You are on page 1of 5

Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1990) 26:301-305

Behavioral Ecology
and Sociobiology
© Springer-Verlag1990

Homing pigeons do extract directional information


from olfactory stimuli
P. Ioal~, M. Nozzolini, and F. Papi
Dipartimento di Scienze del Comportamento Animale dell'Universit&di Pisa and Centro di Studio
per la Faunistica ed Ecologia Tropicali del C.N.R., Firenze, Italy

Received May 2, 1989 / Accepted November 9, 1989

Summary. Two groups of pigeons were kept from fledg- To explain how local odors can inform pigeons about
ing time in two cages fully exposed to winds. From time their position with respect to home, we assume that birds
to time, the cage containing experimentals was addition- acquire information about odors prevailing over large
ally exposed to an artificial air current coming from areas in their region by smelling wind-borne odors. At
a specific direction and carrying a scent of benzaldehyde. their loft, pigeons would then be able to associate wind-
When both groups were exposed to benzaldehyde scent borne odors with the direction from which they come,
during transportation and at the release site, the control thus building up their navigational map. When trans-
birds flew homeward, whereas the experimentals ori- ported and released from an unfamiliar site, the birds
ented in the direction roughly opposite that from which could determine the home direction if the odors smelt
they were used to perceiving the benzaldehyde at the during the outward journey and/or at the release site
loft. When benzaldehyde was not applied, experimental had already been perceived at the loft and associated
pigeons were homeward oriented like controls. with a specific direction. The home direction would be
the opposite one and the appropriate course would be
selected by means of the sun or magnetic compass (Papi
Introduction et al. 1972; Papi 1986). For a somewhat different hy-
pothesis on the pigeon olfactory map (the so-called gra-
The finding that homing pigeons are impaired in homing dient hypothesis), see Wallraff 1980, 1988.
from unfamiliar sites when prevented from smelling has According to our hypothesis, pigeons extract olfacto-
given rise to much subsequent work and a good deal ry information from olfactory stimuli; information of
of controversy. Two research teams, one at Pisa and a different nature is not assumed to contribute to the
one at Seewiesen, have repeatedly presented evidence navigation over completely unfamiliar areas. Though
that pigeons extract directional information from local much evidence supporting this hypothesis already exists
atmospheric odors perceived during the outward journey (see "Discussion"), we decided to test it further with
and at the release site (for reviews see Papi 1982, 1986; a new kind of experiment. In addition to the natural
Wallraff 1986). Other authors have either confirmed winds, pigeons were exposed to an artificial, scented air
these findings (Snyder and Cheney 1975; Fiaschi and current. The effect of sensing that scent during the out-
Wagner 1976; Kiepenheuer 1985) or are o f differing ward journey and at the release site was then tested on
opinions (Keeton 1980; Schmidt-Koenig 1987; Wiltsch- experimental pigeons and on controls that had not per-
ko and Wiltschko 1989). Schmidt-Koenig (1987) believes ceived that odor as a wind-borne odor. The results con-
that olfactory stimuli "seem to be necessary yet insuffi- firm that pigeons actually infer directional information
cient for homing unless another kind of information is from olfactory cues.
also available," whereas Wiltschko and Wiltschko
(1989) think that the old idea put forward by Keeton Methods
(1979 a) that anosmia might produce an aspecific distur-
bance in birds, for instance a demotivation to fly, has The pigeons and their treatment. The 160 pigeons used were bought
still to be considered. (For contrary evidence see Wallraff at the time of fledging. They arrived at Pisa on 1 June 1988 and
1979, 1983; Papi 1986). were randomly subdivided into two groups of 80 controls (C) and
80 experimentals (E). The two groups were housed in two wire-
Offprint requests to . F. Papi, Dipartimento di Scienze del Compor- netting cages 2.50 m x 2.50 m x 2.20 m high that were installed
tamento Animale dell'Universitfi, Via A. Volta 6, 1-56126 Pisa, Italy 100 m apart on the ground around our field station at Amino
302

340 ° ments, the air had to pass through a filter made of activated char-
coal (for details, see Wallraff and Foa' 1981). Flow rate in the
containers was about 210-240 1/min with filter, and 270-290 l/rain
without filter. The artificial ventilation inside the containers was
continued during the release. Care was taken to reduce air exchange
j..- between the environment inside and outside the container by taking
out pigeons singly through an elastic slit. Each pigeon was ~vith-
drawn 5 rain before release.
Four different treatments were applied during transportation
and release:

Treatment A. No filters were applied to the containers; therefore,


birds had access to environmental odors both during transporta-
tion and at the release site. They were not exposed to BA-odor.

