You are on page 1of 43

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/43532482

Profiling oral narrative ability in young school-aged children

Article  in  International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology · June 2010


DOI: 10.3109/17549500903194125 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
56 2,457

2 authors:

Marleen F Westerveld Gail Gillon


Griffith University University of Canterbury
122 PUBLICATIONS   1,836 CITATIONS    105 PUBLICATIONS   2,834 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Longtitudinal Study of Australian Students with Autism (LASA) View project

Effectiveness of early storybook reading for babies with and without a hearing loss. View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Marleen F Westerveld on 10 July 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Profiling Oral Narrative Ability in Young School-aged Children

Marleen F. Westerveld

Gail T. Gillon

College of Education

University of Canterbury

Contact Details

Marleen F. Westerveld, PhD

School of Literacies and Arts in Education

College of Education

University of Canterbury

Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New Zealand

Email: marleen.westerveld@canterbury.ac.nz

Citation

Westerveld, M. F., & Gillon, G. T. (2010). Profiling oral narrative ability in young school-aged

children. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12(3), 178-189. doi:

10.3109/17549500903194125

1
Abstract

This study aimed to determine if oral narrative comprehension and production measures derived

in a fictional story retelling task could be used to create a profile of strengths and weaknesses in

oral narrative ability (PONA) in young school-aged children. The story retelling task was field-

tested with 169 typically developing children, aged between 5;0 and 7;6 years. Children listened

twice to an unfamiliar story while looking at the pictures in a book. Comprehension questions

were asked after the first exposure. Following the second exposure, children were asked to retell

the story without the use of the pictures. Story retellings were analysed on measures of

semantics, morphosyntax, verbal productivity, and narrative quality. Results indicated sensitivity

for age on measures of comprehension, narrative quality, semantics, and verbal productivity, but

not for morphosyntactic measures. Factor analysis indicated that oral narrative performance

comprised three factors, explaining more than 80% of the variance. Two clinical case examples

are presented, which show the potential of the PONA to reveal different patterns of strengths and

weaknesses across the oral narrative measures. Although early evidence suggests the potential

usefulness of the PONA, further research is now needed to test the validity, reliability and

clinical application of this tool.

Key words: oral narrative skills, language sampling analysis, school-age children

2
Profiling Oral Narrative Ability in Young School-aged Children

Introduction

In the last decade, researchers and clinicians have become increasingly interested in finding ways

in which children’s oral narrative abilities can be described in order to assist clinicians in making

clinical judgments about their students’ oral narrative performance (e.g., Bliss, McCabe, &

Miranda, 1998; Justice et al., 2006; Petersen, Gillam, & Gillam, 2008; Strong, 1998). This

interest stems from research indicating that the ability to understand and produce oral narratives

is strongly linked to academic performance, particularly with respect to reading comprehension

(e.g., Feagans & Appelbaum, 1986). Clinical assessment tools can be divided into standardised

tests and criterion-referenced procedures. Although standardised tests of oral narrative ability

will determine if a child functions significantly below expectations compared to his or her peers

(e.g, Bishop, 2004; Gillam & Pearson, 2004), in general these tests do not yield the detail in oral

narrative comprehension or production performance that is needed for intervention. In contrast,

criterion-referenced procedures provide the clinician with detail about a child’s oral narrative

performance (e.g., Applebee, 1978; Justice et al., 2006; Petersen et al., 2008). Ideally, both

comprehension and production of oral narratives should be addressed (Boudreau, 2008) and

macro- as well as microstructural aspects of performance should be included (Liles, Duffy,

Merritt, & Purcell, 1995). The current study, therefore, examined if a story retelling task that was

developed as part of a language sampling protocol (which also included elicitation of

conversation and personal narratives, see Westerveld, Gillon, & Miller, 2004, for a copy of the

protocol) would yield descriptive data in all these aspects of oral narrative performance to

provide the clinician with a profile of a child’s strengths and weaknesses in oral narrative ability.

3
The importance of oral narrative skills for academic achievement

Research supports the predictive and concurrent relationship between aspects of oral narrative

ability and academic performance (e.g., Feagans & Appelbaum, 1986). Moreover, studies of oral

narrative performance in children with language and/or reading disabilities have revealed

weaknesses in these children’s oral narrative ability when compared to chronologically age-

matched and/or reading-age matched peers (Feagans & Short, 1984; Snyder & Downey, 1991;

Westerveld, Gillon, & Moran, 2008). It is therefore not surprising, that general consensus exists

that oral narrative analysis should play a pivotal role in the speech-language pathologist’s

assessment and intervention practices (Boudreau, 2008; Johnston, 2008). Apart from

standardised testing to confirm or refute language impairment, international best practice

recommends the use of spontaneous language sampling, not only to support standardised test

results but also to provide the clinician with the linguistic detail needed for intervention and

monitoring purposes (Gillon, Moriarty, & Schwarz, 2006; Miller, 1996). Although a plethora of

guidelines have been published on how to elicit and analyse oral narrative samples (see Hughes,

McGillivray, & Schmidek, 1997), local field-test data are urgently needed to allow the clinician

to determine a child’s performance relative to his or her age-matched peers.

Assessment of oral narrative ability

Oral narrative ability can be assessed in two modalities (comprehension and production) and

analysed at two levels (macrostructure and microstructure levels). From a clinical perspective, a

child needs adequate skills in all of these aspects to meet the ever increasing higher-level

discourse demands of the school curriculum. Children are exposed to narratives on a daily basis;

They are required to listen to and understand the teacher’s narrative explanations, are expected to

4
share personal stories and re/tell fictional stories, and are exposed to written narratives that

increase in complexity as children progress through the reading levels (Milosky, 1987).

Theoretical perspectives on the links between oral narrative ability and reading comprehension

confirm the need for evaluation of oral narrative ability across domains and modalities (Graesser,

Singer, & Trabasso, 1994; Kintsch, 1988). It is thus proposed that analysis of oral narrative

ability may be used to obtain important information about a child’s spoken language abilities that

are deemed important to academic participation and reading comprehension (see also Cain,

Oakhill, & Bryant, 2004). More specifically, analysis of oral narrative comprehension ability will

help determine the child’s ability to understand text-level language (e.g., Gough & Tunmer,

1986). Analysis of oral narrative production ability at microstructure level will potentially

provide an indication of the child’s strengths and weaknesses in the spoken language domains of

semantics, syntax, and morphology, whereas macrostructure analysis of oral narrative production

ability will provide insight into the child’s ability to apply narrative structure knowledge and

produce a coherent narrative that contains all the important story grammar elements (e.g., see

Hughes et al., 1997).

