to a-kalil@uchicago.edu. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2005/7601-0014
Single Mothers’ Employment Dynamics 197

Elder, 1994; McLoyd, Jayaratne, Ceballo, & Borquez, single mothers’ work on child development should
1994), which in turn inhibits parents’ emotional distinguish among the types of jobs in which moth-
warmth and increases parents’ harsh or erratic be- ers are employed. Indeed, there is also evidence of
haviors. These negative aspects of the families’ negative effects of maternal employment in low-in-
emotional climate are posited to increase children’s come families, especially when job quality is con-
maladjustment. sidered. Researchers drawing on national data
New evidence from nonexperimental studies (Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of
generally shows neutral or positive effects of low- Youth [CNLSY]) collected in the 1990s reported that
income mothers’ employment on young children’s parents employed in low-wage, low-complexity jobs
development. Results from the Three-City Study, a provided less nurturing home environments than
representative sample of low-income single mothers, did parents with jobs that paid more or offered more
indicated that for preschool children, mothers’ tran- complexity and autonomy, and that this effect was
sitions into employment over a 2-year period had no particularly pronounced for single mothers (Men-
effect on changes in child behavior problems (Chase- aghan & Parcel, 1995). Similarly, children of parents
Lansdale et al., 2003). Moreover, mothers’ transitions employed in lower wage, lower quality jobs showed
into work were associated with improvements in less favorable development than their counterparts
adolescents’ mental health, and stability in employ- in families with higher wage, higher quality jobs
ment was related to declines in adolescents’ exter- (Moore & Driscoll, 1997; Parcel & Menaghan, 1990).
nalizing behavior problems. In contrast, mothers’ This raises some concerns about the effects of em-
job losses were linked with adolescents’ increased ployment in low-wage, low-benefit jobs.
behavioral problems. Kalil, Dunifon, and Danziger Finally, a noteworthy finding from the recent ex-
(2001) showed similar findings using data from the perimental studies (such as MFIP) is that adolescents
Women’s Employment Survey, a longitudinal survey in families where parents are leaving welfare and
of current and former welfare recipients in Michigan. increasing their employment under mandatory as
The authors consistently found that the intensity of well as voluntary conditions were at some increased
workFmonths worked and hours worked per risk of school difficulties, in contrast to the positive
weekFhas little effect on school-age children’s be- effects of mandated work identified for younger
havior problems. However, the number of transitions children (Gennetian et al., 2002). Negative effects for
between working and not working increased chil- older children seemed to be concentrated among
dren’s anxious and depressed behavior, net of other those who had a younger sibling at home, suggesting
factors. that some adolescents might take on increased (and
Recent experimental evaluations have identified possibly stressful) household responsibilities in the
more consistently positive aspects of mandated work face of their mothers’ transitions into work.
programs on low-income young children, particu- Why might low-income mothers’ employment
larly when the programs not only encourage work experiences differentially affect adolescents’ devel-
but also make work pay. In the Minnesota Family opment (compared with younger children)? The
Investment Program (MFIP), young children of sin- present article focuses on the quality and stability of
gle-parent, long-term welfare recipients who were employment; there are several reasons to think that
required to work, but also benefited from financial these characteristics might have a greater impact on
incentives to do so, had positive outcomes on older children.
measures of school performance and behavior First, self-care during the afternoon and evening
problems compared with a control group who par- hours when single mothers are working is presum-
ticipated in the traditional welfare program (Gen- ably more common among older children. However,
netian & Miller, 2002). An important pathway of single mothers in low-wage, low-quality jobs may
influence was through the program’s financial in- have little flexibility to attend to supervisory needs
centives, which led to increased income and reduced at home or monitor their children, even from afar
poverty. Similarly, another experimental work-based (Heymann, 2000). This might create more opportu-
income-supplement programFNew HopeFim- nities for older children to engage in risky behaviors
proved school performance and social behavior or fall under the negative influence of peers. Thus,
among school-age boys (Huston et al., 2001). unsupervised time in low-income neighborhoods
In sum, these recent findings lend support to the may be especially problematic for adolescents’
contention that work that provides a living wage can social and emotional development (Furstenberg,
benefit the development of children of single moth- Cook, Eccles, Elder, & Sameroff, 1999). Time
ers and suggests that studies of the influence of spent unsupervised or engaged in nonproductive
198 Kalil and Ziol-Guest

activities might also detract from adolescents’ aca- This suggests that the positive impacts of increased
demic progress. employment on the children of low-income, single
Second, adolescence is a period of identity for- mothers may be driven by incremental increases in
mation during which emerging visions of oneself income (see also Harvey, 1999). Conversely, negative
and the future begin to influence educational and effects of job loss and unemployment in this popu-
occupational planning and behavior. Parental role lation may be driven by income decline.
modeling may be especially salient during this de-
velopmental stage. Thus, for example, adolescents’
observations of their mothers’ work experiences may
shape their own views of the economic opportunities Method
associated with performing well, or remaining, in
Sample
school (Haveman & Wolfe, 1994). On the one hand,
adolescents may be motivated to stay in school to Data for this study are drawn from the National
secure eventually better or more stable jobs than the Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 (NLSY79) and
jobs their mothers are able to obtain. However, an CNLSY. The NLSY79 is a nationally representative
adolescent’s perception of the returns to education sample of 12,686 youth (6,283 females, 6,403 males)
might diminish on seeing his or her single mother ages 14 to 22 years in 1979 (Hispanic, Black, and low-
lose a job or remain persistently employed in a low- income youth were oversampled). The primary re-
quality job. search focus of the NLSY79 is labor force behavior,
We used longitudinal data from a national sample but a range of other important demographic and be-
of single mothers whose employment experiences havioral information has also been collected. These
we observed over a 2-year period during the middle youth were reinterviewed every year until 1994 and
to late 1990s. We advanced the hypotheses tested in biennially since 1994. In 1986 a separate survey of the
recent studies in several ways. First, we relied on children of the original NLSY79 female respondents
mothers’ weekly work histories to create detailed were interviewed (CNLSY). Children’s cognitive,
patterns of employment, taking into account the socioemotional, and physiological development as
stability of mothers’ employment over this period as well as a variety of attitude, aspiration, and psycho-
well as the quality of jobs in which mothers were logical well-being questions have been administered
employed as indicated by the jobs’ wages and ben- for age-appropriate children biennially. Starting in
efits. We then linked these employment patterns to 1994, and biennially thereafter, children who were 15
change over time in measures of emotional well- or older (young adults) completed an extensive
being (self-esteem and mastery) and academic pro- questionnaire modeled after the NLSY79 question-
gress (school dropout and grade repetition) among naire. Given their emphasis on the labor market ex-
these mothers’ adolescent children. periences and household economic well-being of
Next, we examined the potential mediating role of adults and high-quality information on the activities
changes in mothers’ income between survey waves and well-being of children, these data are ideally
as a potential explanation for any observed associa- suited for the research questions posed here.
tions between mothers’ employment experiences We merged the female respondents from the
and adolescents’ development. Unfortunately, the NLSY79 with their young adult children. We drew
data we used did not provide measures of changes in respondents from the 1994 and 1996 survey waves
parenting or family socialization processes; thus, we and followed these respondents for 2 years (until
were not able to test theoretically important media- their next interview) in 1996 and 1998, respectively.
tors laid out in the family stress model. However, We then limited the sample to young adults (here-
recent studies suggest a relatively greater impor- after, adolescents) who were 14, 15, or 16 at the be-
tance of economic resources in linking maternal ginning of the 2-year observation period. The 1994 to
employment to child development in low-income 1996 cohort of 14-, 15-, and 16-year-olds consists of
families. In both the MFIP and New Hope, for ex- 495 mothers with 538 adolescent children; the 1996
ample, there were virtually no program impacts on to 1998 cohort of 14-, 15-, and 16-year-olds consists of
parenting behavior or on the quality of the home 657 mothers with 695 adolescent children. We re-
environment (Gennetian & Miller, 2002; Huston et stricted this sample to families in which mothers
al., 2001). Moreover, in Chase-Lansdale et al. (2003), were single (unmarried) at the beginning of the 2-
the quality of mothers’ parenting (e.g., structured year observation period. The sample sizes for single
family routines or cognitive stimulation) did not mothers with children in the age range of interest
change as mothers’ employment status changed. include 231 mothers with 258 children in the 1994 to
Single Mothers’ Employment Dynamics 199

