You are on page 1of 7

CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES

Understanding Socioeconomic Differences in Parents’


Speech to Children
Meredith L. Rowe
Harvard University

ABSTRACT—In this article, I address our understanding of and extent of that relationship, the mechanisms underlying it,
the word gap, or why parents’ talk to children differs by and the efficacy of interventions targeting parents’ input to
socioeconomic status. The differences in quantity and improve children’s language development. We now know that
quality of parents’ input across early childhood predict socioeconomic differences in children’s vocabulary development
children’s language development and their readiness for are due, in part, to differences in parents’ input (5). However, a
school. As a result, a growing number of interventions tar- question that has not received much attention in the literature
get parent–child interactions to enhance children’s early (although see 6 for a review) remains: Why do average differ-
experiences and promote learning. Understanding the fac- ences in parents’ input across SES groups exist? Understanding
tors that shape parents’ communication with their children why parents differ in the communicative environments they offer
is essential and has implications for empirical research as their children will help improve efforts to develop interventions
well as interventions to reduce early disparities in chil- that promote children’s language learning.
dren’s language development. In this article, I summarize what we know about the relation-
ship between the quantity and quality of parents’ language input
KEYWORDS—language development; socioeconomic status;
and children’s language development. Then I describe the
parenting; word gap
variability in measures of parents’ input in relation to specific
indicators of SES, discuss related factors that can help us under-
In the 1970s and 1980s, researchers studied differences in par- stand the effects of SES on input, and highlight implications for
ents’ communication style with children across socioeconomic interventions and the need for further research.
status (SES), primarily from a sociolinguistic and ethnographic
perspective (1, 2). However, in the 1990s, two seminal longitudi- PARENTS’ INPUT AND CHILDREN’S LANGUAGE
nal studies quantified extensive variation in parents’ talk to chil- DEVELOPMENT
dren (within and across SES) that predicted rate of vocabulary
development during early childhood (3, 4). The link between Several reviews focus on the role of parents’ input in children’s
parents’ input and children’s language learning spurred research language learning (7–9). This work is rooted in sociocultural
by developmental and educational psychologists into the nature and social-interactionist theoretical perspectives (10, 11), which
stress the importance of children’s early social and communica-
Meredith L. Rowe, Graduate School of Education, Harvard tive experiences for language development. Next, I briefly sum-
University. marize the main findings as a starting point for discussing why
The work described in this article was supported by grant R21 parents vary in the input they offer their children.
HD078771 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of First, the quantity of the input matters. In many studies, the
Child Health and Human Development. I thank Paul Harris for help- quantity of parents’ talk is associated positively with the rate of
ful feedback on an earlier draft of the article. children’s vocabulary growth (4). How much parents0 talk is also
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to associated with the efficiency of children’s language processing
Meredith L. Rowe, Harvard Graduate School of Education, 504 Lar-
in tasks measuring reaction time to verbal prompts. Thus, hear-
sen Hall, Appian Way, Cambridge, MA 02138; e-mail: meredith
_rowe@gse.harvard.edu. ing more input may accelerate vocabulary growth because it
builds children’s abilities to process spoken language (12).
© 2017 The Authors
Child Development Perspectives © 2017 The Society for Research in Child Development Second, certain qualitative features of parents’ input predict
DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12271 language learning, often more so than the quantity of the input.

