You are on page 1of 14

Communication Disorders Quarterly

A Systematic Review of Narrative-Based 32(4) 207­–220


© Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2011
Reprints and permission: http://www.
Language Intervention With Children Who sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1525740109353937

Have Language Impairment http://cdq.sagepub.com

Douglas B. Petersen1

Abstract
This systematic review focuses on research articles published since 1980 that assess outcomes of narrative-based language
intervention for preschool and school-age children with language impairment. The author conducted a comprehensive
search of electronic databases and hand searches of other sources for studies using all research designs except
nonexperimental case studies.The results of the studies were converted to a common metric using effect sizes.The review
yielded nine studies that met inclusion criteria.The majority of studies reported moderate to large effect sizes for narrative
macrostructure and microstructure. Although the results of the studies were generally positive, each of the studies had a
limited number of participants, limited experimental control, and considerable variation in the procedures and materials
used. Clinicians should be cautious when interpreting the results of these studies. Narrative intervention is at an emerging
stage of evidence and needs further investigation.

Keywords:
narratives, storytelling, intervention, language impairment

Language intervention with children often focuses on almost exclusively through language (Snow, 1991). This
aspects of conversation. Conversation is a type of dialogic is because narratives are usually focused on temporally
discourse that involves two or more participants engaged in removed events that are fully understood only by the narra-
cooperative communication (Sacks, Schegloff, & Jefferson, tor. This places a greater demand on the narrator to use
1974). Due to a mutual social, temporal, and physical con- complex, descriptive language. For example, if a child were
text, conversation can involve the use of less precise, infor- to narrate the events of the conversation above to a teacher
mal oral language. Conversational language, therefore, is who was not present, the narrative might read as follows:
highly contextualized, where the message being conveyed
need not be entirely embedded in the oral language itself. At recess David gave John the red football and they
For example, a conversation between two children could be ran over to the field to play. David told John to throw
transcribed as follows: the ball, so he threw the ball really hard and hit him
in the face. David got a bloody nose and he was
That one. This one? Yeah. Let’s go over there and screaming. John said, “Pinch your nose,” then John
play with it. Ok. Throw it to me! Oh, I’m so sorry! ran and got the teacher.
Pinch your nose. Hang on. I’ll go get help.
The differences between the conversational language
Although the two children conversing in the sample sample and the narrative language sample are quite evident.
above understood each other, this conversation would make The narrative language sample contains macrostructural
little sense to a person not present. Conversational lan-
guage, because of its contextual support, functions quite
well even when simple, ambiguous language is used. 1
University of Wyoming, Laramie, USA
In contrast, narration (storytelling) is primarily a mono-
Corresponding Author:
logic style of discourse that requires one person to convey a
Douglas B. Petersen, University of Wyoming, College of Health Sciences,
message while others take a lesser expressive role and serve Division of Communication Disorders, Department 3311, 1000 E.
as listeners. When children use narratives to communicate, University Avenue, Laramie, WY 82071, USA
they are required to construct a context for their listener Email: dpeter39@uwyo.edu

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


208 Communication Disorders Quarterly 32(4)

elements that enhance the general organization of the narra- resulted in 1,442 potential articles using an exhaustive combi-
tive and more complex, finite linguistic features (micro- nation of the search terms listed above. Using the same combi-
structure) that further increase narrative clarity. The ability nation of search terms, a search using Highwire.org resulted in
to produce well-formed, clear narratives is founded on rela- 1,276 potential articles. The title of each article was read, and
tively advanced linguistic skills. if the title warranted further investigation, the abstract or full
Given the complexity of language required for narration, text was reviewed. A total of 56 article abstracts and/or full
it is not surprising that children with language and learning texts was reviewed and screened for inclusion criteria. In addi-
disabilities often have difficulty understanding and produc- tion, the references from those 56 articles were examined,
ing narratives (Bishop & Adams, 1992; Boudreau & resulting in an additional 152 article abstracts/full texts that
Chapman, 2000; Fey, Catts, Proctor-Williams, Tomblin, were reviewed.
& Zhang, 2004; Gillam & Johnston, 1992; Greenhalgh &
Strong, 2001; Newman & McGregor, 2006; Scott
& Windsor, 2000). This difficulty with narration can Inclusion Criteria for Narrative Intervention Studies
adversely affect academic progress (Bishop & Adams, Narrative intervention was defined as an intervention proce-
1992; Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999; Gillam & Johnston, dure that used oral narratives as a medium whereby lan-
1992). Narration, both written and oral, is used in the school guage-related features were modeled by the clinician and
setting to impart information and assess comprehension practiced by the participant. Only studies that employed a
(Petersen, Gillam, Spencer, & Gillam, in press). narrative intervention procedure with preschool or school-
Narratives are an important aspect of school curriculum age children between the ages of 3 and 21 years who had
and a naturalistic means of targeting specific language diffi- language or learning disabilities were included. The severity
culties. Embedding language intervention within the context of language disability and/or learning disability was defined
of narration may have many benefits for children who have and described in the Participants section of each study.
language impairment. Teaching children to produce narra- Studies that required only one narrative retelling were not
tives for the specific purpose of improving storytelling can included because these studies typically described the effect
have academic value. Narratives can also be used as a means of differing narrative elicitation procedures on narrative pro-
whereby complex language is introduced and practiced. duction for assessment purposes—intervention was not the
Treating narratives in this manner shifts the focus of interven- aim of these studies. Procedures using direct reading from a
tion from the production of satisfactory narratives to the pro- text as the independent variable were not included. All
duction of correct language features. By embedding direct research studies conducted from 1980 to 2008 were consid-
language targets (e.g., morphosyntax) in a narrative context, ered with the exception of nonexperimental case studies.
children can practice targeted language skills while construct-
ing a meaningful form of communication that is naturalistic.
Narrative-based language intervention is emerging as a Outcome Measures Compared Across Studies
language intervention procedure and clinicians need to be The dependent variables of each study were classified as
aware of the degree to which empirical evidence supports relating to narrative macrostructure (expressive and recep-
the effectiveness of narrative intervention. Therefore, a sys- tive) and narrative microstructure. Macrostructure was
tematic review of the existing research pertaining to narra- defined according to Stein and Glenn’s (1979) story gram-
tive intervention for preschool and school-age children with mar analysis and Peterson and McCabe’s (1983) episodic
language or learning disabilities was conducted. complexity analysis. Microstructure included causal and
temporal subordinating conjunctions, coordinating con-
junctions, adverbs, elaborated noun phrases, cognitive and
Method linguistic verbs, dialogue, length (e.g., number of clauses,
Electronic databases were searched to find appropriate studies. total number of words [TNW], or number of C-units in a
Various combinations of the following search terms were used: narrative), and complexity (e.g., syntax, morphosyntax,
narrative, discourse, therapy, intervention, narrative interven- mean length of utterance [MLU], and number of different
tion, language, impaired, language impaired, storytelling, story words [NDW]) (Greenhalgh & Strong, 2001; Nippold &
telling, story-telling, and retelling. A total of 3,958 article titles Taylor, 1995; Strong & Shaver, 1991; Westby, 1999).
was reviewed. The specific results of the electronic search
were as follows: Scholar.google.com resulted in 1,240 poten-
tial articles with the terms language intervention and narra- Results
tive. The databases Medline, CINAHL, Psychology and A total of nine research studies met the inclusion criteria for
Behavioral Sciences Collection, PsychINFO, Pre-CINAHL, narrative intervention with preschool or school-age chil-
Health Source-Consumer Edition, Humanities Abstracts, dren with language impairment or learning disabilities (see
Social Sciences Abstracts, MasterFILD Premier, Academic Table 1). Effect sizes were calculated when sufficient data
Search Premier, and Dissertation Abstracts International were available. Effect sizes using Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988)

