You are on page 1of 16

Child Neuropsychology 0929-7049/00/0604-297$15.

00
2000, Vol. 6, No. 4, pp. 297-312 © Swets & Zeitlinger

Inhibitory Deficits in Reading Disability Depend on Subtype:


Guessers but not Spellers*
Menno van der Schoot, Robert Licht, Tako M. Horsley, and Joseph A. Sergeant
Department of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, The Netherlands

ABSTRACT

In this study, children with the guessing subtype of dyslexia (who read fast and inaccurately) were com-
pared with children with the spelling subtype (who read slowly and accurately) on three aspects of execu-
tive functioning (EF): response inhibition, susceptibility to interference from irrelevant information, and
planning. It was found that guessers were impaired in their ability to inhibit inappropriate responding on
all tasks used to assess EF (the stop signal task, the Stroop task, and the Tower of London task). This raises
the question of whether the specific reading disorder of guessers may be linked to the same executive
deficits which underlie ADHD. In order to unite a fast/inaccurate reading style with executive deficiencies,
an attempt is made to incorporate the concept of executive control into models of lexical activation.

It is generally accepted that dyslexia is not a other), and other word-mutilating errors. The
homogenous entity and that there are a number second type, referred to as spellers, reads slowly
of subtypes of reading-disabled children and fragmentedly, since the identification of
(Benton, 1978; Rutter, 1978; Satz & Morris, words is mainly based on an elaborate grapheme
1981). Although the concept of subtypes is to phoneme translation process. The speller’s
widely accepted, the manner in which subgroups reading style is accurate in that it leaves the ulti-
are identified varies. For example, each of the mate reading response intact.
studies listed by Hooper and Willis (1989, pp. At the word recognition level, the slow/accu-
42–44) used different measures of achievement rate-fast/inaccurate dichotomy has been associ-
and cognition as the basis for group separation. ated with indirect- versus direct-word approach
In spite of this, we currently argue that the sub- (Licht, 1989; Van Strien, Bouma, & Bakker,
types that have been distinguished by a number 1993). In the indirect or phonological route,
of dual-subtype models – e.g., Bakker’s L and P word identification is attained through genera-
type (1979, 1981); Van der Leij’s guessers and tion of a phonological representation, formed by
spellers type (1983); Lovett’s accuracy and rate the stepwise translation of graphemes into pho-
disabled readers (1984), and Mitterer’s whole- nemes. The direct or lexical route does not re-
word and recoding subtypes (1982) – show quire an intermediate phonological code, since
some overlap and, in view of their reading style, the use of specific orthographic codes enables
seem to converge as two types of dyslexic chil- direct access to word memory. Licht (1989),
dren. The first type, referred to as guessers, Licht and Van Onna (1995), Van der Leij
manifests a fast and global reading style. This is (1983), and Van Strien et al. (1993) argued that
characterized by errors such as omissions, addi- guessers and spellers may predominantly rely on
tions, substitutions, letter reversals, false word the direct and indirect word recognition strategy,
identifications (misreading one word as an- respectively.

*
Address correspondence to: Menno van der Schoot, Department of Clinical Neuropsychology, Vrije Universiteit
Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
Accepted for publication: May 7, 2001.
298 MENNO VAN DER SCHOOT ET AL.

In the present study, dyslexic children have of processing required for efficient word recog-
been classified as guessers and spellers on the nition. However, an alternative explanation has
basis of reading errors and reading speed ac- come from recent findings in the field of execu-
cording to criteria initially developed by Bakker tive functioning (EF), which is thought responsi-
(1981). The guesser-speller classification is ble for the control of cognition and the regula-
based on a so-called clinical-inferential model tion of behavior. There is a growing body of
for subtyping, and has been found to cover about evidence that specific patterns of executive defi-
65% of the variability in the reading of dyslexics cits exist in childhood psychopathological disor-
(Van Strien, Bakker, Bouma, & Koops, 1990; ders such as ADHD, PDD/NOS, and autism
Van Strien, Bouma, & Bakker, 1993). Although (Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996). Children with
empirical classification studies generally ex- ADHD appear to have inhibitory deficits that are
plain more variance in reading, they do so by revealed by ‘‘impulsive behaviors such as re-
extracting a larger number of subtypes. For ex- sponding before a task is understood, answering
ample, by using multiple methods of cluster before sufficient information is available, allow-
analysis, Morris et al. (1998) identified nine ing attention to be captured by irrelevant stimuli
subtypes that represented 90% of their sample of (i.e., distractibility), or failing to correct obvi-
232 children. For a discussion of the clinical- ously inappropriate responses’’ (Schachar & Lo-
inferential and empirical models, see Hooper gan, 1990).
and Willis (1989). However, the goal of the The hasty and inaccurate reading characteris-
present study is not to explain as much variance tics of guessers seem to overlap with some of
in reading as possible, but to further differenti- the criteria for ADHD, specifically impulsivity
ate fast/inaccurate and slow/accurate readers on and distractibility. This raises the question
a number of tasks that measure the efficiency whether guessers suffer from a mild form of
and speed of the executive process of inhibition. ADHD. It is possible that impulsive behaviors
Clearly, the distinction between guessers and go unnoticed in children who are primarily re-
spellers differs from the classical distinction ferred for poor reading performance. The pur-
between phonological and surface dyslexics pose of the present study is to discover whether
(e.g., Castles & Coltheart, 1993; Stanovich, Sie- the differences in reading style and word recog-
gel, & Gottardo, 1997). Whereas the former dis- nition strategy between guessers and spellers are
tinction refers to differences in reading style associated with differences in basic executive
(fast/direct vs. slow/indirect), the latter distinc- processes, in particular those concerning inhibi-
tion refers to differences in deficits underlying tory control.
word recognition problems (phonological vs.
visuo-orthographical deficits). Yet, spellers may Association of Reading and Attentional Dis-
be equated with surface dyslexics in that they orders
are presumed to have difficulties using visuo- Our assumption that impulsivity and distractibil-
orthographic cues for fast whole-word recogni- ity are associated with a guessing-like reading
tion (as a consequence of which they have to style is corroborated by a number of epidemio-
employ a spelling-like approach). Guessers, on logical studies. Estimates of coexisting RD in
the other hand, cannot be so easily equated with the ADHD population range between 9% (Hal-
phonological dyslexics. Although guessers show perin, Gittelman, Kline, & Ruddel, 1984) and
a number of reading characteristics that are sim- 39% (August & Garfinkel, 1990) to 80% (Mc-
ilar to the phonological dyslexia subtype, their Gee & Share, 1988), whereas the prevalence of
fast, hasty reading style is not easy to explain. ADHD in RD has been estimated to range from
26% to 41% (Holborow & Berry, 1986; Silver,
Reading and Executive Function 1981) to 50% (Lambert & Sandoval, 1980) (this
The previous section suggests that differences overview is based on Shaywitz et al., 1995).
between guessers and spellers may boil down to Given the frequent co-occurrence of both disor-
differences in the computational skills and speed ders, a number of hypotheses have been put for-
INHIBITORY DEFICITS IN READING DISABILITY 299