Oral narrative comprehension

Procedures used for assessing oral narrative comprehension include asking the child to act out

action units contained in a narrative (Feagans & Short, 1984), asking the child comprehension

questions following exposure to a story (and prior to retelling that story) (Norbury & Bishop,

2002; Westerveld et al., 2004), or asking comprehension questions immediately after a child has

retold a story (Liles, 1987; Snyder & Downey, 1991; Wagner, Sahlen, & Nettelbladt, 1999).

Although some researchers support the use of story retell to assess a child’s narrative

understanding (e.g., Skarakis-Doyle & Dempsey, 2008), others argue that comprehension and

5
production of oral narratives may be distinct skills (Wagner et al., 1999). Even though both

methods rely to some extent on the child’s productive language abilities, it can be argued that

answering questions may yield more specific information about the child’s receptive abilities

than a story retelling task. A retelling task is more complex, as it not only involves the memory

processes underlying successful comprehension but also entails a semantic and morphosyntactic

component (see Mar, 2004).

Merritt and Liles (1989) suggested using comprehension questions following a story

retelling to identify if factors other than expressive spoken language skills influenced the child’s

oral narrative production performance. The advantage of asking comprehension questions prior

to the story retelling task is, however, that a more immediate measure of oral narrative

comprehension is obtained (i.e., which has not been affected by a child’s difficulty to store

information in short-term memory and/or which has not been influenced by the child’s attempt to

retell the story). Furthermore, previous research has shown high correlations between story

comprehension and the ability to tell the story afterwards both in children with language

impairment and in children with typically developing skills (Norbury and Bishop, 2002).

Oral narrative elicitation: context and conditions

Both story generation and story retelling tasks are used in the research literature to elicit oral

narrative production skills. Examples of generation tasks include telling a story about a single

picture (Justice et al., 2006; Ripich & Griffith, 1988), completing a story started by the examiner

(Merritt & Liles, 1989), and creating a story about something that is not real (Roth & Spekman,

1986). Story retelling tasks can be described as either spontaneous (e.g., retelling a familiar story

or a TV programme) or more directed. Directed tasks include story productions after movie

viewings (Gummersall & Strong, 1999; Liles, 1985) and story retelling, with or without pictures,

6
after listening to a story (with or without pictures) (Ripich & Griffith, 1988; Schneider, 1996).

Merritt and Liles (1989) found clear advantages to using a story retelling task rather than a story

generation task for assessing oral narrative production abilities. Results indicated that retold

narratives were generally longer and contained more story grammar components and complete

episode structures than story generations. In addition, generated stories were often confusing to

the listener and, therefore, difficult to score at macrostructure level.

When asking a child to retell a story, both the quality and the length of the resulting oral

narrative will depend on the elicitation tasks and the sampling conditions (Schneider, 1996).

Factors that may influence task performance include the child’s perception of the examiner’s

familiarity with the model story (Masterson & Kamhi, 1991), the number of exposures to the

model story (Gummersall & Strong, 1999), the contextual support for the story (e.g., single

pictures, picture books, see Masterson & Kamhi, 1991), and the linguistic complexity and length

of the model story (e.g., Holloway, 1986). After careful evaluation of the literature, Westerveld

and Gillon (1999/2000) concluded that optimal sampling conditions for eliciting fictional oral

narratives in school-age children (with and without spoken language disorders) involved:

• A listener who is unfamiliar with the story the child is retelling (Liles, 1985; Masterson &

Kamhi, 1991). For example, Masterson and Kamhi demonstrated that children retell

longer, grammatically more complex stories to naive listeners.

• Two exposures to the model story (Gummersall & Strong, 1999). The results from

Gummersall and Strong’s study indicated that a more informative oral narrative language

sample (containing more words and more complex syntactic structures) may be obtained

if a child listens to a model story twice, before being asked to retell the story.

7
• A retell of the story without the support of picture-sequences (Masterson & Kamhi,

1991). Masterson and Kamhi found that children’s story retelling samples that were

collected without the support of picture books contained more compound sentences,

higher lexical accuracy, and better fluency than those obtained in a picture supported

condition. In contrast, grammatical accuracy decreased in the absence of picture books.

• Stimulus materials that contain levels of linguistic complexity that are similar to, or more

advanced than the child’s own (Griffith, Ripich, & Dastoli, 1986; Holloway, 1986).

Oral narrative production measures

Consideration needs to be given to the type of measures that are included when investigating a

child’s oral narrative performance. Some recent research studies investigating clinical

applications of scoring systems have focused on relatively complex measures; one example is the

Index of Narrative Microstructure (INMIS) by Justice et al. (2006) that is an aggregated score of

a child’s performance at microstructure level. Another example is the Index of Narrative

Complexity (Petersen et al., 2008) that assigns scores to a range of categories related to oral

narrative performance, including macro- and microstructural aspects. For the current profile of

narrative ability it was decided to use transparent, frequently used microstructure measures of

oral narrative performance that are known to correlate significantly with advancing age and to be

sensitive to disordered language performance (e.g., Botting, 2002; Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999;

Fey, Catts, Proctor-Williams, Tomblin, & Zhang, 2004; Watkins, Kelly, Harbers, & Hollis,

1995). These measures include mean length of utterance, number of different words and

grammatical accuracy. However, consistent with previous studies, to capture the quality of the

oral narrative, a total score will be assigned that covers a range of characteristics such as story

8
grammar elements, theme, and coherence (e.g., Fey et al., 2004; Miller, Heilmann, & Nockerts,

2006).

The current study

The current study aimed to determine if a Profile of Oral Narrative Ability (PONA), based on

measures derived from a story retelling task, has clinical utility with young school-aged children.

The story retelling task was field-tested with 169 typically developing children, aged between

5;0 and 7;6. Children were asked comprehension questions following the first exposure to the

story, and story retelling samples were analysed at macrostructure and microstructure levels.

Method

Participants

The 169 children in this study were participants in the New Zealand database project (see

Westerveld et al., 2004). All story retelling transcripts from children who were aged between 5;0

and 7;6 were included in the current study. The children were randomly selected from

mainstream primary schools in major urban areas in New Zealand. Children with diagnosed

disorders such as autism, Down syndrome, and cerebral palsy were excluded prior to the random

selection. The schools reflected a range of socio-economic areas. Only children who spoke

English as their first language and who had no history of sensory deficits or neurological

disorder were included in the study. There were slightly more girls than boys (ranging from 53%

to 56 % per age-group). The ethnicity of the groups resembled the largest cultural groups in New

Zealand (Ministry of Education, 1998) and comprised the following subcategories: New Zealand

European (61.5%), Maori (20.7%), Pacific Island (4%), Asian (3%), and Other (9.8%). Ethnicity

was based on the cultural groups the parents had identified on their child’s school-enrolment

form. Details on the age and gender distribution of the participants are shown in Table I.

