You are on page 1of 12

This article was downloaded by: [Wayne State University]

On: 09 April 2015, At: 08:25


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Applied Neuropsychology: Child


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hapc20

A Neuropsychological Profile of Childhood


Maltreatment Within an Adolescent Inpatient Sample
a b b
Brian Kavanaugh , Karen Holler & Gregg Selke
a
Department of Clinical Psychology , Antioch University New England , Keene , New
Hampshire
b
Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior , Alpert Medical School of Brown
University , Providence , Rhode Island
Published online: 24 Oct 2013.

Click for updates

To cite this article: Brian Kavanaugh , Karen Holler & Gregg Selke (2015) A Neuropsychological Profile of
Childhood Maltreatment Within an Adolescent Inpatient Sample, Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 4:1, 9-19, DOI:
10.1080/21622965.2013.789964

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2013.789964

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD, 4: 9–19, 2015
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 2162-2965 print/2162-2973 online
DOI: 10.1080/21622965.2013.789964

A Neuropsychological Profile of Childhood Maltreatment


Within an Adolescent Inpatient Sample
Brian Kavanaugh
Department of Clinical Psychology, Antioch University New England, Keene, New Hampshire

Karen Holler and Gregg Selke


Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Alpert Medical School of Brown University,
Providence, Rhode Island
Downloaded by [Wayne State University] at 08:25 09 April 2015

Recent research has begun to identify the neurocognitive and psychological effects of
childhood maltreatment, although information is limited on the neuropsychological
presentation of maltreatment in psychiatrically hospitalized adolescents. This study
examined the executive-functioning and language abilities as well as psychopathological
presentation of childhood maltreatment victims in an adolescent psychiatric inpatient
setting. The sample consisted of adolescent inpatients (ages 13–19 years old) who
completed a neuropsychological/psychological assessment during hospitalization
(n = 122). The sample was grouped based on childhood maltreatment history, with one
group categorized by maltreatment history (n = 49) and the other group characterized by
no maltreatment history (n = 73). Analyses revealed statistically significant differences
(p < .01) between maltreatment groups on executive functioning, as well as on measures
of self-reported depression and anxiety symptoms. No group differences remained after
controlling for posttraumatic stress disorder. Further, distinct neuropsychological profiles
were identified for specific types of maltreatment experienced. These findings suggest
that while childhood maltreatment is associated with a range of neuropsychological
impairments, the specific type of maltreatment experienced may have a significant
influence on the type and severity of impairments. These findings contribute to the
growing body of research on the significant consequences of childhood maltreatment.

Key words:  adolescence, inpatient, maltreatment, neurocognitive, psychopathology

Childhood maltreatment is defined as sexual abuse, phys- Many victims of childhood maltreatment do not experi-
ical abuse, emotional abuse, or neglect toward a child (De ence significant developmental consequences, with some
Bellis et al., 1999; Mash & Barkley, 2007). Childhood even experiencing select positive outcomes, such as post-
maltreatment is a significant national health concern, traumatic growth (Alisic, van der Schoot, van Ginkel, &
with recent statistics estimating that approximately 5.9 Kleber, 2008; Wilson, Hansen, & Li, 2011). Despite this,
million children in the United States were maltreated in childhood maltreatment is a significant risk factor for
2010, indicated by referrals to Child Protective Services various developmental, neurocognitive, and psychiatric
(U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, conditions and is characterized by severe and long-term
Administration on Children, Youth, and Families, 2012). consequences (De Bellis, Spratt, & Hooper, 2011;
Kendall-Tackett, 2000; Mash & Barkley, 2007; Pechtel &
Address correspondence to Brian Kavanaugh, Department of
Pizzagalli, 2011; Wilson et al., 2011).
Clinical Psychology, Antioch University New England, 40 Avon Street, While enriching environments enhance typical brain
Keene, NH 03431. E-mail: BKavanaugh@antioch.edu development, executed through neurogenesis and selective
10    KAVANAUGH, HOLLER, & SELKE

neuronal pruning, brain development can be significantly critical developmental periods (De Bellis et al., 2011;
restricted by negative experiences such as stress (Taber, Pechtel & Pizzagalli, 2011; Wilson et al., 2011). Two of
Salpekar, Wong, & Hurley, 2011; Teicher, Tomada, & these neurocognitive domains include executive func-
Andersen, 2006). When the body’s major biological stress tions, predominantly correlated with the prefrontal
systems (e.g., hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, sym- cortex, and language, primarily correlated with the left
pathetic nervous system, and serotonin system) are acti- cerebral cortex, along with the posterior corpus callosum
vated to manage elevated and prolonged exposure to and cerebellum (De Bellis et al., 2011; Pechtel & Pizzagalli,
stress, a shift occurs from a process of development and 2011; Wilson et al., 2011). Executive functions are self-
growth to one of preservation and survival. The neurobio- regulatory actions to achieve goals, such as those actions
logical consequences of extreme childhood stress include of planning and problem solving, cognitive flexibility,
the elevated presence of catecholamines (e.g., epinephrine, response inhibition, working memory, processing flu-
norepinephrine, and dopamine), corticotropin-releasing ency, and attention (Anderson, 2002; Barkley, 2012;
hormones, cortisol, and serotonin in the circulatory Baron, 2004; Henry & Bettenay, 2010; Luria, 1965;
system (De Bellis, Keshavan, Spencer, & Hall, 2000; De Mahone & Slomine, 2007; Willcutt, 2010). Further, these
Bellis et al., 2011; Kirsch, Wilhelm, & Goldbeck, 2011; theoretical hypotheses have been proven in human stud-
Lupien et al., 2005; Teicher et al., 2003; Twardosz & ies, which have identified executive and language impair-
Lutzker, 2010; Wilson et al., 2011). Especially in child- ments in child and adolescent victims of maltreatment
hood and adolescence, critical periods of postnatal brain (Allen & Oliver, 1982; Beers & De Bellis, 2002; De Bellis,
Downloaded by [Wayne State University] at 08:25 09 April 2015