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015



Table 1. Summary of Narrative Intervention Studies With Preschool and School-Age Children Who Have Language or Learning Disabilities
Study n Participants Design Setting Duration Mnt Gen DV Sig. ES Avg. ES
Davies, Shanks, & 31 t kindergarten pre–post small three 40-min. no no a) RAPT Info a) > .01 a) 1.32 (1.10) macro exp
Davies (2004) group & sessions/ (a + c + e + h)
classroom week for 0.73
8 weeks
EA no control 960 total b) RAPT Grammar b) > .01 b) 0.94 (0.74) microstructure
English group min. c) Bus Story Info c) .008 c) 0.68 (0.44) (b + d + f + g)
d) Bus Story C-Units d) .005 d) 0.75 0.77
e) episode e) .005 e) 0.69
f) number f) > .01 f) 0.92
conjunctions
g) type conjunctions g) > .01 g) 0.66
h) story type h) .01 h) 0.67
Gillam, McFadden, & 4t 9–12 years pre–post small group three 120- no no oral and written NA oral/written oral/written
van Kleek (1995) (third–sixth min.
grades) sessions/
week for
6 weeks
4 c EA control 2,160 total a) naïve judge a) NA/NA macro exp
group min. (b + c + d)
(nonrandom) 1.52/0.24
English b) prop/T-units b) 0.82/–0.44 microstructure (g +
c) episodes c) 2.26/0.43 e + f) –0.97/–0.96
d) multiple d) 1.47/0.74
episodes
e) MLU e) –1.07/–1.05
f) syntax f) –1.08/–1.19
g) causal g) –0.77/–0.64

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


conjunctions
Hayward & Schneider 11 t pre-K pre–post small group two 20-min. no no a) story a) .001 a) 1.0 macro exp 1.48
(2000) sessions/ grammar
week;
8–12
sessions
EA 320–480 b) episode level b) .001 b) 1.96
total min.
English
Klecan-Aker, Flahive, & 8t 6–9 years old RCT pre–post individual three 30-min. no no a) number T-units a) no a) NA macro exp (f) 0.95
Fleming (1997) control group sessions/
week for
12 weeks
7 c EA 1,080 total b) number b) .04 b) 1.0 microstructure
min. clauses (b + c + d) 1.33
English c) number words c) < .05 c) 1.2

209
(continued)
210
Table 1. (continued)
Study n Participants Design Setting Duration Mnt Gen DV Sig. ES Avg. ES
d) number d) < .01 d) 1.8
words/T-unit
e) number e) no e) NA
clauses/T-unit
f) story level f) < .05 f) 0.95
Peña et al. (2006) 14 t first & second pre–post individual two 30-min. no no a) total story score a) no NA NA
(Study 2) grades sessions (b, c, d)
57 c 35% AA English control 60 total min. b) story b) no
group components
32% EA English (nonrandom) c) ideas and c) no
language
30% Latino d) episodes d) no
English/ e) MLU e) no
Spanish f) TNW f) no
g) NDW g) no
h) number clauses h) no
i) number i) no
clauses/CU
Petersen, Gillam, 3t 6–8 years multiple baseline individual 10 90-min. yes yes picture prompt picture prompt
Spencer, & Gillam sessions
(in press) EA 1,090 total a) story grammar a) NA a) 80%, 70%, macro exp (a) 73%
min. episode structure 70% (PNDs)
English b) causality b) NA b) 60%, 60%, microstructure (b)
50% (PNDs) (c + d + e + f + h
+ I + j) 57% PND
0.79
c) temporal adv. c) NA c) 0.67, –0.59, verbal prompt
clauses 0.46
d) adverbs d) NA d) 0.88, 1.5, macro exp 100%

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


1.25
e) elaborated noun e) NA e) 0.87, microstructure 67%
phrases 0.89, 1.29
f) mental & linguistic f) NA f) –0.88,
verbs 0.70, 1.15
g) reference g) NA g) NA
cohesion
h) MLU h) NA h) 0.62,
1.22, 0.10
i) TNW i) NA i) 0.02, 1.52, 1.3
j) NDW j) NA j) 0.16, 1.36, 1.53
verbal prompt
a) 100%,
100%, 100%
b) 0%,
100%, 100%

(continued)