Treatment B. A few drops of BA were injected into the tube convey-


ing air to the containers twice during the trip (15 and 7 km before
arrival), once on arrival and then every 30 rain at the release site.
Immediately after extraction from the container, each bird was
Fig. 1. Sketch showing the experimental loft during treatment soon painted on its ceres and beak with a solution (1:1) of BA
in liquid paraffin. The air flowing through the containers was unfil-
tered. Thus, the birds had access to environmental odors during
transportation and at the release site, but were exposed from time
(10 km SW of Pisa). The cages were oriented in the same way, to time to BA odor.
with a wall facing N N W (exactly 340°).
While in their cages, the birds were exposed to the natural Treatment C. The treatment was as in B, but the air flowing through
winds from all directions. Moreover, from time to time an artificial the containers was filtered. Thus, the birds could smell atmospheric
wind was directed at the cage of the E-birds by means of two odors at the release site in the 5 min prior to release and thereafter
big fans (E. Marelli, VA 51/63 NV90L4) located 4 m outside the and were exposed to BA odor from time to time.
wall facing N N W (Fig. I). The speed of the artificial wind mea-
sured inside the cage was 3.5-4 m/s. On the ground, 1 m from Treatment D. As in C, except that after extraction from the contain-
the front of the cage wall, facing the fans, were six dishes (total er, the birds were not painted with BA. Instead, a local anaesthetic,
upper surface 0.27 m 2) filled with a solution of benzaldehyde (BA) Xylocain, was sprayed into their nostrils to prevent olfaction (see
in liquid paraffin (1:4), which was changed once every day of Wallraff and Foa' 1981 for details). In this way the birds were
the treatment. Both compounds were pure; the function of liquid exposed to BA odor but not to atmospheric odors.
paraffin was to slow down evaporation of the BA. When the fans The same treatment was applied to both C- and E-birds in-
were on, the BA with its strong smell of bitter almonds was clearly volved in a single release. Treatment A was aimed to test homing
sensed by the human nose even beyond the aviary. In both cages behavior without exposing birds to BA. In treatments B-D, pigeons
perches were situated 40 cm from the ground; this arrangement were exposed to BA, while access to natural odors was progressive-
fully exposed experimental birds to the scented artificial wind. ly reduced.
Time and length of each treatment were decided by the experi- The treatment in a single experiment is indicated by the abbre-
menters according to the atmospheric situation. The fans were only viation for the release (AI and A2 releases were performed with
turned on during the daytime and only when there was no wind treatment A, BI-B3 with treatment B, etc). "Inexperienced birds"
or a wind gently blowing from N N W +40 °, (i.e., roughly from means that, except for training, the birds had never been released
the same direction in which the fans were located). Since the BA- before. For the exact distances and directions of the release sites
dishes were removed when the fans were off and the C-cage was see Figs. 2-4 and Table 1.
located to the West of the E-cage, C-birds were never exposed The following test releases were performed:
to BA-scented air currents. A I : from E, 8 September; inexperienced birds
Between the beginning of treatment (2 June) and the first test A2: from SSE, 23 September; veterans from B1
release (8 Sept), the E-birds were exposed to the BA-scented wind B1 : from E, 9 September; inexperienced birds
on 49 different days, for a total of 298 h. The treatment continued B2: from SSE, 15 September; inexperienced birds
until 27 Sept, the day prior to the last release test. On this day, B3: from NNW, 20 September; about half of the birds were
the treatment totalled 56 days (347 h). veterans from B2, the others from the first part of D1
C-birds were subjected neither to artificial winds nor odors. CI: from E, 12 September; inexperienced birds
Both C- and E-pigeons were always kept closed in their cages D1 : from SSE, 16 and 27 September; the release of 16 Sep-
except for one series of 3 consecutive days, in which free flights tember was interrupted owing to changing weather. About half
were allowed, and on the occasion of four training releases in of the birds were veterans from BI, the others from C1.
sight of the cages: 100 m S, 500 m W, 500 m E, and 800 m N. All the test releases were performed with the sun disc visible
When the test releases began, 58 C- and 59 E-birds were avail- and with light wind or none. The birds were tossed singly from
able. The odorant added to the artificial wind was chosen according the hand, alternating between the groups, and were followed with
to several criteria, such as cost, harmlessness, volatility, and the 10 x 50 binoculars until they disappeared from view. From the van-
birds' sensitivity to it. The threshold of pigeons to BA, as ascer- ishing bearings, the mean vector direction and length were calculat-
tained with the method of cardiac response, was found to be ap- ed for each group. Bearing sets were tested for randomness by
proximately 10-3.3 of vapor saturation (Walker et al. 1986). the Rayleigh test and compared by means of Watson's U 2 test;
clustering of bearings around the home direction or a predicted
Test releases. Procedures and treatment during transportation to direction was subjected to the V-test (Batschelet 1981). Both van-
and at the release site were exactly the same for both C- and ishing intervals and homing performances in the single experiments
E-birds. The birds were transported in air-tight aluminium contain- were compared using the Mann-Whitney U test (Siegel 1956).
ers (60 x 50 x 25 cm, up to 15 birds in a container) through which Arrivals at the loft were recorded continuously during the re-
ambient air was sucked by a vacuum cleaner. In some of the experi- lease day.
303