development, the lack of healthy opportunity for growth Hooper, Spratt, & Woolley, 2009; DePrince, Weinzierl, &
can negatively influence the development of critical neural Combs, 2009; Fishbein et al., 2009; Kocovska et al., 2012;
mechanisms (De Bellis et al., 2000, 2011; Heim, Shugart, Nolin & Ethier, 2007; Samuelson, Krueger, Burnett, &
Craighead, & Nemeroff, 2010; Kearney, Wechsler, Kaur, Wilson, 2010; Spann et al., 2012; Stacks, Beeghly,
& Lemos-Miller, 2010; Lupien et al., 2005; Pechtel & Partridge, & Dexter, 2011; Sylvestre & Merette, 2010).
Pizzagalli, 2011; Taber et al., 2011; Teicher et al., 2003; Two studies by De Bellis and colleagues have been par-
Twardosz & Lutzker, 2010; Wilson et al., 2011). ticularly influential. Beers and De Bellis (2002) found
Childhood maltreatment has been associated with that compared with nonmaltreated children, children
reduced volume of the cerebral cortex and subcortical with maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder
structures, such as the prefrontal cortex, right temporal (PTSD) displayed significantly lower levels of attention
lobe, corpus callosum, and hippocampus (Andersen and executive functioning, although domains such as lan-
et al., 2008; De Bellis et al., 1999, 2002; Teicher et al., guage were not significantly lower in these maltreated
2004). In addition, neuroimaging studies have shown children. De Bellis and colleagues (2009) compared the
decreased neural connectivity in frontal, central, tem- neuropsychological functioning of neglected children
poral, and parietal areas of the brain associated with with related PTSD, neglected children without PTSD,
childhood trauma in adult brains, as well as diminished and healthy children. Results indicated that both groups
left-hemisphere differentiation and left-sided electroen- of neglected children displayed significantly lower levels
cephalography abnormalities in children and adolescent of intelligence, attention/executive functioning, language,
abuse victims (Cook, Ciorciari, Varker, & Devilly, 2009; and memory. Lower levels of attention/executive func-
Teicher et al., 1997). Further, it appears that certain areas tioning, language, and memory remained a statistically
of the brain may be particularly vulnerable to the neuro- significant impairment when controlling for intelligence.
biological consequences of childhood maltreatment, Only one individual score in visual memory differentiated
notably those areas with protracted postnatal develop- the neglected groups from one another, suggesting that
ment well into adolescence (e.g., prefrontal cortex, corpus neglected children may possess similar neuropsychologi-
callosum, cerebellum, and hippocampus; De Bellis et al., cal profiles, regardless of PTSD diagnosis.
2011; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, 2011; Wilson et al., 2011). The Research has suggested that childhood maltreatment
dysregulation of the neurobiological system and subse- may contribute to the presence of severe psychopathol-
quent neuroanatomical changes can result in direct neural- ogy (e.g., anxiety, depression, personality differences,
functioning dysregulation, contributing to the development impulsivity, violence, suicidality; Cloitre et al., 2009;
of various psychiatric, neurodevelopmental, and neuro- Kearney et al., 2010; Kendall-Tackett, 2000; Wilson
cognitive impairments (Andersen et al., 2008; De Bellis et al., 2011). The psychiatric adolescent inpatient popula-
et al., 2011; Hedges & Woon, 2011; Heim et al., 2010; tion is one group with high rates of childhood maltreat-
Kearney et al., 2010; Pechtel & Pizzagalli, 2011; Teicher ment (ranging from 31% to 61% depending on type of
et al., 2003; Wilson et al., 2011). maltreatment), with maltreatment suggested as a signifi-
It has been suggested that certain higher-order cogni- cant risk factor for psychiatric hospitalization during
tive functions are at increased risk for maltreatment- childhood (Boxer & Terranova, 2008; Fehon, Grilo, &
related dysfunction due to extreme stress exposure during Lipschitz, 2001). Generally, maltreatment is associated
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF MALTREATMENT    11

with significant psychopathological symptoms and received a discharge diagnosis by the attending psychia-
impaired functioning in the child and adolescent inpa- trist (or psychiatry resident under direct supervision of
tient setting and contributes to long-term psychological the psychiatrist) based on the Diagnostic and Statistical
struggles beyond that of acute emotional distress (Boxer Manual of Mental Disorders-Fourth Edition, Text Revision
& Terranova, 2008; Grilo, Sanislow, Fehon, Martino, & (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). A clinical
McGlashan, 1999; Sullivan, Fehon, Andres-Hyman, child neuropsychologist, a professional psychometrist, or
Lipschitz, & Grilo, 2006). a doctoral student in clinical psychology under direct
Increasingly, research is examining the neurocognitive supervision of a child neuropsychologist conducted the
and psychological presentation of those young victims neuropsychological/psychological evaluations. Reasons for
of childhood maltreatment. However, few studies have evaluation were based on parent/guardian concerns and/
simultaneously examined the neurocognitive and psy- or a request for additional information by the attending
chological components of maltreatment, which has lim- psychiatrists. Evaluations were typically completed within
ited the comprehensive profile we can identify in this a few days of hospital admission.
population. Further, fewer studies have examined the The present study examined the neuropsychological
neurocognitive and psychological components of child- profile (i.e., executive functions, language, anxiety, and
hood maltreatment within the adolescent psychiatric depression) of childhood maltreatment within an ado-
inpatient setting. To contribute to the growing neuropsy- lescent inpatient sample. Given the nature of the hospital
chological literature on childhood maltreatment, the in relation to the other medical school-affiliated hospitals,
Downloaded by [Wayne State University] at 08:25 09 April 2015

present study examined the neurocognitive (e.g., execu- those patients treated at the current hospital primarily
tive functions and language) and psychological (e.g., present with a psychiatric presentation in the absence of
anxious and depressive symptoms) presentation of a comorbid medical or neurological conditions. While rep-
group of adolescent psychiatric inpatient victims of resenting a minority component of the overall popula-
childhood maltreatment. It was hypothesized that com- tion served, those patients with a comorbid medical or
pared with those adolescent inpatients without a history neurological condition were not included in the present
of childhood maltreatment, maltreatment victims would study (e.g., traumatic brain injury, seizure disorders,
display lower scores on measures of executive function- cerebral palsy, spina bifida). Inpatients diagnosed with
ing and language, while reporting elevated symptoms of bipolar disorder, a pervasive developmental disorder
anxiety and depression. (e.g., autistic disorder, Asperger syndrome, Rett syn-
drome, childhood disintegrative disorder, and pervasive
developmental disorder-not otherwise specified), or a
psychotic disorder were also excluded from the current
METHOD
sample. However, those patients with a diagnosis of
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were
Participants and Procedure
included in the current sample. As noted in Table 1, there
The present study received institutional review board were no significant differences between groups regarding
(IRB) approval from Butler Hospital/Alpert Medical the presence of ADHD. One notable weakness of the
School of Brown University and followed the ethical prin- current study is that intellectual and academic assess-
ciples set forth by the American Psychological Association. ment was not available to researchers due to limited use
It is part of a research project at Butler Hospital, an inpa- of these instruments in the neuropsychological inpatient
tient psychiatric hospital. Specifically, the research project battery at the hospital. Although intellectual functioning
is examining the neuropsychological correlates of psychi- was not typically assessed formally in these evaluations,
atric conditions based on retrospective chart review of those individuals who received the described evaluation
adolescents admitted to the hospital who received a neu- were deemed intellectually competent and capable of
ropsychological/psychological evaluation during the years testing, as assessed by the absence of a history of intel-
from 2002 to 2012. Of note, there was one change in the lectual disability or borderline intelligence. This limita-
version of the tests administered in the neuropsychologi- tion will be discussed elsewhere, but it is important to
cal inpatient battery during these noted years (such as note that information regarding the presence of learning
transitioning from the first edition to the second edition and intellectual disabilities was not available. A total of
of a given instrument). As noted in later sections, a small 122 adolescent inpatients were included in the present
portion of participants completed the first edition of the study, with 49 participants included in the maltreatment
Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS). group and 73 participants included in the no maltreat-
Due to similar clinical subscale categorization, individuals ment group based on chart review. Demographics and
who completed the RCMAS-First Edition (RCMAS-1) comorbidities were examined either using chi-squared or
and RCMAS-Second Edition (RCMAS-2) were included analyses of variance (ANOVAs). Results are provided in
in this sample. Each individual admitted to the hospital Table 1.
12    KAVANAUGH, HOLLER, & SELKE