Table 1. (continued)
Study n Participants Design Setting Duration Mnt Gen DV Sig. ES Avg. ES
Petersen, Gillam, & 12 school-age alternate treat- small group four 90-min. no no a) INC a) < .05 a) 1.58, 1.02, INC (macro & micro
Gillam (2008) ment pre–post sessions/ 1.34, 0.49, exp)
week for 4 1.53
weeks
1,440 total b) TNL (Exp) b) < .05 b) 1.57 1.19
min. c) TNL (Rec) c) < .05 c) 0.81 TNL
d) TNL (NLI) d) < .05 d) 1.31 macro exp 1.57
macro rec 0.81
Swanson, Fey, Mills, & 10 t second grade pre–post individual three 50-min. no yes gain NA NA
Hood (2005) sessions/ scores
week for 6
weeks
EA no control lab, school, 900 total min. a) NQ: narrative a) yes
group home quality
English b) NDW b) no
c) generalization c) no
d) recalling d) no
sentences
e) nonword e) no
repetition
Tyler & Sandoval (1994) 6t pre-K multiple baseline individual two to three no yes a) MLU NA NA microstructure
45–min. (Note: Only
sessions/ participants with

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


week for mild/moderate
12 weeks disabilities
improved
microstructure.)
phonology group 1,080–1,620 b) Brown’s stages
as a control total min.
group
c) morphemes

Note: Effect sizes (ES) calculated using the following methods: a) (M1pre – M1post) – (M2pre – M2post) divided by pooled SD, b) (treatment group mean – control group mean) divided by pooled SD,
c) using ES calculations from F test. Mnt = maintenance; Gen = generalization; Sig. = statistical significance; DV = dependent variable(s); EA = European American; RAPT = Renfrew Action Picture Test;
n = number of participants; t = treatment group; c = control group; MLU = mean length of utterance; macro exp = expressive macrostructure; macro rec = receptive macrostructure; RCT =
Randomized Control Trial; AA = African American; PND = Percentage of over-lapping Data Points; TNW = total number of words; NDW = number of different words; INC = Index of Narrative
Complexity; TNL = Test of Narrative Language; NLI = Narrative Language Index.

211
212 Communication Disorders Quarterly 32(4)

Table 2. Critical Appraisal Questions for Group Research Studies


Control group Did the study include a control group and one or more treatment groups?
Random assignment Were the participants randomly assigned to the treatment and control groups?
Participants Did the authors provide information about the age, gender, race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, speech
and language abilities, and/or cognitive status of the children who participated in the study?
Initial group similarity Were the groups the same in all important ways except for the treatment under investigation?
Blinding Did the individuals who conducted the assessments and analyzed the data know which groups (treat-
ment, comparison, or control) the participants were assigned to?
Measures Were the formal and informal measures used to assess the treatment outcomes valid and reliable?
Statistical significance Did the authors report p values (the probability that a difference between the mean scores of two or
more groups would not occur by chance alone) that were less than .05?
Practical significance Did the authors report Eta squared values (percentage of variance accounted for) or standardized d
values (number of standard deviations of difference between pre- and posttesting or between groups)
that were at lest moderately large? If not, could moderately large values be calculated from the data
that were provided in the article?

Note: Adapted from Gillam and Gillam (2006).

were calculated for studies that employed a no-treatment one of three different treatment conditions: (a) elicited imita-
control group. When a study did not include a no-treatment tion and contrasted minimal pairs of phonological targets
control group, effect sizes were calculated by comparing (phonological intervention), (b) indirect narrative language
the differences between pretest and posttest scores of the intervention (narrative intervention), or (c) combined inter-
treatment and alternate treatment groups (Cirrin & Gillam, vention using both treatments (combined intervention). The
2008). In addition, effect sizes from gain scores and F tests participants in the three treatment groups differed in severity
were calculated if necessary (Thalheimer & Cook, 2002). of disability. The children in the narrative intervention group
When effect sizes are calculated from gain scores, and no were classified as having more severe disabilities than the
control group is available for comparison, considerable children in the phonological intervention and combined inter-
caution must be taken in interpreting the validity of the vention groups. The two groups of children assigned to the
effect sizes. Multiple threats to internal validity potentially conditions involving narrative intervention (narrative inter-
confound the study outcomes. vention and combined intervention) were asked to listen to
Not all studies included in the review were of equal qual- narratives containing multiple exemplars of morphosyntatic
ity. Each of the nine studies was coded for internal and exter- targets and then retell those narratives with clinician support
nal validity using the methods proposed by Gillam and Gillam to a naïve listener. The two participants assigned to the phono-
(2006; see Table 2) and Horner et al. (2005; see Table 3). The logical intervention group were asked to repeat target pho-
results of the study quality appraisals are found in Tables 4 nemes. Forty-five minute, individual therapy sessions were
and 5. Generally speaking, the greater the number of appraisal conducted in a clinic two to three times a week for 12 weeks.
points awarded a study, the greater confidence one can have in Two 6-week intervention cycles were planned, however, one
the causal relationship between the intervention and study participant assigned to the narrative intervention treatment
outcomes. The nine studies reviewed in this article received withdrew from the study after the first 6-week cycle. All six
an average of 4.8 out of 8 appraisal points (ranging from 2 to participants across the three treatment conditions increased
7). Low to moderately high confidence can be placed in the their MLU in spontaneous language samples. Likewise, all
results of the studies reviewed. Table 6 outlines the different six participants demonstrated an increase in morphosyntax
narrative intervention procedures used in each study. The nar- usage during intervention sessions. It is interesting that the
rative intervention procedures were classified as having a two participants who were assigned to the narrative interven-
focus on (a) expressive narrative macrostructure, (b) receptive tion group were the only children who did not show morpho-
narrative macrostructure, and (c) microstructure. syntactic gains in postintervention spontaneous language
probes. Effect sizes for the microstructure variables could not
be calculated. Given that the groups demonstrated different
Summary of Narrative Intervention Studies levels of severity of delays prior to intervention and outcomes
The nine studies under review are summarized below for each group mimicked initial levels, little can be concluded
according to the date of publication. from these results. The study quality assessment rating was 2
Tyler and Sandoval (1994) assessed the phonology and for this study, suggesting that very little confidence can be
language production of six preschool children with both pho- placed in the causal relation between the intervention and
nological and language impairments after assigning them to study outcomes.