ward to explain the nature and etiology of this Only two studies have examined the extent to
comorbidity. which slow/accurate and fast/inaccurate sub-
One generally accepted view is that RD and types of dyslexia are differentially capable of
ADHD share some common underlying factors inhibiting irrelevant responses (De Sonneville,
(e.g., neurocognitive) which explain the co- Neijens, & Licht, 1993; Licht, 1989). It ap-
morbid symptoms. However, several investiga- peared that guessers have greater difficulty than
tors have argued that the co-occurrence of spellers in inhibiting an experimentally induced
ADHD behaviors in children with RD does not response bias when performing a sustained at-
reflect a ‘‘true’’ comorbid association (e.g., tention task (De Sonneville et al., 1993). In ad-
Pennington, Grossier, & Welsh, 1993), but may dition, guessers were more susceptible to inter-
be attributed to separate sources of cognitive ference in the Stroop Color-Word Test than
morbidity for ADHD and RD (August & spellers (Licht, 1989).
Garfinkel, 1990; see also Ackerman, Dykman, These findings and the hasty and impulsive
& Gardner, 1990; Felton, Wood, Brown, Camp- reading behaviors of guessers suggest that these
bell, & Harter, 1987; O’Neill & Douglas, 1991). children may have an inhibitory deficit in addi-
As a possible approach to disentangling the dis- tion to or underlying their reading problem. Al-
tinct versus shared sources of cognitive co- though guessers typically are not diagnosed as
morbidity, Cantwell and Baker (1991) suggested ADHD (their primary problems present as RD),
that studies need to take into account the possi- this does not exclude the possibility that their
bility that there are subtypes of ADHD and RD. reading disorder is associated, at least in part,
One of the goals of the present study was, there- with the same executive deficits found in
fore, to assess whether ADHD-like executive ADHD.
dysfunctions are associated more with the
guesser than speller subtype in RD. Research Objectives
The goal of the present investigation is to estab-
Dyslexia and Inhibition lish a distinct pattern of deficient EF skills that
As noted previously, the field of EF is a promis- would distinguish the guessing type of dyslexia
ing one for determining the underlying process from the spelling type. For this purpose, guess-
deficit(s) of guessers and spellers. In a study by ers were compared with spellers on the follow-
Kelly, Best, and Kirk (1989), it was indeed ing executive functions: response inhibition, as
found that reading-disabled children have exec- determined by the stop signal task (Logan &
utive difficulties in, for example, selective and Cowan, 1984); susceptibility to interference
sustaining attention, inhibition of routine re- from irrelevant information, as determined by
sponses and set maintenance over and above the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935); and planning, as
specific reading deficits. Other studies have determined by the Tower of London (TOL;
used the EF approach to address the question of Shallice, 1982). These measures of impulse con-
comorbid RD and ADHD. Willcutt and Pen- trol were chosen as ADHD children perform
nington (2000) found stronger associations be- more poorly than controls on these tasks. In ad-
tween RD and the ADHD inattention subtype dition, the Abbreviated Conners Teacher Rating
than between RD and the hyperactive-impulsive Scale (ACTRS) was used to assess ‘‘daily-life’’
subtype. Purvis and Tannock (2000) assessed symptoms of inattention, impulsivity, and hy-
inhibitory performance in RD and ADHD em- peractivity (Goyette, Conners, & Ulrich, 1978).
ploying a classical paradigm for inhibition: the It was hypothesized that only guessers would
stop signal task. They reported inhibitory defi- show a pattern of EF deficits similar to that re-
cits in both RD and ADHD. They concluded that ported for ADHD children and that spellers
inhibitory control does not differentiate RD would not.
from ADHD, but that phonological processing
does. However, neither study specified the RD
subtype.
300 MENNO VAN DER SCHOOT ET AL.

METHOD All of the control children (N = 20) came from


the normal primary school and their RAEs approx-
Subjects imated their ERAs.
Subjects were children of 9–12 years who were
recruited from two special schools for learning- Classification of Guessers and Spellers
disabled children and from one normal primary Subsequent to the TMT, the dyslexic children were
school. Learning-disabled children whose reading given a standardized Dutch sentence-reading test
disturbance could be attributed to emotional prob- (AVI; Van den Berg & Te Lintelo, 1977). This test
lems, sociocultural factors, or gross neurological consists of nine texts of increasing difficulty. The
deficits on the basis of school records were not number of texts actually mastered (i.e., read within
included in the sample. All children who partici- time and error limits) determines the child’s level
pated (N = 80 for reading disabled and N = 20 for of text reading.
controls) were healthy and had normal or The AVI was employed to classify the dyslexics
corrected-to-normal vision, and their IQ scores as spellers or guessers on the basis of reading
(obtained from school records) were in the normal speed, the number of substantive errors (SE; e.g.,
range (IQ > 85). None of the children was diag- omissions, additions, substitutions, letter reversals)
nosed as ADHD using DSM-IV criteria (American and the number of time-consuming errors (TE;
Psychiatric Association, 1994), nor did they partic- e.g., hesitations, stammerings, fragmentations, rep-
ipate (or had been participating) in ADHD treat- etitions, corrections). In order to evoke a sufficient
ment programs. number of errors on which to base the speller (rela-
tively many time-consuming errors) – guesser (re-
Assessment of Dyslexia latively many substantive errors) classification, a
To assess current reading levels, all children were text two levels above the child’s mastery level was
administered a standard Dutch word-reading test, presented and assessed on reading speed and read-
the Two-Minutes Test (TMT; Brus & Voeten, ing errors.
1973), which consists of lists of words that become Reading speed (RS) was expressed as the total
progressively more difficult. The TMT score (the reading time divided by the time norm for the text
number of words read correctly in two minutes) * 100, whereas reading error (RE) was expressed
was converted into a reading-age equivalent (RAE; as the proportion of TE errors relative to the total
Struiksma, Van der Leij, & Vieijra, 1989) reflect- number of errors (SE + TE). A child was classified
ing the child’s actual reading level expressed in as having the guessing type of dyslexia when RS <
the number of months of reading instruction (one 115 and RE < .40 (more than 60% of errors made
year of instruction being equivalent to 10 months). were substantive errors), and as having the spelling
The expected reading-age (ERA) is equivalent to type of dyslexia when RS > 135 and RE> .60
the number of months that a child has actually re- (more than 60% of errors made were time-consum-
ceived formal reading instruction. The Netherlands ing errors). The classification criteria were similar
employs a very systematic method of reading in- to those used by Van Strien (1999) and Patel and
struction, so the ERA-RAE difference enabled us Licht (in press) and were adapted from Bakker and
to assess any lag in reading performance almost Vinke (1985) and Van Strien, Bakker, Bouma, and
down to one month. Children who lagged 15 Koops (1990). Using this classification system, we
months or more in reading (ERA-RAE) were con- were able to classify about 60% of our dyslexics as
sidered to be dyslexic (N = 75; five learning-dis- either spellers or guessers (N = 45). The final
abled children did not fulfill this criterion and were groups of guessers (N = 20) and spellers (N = 20)
removed from the sample). Consequently, only were formed by selecting those children who
those children were admitted to the subsequent showed most clearly the characteristics of each
guesser-speller classification procedure. type.
It should be emphasized that the ERA-RAE pro-
cedure goes beyond a simplistic chronological age- Symptoms of ADHD in Dyslexics
grade level discrepancy formula in that the number In order to evaluate possible comorbid ADHD-like
of months of actual reading instruction, and not symptoms in our sample of dyslexics, teachers of
chronological age, is used to define reading lag. In the dyslexic children and controls rated the chil-
addition, the educational age-norms for average dren with the ACTRS (Goyette, Conners, &
reading level were obtained in extensive standard- Ulrich, 1978). A one-way analysis of variance per-
ization studies on reading in the Dutch population formed on the rating scores revealed a significant
of primary-school children. group effect: F(2,56) = 9.51, p < .001, eta = .254
(for one control child, a rating was not available).
INHIBITORY DEFICITS IN READING DISABILITY 301