Table 1. Results of the single tests

Re- Date Home Home Group Birds released Mean Mean Homeward P P
lease distance direction and (bearings bearing vector component (V test) (U 2 test)
(km) recorded) length

A1 8 September 50.2 264° C 13 (12) 250° 0.948 +0.920 <0.001 N.S.


1988 E 14 (14) 272° 0.924 +0.916 <0.001
A2 23 September 54.8 336° C 12 (11) 316° 0.851 +0.797 <0.001 N.S.
1988 E 12(11) 334° 0.890 +0.890 <0.001
B1 9 September 50.2 264° C 15 (13) 246° 0.863 +0.823 <0.001 <0.001
1988 E 15 (12) 167° 0.684 -0.088 N.S.
B2 15 September 54.8 336° C 15 (14) 310° 0.849 +0.761 <0.001 <0.001
1988 E 15 (15) 193° 0.628 -0.500 N.S.
B3 20 September 40.4 161° C 15(13) 208° 0.806 +0.549 <0.01 N.S.
1988 E 15 (13) 187° 0.749 + 0.672 < 0.01
C1 12 September 50.2 264° C 15 (10) 262° 0.751 +0.750 <0.01 <0.001
1988 E 15 (10) 168° 0.678 -0.075 N.S.
D1 16+27 September 54.8 336° C 25 (21) 236° 0.233 - 0.042 N.S. < 0.05
1988 E 30 (22) 174° 0.529 -0.504 N.S.