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


Petersen 213

Table 3. Critical Appraisal Questions for Single Subject Research Studies


Description of participants and settings Were the participants described with sufficient detail?
Was the process for selecting participants described with replicable precision?
Were the critical features of the physical setting described with replicable precision?
Dependent variable Were the dependent variables described with operational precision?
Were the dependent variables quantifiable?
Were the dependent variables valid and described with replicable precision?
Were the dependent variables measured repeatedly over time?
Was interobserver agreement reliability at least 80%?
Independent variable Was the independent variable described with replicable precision?
Were the independent variables systematically manipulated and under the control of the
experimenter?
Fidelity of treatment implementation data?
Baseline Were there repeated measures indicating a predictable pattern during baseline?
Were the baseline conditions described with replicable precision?
Blinding Were the individuals who conducted the assessments and analyzed the data unaware of
subject identity, the baseline, treatment, and follow-up conditions?
Internal validity Did the design provide at least three demonstrations of experimental effect at three
different points in time?
Did the design control for common threats to internal validity?
Did the results document a pattern that demonstrated experimental control?
External validity Were the experimental effects replicated across participants, settings, or materials?
Social validity Was the dependent variable socially important?
Was the magnitude of change in the dependent variable socially important?
Was the implementation of the independent variable practical and cost-effective?
Was the intervention implemented using typical intervention agents in typical physical and
social contexts?
Note: Adapted from Horner et al. (2005).

Table 4. Group Study Quality Appraisal


Control Random Group Dependent Statistical Practical Appraisal
Study Group Assignment Participants Similarity Blinding Variable(s) Significance Significance Points
Gillam et al. (1995) yes yes yes no yes no yes yes 6 moderate
Klecan-Aker, Flahive, & yes yes yes yes no no yes yes 6 moderate
Fleming (1997)
Peña et al. (2006) yes no yes yes yes yes no no 5 low/moderate
Petersen, Gillam, & no yes yes yes yes yes yes yes 7 moderate/high
Gillam (2008)
Tyler & Sandoval (1994) yes no yes no no no NA NA 2 low

Table 5.  Single Subject Study Quality Appraisal


Dependent Independent Internal External Social Appraisal
Study Description Variable Variable Baseline Blinding Validity Validity Validity Points
Davies, Shanks, & Davies (2004) no yes yes no no no no yes 3 low
Hayward & Schneider (2000) yes no no no yes no no yes 3 low
Petersen, Gillam, Spencer, & yes yes yes yes yes yes no yes 7 moderate/high
Gillam (in press)
Swanson, Fey, Mills, & Hood (2005) yes yes yes no no no no yes 4 low
Note: Adapted from Horner et al. (2005).

Gillam, McFadden, and van Kleek (1995) compared the enrolled in a story-oriented narrative intervention program
effects of two different language intervention procedures on and four children were enrolled in a language skills program
the oral and written narratives produced by eight 9- to 12-year- (treated as a control group for this review). The narrative pro-
old children with language impairment. Four children were gram revolved around children’s literature and the retelling

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


214 Communication Disorders Quarterly 32(4)

Table 6. Narrative Intervention Procedures and Materials

Microstructure:
Study Macrostructure: Expressive Macrostructure: Receptive Literate/Non-Literate
Davies, Shanks, & Davies (2004) Narrative Production/Retelling answer “wh” questions NA
peer teaching cue cards
puppets
role-play
think-aloud
recorded narratives
Gillam, McFadden, & van Kleek (1995) Narrative Production/Retelling reflective questioning temporal relations
causal relations
teacher/peer modeling
inferential/factual questions
semantic mapping
role-play
songs
games
pictures
hand-drawn pictures
semantic maps (for writing)
book making
children’s literature
Hayward & Schneider (2000) Narrative Production/Retelling comprehension monitoring temporal relations
causal relations
wordless picture books
close procedures
vocabulary building
role-play
cue cards
sorting
pictures/photos
Klecan-Aker, Flahive, & Fleming (1997) Narrative Production/Retelling NA syntax
sequence cards
photos
hand-drawn pictures
multiple choice resolution
close procedure
Peña et al. (2006) Narrative Production/Retelling NA Narrative Production/
Retelling
wordless picture books wordless picture books
story grammar temporal markers
episode structure causality
Petersen, Gillam, & Gillam (2008) Narrative Production/Retelling answer “wh” questions causal relations
wordless picture books
icons/cue cards
photos
hand-drawn pictures
story grammar
episode structure
Petersen, Gillam, Spencer, & Gillam Narrative Production/Retelling answer “wh” questions causal relations
(in press ) temporal relations
wordless picture books
icons/cue cards
photos
hand-drawn pictures
story grammar
episode structure

(continued)

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


Petersen 215

Table 6. (continued)
Microstructure:
Study Macrostructure: Expressive Macrostructure: Receptive Literate/Non-Literate
Swanson, Fey, Mills, & Hood (2005) Narrative Production/Retelling NA Narrative Production/
Retelling
wordless picture books dialogue
single photo/picture morphosyntax
hand-drawn pictures conjunctions
story grammar subordinationcomplex
episode structure verbs
sequencing postmodification of nouns
sentence repetition
Tyler & Sandoval (1994) NA NA Narrative Retelling
Group A
focused stimulation
modeling
paraphrasing
expansion
recasting
naïve listeners
Group B
direct phonology/
morphophonemes
(plural/past tense)

