You are on page 1of 12

Received: 6 December 2019 Revised: 14 April 2020 Accepted: 15 April 2020

DOI: 10.1002/eat.23285

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Perfectionism and impulsivity based risk profiles


in eating disorders

Kärol Soidla MA | Kirsti Akkermann PhD

Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu,


Näituse, Estonia Abstract
Objective: The categorical classification of eating disorders (ED) has several limita-
Correspondence
Kirsti Akkermann, Institute of Psychology, tions, for example, high symptom variability within the diagnosis and limited predic-
University of Tartu, Näituse 2, 50409, Estonia. tive validity for treatment response. An alternative is classifying individuals with ED
Email: kirsti.akkermann@ut.ee
based on personality traits, which can reflect underlying etiological mechanisms. We
Action Editor: Ruth Weissman aimed to find latent profiles based on facets of maladaptive and adaptive perfection-
ism, impulsivity and ED symptoms.
Method: The sample comprised of 274 women—164 had an ED diagnosis and
110 were controls. Two separate latent profile analyses were performed—one on the
mixed sample (controls and individuals with ED) and the other on the sample of indi-
viduals with ED only.
Results: We identified a five-class model to be the best fit for the mixed sample. The
classes were: (a) moderately impulsive, (b) high functioning, (c) purely perfectionistic,
(d) emotionally dysregulated, (e) behaviorally dysregulated. Among the individuals
with ED, a four-class solution was found to be the best fit. The classes were very sim-
ilar in their response patterns on indicator variables to response patterns observed in
the mixed sample, except the emotionally and behaviorally dysregulated classes that
formed into one class.
Discussion: In addition to the well-known high-functioning, overcontrolled and
undercontrolled classes, two to three undercontrolled classes (moderately impulsive,
behaviorally, and emotionally dysregulated class) emerged. Those classes differenti-
ated on perfectionism and impulsivity levels as well as on ED symptom severity and
psychiatric comorbidities—all of which may influence maintenance of ED, appropriate
treatment choice and therefore treatment response.

KEYWORDS

eating disorders, impulsivity, latent profile analysis, perfectionism, personality traits

1 | I N T RO DU CT I O N Holland, & Bodell, 2012; Peat, Mitchell, Hoek, & Wonderlich, 2009;
Widiger & Samuel, 2005).
There is a growing interest in alternative ways to classify eating disor- According to Wildes and Marcus (2013), two main alternative
ders (ED) (Insel et al., 2010; Wildes & Marcus, 2013) as the current cat- models have been proposed. The first model focuses on classifying indi-
egorical classification of ED has several limitations, including high viduals on the basis of ED symptoms and the second on the basis of
symptom variability within diagnoses, lack of diagnostic stability, and comorbid psychopathology and associated features, including personal-
limited predictive validity for treatment (Insel et al., 2010; Keel, Brown, ity traits. The second model is particularly promising, as comorbid

Int J Eat Disord. 2020;1–12. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/eat © 2020 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 1


2 SOIDLA AND AKKERMANN

psychopathology and personality traits may reflect distinctive pathways (Brunas-Wagstaff, Bergquist, & Wagstaff, 1994; Dickman, 1990).
to disordered eating (Westen & Harnden-Fischer, 2001) and help to Analyzing different aspects of impulsivity Whiteside and Lynam (2001)
shed light on treatment prognosis, heterogeneity in symptomatic pro- found that dysfunctional impulsivity is positively correlated with lack of
files and maintenance of ED (Farstad, McGeown, & von Ranson, 2016). premeditation, while functional impulsivity is positively correlated
Several studies, regardless of the sample, type of assessment and with sensation seeking and negatively with urgency (Whiteside &
statistical analyses used, have identified at least three main latent Lynam, 2001). The association between disordered eating behavior and
classes among EDs: an undercontrolled (high impulsivity, emotional impulsivity is strongest within EDs characterized by binge eating or
reactivity, risky behaviors), an overcontrolled (high perfectionism, purging behaviors (Waxman, 2009). However, heightened levels of
rigidity, compulsivity, inhibition), and a high functioning class (for an impulsive behavior have been found in all ED subgroups, including
overview see Wildes & Marcus, 2013). In most of the studies, three to restricting types (Claes, Robinson, Muehlenkamp, Vandereycken, &
six class solutions have been found (Wildes & Marcus, 2013). In stud- Bijttebier, 2010). Therefore, it has been suggested that impulsivity does
ies which have found more than three classes, the undercontrolled, not differentiate well between ED diagnoses, but rather their clinical
overcontrolled and high functioning class are still recognizable (Krug presentation (for a review see Waxman, 2009).
et al., 2011; Thompson-Brenner et al., 2008; Turner et al., 2014). Two studies have previously examined the profiles based on
perfectionism and impulsivity and four latent classes have been found.
Boone, Claes, and Luyten (2014) study was conducted on healthy
1.1 | Perfectionism, impulsivity, and eating adolescents and the highest level of ED psychopathology was
disorders reported by individuals who had both high levels of perfectionism and
impulsivity. In Slof-Op't Landt, van Furth, and Claes (2016) study,
One possible and promising way to classify individuals with ED is by which was conducted in a sample of individuals with ED, the highest
using dimensions of perfectionism (associated with overcontrol and level of psychopathology was present in the high adaptive/high mal-
compulsivity) and impulsivity, as both of those traits are central fea- adaptive perfectionism class.
tures of EDs and may play an important role in influencing treatment
outcome (Bardone-Cone et al., 2007; Egan, Wade, & Shafran, 2011;
Farstad et al., 2016; Waxman, 2009). 1.2 | The present study
Perfectionism is a well-established multidimensional construct, con-
sisting of adaptive (organization, personal standards) and maladaptive The aim of the current study was to classify individuals based on
(concern over mistakes, parental criticism) dimensions (Frost, Marten, dimensions of impulsivity and perfectionism, as well as ED symptoms.
Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990; Lo & Abbott, 2013). Perfectionism has been In most of the previous studies, ED symptoms were not included in
associated with disordered eating behaviors like dieting, bulimic symp- the latent profile analysis (LPA) as indicators. Including ED symptoms
toms, preoccupation with food, weight, shape and compensatory behav- as indicators in the latent profile model may reduce statistical bias,
iors (Bardone-Cone et al., 2007; Egan et al., 2011). Both perfectionistic produce stronger relations and therefore reveal possible reciprocal
concerns (maladaptive perfectionism) and strivings (adaptive perfection- relationship between symptoms and risk factors in the classes, which
ism) are strongly associated with EDs (Limburg, Watson, Hagger, & the traditional classify-analyze approach may not account for (more
Egan, 2016). A recent meta-analysis also concluded that individuals with about the statistical disadvantages about the traditional classify-
anorexia nervosa (AN) had both higher adaptive and maladaptive perfec- analyze approach see Lanza, Tan, & Bray, 2013). Adding the ED
tionism compared to controls. However, there were no significant differ- symptoms as indicators in the analysis is also theoretically meaningful.
ences in maladaptive perfectionism between individuals with AN and Widiger (2011) argues in his paper that personality and psychopathol-
bulimia nervosa (BN) (Dahlenburg, Gleaves, & Hutchinson, 2019). A com- ogy both influence each other (pathoplastic relationship). Lilenfield,
bination of high personal standards and high evaluative concerns is asso- Wonderlich, Riso, Crosby, and Mitchell (2006) have proposed a
ciated with the highest level of ED symptoms, such as restrained eating, pathoplastic model explaining the relationship between EDs and per-
concern over eating, weight, and shape (Boone, Soenens, Caroline, & sonality traits. The pathoplastic model is also supported by studies
Goossens, 2010). that have shown the effect of starvation and chronic dieting on brain
Impulsivity as a multidimensional construct can be divided into functioning, including amplification of rigidity and emotion dys-
negative urgency (tendency to act reckless and without further thought regulation (for a review see Kaye, Fudge, & Paulus, 2009).
in stressful situations while experiencing negative affect), positive Considering the dimensional nature of personality traits and ED
urgency (tendency to engage in impulsive behavior when experiencing symptoms, and that disordered eating behavior is prevalent in general
strong positive emotions), lack of premeditation, sensation seeking and population, we decided to include both individuals with ED and con-
lack of perseverance (Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). However, impulsivity trols in our study. We also did additional LPA including only individ-
can also be divided into functional (characterized by rapid style of infor- uals with ED to control whether the results are affected by using a
mation processing resulting in quick thinking and responses, when such mixed sample.
style is optimal) and dysfunctional (failure to inhibit inappropriate We hypothesized that at least a four-class solution emerges and the
responses, characterized by recklessness and excessive haste) domains classes resemble the well established overcontrolled, undercontrolled,
SOIDLA AND AKKERMANN 3