TABLE 1
Clinical and Demographic Characteristics for Maltreatment Groups

Total Sample History of Maltreatment No History of Maltreatment


Variable (N = 122) (n = 49) (n = 73) Pearson X2 F

% Male 60% 49% 34% 2.65


Age 15.31 (1.31) 15.48 (1.25) 15.19 (1.34) 1.45
Beery VMI Total 40.09 (9.47) 37.00 (9.68) 41.99 (8.89) 7.83*
% With Comorbidity 77% 84% 73% 2.03
# of Diagnoses 2.01 (0.76) 2.14 (0.68) 1.92 (0.80) 2.64
Diagnoses
Mood NOS 45% 49% 42% 0.50
Depression 41% 39% 42% 0.17
Anxiety 24% 29% 21% 1.04
PTSD 10% 25% 0% 19.83**
Behavioral 35% 29% 40% 1.60
ADHD 46% 45% 47% 0.03

Beery VMI Total = Beery Visual-Motor Integration Test (Fourth Edition, representing an intellectual proxy); % With Comorbidity = percentage
of participants who had more than one psychiatric diagnosis; Diagnoses = psychiatric diagnoses at hospital discharge; Anxiety = anxiety disorders;
Downloaded by [Wayne State University] at 08:25 09 April 2015

PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder; Behavioral = disruptive behavioral disorders; ADHD = attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.


*p < .01. **p < .001.

Materials The RCMAS-2 is a self-report measure of symptoms


of childhood anxiety, specifically physiological anxiety,
History of Maltreatment
worry, and social anxiety (Reynolds & Richmond, 2008).
Information regarding the history of maltreatment It also contains scales that assess defensiveness and
was gathered directly from the patient’s chart. At the time inconsistent reporting. The RCMAS-2 contains 49 items
of hospital admission, detailed information was obtained and can be used for children and adolescents aged 6 to 19
by primary clinicians regarding the recent behaviors years old. Of note, some of the older participants com-
exhibited by patients, recent stressors to patient and pleted the RCMAS-1. Because the scoring criteria
family, and the child’s developmental history. Specific remained consistent in the transition from RCMAS-1 to
examination of potential maltreatment history was stan- RCMAS-2, along with same clinical subscales, it was
dard in the admission/interview protocol with parents. deemed appropriate to include these cases.
Any history of maltreatment was recorded by clinicians,
including maltreatment occurring immediately prior to Task of Intellectual Estimation
hospitalization and maltreatment occurring years prior,
such as in early childhood. Due to the nature of the clini- No information on participant intelligence (IQ) was
cal hospital admission process, specific maltreatment available. The Beery Visual Motor Integration Test-
history including duration and time since maltreatment Fourth Edition (Beery VMI-4) was used as an intellectual
were not consistently included in patient medical charts. proxy to provide information on overall functioning. The
However, the specific types of maltreatment were obtained Beery VMI is a task of visual-motor integration, where
by researchers. Maltreatment history was defined by the the participant copies increasingly complex figures as
experience of physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional/ correctly as possible (Beery, 1997). The Beery VMI has
verbal abuse, or neglect. Any history of these maltreat- been highly correlated with intelligence in children and
ment experiences resulted in a positive identification of adolescents (r = .62 with Full-Scale IQ) and has been cat-
maltreatment history. egorized as possessing predictive intellectual utility
(Campbell, Brown, Cavanagh, Vess, & Segall, 2008). The
Self-Reported Anxiety/Depressive Symptoms Beery VMI was used in the present study as a proxy for
intelligence.
The Childhood Depression Inventory (CDI) is a self-
report measure of depressive symptoms, specifically in
Tasks of Executive Functioning
the domains of negative mood, interpersonal problems,
ineffectiveness, anhedonia, and negative self-esteem. The Based on a review of pediatric neuropsychological litera-
CDI contains 27 items and can be used for children and ture (Anderson, 2002; Baron, 2004; Henry & Bettenay, 2010;
adolescents aged 7 to 17 years old (Kovacs, 1992). The Willcutt, 2010), five executive-functioning subdomains were
total score and subdomain scores on the CDI were used constructed: (a) planning/problem solving, (b) set shifting/
for the present study. cognitive flexibility, (c) response inhibition/interference
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF MALTREATMENT    13

control, (d) fluency, and (e) working memory/simple challenge the executive functions of the client. The Stroop
attention. C-W score was the only Stroop score used for this study,
and it was used to assess response inhibition/interference
Planning/problem solving.  The Wisconsin Card-Sorting control. In addition, the WCST FMS score was used to
Test (WCST) is a test of executive function that assesses assess response inhibition/interference control (Strauss
skills in abstraction, shifting and maintaining focus, goal et al., 2006).
orientation, and impulse control (Baron, 2004; Henry &
Bettenay, 2010; Strauss, Sherman, & Strauss, 2006; Willcutt, Fluency.  The Controlled Oral Word Association Test
2010). This study used the following WCST scores: amount (COWAT) is a verbal task that requires the individual to
of categories achieved (WCST Categories), failures to produce words based on clinician-delivered characteristics
maintain set (WCST FMS), and perseverative errors and is typically viewed as a task assessing executive
(WCST Perseverative Errors). The WCST Categories functioning, specifically verbal fluency (Baron, 2004;
score was used as a measure of planning/problem solving Henry & Bettenay, 2010; Strauss et al., 2006). Two
(Baron, 2004; Strauss et al., 2006). The Rey Osterreith conditions were used in this administration, phonemic and
Complex Figure (ROCF) is a neuropsychological task semantic. The phonemic condition, FAS, asks the client to
designed to assess visual-spatial, perceptual, planning, produce words starting with the letters F, A, and S for 1
integration, and organizational abilities (Strauss et al., min per letter. The semantic condition, Animals, asks the
2006). The ROCF requires the participant to copy a client to say the names of various animals for 1 min.
Downloaded by [Wayne State University] at 08:25 09 April 2015

picture of a complex geometric design as accurately as Although the COWAT has been used as a language
possible. The ROCF was used in the present study to measure, for the present study, it was used to assess verbal
assess planning. fluency, identified as a subdomain of executive functioning.