Volume 12, Number 2, 2018, Pages 122–127


Socioeconomic Status and Parent Input 123

These features can be divided into two categories, linguistic fea- than parents from less educated and advantaged backgrounds.
tures and interactional features (see 7 for a review). Some lin- But in every sample, some parents from low-SES backgrounds
guistic features of input that are positively associated with talk more than parents from high-SES backgrounds. For exam-
language development include the diversity and sophistication ple, in their seminal study, Hart and Risley ranked 42 families
of parents’ vocabulary (13), the syntactic complexity of their on a socioeconomic occupational index (3, appendix A).
utterances (14), and the use of questions rather than directives Although they found large average SES differences in measures
(3). Interactional features that play a role include responding of parents’ talk that favored higher-SES families, parents in fam-
verbally and contingently to infants’ actions (15), and engaging ilies 31 and 36 (relatively low on the index) produced more talk
toddlers in more fluid and connected back-and-forth conversa- and more different vocabulary words, and asked as many, if not
tions (16). Furthermore, infants and young toddlers benefit from more, questions than families ranked 1 and 3 (high on the
conversations about objects they are attending to (17), whereas index). In addition, even within groups that are homogeneous in
older toddlers and preschoolers benefit from input that is more SES, the quantity and quality of parents’ input that predicts
abstract and less grounded in the here and now, such as talk children’s language development vary considerably (4, 16, 23).
about the past or the future (18). Thus, we know much about the Furthermore, some features of input that positively predict chil-
relations between specific types of input and language develop- dren’s language development vary based on SES, yet some do
ment, but we know less about why parents vary in how they not. For example, the lower-SES families in the Hart and Risley
communicate with their children. Next, I address possible study asked fewer questions on average than the higher-SES
sources of this variability. families. However, other studies have reported little difference
across SES in the extent to which parents talk with their chil-
FACTORS RELATED TO CHILDREN dren about past events (2, 18).
Some researchers have suggested that SES differences in par-
Communication is by nature a transaction. Before turning to fac- ents’ input stem from general class differences in communica-
tors related to parents, we need to consider the potential role of tion style (1, 24). However, we found that the quantity, lexical
children’s characteristics in shaping parents’ input. Indeed, in diversity, and syntactic complexity of talk parents used with
longitudinal studies within and across SES groups, parents their children were not associated with these characteristics of
adjust their input over time based on the age of the child (19), their talk when they conversed with a researcher. Moreover,
and the diversity and complexity of the child’s language use in although SES was associated significantly with measures of par-
an interaction (14). Other factors related to children, such as ents’ talk to children, it was not associated with parents’ talk to
motivation or curiosity, also interact with parents’ input. For the researcher (25). This suggests that the effects of SES on par-
example, the quality of parents’ input as they read books with ents’ child-directed speech might be due not to general differ-
their toddlers (e.g., use of metalingual talk) and children’s ences in communication style but to differences in parents’
expressed interest during these interactions combine to predict approach toward their children or other factors.
children’s vocabulary several years later (20, 21). Thus, making SES itself is a construct, typically measured as family income,
input engaging while reading books may keep children inter- parents’ education, parents’ occupation, or a combination of the
ested in the activity, and children who are interested may be three. These components of SES may affect parents’ input differ-
more likely to learn from the input. ently and independently. Thus, even in studies of low-income
We see similar interactional cycles in analyses of children’s families, other measures of SES vary. For example, in a study
questions and parents’ explanations. Specifically, children whose my colleagues and I conducted of families participating in the
parents provide causal explanations in response to their ques- national evaluation of Early Head Start (19), parents’ education
tions are more likely to ask follow-up questions and to continue varied widely from 8 to 18 years, which predicted the amount
seeking information. In contrast, when children receive nonex- and diversity of parents’ input. Therefore, when interpreting
planations, they either provide their own explanation or try to findings based on SES, we need to understand that differences
ask their question again (22). These examples highlight how par- are average differences and that the different components of
ents’ input can shape, and be shaped by, children’s contribu- SES may relate to parents’ input in different ways. Furthermore,
tions to the interaction. Additional research into this area is understanding the myriad factors that contribute to variation in
needed because we have more to learn about children’s contri- how parents communicate with their children can help us under-
butions to parent–child interactions within and across SES. stand the mechanisms underlying these average SES effects.