As expected, guessers displayed higher scores than requires the subjects to respond to a visual stimu-
controls (p < .001) and spellers (p < .08). Group lus and to inhibit their response on the infrequent
characteristics are presented in Table 1. presentation of an auditory stop signal (Logan &
Cowan, 1984; Logan, Cowan, & Davis, 1984).
Tasks and Procedure Each trial began with the presentation of a
Four tasks were administered: (1) a Word Decod- square warning stimulus (1.40 cm * 1.40 cm) for a
ing Task; (2) the Stroop Color-Word task; (3) the duration of 500 ms. This was followed by the pri-
Tower of London task; and (4) a stop signal task. mary task stimulus, which was displayed for 125
Tasks 1, 2 and 3 were presented to guessers and ms. After the imperative signal was extinguished,
spellers only, since the primary interest here was the screen was blank for 2,375 ms. The stimuli for
how these tasks might differentiate between guess- the primary task were the uppercase letters X, A,
ers and spellers. Task 4 was also administered to O, and P. Each letter was 1.80 cm wide and 2.90
normal readers. cm high. Both the warning stimuli and the stimulus
letters were presented in black-on-white at the cen-
The Word Decoding Task (WDT) ter of the screen. The primary choice reaction time
The WDT (Van Aarle & Volleberg, 1986) required task was simple: a capital X or A required a re-
the child to read aloud a random list of 30 phono- sponse with one hand, a capital O or P required a
logical regular words, 30 irregular words (IW), and response with the other. Mapping of letters onto
30 pseudo words (PW). Each word was centrally response hands was counterbalanced across sub-
presented on a computer screen (black-on-white) jects.
for 5 s. The WDT was included to test the working The stop signal was a 1,000 Hz tone, with an
hypothesis that spellers have difficulty using intensity of 65 dB(A) and a duration of 350 ms. It
visuo-orthographical cues for fast whole-word rec- was presented binaurally on 25% of the trials, oc-
ognition, and that guessers have problems with curring an equal number of times at each of six
phonological processing. Spellers are expected to stop signal delays as with an X, A, O, and P. The
make more errors on irregular words (that call sequence of primary task stimuli, stop signals, and
upon the direct route for identification), and guess- stop signal delays was pseudo-randomized.
ers are expected to make more errors on pseudo A practice block was presented first, followed
words (that call upon the indirect route for identifi- by nine test blocks of 48 trials yielding 18 stop
cation). As the number of errors in reading regular signals in each stop signal delay. The test blocks
words is not a critical factor with regard to the were arranged in groups of three, in between which
present hypothesis, we decided to restrict the anal- the subjects took a short break.
yses to pseudo words and irregular words. Stop signal delays were set relative to the
child’s mean primary task reaction time (MRT):
The Stroop Color-Word task (STROOP) MRT-500, MRT-400, MRT-300, MRT-200, MRT-
The STROOP, adapted for use with Dutch-speak- 100, and MRT-0 ms. To correct for differences
ing children by Hammes (1978), was administered. between subjects in MRT and strategy (e.g., a sub-
ject may delay his/her response in an attempt to
The Tower of London task (TOL) enhance the probability of inhibiting), stop signal
In the TOL, subjects had to move a pattern of delays in block n were set relative to the MRT in
beads from a start configuration to a goal configu- block n-1 (block-to-block tracking). In the first
ration as efficiently as possible. The task requires block, stop signal delays were set relative to the
the forward planning of sequences of actions in MRT in the practice block.
order to solve a particular problem. The level of Subjects were instructed to respond as quickly
problem difficulty progressed through the test by and as accurately as possible to the primary task
increasing the minimum number of moves required stimuli and to withhold their response whenever a
for a solution. In total, the subject was asked to stop signal occurred. It was explained that stop
solve 12 problems. Here, TOL performance is re- signal delays were variable and that stop signals
flected only by the minimum number of moves could be presented so late that it would be difficult
necessary to solve all problems (n = 46) minus the to suppress the primary response. Finally, subjects
total number of additional (i.e., incorrect) moves. were explicitly instructed not to delay their re-
The higher the score, the higher the problem-solv- sponses to the primary task in order to improve
ing capability. stopping.
For each child, the following primary task mea-
The stop signal task (STOP) sures were derived from the go-trials: mean reac-
The stop task is a choice reaction time task that tion time (MRT), standard deviation (SD), percent-
302
Table 1. Characteristics for Each Reading Group.

Boys Girls Age Reading Agea Reading Error Typec Conners Rating
Speedb (on (on AVI) Scale
Expected Actual Difference AVI)
Spellers 13 7 10.5 (1.0) 41.7 (11.4) 21.1 (10.1) 20.6 (5.0) 153.9 (29.8) .67 (.14) 16.5 (6.8)
Guessers 14 6 10.6 (1.0) 40.4 (10.2) 18.2 1(6.6) 22.2 (6.9) 184.7 (21.4) .30 (.12) 10.8 (8.0)
Controls 12 8 10.9 (0.4) 40.0 1(5.6) 42.1 1(7.1) –2.1 (9.3) – – 12.1 (2.1)

Note. Numbers in parentheses are standard deviations.


a
Reading age is in months; 10 months equals 1 year of reading instruction.
(The Actual Reading Age is derived from the Two-Minutes-Test (TMT) (Brus & Voeten, 1973)).
b
Reading speed is expressed as 100 * (time needed / time norm).