1996 cohort and 299 mothers with 314 children in the Independent Variables
1996 to 1998 cohort.
Employment patterns. Mothers’ employment pat-
terns were classified based on several labor force
Dependent Variables
characteristics associated with the 2-year interval
Mastery. Adolescents responded to seven items between survey waves. Mothers’ employment pat-
from the Pearlin Mastery scale (Pearlin, Lieberman, terns were assigned to six mutually exclusive
Menaghan, & Mullan, 1981). Sample items include groups: (a) continually employed in a good job, (b)
‘‘There is really no way I can solve some of the continually employed in a bad job, (c) continually
problems that I have’’ and ‘‘Sometimes I feel that I’m unemployed, (d) exactly one job loss followed by
being pushed around in life’’ (1994: a 5 .72; 1996: reemployment, (e) exactly one job loss without re-
a 5 .70; 1998: a 5 .74). The mastery questions were gaining employment, and (f) more than one job loss
measured on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) (reemployment not specified). Employment patterns
to 4 (strongly agree), and the measure represents the were available only for women for whom all weeks
sum of responses. Responses to negative items were between interviews were accounted for. Mothers
reverse-coded so that higher scores on the summary were characterized as continually employed if they
scale represent greater mastery. worked in each week since the first interview. The
Self-esteem. Adolescents responded to 10 items continually employed mothers were further classi-
assessing self-esteem using the Rosenberg self-es- fied as continually working in good or bad jobs.
teem scale (Rosenberg, 1979/1986). Designed for Following Johnson and Corcoran (2003), a good job
adolescents and adults to measure an individual’s is one that is (a) at least 35 hr per week, pays at least
self-evaluation, the scale describes a degree of ap- $7 per hour (1999$), and offers health insurance, or
proval or disapproval toward oneself. Sample items (b) at least 35 hr per week and pays more than $8.50
include ‘‘I feel that I’m a person of worth, at least on per hour (1999$) if it does not offer health insurance.
an equal basis with others’ and ‘‘I feel that I have a A bad job (a) pays less than $7 per hour (1999$) even
number of good qualities’’ (1994: a 5 .85; 1996: if it offers health insurance or (b) pays between $7
a 5 .85; 1998: a 5 .87). The self-esteem questions and $8.50 per hour (1999$) and offers no health in-
were measured on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly surance. For a single mother with two children the
disagree) to 4 (strongly agree), and the measure rep- net annual income of a full-time worker at these
resents the sum of responses. Responses to negative wage levels would be 19% and 13% above the 1999
items were reverse-coded so that higher scores on federal poverty line for a family of three, respec-
the summary scale represent greater self-esteem. tively (see Johnson & Corcoran, 2003, Table 1, for
Grade repetition. We determined whether adoles- more details).
cents repeated a grade in the 2-year interval in which Because mothers’ work characteristics were re-
they appeared in the study. Responses were coded as ported for up to five jobs held during the 2-year
a dichotomous variable (coded 1 if yes and 0 if no). window, those who were continually employed but
Adolescents were asked if they had ever repeated a switched jobs between interviews were character-
grade at both the beginning and ending of the interval ized as having a good job if the average of their job
and which grades they had repeated. Adolescents characteristics was equivalent to a good job. Simi-
also reported what grade in school they were cur- larly, mothers with more than one job were charac-
rently in at both the beginning and end of the interval. terized as having a bad job if the average of their job
If the reported grades repeated at the end of the in- characteristics was equivalent to a bad job.
terval correspond to grades held back between the The mothers coded as continually unemployed
two time points, the adolescent was characterized as were either unemployed or out of the labor force for
having repeated a grade during that time. all of the weeks since the first interview. For parsi-
Dropout. Adolescents specified whether they ever mony, we combined these two groups of nonem-
dropped out of regular school for at least 1 month, ployed mothers and refer to them as unemployed.
regardless of whether they returned to school, and Mothers were characterized as losing exactly one job
the month and year of the most recent dropout. If a and regaining employment if they reported one gap
dropout occurred between the beginning and the in employment and were employed at the end of
end of the 2-year interval and a return to school was their 2-year interval. Mothers were characterized as
not indicated, the respondent was coded as having losing exactly one job and not regaining employ-
dropped out of school. Responses are coded as a ment if they reported one gap in employment and
dichotomous variable (coded 1 if yes and 0 if no). were not working at the end of the 2-year interval.
200 Kalil and Ziol-Guest