Set shifting/cognitive flexibility.  The Trail-Making Test Working memory/simple attention.  The Wide Range
(TMT) is a neuropsychological instrument that assesses Assessment of Memory and Learning (WRAML) is a set
attention, speed, and cognitive flexibility (Baron, 2004; of tasks that assesses the learning and memory abilities
Henry & Bettenay, 2010; Strauss et al., 2006). It consists of children and adolescents (Sheslow & Adams, 1990).
of two versions that are administered consecutively to One subtest, Sentence Repetition, involves the clinician
the client. TMT Part A (TMT-A) requires the client to verbally presenting increasingly difficult sentences to the
use a pencil and connect encircled numbers in numerical client and asking the client to repeat the sentence. This
order. TMT Part B (TMT-B) requires the client to use a subtest, similar to other verbally presented digit span and
pencil and connect encircled numbers and letters in sentence repetition tasks, is believed to assess working
numerical and alphabetical order, alternating between memory and simple attention/concentration (Burton,
numbers and letters until completed. The adult form is Donders, & Mittenberg, 1996). Therefore, WRAML
for ages 15 to 89 years old and the child form is for ages Sentence Repetition was used in this study to assess
9 to 14 years old. The child form is very similar to the working memory. TMT-A was also used in the present
adult form, although it contains fewer numbers and study to assess simple attention (Baron, 2004; Strauss
letters. TMT-B was used in the present study to assess et al., 2006).
set shifting/cognitive flexibility (Strauss et al., 2006). In
addition, the WCST Perseverative Errors score was
Tasks of Language
used to assess set shifting/cognitive flexibility (Baron,
2004). T scores were used for all language measures.
The Boston Naming Test is a test of visual naming
Response inhibition/interference control.  The Stroop that requires the individual to produce the word descrip-
Test is a verbal task of executive function that assesses tions of a series of black-and-white drawings (Strauss
processing speed, attention, cognitive flexibility, resistance et al., 2006). The picture drawings range from frequently
to distraction, and response inhibition (Baron, 2004; seen words to words that are rarely seen. The total score
Henry & Bettenay, 2010; Strauss et al., 2006; Willcutt, on the Boston Naming Test was used in the present
2010). It consists of three conditions, which require the study and was used to indicate express language. The
client to read increasingly difficult patterns of words and Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition (PPVT-
colors. The third condition, called Color-Word (Stroop III) is a task of receptive vocabulary (Strauss et al.,
C-W), presents a word list of colors, with each color 2006). The PPVT-III is used for ages 2 to 90+ years and
name printed in the ink of another color. For example, has been recommended as an intellectual screening and/
the word “RED” would be printed in blue ink, with the or academic achievement tool. For the present study, the
participant required to say the color of the ink, not the PPVT-III was used as a language measure, specifically
written word. The increasingly difficult conditions receptive language.
14    KAVANAUGH, HOLLER, & SELKE

RESULTS Analysis of Variance Between Groups When


Controlling for VMI (IQ Proxy)
Maltreatment History and Executive
ANCOVAs were conducted to examine the differ-
Functioning/Language/Psychopathology
ences between maltreatment groups on executive func-
Composite scores were calculated for each executive- tioning, language, anxiety, and depression measures
functioning/language subdomain based on the mean while identifying Beery VMI (IQ proxy) as a covariate.
performance of administered executive-functioning and The ANCOVA identified a statistically significant dif-
language measures: cognitive flexibility/set shifting (TMT-B ference between maltreatment groups on the executive
and WCST Perseverative Errors), interference control/ function composite, F(1, 112) = 10.24, p = .002, cogni-
response inhibition (Stroop C-W and WCST FMS), plan- tive flexibility/set shifting, F(1, 112) = 10.98, p = .001,
ning/problem solving (ROCF and WCST Categories), problem solving/planning, F(1, 108) = 11.16, p = .001,
working memory/simple attention (WRAML Sentence CDI Total, F(1, 108) = 7.48, p = .007, and CDI Mood,
Repetition and TMT-A), verbal fluency (COWAT FAS F(1, 107) = 7.12, p = .009. No differences were observed
and COWAT Animals), and language (Boston Naming in the following domains: working memory/simple atten-
Test and PPVT-III). tion, response inhibition/interference control, fluency,
As noted previously, information regarding IQ was language, and the RCMAS. The results are provided in
not available. To provide information on level of func- Table 2.
Downloaded by [Wayne State University] at 08:25 09 April 2015

tioning at the time of evaluation, the Beery VMI was


used as an intelligence proxy. Analysis of Variance Between Groups When
Based on original ANOVA and chi-squared analysis Controlling for PTSD
on diagnostic and demographic variables between
groups, PTSD diagnosis, X2 = 19.83, and Beery VMI (IQ ANCOVAs were conducted to examine the differences
proxy), F(1, 111) = 7.83, p = .006, were identified as pos- between maltreatment groups on executive functioning,
sessing significant differences between groups. Initial language, anxiety, and depression measures while identi-
analyses of group differences on clinical measures were fying PTSD diagnosis as a covariate. The ANCOVA
conducted without controlling for these variables identified no statistically significant difference between
(ANOVA). Because Beery VMI served as an IQ proxy in maltreatment groups on any executive-functioning, lan-
the current sample, analyses were then conducted while guage, anxiety, or depression measures/domains. The results
controlling for Beery VMI (analysis of covariance are provided in Table 2.
[ANCOVA]). Due to the relevance of PTSD within child-
hood maltreatment, analyses were also conducted while Correlation Analysis
controlling for PTSD diagnosis (ANCOVA). To protect
Pearson and Point biserial correlations between the
statistical integrity during multiple comparisons, statisti-
neuropsychological measures and the type of maltreat-
cal significance was set at p < .01 for all ANOVAs and
ment experienced are shown in Table 3. Results indicate
correlations.
that a history of physical abuse was negatively correlated
with the executive-functioning composite, cognitive flex-
Analysis of Variance Between Groups Without
ibility/set shifting, and problem solving/planning, and
Controlling for PTSD/VMI
was positively correlated with the RCMAS. A history of
ANOVAs were conducted to examine the differences sexual abuse was negatively correlated with the executive-
between maltreatment groups on executive functioning, functioning composite, cognitive flexibility/set shifting,
language, anxiety, and depression measures. The ANOVA problem solving/planning, and language (primarily the
identified a statistically significant difference between PPVT). A history of emotional abuse was negatively cor-
maltreatment groups on the executive function composite, related with working memory/simple attention and posi-
F(1, 119) = 16.31, p = .000, cognitive flexibility/set shifting, tively correlated with the CDI. A history of neglect was
F(1, 119) = 15.26, p = .000, problem solving/planning, F(1, not correlated with any neuropsychological measures.
114) = 12.7, p = .001, CDI Mood, F(1, 113) = 7.08, p = .009, PTSD diagnosis was negatively correlated with the execu-
CDI Anhedonia, F(1, 113) = 7.66, p = .007, RCMAS Total, tive-functioning composite, cognitive flexibility/set shift-
F(1, 107) = 7.28, p = .008, and RCMAS Physiological Anxiety, ing, working memory/simple attention, problem solving/
F(1, 105) = 7.61, p = .007. No differences were observed in planning, and language (primarily Boston). In addition,
the following domains: working memory/simple atten- age was negatively correlated with problem solving/plan-
tion, response inhibition/interference control, fluency, ning, while sex (male = 1; female = 2) was negatively cor-
language, CDI Total, or RCMAS/CDI subdomains. The related with both self-report psychological measures
results are provided in Table 2. (CDI and RCMAS).
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF MALTREATMENT    15