and high functioning classes as well as the previously found combined criticism/parental expectations (α = 0.89, ω = 0.89), for example, “I never
high perfectionism and impulsivity class. feel that I can meet my parents' standards.”
Dickman's Impulsivity Inventory (DII) (Dickman, 1990) is a 24-item
questionnaire, which is answered on a 5-point Likert scale (from
2 | METHOD “totally agree” to “do not agree at all”). It consists of two subscales:
functional impulsivity (FI; e.g., “I am able to act quickly in difficult sit-
2.1 | Participants uations”) (α = 0.76; ω = 0.78) and dysfunctional impulsivity (DFI;
e.g., “I will often say whatever comes into my head without thinking
The sample for the latent profile analysis comprised of 274 women, of first”) (α = 0.83; ω = 0.84).
whom 164 where individuals with ED (age 14–48 years, M = 22.4, Eating Disorders Assessment Scale (EDAS) (Akkermann, 2010) is a
SD = 7.03) recruited from the inpatient unit of EDs, and 110 controls 29-item self-report questionnaire, which assesses ED symptoms.
(age 14–47 years, M = 24.4, SD = 8.19) recruited via public advertise- Items are answered on a 6-point Likert scale (from “never” to
ments and university lists using chain sampling method. Controls were “always”). The scale consists of four subscales (a) Restrained eating
screened for any psychiatric disorders. As it is known that impulsivity (e.g., “I prefer low-calorie food”) (α = 0.95; ω = 0.95), (b) Binge eating
decreases with age (Steinberg et al., 2008) we controlled for age (e.g., “There have been occasions when I cannot stop eating”)
differences. No significant differences between age groups (14–17, (α = 0.93; ω = 0.93), (c) Purging (e.g., “When I have been eating a large
18–25, 26–48-years) were found neither for dysfunctional impulsivity amount of food I vomit to alleviate discomfort”) (α = 0.97; ω = 0.97),
(DFI) Welch's F(2, 122.34) = 0.45, p = 0.64 nor functional impulsivity (d) Preoccupation with body image and body weight (α = 0.96;
(FI) Welch's F(2,117.57) = 1.72, p = 0.18. ω = 0.96) (e.g., “I am bothered by the thoughts that people may criti-
All the patients with ED who were voluntarily hospitalized were cize the way I look”). The latter assesses the cognitive-affective com-
given the opportunity to participate in the study. Individuals with ponent of ED, for example, body dissatisfaction, related negative
intellectual disability or current psychotic episode were excluded. The emotions, and concerns over one's appearance; subsequently referred
patients were either diagnosed with AN restricting type (AN-R) to as preoccupation. EDAS has been used in several studies (Uusberg,
(n = 53), AN binge-eating/purging type (AN-BP) (n = 11), atypical Peet, Uusberg, & Akkermann, 2018; Vainik, Neseliler, Konstabel, Fel-
AN-R (n = 6), BN binge-eating/purging type (BN-BP) (n = 79) or binge- lows, & Dagher, 2015) and the construct validity of the scale has been
eating disorder (BED) (n = 13). As all the atypical AN cases were confirmed by strong correlations between Eating Disorders
restricting type and the group was too small for separate analyses, Inventory-2 (EDI-2; Garner, 1991) and EDAS subscales
atypical AN-R cases were merged into one group with AN-R. (Akkermann, 2010).

2.2 | Measures 2.3 | Procedure

Participants were asked about their age, education level, weight The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the
history, duration of ED, and medications taken during the study University of Tartu. Written informed consent was obtained from the
2
period. BMI (kg/height m ) was calculated based on participants' participants, and in the case of adolescents also from their parents.
actual weight and height, which was measured by standardized proce- Participants filled in the questionnaires on the first days of hospitaliza-
dures in the hospital. tion and controls filled the questionnaires in a laboratory at the uni-
The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview MINI 5.0.0. versity setting.
(Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, & Amorim, 1994) is a short structured
psychiatric interview that was developed to diagnose DSM-IV-R and
ICD-10 mental disorders. Clinical interviews were conducted by a 2.4 | Data analysis
trained clinical psychologist. In individuals with ED both ED and
comorbid diagnoses were confirmed by a treating psychiatrist. The primary purpose of latent profile analysis (LPA) is to use posterior
Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (FMPS) (Frost et al., 1990). probabilities to model response pattern heterogeneity in a population,
The Estonian version consists of 28 self-report items, which are resulting in classification of individuals into more homogeneous
answered on a 5-point Likert scale (from “strongly disagree” to “strongly groups (Gibson, 1959; Oberski, 2016; Vermunt & Magidson, 2002).
agree”). The Estonian version consists of 4 subscales: (a) Organization FMPS, DII and EDAS subscales were treated as indicator variables
(α = 0.81; MacDonald's ω = 0.82), for example, “Neatness is very impor- in the model. LPA was performed in Mplus version 6.12. with robust
tant to me,” (b) Personal standards (α = 0.94; ω = 0.94), for example, likelihood maximum method. To avoid converging on a local solution
“Other people seem to accept lower standards from themselves than I 1000 random sets of starting values were used in the initial stage and
do,” (c) Concern over mistakes/doubts about actions (α = 0.92; ω = 0.92), 250 optimizations were used in the final stage. Bootstrap draws were
e.g., “If I fail partly, it is as bad as being a complete failure,” (d) Parental set for 100, each with two sets of random starting values and one
4

TABLE 1 Descriptive statistics and differences between eating disorder subtypes and controls in age, BMI, duration of eating disorder, FMPS, DII, and EDAS scores