Table 1 was measured as a dichotomous variable (coded 1 if


Descriptive Statistics of Study Variables boy and 0 if girl).
Control variables: Mothers’ characteristics. We con-
M or % SD
trolled for several maternal demographic character-
Employment patterns istics. First, age and years of education (both
Good job 22% measured at the first interview) were entered as
Bad job 17% continuous variables. Second, we controlled for
Not working 24% race/ethnicity, coded as Hispanic, Black, and White
Lose and do not regain 6% (the reference group). Although all of the mothers in
Lose and regain 18% the sample were single at baseline, we included a
Lose more than one 13% dichotomous variable for whether the mother was
Youth characteristics
never married at the first interview (coded 1 if never
Age (baseline) 15.05 0.72
married and 0 if ever married). Household compo-
Gender (boy) 51%
Mother characteristics sition was captured with four variables measured at
Age (baseline) 34.56 1.99 the baseline interview, drawing on detailed infor-
Hispanic 19% mation from the household roster to identify the
Black 62% presence of individuals who reside in the household.
White 19% The first measure was the total number of children
Baseline years of education 11.73 2.01 under the age of 18 residing in the household and
Baseline never married 40% could be the child of the mother in the sample or any
Baseline number of children 2.59 1.49 other related or nonrelated person. The second
Baseline number of adults 0.52 0.92 measure was the total number of adults residing in
Baseline presence of cohabitor 15%
the household (not including a domestic partner).
Baseline age of youngest child in home 8.91 5.07
AFQT (1980) 20.29 19.44
The third measure was a dichotomous variable for
CES – D (1992) 14.33 10.85
whether the mother reported a partner or cohabitor
Mediator residing in the household (coded 1 if yes and 0 if no).
Change in income ($1) 674.10 15,583.57 Finally, the fourth measure was a continuous varia-
Baseline scores on outcome variables ble representing the age of the youngest child (re-
Mastery 21.33 2.87 gardless of whether he or she is the mother’s own
Self-esteem 31.75 4.19 child) in the household.
Ever previously repeated a grade 36% We also included measures of mother’s ability and
Ever previously dropped out of school 8% depressive symptoms. These two measures were
Outcomes assessed before the assessment of mothers’ work
Mastery 21.90 3.14
patterns and are important to include as control
Self-esteem 32.64 4.13
variables to the extent that they help select mothers
Repeated a grade 11%
Dropped out of school 13% into different patterns of employment and may in-
fluence adolescents’ well-being. Mothers’ ability was
Note. Sample sizes differ for each variable. Youth characteristics measured with the Armed Forces Qualification Test
are reported for all 439 children. Mother characteristics that are (AFQT) percentile score. The AFQT is a measure of
measured once (race, Armed Forces Qualification Test [AFQT],
and Center for Epidemiologic Studies – Depression [CES – D]) are basic skills, or human capital, attained (Neal &
reported for 369 mothers. Baseline characteristics (employment Johnson, 1996), which was administered to NLSY
patterns, mother’s age, education, never married, and family size) sample members in 1980. It assesses paragraph
are reported for 409 families. Mastery and self-esteem are reported
for 431 youth, grade repetition is reported for 427 youth, and comprehension, arithmetic reasoning, word knowl-
dropped out of school is reported for 435 youth. edge, and mathematics knowledge. Mothers’ de-
pressive symptoms were measured in the NLSY 1992
interview with the Center for Epidemiologic Stud-
Finally, mothers were characterized with multiple ies – Depression (CES – D; Radloff, 1977) scale. The
job losses if they reported more than one gap in 20-item CES – D is a widely used, standardized, self-
employment during that time. Small sample sizes report scale designed to measure depressive symp-
precluded further specification of the latter group. toms in the general population. Sample items in-
Control variables: Adolescents’ characteristics. We clude ‘‘I was bothered by things that usually don’t
controlled for two child demographic characteristics bother me’’ and ‘‘I felt that I couldn’t shake off the
in the models: age and gender. Age was measured as blues even with help from my family and friends.’’
a continuous variable at the first interview. Gender The questions were measured on a scale ranging
Single Mothers’ Employment Dynamics 201

from 0 (rarely/none of the time/1 day) to 3 (most/all of the some of her baseline characteristics, as well as em-
time/5 – 7 days), and the measure represents the sum ployment patterns, may differ for those two children.
of responses (a 5 .89). Responses to positive items There is wide variation in mothers’ employment
were reverse-coded so that higher scores on the patterns. Although the largest single group of single
summary scale represent greater risk of depression. mothers is not working during the 2-year window
Scores of 16 or more are commonly taken as indica- (24%), the next largest group is continually working
tive of depression (Weissman, Sholomskas, Pot- in a good job (22%). The balance is distributed be-
tenger, Prusoff, & Locke, 1977). tween those continually working in a bad job (17%),
Mediator variable: Change in income. Annual in- losing one job and not regaining employment (6%),
come was measured as the sum of reported income losing one job and regaining employment (18%), and
in the following categories: military income; wages/ losing more than one job (13%). In analysis not
salary/tips; net business and farm income; unem- shown, we examined the types of jobs those who lose
ployment compensation; child support; welfare, and regain are employed in at the end of the 2-year
food stamps, other welfare, and social security in- interval and find that almost all of the mothers re-
surance; disability/Veteran’s Affairs benefits; inher- gain employment in bad jobs. We therefore do not
itances/gifts; and income from other sources. Our further distinguish the patterns of job loss followed
measure of reported income differs from the com- by reemployment.
puted measure of income provided in the NLSY data Mothers continually working in good jobs earned
in that if respondents indicated that they received an average hourly wage of $12.00 (1999$) and
income in a particular category but did not specify worked an average of 41 hr per week. Ninety-two
an amount received, they were assigned the mean percent of these mothers were covered by health
value of that category based on the valid responses. insurance the entire time we observed them (data not
This helps reduce the amount of missing data in the shown in tables). Mothers continually working in
analyses. Prior-year income was collected from the bad jobs earned $7.82 per hour and worked an av-
mother at the interview date, such that in 1994 erage of 33 hr per week. Seventy-one percent of these
mothers report 1993 calendar year income. The in- mothers were covered by health insurance the entire
comes for each year available (1993, 1995, and 1997) time we observed them.
were computed in 1999 dollars. We created a change Among mothers who lost one job but regained
in income score such that the baseline income (1993 employment, the hourly wage (averaged across jobs)
and 1995) was differenced from the ending income was $8.07 and hours worked were about 35 per
(1995 and 1997). Change in income was used as a week. The median proportion of weeks these moth-
proxy for changes in families’ economic circum- ers were out of work in the 2-year interval was 34%.
stances and was measured in thousands in the re- Mothers who lost a job without regaining one earned
gression analysis. $8.64 on that job and worked 38 hr per week. The
median proportion of weeks these mothers were out
of work in the 2-year interval was 59%. Finally,
Results mothers who cycled between jobs earned an average
of $6.65 per hour and worked about 39 hr per week.
Sample Description
The median proportion of weeks these mothers were
Table 1 presents the overall means and standard out of work in the 2-year interval was 52%.
deviations of all variables in the analysis. Sample Adolescents were on average 15 years old, with an
sizes for each variable vary depending on when and equal number of boys and girls. Mothers were on
how it was measured. Adolescents’ characteristics average 35 years old at baseline. At the time of their
are reported for the 439 children in the sample. adolescent’s birth, these mothers were relatively
Mothers’ characteristics that are measured once, in- young; on average they were about 20 years old and
cluding her race/ethnicity and AFQT and CES – D 20% were less than 18 at that time. This young ma-
scores, are reported for 369 mothers. Time-varying ternal age is comparable to participants in other re-
characteristics of the mothers that are drawn from cent studies of single mothers’ employment and
the baseline observation in each of the two cohorts, adolescent outcomes. For example, low-income
such as mother’s age and education, are reported for mothers with children ages 10 to 14 years old in the
409 families. A family refers to the number of times Three-City Study were, on average, 38 years old
a mother’s information is counted. For example, a (Chase-Lansdale et al., 2003). In the experimental
mother may have one adolescent in the 1994 to 1996 welfare studies of adolescents and their families,
cohort and another in the 1996 to 1998 cohort, and about 19% of mothers were less than 18 at the child’s
202 Kalil and Ziol-Guest