TABLE 2
Analyses of Variance on Maltreatment and Executive-Functioning/Language Domains

Not Controlling for Controlling for VMI Controlling for


PTSD/VMI (IQ Proxy) PTSD
Maltreatment No Maltreatment
Domain Composites (n = 49) M (SD) (n = 73) M (SD) F Sig. F Sig. F Sig.

EF Composite 40.69 (8.96) 46.19 (6.04) 16.31 .000** 10.24 .002* 5.59 .020
CF/SS 39.96 (14.18) 48.82 (10.74) 15.26 .000** 10.98 .001* 6.02 .016
WM/SA 40.00 (11.53) 44.64 (10.21) 5.4 .022 2.25 .137 1.57 .213
RI/IC 49.05 (8.87) 46.80 (8.40) 1.78 .185 2.57 .112 2.64 .107
PS/P 36.68 (14.51) 44.85 (10.14) 12.70 .001* 11.16 .001* 2.54 .114
Fluency 42.67 (10.28) 46.87 (9.34) 4.95 .028 1.05 .308 1.61 .207
Language 37.43 (15.33) 44.25 (13.01) 6.44 .013 2.22 .139 1.90 .171
Boston 34.60 (18.17) 41.27 (17.14) 3.42 .067 0.458 .501 0.286 .594
PPVT-III 40.39 (13.72) 45.96 (12.08) 4.34 .040 2.17 .145 2.64 .108
CDI Total 62.17 (14.85) 55.07 (13.90) 6.94 .010 7.48 .007* 3.47 .065
CDI Mood 60.29 (14.46) 53.03 (14.17) 7.08 .009* 7.12 .009* 3.03 .084
CDI Inter. 61.04 (15.56) 55.91 (13.32) 3.56 .062 4.42 .038 2.76 .100
CDI Ineffective 60.42 (12.06) 56.49 (13.67) 2.49 .118 3.44 .066 2.80 .097
CDI Anhed. 57.96 (12.23) 52.07 (10.37) 7.66 .007* 6.75 .011 3.73 .056
Downloaded by [Wayne State University] at 08:25 09 April 2015

CDI S-E 56.36 (14.96) 52.09 (12.55) 2.72 .102 3.49 .065 1.09 .299
RCMAS Total 58.00 (11.99) 52.03 (10.81) 7.28 .008* 4.47 .037 3.67 .058
RCMAS Phys. 57.41 (11.17) 51.50 (10.54) 7.61 .007* 4.28 .041 3.35 .070
RCMAS Worry 54.20 (10.44) 48.86 (11.07) 6.12 .015 4.28 .041 4.19 .043
RCMAS Social 55.73 (10.55) 51.27 (9.94) 4.86 .030 3.43 .067 2.34 .129

EF Composite = executive-functioning composite score; CF/SS = cognitive flexibility/set shifting; WM/SA = working memory/simple attention; RI/
IC = response inhibition/interference control; PS/P = problem solving/planning; Language = language composite score; Boston = Boston Naming Test
(expressive language); PPVT-III = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition (receptive language); CDI Total = Childhood Depression Inventory
Total score; CDI Mood = CDI Negative Mood; CDI Inter. = CDI Interpersonal Problems; CDI Ineffective = CDI Ineffectiveness; CDI Anhed. = CDI
Anhedonia; CDI S-E = CDI Self-Esteem; RCMAS Total = Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale Total score; RCMAS Phys. = RCMAS
Physiological Anxiety; RCMAS Social = RCMAS Social Anxiety.
*p < .01. **p < .001.

DISCUSSION dysfunction in childhood maltreatment (Beers & De


Bellis, 2002; De Bellis et al., 2009; DePrince et al., 2009;
This study examined the neuropsychological presentation Fishbein et al., 2009; Hedges & Woon, 2011; Nolin &
of adolescent inpatients with a history of childhood Ethier, 2007; Spann et al., 2012). Additionally, specific
maltreatment. Results from the study indicated that subdomains of executive functioning appear to be partic-
compared with adolescent inpatients without maltreat- ularly implicated in childhood maltreatment, including
ment history, adolescents with a history of childhood cognitive flexibility/set shifting, problem solving/plan-
maltreatment scored significantly lower on measures of ning, and working memory/simple attention. Alternatively,
executive functioning, as well as significantly higher on maltreated adolescents did not differ from nonmaltreated
self-reported measures of anxiety and depression. The adolescents in the executive subdomains of response inhi-
majority of these findings remained significant when bition/interference control and fluency. These differing
controlling for the influence of intelligence (Beery VMI results highlight the importance of viewing “executive
served as IQ proxy). However, no significant group dif- functioning” as a heterogeneous domain (Baron, 2010),
ferences remained after controlling for the influence of with certain subdomains potentially more sensitive to the
PTSD diagnosis, suggesting that PTSD diagnosis may neurobiological effects of childhood maltreatment than
be a large factor in the neuropsychological presenta- other subdomains. In addition, general language perfor-
tion of childhood maltreatment. In addition, specific mance was not significantly lower in the maltreated
types of maltreatment were associated with unique adolescents, suggesting that language may be relatively
neuropsychological profiles. more resilient toward childhood maltreatment. The
The findings of this study identified the lower perfor- lack of observed language impairments in maltreated
mance on measures of executive functioning in a group of ­adolescent inpatients is in contrast with previous studies,
adolescent inpatients with a history of maltreatment. The which have shown childhood maltreatment to be associ-
current findings suggest that childhood maltreatment is ated with language impairments (De Bellis et al., 2009).
associated with executive dysfunction in adolescent However, these findings are consistent with the work of
inpatients, consistent with previous studies on executive Beers and De Bellis (2002), who found executive deficits,
16    KAVANAUGH, HOLLER, & SELKE