N H N
H
4~ 2~2!,,,.~ ~_ ~3~3ak6~ ~
26
H
50.2k~nC ~
g~k°A ~
5 5 5 5
Fig. 2. Results of releases A1 and A2. Each circular diagram shows Fig. 4. Results of releases C1 and DI. Other explanations as in
the initial orientation of control (open dots) and experimental Fig. 2
(filled dots) pigeons. The outer filled arrow indicates home, the
direction and distance of which are given; inner arrows represent
mean vectors, the length of which can be read with the scale in
the first diagram. The outer open arrow indicates the direction t i o n ; the strongest deflection t o w a r d s the preferred com-
opposite to that from which the experimental birds were accus- pass direction (see " D i s c u s s i o n " ) was f o u n d in B3. De-
tomed to perceive the benzaldehyde odor at the loft
spite the changes in h o m e direction f r o m release to re-
lease, E - g r o u p s r o u g h l y o r i e n t a t e d t o w a r d s the same di-
rection in all B-releases. The m e a n directions were 167 °,
Results 193 ° , a n d 187 ° , all very close to the expected direction
(160°). I n release B3, where the h o m e direction a n d the
D a t a c o n c e r n i n g initial o r i e n t a t i o n are given in Table 1 expected direction for E - b i r d s coincided, E - b i r d o r i e n t a -
a n d Figs. 2 4 . Bearing d i s t r i b u t i o n always differed f r o m tion revealed a significant clustering o f bearings a r o u n d
r a n d o m , except C - b i r d s in D~ (Fig. 4). I n releases A1 h o m e d i r e c t i o n ; n o significant difference was f o u n d be-
a n d A2, b o t h g r o u p s were h o m e w a r d o r i e n t e d (Fig. 2). tween C- a n d E-bearings. I n BI a n d B2 the two g r o u p s
N o significant differences b e t w e e n t h e m were f o u n d . significantly differed in o r i e n t a t i o n .
I n releases B 1 - B 3 (Fig. 3), the b e a r i n g sets o f C-birds I n release C1 (Fig. 4), C-birds were h o m e w a r d ori-
showed a significant clustering a r o u n d the h o m e direc- ented, whereas E-birds flew close to the expected direc-

N H N N
54.8
km

Ho
~e4
50.k2m

5 5 H Fig. 3. Results of releases B1, B2, and B3. Other


5 explanations as in Fig. 2
304

tion. The two bearing sets significantly differed from the artificial wind, turned out to be of paramount impor-
each other. tance even in the presence of the odors which, in natural
In release D1 (Fig. 4), E-birds again orientated to- situations, guide the birds home.
wards the expected direction, whereas the C-bearings Exposure to BA before release deceived E-birds
did not differ from random. The two bearing distribu- about their position: they behaved as if they had been
tions significantly differed from each other. This release displaced in the direction from which BA scent arrived
was performed over 2 days; no significant difference was at their cage. They oriented in the same direction in
found between the data recorded in either of the 2 days. different locations: no influence of non-olfactory, locally
When exposed to benzaldehyde odor during the out- variable factors was detectable. Assuming that pigeons
ward journey and release (releases B1 B3, C1, D1), the have the same reaction to natural atmospheric odors
bearing sets of E-birds always showed a significant clus- as the E-birds with respect to BA, we can argue that
tering around the expected direction ( P < 0.01). olfactory information alone is sufficient to explain hom-
No differences in vanishing and homing times were ing ability. Matching with another kind of information,
found between the two groups. The homing perfor- as postulated by Schmidt-Koenig (1987), appears to be
mances of C- and E-birds were poor, with only 35% contrary to the principle of Ockham's razor.
of birds in each group homing on the day of the release. The lack of difference in homing performance be-
tween the groups can be explained by the transient effect
of treatment with BA prior to release. When the sub-
Discussion stance painted on the beak and ceres evaporated, the
birds did, in fact, have to rely on the natural olfactory
The main result of the present experiments is that E- cues, which correctly informed them about their posi-