birth (Gennetian, 2004). The modal number of although this figure varied widely across the six
mothers’ own children in the household was two employment groups (data not shown). Only mothers
(data not shown), 15% of mothers reported residing who were continually working demonstrated in-
with a cohabitor, and the youngest child in the come growth, on average, over the 2-year interval,
household was 8.9 years on average. Forty percent of although the median amount of growth was small
the mothers had never married, and a majority was (4% for continually employed mothers in both good
Black. The average score on the CES – D scale was and bad jobs). All other groups experienced income
relatively high and was approaching the cutoff in- declines on average, with unemployed mothers
dicating risk for clinical levels of depressive symp- experiencing the least amount of decline ($1,140)
toms. Forty-one percent of mothers overall were at and cyclers experiencing the most ($2,759; data not
risk for depression according to their CES – D score shown).
(data not shown).
Regression Analyses
Adolescents’ Outcomes
Multivariate regression analyses were conducted
On average, the adolescents reported scores that predicting each of the four adolescent outcomes.
were higher than the statistical midpoint on the Different regression techniques were adopted de-
mastery and self-esteem measures, with averages of pending on the nature of the outcome variable. Or-
22 and 33, respectively. For a benchmark comparison dinary least squares (OLS) regression was used to
of these figures, we estimated the average mastery estimate mastery and self-esteem. The dichotomous
and self-esteem scores in a cross-section of all 14- to outcomes (grade repetition and school dropout)
16-year-olds in the 2000 CNLSY (approximately 745 were modeled using a logistic regression. We cor-
youth). The average mastery score was 22 and the rected the standard errors (using the cluster option in
average self-esteem score was 27 in this population. STATA) in all analyses to account for the presence of
Thus, although the mastery scores are comparable, siblings in the data (clustering on the mother).
the self-esteem scores of the adolescents from these Variables were entered into the analysis in blocks
two cohorts of predominantly Black, low-income, in three separate models. Model 1 included the
single mothers, compared with a more representative mothers’ employment pattern over the 2-year inter-
population, were substantially higher. This corre- val (continually employed in a good job is the ref-
sponds to results reported in a recent meta-analysis erence group) and the baseline value of each child
showing that Black children have higher self-esteem outcome measure. Including the baseline value of
scores than do Whites and that this advantage not the outcome measure as an independent variable
only increases with age but is greater among low- provided a proxy for (a) unmeasured genetic influ-
rather than among middle- or high-socieconomic- ences; (b) selection characteristics that discriminate
status groups (Gray-Little & Hafdahl, 2000). families with different employment patterns, but
With respect to the educational outcomes, 11% of only those selection factors that are captured by the
the adolescents in this sample repeated a grade in the baseline outcome measure; and (c) adolescent’s prior
2-year interval and 13% dropped out during this functioning, which would at least partially reflect the
time. No national statistics are collected on grade effects of earlier maternal employment histories
retention; however, it is estimated that 5% to 7% of (Cain, 1975). In the grade repetition and dropout
public school children are retained annually (Center regressions, we included a dichotomous variable
for Policy Research in Education, 1990). National indicating whether the adolescent reported ever
statistics on high school dropout rates suggest that in previously repeating a grade or dropping out, re-
any given year (within the last 15 years) approxi- spectively, before our baseline observation. This
mately 5% of young adults enrolled in high school differs from the baseline outcome variables used in
will leave without successfully completing the pro- the analyses of self-esteem and mastery because
gram (Kaufman, Alt, & Chapman, 2001). Thus, our grade repetition and dropping out could have oc-
sample generally mirrors a national sample with curred at any time in the child’s life leading up to the
respect to educational progress. day they entered the survey.
The method of including a lagged dependent
variable, sometimes referred to as an autoregressive
Change in Income
model, was used for these analyses rather than a
The average level change in income across the fixed-effects approach. We adopted this empirical
entire sample was a decline of about $674 (1999$), strategy because we are interested in examining the
Single Mothers’ Employment Dynamics 203

effects of mothers’ employment patterns that did not (p 5 .064). These findings suggest that mothers’ de-
change over our 2-year observation period (e.g., mographic and personality characteristics and abili-
persistent unemployment) and it is not possible to ty explain much of the associations observed
derive these effects with a fixed-effects regression between employment patterns and adolescents’
model. However, it is important to point out that the mastery. Nevertheless, the effect size for decline in
assumption underlying (and a limitation of) the au- mastery associated with losing one job and not re-
toregressive approach is that it only controls for se- gaining one, although only marginally statistically
lection factors that are perfectly determined by the significant, is about .33 SD, a sizable effect. We also
prior measure of the specific outcome. In contrast, a observed that children with Black mothers had
fixed-effects model is more flexible insofar as it greater increases over time in their mastery scores. A
controls for any time-invariant unobserved factor mother’s educational attainment at baseline is asso-
that equally affects both the prior and current ciated with a decline in mastery over time, whereas
measures of the outcome variable. mothers’ higher AFQT scores are associated with
It should also be noted that including the lagged increased mastery over time. These same patterns
variable in the present analyses does not control for hold in the mediated model (Model 3), but change in
mother- or family-level unobserved characteristics. income is not a significant predictor of change over
For example, a mother who is highly motivated to time in adolescents’ mastery.
work might also be highly motivated to make sure Table 3 presents the regression results predicting
her adolescent succeeds in school. The fact that we adolescents’ self-esteem. Model 1 is significant, and
cannot account for this maternal characteristic po- the measures of continual unemployment, losing a
tentially produces an upward bias of the estimates job and not regaining employment, and losing a job
for employment patterns on adolescent outcomes. with reemployment, are all associated with a decline
In addition, the lagged dependent variable might over time in adolescents’ self-esteem relative to those
not control for time-varying unobserved adolescent- whose mothers are continually employed in a good
level characteristics. An adolescent who develops job. The baseline measure of self-esteem is also sig-
anxiety or depression might affect a mother’s nificant. With the inclusion of the block of variables
employment decision as well as that adolescent’s in Model 2, the continual unemployment variable is
success in school. We note this limitation in the dis- no longer significant; however, losing a job and not
cussion while acknowledging that our models con- regaining employment continues to be associated
trol for a rich set of observable characteristics that are with a decline over time in adolescents’ self-esteem,
most likely to be sources of selection bias. as does losing a job and regaining one (albeit at
Model 2 added the adolescents’ demographic marginal levels of significance, p 5 .058). Adoles-
characteristics as well as the measures of mothers’ cents with Black mothers had increases in self-
demographic characteristics, household composi- esteem over time, and, as with mastery, mother’s
tion, and AFQT and CES – D scores. A final model AFQT is associated with increases in self-esteem.
(Model 3) tested change in the level of reported in- Model 3 suggests that even after controlling for
come over the 2-year interval as a potential mediator changes over time in income, mothers’ losing one job
of any observed effects of the employment patterns. and not regaining employment is predictive of a
We also analyzed the data using the percentage decline in adolescents’ self-esteem, and losing one
change in income as a mediator, but doing so did not job and regaining employment is marginally pre-
change any of the point estimates reported here. dictive (p 5 .061) of a decline in adolescents’ self-
Mastery and self-esteem. Table 2 presents the re- esteem. The effect of the mother’s losing one job and
gression results for mastery. Model 1 is significant not regaining employment is equivalent to a .40 SD
and several of the employment patterns are indi- decline in adolescents’ self-esteem.
vidually significant. The adolescents whose mothers Grade repetition and school dropout. Table 4 presents
were continually unemployed or lost one job and did the results for grade repetition. Model 1 shows that
not regain employment showed a decline over time having a mother who is continually employed in a
in their mastery scores relative to those whose bad job is associated with greater odds of grade
mothers were continually employed in a good job. repetition. The prior grade repetition variable is also
The baseline mastery score is also significant. Model highly significant. Once the child and mother char-
2 is also significant, but the coefficient for continual acteristics are introduced as controls in Model 2, this
unemployment drops below significance, whereas employment pattern drops just above conventional
the coefficient for losing one job and not regaining levels of significance (p 5 .053). Among the back-
employment drops to marginal levels of significance ground variables, having a mother with higher
204 Kalil and Ziol-Guest