TABLE 3
Correlations Between Psychological/Executive Measures and Type of Abuse Experienced

Sex Age Physical Sexual Emotional Neglect PTSD

Sex — –.018 (p = .84) –.076 (p = .40) –.177 (p = .05) –.025 (p = .79) .130 (p = .15) –.122 (p = .179)
Age –.018 (p = .84) — .116 (p = .20) .123 (p = .18) –.011 (p = .91) .032 (p = .73) .063 (p = .489)
EF –.047 (p = .61) –.148 (p = .11) –.238* (p = .009) –.284* (p = .002) –.179 (p = .049) –.146 (p = .109) –.418* (p = .000)
CF/SS –.053 (p = .56) –.157 (p = .086) –.244* (p = .007) –.250* (p = .006) –.112 (p = .22) –.146 (p = .109) –.367* (p = .000)
WM/SA –.105 (p = .25) –.049 (p = .59) –.153 (p = .094) –.172 (p = .059) –.269* (p = .003) –.021 (p = .815) –.263* (p = .004)
RI/IC –.068 (p = .476) .170 (p = .075) .118 (p = .217) .005 (p = .960) .020 (p = .834) .066 (p = .491) –.034 (p = .722)
PS/P –.023 (p = .084) –.239* (p = .010) –.253* (p = .006) –.292* (p = .001) .046 (p = .622) –.189 (p = .042) –.455* (p = .000)
Fluency .014 (p = .881) –.125 (p = .190) –.161 (p = .091) –.160 (p = .093) –.164 (p = .083) –.093 (p = .329) –.232 (p = .014)
Language .218 (p = .020) –.061 (p = .523) –.037 (p = .701) –.368* (p = .000) .035 (p = .712) –.167 (p = .076) –.298* (p = .001)
Boston .312* (p = .001) –.025 (p = .802) –.043 (p = .67) –.254 (p = .010) .012 (p = .905) –.165 (p = .099) –.329* (p = .001)
PPVT-III .059 (p = .568) –.06 (p = .564) –.07 (p = .498) –.287* (p = .005) .056 (p = .591) –.118 (p = .25) –.154 (p = .135)
CDI –.40* (p = .000) .016 (p = .86) .215 (p = .02) .119 (p = .20) .273* (p = .003) –.125 (p = .18) .215 (p = .02)
RCMAS –.285* (p = .003) .035 (p = .72) .261* (p = .006) .108 (p = .263) .180 (p = .061) –.099 (p = .308) .235 (p = .014)

EF = executive functioning; CF/SS = cognitive flexibility/set shifting; WM/SA = working memory/simple attention; RI/IC = response inhibition/
interference control; PS/P = problem solving/planning; Language = language composite score; Boston = Boston Naming Test (expressive language);
Downloaded by [Wayne State University] at 08:25 09 April 2015

PPVT-III = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition (receptive language); CDI = Childhood Depression Inventory; RCMAS = Revised
Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale.
*p < .01.

but no language deficits, in a group of maltreated negatively correlated with executive and language mea-
children. sures. The potential difference between maltreated ado-
The present study also found childhood maltreatment lescents with PTSD and those adolescents without PTSD
to be associated with elevated symptoms of self-reported is in contrast to the research of De Bellis and colleagues
anxiety and depression, notably in symptoms of negative (2009), who found only minimal differences (i.e., one task
mood and physiological anxiety. These findings are in line of visual memory) between those neglected children with
with previous research, which has identified internalizing, PTSD and those neglected children without PTSD.
externalizing, and general psychopathological symptoms The neuropsychological presentation of specific
to be associated with maltreatment in adolescent inpa- types of maltreatment was also examined. Results of
tients (Boxer & Terranova, 2008; Grilo et al., 1999; Sullivan this analysis revealed notable differences in neuropsy-
et al., 2006). Further, previous research has noted that the chological presentation between four types of childhood
adolescent inpatient population is a highly victimized maltreatment. For example, although physical abuse
group, with the experience of maltreatment potentially and sexual abuse were both negatively correlated with
serving as a risk factor for psychiatric hospitalization cognitive flexibility/set shifting and problem solving/
(Boxer & Terranova, 2008; Fehon et al., 2001; Sullivan planning, sexual abuse was additionally correlated with
et al., 2006). Research has suggested that the experience of language, while physical abuse was positively correlated
maltreatment may limit one’s ability to develop appropri- with self-reported anxiety symptoms. In addition, emo-
ate coping abilities, putting one at risk for heightened dis- tional abuse was negatively correlated with working
tress from posttrauma stressors (Kearney et al., 2010; memory/simple attention and positively correlated with
Kendall-Tackett, 2000). Therefore, these individuals may self-reported depressive symptoms. Alternatively, neglect
continue to experience impairments well after the mal- was not correlated with any neuropsychological mea-
treatment has ended, and this prolonged impairment may sures. Although a clear explanation for these differences
potentially contribute to a psychiatric hospitalization. Our cannot be determined, it is possible that direct threat to
research is consistent with past research, with 40% of our bodily harm, such as in sexual and physical abuse, may
sample having experienced some form of maltreatment. cause more dysregulated neurobiological response and
Although neuropsychological differences were noted subsequently more severe neurocognitive dysregulation
between the maltreatment and no-maltreatment groups, and physiological arousal (i.e., anxiety symptoms). Abuse
these differences did not remain after statistically con- that may threaten emotional but not bodily harm, such
trolling for PTSD. This suggests that although as a as emotional abuse, may affect psychological domains
group maltreated participants displayed a unique neu- more than neurocognitive domains, resulting in more
ropsychological presentation, the diagnosis of PTSD depressive symptoms than other forms of abuse. Further,
appeared to be associated with significant differences in neglect may negatively affect more global areas of devel-
neuropsychological functioning. This was supported in opment, such as intelligence and academic skills, rather
the correlational analyses, which showed PTSD to be than specific neurocognitive domains. Taken together, it
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF MALTREATMENT    17

appears that specific types of childhood maltreatment the objective of this study was to examine childhood mal-
may be associated with specific neurocognitive impair- treatment across diagnoses, not solely restricted to those
ments across domains of executive functioning, lan- individuals with PTSD.
guage, and psychopathology. Future research should In conclusion, results from the current study revealed
continue to examine the distinct profiles of abuse types that within an adolescent inpatient setting, childhood
to provide more clarity regarding these findings. maltreatment is associated with neurocognitive and psy-
Despite these important findings, there are several limi- chopathological impairments, specifically in executive
tations to the current study. The primary limitation to this function and self-reported anxious and depressive
study is the reliance on retrospective record review. symptoms. The presence of PTSD appeared to have a
Researchers did not have information regarding intelli- significant influence on the neuropsychological profile
gence, grade level, academic skills, family information, of the adolescents. Various types of maltreatment were
and other variables that may be important to consider in associated with differing neuropsychological profiles.
childhood maltreatment. Most notably, a core intelligence Specifically, sexual and physical abuse were associated
(IQ) test was predominantly not administered within the with decreased executive functions, while sexual abuse
inpatient setting during this time period. Clearly, not was uniquely associated with decreased language and
having objective data on patient IQ severely limits our physical abuse was associated with increased symptoms
findings, as we are unable to control for the influence of of anxiety. Further, emotional abuse was associated
IQ on other neurocognitive domains or place our findings with decreased executive function and increased symp-
Downloaded by [Wayne State University] at 08:25 09 April 2015