ANOVA
AN-R (n = 59) AN-BP (n = 11) BN-BP (n = 79) BED (n = 13) Controls (n = 110)
Variable M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F(4,273) p η2
Age (years) 21.19 (6.39)d,* 22.30 (7.65) 22.15 (5.82)d,* 30.08 (11.32)a,c,* 24.42 (8.19) 5.09 <.001 0.072
c,d,e, d,e, ,c, a,d, ,b,
BMI 15.53 (2.89) ** 16.88 (1.01) ** * 21.69 (4.29) ** * 32.53 (8.88)a,b,c,e,** 22.25 (2.68)a,b,d,** 58.66 <.001 0.468
b,c,d, a, a, a,
Duration of ED (years) 2.79 (3.44) ** 6.35 (7.08) ** 5.91 (4.85) ** 9.77 (8.46) ** — 57.01(4,163) <.001 0.138
EDAS
EDAS total 49.25 (25.55)c,d,e,**,b,* 73.27 (33.26)a,*,e,** 90.51 (23.02)a,e,** 88.77 (15.47)a,e,** 32.36 (16.42)a,b,c,d,** 95.33 <.001 0.588
b,c, ,e, a, ,e, a,e, e,
Restrained eating 19.14 (12.78) * ** 27.91 (11.09) * ** 24.59 (8.81) * 19.54 (6.02) * 11.88 (7.09)a,b,c,*,d,** 25.77 <.001 0.278
c,d, c,d, a,b,e, a,b,e, c,d,
Binge eating 10.86 (7.54) ** 13.73 (8.49) ** 26.84 (8.95) ** 30.92 (10.04) ** 10.90 (6.11) ** 71.12 <.001 0.516
Purging 1.81 (3.19)b,c,** 8.91 (7.09)a,e,**,d,* 12.19 (6.01)a,d,e,** 3.69 (3.59)b,e,*,c,** 0.34 (1.18)b,c,**,d,* 114.33 <.001 0.631
c,d,e, d, ,e, a,e, ,d, a,e, ,b,c, a,b,c,d,
Preoccupation 17.44 (10.99) ** 22.73 (12.69) * ** 26.89 (9.18) ** * 34.62 (5.68) ** * 9.25 (7.49) ** 73.35 <.001 0.461
FMPS
Organization 20.32 (4.50) 22.45 (4.09) 20.27 (4.18) 19.54 (4.58) 19.43 (4.35) 1.56 .186 0.023
Standards 17.95 (8.19)e,** 18.91 (7.33) 17.99 (7.27)e,** 15.77 (9.51) 13.31 (6.26)a,c,** 6.85 <.001 0.093
e, e, e, e, a,c,d, ,b,
Mistakes 11.36 (8.49) ** 12.36 (8.35) * 11.77 (7.52) ** 13.62 (8.70) ** 5.13 (5.01) ** * 15.36 <.001 0.188
Parental criticism 7.92 (7.34) 11.00 (7.96) 10.01 (7.94)e,* 9.92 (7.29) 6.47 (6.18)c,* 3.55 .008 0.051
DII
DFI 15.12 (7.68)c,* 15.64 (7.37) 19.57 (7.71)a,*,e,** 16.46 (9.66) 14.33 (6.79)c,** 6.09 <.001 0.084
e, e, e, a,c,d,
FI 21.29 (8.98) * 22.27 (4.76) 22.22 (7.63) * 17.08 (7.19) * 25.75 (7.84) * 5.97 <.001 0.082

Note: p-values depicted are Sidak's adjusted p-values for multiple comparisons.
Abbreviations: η2, partial eta squared; AN-R, anorexia nervosa restricting; AN-BP, anorexia nervosa binge-eating/purging; BED, binge-eating disorder; BMI, body mass index; BN-BP, bulimia nervosa
binge-eating/purging; DFI, dysfunctional impulsivity; DII, Dickman's Impulsivity Inventory; EDAS, Eating Disorder Assessment Scale; preoccupation, preoccupation with body image and body weight; FI, func-
tional impulsivity; MPS, Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale; mistakes, concern over mistakes.
a
Statistically significant differences from AN-R.
b
Statistically significant differences from AN-BP group.
c
Statistically significant differences from BN group.
d
Statistically significant differences from BED group.
e
Statistically significant differences from control.
*p < .05.
**p < .001.
SOIDLA AND AKKERMANN
SOIDLA AND AKKERMANN 5

final stage optimization for the model with one less class, 50 sets of ranges from 0 to 1, higher values showing greater accuracy. Bootstrap
random starting values and 15 final stage optimizations for the alter- Likelihood Ratio test (BLRT; McLachlan & Peel, 2000) and
native model (for guidelines see: Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2010). Lo–Mendell–Rubin test (LMR; Lo, Mendell, & Rubin, 2001) were used
Model selection was based on the following information criteria: to compare if the improvement in the model was statistically signifi-
(a) Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwartz, 1978), (b) Akaike cant when one more class was included in the model.
Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1987), (c) Sample-Size Adjusted All other analyses were performed in SPSS Statistics version 20.
BIC (SSABIC; Sclove, 1987). Lower values of these three fit statistics Classes were compared on the measures which were included in LPA
indicate better model fit. To estimate the accuracy with which models by one-way ANOVA using Sidak's post-hoc for assessing pairwise dif-
classify individuals into classes Entropy was used. Entropy value ferences. Chi-square test of independence was used to compare the

TABLE 2 Fit indices for 1 to 7 class solution for two samples—mixed sample (individuals with ED and controls) and individuals with ED only

Free parameters LL AIC BIC Adjusted BIC Entropy BLRT LMR


Number of classes for the mixed sample
1 20 −9560.53 19,161.07 19,233.33 19,169.91 — — —
2 31 −9227.52 18,517.05 18,629.06 18,530.76 0.926 0.00001 0.00001
3 42 −9113.89 18,311.78 18,463.54 18,330.37 0.938 0.00001 0.008
4 53 −9078.19 18,262.38 18,453.88 18,285.83 0.928 0.00001 0.410
5 64 −9031.05 18,190.11 18,421.35 18,218.42 0.946 0.00001 0.320
6 75 −8994.83 18,139.67 18,410.65 18,172.84 0.934 0.00001 0.425
7 86 −8960.72 18,093.44 18,404.17 18,131.48 0.945 0.00001 0.418
Number of classes for the sample of individuals with ED
1 20 −5755.55 11,551.09 11,613.09 11,549.77 — — —
2 31 −5629.10 11,320.17 11,416.29 11,318.15 0.896 0.00001 0.00001
3 42 −5581.08 11,246.16 11,376.35 11,243.38 0.889 0.00001 0.032
4 53 −5556.86 11,219.73 11,384.02 11,216.23 0.867 0.00001 0.343
5 64 −5532.28 11,192.95 11,390.95 11,188.33 0.865 0.00001 0.749
6 75 −5514.38 11,178.76 11,411.25 11,173.80 0.873 0.00001 0.712
7 86 −5497.36 11,166.72 11,433.31 11,161.04 0.873 0.04 0.261

Note: Best fitting model is depicted in bold.


Abbreviations: AIC, Akaike information criterion; BIC, Bayesian information criterion; BLRT, Bootstrap Likelihood Ratio test; LL, log likelihood; LMR,
Lo-Mendell Rubin test.

