You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/221765228

The value of a non-sport-specific motor test battery in predicting


performance in young female gymnasts

Article  in  Journal of Sports Sciences · March 2012


DOI: 10.1080/02640414.2012.654399 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

88 1,407

7 authors, including:

Barbara Vandorpe Joric Vandendriessche


Ghent University Ghent University
12 PUBLICATIONS   1,293 CITATIONS    12 PUBLICATIONS   1,318 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Roel Vaeyens Johan Pion


Ghent University Hogeschool Arnhem and Nijmegen
79 PUBLICATIONS   7,332 CITATIONS    59 PUBLICATIONS   1,827 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Motor coordination and sport talent in children View project

Talent identification within cycling View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Johan Pion on 24 March 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


This article was downloaded by: [Johan Pion]
On: 31 January 2012, At: 09:19
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Sports Sciences


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rjsp20

The value of a non-sport-specific motor test battery in


predicting performance in young female gymnasts
a a a a
Barbara Vandorpe , Joric B. Vandendriessche , Roel Vaeyens , Johan Pion , Johan
b a a
Lefevre , Renaat M. Philippaerts & Matthieu Lenoir
a
Department of Movement and Sport Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences,
Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
b
Department of Biomedical Kinesiology, Faculty of Kinesiology and Rehabilitation Sciences,
KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

Available online: 23 Jan 2012

To cite this article: Barbara Vandorpe, Joric B. Vandendriessche, Roel Vaeyens, Johan Pion, Johan Lefevre, Renaat M.
Philippaerts & Matthieu Lenoir (2012): The value of a non-sport-specific motor test battery in predicting performance in
young female gymnasts, Journal of Sports Sciences, DOI:10.1080/02640414.2012.654399

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2012.654399

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to
anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents
will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should
be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims,
proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in
connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
Journal of Sports Sciences, 2012; 1–9, iFirst article

The value of a non-sport-specific motor test battery in predicting


performance in young female gymnasts

BARBARA VANDORPE1, JORIC B. VANDENDRIESSCHE1, ROEL VAEYENS1,


JOHAN PION1, JOHAN LEFEVRE2, RENAAT M. PHILIPPAERTS1, & MATTHIEU LENOIR1
1
Department of Movement and Sport Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
and 2Department of Biomedical Kinesiology, Faculty of Kinesiology and Rehabilitation Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

(Accepted 1 January 2012)

Abstract
Downloaded by [Johan Pion] at 09:19 31 January 2012

Gymnastics talent identification focuses on the identification of young gymnasts who display characteristics for potential
success in the future. The aim of this study was to identify which current performance characteristics are related to
performance in competition 2 years later. Twenty-three female gymnasts aged 7–8 years completed a multidimensional test
battery measuring anthropometric, physical, and coordinative characteristics and were technically evaluated by expert
coaches. Two years later, the all-around competition results of those gymnasts now participating in elite (n ¼ 12) and sub-
elite (n ¼ 11) competition were obtained. None of the initial measurements significantly correlated with the results of the
sub-elite gymnasts 2 years later. For the elite gymnasts, a non-sport-specific motor test battery correlated strongly with the
competition result, with more than 40% of the variation in competition performance being explained by the result on that
test 2 years earlier. Neither the coaches’ judgement nor the anthropometric and physical characteristics were sensitive
enough to predict performance. A motor coordination test might be valuable in the early identification of gymnasts, as its
discriminative and predictive qualities might be sufficiently powerful for selection within a relatively homogeneous
population of gymnasts exhibiting similar anthropometric and physical profiles.

Keywords: Talent identification, motor coordination, anthropometry, physical tests, expert judgement, longitudinal study

training (Russell, 2010), early identification from the


Introduction
age of 6 years is common practice (Prescott, 1999).
A preoccupation of many sport federations, clubs, In the quest for expertise, talent identification is
and coaches is recognizing current participants with preceded by talent detection (i.e. discovering poten-
the potential to excel in that particular sport (De tial performers not currently involved in the sport;
Bosscher, Bingham, Shibli, van Bottenburg, & De Williams & Reilly, 2000) and succeeded by talent
Knop, 2008; Régnier, Salmela, & Russell, 1993; development (i.e. the ongoing process of guidance
Williams & Reilly, 2000). This process, often towards high-level competition by providing the
referred to as talent identification, is especially athlete with a suitable learning environment; Wil-
important in small countries with a relatively small liams & Reilly, 2000). Traditional talent identifica-
pool of talent where it is unrealistic to assume that tion models in gymnastics and other sports select
gifted individuals will automatically make it onto the athletes by considering a test battery consisting of a
sports scene (Vaeyens, Güllich, Warr, & Philip- combination of physical, anthropometric, psycholo-
paerts, 2009). Moreover, identification of gifted gical, sociological, coordinative or technical variables
athletes would allow proper monitoring through the (Régnier et al., 1993; Vaeyens, Lenoir, Williams, &
developmental process from child prodigy to elite, Philippaerts, 2008). Cross-sectional studies have
thus increasing the likelihood of success (Williams & highlighted the importance of an appropriate build
Reilly, 2000). In women’s gymnastics, senior status and physical profile for performance in gymnastics by
is reached at the age of 16 years. Since the attainment distinguishing young gymnasts from other athletic
of international success requires a large amount of populations or controls (Bencke et al., 2001;
intense preparation, which takes about 8–10 years of Maffulli, King, & Helms, 1994; Prescott, 1999), or