MENNO VAN DER SCHOOT ET AL.


c
Error type is expressed as N(time-consuming errors) / N(substantive + time-consuming errors).
INHIBITORY DEFICITS IN READING DISABILITY 303

age of errors (pressing with the X/A-finger when word category was substantially larger for spell-
an O or P was presented or vice versa) and percent- ers (average increase of 4.7 errors) than for
age of omissions (non-responses). guessers (average increase of 0.8 errors), as was
Inhibition functions were generated by comput-
evident by the interaction between reading
ing the proportion of stop signal trials, at each stop
signal delay, on which subjects successfully inhib- group and word category (F(1,38) = 4.33, p <
ited their primary response. Effects of subject .05, eta = .102). An Independent-Samples t test
group (guessers, spellers, and normal readers) on revealed that spellers tended to make more er-
the probability of inhibition were examined in re- rors in reading irregular words than guessers
peated measures analyses of variance (ANOVA) (t(38) = 1.78, p < .08).
with groups a as between-subject factor and delay
as a within subjects factor. Follow-up tests were
carried out when necessary. To analyze the differ-
STROOP
ences in the shape of the inhibition function in a Figure 2 (left panel) shows that guessers needed
more accurate way, ANOVAs were performed on 74 additional seconds to complete the ‘‘color
the slopes of the regression lines that were fitted to naming of words’’ condition relative to the
the inhibition functions when plotted as a function ‘‘color naming of blocks’’ condition. Spellers
of a Z-score that represents the Relative Finishing required 66 extra seconds. The between-group
Time (ZRFT) of the stop and go processes in stan- difference was not significant (t(34) = .76; the
dard deviation units of the primary task RTs (see
Logan, Cowan, & Davis, 1984). When inhibition Stroop test could not be obtained from two
functions from different groups cannot be aligned guessers and two spellers).
by plotting them against ZRFT, it may be con- The right panel of Figure 2 displays the inter-
cluded that a lower and flatter function represents ference effect on the number of errors for both
a ‘‘deficiency’’ in the executive process of inhibi- reading groups. An analysis of variance revealed
tion. a significant interaction between group and con-
To explore more specific deficits in the stop-
dition (F(1,34) = 4.62, p < .05). The interference
ping process, mean stop signal reaction times
(SSRTs) were estimated for each individual, taking effect was more pronounced for guessers (aver-
into account the probability of response at each age increase of 5.9 errors) than for spellers (av-
stop signal delay and the distribution of primary erage increase of 3.2 errors) (t(34) = 2.2, p <
task reaction times (for the estimation procedure,
see Logan, Cowan, & Davis, 1984). Analyses of
14
variance and subsequent post-hoc tests (to locate
between-group differences) were conducted to ex- Spellers
amine the effects of reading group on both SSRT 13 Guessers
Number of Reading Errors

and ZRFT slope.


12
Apparatus
Stimuli were presented with a 386SX-25 PC, with
timing control from a master computer, a 486DX2- 11
66 PC. The master computer recorded the manual
responses. The stimuli were presented on a NEC 10
Multisync 5FG monitor positioned at 70.00 cm
from the subject’s eyes. 9

8
RESULTS
7
WDT
Pseudo Irregular
As shown in Figure 1, more errors were made in
Word Category
reading irregular words than in reading pseudo
words (F(1,38) = 8.24, p < .01, eta = .178, Fig. 1. The Interaction Effects Between Word Cate-
pooled across reading group). The effect of gory (Pseudo vs. Irregular) and Reading
Group.
304 MENNO VAN DER SCHOOT ET AL.

.05). Apparently, guessers were more suscepti- = 5.12, p < .01, eta = .152). Subsequent post-hoc
ble to interference due to the automatic genera- tests (Tukey’s HSD) revealed that the MRT was
tion of written word meaning when naming the significantly slower in the dyslexia subgroups
color. compared to the control group (p < .005 and p <
.05 for spellers and guessers, respectively). In
TOL addition, spellers (p < .001) and guessers (p <
A between-subject t test showed that the mean .001) showed a greater amount of primary task
TOL score of guessers (28.4) was significantly variability than normal readers. Finally, dys-
lower than that of spellers (33.6) (t(38) = 2.64, p lexics made the most hand errors, as was evident
< .05), indicating that fast/inaccurate readers in a significant guesser-control (p < .05) and a
made more incorrect moves than slow/accurate marginally significant speller-control (p < .06)
readers. difference. The rate of omission errors did not
differ among groups.
The Stop Signal Task Figure 3 displays the probabilities of inhibi-
One-way analyses of variance were conducted tion as a function of MRT delay and as a func-
separately for MRT to go-trials in the primary tion of Z-relative finishing times (ZRFT).
task, the standard deviation (SD) of MRT, the An analysis of variance with one between-
percentage of errors, and the percentage of subject factor (i.e., Group; three levels) and one
omissions of the primary task. Means and stan- repeated factor across Delay (six levels) was
dard deviations of each of these dependent mea- conducted for the probability of inhibition
sures in each subject group (controls, spellers, (P(Inhibit)). The effect of Group on the mean
and guessers) are provided in Table 2. P(Inhibit) (over all delays) was marginally sig-
Significant group effects were obtained for nificant (F(2,57) = 2.62, p < .08, eta = .084),
MRT (F(2,57) = 6.69, p < .005, eta = .190), signifying slight differences in the height of the
standard deviation of MRT (F(2,57) = 10.40, p < inhibition function. As predicted by the race
.001, eta = .267), and percentage errors (F(2,57) model, P(Inhibit) was strongly affected by De-

8
160
Spellers Spellers
7
Guessers Guessers

145
6
Naming Time (sec)

Number of Errors

130 5

4
115
3

100
2

85 1
Blocks Words Blocks Words
Color Naming of Blocks / Color Naming of Words Color Naming of Blocks / Color Naming of Words

Fig. 2. The Stroop Interference Effect on Naming Time (Left Panel) and Number of Errors (Right Panel) for
Each Reading Group.
INHIBITORY DEFICITS IN READING DISABILITY 305

Table 2. Performance on the Stop Signal Paradigm as Reflected by the Means and Standard Deviations for the
Dependent Measures for Each Reading Group.

Measure Reading Group

Spellers Guessers Controls

M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)


MRT (go-task) 795.70 (121.21) 758.62 (109.52) 676.73 (81.00)
SD of MRT 287.87 (80.71) 290.22 (94.62) 190.82 (55.63)
% of errors 6.92 (5.55) 8.00 (5.65) 3.09 (3.92)
% of omissions 3.78 (3.15) 3.43 (2.47) 2.36 (2.25)
slope (ZRFT) 26.95 (14.52) 19.29 (9.52) 16.42 (8.42)
SSRT 225.07 (88.21) 301.40 (138.65) 256.93 (78.19)

Note. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; MRT = Mean Reaction Time; SSRT = Stop Signal Reaction Time;
ZRFT = Z-score Relative Finishing Time; all times are in ms.

lay (F(5,285) = 111.23, p < .001, eta = .661). Subsequent post-hoc comparisons (simple
The groups were affected similarly; that is, the effects; Winer, 1971) indicated that the inhibi-
interaction between Group and Delay did not tion functions for guessers were significantly
reach conventional levels of significance lower (p < .05) than those observed for spellers.
(F(10,285) = 1.56, p = .12, eta = .052). In addition, they tended to be flatter (p < .08).

0,7 0,7
Controls Controls
Spellers Spellers
Guessers Guessers
0,6 0,6

0,5 0,5
P(Inhibit)
P(Inhibit)

0,4 0,4

0,3 0,3

0,2 0,2
-500 -400 -300 -200 -100 0 -1 0 1 2 3
MRT - Stop Signal Delay Z-score (Relative Finishing Time)

Fig. 3. The Probability of Inhibition as a Function of MRT Delay (Left Panel) and ZRFT (Right Panel) for
Each Reading Group.
Note. MRT = Mean Reaction Time; ZRFT = Z Score Relative Finishing Time.
306 MENNO VAN DER SCHOOT ET AL.