F I G U R E 1 z-Scores for Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale, Dickman's Impulsivity Inventory and Eating Disorder Assessment Scale in
the five-class (on the left) and four-class (on the right) solution [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
6

TABLE 3 Means, SDs and differences in age, BMI, duration of eating disorder, EDAS, FMPS, and DII scores between the five classes in the mixed sample

Moderately High Purely Emotionally Behaviorally


ANOVA
impulsive (n = 23) functioning (n = 142) perfectionistic (n = 53) dysregulated (n = 16) dysregulated (n = 40)
Variables M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F(4,273) p η2
Age (years) 22.46 (8.67) 23.95 (7.97) 21.76 (6.69) 23.27 (8.16) 23.03 (6.25) 0.86 .490 0.013
BMI 21.17 (5.62) 20.33 (4.52) 21.07 (8.20) 22.81 (5.11) 21.78 (4.30) 1.14 .339 0.017
Duration of ED (years) 6.35 (8.28) 3.59 (4.23) 3.43 (3.26) 7.54 (4.19) 6.37 (5.24) 103.26(4,163) .004 0.735
EDAS
EDAS total 79.61 (20.92)b,e,**,d,* 30.10 (14.04)a,c,d,e,** 75.36 (16.03)b,d,e,** 98.25 (12.08)b,c,**,a,* 100.53 (16.73)a,b,c,** 247.84 <.001 0.787
b, a,c,d,e, b, b, b,
Restrained eating 23.70 (8.56) ** 10.75 (7.31) ** 25.91 (8.02) ** 29.50 (6.53) ** 26.23 (7.65) ** 71.29 <.001 0.515
Binge eating 22.52 (8.18)b,** 10.44 (5.96)a,c,d,e,** 19.15 (11.34)b,e,** 25.06 (13.66)b,** 28.48 (8.55)b,c,** 47.64 <.001 0.415
b,c,e, a,c,d,e, a,b,d,e, b,c,e, a,b,c,d,
Purging 10.65 (2.23) ** 0.31 (0.81) ** 2.19 (2.09) ** 10.06 (2.14) ** 17.40 (1.97) ** 1116.49 <.001 0.943
Preoccupation 22.74 (8.92)b,d,**,c,e,* 8.61 (5.84)a,c,d,e,** 28.11 (8.13)a,*,b,** 33.63 (6.34)a,b,** 28.43 (8.96)a,*,b,** 130.87 <.001 0.661
FMPS
Organization 18.87 (4.52) 19.42 (4.38) 21.34 (3.76) 21.88 (3.96) 19.95 (4.51) 3.12 .016 0.044
c,d,e, c,d,e, a, ,b, a, ,b, a, ,b,
Personal standards 14.17 (6.06) * 13.32 (7.06) ** 19.21 (6.76) * ** 23.00 (3.95) * ** 19.45 (7.15) * ** 49.25 <.001 0.189
Concern over mistakes 7.14 (4.93)c,d,**,e,* 5.06 (5.06)c,d,e,** 14.40 (7.05)a,b,**,d,* 21.00 (4.41)a,b,e,** 12.88 (7.44)b,d,**,a,* 15.65 <.001 0.424
e, ,d, d,e, ,c, a,b,d, a,b, ,c,e, a,d, ,b,
Parental criticism 5.52 (6.16) * ** 6.10 (6.26) ** * 10.17 (6.83) * 16.69 (7.91) ** * 11.10 (7.26) * ** 14.18 <.001 0.174
DII
DFI 17.78 (5.49) 14.06 (6.80)e,d,** 15.96 (7.35)e,* 21.31 (10.44)b,* 21.20 (7.94)b,**,c,* 10.12 <.001 0.132
c,d, b, b,
FI 24.39 (7.86) 25.00 (7.69) * 20.40 (9.11) * 17.75 (6.59) * 21.83 (7.59) 5.77 <.001 0.079

Note: p-values depicted are Sidak's adjusted p-values for multiple comparisons.
Abbreviations: η2, partial eta squared; AN-R, anorexia nervosa restricting; AN-BP, anorexia nervosa binge-eating/purging; BED, binge-eating disorder; BMI, body mass index; BN-BP, bulimia nervosa
binge-eating/purging; DFI, dysfunctional impulsivity; DII, Dickman's Impulsivity Inventory; EDAS, Eating Disorder Assessment Scale; preoccupation, preoccupation with body image and body weight; FI, func-
tional impulsivity; MPS, Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale; mistakes, concern over mistakes.
a
Statistically significant differences from moderately impulsive class.
b
Statistically significant differences from high functioning class.
c
Statistically significant differences from purely perfectionistic class.
d
Statistically significant differences from emotionally dysregulated class.
e
Statistically significantly differences from behaviorally dysregulated class.
*p < .05.
**p < .001.
SOIDLA AND AKKERMANN
SOIDLA AND AKKERMANN 7

frequency of ED and comorbid disorders between classes. Partial eta solution for both the whole sample as well as individuals with ED only
squared (η2) was used as an effect size statistic (rule of thumb for the are presented in Table 2. Based on fit indices and considering clinical
effect sizes: 0.01 small, 0.06 medium, 0.14 large). and theoretical meaningfulness, a five-class model was selected for
the whole sample and a four-class solution was selected for the indi-
viduals with ED only.
3 | RESULTS

3.1 | Demographics and descriptive statistics 3.3 | Statistical differences on indicator variables:
EDAS, FMPS, and DII
Descriptive statistics (age, BMI, duration of ED, as well as mean scores
of FMPS, DII, and EDAS) of individuals with AN-R, AN-BP, BN-BP, Both the five-class and the four-class solution are depicted in Figure 1.
BED, and controls are presented in Table 1. Table 3 presents the means and SDs for the indicators of profiles
as well as ANOVA results for the five-class solution and Table 4 pre-
sents the same indicators for the four-class solution (only individuals
3.2 | Latent profile analysis with ED).
The five classes were labeled as follows: “moderately impulsive,”
A series of one to seven profile models were estimated based on indi- “high functioning,” “purely perfectionistic,” “emotionally dysregulated,”
cator (FMPS, DII, EDAS) variables. Statistical fit indices of each and “behaviorally dysregulated.”

TABLE 4 Means, SDs and differences in age, BMI, duration of eating disorder, EDAS, FMPS, and DII scores between the four classes of
individuals with ED only sample

Moderately High Purely


impulsive functioning perfectionistic Dysregulated
ANOVA
(n = 37) (n = 40) (n = 55) (n = 32)
Variables M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F(3,163) p η2
Age (years) 22.32 (6.28) 22.45 (7.63) 21.86 (6.65) 23.45 (7.87) 0.33 .805 —
b, a,d, ,c, a, a,
BMI 22.15 (5.32) ** 16.72 (3.59) ** * 20.26 (7.31) * 22.30 (4.45) ** 8.01 <.001 0.128
Duration of ED (years) 5.84 (5.64) 4.01 (6.01) 3.54 (3.28) 7.37 (5.53) 4.40 .005 0.075
EDAS
EDAS total 88.70 (16.45)b,d,**,c,* 32.28 (19.22)a,c,d,** 75.44 (17.03)b,d,**,a,* 106.65 (11.99)a,b,c,** 133.03 <.001 0.710
b, ,d, a,c,d, b, a, ,c,
Restrained eating 23.38 (7.58) ** * 8.87 (8.21) ** 27.35 (7.55) ** 29.20 (6.23) ** * 60.44 <.001 0.527
Binge eating 26.11 (7.74)b,c,** 11.89 (8.69)a,d,**,c,* 17.71 (11.33)a,d,**,b,* 29.41 (9.73)a,b,** 25.32 <.001 0.318
Purging 14.60 (3.60)b,c,** 1.18 (2.41)a,d,**,c,* 3.16 (3.44)a,d,**,b,* 14.50 (4.21)a,b,** 172.21 <.001 0.760
b,c, a,c,d, b, ,d,
Preoccupation 24.62 (8.84) ** 10.35 (6.74) ** 27.22 (8.38) ** * 33.47 (6.32)a,b,**,c,* 60.44 <.001 0.527
FMPS
Organization 18.67 (4.71)c,* 19.28 (4.57)c,* 21.72 (3.55)a,b,* 21.19 (3.87) 5.30 .002 0.089
d, ,c, c,d, b, ,a, a,c,
Personal standards 15.22 (6.82) ** * 13.13 (9.05) ** 19.76 (6.28) ** * 23.22 (4.02) ** 16.19 <.001 0.229
Concern over 8.47 (5.68)c,d,** 5.08 (5.56)c,d,** 14.89 (6.87)b,a,**,d,* 18.69 (5.91)a,c,**,b,* 37.77 <.001 0.411
mistakes
Parental criticism 6.38 (5.69)d,**,c,* 4.82 (5.98)d,**,c,* 10.18 (6.94)d,**,b,a,* 16.94 (6.69)a,b,c,** 24.66 <.001 0.312
DII
DFI 20.38 (5.96)b,**,c,* 12.93 (6.72)a,d,** 15.98 (7.28)d,**,a,* 22.31 (9.23)a,**,b,* 12.56 <.001 0.188
FI 22.81 (7.44) 23.60 (7.94) 19.48 (8.51) 20.53 (7.17) 2.63 .052 0.046