within the context of overall intelligence. We attempted to toms of depression, while neglect was not associated
mitigate the limitations of not having a measure of IQ by with any neurocognitive or psychological domains.
using the Beery VMI as an intelligence proxy. We under- These findings have important implications for the
stand this limitation to our study, although we do consider treatment of childhood maltreatment, as well as for
the information gathered to be an adequate neuropsycho- future research examining the underlying neuropsycho-
logical evaluation of childhood maltreatment. logical presentation of childhood maltreatment.
Further, information gathered on maltreatment history
was taken directly from the patient records and therefore ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
was dependent on the examination of maltreatment by
the assigned clinician. Future studies would benefit from We would like to thank George Tremblay at Antioch
more specific information on the type and severity of mal- University New England for his statistical guidance and
treatment to more closely examine the associations the entire neuropsychology team at Butler Hospital.
between neurocognitive and psychopathological impair-
ment and childhood maltreatment. Although impairments REFERENCES
were associated with maltreatment, the direction of the
association was unable to be determined and therefore Alisic, E., van der Schoot, T. A., van Ginkel, J. R., & Kleber, R. J.
findings cannot be considered a causal direction between (2008). Looking beyond posttraumatic stress disorder in children:
maltreatment and impairments. It is possible that those Posttraumatic stress reactions, posttraumatic growth, and quality of
participants with a history of maltreatment possessed life in a general population sample. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69,
1455–1461.
neurocognitive and psychopathological impairments prior Allen, R. E., & Oliver, J. M. (1982). The effects of child maltreatment on
to or independent of maltreatment. Future studies could language development. Child Abuse and Neglect, 6, 299–305.
benefit from longitudinal methodology, allowing for exami- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man-
nation of abilities prior to and following a high-stress ual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
series of events such as maltreatment. Andersen, S. L., Tomada, A., Vincow, E. S., Valente, E., Polcari, A., &
Teicher, M. H. (2008). Preliminary evidence for sensitive periods in the
Another limitation to this study is the limited data on effect of childhood sexual abuse on regional brain development. The
PTSD. Research has begun to examine the neurocogni- Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 20, 292–301.
tive and psychopathological differences between those Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive func-
children and adolescent victims of maltreatment who tion (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 71–82.
develop PTSD and those who do not develop PTSD Barkley, R. A. (2012). Executive functions: What they are, how they
work, and why they evolved. New York, NY: Guilford.
(Beers & De Bellis, 2002; De Bellis et al., 2009; De Bellis, Baron, I. S. (2004). Neuropsychological evaluation of the child. New
Hooper, Woolley, & Shenk, 2010; Sullivan et al., 2006). York, NY: Oxford University Press.
While identification of the neuropsychological profile of Baron, I. S. (2010). Maxims and a model for the practice of pediatric
childhood PTSD is critical, the present study examined neuropsychology. In K. O. Yeates, M. D. Ris, H. G. Taylor, & B. F.
childhood maltreatment in a broader context than PTSD. Pennington (Eds.), Pediatric neuropsychology: Research, theory, and
practice (2nd ed., pp. 473–498). New York, NY: Guilford.
Further, our sample of PTSD due to maltreatment was Beers, S. R., & De Bellis, M. D. (2002). Neuropsychological function in
too small (n = 12) to conduct additional analyses on the children with maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder. The
role of PTSD. Although we understand this limitation, American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 483–486.
18    KAVANAUGH, HOLLER, & SELKE

Beery, K. (1997). The Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual Kearney, C. A., Wechsler, A., Kaur, H., & Lemos-Miller, A. (2010). Post-
Motor Integration: VMI with supplemental tests of visual perception traumatic stress disorder in maltreated youth: A review of contemporary
and motor coordination: Administration, scoring and teaching manual. research and thought. Clinical Child and Family Psychological Review,
Parsippany, NJ: Modern Curriculum. 13, 46–76.
Boxer, P., & Terranova, A. M. (2008). Effects of multiple maltreatment Kendall-Tackett, K. A. (2000). Physiological correlates of childhood
experiences among psychiatrically hospitalized youth. Child Abuse & abuse: Chronic hyperarousal in PTSD, depression, and irritable
Neglect, 32, 637–647. bowel syndrome. Childhood Abuse & Neglect, 24, 799–810.
Burton, D. B., Donders, J., & Mittenberg, W. (1996). A structural Kirsch, V., Wilhelm, F. H., & Goldbeck, L. (2011). Psychophysiological
equation analysis of the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and characteristics of PTSD in children and adolescents: A review of the
Learning in the standardization sample. Child Neuropsychology, 2, literature. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 24, 146–154.
39–47. Kocovska, E., Puckering, C., Follan, M., Smillie, M., Gorski, C., Barnes,
Campbell, J. M., Brown, R. T., Cavanagh, S. E., Vess, S. F., & Segall, M. J. J., ... Minnis, H. (2012). Neurodevelopmental problems in maltreated
(2008). Evidence-based assessment of cognitive functioning in pediatric children referred with indiscriminate friendliness. Research in Devel-
psychology. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 33, 999–1014. doi: 10.1093/ opmental Disabilities, 33, 1560–1565.
jpepsy/jsm138 Kovacs, M. (1992). Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI) manual. New
Cloitre, M., Stolbach, B. C., Herman, J. L., van der Kolk, B., Pynoos, R., York, NY: Multi-Health Systems.
Wang, J., & Petkova, E. (2009). A developmental approach to com- Lupien, S. J., Fiocco, A., Wan, N., Maheu, F., Lord, C., Schramek, T.,
plex PTSD: Childhood and adult cumulative trauma as predictors of & Tu, M. T. (2005). Stress hormones and human memory function
symptom complexity. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 22, 399–408. across the lifespan. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 225–242.
Cook, F., Ciorciari, J., Varker, T., & Devilly, G. J. (2009). Changes in Luria, A. R. (1965). L. S. Vygotsky and the problem of localization of
long term neural connectivity following psychological trauma. Clinical functions. Neuropsychologia, 3, 387–392.
Downloaded by [Wayne State University] at 08:25 09 April 2015