9
Insert Table I about here

Procedures

Elicitation procedures. All testing was conducted by certified speech pathologists working for

Group Special Education (New Zealand Ministry of Education). These clinicians were trained by

the first author on the elicitation procedures. Each child was seen individually in the child’s

school setting and was administered the full Westerveld & Gillon Language Sampling Protocol

(LSP). The original protocol included elicitation of spoken language in three conditions:

conversation, personal narratives, and story retelling. The protocol has been included in

Westerveld et al. (2004) or can be downloaded from:

http://www.education.canterbury.ac.nz/documents/gillon/

languageprotocol.pdf. In addition, a New Zealand speech and language screening test (Gillon &

Schwarz, 1998) was administered. Any child who performed very poorly on the receptive

language screening task (i.e., who could not follow basic instructions) was excluded from the

database study. Children’s language samples were excluded from the database for reasons such

as poor taping quality or not engaging in the task (i.e., unwilling to retell the story).

The data presented in the current study relate to the story retelling condition of the

language sampling protocol. In this context, the child was required to listen twice to an audio-

recording of an unfamiliar story (while looking at the pictures in a story book). The story was an

English translation of Ko au na Galo [Ana gets lost] (Swan, 1992). The story is about a Pacific

Islands girl who gets lost in the city, while looking for her mum and dad. It is a 10-page reader of

the type typically used in Year 1 and Year 2 New Zealand classrooms, with coloured pictures

and Tokelauan text. The original translation of Ko au na Galo was adapted slightly to add a little

further length and complexity to the story. A transcript of the story is presented in Appendix A.

10
Following the first exposure to the story, the child was asked eight questions about the

story to evaluate story comprehension. To ensure all children had access to the same information

prior to the children listening to the story for a second time, children were provided with the

correct information after answering the questions if they did not respond to the question, or if

their answers were clearly incorrect. This method was used to make sure that if children

generated weak stories, they were likely to be the result of the children’s difficulty in applying

story structure knowledge when retelling the story, rather than the result of the children’s failure

to remember and/or pay attention to important elements of the story. Following the second

exposure to the story, the child was asked to retell the story without the use of pictures.

Transcription procedures. All language samples were tape-recorded. Samples were transcribed

by speech pathology students who were trained in the standard Systematic Analysis of Language

Transcripts – New Zealand version (SALT-NZ) transcription conventions (Miller, Gillon, &

Westerveld, 2008). Utterance segmentation was based on communication units (CU), using

Loban's (1976) rules. Defined by Loban, a CU consists of ‘‘each independent clause with its

modifiers and associated independent clauses, or of an answer to a question lacking only the

repetition of the question elements to satisfy the criterion of independent predication’’ (p. 9).

Only complete and intelligible (C&I) utterances were used for analysis; Interrupted and

abandoned sentences were excluded as well as utterances containing unintelligible segments.

Measures

Oral narrative comprehension (ONC). Each child was asked eight questions following the first

exposure to the story. ONC was calculated as a percent questions correct score. The questions

are listed in Appendix B.

11
Microstructure. Only quantitative measures of oral narrative ability that are relatively easy to

compute by clinicians, that have been shown to distinguish between children with spoken

language impairment and children with typical development, and that are likely to change with

age or ability were included: number of different words (NDW, see Miller, 1996; Watkins et al.,

1995), total number of utterances (UTT), mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLU-M, see

Brown, 1973), and grammatical accuracy (percent grammatically correct utterances: GA, see Fey

et al., 2004).

Macrostructure. The full story retelling transcripts were used and analysed at macrostructure

level. To evaluate the child’s ability to apply story structure knowledge when retelling a story,

the story retellings were scored on a story quality rubric. The rubric was adapted from Jones and

Lodholz (1999) and assessed inclusion of six text structure elements: introduction, main

character/s, supporting character/s, conflict, resolution, and conclusion, as well as a measure of

holistic coherence. It also investigated whether the child included the theme of the story. Theme

is defined as ‘‘the overall coherent topic of the text and its essential points’’ (Westby, 2005, p.

162). For more details or to obtain a copy of the rubric used in the current study, please contact

the first author. The child was awarded points for each characteristic: 5 points if the child

proficiently included the characteristic, 3 points if the skill was emerging, and 1 point if the child

provided minimal or no information. The rubric includes specific scoring examples to promote

easy and reliable scoring by other examiners. The scores were totalled to yield an Oral Narrative

Quality (ONQ) score. As a result, the minimum score was 8 and the maximum score was 40.

Reliability

12
Accuracy of transcription and coding of the original stories was reported in detail in Westerveld

et al. (2004). For the present study, all samples were re-checked by the first author and any

transcription or coding errors that were found were corrected.

For the macrostructure analysis, 20% of the transcripts were randomly selected and

scored by an independent examiner who was trained in scoring the stories on the ONQ rubric.

Reliability of the total scores on the ONQ rubric, using Cronbach’s alpha analysis, was 0.96. The

total scores (of all individual stories) awarded by the two examiners differed by 4 points or less.

For individual characteristic scores, the scores never differed more than 2 points (i.e., one

category).

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Table II reports the means and standard deviations by age group on the six measures of oral

narrative ability: oral narrative comprehension (ONC), oral narrative quality (ONQ), mean

length of utterance in morphemes (MLU-M), number of different words (NDW), number of

utterances (UTT) and grammatical accuracy (GA). As expected, there was a general linear

increase in performance with age for all the measures, except for MLU-M which was the same

for the 6- and 7-year-old age groups.

To determine if the oral narrative measures were sensitive to age, separate linear

regression analyses were conducted with Age as the independent variable. The following linear

regressions were significant (p ≤ .001): ONC (r = .25), ONQ (r = .35), NDW (r = .39), UTT (r =

.36). The regression was significant at p ≤ .05 for GA (r = .16) and not significant for MLU-M (r

= .14). It was noted, however, that there were 2 outliers (scoring below 3 standard deviations)

13
when the linear regression was conducted for GA. When excluding those outliers, the effect of

age on GA was no longer significant (p = .53).

Insert Table II here

Correlational Analyses

To determine internal consistency of the measures, correlation coefficients between the oral

narrative measures were calculated and are reported in Table III. This shows that the GA

measure was not correlated to any of the other oral narrative measures. In contrast, all other

measures (ONC, ONQ, MLU-M, NDW, and UTT) showed significant correlations, except for

MLU-M and ONC, and MLU-M and UTT.

Insert Table III here

Factor Analysis

A principal component factor analysis, using Varimax rotation with Kaiser normalisation was

conducted, entering all six oral narrative measures. At first, two factors were identified using

eigenvalue >1 as the cutoff value (explaining 49.2% and 16.7% of the variance respectively).