Correspondence: M. Lenoir, Department of Movement and Sport Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Ghent University, Watersportlaan 2,
B-9000 Ghent, Belgium. E-mail: matthieu.lenoir@ugent.be
ISSN 0264-0414 print/ISSN 1466-447X online Ó 2012 Taylor & Francis
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2012.654399
2 B. Vandorpe et al.

predicting their current performance based upon one and flexibility in the hip and shoulder joints was
or more of the aforementioned attributes (Bradshaw responsible for producing a perfect fit for classifying
& Le Rossignol, 2004; Claessens, Lefevre, Beunen, the future performance of 33 female gymnasts as
& Malina, 1999). However, in the course of the successful (n ¼ 12; achieving at least 80% the
development from childhood to adulthood, anthro- performance score of the highest scoring gymnast)
pometric and physical characteristics might be or unsuccessful (n ¼ 21; scoring below 80%). Since
influenced by growth, maturation, and/or training Prescott’s definition of future performance was based
(Malina, Bouchard, & Bar-Or, 2004; Pearson, upon this dichotomization, the talent characteristics
Naughton, & Torode, 2006). Therefore, researchers could therefore only be used to distinguish between
have argued that such studies have focused on those two groups of gymnasts.
identifying good performance at the time of testing The present longitudinal study aimed to identify
rather than discovering potential for future success those talent characteristics that best predict gymnas-
(Philippaerts, Coutts, & Vaeyens, 2008; Vaeyens tics success in the following years by implementing a
et al., 2008; Williams & Reilly, 2000; Wolstencroft, multidimensional test battery. It was hypothesized
2002). that a motor coordination test would be a valuable
Adequate motor coordination is widely thought to contributor to future success in gymnastics competi-
be a necessary building block for the development of tion. In addition, the role of the expert coach in the
fundamental motor skills required for everyday initial screening for gifted gymnasts was taken into
functioning in childhood and across the lifespan account, since the recommendation of coaches is still
Downloaded by [Johan Pion] at 09:19 31 January 2012

(Henderson & Sugden, 1992; Seefeldt, 1980). In used in practice.


addition, the acquisition of fundamental motor skills
is often considered a prerequisite for the learning of
specific skills in sports (Clark & Metcalfe, 2002; Methods
Malina et al., 2004) and even the ability to excel in
Participants
sport (Moore, Collins, Burwitz, & Jess, 1998). In line
with Burton and Miller (1998), who stated that the In 2008, 33 young female competitive gymnasts aged
purpose of a motor coordination test is not to 7–8 years from 21 Flemish gymnastics clubs took
measure performance of skills but the general traits part in the annual selection day of the Flemish
underlying them, a motor coordination test might be Gymnastics Federation for inclusion in the national
valuable in the search for gymnastics potential. talent development programme for becoming an elite
Indeed, recent findings show moderate to high senior gymnast. All gymnasts completed a multi-
long-term stability in motor coordination from dimensional test battery. In addition, six expert
childhood to adolescence (Ahnert, Schneider, & coaches of the National Training Center, including
Bös, 2009), underlining its possible value for talent the head coach of the Belgian national team, each
identification. However, there is a scarcity of subjectively evaluated the morphology of the gym-
information regarding the motor coordination of nasts and the technical execution of basic gymnastics
gymnasts, which might be attributed to a lack of elements on the four pieces of apparatus by means of
consensus regarding the assessment of this attribute. a 5-item qualitative scale ranging from 0 (‘‘weak’’) to
Previous research has focused on perceptual-motor 2 (‘‘very good’’) with 0.5 intervals. At the end of the
measurements involving static and dynamic balance testing day, the coaches gathered to form a unan-
(Bressel, Yonker, Kras, & Heath, 2007; Prescott, imous decision about each gymnast’s future poten-
1999), postural control (Carrick, Oggero, Pagnacco, tial, based upon their observations independent of
Brock, & Arikan, 2007; Prescott, 1999), and the results on the test battery, and assigned them to
kinaesthetic ability (Prescott, 1999), using difficult one of two groups: ‘‘potentially elite’’ and ‘‘poten-
laboratory procedures that have limited applicability tially sub-elite’’. In Flanders in 2008–2010, there
and reliability (Prescott, 1999). were two competitive levels for girls under 11 years
Although the prediction of future success is an of age, with the A-level (referred to as ‘‘elite’’ in this
important criterion for effective talent identification article) being the highest and the B-level (referred to
test batteries (Kearney, 1999; Régnier et al., 1993), as ‘‘sub-elite’’ in this article) being the lowest of the
there is a lack of longitudinal research aimed at two levels. According to the coaches, the selected
predicting future performance in gymnastics. In one gymnasts were the ones with the potential to become
of the few longitudinal studies, Prescott (1999) elite gymnasts and would thus probably compete at
determined which talent characteristics were able to A-level in the next few years (‘‘potentially elite’’,
predict performance in women’s gymnastics 2 years selected: n ¼ 19; ‘‘potentially sub-elite’’, not selected:
later. Prescott revealed that a combination of n ¼ 14).
anthropometric components, local muscular endur- In 2010, the competition results of those gym-
ance in the abdominals and hip flexor muscle groups, nasts, aged to 9–10 years, now competing at the elite
Prediction of gymnastics performance 3