Table 2
Ordinary Least Squares Results: Mastery (n 5 431)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

B SE B SE B SE

Employment patterns
Bad job 0.01 0.38 0.07 0.39 0.07 0.39
Not working 0.91 0.39 0.63 0.43 0.63 0.43
Lose and do not regain 1.31 0.51 1.03w 0.56 1.03w 0.56
Lose and regain 0.37 0.47 0.29 0.47 0.29 0.47
Lose more than one 0.03 0.52 0.18 0.55 0.18 0.55
Youth characteristics
Age (baseline) 0.10 0.20 0.10 0.20
Gender (boy) 0.09 0.28 0.09 0.28
Mother characteristics
Age (baseline) 0.02 0.08 0.02 0.08
Hispanic 0.66 0.49 0.66 0.49
Black 1.03 0.44 1.03 0.44
Baseline years of education 0.17 0.08 0.17 0.09
Baseline never married 0.13 0.33 0.13 0.33
Baseline number of children 0.15 0.12 0.15 0.12
Baseline number of adults 0.22 0.14 0.21 0.14
Baseline presence of cohabitor 0.41 0.41 0.42 0.41
Baseline age of youngest child in home 0.03 0.04 0.03 0.04
AFQT (1980) 0.03 0.01 0.03 0.01
CES – D (1992) 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01
Baseline scores on outcome variables 0.45 0.05 0.43 0.05 0.43 0.05
Mediator
Change in income (1,000) 0.00 0.01
Constant 12.75 1.18 12.00 4.12 12.02 4.15

F test 15.10 6.88 6.55


R2 0.18 0.22 0.22

Note. AFQT 5Armed Forces Qualification Test; CES – D 5 Center for Epidemiologic Studies – Depression scale.
w
po.10. po.05. po.01. po.001.

educational attainment reduces the odds that the child significant. Adolescents with older mothers and
will repeat a grade, whereas residing in a household those whose mothers never married (vs. divorced)
with a cohabitor increases these odds. Having more are less likely to drop out. In contrast, the greater the
children residing in the household is also associated number of adults residing in the household, the
with increased odds of grade repetition. The mea- higher are the odds of dropping out. Results from
sure of change in income, introduced in Model 3, is Model 3 suggests that above and beyond the child
significant and suggests that an increase in income of and mother characteristics and changes in income,
$1,000 during the 2-year interval decreases the odds of having a mother who is continually unemployed
the adolescent repeating a grade by 2 percentage or who loses more than one job is still predictive
points. Furthermore, the indicator variable for con- of dropping out. All other things constant, adoles-
tinual employment in a bad job is again significant at cents whose mothers were continually unemployed
conventional levels in this final model. were 7.40 times more likely than those whose
Finally, Table 5 presents the findings for dropping mothers were continually employed at a good job
out. Model 1 is highly significant. In addition to the to drop out of school in the 2-year window.
prior measure of dropping out, this model indicates Furthermore, adolescents whose mothers lost more
that having a mother who is continually unemployed than one job were 3.55 times more likely to drop
increases the odds of dropping out by 4.69 and hav- out of school in the 2-year period compared with
ing a mother who loses more than one job increases adolescents whose mothers were continually em-
the odds of dropping out by 3.03. Model 2 is also ployed at a good job.
Single Mothers’ Employment Dynamics 205

Table 3
Ordinary Least Squares Results: Self-Esteem (n 5 431)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

B SE B SE B SE

Employment patterns
Bad job 0.38 0.58 0.31 0.59 0.32 0.59
Not working 1.09 0.50 0.81 0.54 0.80 0.54
Lose and do not regain 2.19 0.83 1.71 0.83 1.69 0.84
Lose and regain 1.28 0.60 1.15w 0.61 1.14w 0.61
Lose more than one 0.26 0.59 0.48 0.62 0.50 0.62
Youth characteristics
Age (baseline) 0.19 0.24 0.20 0.24
Gender (boy) 0.15 0.35 0.15 0.35
Mother characteristics
Age (baseline) 0.09 0.09 0.09 0.09
Hispanic 0.29 0.62 0.29 0.62
Black 1.15 0.57 1.16 0.57
Baseline years of education 0.08 0.10 0.07 0.10
Baseline never married 0.26 0.37 0.26 0.37
Baseline number of children 0.16 0.14 0.17 0.14
Baseline number of adults 0.06 0.18 0.06 0.18
Baseline presence of cohabitor 0.05 0.47 0.04 0.47
Baseline age of youngest child in home 0.03 0.04 0.02 0.04
AFQT (1980) 0.02w 0.01 0.02w 0.01
CES – D (1992) 0.02 0.02 0.02 0.02
Baseline scores on outcome variables 0.46 0.04 0.44 0.04 0.44 0.04
Mediator
Change in income (1,000) 0.01 0.01
Constant 18.60 1.41 19.37 5.15 19.20 5.18

F test 25.28 10.09 9.59


R2 0.25 0.28 0.28

Note. AFQT 5Armed Forces Qualification Test; CES – D 5 Center for Epidemiologic Studies – Depression scale.
w
po.10. po.05. po.01. po.001.

To provide a better sense of this wide range of re- mothers lost more than one job. The predicted prob-
sults on the two academic participation measures, we abilities are similar for boys and girls (data not
computed the predicted probabilities of both grade shown), but White boys and girls with the same base
repetition and dropping out from the regression co- characteristics as their Black counterparts have
efficients in Model 3, and we present the findings in higher predicted probabilities of dropping out com-
Table 6. To do so, we chose a hypothetical base-case pared with the Black youth (data not shown).
adolescent to whom we assigned the mean value of
all continuous predictors and the characteristics
Sensitivity Tests
Black, male, mother was never married, no cohabitor
present, and no previous grade repetition (or drop- The results presented here were obtained in a se-
out) for the predictors coded as dummy variables. ries of fairly conservative tests insofar as they used
The first row of this table indicates, for example, that large-scale panel data that allowed for controlling
for such adolescents, the percentage who repeated a the lagged version of each dependent variable, as
grade in the 2-year interval is .03 for those whose well as a wide range of potentially important selec-
mothers were continually employed in a good job, tion factors, including preexisting measures of
and the percentage who dropped out under such mothers’ achievement and her mental health. In-
conditions is .04. In contrast, the percentage who re- deed, in several cases, the introduction of these
peated a grade in the 2-year interval is .06 and the background characteristics reduced significant as-
percentage who dropped out is .14 for those whose sociations between employment patterns and change
206 Kalil and Ziol-Guest

Table 4
Logistic Regression Results: Repeated a Grade (n 5 427)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