To examine the linear component of the Group * variance in the number of reading errors made
Delay interaction more precisely, a one-way within two minutes. Spellers, on the other hand,
analysis of variance was carried out on the slope neither exhibited a significant relationship be-
of the ZRFT regression lines. This analysis re- tween teacher ratings of impulsivity and reading
vealed a significant group effect (F(2,57) = 4.78, speed (F(1,18) = 1.01, R Square = 5%) nor a
p < .05, eta = .143), suggesting that variation in significant relationship between teacher ratings
stop signal delay differentially affected the rise of impulsivity and reading accuracy (F(1,18) =
of the inhibition functions. Post-hoc tests 1.16, R Square = 6%).
(Tukey’s HSD) revealed that the ZRFT slopes of
spellers were significantly steeper than those of
guessers (p < .05) and controls (p < .05). The DISCUSSION
observed differences in the efficiency of the ex-
ecutive inhibition process were confirmed by a In this study, dyslexic children classified as
one-way analysis of variance conducted for the guessers were compared with children who
estimated stop signal reaction times (SSRTs), showed a spelling type of dyslexia on three as-
demonstrating a significant group effect pects of executive functioning: response inhibi-
(F(2,57) = 3.43, p < .05, eta = .108). Post-hoc tion, susceptibility to interference from irrele-
tests demonstrated that the stopping process in vant information, and planning. Using our clas-
guessers was significantly slower than in spell- sification system, guessers and spellers were
ers (p < .05). Mean values and standard devia- found to cover about 60% of the variability
tions of SSRT and ZRFT slope are presented for among the dyslexics. Obviously, more variabil-
each group in Table 2. ity would have been explained if more subtypes
were included, or if an empirical model for
Teacher Ratings of Impulsivity and Reading subtyping was employed (see Morris et al.,
Performance 1998). It should be emphasized, however, that
Finally, we examined the relationship between the goal of the present study was not to explain
teacher ratings of impulsivity and reading per- as much variance in reading as possible but to
formance on the TMT. The TMT was firstly em- differentiate between guessers and spellers on a
ployed to assess reading lag, and required the number of EF tasks. The discussion of the re-
children to read lists of words for two minutes. sults focuses on the guesser-speller distinction
Although guessers and spellers read about the when it concerns executive deficits and associa-
same number of words correctly, both reading tion with ADHD symptoms. In order to unite a
groups differed in the way this composite score fast/inaccurate reading style with executive defi-
was constructed. As would be expected, guess- ciencies, an attempt is made to incorporate the
ers read faster but more inaccurately than spell- concept of executive control into models of
ers. That is, they read more words in total (10, word recognition and lexical activation.
on average) but made more errors (12, on aver- Clearly, the predicted guesser-speller differ-
age). Interestingly, a regression analysis re- ences were evident from the performance ob-
vealed that, in guessers, there was a significant served in the stop task. In comparison with
relationship between teacher ratings of spellers, guessers were found to have a slower
impulsivity and reading speed (F(1,18) = 8.05, p inhibitory process (SSRT) and a lower and flat-
< .05), with ratings of impulsivity accounting ter inhibition function. Since block-to-block
for 31% of the variance in the total number of tracking allowed for the assessment of inhibitory
words read within two minutes. Additionally, control independently of primary response
guessers demonstrated a significant relationship speed, and since the inhibition function slopes
between teacher ratings of impulsivity and read- were obtained after the application of the ZRFT
ing accuracy (F(1,18) = 9.54, p < .01), with rat- normalization procedure, the differences in inhi-
ings of impulsivity accounting for 35% of the bition functions cannot be explained by group
INHIBITORY DEFICITS IN READING DISABILITY 307

differences in MRT, SSRT, and SD(MRT). These and the ability to control/inhibit reading when
results show that guessers are impaired in their necessary (Rafal & Henik, 1994).
ability to inhibit inappropriate responding. In the present experiment, the impaired abil-
The specific mechanisms underlying the in- ity of guessers to control reading was not only
hibitory deficits in guessers are as yet unclear. manifest in the Stroop Color-Word test, but also
Logan and Cowan (1984) suggest that a high in the AVI text-reading test and TMT word-
variability in the speed of the stop process and a reading test, in which they read too fast and in-
low triggering probability, in addition to re- accurately. In the trade-off between speed and
duced speed of stopping, contribute to inhibitory accuracy, guessers seem to give priority to the
deficits. All of these mechanisms result in a former dimension of reading performance and
lower and flatter inhibition function. neglect the latter. Such an explanation of the
poor reading performance of guessers is no lon-
Do Guessers Suffer from a General Executive ger primarily linked to an underlying disorder of
Deficit? language (Shaywitz, Fletcher, & Shaywitz,
Although the above explanation of inhibitory 1994) but rather refers to more general deficits
deficits in guessers focuses specifically on stop- in information processing style or strategy.
ping mechanisms, it is possible that poor re- It is interesting to note that a fast but inaccu-
sponse inhibition may also be part of a more rate response strategy has also been established
general impairment in executive functions, in ADHD children using tests that measure im-
which in turn may be attributable to a frontal pulse control, such as MFFT (Barkley, 1991;
lobe dysfunction. Such explanations for re- DuPaul, Anastopoulos, Shelton, Guevremont, &
sponse inhibition deficits have been based on Metevia, 1992; Milich & Kramer, 1984; Ser-
studies that aimed at uncovering the mecha- geant, Van Velthoven, & Virginia, 1979). The
nisms that underlie the impulsive behaviors of overlap between response styles of guessers and
ADHD children (Barkley, 1994, 1997; Barkley, ADHD children raises the question whether the
Grodzinsky, & DuPaul, 1992; Grodzinsky & guessing type of dyslexia and disorders of atten-
Diamond, 1992; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; tion and activity may share a common pathway.
Shue & Douglas, 1992). It appeared that these Since the present study lacks a group of chil-
children, but also children with other develop- dren with ADHD as well as a comorbid
mental psychopathologies, displayed abnormali- RD+ADHD group, the issue of (the nature of)
ties in response inhibition on a variety of execu- the comorbidity of RD and ADHD cannot be
tive tasks (i.e., tasks that substantially call upon addressed directly. Nonetheless, the poor perfor-
functions mediated by the (pre)frontal lobes). mance of guessers on the EF tasks suggests that
Evidence in support of the hypothesis that at least a parallel may be drawn between guess-
guessers may have similar executive deficits is ers and ADHD children, as the latter group is
provided by the Stroop task and the Tower of believed to suffer from the same type of deficits
London task. in executive functioning (see Barkley et al.,
On the TOL, guessers made more incorrect 1992; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Pennington
(i.e., impulsive) moves than spellers. Appar- et al., 1993). Yet, ADHD children and guessers
ently, the type of anticipatory planning capabili- seem to differ in the manner of expressing these
ties required in this kind of problem-solving task deficits. In ADHD children, executive deficits
are not fully developed in guessers, or they are result in a broad range of inattentive, hyperac-
disrupted by impulsive responses. In addition, tive, and impulsive behaviors, whereas, in
guessers were less able to inhibit the interfer- guessers, the symptoms of poor impulse control
ence of an irrelevant word (e.g., blue) on naming seem particularly to be manifested in their read-
the color of the ink (e.g., red) in the Stroop task, ing style. It is probable that their symptoms are
as evidenced by their larger number of errors. not sufficiently present to meet the DSM-IV cri-
The Stroop effect is considered to be a good ex- teria for ADHD, since none of the participating
ample of automatic, uncontrolled word reading guessers was classified as ADHD.
308 MENNO VAN DER SCHOOT ET AL.