PARENTS’ INPUT AND SES PARENTS’ INPUT AND OTHER FACTORS

Studies that identify SES differences in parents’ input report av- Adopting the theoretical framework presented by the bioecologi-
erage differences. On average, parents from more educated and cal model of human development (26), we can think of SES and
advantaged backgrounds tend to talk more with their children its underlying components as more distal factors relating to

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 12, Number 2, 2018, Pages 122–127


124 Meredith L. Rowe

parents’ input, with the goal being to uncover the more proximal Parents’ views on learning may also influence their input.
factors that explain the mechanisms of the effects of SES. I cate- Less-educated parents tend to view teaching and learning as
gorize these factors broadly into those that might stem from dif- didactic or teacher-directed. And parents with didactic views
ferences in parents’ education versus those that might be due interact with their preschool children differently (e.g., using
more to the effects of income. The factors related to education flash cards) than parents who view teaching as more child-cen-
include skills parents learned in school, such as vocabulary or tered (e.g., reading more with their children; 31). The extent to
literacy skills, as well as beliefs, goals, mindsets, and knowledge which parents see their children’s skills as malleable is also
about children’s development, all of which relate to education in associated with their input. Parents who see intelligence as more
one way or another. The factors related to income may be more malleable than fixed (i.e., those who have a growth mindset)
situational and include parents’ stress and depression, material praise efforts more than ability in their 1- to 3-year olds, which
hardships, and household chaos and disorder, each of which can predicts the children’s likelihood of having growth mindsets
result from economic pressures. Admittedly, education and later and a belief that success comes with hard work (32). Con-
income are not completely separable—they tend to be corre- versely, the more parents believe abilities are fixed, the less they
lated positively—albeit moderately—across studies. Neverthe- engage in math and literacy activities with their young children
less, they may predict variation in parents’ input through (33). While relations between SES and parents’ intelligence
distinct processes and mechanisms; the more we understand mindsets are inconsistent, helping parents understand that what
about what is driving SES differences in input, the more pre- they do influences their children’s language development might
pared we will be to design interventions that enhance language be useful. Indeed, in studies that promote growth mindsets in
environments for children from low-SES homes. parents, researchers saw positive changes in parent–child inter-
actions, with parents providing more constructive comments and
Factors Related to Education asking more guiding questions during a problem-solving task
Across cultures, parents who have more formal schooling rely (34).
more on verbal interaction with their young children than par-
ents with less schooling. The language and discourse used in Factors Related to Income
formal school environments socialize individuals to communi- Families living in poverty have more household chaos, defined
cate in more verbal and abstract ways that they draw on when as a combination of greater household density, family instability,
they become parents (27). As a distal factor, schooling is likely lack of structure, and higher noise levels (35). In some studies,
to be connected to key features of parents’ verbal input. For these proximal factors help explain the effects of income on par-
example, parents who have acquired larger vocabularies and ents’ input. In a secondary analysis of Hart and Risley’s (3) data
advanced literacy skills use more diverse vocabulary and more that controlled for income, parents who lived in more crowded
complex utterances when interacting with their young children, homes were less responsive and used less varied language with
even after controlling for their years of education (19, 28). This their children between 6 and 36 months than parents who lived
suggests that the language, literacy, and discourse patterns par- in less crowded homes (36). Similarly, in a more recent study
ents learned in school are proximal factors that help explain that controlled for family income and parents’ education, house-
why education relates to parents’ input. hold disorganization predicted children’s language abilities at
Parenting knowledge also plays a role. Parents’ knowledge of age 3, and the effect was mediated by parents’ positive stimula-
children’s development explains the significant effect of SES on tion and engagement with their children (37). Thus, household
parents’ use of language-promoting features of input with tod- organization seems to matter, in part because it affects how par-
dlers, including talking more, using more varied vocabulary, ents interact with their children.
and using longer utterances and fewer directives (25). In one Lower-income families are also more likely than higher-
study, parenting knowledge was related to both parents’ educa- income families to experience material hardships, including food
tion and parents’ income, but the relation was stronger to educa- insecurity, lack of medical insurance, and other financial trou-
tion. Why do parents who are more educated have more bles (38). In one study, positive relations between family income
knowledge of parenting? Parents with more education may be and positive parenting behaviors (including verbal cognitive
more likely than parents with less education to turn to health stimulation) were the result of increased income that reduced
professionals or written materials for information about parent- material hardship and parents’ stress; in turn, those positive par-
ing. Indeed, in one study (29), some of the relation between par- enting behaviors predicted more optimal cognitive outcomes in
ents’ education and parenting knowledge was because of children (38).
parents’ access to written materials. In another study, parents’ Lower-income parents are more likely than higher-income
years of education were related significantly to whether they parents to be depressed (39), and the effects of depression may
reported turning to professionals (e.g., doctors, nurses, child care carry over into interactions with children. For example, among
providers) rather than family members for help with parenting low-SES families, mothers with more symptoms of depression
and information about parenting (30). talked less to their children than mothers with fewer symptoms