B SE B OR B SE B OR B SE B OR

Employment patterns
Bad job 1.43 0.68 4.18 1.32w 0.68 3.76 1.41 0.71 4.08
Not working 1.25w 0.65 3.50 0.63 0.73 1.88 0.65 0.76 1.91
Lose and do not regain 1.28 0.84 3.61 0.74 0.89 2.09 0.75 0.90 2.11
Lose and regain 1.33w 0.73 3.78 0.98 0.76 2.67 1.03 0.78 2.79
Lose more than one 1.03 0.77 2.81 0.83 0.75 2.29 0.77 0.76 2.16
Youth characteristics
Age (baseline) 0.19 0.25 0.83 0.24 0.24 0.79
Gender (boy) 0.58 0.38 1.78 0.57 0.38 1.76
Mother characteristics
Age (baseline) 0.11 0.09 1.11 0.11 0.09 1.11
Hispanic 0.37 0.67 1.45 0.33 0.67 1.39
Black 0.42 0.62 1.52 0.39 0.63 1.47
Baseline years of education 0.21 0.09 0.81 0.23 0.09 0.79
Baseline never married 0.17 0.43 0.84 0.20 0.44 0.82
Baseline number of children 0.22w 0.12 1.24 0.23w 0.12 1.25
Baseline number of adults 0.02 0.21 0.98 0.01 0.20 0.99
Baseline presence of cohabitor 1.07 0.39 2.93 1.03 0.40 2.81
Baseline age of youngest child in home 0.04 0.04 0.96 0.03 0.04 0.97
AFQT (1980) 0.01 0.01 1.01 0.01 0.01 1.01
CES – D (1992) 0.01 0.02 1.01 0.01 0.02 1.01
Baseline scores on outcome variables 1.29 0.35 3.65 1.22 0.35 3.39 1.25 0.36 3.51
Mediator
Change in income (1,000) 0.02 0.01 0.98
Constant 3.87 0.61 3.59 5.94 2.67 5.98

Wald w2 20.65 53.50 52.36


Pseudo R2 0.08 0.17 0.18

Note. OR 5 odds ratios; SE B 5 standard errors of the B; AFQT 5Armed Forces Qualification Test; CES – D 5 Center for Epidemiologic
Studies – Depression scale.
w
po.10. po.05. po.01. po.001.

over time in adolescents’ well-being to nonsignifi- examined a series of regressions that controlled for
cance, highlighting the importance of including preexisting measures of the adolescents’ behavior
these important control variables. Given this fairly problems as well as a measure of whether the ado-
rigorous set of controls, we have a reasonable lescent was in poor physical health. We assessed
amount of confidence in the results presented here. these measures when the child was age 11 or 10,
Nevertheless, we performed a series of sensitivity before our assessment of the adolescent outcome
tests, modifying the original analyses to include measures or the mothers’ work experience. In no
measures that may affect the findings to try to rule case were any of these variables individually sig-
out any additional spurious associations. For exam- nificant, nor did including them in the regressions
ple, in examining these results, one should be con- change any of the associations presented here in any
cerned about the direction of the observed meaningful way (results available on request).
associations. That is, could the results be interpreted In addition, an obvious problem inherent in non-
as suggesting that a decline in adolescents’ psycho- experimental research is determining causality. If
logical well-being causes mothers to lose jobs or joblessness and job termination were random acts
withdraw from the work force? Or is a decline in perpetrated by the market, it would be reasonable to
adolescents’ self-esteem or mastery a proxy for an interpret job-holding patterns as a reflection of the
increase in the adolescents’ behavior problems that environment rather than of the individual’s tastes
interrupt mothers’ work? To address this issue, we and propensities. Of course, the truth is that many of
Single Mothers’ Employment Dynamics 207

Table 5
Logistic Regression Results: Dropped Out of School (n 5 435)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

B SE B OR B SE B OR B SE B OR

Employment patterns
Bad job 0.20 0.66 1.22 0.39 0.69 1.48 0.39 0.69 1.48
Not working 1.55 0.51 4.69 2.00 0.53 7.41 2.00 0.53 7.40
Lose and do not regain 0.25 0.95 1.29 0.38 0.88 1.47 0.38 0.88 1.46
Lose and regain 0.74 0.55 2.10 0.92w 0.56 2.51 0.92w 0.56 2.51
Lose more than one 1.11 0.53 3.03 1.27 0.59 3.55 1.27 0.59 3.55
Youth characteristics
Age (baseline) 0.27 0.23 1.31 0.27 0.23 1.31
Gender (boy) 0.08 0.31 0.92 0.08 0.31 0.92
Mother characteristics
Age (baseline) 0.23 0.08 0.79 0.23 0.08 0.79
Hispanic 0.20 0.52 0.82 0.20 0.53 0.82
Black 0.49 0.42 0.61 0.49 0.42 0.61
Baseline years of education 0.00 0.08 1.00 0.01 0.08 0.99
Baseline never married 0.64w 0.38 0.53 0.64w 0.38 0.52
Baseline number of children 0.18 0.12 0.84 0.18 0.12 0.84
Baseline number of adults 0.29 0.15 1.33 0.29 0.15 1.33
Baseline presence of cohabitor 0.59 0.56 0.55 0.60 0.56 0.55
Baseline age of youngest child in home 0.02 0.04 0.98 0.02 0.04 0.98
AFQT (1980) 0.02 0.01 1.02 0.02 0.01 1.02
CES – D (1992) 0.01 0.01 1.01 0.01 0.01 1.01
Baseline scores on outcome variables 1.55 0.42 1.76 0.52 5.79 1.76 0.52 5.80
Mediator
Change in income (1,000s) 0.00 0.01 1.00
Constant 2.94 0.44 1.42 4.91 1.43 4.92

Wald w2 29.84 50.38 50.76


Pseudo R2 0.09 0.16 0.16

Note. OR 5 odds ratios; SE 5 standard errors of the B; AFQT 5Armed Forces Qualification Test; CES – D 5 Center for Epidemiologic
Studies – Depression scale.
w
po.10. po.05. po.01. po.001.

these mothers might have quit their jobs voluntarily related to the traits that influence job holding and
or provoked a termination by their behavior. It is that it is these characteristics that are responsible for
easy to imagine that the quality of parenting is also the declines over time in adolescents’ functioning
that we observe here. Unfortunately, the data do not
Table 6 provide any information on the reason for job sepa-
Predicted Probabilities for Grade Repetition and Dropping Out rations (i.e., voluntary vs. involuntary). However, we
conducted tests that included indicator variables for
Grade repetition Dropping out whether any of the mothers’ reported income came
from unemployment insurance or from cash welfare
Good job .03 .04
Bad job .11 .06
assistance (i.e., unemployment insurance is only
Not working .05 .25 available to those whose job separation is involun-
Lose and do not regain .06 .06 tary). These variables could possibly proxy for the
Lose and regain .08 .10 nature of the job separation or for mothers’ tastes for
Lose more than one .06 .14 employment (we note, however, that we have no
way of identifying mothers who voluntarily leave
Note. The hypothetical base category is a teenager assigned the employment for reasons such as lack of job flexibility
mean value of all continuous predictors and the characteristics of
Black, male, mother is never married, no cohabitor present, and no or needed time to spend monitoring or supervising
previous grade repetition or drop out. children). Finally, we performed the regressions with
208 Kalil and Ziol-Guest