Because the third factor demonstrated an eigenvalue of .997 and explained an additional 16.6%

of the variance, it was decided to retain the third factor. The first factor reflected a content

component and consisted of the variables NDW, UTT, ONQ and to a lesser extent ONC. The

second factor reflected a grammatical ability component and comprised GA and to a lesser extent

ONC and MLU-M. The third factor reflected a grammatical complexity component and

consisted of MLU-M and to a lesser extent UTT (see Table IV). This factor structure did not

match the hypothesised macrostructure and microstructure components of oral narrative ability,

14
which would have resulted in loadings of NDW, UTT, GA, and MLU-M on one factor

(microstructure) and ONQ and perhaps ONC on the second factor (macrostructure).

Insert Table IV here

Distribution Statistics

To determine the shape of the distribution of the oral narrative performance scores derived in the

story retelling task, several analyses were conducted. First, the 5th, 10th, 25th, 50th, 75th, 90th, and

95th percentiles were calculated, based on weighted averages (see Table V). As shown in Table

V, median scores for all oral narrative measures were similar to mean scores, except for the GA

measure in the 5- and 7-year-old age groups. Next, the distribution of the scores was

investigated, using skewness and kurtosis statistics. Normal distributions have skewness (degree

of asymmetry of the distribution around its mean) and kurtosis (the peakedness or flatness of a

distribution) statistics close to 0. Values of two standard errors of skewness or more (in this case

> 0.38) are considered skewed to a significant degree (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996 ). As

illustrated in Table IV, about half of the oral narrative measures showed a skewed distribution,

with the GA measure showing a very high skewness statistic in every age group. This indicates

that scores for all age-groups on this measure clustered at the high end of the scale. The results

also showed significantly peaked (positive) and flat (negative) distributions, with around half of

the values greater than two standard errors of kurtosis (i.e., > 0.75).

Insert Table V here

Case examples

15
To provide further insights into how the PONA may potentially assist speech pathologists in

clinical practice, two case examples of children with delayed development are presented: James

and Shelley (pseudonyms used).

James, aged 6 years 3 months, was referred to the University clinic because of concerns

about his expressive language skills. James has a twin brother, was born at 32 weeks, and

showed some delay in developmental milestones. James and his brother started school on their

fifth birthday. Although James had received speech-language therapy intervention in the past, he

was not receiving intervention at the time of the study. As part of the routine assessment battery,

the following tests were administered: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Preschool

– 2nd Edition (Wiig, Secord, & Semel, 2004): Core Language standard score (SS) 63, Receptive

language SS 72, Expressive language SS 66; Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – 3rd Edition

(Dunn & Dunn, 1997): SS 86. Preschool and Primary Inventory of Phonological Awareness

(PIPA) (Dodd, Crosbie, McIntosh, Teitzel, & Ozanne, 2000): rhyme awareness SS 3 and

phoneme segmentation SS 7. Spontaneous language was elicited in a personal narrative and a

story retelling condition, using the Westerveld & Gillon LSP (see also Westerveld et al., 2004).

When comparing 50 complete and intelligible utterances derived in the personal narrative

context to the database of New Zealand language samples integrated into SALT (Miller et al.,

2008), the standard measures profile report indicated below average performance on measures of

semantics (NDW) and morphosyntax (high number of omitted words and word errors). Table V

shows James’ performance on the PONA, using the story retelling task of Ana gets lost, and the

transcript is included in Appendix C. As shown in Table VI, James showed adequate

performance on content measures, but demonstrated difficulty in grammatical complexity and

grammatical ability. More specifically, he provided a sufficient number of utterances to retell the

16
story (UTT), showed satisfactory performance on the oral narrative quality (ONQ) measure, and

scored in the 25th percentile for NDW. However, he demonstrated significant difficulties on

measures of comprehension (ONC), and grammar (MLU-M and GA). Further inspection of the

ONQ score shows a weakness in the coherence characteristic and particular difficulty with the

story grammar parts of introduction and conflict.

Insert Table VI here

Shelley, aged 6 years 8 months, was referred for speech pathology evaluation as a potential

candidate for a research project, because of teacher concerns about her language and literacy

skills. As part of the routine assessment battery, the following tests were administered: Test Of

Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI; Brown, Sherbenou, & Johnsen, 1997): SS 93; Clinical Evaluation

of Language Fundamentals – 3rd Edition (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 1995): Total Language SS 65,

Receptive language SS 65, Expressive language SS 69; PPVT-III: SS 83. Neale Analysis of

Reading Ability – 3rd Edition (NARA; Neale, 1999): Reading accuracy percentile rank (PR)

score 16, reading comprehension PR 10. Spontaneous language was elicited in a personal

narrative and a story retelling condition, using the Westerveld & Gillon LSP. When comparing

50 complete and intelligible utterances derived in the personal narrative context to the SALT-NZ

database, the standard measures profile report indicated significant difficulties in areas of

semantics (NDW) and morpho-syntax (MLU-M and grammatical accuracy). Table VII shows

Shelley’s performance on the PONA and the transcript is included in Appendix C. As shown in

Table VII, Shelley’s performance was below the level expected for her age on all measures of

17
oral narrative performance, but consistent with standardised language testing. Further analysis of

the ONQ score shows immature to emerging performance across all characteristics.

Insert Table VII here

Discussion

The current study aimed to determine if the profile of oral narrative ability (PONA) based on the

results of a story retelling task has clinical utility with young school-aged children. The story

retelling task comprised a comprehension and a retelling component. Children’s retelling skills

were analysed at macrostructure (oral narrative quality) and microstructure measures (semantic

and morphosyntactic ability as well as story length). The task was field-tested with a

representative sample of 169 typically developing children, aged between 5;0 and 7;6 years.

Descriptive analyses

First, it was examined if the oral narrative measures derived from the story retelling task were

sensitive to age. Regression analyses revealed sensitivity for age for measures of comprehension

(ONC), narrative quality (ONQ), semantic diversity (NDW), and number of utterances (UTT). In

contrast, there were no age-related changes in measures of grammatical complexity (MLU-M)

and grammatical accuracy (GA). Closer inspection of the MLU-M measure (see Table II) reveals

an increase in MLU-M from 5 to 6 years, and no change between 6 and 7 years. This seems a

reflection of the MLU-M measure, rather than the task, as similar trends have been observed in

previous studies. For example, Justice et al. (2006) analysed performance on a story generation

task and noticed a linear increase in MLU from 5 to 7 years and then a decrease in performance

18
on the MLU-M measure from 7 to 8 years. In contrast, Leadholm and Miller (1992) observed

very similar performances when comparing 5- to 6-year-olds on the MLU-M measure (based on

100 utterances derived in a narrative context), followed by an increase in MLU from 6 to 7 years.