However, when the classroom teachers were addressed is whether these executive deficits
asked to rate the children on the ACTRS may also underlie the guessers’ impulsive read-
(Goyette, Conners, & Ulrich, 1978), guessers ing behaviors or whether a language-based dis-
were found to display higher scores than spellers order has to be assumed. Since the EF tasks did
and normal readers. This finding suggests that not tap critical elements of reading, no direct
there is a link between impulsive responding in (i.e., causal) relationship between executive dys-
the classroom and impulsive responding in a functions and specific reading disturbances can
reading task. Secondly, it demonstrates that be deduced from the present experiment. How-
guessers behave like ADHD children not only ever, the finding that guessers performed more
on the laboratory-based measures of impulse poorly than spellers on the stop task, the Stroop
control but they also resemble them in a more task, and the TOL task, as well as the finding
natural classroom setting. that guessers displayed higher rating scores on
Further support for our notion that guessers the ACTRS, suggests that there is at least some
share executive deficits with ADHD comes from degree of association between them. More direct
distinct patterns of correlations between teacher support for this line of reasoning comes from the
ratings on the Conners scale and inhibitory effi- observation that there is a close relationship be-
cacy on the Stroop task (errors) and on the stop tween teacher ratings of impulsivity and speed
task (ZRFT slope) found for guessers. Within and accuracy of reading only in the guessers.
the group of guessers, the rating scores corre- This result suggests that guessers have a deficit
lated highly with Stroop task performance (r = in EF that is apparent both cognitively and
.54, p < .05) and moderately with stop task per- behaviorally. It remains to be seen whether the
formance (r = –.33, .05 < p < .10). Furthermore, above relationship can be replicated in a group
Stroop interference correlated moderately with of children whose symptoms of impulsivity, hy-
ZRFT slope (r = –.34, .05 < p < .10). In spellers, peractivity, and inattention pass the DSM-IV
the correlations were found to be low (and, in threshold for ADHD.
the case of the ZRFT slope, in the opposite di- In the introduction of this paper, it was cau-
rection) (r = .19, r = .24, and r = –.02, respec- tiously suggested that guessers may have prob-
tively). Apparently, there is a link between lems with phonological processing similar to
teacher ratings of impulsivity, Stroop interfer- those observed in the phonological dyslexia sub-
ence, and ZRFT slope only in guessers. The ob- type described by Castles & Coltheart (1993)
served pattern of association may be explained and Stanovich et al. (1997). However, the results
by postulating a common underlying deficit in of the WDT conflict with this notion, since
executive functions that equally affects the dif- guessers did not make more errors on pseudo
ferent measures of impulse control. words than on irregular words. Clearly, this re-
In sum, the conclusion drawn by Purvis & inforces our argument that we need to focus on
Tannock (2000) that inhibitory control does not an inhibitory explanation for the guessers’ read-
differentiate RD and ADHD may be premature, ing disturbance. Below, it is speculated how the
in that its validity would seem to depend on RD concept of executive control may be incorpo-
subtype. The present data suggest that this con- rated into models of lexical activation (Morton,
clusion may only apply to guessers but not spell- 1979; Morton & Patterson, 1980; Treisman,
ers. 1960).
According to activation models, orthographic
Is There a Link Between the Guessers’ Exec- information about a target word accumulates
utive Deficits and Their Reading Distur- gradually in the visual system, and, as it accu-
bance? mulates, intermediary candidate words are con-
The present study shows that guessers are im- currently primed or activated in the lexicon. The
paired in their ability to inhibit inappropriate basic mechanism of word recognition is then to
responding and that this disability might reflect raise the activation level of one of the candidate
EF deficits. The crucial question that needs to be words (i.e., the target word) above some critical
INHIBITORY DEFICITS IN READING DISABILITY 309

threshold value. We argue that this theoretical word-reading test and the WDT. In the AVI and
framework may account (at least to some extent) TMT, their reading was slow, elaborate, and
for the guessing type of dyslexia, if one assumes marked by many time-consuming errors. In the
that guessers have lower word thresholds than WDT, they displayed the expected problems
normal readers. Another possibility is that with irregular words that are mispronounced
guessers may have more difficulty with ‘‘damp- when merely deciphered phonologically. What
ening’’ the activation of candidates that are type of deficit may underlie these difficulties
likely to be false. Both assumptions predict that, with reading? As previously argued, one expla-
in guessers, false candidate words have an in- nation is that spellers have difficulties using
creased chance of being prematurely identified visuo-orthographic cues for fast whole-word
as the target word. In a reading (aloud) task, this recognition, as a consequence of which they
would be evidenced by an impulsive style of have to fall back on the phonological route of
reading that is characterized by a high preva- word identification. In this context, our spellers
lence of substantive errors. As noted earlier, may be equated with the surface dyslexics as
several classification studies have discovered a identified by Castles & Coltheart (1993) and
(guessing) subtype of dyslexia that displays such Stanovich et al. (1997).
a distinct profile of reading performance Here, spellers proved to be highly capable of
(Bakker, 1979, 1981; Lovett, 1984; Mitterer, inhibiting inappropriate responding on the stop
1982; Van der Leij, 1983). task, the Stroop task, and the TOL task. On the
It should be stressed that the above explana- stop task, they even displayed steeper ZRFT
tion of the guessers’ impulsive reading style is slopes than controls. Although this finding is
highly speculative and that, evidently, more re- intriguing, it is not easy to interpret. One possi-
search is needed to empirically establish ble explanation is that, due to the relatively
whether and, if so, in what way a lack of execu- small sample sizes (n = 20), there may have
tive control affects the processes involved in been insufficient power to align the inhibition
word recognition. In word comprehension, how- functions of spellers and controls. Another ex-
ever, the role of a general cognitive mechanism planation is that the observed superior inhibition
of suppression has been investigated by of spellers is an artifact of our selection proce-
Gernsbacher and Faust (1991). According to dure. Spellers were selected on the basis of slow
Gernsbacher’s (1990) structure-building frame- but accurate reading on the AVI text-reading
work, a presented word activates a number of test. It is possible that the slow/accurate-fast/
potential meanings. The role of the suppression inaccurate dimension of reading performance
mechanism is then to dampen the activation of overlaps with some inhibitory dimension that
the less likely meanings (so that the appropriate corresponds with high and poor inhibitory skills
meaning can be more easily accessed). Interest- in a stop task situation, respectively. The main
ingly, Gernsbacher and Faust concluded that a difference between guessers and spellers con-
less efficient suppression mechanism underlies cerns the nature of the association between both
deficient general comprehension skills. This types of dimensions. In guessers, the hypothe-
reinforces our idea that a suppression mecha- sized executive deficits probably lead to both
nism may (also) play a role in word recognition. poor behavioral and cognitive inhibition, where-
as spellers show superior behavioral inhibition,
The Deficit(s) Underlying the Reading Prob- which may be a burden to them when speeding
lems in Spellers up reading.
The reading of spellers is characterized by the Interestingly, Quay (1988a, 1988b), Ooster-
stepwise conversion of the words’ graphemic laan and Sergeant (1998), Pliszka, Borcherding,
segments into phonemes. These phonemes are Spratley, Leon and Irick (1997), and Oosterlaan
then blended to form a word. This style of read- (2000) found that children with anxiety disor-
ing was manifest in the spellers’ performances ders show more efficient response inhibition
on the AVI sentence-reading test, the TMT than controls. These authors suggested an over-
310 MENNO VAN DER SCHOOT ET AL.