(n ¼ 12) and sub-elite (n ¼ 11) levels were obtained. Body mass (to the nearest 0.1 kg) and percent body
Ten gymnasts were not found on the result sheets fat (to the nearest 0.1%) were determined by means
due to injury or having dropped out from the sport of a bio-electrical impedance scale (TANITA BC-
(Figure 1). 420SMA, Weda B.V., Naarden, Holland).
Permission for this study was received from the To estimate the somatic maturity of the girls,
local Ethics Committee of the Ghent University maturity offset was predicted by means of a gender-
Hospital. For all participants, informed parental specific regression equation reported by Mirwald and
consent was obtained. colleagues (Mirwald, Baxter-Jones, Bailey, & Beu-
nen, 2002) based upon a non-invasive technique
using chronological age, height, body mass, sitting
Multidimensional test battery and expert judgement
height, and leg length measurements: 79.376 þ
The relationship between competition results and (0.0001882*LegLength SittingHeight Interaction) þ
both multidimensional talent characteristics and (0.0022*Age LegLength Interaction) þ (0.005841*
expert judgements was examined using a long- Age SittingHeight Interaction)7(0.002658*Age Body
itudinal design. In 2008, female artistic gymnasts Mass Interaction) þ (0.07693*BodyMass Height Ra-
performed a generic test battery of five anthropo- tio). Age at peak height velocity was then derived by
metric, seven physical performance, and two co- extracting maturity offset from chronological age. Since
ordinative tests. In addition, six expert elite coaches there is an estimation error to the aforementioned
evaluated the general appearance and technical equation and reliability will improve as the chronolo-
Downloaded by [Johan Pion] at 09:19 31 January 2012

abilities of the gymnasts on the four pieces of gical age of the child approaches age at peak height
apparatus. Assessments were conducted at a fully velocity, care is warranted in using this equation to
equipped gymnastics training centre by a group of predict the exact age at peak height velocity. However,
qualified examiners who followed standardized test the equation can be used to give an indication of a
protocols. The tests were arranged as a circuit, with child’s maturity status compared with same-aged peers
the gymnasts moving from one station to another in (Malina et al., 2006).
the following order: anthropometry, coordination,
physical performance, and technical gymnastics Physical performance. Flexibility was assessed by the
abilities. The gymnasts performed the tests barefoot, sit-and-reach (0.1 cm) test of the Eurofit test battery
wearing a leotard. Training status at initial testing (Council of Europe, 1988). Explosive leg power was
was also recorded. In 2010, the all-around results evaluated with the countermovement jump (0.1 cm),
from an actual competition were obtained. performed with hands on hips, using Optojump
(Microgate, Bolzano, Italy) (Cometti & Cometti,
Anthropometry. The anthropometric assessments 2007) and standing broad jump of the Eurofit test
were made following standardized protocols (Loh- battery (Council of Europe, 1988). The knee push-
man, Roche, & Martorell, 1988). Height was ups and sit-ups tests, following the Bruininks-
measured using a portable stadiometer to the nearest Oseretsky test of Motor Proficiency-2 procedures
0.1 cm (Harpenden, Holtain Ltd., Crymych, UK). (BOT-2; Bruininks & Bruininks, 2005), were used to

Figure 1. Flow chart of the selection procedure. Note: Gymnasts who are selected by the coaches for further development under guidance of
the Gymnastics Federation are not however obliged to follow the elite route. In Flanders, gymnasts aged 9–10 years of age may choose to
compete at the elite or sub-elite level.
4 B. Vandorpe et al.

assess upper body strength and core stability. The coaches of the senior national team observed their
gymnasts’ sprinting ability was measured by a 20 m general appearance and gymnastics abilities on all
sprint test with sprint times being recorded using four pieces of apparatus. Each of the six coaches then
Polifemo light photocells with 0.001 s accuracy awarded a score from zero to two (0, 0.5, 1, 1.5, 2),
(Racetime2, Microgate, Bolzano, Italy) (Lidor, ranging from weak to very good in their opinion, to
Hershko, Bilkevitz, Arnon, & Falk, 2007). Anaerobic each gymnast individually. The six scores were then
performance was assessed with a one-minute rope summed yielding a maximum possible score of 12.
jumping test (Vandorpe et al., 2011b). Inter-rater reliability of the six coaches was very good
(Cronbach’s alpha ¼ 0.888; P 5 0.001).
Motor coordination. The KörperkoordinationsTest
für Kinder (KTK; Kiphard & Schilling, 2007) is a Competition results. Since at the top level gymnasts
widely used, valid, and reliable instrument to assess only get one chance (e.g. Olympic Games) to prove
the general motor coordination of children (Ahnert their abilities, one competition was selected to
et al., 2009; Lopes, Rodrigues, Maia, & Malina, provide an indication of the competition perfor-
2011; Vandorpe et al., 2011a). The Körperkoordi- mance of the gymnasts. Hence, the all-around result
nationsTest für Kinder consists of four subtests: (1) at the 2010 Flemish Gymnastics Championship,
walking backwards three times along each of three which is the last and most important competition of
balance beams of decreasing width, with a possible the season for which gymnasts of that age group peak
maximum score of 72; (2) moving sideways on for, was used because all the skills they acquired that
Downloaded by [Johan Pion] at 09:19 31 January 2012