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 12, Number 2, 2018, Pages 122–127


Socioeconomic Status and Parent Input 125

of depression, even if they did not differ in the diversity of their The SES-based achievement gap in language and literacy
vocabulary use (19). In other studies, more depressed parents skills in the United States grows the most during early childhood
were less responsive to infants’ vocalizations and less likely to and is large at the start of kindergarten (45). Most parent-
use child-directed speech in verbal interactions with their focused interventions to improve the development of children’s
infants (40). Not all studies find effects of depression on parents’ language and literacy have targeted middle-SES families or fam-
input, but a meta-analysis identified significant effects, with ilies with children who have language delays, but a growing
depression mediating income and positive parenting (41). number of promising parenting interventions that focus on the
word gap aim to prevent it from emerging (e.g., Providence Talks,
IMPLICATIONS FOR INTERVENTION AND Thirty Million Words). However, researchers should align inter-
SUGGESTIONS FOR RESEARCH ventions with the processes by which these SES-related compo-
nents affect parent–child interactions. More research with low-
The factors related to education and income that I have dis- SES families can help us gain a greater understanding of the
cussed suggest that we should go further than collecting infor- myriad factors that contribute to how parents communicate with
mation about parents’ SES: At the start of parent-focused their children. With greater knowledge, we can tailor interven-
interventions, we should aim to understand parents’ knowledge tions to provide all families with the information they need to
of children’s development, beliefs about learning, and mindsets help their children succeed.
about intelligence. It would also be helpful to understand as
much as possible about the cultural and educational experiences REFERENCES
that contribute to variation among parents. In evaluation
research, we should pay close attention to which kinds of inter- 1. Bernstein, B. (2003). Class, codes and control: Applied studies
towards a sociology of language (Vol. 2). London, UK: Routledge.
ventions work for whom and why. As an example, a recent par-
2. Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life and work in
ent-focused literacy intervention embedded a growth mindset communities and classrooms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
component highlighting helpful ways parents could praise their Press.
second graders’ effort rather than their ability. The intervention 3. Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday
had a large effect on parents’ and children’s participation in the experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
program and on children’s skills, but the effect was even more 4. Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T.
pronounced for parents who initially endorsed more fixed mind- (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and
gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236–248. https://doi.org/10.
sets about their children’s abilities (42). Thus, having a fixed
1037/0012-1649.27.2.236
mindset does not preclude parents from interacting with their 5. Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioe-
children in ways that promote learning, but these parents might conomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal
benefit most from information highlighting the role they can play speech. Child Development, 74, 1368–1378. https://doi.org/10.1111/
in promoting their children’s language and literacy develo- 1467-8624.00612
pment. 6. Hoff, E. (2003). Causes and consequences of SES-related differ-
As another example, the Thirty Million Words intervention, ences in parent-to-child speech. In M. H. Bornstein & R. H. Bradley
(Eds.), Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development (pp.
which is designed to increase the quantity and quality of par-
145–160). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ents’ input in low-SES families, focuses on providing parents 7. Cartmill, E. A. (2016). Mind the gap: Assessing and addressing the
with knowledge of children’s language development and helping word gap in early education. Policy Insights From the Behavioral
parents understand that they can play a role in their children’s and Brain Sciences, 3, 185–193. https://doi.org/10.1177/
learning. Indeed, the significant effect of the intervention on par- 2372732216657565
ents’ input is a result of increases in parenting knowledge (43). 8. Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language
Thus, we have evidence that providing parents with information development. Developmental Review, 26, 55–88. https://doi.org/10.
tailored to their beliefs and knowledge is an effective strategy. 1016/j.dr.2005.11.002
9. Schwab, J. F., & Lew-Williams, C. (2016). Language learning,
The other important proximal factors—such as material hard-
socioeconomic status, and child-directed speech. Wiley Interdisci-
ship, household chaos, stress, and depression—can be plinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 7, 264–275. https://doi.org/10.
addressed less directly through interventions. Yet this does not 1002/wcs.1393
mean they should be ignored. Researchers should continue to 10. Bruner, J. (1981). The social context of language acquisition. Lan-
measure these factors to develop a greater understanding of their guage and Communication, 1, 155–178.
role in moderating the effects of interventions. And these factors 11. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher
should not preclude including parents in intervention studies. psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
12. Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters:
For example, although depressed parents are more likely to drop
Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocab-
out of intervention studies, those who remain participate actively ulary. Psychological Science, 24, 2143–2152. https://doi.org/10.
and benefit as much as nondepressed parents (44). 1177/0956797613488145