and generation of oral and written narratives using various 16 weeks of small group instruction that focused on story
props and activities. The language skills program principally grammar (macrostructure) and syntactic complexity (micro-
consisted of form-focused direct instruction in spoken and structure). Outcomes of the treatment program indicated that
written language skills. Oral and written narratives produced the children receiving the narrative intervention signifi-
by the narrative intervention group were reported to contain cantly improved their narrative macrostructure and increased
more complex macrostructure in comparison with the lan- the number of clauses per narrative when compared with the
guage skills control group. In contrast, the comparison, lan- no-treatment control group. The average narrative macro-
guage skills group produced oral and written narratives with structure effect size was .95 and the average narrative micro-
more advanced microstructure (causal/temporal relations, structure effect size was 1.33. Both effect sizes are considered
syntax, MLU). The average effect sizes for oral and written large and indicate that the narrative intervention had a mean-
narrative macrostructure were 1.52 and .24, respectively. The ingful effect on the children’s narrative ability. In addition,
average effect sizes for oral and written microstructure were the study appraisal score was 6, lending a moderate degree
–.97 and –.96, respectively. The quality of narratives was also of validity to the causal nature of the intervention.
evaluated by blinded examiners, with 50% of the oral narra- Hayward and Schneider (2000) implemented narrative
tives and 25% of the written narratives produced by the chil- intervention with an emphasis on story grammar to 13 pre-
dren in the whole language group judged to be good stories, school children with language impairment. All children
whereas 0% and 12%, respectively, of the stories produced by attended a language-based, early childhood classroom
the language skills group were considered good stories, lend- located in a rehabilitation hospital, and they participated in
ing some support to the possibility that macrostructure is small intervention groups (2–3) twice a week for 20 minutes.
more reflective of narrative quality than microstructure. The Although narratives were central to the ongoing classroom
study appraisal score was 6, suggesting that a moderate degree curriculum, story grammar elements were not directly tar-
of confidence can be placed in the causal (positive and nega- geted outside of the experimental intervention. Pictures from
tive) effect of the intervention. the book Oops by Mercer Mayer (1977) and cue cards were
Klecan-Aker, Flahive, and Fleming (1997) randomly used to aid the children in identifying, sorting, reorganizing,
assigned eight children with language impairment to a narra- and sequencing story grammar components. An AB single
tive intervention treatment group and seven children with subject design was implemented with half the group receiv-
language impairment to a no-treatment control group. The ing two baseline probes and half the group receiving four
researchers investigated the narrative and linguistic com- baseline probes. Group pretest to posttest gains reflect a
plexity in narratives produced by the children after receiving statistically significant difference for story grammar and

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


216 Communication Disorders Quarterly 32(4)

episode level. Based on the single subject graphical displays, improving narrative macrostructure for children with lan-
12 out of 13 participants showed improvements, with the guage impairment. The study appraisal score was 5 out of 8
majority of participants demonstrating substantial gains (i.e., possible points, suggesting that a low-moderate degree of
high percentage of nonoverlapping data points) after only confidence can be placed in the validity of the findings. The
eight intervention sessions. The average effect size for narra- nonsignificant gains in narrative macrostructure are not sur-
tive macrostructure was 1.68. Although causal and temporal prising considering the limited duration of the intervention
relations were targeted during intervention, those variables (two individual 30-minute sessions).
were not measured as dependent variables. The AB design Petersen, Gillam, and Gillam (2008) conducted a pre-
limits confidence in a causal relationship, and because the liminary investigation to determine the effect of narrative
assessment stories were also used during intervention, the intervention on the macrostructure and microstructure
intervention effect cannot be isolated. The study received a of school-age children with language impairment. Twelve
study quality appraisal score of 3, indicating that multiple children (ages 6 years 4 months to 9 years 1 month; M = 8
threats to the validity of the study findings are plausible. years 2 months) with language impairment were assessed
Davies, Shanks, and Davies (2004) assessed the narra- using the Index of Narrative Complexity (INC; Petersen
tive skills of 31 kindergarten children identified by teachers et al., 2008) at 1-month intervals (pretest 1 in early June,
as having language difficulty after participating in three, pretest 2 in early July) before treatment. During the treat-
40-minute narrative intervention sessions per week over 8 ment phase, the children attended four 90-minute narrative
weeks. Results revealed significant differences between all language intervention sessions each week for 4 weeks. The
pre- and posttesting measures, with the children improving participants were tested again immediately after the treat-
narrative macrostructure (story grammar and episodic com- ment (posttest). The Test of Narrative Language (TNL;
plexity) and microstructure (additive, temporal, and causal Gillam & Pearson, 2004) was administered after treatment
conjunctions, syntax, and number of C-units). The average to provide evidence of narrative language growth and to
effect sizes for each of the dependent variables were moder- evaluate criterion validity of the INC. The results indicated
ate. The Davies et al. (2004) study was a simple pre–post that children’s narration did not change appreciably during
design that received a study appraisal score of 3. This low the month that they were not enrolled in school and were
quality appraisal score suggests that limited confidence not receiving intervention. Following intervention, the INC
should be placed in the causal nature of the intervention. scores, which reflect aspects of narrative macrostructure
Swanson, Fey, Mills, and Hood (2005) analyzed the nar- and microstructure, increased significantly. This same pat-
ratives of 10 children (ages 6–8) for various aspects of nar- tern of results was obtained for each of the INC measures
rative macrostructure and microstructure after implementing and for the TNL. Cohen’s d effect size calculations for dif-
a 6-week narrative-based language intervention program. It ferences between pretest 2 and posttest (the treatment phase
was reported that 8 out of the 10 children who participated of the study) yielded moderate to large effect sizes. The
made clinically significant improvement on measures study appraisal score was 7 out of 8, suggesting that moder-
of narrative macrostructure. However, pre–post-narrative ately high confidence can be placed in the results.
microstructure data concerning NDW, syntax, and sentence Petersen et al. (in press) investigated the effect of narra-
imitation tasks were nonsignificant. Insufficient data were tive intervention on the macrostructural and microstructural
reported for the calculation of effect sizes. The study language features of the oral narratives produced by three
appraisal score was 4, suggesting that a low level of confi- children, ages 6 to 8 years, with developmental disabilities
dence can be placed on the validity of the study outcomes as and comorbid receptive and expressive language impair-
a function of the narrative intervention. ment. The three participants engaged in 10 60-minute indi-
Peña et al. (2006) evaluated the influence of two 30- vidual narrative intervention sessions following staggered
minute narrative intervention sessions on the macrostruc- baseline trials. Assessment probes eliciting picture- and ver-
ture of narratives produced by 14 children with language bally prompted narratives were recorded and analyzed. All
impairment. A typically developing group of 27 children three children demonstrated considerable gains in episodic
also participated in the intervention (treated as a control construct and the use of story grammar (macrostructure) and
group in this review) and a group of 30 children with lan- causality (microstructure), with moderate to large effect sizes
guage impairment received no treatment (also treated as a based on percentage of nonoverlapping data points.
control group). Children with language impairment Additional gains were noted in areas of microstructure not
assigned to the language impairment treatment group specifically targeted during intervention such as the use of
showed no greater increase in narrative macrostructure adverbs, elaborated noun phrases, mental/linguistic verbs,
from pretest to posttest than the typically developing chil- pronomial reference cohesion, MLU, TNW, and NDW.
dren in the no-treatment control group, suggesting that very Gains were seen both in picture-prompted narratives that
short-term narrative intervention is not beneficial for were the direct focus of intervention and in verbally prompted