wooden platforms in 20 s with the score being the season were most likely on show in that final
sum of the number of relocations over two trials; (3) competition. The gymnasts were judged according
jumping sideways with two feet over a wooden slat in to Code Of Points 2009–2012 (COP) of the
15 s, with the score being the sum of the number of Fédération Internationale de Gymnastique (FIG)
jumps over two trials; and (4) hopping for height on with panels of three National/Brevet judges per
one leg over a foam obstacle increasing in height by apparatus. In Flanders, the youngest age groups,
5 cm at each step, with a possible maximum score of up to the age of 11 years, are required to perform
78. The raw performance scores of each subtest are compulsory routines, developed and choreographed
transformed into age- and gender-specific motor by the Flemish Gymnastics Federation Technical
quotients, together resulting in a general motor Committee, with the emphasis on technique, form,
quotient (MQKTK). and execution. In these compulsories, the maximum
In addition, nine basic tests (running backwards, possible Difficulty score (D-score) on each apparatus
skipping, hopping, shuffle pass, cross steps, boun- is 10 and gymnasts could be rewarded with a perfect
cing, jumping jacks, tuck jumps, and giant jumps) Execution score (E-score) of 10. The sum of the D-
were judged on a 10-item qualitative scale (i.e. ability and E-scores gives the final apparatus score. Sum-
to perform the movement, without falling, with a ming the four apparatus scores results in the all-
steady rhythm, supported by arm movements, around score with a maximum of 80. Unfortunately,
balanced, with confidence, dynamic, without sloppi- since the elite and sub-elite levels required the
ness, with sufficient amplitude, and seemingly performance of different compulsory exercises (diffi-
effortless) to determine the basic locomotion skills cult vs. easy), further comparisons of the competition
of the gymnasts, with a possible maximum score of results of the two levels was not possible.
90. Reliability coefficients were high (test–retest ¼
0.94, inter-rater reliability ¼ 0.93) (Vandorpe et al.,
Statistical analysis
2011b).
Descriptive statistics were obtained using SPSS
Training status. Training status at the time of initial v.15.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago IL, USA). To identify
testing was requested prior to test assessment via the differences between the two levels in the parameters
coaches and was expressed as previous training measured at initial testing, an independent samples t-
experience in years and actual training hours per test was used. For those variables with a limited
week. amount of data (training status), the Mann-Whitney
U-test was applied. Pearson’s product–moment
Expert coaches’ judgement. Four coaches of the junior correlations were conducted to assess the relation-
national team at the National Training Centre were ship between the variables measured at initial testing
each assigned to one of the four pieces of apparatus and the eventual competition outcome. Again,
(vault, uneven bars, balance beam, floor exercise) in considering the small number of gymnasts for which
order to independently and subjectively evaluate the training status data were available, the Spearman
gymnasts’ morphological build and technical ability rank-order correlation was used to reveal the
on that particular apparatus. Simultaneously, two relationship between training status at year of initial
Prediction of gymnastics performance 5

testing and competition results 2 years later. In line standard deviations of the two levels on all para-
with Cohen (1988), the magnitude of the correlation meters are reported in Vandorpe et al. (2011b). In
coefficient (r) was considered to be 0.1 small, 0.3 line with Vandorpe et al., the present results revealed
moderate, and 0.5 large. Due to the limited sample that the absolute scores of the elite gymnasts were
size, gaining insights into the contribution of each of systematically in favour of those of the sub-elite
the parameters in predicting future success in gymnasts, but statistical significance was only
competition, by applying multiple regression analy- achieved for knee push-ups, motor quotient of the
sis, was not appropriate. As a rule of thumb, there KörperkoordinationsTest für Kinder, jumping side-
should be 10 cases for each predictor in the model to ways, basic locomotion skills, and the sum score of
obtain a reliable regression model (Field, 2009). the six expert coaches.
Therefore, linear regression analysis was applied From the initial test battery, only the motor
using the Enter method for only those variables that quotient of the KörperkoordinationsTest für Kinder,
correlated significantly with the outcome (result in as a measure of motor coordination, and one of its
competition). Statistical significance was set at subtests (moving sideways) correlated significantly
P 5 0.05. with the competition result of the elite gymnasts 2
years later (MQKTK: r ¼ 0.67, P ¼ 0.02; moving
sideways: r ¼ 0.60, P ¼ 0.04), indicating that the
Results
higher the general motor coordination value 2 years
Table I presents the results of the multidimensional before competition, the higher the gymnast scored in
Downloaded by [Johan Pion] at 09:19 31 January 2012

test battery, training status, and expert judgements that competition. None of the anthropometric or
measured at year 1 of the Flemish gymnastics talent physical performance variables correlated signifi-
development programme and the results in competi- cantly with later competition outcomes. Training
tion obtained in year 3. In addition, correlation status at initial testing as well as the expert judgement
coefficients between all variables and the all-around of coaches did not correlate significantly with the
competition result 2 years later are also shown in result in competition 2 years later either. Similarly,
Table I. Results of comparison of the means and for the sub-elite gymnasts, none of the initially

Table I. Means, standard deviations, and t-values of the comparison between the results of the two levels on the generic test battery, training
history, and expert judgement measured at year 1 and correlation coefficients of the parameters measured at year 1 and the competition
results 2 years later (year 3).