When inspecting the GA measure in the current study, a linear upward trend is seen from 5 to 7

years of age, but no significant differences were found between the different age groups. Other

researchers who have used this measure also found little variation with age for typically

developing children (Fey et al., 2004). Despite the finding that the MLU-M and GA measures

did not show a significant increase with age, results from previous studies demonstrating these

measures’ sensitivity to language ability support the importance of including these measures in

the PONA (Fey et al., 2004; Liles et al., 1995; Scott & Windsor, 2000). However, further

research investigating the performance of groups of children with language impairment on the

PONA is needed to confirm this hypothesis.

Descriptive statistics revealed that performance on the oral narrative measures was not

always normally distributed. Although the median scores were generally close to the mean

scores, ONC and GA in particular showed scores that were clustered onto the high end of the

scale. The obvious explanation concerns ceiling effects for ONC and GA, with 10 to 25 percent

of the children in all age groups responding correctly to all comprehension questions and

producing 100% grammatically correct utterances to retell the story.

The lack of a normal distribution of scores on some narrative microstructure measures at

each age group was not unexpected, as previous research has shown a generally large variability

in performance on oral narrative tasks (e.g., Justice et al., 2006; Liles et al., 1995). The debate on

whether language sampling analysis can be standardised is likely to continue (e.g., Miller,

Heilmann, Nockerts, Andriacchi, & Iglesias, 2006), until, as Justice and her colleagues argued, a

19
theoretical explanation can be found for these “distributional irregularities” (Justice et al., 2006,

p. 187). Although this inherent variability in oral narrative performance affects its potential for

norm-referencing and indicates that the results should be used with caution, it should not rule out

its use in clinical practice. It is thus proposed that the PONA may be used by clinicians as part of

their language assessment battery to determine a child’s relative performance compared to his or

her peers.

Analysis of the correlational data showed consistently strong correlations between ONC,

NDW, ONQ, and UTT. MLU-M only showed significant correlations with ONQ and NDW,

whereas GA was not significantly correlated to any of the other measures. The factor analysis

yielded three dimensions, a content factor (containing NDW, UTT, ONQ and to a lesser extent

ONC), a grammatical ability component (comprising GA and to a lesser extent ONC and MLU-

M), and a grammatical complexity component (consisting of MLU-M and to a lesser extent

UTT). These findings do not seem to confirm the hypothesised distinction between

microstructure and macrostructure measures and appear to contradict results from previous

research in this area (Liles et al., 1995). Liles et al. conducted a factor analysis of oral narrative

measures derived in story retelling conditions, which showed two distinct categories, referred to

as “Content Organization” (macro-level) and “Linguistic Structure” (more local level) (p. 421).

The measures under investigation in the Liles et al. study included: 1) Content factor: total

number of episodes, ratio of possible episodes, ratio of complete cohesive ties, and 2) Linguistic

structure factor: mean number of words per subordinate clause, mean number of subordinate

clauses per utterance, and ratio of grammatical utterances. Interestingly, the mean number of

words per main clause measure did not load on the matrix at all. There are several possible

explanations for the differences in findings between the current study and the Liles et al. (1995)

20
study. First, different oral narrative measures were used in the studies, which accounted for only

41.3% of the variance in Liles et al. study, compared to over 82% in the current study. For

example, Liles et al. did not include a NDW measure. Based on results from the current study, it

seems reasonable to expect that NDW would have loaded on to the Content Organisation factor

in the Liles et al. study. In addition, Liles et al. did not include a comprehension measure, which

could have accounted for additional variance. Second, the current study uses a much larger

sample of subjects, which may have provided stronger factor loadings.

Another finding from the factor analysis relates to the separate loadings of the GA and

the MLU-M measure, indicating these measures tap different constructs. Results from Justice et

al. (2006) also showed that microstructure measures of oral narrative performance did not

represent a single construct. When analysing oral narrative production measures derived from a

story generation task (n = 250, age 5-12 years), factor analysis revealed two factors, accounting

for a combined 76.3% of the variance. One factor (labelled productivity) contained measures of

TNW, NDW, story length, number of coordinating conjunctions, number of subordinating

conjunctions, and number of complex sentences. The second factor (complexity) contained

measures of MLU and proportion of complex sentences. The authors admitted, however, that the

factor loadings did not match the hypothesised productivity and complexity loadings. In

summary, results from the current study confirm that oral narrative performance taps multiple

underlying factors (Justice et al., 2006; Liles et al., 1995) and highlight the importance of

analysing oral narratives on a range of measures. Future research should consider a wider range

of oral narrative measures to further investigate the dimensionality of oral narrative performance.

Case studies

21
The two case studies, James and Shelley, showed the potential application of the PONA for

assessment and intervention practices. The percentile scores for the oral narrative measures (see

Table V) can be used to create a profile of a child’s strength and weaknesses in oral narrative

performance across these measures. More interestingly, although James and Shelley showed

similar performance on a broad spectrum standardised language test, their performance on the

PONA differed. As shown in Tables VI and VII, James’ strengths were in measures tapping

semantic ability, including number of different words, oral narrative quality and the number of

utterances used to retell the story. In contrast, Shelley demonstrated difficulties in all aspects of

oral narrative ability, indicating both semantic and grammatical problems. Further inspection of

the children’s performance on the story quality rubric as well as analysis of the types of

grammatical errors produced during story retelling will add further detail to the results obtained

with the PONA. As a result, and consistent with evidence-based practice, these data can thus

inform the programming of intervention goals to specifically address areas of weakness in oral

narrative performance.

Limitations of the study and future directions

One limitation of the current study is the limited sensitivity of the ONC and GA measures.

However, this may be due to the task that was used: a 10-page picture book with a relatively

short story. For typically developing 5- to 7-year-old children this task may not have been

sufficiently challenging, resulting in ceiling effects on both of these measures. Despite the

skewed distribution for both these measures, however, the mean scores were close to the median

scores and the percentile scores showed a linear increase. Furthermore, the case examples

highlighted the difficulties on these two measures demonstrated by the two children with

language impairment. In addition, results from our research involving children with reading

22
disability showed performance on this oral narrative task to differentiate between poor readers

and their typically developing peers on the ONC and the ONQ measures (Westerveld et al.,

2008) and on the MLU-M and the GA measures (Westerveld, Gillon, & Miller, 2006). Further

research with larger groups of children is needed to determine if the PONA can reliably

differentiate children with language impairment from their typically developing peers.

The PONA should be used with caution with children from different cultural and/or

linguistic backgrounds. The results from a pilot study investigating the performance of 6-year-

old children from Maori, Pacific Islands, and New Zealand European backgrounds revealed no

differences in performance between the cultural groups on a range of oral narrative measures

(Westerveld & Gillon, 2001). It is not clear, however, if these findings can be extended to

younger children or to children for whom English is their second language. Future research

should consider cultural-linguistic styles that may influence performance on the PONA to help

distinguish disordered performance from cultural differences in oral narrative performance (see

also Gutierrez-Clellen, Peña, & Quinn, 1995)

It is not clear if the PONA can be used to demonstrate progress in oral narrative ability

following intervention. Because the story is short, a learning effect may be evident, and further

study of the story retelling task to determine test-retest validity is clearly needed. The PONA

should not be used as a screening measure until further research into this application is

conducted. Rather, until further research is undertaken, its purpose is to provide clinicians with

local field test data that can be used as an addition to their battery of language assessments.