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


Petersen 217

narratives, a measure of generalization. Follow-up data were narratives that the children produced. It is, nevertheless,
collected 8 months after intervention, which indicated some important to isolate the causal factors of a particular interven-
maintenance of skills over time. This study received a quality tion so that the clinician’s time and energy (and expense) are
appraisal of 7, indicating that a moderate-high degree of con- focused on the appropriate procedures.
fidence can be placed in the effect of the intervention on the
dependent variables measured.
Microstructure
Even though all of the studies but one (Davies et al., 2004)
Discussion reportedly focused on some aspect of narrative microstruc-
Narrative Intervention Procedures and Materials ture, intervention procedures were typically described in
little detail. Only Petersen et al. (in press) offered insight
There appeared to be limited overlap between the narrative concerning the frequency with which literate microstructure
intervention procedures and materials used in the nine studies was targeted. These authors purposefully emphasized the
reviewed (see Table 6). Each of the studies required partici- microstructure features of causality and temporal relations
pants to retell and/or generate narratives, yet all other while also measuring those features as dependent variables.
described procedures were unique to individual studies, Effect sizes for microstructure resulting from their study
although focused stimulation (Leonard, Camarata, Rowan, & were moderate to large. In contrast, the Gillam et al. (1995)
Chapman, 1982), vertical structuring (Schwartz, Chapman, study reported strongly negative microstructure results for
Terrell, Prelock, & Rowan, 1985), and incidental teaching their narrative intervention group. The reason for the nega-
techniques (Hart & Risley, 1975) were common themes. tive results reported by Gillam et al. and positive results
There was more overlap in the use of materials: Six of the reported by Petersen et al. could be attributed to the degree
nine studies used single photos or pictures to elicit narratives to which narrative microstructure was explicitly targeted
and five used wordless picture books. Five studies had the during intervention. During the Gillam et al. study, narrative
children draw representative pictures of intervention stories. microstructure was never specifically targeted for the narra-
Three of the studies used icons or cue cards and role-playing. tive intervention group, and the control group, which had
All other materials were unique to the individual studies. considerably higher gains in narrative microstructure, par-
ticipated in an intervention founded on direct, explicit
instruction. Petersen et al. strongly emphasized causality,
Macrostructure using a direct, explicit instructional approach in their narra-
The effect sizes for narrative macrostructure were moderate tive intervention. For example, it was reported that causality
to large, ranging from .73 to 1.57, yet there was no clear con- was emphasized by a clinician during one narrative inter-
nection between the extent of positive macrostructure out- vention session 63 times in 45 minutes.
comes and the different intervention procedures and materials Although Petersen et al. (in press) specifically tar-
used. An emphasis on repeated narrative retelling and narra- geted causality in each narrative intervention session, they
tive generation was the only common procedure among the did not specifically target other aspects of microstructure
narrative intervention studies. There is an expressive lan- such as adverbs, elaborated noun phrases, mental and lin-
guage factor inherent in both story retelling and generation, guistic verbs, or reference cohesion. The authors did, how-
and it may be that expressively producing modeled narratives ever, measure those variables because of the nature of their
is key to narrative macrostructure development. Apart from intervention procedures. They found that most measures
the repeated retelling and generation of modeled stories, all of microstructure increased once intervention was imple-
other materials and procedures may be superfluous, not caus- mented, with moderate to large effect sizes. Whereas the
ally related to macrostructure improvement. Thus, narrative Gillam et al. (1995) and Petersen et al. results indicate that
intervention purposed to improve macrostructure could be certain aspects of microstructure such as causal and temporal
simple procedurally. This is not to suggest that methods and relations are best acquired through direct, explicit instruc-
items of interest should not be used to capture the attention of tion, it appears possible that the intensive exposure to narra-
children involved in the narrative intervention sessions. tives that contain various microstructure features may be
Children’s motivation and attention are a foundational requi- sufficient to incidentally improve those features in narration.
site for focused, intensive intervention. In addition, it is Three other studies reportedly focused on the micro-
important to acknowledge the role of the clinician in narra- structure features of causality and temporal relations, how-
tive intervention. Although each study offered limited infor- ever, those features were not measured as dependent
mation concerning the degree and type of scaffolding and variables. More research is needed to clarify the extent to
support that the clinicians offered during intervention, it was which these important microstructure features must be
clear that the clinicians played an active role in shaping the directly targeted in narrative intervention.