Sub-elite Independent Competition


Elite (mean + s) (mean + s) samples Competition score score year 3,
Variable (n ¼ 12) (n ¼ 11) t-test [t(P)] year 3, elite [r(P)] sub-elite [r(P)]

Competition year 3, elite 62.3 + 6.3 – –


Competition year 3, sub-elite 61.8 + 8.8 – –
Height (cm) 123.0 + 6.5 126.6 + 4.3 1.53 0.08 0.23
Body mass (kg) 22.7 + 2.6 24.3 + 3.2 1.32 0.23 0.13
Body fat (%) 15.5 + 3.1 15.5 + 3.1 70.03 0.37 0.32
Age at peak height velocity (years) 11.6 + 0.3 11.5 + 0.2 71.03 70.06 70.43
Sit-and-reach (cm) 31.0 + 2.3 31.0 + 2.3 70.08 70.37 0.16
Countermovement jump (cm) 25.4 + 3.0 25.0 + 1.7 70.41 70.10 0.19
Knee push-ups (n/30 s) 32.8 + 5.0 26.3 + 6.0 72.85* 0.30 0.30
Sit-ups (n/30 s) 34.3 + 8.1 29.3 + 3.9 71.89 0.14 0.27
Standing broad jump (m) 161.7 + 12.1 154.0 + 5.7 71.92 0.42 0.05
20 m sprint (s) 3.9 + 0.2 4.0 + 0.2 1.29 0.10 70.11
Rope jumping (s) 75.2 + 17.0 70.8 + 17.1 70.61 70.04 0.17
MQKTK 133.7 + 4.3 129.4 + 5.3 72.16* 0.67* 70.27
Walking backwards 69.3 + 3.6 65.8 + 4.6 71.99 70.58 70.19
Moving sideways 40.3 + 4.7 38.9 + 4.5 70.70 0.60* 70.07
Jumping sideways 73.0 + 8.1 67.4 + 3.4 72.13* 0.43 70.37
Hopping for height 68.2 + 8.1 68.7 + 8.2 0.16 0.23 70.17
Basic locomotion skills 74.8 + 8.1 66.7 + 10.4 72.10* 0.08 0.42
Training experience (years) 2.3 + 0.7 2.4 + 1.3 20.0þ 0.48# 70.12#
Training status (hours per week) 11.9 + 2.5 10.8 + 1.8 19.5þ 0.56# 0.32#
Sum 6 expert coaches 9.6 + 2.8 5.0 + 4.2 73.13* 0.29 0.23
#
Non-parametric Spearman correlation coefficient (elite: n ¼ 9; sub-elite: n ¼ 6).
þ
Non-parametric Mann-Whitney U-test (elite: n ¼ 9; sub-elite: n ¼ 6).
*P 5 0.05.
6 B. Vandorpe et al.

measured parameters correlated significantly with Damsgaard, Bencke, Matthiesen, Petersen, & Mul-
competition outcome 2 years later. ler, 2000; Malina, 1999), the small size of all
By means of linear regression analysis, we exam- gymnasts in the current sample can be attributed to
ined the predictive value of those variables that previous natural or systematic selection by their
correlated significantly with the outcome (result in coach(es). Thus, the anthropometric measurements
competition) – the motor quotient of the Körper- used in this study cannot be used to distinguish
koordinationsTest für Kinder and moving sideways. within a group of gymnasts and may therefore be
Only for the elite gymnasts did the coordination test more relevant in talent detection rather than identi-
battery significantly predict the outcome. The motor fication processes. Searching for small and light girls
quotient of the KörperkoordinationsTest für Kinder, for gymnastics is beneficial due to the rules of the
measured at initial testing, predicted the results in sport, rewarding difficult acrobatic movements in
competition 2 years later (b ¼ 0.98, t11 ¼ 2.82, combination with grace and artistry. Accordingly,
P ¼ 0.02), and explained a significant proportion of the importance of a small stature for both aesthetic
variance in competition score (R2 ¼ 0.44, F1,11 ¼ and biomechanical advantages has been previously
7.95, P ¼ 0.02) (Figure 2). Moving sideways mea- highlighted (Bencke et al., 2002; Claessens et al.,
sured at initial testing also predicted the results in 1999; Malina, 1999; Russell, 2010).
competition 2 years later (b ¼ 0.80, t11 ¼ 2.38, Similarly, physical performance variables mea-
P ¼ 0.04), with moving sideways explaining a sig- sured at the first test assessment did not correlate
nificant proportion of variance in competition score significantly with results in competition 2 years later.
Downloaded by [Johan Pion] at 09:19 31 January 2012