Note: p-values depicted are Sidak's adjusted p-values for multiple comparisons.
Abbreviations: η2, partial eta squared; BMI, body mass index; DFI, dysfunctional impulsivity; DII, Dickman's Impulsivity Inventory; EDAS, Eating Disorder
Assessment Scale; FI, functional impulsivity; MPS, Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale; Preoccupation, preoccupation with body image and body weight.
a
Statistically significant differences from moderately impulsive class.
b
Statistically significant differences from high functioning class.
c
Statistically significant differences from purely perfectionistic class.
d
Statistically signficant differences from dysregulated class.
*p < .05.
**p < .001.
8 SOIDLA AND AKKERMANN

The “moderately impulsive” class was characterized by moderate “emotionally dysregulated class” had the highest maladaptive perfec-
level of dysfunctional and functional impulsivity, low levels of adap- tionism levels, low functional but high dysfunctional impulsivity and
tive and maladaptive perfectionism and moderate to high levels of all high levels of all ED symptoms. The “behaviorally dysregulated” class
ED symptoms. The “high functioning” class was characterized by the was characterized by moderate adaptive and maladaptive perfec-
lowest level of ED symptoms, low level of concern over mistakes, tionism, high dysfunctional impulsivity and the highest level of purg-
parental criticism, personal standards, dysfunctional impulsivity, and ing and binge eating.
high level of functional impulsivity. Individuals in the “purely perfec- Regarding the four-class solution for the individuals with ED only,
tionistic” class had high levels of both maladaptive and adaptive per- three classes strongly resemble the classes described above (i.e., high
fectionism and low dysfunctional and functional impulsivity. functioning, moderately impulsive, purely perfectionistic). The fourth
Regarding ED symptoms, more restrained eating and preoccupation class encompasses two classes from the five-class solution, namely
with body image and body weight were reported. Individuals in the the emotionally and the behaviorally dysregulated class. The class was

TABLE 5 Frequency of eating disorder diagnoses and comorbid psychopathology in the five-class and four-class solution

Individuals with ED and controls

Moderately High Purely Emotionally Behaviorally


impulsive functioning perfectionistic dysregulated dysregulated
Measures (n = 23) (n = 142) (n = 53) (n = 16) (n = 40)
ED diagnoses
AN-R n = 2 (8.7%) n = 24 (16.9%) n = 25 (47.2%) n = 1 (6.2%) n = 1 (2.5%)
AN-BP n = 3 (13.0%) n = 4 (2.8%) n=0 n = 2 (12.5%) n = 2 (5.0%)
BN-BP n = 16 n = 4 (2.8%) n = 12 (23.1%) n = 10 (62.5%) n = 37 (92.5%)
(69.9%)
BED n = 1 (4.3%) n = 1 (0.7%) n = 8 (15.5%) n = 3 (18.8%) n=0
Atypical-AN n=0 n = 5 (3.5%) n = 1 (1.9%) n=0 n=0
Controls n = 1 (4.3%) n = 103 (73.0%) n = 6 (11.5%) n=0 n=0
Comorbid psychopathology
Major depression n = 10 n = 11 (7.7%) n = 18 (34.0%) n = 11 (68.8%) n = 21 (52.5%)
(43.5%)
Any anxiety n = 8 (34.8%) n = 12 (8.5%) n = 21 (39.6%) n = 8 (50.0%) n = 14 (35.0%)
disorder
PTSD n=0 n=0 n=0 n = 3 (18.8%) n = 4 (10.0%)
Alcohol use disorder n = 4 (17.4%) n = 4 (2.8%) n = 5 (9.4%) n = 3 (18.8%) n = 13 (32.5%)
OCD n = 1 (4.3%) n = 2 (1.4%) n = 5 (9.4%) n = 1 (6.3%) n = 4 (10.0%)

Individuals with ED

Moderately impulsive (n = 37) High functioning (n = 40) Purely perfectionistic (n = 55) Dysregulated (n = 32)
ED diagnoses
AN-R n = 2 (5.4%) n = 23 (57.5%) n = 28 (52.8%) n=0
AN-BP n = 2 (5.4%) n = 3 (7.5%) n = 3 (5.5%) n = 3 (9.4%)
BN-BP n = 32 (86.5%) n = 6 (15.0%) n = 15 (27.3%) n = 26 (81.2%)
BED n = 1 (2.7%) n = 2 (5.0%) n = 8 (14.5%) n = 3 (9.4%)
Atypical-AN n=0 n = 5 (12.5%) n = 1 (1.8%) n=0
Comorbid psychopathology
Major depression n = 17 (45.9%) n = 12 (30.0%) n = 23 (41.8%) n = 19 (59.4%)
Any anxiety disorder n = 12 (32.4%) n = 13 (32.5%) n = 26 (47.3%) n = 12 (37.5%)
PTSD n = 1 (1.8%) n=0 n = 1 (2.7%) n = 5 (15.6%)
Alcohol use disorder n = 8 (21.6%) n = 4 (10.0%) n = 8 (21.6%) n = 9 (28.1%)
OCD n = 2 (5.4%) n = 2 (5.0%) n = 5 (9.1%) n = 4 (12.5%)