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


218 Communication Disorders Quarterly 32(4)

Of the six studies that measured the microstructure fea- that led to independent narrative retellings at the end of each
tures related to length and complexity, all but Gillam et al. intervention session. The results of those studies that focused
(1995) and Swanson et al. (2005) reported moderate to large on generalization and maintenance are not definitive, and
effect sizes. Reasons for the Gillam et al. negative results further investigation concerning these important aspects of
have already been suggested (e.g., limited direct instruction), language acquisition is necessary.
and although Swanson et al. targeted multiple microstructure
features during intervention, only the number of different
words in narratives was measured as a dependent variable. Transfer of Narrative Proficiency
to Academic Success
One of the theoretical underpinnings to narrative interven-
Duration and Intensity of Narrative Intervention tion is that narratives serve as a bridge from oral language to
Only the Peña et al. (2006) study did not show a meaningful written language (Westby, 1985). Because of the decontex-
effect size for any of the dependent variables, and that is tualized, literate nature of narratives, it is possible that narra-
likely due to the limited amount of intervention that the par- tive proficiency will transfer to other academic skills such as
ticipants received (60 total minutes). Most of the other stud- writing and reading comprehension. The Gillam et al. (1995)
ies demonstrated moderate to large effect sizes for narrative study was the only one to examine the extent to which nar-
macrostructure and/or narrative microstructure while imple- rative intervention facilitated progress in written composi-
menting narrative intervention individually or in a small tion. These researchers found a small mean effect size with
group from 320 to 2,160 total minutes of intervention. The macrostructure and a large negative mean effect size with
results of the studies reviewed (Peña et al., 2006, the excep- microstructure when analyzing written narratives. Recall
tion) did not significantly correlate with the duration or that the control group in the Gillam et al. study received
intensity of narrative intervention sessions each week. It can explicit, direct language instruction (oral and written),
be tentatively concluded that children with language impair- whereas the treatment group received a more generalized
ment need more than 60 minutes of narrative intervention to narrative intervention approach. The results from the Gillam
improve their narrative abilities. et al. study indicate that a general narrative intervention
approach may best facilitate macrostructural aspects of writ-
ten narration, whereas direct, explicit instruction may better
Generalization and Maintenance facilitate the development of microstructural features of
of Narrative Intervention written narratives. Future narrative intervention research
If narrative intervention is to be considered effective, it must should explore the degree to which narrative intervention is
be demonstrated that the skills acquired during intervention beneficial in enhancing children’s writing, reading compre-
can be performed without artificial supports over time hension, and other academic skills.
(maintenance) and that those skills will transfer to real-
world situations (generalization). Only Petersen et al. (in
press) investigated the maintenance of narrative skills, Population Sample
with mixed results. Two of the three participants showed Whereas the narrative intervention studies reviewed in this
some maintenance of skills over time, whereas the third par- article focused on children with language impairment, eight
ticipant refused to produce any narratives during the mainte- out of nine exclusively included English-speaking, European
nance phase of the study. The Swanson et al. (2005) study American children attending preschool through sixth grade.
examined generalization, yet the results were nonsignifi- The only exception is the brief Peña et al. (2006) study that
cant, suggesting that the narrative retelling procedures did included mixed ethnicity. No research to date has investi-
not readily transfer to the spontaneous production of narra- gated the effects of sustained narrative intervention on the
tives. The Petersen et al. study also investigated the extent to macrostructure and microstructure of narratives produced
which the narrative intervention would generalize to sponta- by children with language impairment who are culturally
neous language. They found that for most macrostructure and linguistically diverse, or in the seventh grade or higher.
and microstructure measures, generalization was noted. Furthermore, no study that was reviewed included more
Differences between the Petersen et al. and Swanson et al. than 31 participants in the treatment group, and most studies
studies may offer insight as to why generalization occurred included fewer than 10 participants. This relatively homoge-
only in the Petersen et al. study. Petersen et al. targeted nar- neous, limited number of participants reduces the external
rative macrostructure and microstructure during interven- validity of the research findings.
tion at more than twice the rate of Swanson et al., which may
have contributed to the generalization of skills. It is also fea-
sible that the generalization of some skills found in the Social Validity
Petersen et al. study occurred because of the systematic, pur- Narratives are produced from a very young age to convey infor-
poseful introduction and removal of supports and prompts mation, and narrative ability contributes to social and academic

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


Petersen 219

success. There is considerable social validity to improving the progress of their clients by taking frequent, careful data
narrative skills. Both Gillam et al. (1995) and Petersen et al. (in and checking for the transfer of language features in sponta-
press) asked independent listeners to rate the quality of the nar- neous narrative and conversational discourse.
ratives produced by their participants. In each case, the stories
produced by children who had received narrative intervention Declaration of Conflicting Interests
were rated higher. Whereas the participants in the study con- The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
ducted by Petersen et al. demonstrated gains in narrative mac- to the authorship and/or publication of this article.
rostructure and narrative microstructure, the participants
assigned to the narrative intervention condition in the Gillam Funding
et al. study improved only their narrative macrostructure. These The author received no financial support for the research and/or
findings may suggest that it is primarily narrative macrostruc- authorship of this article.
ture that contributes to perceived narrative quality.
References
Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. (1992). Comprehension problems
Summary/Further Research in children with specific language impairment: Literal and
A comprehensive review of the research on narrative inter- inferential meaning. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research,
vention with children who have language impairment 35(1), 119–129.
revealed that narrative intervention with repeated story retell- Boudreau, D. M., & Chapman, R. S. (2000). The relationship
ings and a focus on narrative macrostructure may be suffi- between event representation and linguistic skill in narratives
cient to facilitate a significant improvement in both narrative of children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Journal of
macrostructure and some aspects of narrative microstructure. Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(5), 1146–1159.
However, the majority of the studies reviewed had a limited Boudreau, D. M., & Hedberg, N. L. (1999). A comparison of early
number of participants and were considered lower quality. literacy skills in children with specific language impairment
Caution should be taken when attributing the study outcomes and their typically developing peers. American Journal of
to the intervention applied. Most of the studies included par- Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 249–260.
ticipants who were monolingual, English-speaking European Cirrin, F. M., & Gillam, R. B. (2008). Language intervention prac-
American. Cultural and linguistic diversity is a factor that has tices for school-age children with spoken language disorders:
not been considered. All of the studies that implemented A systematic review. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
intervention for 320 total minutes or longer reported moder- in Schools, 39, 110–137.
ate to large effect sizes on some aspects of narration. Only Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
one study did not result in significant improvement in narra- sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
tive proficiency, and that study implemented narrative inter- Davies, P., Shanks, B., & Davies, K. (2004). Improving narrative
vention for only 60 minutes. Furthermore, only two studies skills in young children with delayed language development.
probed for the generalization of narrative skills acquired dur- Educational Review, 56(3), 271–286.
ing intervention to the spontaneous production of narratives. Fey, M. E., Catts, H. W., Proctor-Williams, K., Tomblin, J. B., &
Only one study reported data on maintenance of narrative Zhang, X. (2004). Oral and written story composition skills of
skills following intervention, with mixed results. Thus far, children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Lan-
only one study has investigated the extent to which narrative guage, and Hearing Research, 47(6), 1301–1318.
proficiency transfers to other language-related academic Gillam, S., & Gillam, R. (2006). Making evidence-based decisions
skills such as writing. about child language intervention in schools. Language, Speech,
The results of the research included in this review warrant and Hearing Services in Schools, 37, 304–315.
further investigation into the effects of narrative intervention Gillam, R. B., & Johnston, J. R. (1992). Spoken and written lan-
on the narrative macrostructure and microstructure of guage relationships in language/learning-impaired and nor-
children who have language impairment. More research is mally achieving school-age children. Journal of Speech &
needed to establish the efficacy and effectiveness of narra- Hearing Research, 35(6), 1303–1315.
tive-based language intervention. In the meantime, clinicians Gillam, R., McFadden, T. U., & van Kleek, A. (1995). Improv-
can continue to treat narratives as a functional language tar- ing the narrative abilities of children with language disor-
get and as a medium whereby language features are modeled ders: Whole language and language skills approaches. In
and practiced. Clinicians should provide multiple opportuni- M. Fey, J. Windsor, & J. Reichle (Eds.) Communication inter-
ties for participants to practice retelling and generating model vention for school-age children (pp. 145–182). Baltimore, MD:
narratives and, when specifically targeting microstructure, Paul H. Brookes.
strategically model and elicit correct language forms with Gillam, R. B., & Pearson, N. (2004). Test of Narrative Language.
considerable intensity. Clinicians should carefully monitor Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015