a series of controls for the unemployment rate for the of job instability on adolescents’ self-esteem and
metropolitan statistical area in which the NLSY mastery correspond with the results from Kalil et al.
mothers resided (these data are limited in the NLSY (2001) and with those from Chase-Lansdale et al.
for confidentiality reasons). Our results changed (2003), both of which examined low-income mothers’
very little under all of these alternative specifications job instability in younger children. In Chase-Lans-
(results available on request). dale et al.’s study of 10- to 14-year-olds, mothers’
Finally, although our data are not as detailed as exits from employment were associated with in-
would be ideal to examine adolescents’ increased creases in adolescents’ depressive symptoms and
responsibilities as a potential mediator of maternal aggressive behavior problems (but not with tests of
employment experiences, we performed the regres- their math or reading skills). In slight contrast to the
sions again separately for adolescents with a recent experimental evidence (e.g., Gennetian, 2004),
younger sibling, and we examined adolescents’ em- we found no negative association between maternal
ployment. In the former case, results did not differ employment and school dropout but we did find a
for adolescents with and without a younger sibling. negative association, as those studies did, on stu-
In the case of adolescent employment, we found that dents’ grade repetition. Collectively, these results
mothers’ employment at a bad job significantly in- suggest that maternal job loss in a low-income pop-
creased the odds by 2.5 that the teenager would be ulation may be especially detrimental for adoles-
employed at follow-up, controlling for teens’ base- cents’ mental health. In our data, we were able to
line employment. Because mothers’ employment at a provide greater insight into the nature of these job
bad job also significantly predicts teenagers’ grade exits, insofar as these negative effects were observed
repetition, we examined whether teenagers’ em- for mothers who lost jobs but failed to regain one
ployment predicts grade repetition. However, it does during the 2-year interval. Comparable declines in
not (p 5 .303). Thus, we find no evidence for the ar- mastery were not observed for those whose mothers
gument that adolescents’ own employment explains lost one job but regained employment and were
negative effects of maternal work experiences. employed at the time of the follow-up interview.
Recall that among the group that lost jobs without
regaining employment, the median proportion of
Discussion
weeks those mothers were out of work in the 2-year
We found that under certain conditions, the em- interval was a fairly sizable 59% (i.e., more than half
ployment experiences of low-income single mothers of the time we observed them). Moreover, the me-
are associated with declines in their adolescents’ dian proportion of weeks unemployed among
well-being. In particular, relative to the group of mothers who cycled back and forth between work
adolescents whose mothers were continuously em- and nonwork was also sizable (52%); this pattern
ployed at a job that paid a living wage, negative ef- was associated with an increased risk of school
fects were observed for adolescents whose mothers dropout. In contrast, the median proportion of weeks
experienced unstable employment trajectories over a unemployed among mothers who lost exactly one
2-year period. Employment instability was associat- job but regained one by the time of the follow-up
ed with an increased risk of school dropout and with interview was only 34%. These findings suggest that
declines in adolescents’ self-esteem and mastery. In it may not only be job loss, per se, that negatively
the case of school dropout, employment instability in affects child well-being, but the length of time spent
the form of maternal cycling back and forth between out of work following the loss.
work and nonwork was detrimental at trend levels. Relatively few prior studies have examined the
In the case of adolescents’ self-esteem and mastery linkages between job conditions and child well-be-
scores, instability in the form of job loss that was not ing. The collective work of Menaghan and Parcel is
followed by reemployment within a 2-year period an exception, but much of their work has focused on
was associated with declines in well-being. A more preschool age children. Parcel and Menaghan (1990),
limited negative impact of employment at a bad job for example, linked the pay rate of mothers’ jobs
was also observed; adolescents whose mothers had and its substantive complexity to young children’s
this type of work experience were at greater risk of (ages 3 – 6) scores on a test of verbal skills, which
grade repetition. Finally, we found a negative effect were mediated in part by children’s experiences in
of mothers’ persistent unemployment on adoles- the home. These authors argued that routinized,
cents’ risk of school dropout. low-quality jobs erode opportunities for parents’
These findings replicate and extend those found intellectual flexibility and exacerbate psychological
in previous reports. For example, the negative effects distress, and that these factors create less respon-
Single Mothers’ Employment Dynamics 209

sive parenting. In our study, we linked mothers’ multiple jobs, or do not work at all may present a
continuous employment in a bad job (i.e., low- more negative self-concept to their children, which
paying or low-benefit job) to adolescents’ increased could be reflected in the children’s own declining self-
risk of grade repetition. To the extent that the out- concepts. In the case of persistent unemployment,
comes in both of these studies reflect children’s school these mothers may be less effective in conveying the
experiences, the results from these two studies appear importance of staying attached to school as an im-
fairly complementary even thought the age groups portant component of future employment success.
examined were different. Finally, we found a negative Finally, the ways in which maternal work experi-
association between mothers’ continuous unemploy- ences are associated with adolescents’ time use or
ment and their adolescents’ increased risk of school experiences at school, with their peers, or in their
dropout, a finding that corresponds to Wilson (1996) neighborhoods are all potentially important as ex-
and McLoyd et al. (1994). planatory variables, but it is regrettable that few
What factors account for these various linkages measures of these behaviors existed in our data.
between different employment experiences and ado- Several issues warrant exploration in future re-
lescents’ well-being? Based on previous research, we search. Most important, researchers need to know
anticipated finding a significant mediating role of what the short- and long-term implications are of the
change in income, and our data were particularly well observed effects on the adolescent outcomes mea-
suited to test this argument. However, we found no sured. Grade repetition, for example, has long-term
support for the role of income as a mediator of these implications for adolescents, primarily as one of the
observed linkages between maternal employment strongest predictors of dropping out of school and not
and adolescents’ well-being. In fact, change in income returning (Jimerson, Anderson, & Whipple, 2002). It
was itself independently associated with only one of could be that dropping out of school may not portend
the four outcomes we examined (grade repetition). negative future employability if the student has the
The changes in income associated with mothers’ dif- opportunity to apprentice in a vocational field or re-
fering employment patterns were possibly too mod- ceive job training. Unfortunately, these opportunities
est to make much difference in adolescents’ lives. are increasingly rare for low-income youth. Between
Income (both level and change) has been identified in 1985 and 2003, the Department of Labor decreased
previous studies as a relatively more important de- investments in the Workforce Investment Act funding
terminant of young children’s rather than older chil- by 33%, with programs for disadvantaged adults and
dren’s development (Duncan & Brooks-Gunn, 1997), youth experiencing the greatest share of funding re-
and this may explain why our analyses with adoles- ductions (Spence & Kiel, 2003). Thus, results pre-
cents found a limited role for income. sented here suggest that the well-being of adolescents
Thus, in the absence of significant mediating may be compromised as their single mothers negoti-
effects of income changes, and the relatively weak ate work in the low-wage labor market.
evidence from prior studies of the explanatory Available jobs in this sector often have unpredict-
effects of quality of mothers’ parenting, what can able or nontraditional schedules that can prompt job
explain the linkages we observed in this study? These separations, or they provide wages and benefits that
questions remain important for future research. Al- are too meager to support the families’ economic vi-
though observable income change, per se, did not ability. Ideally, all single mothers would find and
help explain the linkages between job instability and keep good jobs, but in the real world, many low-in-
adolescents’ declines in psychological functioning, it come mothers are not qualified to get these jobs;
may be that mothers’ or adolescents’ psychological moreover, such jobs are not available to everyone who
perceptions of their families’ current or future eco- wants one, particularly in a period of economic
nomic viability are compromised when job loss is stagnation. Thus, the results from this study point to
followed by a lengthy period of unemployment and the importance of not only helping mothers keep jobs
that these worries and anxieties diminish well-being. once they find them but also providing the economic
Maternal job loss or instability may provoke other supports that make these jobs as much like good jobs
intrafamily stresses or conflicts that threaten youth’s possible, even if the actual wage rate is low. To ad-
adjustment. More detailed psychological measures dress the former issue, further research is needed to
would be useful to pursue these ideas. identify the factors predicting job loss in this popu-
The role of mothers as role models, and the ways in lation. A myriad number of possibilities exist, ranging
which they shape adolescents’ self-concepts, might from transportation problems to a lack of soft skills
also be an important mediating factor. Mothers who that help workers get along with coworkers and su-
lose jobs and have difficulty gaining another one, lose pervisors. Once identified, these problems can be
210 Kalil and Ziol-Guest