birds, when exposed to BA before release, always flew tion. Moreover, the poor homing performance of both
southerly, independent of their position with respect to groups, which is attributable to the inadequancy of their
home. As a result, their orientation was divergent from flying experience and training, may have masked the
that of C-birds, unless the home direction was close to delaying effect of the wrong initial orientation in E-
S (test B3). This phenomenon must be regarded as an birds.
effect of their exposure in the cage to the BA-scented We conclude that pigeons infer directional informa-
wind, since C-birds, which were not exposed to such tion from olfactory stimuli perceived during the outward
a wind, were not affected by BA in their orientation. journey and/or at the release site. This is consistent with
C-birds were also homeward oriented in test C1, in previous results, particularly those of an experiment by
which access to atmospheric odors was permitted at the Papi et al. (1974) in which pigeons were kept in corridor-
release site only, a treatment that when combined with lofts and exposed to artificially scented air currents. The
application of e-pinene on the beak can produce disor- application of the same scent at the time of release in-
ientation (Benvenuti et al. 1977). fluenced the initial orientation in the manner predicted.
In some releases, C-birds showed a tendency to make In that case, however, the birds were protected from
a compromise between the homeward direction and the exposure to natural winds, and their ability to orientate
preferred compass direction (PCD). In our Amino birds, by means of natural atmospheric odors was not tested.
PCD is apparently somewhat variable with time; recent- When taking the consistency of these results with the
ly it was roughly SW. This is the direction often chosen present ones, one can exclude E-bird behavior being a
by Arnino anosmic birds; however, they are sometimes simple PCD-like reaction and not the consequence of
completely disoriented (see for instance both results in associating an odorant with its direction of provenience.
Wiltschko et al. 1986). This latter behavior was dis- More generally, the present results are in agreement
played in the present experiment by C-birds deprived with those of the experiments that had aimed to test
of any olfactory information on the natural environment the olfactory hypothesis by alterating or preventing the
(test D1). association between wind-borne odors and wind direc-
Like controls, E-pigeons developed a normal homing tion (see Papi 1986 for a review). Among them, the ex-
ability. In fact, when not exposed to BA prior to release periment of Foa' et al. (1986), which directly involved
(test A1, A2), they were homeward oriented and did olfaction, is particularly revealing. Similarly, the recent
not differ from the controls. This shows that the artificial experiments of site stimulation by atmospheric odors
air current per se, i.e., as mechanical stimulus, did not (Benvenuti and Wallraff 1985; Kiepenheuer 1985, 1986;
produce any significant modification in homing behav- see also Papi 1986 for comments) are consistent with
ior. the results mentioned above, and the most economic
One could expect E-birds, when exposed both to nat- way of explaining them is that of assuming that olfactory
ural odors and BA (treatments B and C), to make a cues can convey directional information.
compromise between the home direction and the direc- Some authors (e.g., Keeton 1979b; Wiltschko and
tion opposite that of the BA-scented wind. This was Wiltschko 1989; Waldvogel 1989) state that different
not the case, as the tendency was to fly in the second rearing and training procedures, or genetic differences
direction only, just as in test D1 when pigeons could between pigeon stocks, or even regional differences in
only smell BA-odor. Probably the BA-odor, having been the availability of navigational cues may determine
repeatedly and intensely perceived in association with which cues pigeons use to home. Therefore, the conclu-
305