220 Communication Disorders Quarterly 32(4)

Greenhalgh, K. S., & Strong, C. J. (2001). Literate language fea- Schwartz, R. G., Chapman, K., Terrell, B. Y., Prelock, P.,
tures in spoken narratives of children with typical language & Rowan, L. (1985). Facilitating word combination in
and children with language impairments. Language, Speech, language-impaired children through discourse structure.
and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 114–125. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50,
Hart, B. M., & Risley, T. R. (1975). Incidental teaching of language in 31–39.
the preschool. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 8, 411–420. Scott, C. M., & Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance
Hayward, D. V., & Schneider, P. (2000). Effectiveness of teach- measures in spoken and written narrative and expository dis-
ing story grammar knowledge to pre-school children with course of school-age children with language learning disabilities.
language impairment: An exploratory study. Child Language Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(2),
Teaching and Therapy, 16(3), 255–284. 324–340.
Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom, S. L., Snow, C. E. (1991). The theoretical basis for relationships between
& Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-subject research to language and literacy in development. Journal of Research in
identify evidence-based practices in special education. Excep- Childhood Education, 6, 5–10.
tional Children, 71, 165–179. Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story com-
Klecan-Aker, J., Flahive, L. K., & Fleming, S. (1997). Teaching prehension in elementary school children. In R. O. Freedle
storytelling to a group of children with learning disabilities: (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (pp. 53–120).
A look at treatment outcomes. Contemporary Issues in Com- Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
munication Science and Disorders, 24, 23–32. Strong, C. J., & Shaver, J. P. (1991). Stability of cohesion in the
Leonard, L. B., Camarata, S., Rowan, L. E., & Chapman, K. (1982). spoken narratives of language-impaired and normally devel-
The communicative functions of lexical usage by language- oping school-aged children. Journal of Speech and Hearing
impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 3, 109–125. Research, 34, 95–111.
Mayer, M. (1977). Oops. New York: Dial Press. Swanson, L. A., Fey, M. E., Mills, C. E., & Hood, L. S. (2005).
Newman, R. M., & McGregor, K. K. (2006). Teachers and layper- Use of narrative-based language intervention with children
sons discern quality differences between narratives produced who have specific language impairment. American Journal of
by children with or without SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, Speech-Language Pathology, 14, 131–143.
and Hearing Research, 49(5), 1022–1036. Thalheimer, W., & Cook, S. (2002). How to calculate effect sizes
Nippold, M. A., & Taylor, C. L. (1995). Idiom understanding in from published research articles: A simplified methodology.
youth: Further examination of familiarity and transparency. Retrieved December 24, 2008, from http://work-learning.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 426–433. com/effect_sizes.htm
Peña, E. D., Gillam, R. B., Malek, M., Ruiz-Felter, R., Resen- Tyler, A. A., & Sandoval, K. T. (1994). Preschoolers with phonologi-
diz, M., Fiestas, C., & Sabel, T. (2006). Dynamic assessment cal and language disorders: Treating different linguistic domains.
of school-age children’s narrative ability: An experimental Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 25, 215–234.
investigation of classification accuracy. Journal of Speech, Westby, C. E. (1985). Learning to talk—talking to learn: Oral-
Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 1037–1057. literate language differences. In C. S. Simon (Ed.), Communi-
Petersen, D. B., Gillam, S., & Gillam, R. B. (2008). Emerging cation skills and classroom success: Therapy methodologies
procedures in narrative assessment: The index of narrative for language-learning disabled students (pp. 181–218). San
complexity. Topics in Language Disorders, 28(2), 111–126. Diego: College Hill Press.
Petersen, D. B., Gillam, S., Spencer, T. D., & Gillam, R. B. (in Westby, C. E. (1999). Assessing and facilitating text comprehension
press). The effects of literate narrative intervention on children problems. In H. W. Catts, & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), Language and
with language impairment: An early stage study. Journal of reading disabilities (pp. 154–221). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguis- About the Author
tics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New York: Douglas B. Petersen, PhD, CCC-SLP, currently teaches and con-
Plenum Press. ducts research in the Division of Communication Disorders at the
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest University of Wyoming. His interests include narrative assessment
systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversa- and intervention and the identification of early literacy difficulty in
tion. Language, 50(4), 696–735. culturally and linguistically diverse populations.

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at FUDAN UNIV LIB on May 10, 2015

You might also like