targeted for intervention. To address the second issue, Harvey, E. (1999). Short-term and long-term effects of early
the continued provision and expansion of subsidies parental employment on Children of the National Lon-
and incentives, such as the Earned Income Tax Credit, gitudinal Survey of Youth. Developmental Psychology, 35,
as well as child and health care supports, can help 445 – 459.
make even lower wage jobs an economically viable Haveman, R., & Wolfe, B. (1994). Succeeding generations: On
the effects of investments in children. New York: Russell
option for single mothers and their children.
Sage Foundation.
Heymann, J. (2000). What happens during and after school:
Conditions faced by working parents living in poverty
References and their school-age children. Journal of Children &
Becker, G. S., & Thomes, N. (1986). Human capital and Poverty, 6, 5 – 20.
the rise and fall in families. Journal of Labor Economics, 4, Holzer, H. (1996). What employers want: Job prospects for less-
S1 – S139. educated workers. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Blank, R. (1997). It takes a nation. New York: Russell Sage Huston, A. C., Duncan, G. J., Granger, R., Bos, J., McLoyd, V.,
Foundation. Mistry, R., et al. (2001). Work-based anti-poverty programs
Blank, R. (2002). Evaluating welfare reform in the United for parents can enhance the school performance and social
States. Journal of Economic Literature, 15, 1105 – 1166. behavior of children. Child Development, 72, 318 – 336.
Cain, G. (1975). Regression and selection models to im- Jimerson, S. R., Anderson, G. E., & Whipple, A. D. (2002).
prove nonexperimental comparisons. In C. Bennett & Winning the battle and losing the war: Examining the
A. Lumsdaine (Eds.), Evaluation and experiment: Some relation between grade retention and dropping our of
critical issues in assessing social programs (pp. 297 – 317). high school. Psychology in the Schools, 39, 441 – 457.
New York: Academic Press. Johnson, R. C., & Corcoran, M. E. (2003). The road to eco-
Center for Policy Research in Education. (1990). Repeating nomic self-sufficiency: Job quality and job transition
grades in school: Current practice and research evidence. CPRE patterns after welfare reform. Journal of Policy Analysis
Policy Briefs, RB-04-1/90, U.S. Department of Education. and Management, 22, 615 – 640.
Chase-Lansdale, P. L., Moffitt, R., Lohman, B., Cherlin, A., Kalil, A., Dunifon, R., & Danziger, S. K. (2001). Are chil-
Coley, R., Pittman, L., et al. (2003). Mothers’ transitions dren’s behavior problems affected by their mothers’
from welfare to work and the well-being of preschoolers work participation since welfare reform? In G. Duncan
and adolescents. Science, 299, 1548 – 1552. & P. L. Chase-Lansdale (Eds.), For better and for worse:
Conger, R., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1994). Families in troubled Welfare reform and the well-being of children and families
times. New York: Walter de Gruyter. (pp. 154 – 178). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Danziger, S., & Gottschalk, P. (1995). America unequal. New Kaufman, P., Alt, M. N., & Chapman, C. D. (2001, No-
York: Russell Sage Foundation. vember). Dropout rates in the United States: 2000.
Duncan, G., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1997). The consequences of (National Center for Education Statistics, Statistical
growing up poor. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Analysis Report, U.S. Department of Education.
Furstenberg, F., Cook, T., Eccles, J., Elder, G., & Sameroff, McLoyd, V. C., Jayaratne, T. E., Ceballo, R., & Borquez, J.
A. (1999). Managing to make it: Urban families and adoles- (1994). Unemployment and work interruption among
cent success. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. African American single mothers: Effects on parenting
Gennetian, L. (2004). How sibling composition affects ad- and socioemotional functioning. Child Development, 65,
olescent schooling outcomes when welfare reform poli- 562 – 589.
cies increase maternal employment. Eastern Economic Menaghan, E., & Parcel, T. (1995). Social sources of change
Journal, 30, 81 – 100. in children’s home environments: The effects of parental
Gennetian, L., Duncan, G., Knox, V., Vargas, W., Clark- occupational experiences and family conditions. Journal
Kauffman, E., & London, A. (2002). How welfare and of Marriage and the Family, 57, 69 – 84.
work policies for parents affect adolescents: A synthesis of Meyer, B. D., & Rosenbaum, D. T. (2000). Making single
research. New York: Manpower Demonstration Research mothers work: Recent tax and welfare policy and its
Corporation. effects. National Tax Journal, 53, 1027 – 1062.
Gennetian, L., & Miller, C. (2002). Children and welfare Mishel, L., Berstein, J., & Boushey, H. (2003). The state of
reform: A view from an experimental welfare program working America. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
in Minnesota. Child Development, 73, 601 – 620. Moore, K. A., & Driscoll, A. K. (1997). Low-wage maternal
Gray-Little, B., & Hafdahl, A. (2000). Factors influencing employment and outcomes for children: A study. The
racial comparisons of self-esteem: A quantitative review. Future of Children (special edition Welfare to Work), 7,
Psychological Bulletin, 126, 26 – 54. 122 – 127.
Harris, K. (1993). Work and welfare among single mothers Neal, D. A., & Johnson, W. R. (1996). The role of premarket
in poverty. American Journal of Sociology, 99, 317 – 352. factors in Black-White wage differences. Journal of Po-
Harris, K. (1996). Life after welfare: Women, work, and litical Economy, 104, 869 – 895.
repeat dependency. American Sociological Review, 61, Parcel, T., & Menaghan, E. (1990). Maternal working condi-
407 – 426. tions and children’s verbal facility: Studying the inter-
Single Mothers’ Employment Dynamics 211

generational transmission of inequality from mothers to Spence, R., & Kiel, B. (2003). Skilling the American workforce
young children. Social Psychology Quarterly, 53, 132 – 147. ‘‘on the cheap’’: Ongoing shortfalls in Federal funding for
Pavetti, L. (1993). The dynamics of welfare and work: Exploring workforce development. Workforce Alliance Report,
the process by which women work their way off welfare. Washington, DC.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Weissman, M. M., Sholomskas, D., Pottenger, M., Prusoff,
Pearlin, L., Lieberman, M., Menaghan, E., & Mullan, J. B. A., & Locke, B. Z. (1977). Assessing depressive
(1981). The stress process. Journal of Health and Social symptoms in five psychiatric populations: A validation
Behavior, 22, 337 – 356. study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 106, 203 – 214.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES – D scale: A self-report de- Wilson, W. J. (1987). The truly disadvantaged: The inner city,
pression scale for research in the general population. the underclass, and public policy. Chicago: University of
Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385 – 401. Chicago Press.
Rosenberg, M. (1986). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Wilson, W. J. (1996). When work disappears: The world of the
Books. (Original work published 1979) new urban poor. New York: Knopf.

You might also like