(R2 ¼ 0.36, F1,11 ¼ 5.64, P ¼ 0.04). In line with previous studies, the absolute values of
the measured variables in this study did however
tend to indicate that the physical performance
Discussion
variables are able to separate gifted gymnasts from
The aim of the present longitudinal study was to their less gifted peers, with the gifted performers
investigate the predictive value of a test battery showing better strength, flexibility, speed, and
measuring anthropometric, physical performance endurance than their less successful counterparts
and motor coordination characteristics, training (Prescott, 1999; Vandorpe et al., 2011b). The
status, and the technical judgement of expert coaches findings of the present study are in agreement with
to predict eventual competition outcomes for female those of Philippaerts et al. (2008), who reported that
gymnasts. general physical performance characteristics do not
None of the anthropometric variables assessed at seem sensitive enough to discriminate at the top level
initial testing correlated significantly with competi- in many sports. Considering the possible impact of
tion performance 2 years later. This might be training, growth, and maturation on these variables
explained by the homogeneity of the current sample. (Malina et al., 2004; Pearson et al., 2006), predicting
Indeed, previous studies (Prescott, 1999; Vandorpe future performances based upon these variables is
et al., 2011b) revealed that competitive gymnasts difficult. It seems that physical performance mea-
generally exhibit similar anthropometric profiles sures reflect current ability rather than potential to
from as early an age as 6 years. Since growth and excel (Philippaerts et al., 2008; Vaeyens et al., 2008;
maturation are not affected by intensive training Wolstencroft, 2002). Thus, even though the sample
(Baxter-Jones, Thompson, & Malina, 2002; in the present study was rather small, the lack of
significant correlations between performance results
and previous training status is probably also evi-
denced in the homogeneity of the entire group,
having experienced similar systematic training pro-
grammes (Vandorpe et al., 2011b).
Interestingly, based upon the coaches’ judge-
ments, no significant predictions of eventual compe-
tition outcomes could be made either. Traditionally,
initial screening for gifted gymnasts is based upon
the recommendation of coaches (Prescott, 1999).
The elite coach is able to separate the cream from the
top fairly accurately (Vandorpe et al., 2011b), but the
present results indicated experts had difficulties
discriminating within a group of either elite or sub-
Figure 2. Correlation plot with regression line of the relationship of
elite gymnasts. Indeed, in addition to the physical
MQKTK score versus performance results at the Flemish performance measurements, it is possible that the
Gymnastics Championship 2 years later. coaches’ judgements relied on already developed
Prediction of gymnastics performance 7

traits rather than the discovery of potential talent. tion, whereas jumping sideways and hopping for
This finding could however also be attributed to the height also require strength in combination with
nature of the instrument that was used to quantify speed for good test results. Since gymnastics training
expert judgement. Since the aim of the coaches was involves some reactivity training, the inability of
to identify the elite-level potential gymnasts and not jumping sideways and hopping for height to distin-
to rank them from the least to most talented, this guish within the current sample of gymnasts might
method was probably not sensitive enough to again be attributed to its homogeneity due to similar
distinguish within the homogeneous samples of training experiences or previous selection. In addi-
either the elite or sub-elite gymnasts. tion, the ceiling effect on the walking backwards test
The usefulness of the KörperkoordinationsTest might also reflect the homogeneity of the gymnasts
für Kinder (Kiphard & Schilling, 2007) for gymnas- regarding balance tasks, being preselected or highly
tics talent identification was demonstrated as the trained on that performance variable.
coordination test battery was not only able to The criteria necessary for an effective talent
distinguish between elite and sub-elite gymnasts identification test battery have been suggested
(Vandorpe et al., 2011b), but also predicted the (Kearney, 1999; Régnier et al., 1993) to include
results (440%) of the top (elite) gymnasts in highly predictive, stable variables (only minimally
competition 2 years later. Therefore, it might be impacted by training and/or growth and maturation),
concluded that in a homogeneous sample of elite relevant to performance, with sufficient validity,
gymnasts, the relative importance of anthropometric reliability, objectivity, and applicability. Previous
Downloaded by [Johan Pion] at 09:19 31 January 2012

and physical performance variables to distinguish studies have highlighted its stability (Ahnert et al.,
between the gymnasts decreases, whereas the role of 2009), validity, reliability, objectivity, and applic-
motor coordination increases. Motor coordination as ability in normal as well as sport-specific gymnastics
assessed by the KörperkoordinationsTest für Kinder populations (Kiphard & Schilling, 2007; Vandorpe
seems to measure a general trait underlying a wide et al., 2011a, 2011b). The present study highlighted
variety of skills and is sensitive enough to give an the value of measuring motor coordination by means
indication about the potential to develop rather than of the KörperkoordinationsTest für Kinder by
to determine current performance. Indeed, as adding to the criterion of prediction.
Williams and Ford (2009) argued, at the top level Although the test battery used in this study was
(elite), being more skilled than the other gymnasts quite extensive, we acknowledge its limitation by not
will determine who wins medals. On the other hand, including important psychological (Abbott & Col-
at a lower level (sub-elite), the importance of either lins, 2004) as well as environmental (Baker &
parameter in predicting competition outcomes is less Horton, 2004; Côté, 1999) factors, which might also
strong. The larger standard deviations of the results substantially contribute to the outcome of the
of the sub-elite gymnasts indicate that they are a multidimensional developmental process from gift-
more heterogeneous group with respect to motor edness to talent (Gagné, 2004). However, in agree-
coordination. Hence, the sub-elite gymnasts might ment with Falk and colleagues (Falk, Lidor, Lander,
compensate a deficiency in the coordination domain & Lang, 2004) and Baker and Horton (2004), we
by better physical capacities. At the elite level, a believe that the influence of the environment is more
similar compensation mechanism is less likely as the on talent development than talent identification. In
difficulty of the programme requires them to be well- our opinion, the basic critical antecedents of expert
coordinated and have sufficient physical capacities. performance were addressed in this study. Baker and
Regarding the non-sport-specific nature of the test, it Horton (2004) described this as a metaphor, with the
could be suggested that this test battery could also be genes determining the size of an empty bucket while
used for talent detection and identification purposes the environment determines its content. In other
in other sports. Considering the fairly accurate words, even with the best support and willpower, a
prediction of future gymnastics performance of tall and heavy child with low coordination will be
moving sideways (435%), when time is limited or unlikely to reach the top in gymnastics.
when a lot of children need to be measured, the In conclusion, anthropometric and physical per-
administration of moving sideways only might be a formance tests as well as coach judgements were less
valuable substitute for the entire Körperkoordina- sensitive to distinguish at the top but rather reflect
tionsTest für Kinder test battery in measuring the current performance instead of the potential to
coordination of athletes. The finding that none of the develop. The ability of the coach to identify talent
other three subtests contributed much to the should however not be underestimated, but scientific
competition score 2 years later could be explained observations using objective criteria should comple-
by the task requirements of these subtests. In line ment these intuitive judgements (Vaeyens et al.,
with Vandorpe et al. (2011a), this further suggests 2008; Williams & Reilly, 2000). The present study
that moving sideways primarily measures coordina- has highlighted the value of a non-sport-specific
8 B. Vandorpe et al.