active inhibition system underlying the anxiety Barkley, R.A. (1994). Impaired delayed responding:
problems in these children. A similar mecha- A unified theory of attention deficit disorder. In
D.K. Routh (Ed.), Disruptive behaviour disorders
nism may also underlie the superior inhibitory
in childhood (pp. 11–57). New York: Plenum
performance of spellers on the stop task. Press.
The suggestion that spellers may have an Barkley, R.A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained
overactive inhibition system raises the question attention, and executive functions: Constructing a
whether the slow and fragmented style of read- unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin,
121, 65–94.
ing of spellers can also be ascribed to this sys- Barkley, R.A., Grodzinsky, G., & DuPaul, G. (1992).
tem. If we follow the same line of reasoning Frontal lobe functions in attention deficit disorder
used to explain the fast/inaccurate reading style with and without hyperactivity: A review and re-
of guessers, we have to assume that an overac- search report. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
tive inhibition system slows down the mecha- ogy, 20, 163–188.
Benton, A.L. (1978). Some conclusions about dys-
nism of word recognition. In terms of lexical lexia. In A.L. Benton & D. Pearl (Eds.), Dyslexia:
activation, it may do so because spellers have An appraisal of current knowledge (pp. 451–476).
critical word thresholds that are too high, as a New York/London: Oxford University Press.
result of which they waste too much time raising Brus, B.T., & Voeten, M.J.M. (1973). Een-minuuttest,
the activation level of a target word above vorm A en B, verantwoording en handleiding.
Nijmegen: Berkhout.
threshold and ensure recognition. Or, it can be Cantwell, D.P., & Baker, L. (1991). Association be-
speculated that spellers have problems with tween attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder and
‘‘enhancing’’ the activation of the most likely learning disorders. Journal of Learning Disabili-
candidate words. Future research will be needed ties, 24, 88–95.
to examine these explanations. Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of de-
velopmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149–180.
De Sonneville, L.M.J., Neijens, L., & Licht, R.
(1993). Leer- en aandachtstoornissen: vroegtijdige
onderkenning en samenhang. In E.J.M. Van Aarle
REFERENCES
& K. Hennemans (Eds.), Dyslexie 1992, verslag
van het IDA congres. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Ackerman, P.T., Dykman, R.A., & Gardner, M.Y. DuPaul, G.J., Anastopoulos, A.D., Shelton, T.L.,
(1990). ADD students with and without dyslexia Guevremont, D.C., & Metevia, L. (1992). Multi-
differ in sensitivity to rhyme and alliteration. Jour- method assessment of attention deficit hyperactiv-
nal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 279–283. ity disorder: The diagnostic utility of clinic-based
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic tests. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21,
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th 394–402.
ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Asso- Felton, R.H., Wood, F.B., Brown, I.S., Campbell,
ciation. S.K., & Harter, M.R. (1987). Separate verbal mem-
August, G.J., & Garfinkel, B.D. (1990). Comorbidity ory and naming deficits in attention deficit disorder
of ADHD and reading disability among clinic-re- and reading disability. Brain and Language, 31,
ferred children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- 171–184.
chology, 18, 29–45. Gernsbacher, M.A. (1990). Language comprehension
Bakker, D.J. (1979). Hemispheric differences and as structure building. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
reading strategies: Two dyslexias? Bulletin of the Gernsbacher, M.A., & Faust, M.E. (1991). The mech-
Orton Society, 29, 84–100. anism of suppression: A component of general
Bakker, D.J. (1981). A set of brains for learning to comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psy-
read. In K.C. Diller (Ed.), Individual differences chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17,
and universals in language learning aptitude (pp. 245–262.
56–71). Rowley: Newbury House. Goyette, C.H., Conners, C.K., & Ulrich, R.F. (1978).
Bakker, D.J., & Vinke, J. (1985). Effects of hemi- Normative data on revised Conners parent and
sphere-specific stimulation on brain activity and teacher rating scales. Journal of Abnormal Child
reading in dyslexics. Journal of Clinical and Ex- Psychology, 6, 221–236.
perimental Neuropsychology, 7, 505–525. Grodzinsky, G.M., & Diamond, R. (1992). Frontal
Barkley, R.A. (1991). The ecological validity of labo- lobe functioning in boys with attention-deficit hy-
ratory and analogue assessment methods of ADHD peractivity disorder. Developmental Neuropsychol-
symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, ogy, 8, 427–445.
19, 149–178.
INHIBITORY DEFICITS IN READING DISABILITY 311