Conclusion

The PONA provides the clinician with an overview of a young school-aged child’s strengths and

weaknesses in several aspects of oral narrative performance compared to his/her typically

23
developing peers. However, clinicians should be careful not to use the story retelling sample as

the basis for a full microstructure analysis. It is well established that analysis of at least 50

complete and intelligible utterances is needed to perform a comprehensive analysis of children’s

semantic, syntactic, and morphological skills (Miller, 1996). Therefore, the PONA shows

potential to be used as one aspect of the clinician’s assessment battery to describe children’s

spoken language performance. This profile of oral narrative ability will then contribute to the

clinical evaluation of a child’s spoken language ability and will potentially provide detailed

direction for oral narrative intervention.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the children and the children’s parents and class teachers for

their cooperation in the database project. Thanks are also extended to the speech pathologists

who assisted in the data collection process. The support of the New Zealand Ministry of

Education, the New Zealand Speech-Language Therapists’ Association and the Don Bevan

Travel Scholarship was greatly appreciated. Finally, this project would not have been possible

without the assistance from Professor Jon Miller and Ann Nockerts from SALT Software, LLC.

References

Applebee, A. (1978). The child's concept of story. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Bishop, D. V. (2004). Expression, Reception and Recall of Narrative Instrument (ERRNI).

Oxford, UK: Harcourt Assessment.

Bliss, L. S., McCabe, A., & Miranda, A. E. (1998). Narrative assessment profile: Discourse

analysis for school-age children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 31, 347-363.

Botting, N. (2002). Narrative as a tool for the assessment of linguistic and pragmatic

impairments. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 18(1), 1-21.

24
Boudreau, D. (2008). Narrative abilities: Advances in research and implications for clinical

practice. Topics in Language Disorders, 28(2), 99-114.

Boudreau, D. M., & Hedberg, N. L. (1999). A comparison of early literacy skills in children with

specific language impairment and their typically developing peers. American Journal of

Speech-Language Pathology, 8(3), 249-260.

Brown, L., Sherbenou, R., & Johnsen, S. (1997). Test of Nonverbal Intelligence: A Language-

Free Measure of Cognitive Ability (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-ed.

Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University

Press.

Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children's reading comprehension ability: Concurrent

prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 96(1), 31-42.

Dodd, B., Crosbie, S., McIntosh, B., Teitzel, T., & Ozanne, A. (2000). Preschool and Primary

Inventory of Phonological Awareness (PIPA): London, Psychological Corporation.

Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test - III. Circle Pines, MN:

American Guidance Service.

Feagans, L., & Appelbaum, M. I. (1986). Validation of language subtypes in learning disabled

children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 358-364.

Feagans, L., & Short, E. J. (1984). Developmental differences in the comprehension and

production of narratives by reading-disabled and normally achieving children. Child

Development, 55, 1727-1736.

25
Fey, M. E., Catts, H. W., Proctor-Williams, K., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2004). Oral and

written story composition skills of children with language impairment. Journal of Speech,

Language, and Hearing Research, 47(6), 1301-1318.

Gillam, R. B., & Pearson, N. A. (2004). Test of Narrative Language. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.

Gillon, G., Moriarty, B., & Schwarz, I. (2006). An international literature review of best

practices in speech and language therapy; Assessment and intervention practices for

children with speech and language impairment. Update January 2006. Christchurch,

New Zealand: Department of Communication Disorders, University of Canterbury.

Gillon, G., & Schwarz, I. (1998). Effective provision and resourcing of speech and language

services for Special Education 2000; Resourcing speech and language needs in Special

Education. Database and best practice validation. New Zealand: Ministry of Education.

Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and

Special Education, 7(1), 6-10.

Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text

comprehension. Psychological Review, 101(3), 371-395.

Griffith, P. L., Ripich, D. N., & Dastoli, S. L. (1986). Story structure, cohesion, and propositions

in story recalls by learning-disabled and nondisabled children. Journal of

Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 539-555.

Gummersall, D. M., & Strong, C. J. (1999). Assessment of complex sentence production in a

narrative context. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 152-164.

Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F., Peña, E., & Quinn, R. (1995). Accommodating cultural differences in

narrative style: A multicultural perspective. Topics in Language Disorders, 15, 54-67.

26
Holloway, K. F. C. (1986). The effects of basal readers on oral language structures: A

description of complexity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 141-151.

Hughes, D., McGillivray, L., & Schmidek, M. (1997). Guide to narrative language: Procedures

for assessment. Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications.

Johnston, J. R. (2008). Narratives: Twenty-five years later. Topics in Language Disorders, 28(2),

93-98.

Jones, M. A., & Lodholz, C. (1999). Story Grammar Rubric. Madison Metropolitan School

District and the Language Analysis Lab, Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin,

Madison: Unpublished document created by the SALT working group, Jon Miller Chair.

Justice, L. M., Bowles, R. P., Kaderavek, J. N., Ukrainetz, T. A., Eisenberg, S. L., & Gillam, R.

B. (2006). The index of narrative microstructure: A clinical tool for analyzing school-age

children's narrative performances. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,

15(2), 177-191.

Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction

integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163-182.

Liles, B. Z. (1985). Cohesion in the narratives of normal and language-disordered children.

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 123-133.

Liles, B. Z. (1987). Episode organization and cohesive conjunctives in narratives of children

with and without language disorder. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 185-

196.

Liles, B. Z., Duffy, R. J., Merritt, D. D., & Purcell, S. L. (1995). Measurement of narrative

discourse ability in children with language disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing

Research, 38, 415-425.

27
Loban, W. (1976). Language development: Kindergarten through grade twelve. Urbana, IL:

National Council of Teachers of English.

Mar, R. A. (2004). The neuropsychology of narrative: Story comprehension, story production

and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia, 42(10), 1414-1434.

Masterson, J. J., & Kamhi, A. G. (1991). The effects of sampling conditions on sentence

production in normal, reading-disabled, and language-disabled children. Journal of

Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 549-558.

Merritt, D. D., & Liles, B. Z. (1989). Narrative analysis: Clinical applications of story generation

and story retelling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 429-438.

Miller, J., Gillon, G., & Westerveld, M. (2008). SALT NZ: Systematic Analysis of Language

Transcripts - New Zealand Version. Madison, WI: SALT Software, LLC.