Note: The values given in the parentheses depict percentage of individuals within the same class.
Abbreviations: AN-R, anorexia nervosa restricting; AN-BP, anorexia nervosa binge-eating/purging; BED, binge-eating disorder; BN, bulimia nervosa
binge-eating/purging; OCD, obsessive compulsive disorder; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.
SOIDLA AND AKKERMANN 9

characterized by moderate to high perfectionism, high dysfunctional mixed sample (including individuals with ED and controls) and in a
impulsivity and high levels of all ED symptoms, and therefore named sample of individuals with ED only.
the class “dysregulated.” In the mixed sample, we found support for a five-class solution.
The classes were named as following: high functioning, purely perfec-
tionistic, moderately impulsive, emotionally dysregulated and behavior-
3.4 | Clinical features of the five-class and ally dysregulated. In the ED sample, we found support for a four-class
the four-class solution solution (high functioning, purely perfectionistic, moderately impulsive
and dysregulated). The latter classes were very similar in their response
Table 5 depicts the relationship between class membership and ED patterns and mean scores on indicator variables to the five-class solu-
and comorbid psychopathology for both the five- and the four-class tion in the mixed sample. The main difference between the two solu-
solutions. A chi-square test of independence was conducted between tions was the separation of the emotionally and the behaviorally
ED diagnoses and class membership. Significant differences emerged dysregulated class in the mixed sample.
among ED diagnostic categories and classes (χ 2[4] = 251.83, p < .001), The four-class solution is consistent with Boone et al. (2014)
the association was moderately strong (Cohen, 1988), Cramer's study, where similar four classes (resilient, purely impulsive, purely
V = 0.481. Post-hoc analyses using standardized residuals with (±1.96) perfectionistic, and a combined high perfectionism and high impulsiv-
indicating a difference in frequency of ED diagnoses were used. The ity class) were found in the sample of healthy adolescents. Another
majority of BN cases belonged to the moderately impulsive and both study conducted among individuals with ED also found support for a
emotionally and behaviorally dysregulated classes. A large proportion four-class solution, however, the classes were quite different (possibly
of BED cases belonged to the purely perfectionistic and emotionally due to not including ED symptoms in the LPA), for example, no
dysregulated class. There were more AN-BP cases in the moderately combined high perfectionism and high impulsivity class was found
impulsive and emotionally dysregulated class, but more AN-R cases in (Slof-Op't Landt et al., 2016).
the high functioning and purely perfectionistic class. Most of the con- Studies that have used a variety of personality traits have also
trols, as expected, belonged to the high functioning class. found support for a five-class model, for example, Thompson-Brenner
Significant differences also emerged among ED diagnostic catego- et al. (2008) found the following five classes: high-functioning, behav-
ries and classes for the individuals with the sample of individuals with iorally dysregulated, emotionally dysregulated, avoidant-insecure, and
ED (χ 2[3] = 79.90, p < .001, V = 403). The majority of BN cases belonged obsessional-sensitive. Our study also supports the distinction of the
to the moderately impulsive and the dysregulated class, most BED cases emotionally and the behaviorally dysregulated class. The importance
were in the purely perfectionistic class, most AN-R cases belonged to of this distinction is further discussed below.
the high functioning and the purely perfectionistic class, while more In general, the classes revealed important similarities with under-
atypical-AN cases belonged to the high functioning class. There were no controlled, overcontrolled and resilient/high-functioning classes
differences between the classes in frequencies on AN-BP cases. consistently found in several previous studies (Westen & Harnden-
Statistically significant differences emerged between class member- Fischer, 2001; Wonderlich et al., 2005). Three classes, which resem-
ship and frequency of comorbid anxiety disorder (χ 2[4] = 26.89, p < .001, bled the undercontrolled classes were identified in the mixed sample
V = 0.367), depression (χ 2[4] = 59.92, p < .0001, V = 0.468), alcohol use (moderately impulsive, emotionally dysregulated, behaviorally dys-
disorder (χ 2[4] = 31.68, p = .001, V = 0.340) and post-traumatic stress dis- regulated). The moderately impulsive class had higher dysfunctional
order (χ 2[4] = 31.48, p < .001, V = 0.339). Post-hoc analyses indicated that impulsivity, but low levels of perfectionism while the emotionally and
significantly more ED cases with comorbid depression belonged to mod- behaviorally dysregulated classes had both high levels of perfection-
erately impulsive, and both emotionally and behaviorally dysregulated ism and dysfunctional impulsivity. The emotionally dysregulated class
classes. The highest proportion of ED cases with comorbid anxiety disor- had the highest percentage of comorbid anxiety and mood disorders,
der belonged to the purely perfectionistic and emotionally dysregulated which might indicate that individuals in this class do not have adaptive
class. Most ED cases with comorbid alcohol use disorder belonged to the ways to cope with distress. In concordance with Thompson-Brenner,
behaviorally dysregulated class. The highest proportion of comorbid PTSD Eddy, Franko, et al. (2008), the behaviorally dysregulated class was
cases were in the emotionally and behaviorally dysregulated classes. characterized by multiple forms of impulsive behavior, evident in
For the sample of individuals with ED, significant differences the highest frequency of comorbid substance use disorders and the
emerged between class membership and frequency of comorbid PTSD highest scores on Purging subscale. Both the emotionally and the
(χ 2[3] = 12.91, p = .005, V = 0.281) (as expected, more PTSD cases behaviorally dysregulated classes had a higher percentage of PTSD
were in the dysregulated class) (Table 5). cases, consistent with the association between PTSD and high levels
of impulsive behavior (Tull, Weiss, & McDermott, 2016).
In the four-class solution, only one dysregulated class was found.
4 | DISCUSSION This may be due to lack of power in the sample of individuals with ED
only, which may have influenced LPA results by impeding finding
The purpose of our study was to find latent profiles based on facets small but significant classes. Nevertheless, we believe that the distinc-
of perfectionism and impulsivity as well as ED symptoms in both a tion between the two dysregulated classes is important—they differ in
10 SOIDLA AND AKKERMANN