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 12, Number 2, 2018, Pages 122–127


126 Meredith L. Rowe

13. Weizman, Z. O., & Snow, C. E. (2001). Lexical output as related to changes the lives of the world’s children. New York, NY: Oxford
children’s vocabulary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure University Press.
and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology, 37, 265–279. 28. Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O., & Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.2.265 of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of
14. Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, Child Language, 25, 367–393.
L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. 29. Bornstein, M. H., Cote, L. R., Haynes, O. M., Hahn, C., & Park, Y.
Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343–365. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cog (2010). Parent knowledge: Experiential and socioedemographic fac-
psych.2010.08.002 tors in European American mothers of young children. Developmen-
15. Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., & Song, L. (2014). Why is tal Psychology, 46, 1677–1693. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020677
infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness? 30. Rowe, M. L., Denmark, N., Jones Harden, B., & Stapleton, L.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 121–126. https://d (2016). The role of parent education and parenting knowledge in
oi.org/10.1177/0963721414522813 children’s language and literacy skills among White, Black, and
16. Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Latino American families. Infant and Child Development, 25, 198–
Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., . . . Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of 220. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1924
early communication quality to low-income children’s language suc- 31. Stipek, D., Milburn, S., Clements, D., & Daniels, D. H. (1992). Par-
cess. Psychological Science, 26, 1071–1083. https://doi.org/10.1177/ ents’ beliefs about appropriate education for young children. Journal
0956797615581493 of Applied Developmental Psychology, 13, 293–310. https://doi.org/
17. Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Embodied attention and word learn- 10.1016/0193-3973(92)90034-F
ing by toddlers. Cognition, 125, 244–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.c 32. Gunderson, E. A., Gripshover, S. J., Romero, C., Dweck, C. S.,
ognition.2012.06.016 Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2013). Parent praise to 1- to 3-
18. Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quan- year-olds predicts children’s motivational frameworks 5 years later.
tity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 84, 1526–1541. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.
Child Development, 83, 1762–1774. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- 12064
8624.2012.01805.x 33. Meunks, K., Miele, D., Ramani, G. B., Stapleton, L., & Rowe, M. L.
19. Rowe, M. L., Pan, B. A., & Ayoub, C. (2005). Predictors of variation (2015). Parent beliefs about the fixedness of ability. Journal of
in maternal talk to children: A longitudinal study of low-income Applied Developmental Psychology, 41, 78–89. https://doi.org/10.
families. Parenting: Science and Practice, 5, 285–310. https://doi. 1016/j.appdev.2015.08.002
org/10.1207/s15327922par0503_3 34. Moorman, E. A., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2010). Ability mindsets influ-
20. Deckner, D. F., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (2006). Child ence the quality of mothers’ involvement in children’s learning: An
and maternal contributions to shared reading: Effects on lan- experimental investigation. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1354–
guage and literacy development. Journal of Applied Developmen- 1362. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020376
tal Psychology, 27, 31–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2005. 35. Evans, G. W., & Wachs, T. D. (2010). Chaos and its influence on
12.001 children’s development. Washington, DC: American Psychological
21. Malin, J. L., Cabrera, N. J., & Rowe, M. L. (2014). Low-income Association.
minority mothers’ and fathers’ reading and children’s interest: Lon- 36. Evans, G. W., Maxwell, L. E., & Hart, B. (1999). Parental language