Neurophysiology, 120, 309–314. Mahone, E. M., & Slomine, B. S. (2007). Managing dysexecutive disor-
De Bellis, M. D., Hooper, S. R., Spratt, E. G., & Woolley, D. P. (2009). ders. In S. J. Hunter & J. Donders (Eds.), Pediatric neuropsychologi-
Neuropsychological findings in childhood neglect and their relation- cal intervention (pp. 287–313). New York, NY: Cambridge University
ships to pediatric PTSD. Journal of the International Neuropsycho- Press.
logical Society, 15, 868–878. Mash, E. J., & Barkley, R. A. (Eds.). (2007). Assessment of childhood
De Bellis, M. D., Hooper, S. R., Woolley, D. P., & Shenk, C. E. (2010). disorders (4th ed.). New York, NY: Guilford.
Demographic, maltreatment, and neurobiological correlates of Nolin, P., & Ethier, L. (2007). Using neuropsychological profiles to clas-
PTSD symptoms in children and adolescents. Journal of Pediatric sify neglected children with or without physical abuse. Child Abuse &
Psychology, 35, 570–577. Neglect, 31, 631–643.
De Bellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Clark, D. B., Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. Pechtel, P., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2011). Effects of early life stress on cog-
N., Boring, A. M., ... Ryan, N. D. (1999). Developmental traumatol- nitive and affective function: An integrated review of human litera-
ogy. Part II: Brain development. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1271–1284. ture. Psychopharmacology, 214, 55–70.
De Bellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Shifflett, H., Iyengar, S., Beers, S. R., Reynolds, C. R., & Richmond, B. O. (2008). Revised Children’s Manifest
Hall, J., & Moritz, G. (2002). Brain structures in pediatric maltreat- Anxiety Scale, Second Edition (RCMAS-2): Manual. Los Angeles,
ment-related posttraumatic stress disorder: A sociodemographically CA: Western Psychological Services.
matched study. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 1066–1078. Samuelson, K. W., Krueger, C. E., Burnett, C., & Wilson, C. K. (2010).
De Bellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Spencer, S., & Hall, J. (2000). N-acet- Neuropsychological functioning in children with posttraumatic stress
ylaspartate concentration in the anterior cingulate of maltreated chil- disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 16, 119–133.
dren and adolescents with PTSD. American Journal of Psychiatry, Sheslow, D., & Adams, W. (1990). WRAML: Wide Range Assessment
157, 1175–1177. of Memory and Learning administration manual. Wilmington, DE:
De Bellis, M. D., Spratt, E. G., & Hooper, S. R. (2011). Neurodevelop- Jastak Assessment Systems.
mental biology associated with childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Spann, M. N., Mayes, L. C., Kalmar, J. H., Guiney, J., Womer, F. Y.,
Child Sexual Abuse, 20, 548–587. Pittman, B., ... Blumberg, H. P. (2012). Childhood abuse and neglect
DePrince, A. P., Weinzierl, K. M., & Combs, M. D. (2009). Executive and cognitive flexibility in adolescents. Child Neuropsychology, 12,
function performance and trauma exposure in a community sample 182–189.
of children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33, 353–361. Stacks, A. M., Beeghly, M., Partridge, T., & Dexter, C. (2011). Effects
Fehon, D. C., Grilo, C. M., & Lipschitz, D. S. (2001). Correlates of of placement type on the language developmental trajectories of mal-
community violence exposure in hospitalized adolescents. Compre- treated children from infancy to early childhood. Child Maltreatment,
hensive Psychiatry, 42, 283–290. 16, 287–299.
Fishbein, D., Warner, T., Krebs, C., Trevarthen, N., Flannery, B., & Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M., & Strauss, O. (2006). A compendium of
Hammond, J. (2009). Differential relationships between personal and neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms and commentary (3rd
community stressors and children’s neurocognitive functioning. Child ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Maltreatment, 14, 299–315. Sullivan, T. P., Fehon, D. C., Andres-Hyman, R. C., Lipschitz, D. S.,
Grilo, C. M., Sanislow, C., Fehon, D. C., Martino, S., & McGlashan, & Grilo, C. M. (2006). Differential relationships of childhood abuse
T. H. (1999). Psychological and behavioral functioning in adolescent and neglect subtypes to PTSD symptom clusters among adolescent
psychiatric inpatients who report histories of childhood abuse. The inpatients. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19, 229–239.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 538–543. Sylvestre, A., & Merette, C. (2010). Language delay in severely neglected
Hedges, D. W., & Woon, F. L. (2011). Early-life stress and cognitive children. A cumulative or specific effect of risk factors? Child Abuse
outcome. Psychopharmacology, 214, 121–130. & Neglect, 34, 414–428.
Heim, C., Shugart, M., Craighead, W. E., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2010). Taber, K. H., Salpekar, J., Wong, A. H., & Hurley, R. A. (2011).
Neurobiological and psychiatric consequences of child abuse and Developmental origins for neuropsychiatric illness. The Journal of
neglect. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 671–690. Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 23, 1–5.
Henry, L. A., & Bettenay, C. (2010). The assessment of executive func- Teicher, M. H., Andersen, S. L., Polcari, A., Anderson, C. M., Navalta,
tioning in children. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 15, 110–119. C. P., & Kim, D. M. (2003). The neurobiological consequences of early
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF MALTREATMENT    19
stress and childhood maltreatment. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Twardosz, S., & Lutzker, J. R. (2010). Child maltreatment and the devel-
Reviews, 27, 33–44. oping brain: A review of neuroscience perspectives. Aggression and
Teicher, M. H., Dumont, N. L., Ito, Y., Vaituzis, C., Giedd, J. N., & Violent Behavior, 15, 59–68.
Andersen, S. L. (2004). Childhood neglect is associated with reduced U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration on
corpus callosum area. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 80–85. Children, Youth, and Families. (2012). Childhood maltreatment 2010.
Teicher, M. H., Ito, Y., Glod, C. A., Andersen, S. L., Dumont, N., & Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Ackerman, E. (1997). Preliminary evidence for abnormal cortical Willcutt, E. G. (2010). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In K.
development in physically and sexually abused children using EEG O. Yeates, M. D. Ris, H. G. Taylor, & B. F. Pennington (Eds.), Pedi-
coherence and MRI. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, atric neuropsychology: Research, theory, and practice (2nd ed., pp.
821, 160–175. 393–417). New York, NY: Guilford.
Teicher, M. H., Tomada, A., & Andersen, S. L. (2006). Neurobiological Wilson, K. R., Hansen, D. J., & Li, M. (2011). The traumatic stress
consequences of early stress and childhood maltreatment: Are results response in child maltreatment and resultant neuropsychological
from human and animal studies comparable? Annals of the New York effects. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16, 87–97.
Academy of Sciences, 1071, 313–323.
Downloaded by [Wayne State University] at 08:25 09 April 2015

You might also like