levels of maladaptive perfectionism, functional impulsivity, comorbid The emergence of both high perfectionism and high impulsivity
psychopathology, and ED symptoms, all of which may influence both class found in both solutions is consistent with the finding that both
risk and maintaining factors of ED, appropriate treatment choice and overcontrolling and undercontrolling traits can characterize ED
treatment response. patients (Claes, Vandereycken, & Vertommen, 2002). As the duration
of ED was longer in the emotionally and behaviorally dysregulated
class than in the high functioning and the purely perfectionistic class,
4.1 | Diagnostic distribution in the five classes it may indicate that some patients might have belonged, in earlier
stages of illness, to the overcontrolled class, but crossed over to the
Most of the individuals with BN-BP and AN-BP split quite equally undercontrolled classes in a later stage of their disorder. Boone
between three classes characterized by higher impulsivity confirming et al. (2014) have proposed that these individuals have developed
the higher impulsivity levels of binge-eating/purging type of EDs high levels of restrained eating to control their impulsivity but over
(Waxman, 2009). A large proportion of individuals with BN and BED as time this strategy fails.
well as AN-R belonged to the purely perfectionistic class, indicating that
high levels of perfectionism are not confined to categorical diagnoses.
Interestingly, there was a substantial number of AN-R cases who 4.3 | Limitations and future directions
belonged to the high functioning class. This rather unexpected finding
is in concordance with Krug et al. (2011), who found that a high number This study also has several limitations to consider. First, though adding
of individuals with restrictive symptoms belonged to the “adaptive” ED symptoms in the analysis has several benefits (reducing bias
profile. This may imply that underweight, particularly purely restrictive associated with classify-analyze approach and accounting for the
individuals with ED minimize their symptoms (Eddy et al., 2009). Con- pathoplastic relationship between ED symptoms and personality), it
sistent with our hypothesis about the possible underreporting of symp- may also bring up two main problems: possibly finding more classes
toms, Wildes et al. (2011) reported that there was a high rate of than could have been found using only personality traits as indicators
readmission to hospital in the low psychopathology class. and the question of the possible instability of the profiles as ED symp-
toms tend to be more unstable than personality traits. In future stud-
ies, therefore, it would be worthwhile to run the analysis with and
4.2 | Perfectionism, impulsivity and eating disorder without using ED symptoms in the LPA.
symptoms Second, our study has a cross-sectional design, which does not
allow any causal conclusion to be made. Taking into account the two
Our results are in line with previous findings reporting that personal previously mentioned limitations, the assessment of the stability of
standards, concern over mistakes as well as parental criticism are the profiles by longitudinal studies would be needed. In addition, as
associated with disordered eating behavior (Dahlenburg et al., 2019; we have no prospective information (e.g., treatment response), we
Limburg et al., 2016). Personal standards, which has previously been cannot make any clear conclusions about the clinical utility of those
considered as an adaptive dimension (Lo & Abbott, 2013), differenti- classes. Therefore, future studies are well needed to study response
ated the latent profiles, while Organization seems not to be a good to treatment in the emerged classes. Previously it has been found,
subscale for differentiating between latent profiles. that compared to individuals in the high-functioning and the over-
Division of the concepts of dysfunctional and functional impulsiv- controlled class, individuals in the undercontrolled class had a less
ity also seems to be important in eating pathology. Functional successful response to treatment (Wildes et al., 2011). Presumably,
impulsivity is related to rapid information processing, being a more choosing treatments considering differences in multiple domains
complex process which involves considering alternatives quickly (e.g., personality traits and ED symptoms) can lead to improved patient
(Brunas-Wagstaff, Bergquist, Morgan, & Wagstaff, 1996; Smillie & care by informing the clinicians about the appropriate treatment inten-
Jackson, 2006), and is associated with lower neuroticism (Brunas- sity, setting, and type. For example, it might be that for the purely per-
Wagstaff, Bergquist, Richardson, & Connor, 1995), whereas dysfunc- fectionistic class cognitive-behavioral therapy targeting restrictive
tional impulsivity reflects difficulties of inhibiting competing informa- eating and maladaptive perfectionism would suit well, while for the dys-
tion sources and irrelevant material (Brunas-Wagstaff et al., 1996; regulated classes treatments targeting distress tolerance and emotion
Morgan & Norris, 2010). It seems that dysfunctional impulsivity itself regulation (e.g., dialectical-behavioral therapy) would fit better.
can be associated with some disordered eating symptoms, but the Thirdly, we only focused on dimensions of perfectionism, impul-
severity of those symptoms is influenced by the interplay between sivity and ED symptoms for classifying individuals. To address this lim-
impulsivity and other personality traits—in this case, perfectionism. In itation, future studies would benefit from combining psychological
our study, the moderately impulsive class had lower levels of adaptive and biological markers when examining profiles of ED patients to
and maladaptive perfectionism, but higher functional impulsivity com- strengthen the basis and get a more comprehensive picture of the dif-
pared to the dysregulated classes. Higher functional impulsivity, but ferent profiles in EDs.
also lower levels of perfectionism may help those individuals to act Lastly, as the class sizes are small to moderate and the sample is
more adaptively in stressful situations. biased toward young females, the replication of the classes is needed
SOIDLA AND AKKERMANN 11

in larger and more diverse samples (e.g., also in males, children and Boone, L., Claes, L., & Luyten, P. (2014). Too strict or too loose? Perfec-
teenagers, in different cultural environments, in outpatient settings, tionism and impulsivity: The relation with eating disorder symptoms
using a person-centered approach. Eating Behaviors, 15, 17–23.
using other measures to assess impulsivity, ED symptoms, etc.).
Boone, L., Soenens, B., Caroline, B., & Goossens, L. (2010). An empirical
typology of perfectionism in early-to-mid adolescents and its relation
with eating disorder symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48,
5 | C O N CL U S I O N S 686–691.
Brunas-Wagstaff, J., Bergquist, A., Morgan, K., & Wagstaff, G. F. (1996).
Impulsivity, interference on perceptual tasks and hypothesis testing.
Our results suggest that profiles based on perfectionism, impulsivity Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 471–482.
and ED symptoms can be meaningful for clinicians and researchers by Brunas-Wagstaff, J., Bergquist, A., Richardson, P., & Connor, A. (1995).
shedding light on the heterogeneity of possible risk as well as mainte- The relationships between functional and dysfunctional impulsivity
and the Eysenck personality questionnaire. Personality and Individual
nance factors in EDs. Methodologically, our study suggests that both
Differences, 5, 681–683.
controls and ED patients can be included in the LPA. Unlike previous Brunas-Wagstaff, J., Bergquist, A., & Wagstaff, G. F. (1994). Cognitive cor-
studies, we also included ED symptoms in the LPA and found that in relates of functional and dysfunctional impulsivity. Personality and Indi-
addition to the well-known high-functioning, overcontrolled and under- vidual Differences, 17, 289–292.
Claes, L., Robinson, M. D., Muehlenkamp, J. J., Vandereycken, W., &
controlled classes, two to three undercontrolled classes emerged—
Bijttebier, P. (2010). Differentiating bingeing/purging and restrictive
moderately impulsive, behaviorally dysregulated and emotionally
eating disorder subtypes: The roles of temperament, effortful control,
dysregulated class. Finally, as already stated by Kazdin (2007), individual and cognitive control. Personality and Individual Differences, 48,
differences among the patients need to be acknowledged and dealt 166–170.
with to improve patient care and as well as the cost-effectiveness of Claes, L., Vandereycken, W., & Vertommen, H. (2002). Impulsive and com-
pulsive traits in eating disordered patients compared with controls.
treatments. Therefore, for advancing the classification of EDs, future
Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 707–714.
studies need to focus on the clinical utility of those classes by studying Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New
the ability of those classes to predict clinical course of ED and inform York, NY: Routledge Academic.
about the most suitable treatment for the patient. Dahlenburg, S. C., Gleaves, D. H., & Hutchinson, A. D. (2019). Anorexia
nervosa and perfectionism: A meta-analysis. International Journal of
Eating Disorder, 52, 219–229.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Dickman, S. J. (1990). Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity: Personality
We are grateful to the participants for their time and effort in support and cognitive correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
of this study. We thank Dr Anu Järv, Kerttu Petenberg, Sheryl Võsu, 58, 95–102.
Eddy, K. T., Crosby, R. D., Keel, P. K., Wonderlich, S. A., le Grange, D.,
Elis Paasik, and nurses of eating disorder unit for their help in data col-
Hill, P., … Mitchell, J. E. (2009). Empirical identification and validation
lection. This research was a part of Kärol Soidla's doctoral thesis and of eating disorder phenotypes in a multisite clinical sample. The Journal
did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 197, 41–49.
commercial, or nonprofit sectors. Egan, S. J., Wade, T. D., & Shafran, R. (2011). Perfectionism as a trans-
diagnostic process: A clinical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 31,
203–212.
CONF LICT OF IN TE RE ST Farstad, S. M., McGeown, L. M., & von Ranson, K. M. (2016). Eating disor-
The authors declare no potential conflict of interest. ders and personality, 2004–2016: A systematic review and meta-anal-
ysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 46, 91–105.
Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions
DATA AVAI LAB ILITY S TATEMENT
of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449–468.
The data that supports the findings of this study are available on
Garner, D. M. (1991). Eating disorder inventory-2: Professional manual.
reasonable request from the corresponding author. The data are not Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.
publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions. Gibson, W. A. (1959). Three multivariate models: Factor analysis, latent
structure analysis, and latent profile analysis. Psychometrika, 24,
229–252.
ORCID Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., …
Kärol Soidla https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9286-760X Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new clas-
sification framework for research on mental disorders. American Jour-
Kirsti Akkermann https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1402-8365
nal of Psychiatry, 167, 748–751.
Kaye, W. H., Fudge, J. L., & Paulus, M. (2009). New insights into symptoms
RE FE R ENC E S and neurocircuit function of anorexia nervosa. Nature Reviews Neuro-
Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317–332. science, 10, 573–584.
Akkermann, K. (2010). Serotonin-related biomarkers and symptoms of eating Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanism of change in psychother-
disorders (Doctoral dissertation, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia). apy research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 1–27.
Retrieved from http://dspace.ut.ee/handle/10062/15300 Keel, P. K., Brown, T. A., Holland, L. A., & Bodell, L. P. (2012). Empirical
Bardone-Cone, A. M., Wonderlich, S. A., Frost, R. O., Bulik, C. M., classification of eating disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology,
Mitchell, J. E., Uppala, S., & Simonich, H. (2007). Perfectionism and 8, 381–404.
eating disorders: Current status and future directions. Clinical Psychol- Krug, I., Root, T., Bulik, C., Granero, R., Penelo, E., Jimenez-Murcia, S., &
ogy Review, 27, 384–405. Fernandez-Aranda, F. (2011). Redefining phenotypes in eating
12 SOIDLA AND AKKERMANN