gitudinal contributions to children’s receptive vocabulary skills. and verbal responsiveness to children in crowded homes. Develop-
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 425–432. https://doi.org/ mental Psychology, 35, 1020–1023. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-
10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.04.010 1649.35.4.1020
22. Frazier, B. N., Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H. M. (2009). Preschool- 37. Vernon-Feagons, L., Garrett-Peters, P., Willoughby, M., Mills-
ers’ search for explanatory information within adult–child conversa- Koonce, R.; The family life project key investigators. (2012). Chaos,
tion. Child Development, 80, 1592–1611. https://doi.org/10.1111/j. poverty and parenting: Predictors of early language development.
1467-8624.2009.01356.x Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 339–351. https://doi.org/
23. Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Mater- 10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.11.001
nal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low- 38. Gershoff, E. T., Aber, J. L., Raver, C. C., & Lennon, M. C. (2007).
income families. Child Development, 76, 763–782. https://doi.org/ Income is not enough: Incorporating material hardship into models
10.1111/1467-8624.00498-i1 of income associations with parenting and child development. Child
24. Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother–child conversation in different Development, 78, 70–95. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.
social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62, 00986.x
782–796. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01569.x 39. Berger, L. M., Paxson, C., & Waldfogel, J. (2009). Income and child
25. Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeco- development. Children and Youth Services Review, 31, 978–989.
nomic status, knowledge of child development, and child vocabulary https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2009.04.013
skill. Journal of Child Language, 35, 185–205. https://doi.org/10. 40. Breznitz, Z., & Sherman, T. (1987). Speech patterning of natural dis-
1017/S0305000907008343 course of well and depressed mothers and their young children.
26. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological Child Development, 58, 395–400. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130516
model of human development. In W. Damon, R. Lerner (Series 41. Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O’Hare, E., & Neuman, G. (2000).
Eds.), & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. Maternal depression and parenting behavior: A meta-analytic
1: Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 793–828). review. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 561–592. https://doi.org/10.
New York, NY: Wiley. 1016/S0272-7358(98)00100-7
27. LeVine, R. A., LeVine, S. E., Schnell-Anzola, B., Rowe, M. L., & 42. Andersen, S. C., & Nielsen, H. S. (2016). Intervention with a
Dexter, E. (2012). Literacy and mothering: How women’s schooling growth mindset approach improves children’s skills. Proceedings

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 12, Number 2, 2018, Pages 122–127


Socioeconomic Status and Parent Input 127

of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of 44. Bigatti, S. M., Cronan, T. A., & Anaya, A. (2001). The effects of
America, 113, 12111–12113. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas. maternal depression on the efficacy of a literacy intervention pro-
1607946113 gram. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 32, 147–162.
43. Suskind, D. L., Leffel, K. R., Graf, E., Hernandez, M. W., Gunder- https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012250824091
son, E. A., Sapolich, S. G., . . . Levine, S. C. (2016). A parent-direc- 45. vonHippel, P. T., & Hamrock, C. (2016). Do test score gaps grow
ted language intervention for children of low socioeconomic status: before, during, or between the school years? Measurement artifacts
A randomized controlled pilot study. Journal of Child Language, and what we can know in spite of them. SSRN. Retrieved from
43, 366–406. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000033 https://ssrn.com/abstract=2745527

Child Development Perspectives, Volume 12, Number 2, 2018, Pages 122–127


Copyright of Child Development Perspectives is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

You might also like