motor coordination test in contributing to initial Carrick, F. R., Oggero, E., Pagnacco, G., Brock, J. B., & Arikan,
gymnastics talent identification processes, as it was T. (2007). Posturographic testing and motor learning predict-
ability in gymnasts. Disability and Rehabilitation, 29, 1881–1889.
found to be an important identifier of potential talent Claessens, A. L., Lefevre, J., Beunen, G., & Malina, R. M. (1999).
within a group of highly gifted gymnasts exhibiting Contribution of anthropometric characteristics to performance
similar anthropometric and physical profiles. In line scores in elite female gymnasts. Journal of Sports Medicine and
with the sliding populations approach of Régnier Physical Fitness, 39, 355–360.
Clark, J. E., & Metcalfe, J. S. (2002). The mountain of motor
et al. (1993), the present study was able to predict
development: A metaphor. In J. E. Clark & J. H. Humphrey
short-term gymnastics success over a period of 2 (Eds.), Motor development: Research and reviews (Vol. 2, pp. 163–
years in young female gymnasts. However, care is 190). Reston, VA: National Association of Sport and Physical
warranted when applying this approach to other age Education.
groups for which a modified test battery should be Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
developed. (2nd edn.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cometti, G., & Cometti, D. (2007). La Pliométrie-Méthodes,
entraı̂nement et exercises. Paris: Chiron.
Acknowledgements Côté, J. (1999). The influence of the family in the development of
talent in sport. The Sport Psychologist, 13, 395–417.
This project was funded by the Department of Council of Europe (1988). EUROFIT: Handbook for the European
Culture, Youth, Sports, and Media of the Flemish Test of Physical Fitness. Rome: Council of European Committee
for Development in Sports.
Government. We would like to thank the Flemish Damsgaard, R., Bencke, J., Matthiesen, G., Petersen, J. H., &
Gymnastics Federation for allowing us to contribute Muller, J. (2000). Is prepubertal growth adversely affected by
Downloaded by [Johan Pion] at 09:19 31 January 2012

to their talent identification and development pro- sport? Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 32, 1698–1703.
gramme and for their valuable help in the practical De Bosscher, V., Bingham, J., Shibli, S., Van Bottenburg, M., &
organization of the data collection. Warm thanks to De Knop, P. (2008). The global sporting arms race: An
international comparative study on sports policy factors leading to
the gymnasts, elite and club coaches for their international sporting success. Aachen: Meyer & Meyer.
enthusiasm during testing. Falk, B., Lidor, R., Lander, Y., & Lang, B. (2004). Talent
identification and early development of elite water-polo players:
A 2-year follow-up study. Journal of Sports Sciences, 22, 347–355.
References Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: Sage.
Gagné, F. (2004). Transforming gifts into talents: The DMGT as
Abbott, A., & Collins, D. (2004). Eliminating the dichotomy a developmental theory. High Ability Studies, 15, 119–147.
between theory and practice in talent identification and Henderson, S. E., & Sugden, D. A. (1992). Movement Assessment
development: Considering the role of psychology. Journal of Battery for Children. London: Psychological Corporation.
Sports Sciences, 22, 395–408. Kearney, J. T. (1999). Sport performance enhancement: Design
Ahnert, J., Schneider, W., & Bös, K. (2009). Developmental and analysis of research. Letter to the editor in chief. Medicine
changes and individual stability of motor abilities from the and Science in Sports and Exercise, 31, 755–756.
preschool period to young adulthood. In W. Schneider & M. Kiphard, E. J., & Schilling, F. (2007). Körperkoordinationstest für
Bullock (eds), Human development from early childhood to early Kinder. 2. Überarbeitete und ergänzte Auflage. Weinheim: Beltz
adulthood: Evidence from the Munich Longitudinal Study on the Test GmbH.
Genesis of Individual Competencies (LOGIC) (pp. 35–62). Lidor, R., Herschko, Y., Bilkevitz, A., Arnon, M., & Falk, B.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. (2007). Measurement of talent in volleyball: 15-month follow-
Baker, J., & Horton, S. (2004). A review of primary and secondary up of elite adolescent players. Journal of Sports Medicine and
influences on sport expertise. High Ability Studies, 15, 211–226. Physical Fitness, 47, 159–168.
Baxter-Jones, A. D., Thompson, A. M., & Malina, R. M. (2002). Lohman, T. G., Roche, A. F., & Martorell, R. (1988).
Growth and maturation in elite young female athletes. Sports Anthropometric standardization reference manual. Champaign,
Medicine and Arthroscopy Review, 10, 42–49. IL: Human Kinetics.
Bencke, J., Damsgaard, R., Saekmose, A., Jorgensen, P., Lopes, V. P., Rodrigues, L. P., Maia, J. A. R., & Malina, R. M.
Jorgensen, K., & Klausen, K. (2002). Anaerobic power and (2011). Motor coordination as predictor of physical activity in
muscle strength characteristics of 11 years old elite and non- childhood. Scandinavian Journal of Medicince and Science in
elite boys and girls from gymnastics, team handball, tennis and Sports, 21, 663–669.
swimming. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Maffulli, N., King, J. B., & Helms, P. (1994). Training in elite
Sports, 12, 171–178. young athletes (the Training of Young Athletes (TOYA)
Bradshaw, E. J., & Le Rossignol, P. (2004). Anthropometric and Study): Injuries, flexibility and isometric strength. British
biomechanical field measures of floor and vault ability in 8 to 14 Journal of Sports Medicine, 28, 123–136.
year old talent-selected gymnasts. Sports Biomechanics, 3, 249– Malina, R. M. (1999). Growth and maturation of female
262. gymnasts: Is training a factor? In F. E. Johnston, B. Zemel, &
Bressel, E., Yonker, J. C., Kras, J., & Heath, E. M. (2007). P. B. Eveleth (Eds.), Human growth in context (pp. 291–301).
Comparison of static and dynamic balance in female collegiate London: Smith-Gordon.
soccer, basketball, and gymnastics athletes. Journal of Athletic Malina, R., Bouchard, C., & Bar-Or, O. (2004). Growth,
Training, 42, 42–46. maturation and physical activity (2nd edn.). Champaign, IL:
Bruininks, R. H., & Bruininks, B. D. (2005). Bruininks-Oseretsky Human Kinetics.
Test of Motor Proficiency (2nd edn.). Minneapolis, MN: AGS Malina, R. M., Claessens, A. L, Van Aken, K., Thomis, M.,
Publishing. Lefevre, J., Philippaerts, R. et al. (2006). Maturity offset in
Burton, A. W., & Miller, D. E. (1998). Movement skill assessment. gymnasts: Application of a prediction equation. Medicine and
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Science in Sports and Exercice, 38, 1342–1347.
Prediction of gymnastics performance 9