disorders based on personality: A latent profile analysis. Psychiatry Thompson-Brenner, H., Eddy, K. T., Franko, D. L., Dorer, D.,
Research, 188, 439–445. Vashchenko, M., & Herzog, D. (2008). Personality pathology and sub-
Lanza, S. T., Tan, X., & Bray, B. C. (2013). Latent class analysis with distal stance abuse in eating disorders: A longitudinal study. International
outcomes: A flexible model-based approach. Structural Equation Journal of Eating Disorders, 41, 203–208.
Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 20, 1–26. Tull, M. T., Weiss, N. H., & McDermott, M. J. (2016). Post-traumatic stress dis-
Lilenfield, L. R. R., Wonderlich, S., Riso, L., Crosby, R., & Mitchell, J. (2006). order and impulsive and risky behavior: Overview and discussion of poten-
Eating disorders and personality: A methodological and empirical tial mechanisms. In C. Martin, V. Preedy, & V. Patel (Eds.), Comprehensive
review. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 299–320. guide to post-traumatic stress disorders. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.
Limburg, K., Watson, H. J., Hagger, M. S., & Egan, S. J. (2016). The relation- Turner, B. J., Claes, L., Wilderjans, T. F., Pauwels, E., Dierckx, E.,
ship between perfectionism and psychopathology: A meta-analysis. Chapman, A. L., & Schoevaerts, K. (2014). Personality profiles in eating
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 73, 1301–1326. disorders: Further evidence of the clinical utility of examining subtypes
Lo, A., & Abbott, M. J. (2013). Review of the theoretical, empirical, and based on temperament. Psychiatry Research, 219, 157–165.
clinical status of adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism. Behaviour Uusberg, H., Peet, K., Uusberg, A., & Akkermann, K. (2018). Attention
Change, 30, 96–116. biases in preoccupation with body image: An ERP study of the role of
Lo, Y., Mendell, N., & Rubin, D. (2001). Testing the number of components social comparison and automaticity when processing body size. Biolog-
in a normal mixture. Biometrika, 88, 767–778. ical Psychology, 135, 136–148.
McLachlan, G., & Peel, D. (2000). Finite mixture models. New York, NY: Vainik, U., Neseliler, S., Konstabel, K., Fellows, L. K., & Dagher, A. (2015).
Wiley. Eating traits questionnaires as a continuum of a single concept.
Morgan, K., & Norris, G. (2010). An exploration into the relevance of Uncontrolled eating. Appetite, 90, 229–239.
Dickman's functional and dysfunctional impulsivity dichotomy for Vermunt, J. K., & Magidson, J. (2002). Latent class cluster analysis. In
understanding ADHD-type behaviours. Individual Differences Research, J. A. Hagenaars & A. L. McCutcheon (Eds.), Applied latent class analysis
8, 34–44. (pp. 89–106). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2010). Mplus user's guide (6th ed.). Waxman, S. E. (2009). A systematic review of impulsivity in eating disor-
Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. ders. European Eating Disorders Review, 17, 408–425.
Oberski, D. L. (2016). Mixture models: Latent profile and latent class analy- Westen, D., & Harnden-Fischer, J. (2001). Personality profiles in eating dis-
sis. In J. Robertson & M. Kaptein (Eds.), Modern statistical methods for orders: Rethinking the distinction between axis I and axis II. American
HCI: A modern look at data analysis for HCI research. Cham, Switzerland: Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 547–562.
Springer. Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impul-
Peat, C., Mitchell, J. E., Hoek, H., & Wonderlich, S. (2009). Validity and util- sivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsiv-
ity of subtyping anorexia nervosa. The International Journal of Eating ity. Personality and Individuals Differences, 30, 669–689.
Disorders, 42, 590–594. Widiger, T. A. (2011). Personality and psychopathology. World Psychiatry:
Schwartz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association, 10, 103–106.
Statistics, 6, 461–464. Widiger, T. A., & Samuel, D. B. (2005). Diagnostic categories or dimensions?
Sclove, L. (1987). Application of model-selection criteria to some problems A question for the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika, 52, 333–343. disorders—Fifth edition. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 494–504.
Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., & Amorim, P. (1994). The Wildes, J. E., & Marcus, M. D. (2013). Alternative methods of classifying
Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I): The development eating disorders: Models incorporating comorbid psychopathology and
and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV associated features. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 383–394.
and ICD-10. Tampa, FL: University of South Florida College of Wildes, J. E., Marcus, M. D., Crosby, R. D., Ringham, R. M., Dapelo, M. M.,
Medicine. Gaskill, J. A., & Forbush, K. T. (2011). The clinical utility of personality
Slof-Op't Landt, M. C. T., van Furth, E. F., & Claes, L. (2016). Classifying subtypes in patients with anorexia nervosa. Journal of Consulting and
eating disorders based on “healthy” and “unhealthy” perfectionism and Clinical Psychology, 79, 665–674.
impulsivity. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 49, 673–680. Wonderlich, S. A., Crosby, R. D., Joiner, T., Peterson, C. B., Bardone-
Smillie, L. D., & Jackson, C. J. (2006). Functional impulsivity and reinforce- Cone, A., Klein, M., … Vrshek, S. (2005). Personality subtyping and
ment sensitivity theory. Journal of Personality, 74, 47–83. bulimia nervosa: Psychopathological and genetic correlates. Psychologi-
Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Caufmann, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., & cal Medicine, 35, 649–657.
Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsiv-
ity as indexed by behavior and self-report: Evidence for a dual systems
model. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1764–1778. How to cite this article: Soidla K, Akkermann K. Perfectionism
Thompson-Brenner, H., Eddy, K., Dorer, D., Franko, D., Vashchenko, M., &
and impulsivity based risk profiles in eating disorders. Int J Eat
Herzog, D. (2008). A personality classification system for eating
disorders: A longitudinal study. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 49, Disord. 2020;1–12. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23285
551–560.

You might also like