Miller, J. F. (1996). Progress in assessing, describing, and defining child language disorder. In K.

N. Cole, P. S. Dale & D. J. Thal (Eds.), Assessment of communication and language (6th

ed., pp. 309-324). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

Miller, J. F., Heilmann, J., & Nockerts, A. (2006). Oral language and reading in bilingual

children. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 21(1), 30-43.

Miller, J. F., Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., Andriacchi, & Iglesias. (2006). Can language sample

analysis be standardized? Paper presented at the ASHA Convention, Miami Beach,

Florida.

Milosky, L. M. (1987). Narratives in the classroom. Seminars in Speech and Language, 8(4), 329

- 343.

Neale, M. D. (1999). Neale Analysis of Reading Ability (3rd ed.). Melbourne, VIC: Australian

Council for Educational Research.

28
Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D. V. (2002). Inferential processing and story recall in children with

communication problems: A comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic

language impairment and high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language

and Communication Disorders, 37(3), 227-251.

Petersen, D. R., Gillam, S. L., & Gillam, R. R. (2008). Emerging procedures in narrative

assessment - The index of narrative complexity. Topics in Language Disorders, 28(2),

115-130.

Ripich, D. N., & Griffith, P. L. (1988). Narrative abilities of children with learning disabilities

and nondisabled children: Story structure, cohesion, and propositions. Journal of

Learning Disabilities, 21, 165-173.

Roth, F. P., & Spekman, N. J. (1986). Narrative discourse: Spontaneously generated stories of

learning-disabled and normally achieving students. Journal of Speech and Hearing

Disorders, 51(1), 8-23.

Schneider, P. (1996). Effects of pictures versus orally presented stories on story retellings by

children with language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 5,

86-96.

Scott, C. M., & Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance measures in spoken and

written narrative and expository discourse of school-age children with language learning

disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(2), 324-339.

Semel, E. M., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1995). CELF: Clinical Evaluation of Language

Fundamentals (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace

Jovanovich.

29
Skarakis-Doyle, E., & Dempsey, L. (2008). Assessing story comprehension in preschool

children. Topics in Language Disorders, 28(2), 131-148.

Snyder, L. S., & Downey, D. M. (1991). The language-reading relationship in normal and

reading-disabled children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 129-140.

Strong, C. J. (1998). The Strong Narrative Assessment Profile. Eau Claire, WI: Thinking

Publications.

Swan, E. (1992). Ko au na galo (Ana gets lost). Wellington, New Zealand: Learning Media,

Ministry of Education.

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996 ). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). NY: Harper

Collins.

Wagner, C. R., Sahlen, B., & Nettelbladt, U. (1999). What's the story? Narration and

comprehension in Swedish preschool children with language impairment. Child

Language Teaching and Therapy, 15(2), 113-137.

Watkins, R. V., Kelly, D. J., Harbers, H. M., & Hollis, W. (1995). Measuring children's lexical

diversity: Differentiating typical and impaired language learners. Journal of Speech and

Hearing Research, 38(6), 1349-1355.

Westby, C. E. (2005). Assessing and remediating text comprehension problems. In H. W. Catts

& A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (2nd ed., pp. 157-232). Boston,

MA: Pearson Education.

Westerveld, M., & Gillon, G. (1999/2000). Narrative language sampling in young school-age

children. New Zealand Journal of Speech-Language Therapy, 53/54, 34-41.

30
Westerveld, M., & Gillon, G. (2001). Oral language sampling in 6-year-old New Zealand

children from different cultural backgrounds. New Zealand Journal of Speech-Language

Therapy, 56, 5-17.

Westerveld, M. F., Gillon, G. T., & Miller, J. F. (2004). Spoken language samples of New

Zealand children in conversation and narration. Advances in Speech-Language

Pathology, 6(4), 195-208.

Westerveld, M. F., Gillon, G. T., & Miller, J. F. (2006). Morpho-syntactic language measures

obtained in personal narrative versus story-retelling conditions. Poster presented at the

ASHA Convention, Miami Beach, Florida.

Westerveld, M. F., Gillon, G. T., & Moran, C. (2008). A longitudinal investigation of oral

narrative skills in children with mixed reading disability. International Journal of

Speech-Language Pathology, 10(3), 132-145.

Wiig, E. H., Secord, W., & Semel, E.M. (2004). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals

Preschool - Second Edition: San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

31
Table I. Age and gender distribution of the participants

Age group n Age (year; months) Age range Female/Male


Mean SD n %

5-year-olds 39 5;6 0;3 5;0 – 5;11 22/17 56 / 44


6-year-olds 81 6;5 0;3 6;0 – 6;11 44/37 54 / 46
7-year-olds 49 7;3 0;2 7;0 – 7;6 26/23 53 / 47

32
Table II. Group performance on oral narrative measures

5-year-olds 6-year-olds 7-year-olds


Measures M SD M SD M SD
Comprehension ONC 6 1.5 6.4 1.4 7 1.2

Production
Macrostructure ONQ 21.5 5.9 24.5 6.6 28.2 5.9

Microstructure MLU-M 6.9 1.2 7.3 0.9 7.3 0.9


NDW 40 13 49 16 59 14
UTT 10.6 4 13 5 16 4.6
GA 86.2 14.9 87.9 11 91.8 9.8

Note: ONC = Oral narrative comprehension, maximum score is 8; ONQ = oral narrative quality,

range of scores is 8 – 40; MLU-M = Mean length of utterance in morphemes; NDW = number of

different words; UTT = total number of utterances; GA = grammatical accuracy in percent

grammatically correct utterances.

33
Table III. Correlations between the oral narrative measures

Measures ONC ONQ MLU-M NDW UTT GA


ONC --- .38** .13 .37** .33** .19
ONQ --- .26* .79** .73** .18
MLU-M --- .27* .01 .03
NDW --- .93** .14
UTT --- .13
GA ---
Note: ONC = Oral narrative comprehension; ONQ = Oral narrative quality; MLU-M = Mean

length of utterance in morphemes; NDW = Number of different words; UTT = number of total

utterances; GA = Grammatical accuracy.

* correlation is significant at p < .05. ** correlation is significant at .001 level.

34
Table IV. Rotated factor loadings

Variables Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3

ONC .394 .502 .170


NDW .950 .094 .162
UTT .957 .080 -.116
ONQ .854 .172 .197
GA .011 .930 -.047
MLU-M .094 .031 .984

Eigenvalues 2.951 1.002 0.997


% Variance 49.19 16.70 16.62

Note: ONC = Oral narrative comprehension; NDW = Number of different words; UTT = number

of total utterances; ONQ = Oral narrative quality; GA = Grammatical accuracy; MLU-M = Mean

length of utterance in morphemes.