Mirwald, R. L., Baxter-Jones, A. D., Bailey, D. A., & Beunen, G. Vaeyens, R., Güllich, A., Warr, C. R., & Philippaerts, R. (2009).
P. (2002). An assessment of maturity from anthropometric Talent identification and promotrion programmes of Olympic
measurements. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 34, athletes. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27, 1367–1380.
689–694. Vaeyens, R., Lenoir, M., Williams, A. M., & Philippaerts, R. M.
Moore, P. M., Collins, D., Burwitz, L., & Jess, M. C. (1998). The (2008). Talent identification and development programmes in
development of talent study. London: English Sports Council. sport: Current models and future directions. Sports Medicine,
Pearson, D. T., Naughton, G. A., & Torode, M. (2006). 38, 703–714.
Predictability of physiological testing and the role of maturation Vandorpe, B., Vandendriessche, J., Lefevre, J., Pion, J., Vaeyens,
in talent identification for adolescent team sports. Journal of R., Matthys, S. et al. (2011a). The KorperkoordinationsTest
Science and Medicine in Sport, 9, 277–287. fur Kinder: Reference values and suitability for 6–12-year-old
Philippaerts, R. M., Coutts, A., & Vaeyens, R. (2008). Physiolo- children in Flanders. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and
gical perspectives on the identification and development of Science in Sports, 21, 378–388.
talented performers in sport. In R. Fisher & R. Bailey (Eds.), Vandorpe, B., Vandendriessche, J., Vaeyens, R., Pion, J., Lefevre,
Talent identification and development: The search for sporting J., Philippaerts, R. M. et al. (2011b). Factors discriminating
excellence (pp. 49–67). Berlin: ICSSPE. gymnasts by competitive level. International Journal of Sports
Prescott, J. (1999). Identification and development of talent in young Medicine, 32, 591–597.
female gymnasts. Dissertation, Loughborough University, Williams, A. M., & Ford, R. R. (2009). Promoting a skills-based
Loughborough. agenda in Olympic sports: The role of skill-acquisition
Régnier, G., Salmela, J. H., & Russell, S. J. (1993). Talent specialists. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27, 1381–1392.
detection and development in sport. In R. N. Singer, M. Williams, A. M., & Reilly, T. (2000). Talent identification and
Murphy, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport development in soccer. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18, 657–667.
psychology (pp. 290–313). New York: Macmillan. Wolstencroft, E. (2002). Talent identification and development: An
Russell, K. (2010). Young athletes: The dichotomy of the child academic review. Edinburgh: Sport Scotland.
Downloaded by [Johan Pion] at 09:19 31 January 2012

prodigy. In Proceedings of the International Congress of the Growing


Child in High Performance Sport. Birmingham: University of
Birmingham.
Seefeldt, V. (1980). Developmental motor patterns: Implications
for elementary school physical education. In C. Nadeau, W.
Holliwell, K. Newell, & G. Roberts (Eds.), Psychology of motor
behavior and sport (pp. 314–323). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.

View publication stats

You might also like