Halperin, J.M., Gittelman, R., Klein, D.F., & Ruddel, L., Francis, D.J., & Shaywitz, B.A. (1998). Sub-
R.G. (1984). Reading disabled hyperactive chil- types of reading disability: Variability around a
dren: A distinct subgroup of attention deficit disor- phonological core. Journal of Educational Psy-
der with hyperactivity? Journal of Abnormal Child chology, 90, 347–373.
Psychology, 12, 1–14. Morton, J. (1979). Word recognition. In J. Morton &
Hammes, J. (1978). Stroop-Kleur-Woord-Test. Lisse: J.C. Marshall (Eds.), Psycholinguistics (Series 2,
Swets & Zeitlinger. pp. 109–156). London: Elek.
Holborow, P., & Berry, P. (1986). A multinational, Morton, J., & Patterson, K. (1980). A new attempt at
cross-cultural perspective on hyperactivity. Ameri- an interpretation, or, an attempt at a new interpre-
can Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 320–322. tation. In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, & J.C. Mar-
Hooper, S.R., & Willis, W.G. (1989). Learning dis- shall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 91–118). London:
ability subtyping. New York: Springer. Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Kelly, M.S., Best, C.T., & Kirk, U. (1989). Cognitive O’Neill, M.E., & Douglas, V.I. (1991). Study strate-
processing deficits in reading disabilities: A pre- gies and story recall in attention deficit disorder
frontal cortical hypothesis. Brain and Language, and reading disability. Journal of Abnormal Child
11, 275–293. Psychology, 19, 671–692.
Lambert, N.M., & Sandoval, J. (1980). The preva- Oosterlaan, J. (2000). Behavioural inhibition and the
lence of learning disabilities in a sample of chil- development of childhood anxiety disorders. In
dren considered hyperactive. Journal of Abnormal W.K. Silverman & Ph.D.A. Treffers (Eds.), Anxi-
Child Psychology, 8, 33–50. ety disorders in children and adolescents (pp.
Licht, R. (1989). Reading disability subtypes: Cogni- 45–71). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
tive and electrophysiological differences. In D.J. Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J.A. (1998). Response in-
Bakker & G. Van der Vlugt (Eds.), Learning dis- hibition and response re-engagement in attention-
abilities: Neuropsychological correlates and treat- deficit/hyperactivity disorder, disruptive, anxious
ment. Amsterdam/Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger. and normal children. Behavioural Brain Research,
Licht, R., & Van Onna, J. (1995). Differences in com- 94, 33–43.
ponents of word recognition between P- and L-type Patel, T.K., & Licht, R. (2000). Verbal and affective
reading disability. In C.K. Leong & R.M. Joshi laterality effects in P-dyslexic, L-dyslexic and nor-
(Eds.), Developmental and acquired dyslexia (pp. mal children. Child Neuropsychology, 6, 157-174.
41–50). Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publish- Pennington, B.F., Grossier, D., & Welsh, M.C.
ers. (1993). Contrasting cognitive deficits in attention
Logan, G.D., & Cowan, W.B. (1984). On the ability deficit hyperactivity disorder versus reading dis-
to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of ability. Developmental Psychology, 29, 511–523.
control. Psychological Review, 91, 295–327. Pennington, B.F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive
Logan, G.D., Cowan, W.B., & Davis, K.A. (1984). On functions and developmental psychopathology.
the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37,
time responses: A model and a method. Journal of 51–87.
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Pliszka, S.R., Borcherding, S.H., Spratley, K., Leon,
Performance, 10, 276–291. S., & Irick, S. (1997). Measuring inhibitory control
Lovett, M.W. (1984). A developmental perspective on in children. Developmental and Behavioral Pediat-
reading dysfunction: Accuracy and rate criteria in rics, 18, 254–259.
the subtyping of dyslexic children. Brain and Lan- Purvis, K.L., & Tannock, R. (2000). Phonological
guage, 22, 67–91. processing, not inhibitory control, differentiates
McGee, G., & Share, D.L. (1988). Attention deficit ADHD and reading disability. Journal of the Amer-
disorder-hyperactivity and academic failure: ican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Which comes first and what should be treated? 39, 485–494.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Quay, H.C. (1988a). Attention deficit disorder and the
Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 318–325. behavioral inhibition system: The relevance of the
Milich, R., & Kramer, J. (1984). Reflections on neuropsychological theory of Jeffrey A. Gray. In
impulsivity: An empirical investigation of L.M. Bloomingdale & J.A. Sergeant (Eds.), Atten-
impulsivity as a construct. In K.D. Gadow (Ed.), tion Deficit disorder: Criteria, cognition, interven-
Advances in learning and behavioral disabilities tion (pp. 117–125). Oxford: Pergamon.
(Vol. 3, pp. 57–94). Greenwich, CT: JAI. Quay, H.C. (1988b). The behavioral reward and inhi-
Mitterer, J.O. (1982). There are at least two kinds of bition system in childhood behavior disorder. In
poor readers: Whole word readers and recoding L.M. Bloomingdale (Ed.), Attention deficit disor-
poor readers. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, der (Vol. 3, pp. 176–185). Oxford: Pergamon.
445–461. Rafal, R., & Henik, A. (1994). The neurology of inhi-
Morris, R.D., Stuebing, K.K., Fletcher, J.M., Shay- bition: Integrating controlled and automatic pro-
witz, S.E., Lyon, G.R., Shankweiler, D.P., Katz, cesses. In D. Dagenbach & T.H. Carr (Eds.), Inhib-
312 MENNO VAN DER SCHOOT ET AL.

itory processes in attention, memory, and lan- Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial
guage. San Diego: Academic Press. verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
Rutter, M. (1978). Prevalence and types of dyslexia. ogy, 18, 643–662.
In A.L. Benton & D. Pearl (Eds.), Dyslexia: An Struiksma, A.J.V., Van der Leij, A., & Vieijra, J.P.M.
appraisal of current knowledge (pp. 3–28). New (1989). Diagnostiek van technisch lezen en
York/London: Oxford University Press. aanvankelijk spellen [Diagnostics of technical
Satz, P., & Morris, R. (1981). Learning disabilities reading and early spelling ability]. Amsterdam:
subtypes: A review. In F.J. Pirozzolo & M.C. VU Uitgeverij.
Wittrock (Eds.), Neuropsychological and cognitive Treisman, A.M. (1960). Contextual cues in selective
processes in reading (pp. 109–141). New York: listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psy-
Academic Press. chology, 12, 242–248.
Schachar, R., & Logan, G.D. (1990). Impulsivity and Van Aarle, E.J.M., & Volleberg, M.J. (1986). Raders
inhibitory control in normal development and en spellers: Wat is de betekenis van dit onder-
childhood psychopathology. Developmental Psy- scheid in de groep zwakke lezers? Pedagogische
chology, 26, 710–720. Studiën, 63, 339–346.
Sergeant, J.A., Van Velthoven, R., & Virginia, A. Van den Berg, R.M., & Te Lintelo, H.G. (1977). AVI-
(1979). Hyperactivity, impulsivity and reflectivity: pakket [The AVI-system]. Den Bosch: KPC.
An examination of their relationship and implica- Van der Leij, D.A.V. (1983). Ernstige leesproblemen.
tions for clinical child psychology. Journal of Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 20, 47–60. Van Strien, J.W., Bakker, D.J., Bouma, A., & Koops,
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. W. (1990). Familial resemblance for cognitive
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of abilities in families with P-type dyslexic, L-type
London, B, 298, 199–209. dyslexic, or normal reading boys. Journal of Clini-
Shaywitz, B.A, Fletcher, J.M., & Shaywitz, S.E. cal and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12,
(1994). Defining and classifying learning disabili- 843–856.
ties and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Van Strien, J.W., Bouma, A., & Bakker, D.J. (1993).
Journal of Child Neurology, 10 (Supp. 1), 50–57. Lexical decision performances in P-type dyslexic,
Shaywitz, B.A., Fletcher, J.M., Holahan, J.M., L-type dyslexic and normal reading boys. Journal
Shneider, A.E., Marchione, K.E., Stuebing, K.K., of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,
Francis, D.J., Shankweiler, D.P., Katz, L., 15, 516–524.
Liberman, I.Y., & Shaywitz, S.E. (1995). Interrela- Van Strien, J.W. (1999). Verbal learning in boys with
tionships between reading disability and attention- P-type dyslexia, L-type dyslexia and boys without
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Neuropsychol- learning disabilities: Differences in learning curves
ogy, 1, 170–186. and in serial position curves. Child Neuropsychol-
Shue, K.L., & Douglas, V.I. (1992). ADHD and the ogy, 5, 145–153.
frontal lobe syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 20, Willcutt, E.G., & Pennington, B.F. (2000). Co-
104–124. morbidity of reading disability and attention-defi-
Silver, L. (1981). The relationship between learning cit/hyperactivity disorder: Differences by gender
disabilities, hyperactivity, distractibility, and be- and subtype. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33,
havioral problems. Journal of the American Acad- 179–191.
emy of Child Psychiatry, 20, 385–397. Winer, B.J. (1971). Statistical principles in experi-
Stanovich, K.E., Siegel, L.S., & Gottardo, A. (1997). mental design. Tokyo: McGraw-Hill Kogakusha.
Converging evidence for phonological and surface
subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 89, 114–127.

You might also like