sion we d r a w f r o m the present experiment w o u l d n o t Int Ornithol (•978). Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York,
apply to pigeons o f a n y area or loft, as some o f t h e m pp 137-157
m a y rely on n o n - o l f a c t o r y cues. We note however that Kiepenheuer J (•985) Can pigeons be fooled about the actual re-
lease site position by presenting them information from another
(/) a n y a t t e m p t to show the existence o f a " m a p sense"
site? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:75-82
alternative to the olfactory one failed, (2) pigeons o f Kiepenheuer J (•986) Are site-specific airborne stimuli relevant
A m e r i c a n and Italian stocks, reared together in Italy, for pigeon navigation only when matched by other release-site
were equally affected by olfactory deprivation (Benvenu - information? Naturwissenschaften 73:42-43
t i e t al. 1989), (3) Italian pigeons, m a d e anosmic early Papi F (1982) Olfaction and homing in pigeons: ten years of experi-
in development do n o t acquire the ability to use cues ments. In: Papi F, Wallraff HG (eds) Avian navigation. Sprin-
ger, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 149-159
alternative to those p r o v i d e d by olfaction (Papi et al.
Papi F (1986) Pigeon navigation: solved problems and open ques-
1989), and (4) according to new experiments on Cornell tions. Monit Zool Ital (Nuova Serie) 20:471-517
pigeons, a n o s m i a reduces h o m e w a r d orientation to a Papi F, Fiore L, Fiaschi V, Benvenuti S (•972) Olfaction and hom-
nonsignificant residual tendency, regardless o f training ing in pigeons. Monit Zool Ital (Nuova Serie) 6:85-95
and rearing procedures (Benvenuti and Brown, in press). Papi F, Ioale' P, Fiaschi V, Benvenuti S, Baldaccini NE (1974)
All this reduces the likelihood o f the a b o v e - m e n t i o n e d Olfactory navigation of pigeons: the effect of treatment with
assertion being correct. odorous air currents. J Comp Physiol 94:•87-•93
Papi F, Gagliardo A, Fiaschi V, Dall'Antonia P (1989) Pigeon
homing: does early experience determine what cues are used
Acknowledgements. The authors are grateful to Camillo Padoa to navigate ? Ethology 82:208-215
Schioppa for his help during the treatment of the birds. The re- Schmidt-Koenig K (•987) Bird navigation: has olfactory orienta-
search was partially supported by the Ministero della Pubblica tion solved the problem? Q Rev Biol 62:31-47
Istruzione. Siegel J (1956) Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sci-
ences. McGraw-Hill, New York
Snyder RL, Cheney CD (1975) Homing performance of anosmic
References pigeons. Buli Psychon Soc 6:592-594
Waldvogel JA (•989) Olfactory orientation by birds. In: Power
Batschelet E (1981) Circular statistics in biology. Academic Press, DM (ed) Current ornithology, vol 6. Plenum Press, New York,
London pp 269-321
Benvenuti S, Brown AI (•990) The influence of olfactory depriva- Walker JC, Walker DB, Tambiah CR, Gilmore KS (•986) Olfactory
tion on experienced and inexperienced American pigeons. and non-olfactory odor detection in pigeons: elucidation by
Behaviour (in press) a cardiac accelerator paradigm. Physiol Behav 38 : 575-580
Benvenuti S, WallraffHG (•985) Pigeon navigation: site simulation Wallraff HG (•979) Olfaction and homing in pigeons. Natur-
by means of atmospheric odours. J Comp Physiol [A] 156: 737- wissenschaften 66 : 269
746 Wallraff HG (1980) Oifaction and homing in pigeons: nerve-section
Benvenuti S, Fiaschi V, Foa' A (•977) Homing behaviour of pi- experiments, critique, hypotheses. J Comp Physiol 139:209-224
geons disturbed by application of an olfactory stimulus. J Comp Wallraff HG (1983) Relevance of atmospheric odours and geomag-
Physiol 120 : 175 179 netic field to pigeon navigation: what is the "map basis"?
Benvenuti S, Fiaschi V, Foa' A, Ioale' P, Papi F (•989) The acquisi- Comp Biochem Physiol 76A : 643-663
tion of the olfactory map in homing pigeons. In: RIN •989 Wallraff HG (1986) Relevance of olfaction and atmospheric odours
Orientation and navigation - Birds, humans and other animals, to pigeon homing. In: Beugnon G (ed) Orientation in space.
paper no. 17, The Royal Institut of Navigation, London Privat, Toulouse, pp 7•-80
Fiaschi V, Wagner G (1976) Pigeons' homing: some experiments Wallraff HG (•988) Navigation mit Duftkarte und Sonnenkom-
for testing the olfactory hypothesis. Experientia 32:99•-993 pass: das Heimfindevermoegen der Brieftauben. Naturwissen-
Foa' A, Bagnoli P, Giongo F (1986) Homing pigeons subjected schaften 75: 380-392
to section of the anterior commissure can build up two olfactory Wallraff HG, Foa' A (1981) Pigeon navigation: charcoal filter re-
maps in deflector lofts. J Comp Physiol [A] 159:465-472 moves relevant information from environmental air. Behav
Keeton WT (1979a) Avian orientation and navigation. Annu Rev Ecol Sociobiol 9 : 67-77
Physiol 41 : 353-366 Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W (1989) Pigeon homing: olfactory orien-
Keeton WT (1979b) Pigeon navigation. In: Granda AM, Maxwell tation - a paradox. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 24:163-173
JH (eds) Neural mechanisms of behavior in the pigeon. Plenum Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W, Foa' A, Benvenuti S (•986) Orientation
Publishing Corporation, London New York, pp 5-20 behaviour of pigeons deprived of olfactory information during
Keeton WT (1980) Avian orientation and navigation: new develop- the outward journey and at the release site. Monk Zool Ital
ments in an old mystery. In: Noehring E (ed) Acta XVII Congr (Nuova Serie) 20:183-193

You might also like