35
Table V. Distributional performance and statistics by age on the oral narrative measures

Age Group M Mdn SD 5% 10% 25% 50% 75% 90% 95% Skewness Kurtosis
5-year-olds
(n = 39)
NDW 40 39 13.3 18 22 32 38 50 62 63 0.03 - 0.81
UTT 10.6 10 4 5 6 8 10 14 15 16 0.43 - 0.61
ONQ 21.5 22 5.8 14 16 18 22 25 30.4 32 0.43 - 0.57
ONC 6 6 1.5 3 4 5 6 7 8 8 - 0.66 0.012
GA 86 90 14.9 56 61 85 90 100 100 100 - 1.3 1.01
MLU-M 6.9 7.1 0.2 5.2 5.4 6 7.1 7.9 8.5 8.5 0.007 - 0.9

6-year-olds
(n = 81)
NDW 48.5 48 16 25 29 36 48 62 69 77 0.25 -0.95
UTT 13 13 5 6 7 9 12 17 20 22 0.29 - 0.9
ONQ 24.5 24 6.6 12 16 20 24 28 34 36 0.036 - 0.45
ONC 6.4 7 1.4 4 4 6 7 7 8 8 -0.82 0.11
GA 87.9 88.9 11 66.7 77.8 81.8 88.9 95.2 100 100 -1.59 4.8
MLU-M 7.3 7.3 0.9 5.8 6.1 6.7 7.3 7.9 8.4 8.5 - .184 -0.53

7-year-olds
(n = 49)
NDW 59 63 14.4 35 37 50 63 69 73 76 -0.71 0.29
UTT 16 17 4.6 8 9 13 17 19 21 21.6 -0.28 -0.66
ONQ 28.2 28 5.9 18 20 24 28 32 35 38 -0.17 -0.32
ONC 7 7 1.2 5 5 6 7 8 8 8 -0.97 0.025
GA 91.8 94.4 9.8 73.3 77.9 88.2 94.4 100 100 100 -1.36 1.73
MLU-M 7.3 7.2 0.9 5.9 6.1 6.8 7.2 7.8 8.4 8.5 0.41 0.9
Note: NDW = number of different words; UTT = total number of utterances; ONQ = oral narrative quality, range of scores is 8 – 40;
ONC = Oral narrative comprehension, maximum score is 8; GA = grammatical accuracy in percent grammatically correct utterances;
MLU-M = Mean length of utterance in morphemes.

36
Table VI. James’ performance on the PONA

NDW UTT ONQ ONC GA MLU-M


Performance 38 12 24 2 58.3% 6.17
95
90
75
50 X X
25 X
10 X
5 X X
Note: NDW = number of different words; UTT = total number of utterances; ONQ = oral

narrative quality, range of scores is 8 – 40; ONC = Oral narrative comprehension, maximum

score is 8; GA = grammatical accuracy in percent grammatically correct utterances; MLU-M =

Mean length of utterance in morphemes. Performance is displayed (X) as a percentile score.

37
Table VII. Shelley’s performance on the PONA

NDW UTT ONQ ONC GA MLU-M


Performance 28 7 18 4 58.1% 6.43
95
90
75
50
25
X X
10 X X X
5 X
Note: NDW = number of different words; UTT = total number of utterances; ONQ = oral

narrative quality, range of scores is 8 – 40; ONC = Oral narrative comprehension, maximum

score is 8; GA = grammatical accuracy in percent grammatically correct utterances; MLU-M =

Mean length of utterance in morphemes. Performance is displayed (X) as a percentile score.

38
Appendix A

Ana Gets Lost (Swan, 1992)

One Saturday morning, Ana's mum and dad went fishing on the beach.

Ana had been sick all week, so she had to stay at home with her big brother Tom.

She asked Tom if he wanted to play with her.

No thanks, he said, I want to read a sports magazine.

Ana got bored.

So when Tom fell asleep, she decided to go looking for her mum and dad.

She quietly opened the front door and went outside.

Ana walked towards the beach, but she got lost.

She kept walking until it got dark.

Ana got very scared and she started to cry.

She stopped outside a dairy.

She was still crying and didn't know what to do.

Then Ana felt a pat on her shoulder.

She looked around and saw a policeman.

Hello, he said, are you Ana?

Yes, said Ana, giving him a big smile.

The policeman took Ana home in the police car.

Mum and Dad were very happy to see Ana.

They thanked the policeman for finding Ana and bringing her home safely.

The policeman told Ana not to get lost again.

Then he smiled and drove away.

39
Appendix B

Comprehension questions

1. Who is the story about?

2. Why did Ana have to stay at home?

3. Why did Ana get bored?

4. Where did Ana go to find her parents?

5. Why did Ana get scared?

6. Who found Ana?

7. What did the policeman do?

8. Why were Ana's parents happy to see her?

40
Appendix C

Story retelling by James, age 6;3 years

C (uhm) she (uhm) she walked to town.


C she we* {yawns}>
E what was the story about?
C (uhm) Ana.
E uhuh.
E anything else you can remember?
C (uh) she got lost.
E uhuh.
C and she cried and cried.
C (and then got when) when the sky got dark and dark [NGA].
E ok.
C and she asked (her brother to can) his brother to play with her [NGA].
E ok, and then?
C (she wa* she she w*) she quietly open the front door [NGA].
C and (she she) she got lost.
E oh and anything else you can remember?
C (and) and (uhm) the police (asked her to uhm uhm) pat him on the shoulder [NGA].
C and (uhm she she uhm) he got her back home.
E oh <and>.
C (<a*>) and his parents got happy [NGA].
C and the man told her (uhm uhm) not to get lost again.

C = child; E = examiner. Mazes (reformulations, filled pauses) are put in brackets. [NGA] = not

grammatically accurate. * indicates unfinished word. > indicates unfinished sentence. < >

indicates overlapping speech.

ONQ score: 24

Introduction 1; Theme 5; Main characters: 3; Supporting characters: 3; Conflict: 1; Coherence: 1;

Resolution: 5; Conclusion: 5.

ONC score: 2. James correctly answered questions 6 and 7.

41
Story retelling by Shelley, age 6;8 years

C Ana got lost.


E uhuh and then?
C and (then) then she open the door quietly [NGA].
C and (she thought she has t* she x) she has to find her mum [NGA].
E uhuh and then?
C (uhm and then she) the police took her home back to her mum and dad.
E uhuh.
C and her brother.
E uhuh and then?
C then (uhm) Annie (had) had a smile and then waved [NGA].
E <yes>.
C <and the> police (go*) was gone.

ONQ score: 18

Introduction: 1; Theme: 3; Main character: 5; Supporting characters:1; Conflict: 1; Coherence: 1;

Resolution: 3; Conclusion: 3.

ONC score 4. Shelley correctly answered questions 1, 5, 6 and 7.

42

View publication stats

You might also like