You are on page 1of 6

Psychology of Addictive Behaviors © 2013 American Psychological Association

2014, Vol. 28, No. 2, 619 – 624 0893-164X/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0034127

BRIEF REPORT

Ecological Momentary Assessment of the Effects of Craving and Affect on


Risk for Relapse During Substance Abuse Treatment
Todd M. Moore, Amanda Seavey, Kathrin Ritter, James K. McNulty, Kristina C. Gordon,
and Gregory L. Stuart
University of Tennessee
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

The primary goals of this study were to use ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to examine the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

extent to which proximal factors (cravings and affect) were temporally associated with relapse, and to
assess the role of distal factors (e.g., coping styles) in moderating these associations. We also examined
whether using EMA procedures impacted relapse rates. A sample of 100 male (n ⫽ 66) and female
(n ⫽ 34) patients entering outpatient treatment for substance abuse completed a baseline assessment of
substance dependence, stress, social support, coping styles, family history of substance abuse, and
self-efficacy. Half of the participants were randomly assigned to receive training in using a handheld
computer to respond to 3 random prompts each day for 4 months regarding cravings, affect, and
substance use. All participants completed 2- and 4-month follow-up assessments of substance use.
Results showed that using EMA procedures did not influence relapse rates. Hierarchical linear modeling
showed that individuals who reported an increase in cravings on a given prompt were 14 times more
likely to report relapse on the subsequent prompt than individuals who did not experience an increase in
cravings. In addition, women, older individuals, and individuals who use distraction and disengagement
as coping styles were at increased risk for relapse when experiencing an increase in cravings. Individuals
who use acceptance as a coping style were at decreased risk for relapse when experiencing an increase
in cravings. The study highlights the importance of tailoring treatments to address the needs of particular
individuals and risk factors.

Keywords: relapse, craving, coping, electronic diary, ecological momentary assessment

Although treatments designed to reduce substance use show imperative to better understand the complex interplay of risk
promise, up to 50% of patients relapse within 6 months of treat- factors in the relapse process to improve treatment outcome.
ment (e.g., McKay & Weiss, 2001), and up to 40% cycle through Witkiewitz and Marlatt’s (2004, 2007) Systems Model views relapse
periods of relapse, treatment reentry, and recovery (e.g., Scott, as a complex process involving links between distal and proximal
Foss & Dennis, 2005). Though operational definitions of relapse processes occurring during high-risk situations that increase the
vary widely, this study used the commonly recognized definition risk for relapse. Distal risk factors are stable or enduring charac-
of relapse of a return to any use of a substance (see Chung & teristics of the individual or his or her environment (e.g., coping
Maisto, 2006; Witkiewitz & Marlatt, 2007, for reviews), and this skills, chronic stress) theorized to increase risk for relapse (Mc-
definition has been used in prior ecological momentary assessment Kay, Franklin, Patapis, & Lynch, 2006; Witkiewitz & Marlatt,
(EMA) studies of relapse (e.g., Marhe, Waters, van de Wetering, & 2007). Studies examining the influence of these factors on relapse
Franken, 2013). Given the high rate and chronicity of relapse, it is have yielded mixed results. Theoretically, these mixed results are
not surprising, given that distal risk factors may not exert main
effects on relapse, but rather, they may influence relapse by
This article was published Online First October 14, 2013. interacting with proximal risk factors. Proximal risk factors are the
Todd M. Moore, Amanda Seavey, Kathrin Ritter, James K. McNulty, temporally immediate precursors to relapse, with cravings and
Kristina C. Gordon, and Gregory L. Stuart, Department of Psychology, affect being among the most robust (Witkiewitz & Marlatt, 2004).
University of Tennessee. Although most research examining proximal factors used retro-
James K. McNulty is now at Department of Psychology, Florida State spective questionnaires, recent advances in technology have af-
University. forded the opportunity to more precisely measure within-day vari-
This research was supported by Grant R03DA025781 from the National
ations of factors that may increase immediate risk for relapse.
Institute on Drug Abuse awarded to Todd M. Moore. We also want to
In one of the first studies to use EMA to examine relapse to
express our tremendous gratitude to all the staff and patients at Cornerstone
of Recovery for supporting this research. alcohol, Krahn, Bohn, Henk, Grossman, and Gosnell (2005) re-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Todd M. cruited men in alcohol treatment who used handheld computers to
Moore, University of Tennessee, Department of Psychology, Austin Peay answer random prompts about cravings and affect. Results of
Building (416F), Knoxville, TN 37996. E-mail: tmoore24@utk.edu cluster analyses indicated that clusters differed in terms of levels of

619
620 MOORE, SEAVEY, RITTER, MCNULTY, GORDON, AND STUART

cravings and negative affect but not relapse. Cooney et al. (2007) pleted study measures. This approach allowed us to rule out the
recruited patients from outpatient substance abuse programs who possibility that using EMA procedures significantly influenced the
used handheld computers to respond to random prompts assessing relapse process.
alcohol cravings, mood states, and alcohol abstinence self-
efficacy. Relapse was predicted by lower ratings of self-efficacy to Method
resist drinking, and a trend was found for increases in cravings to
predict relapse. Positive and negative affect did not predict relapse.
Holt, Litt, and Cooney (2012) did not find an association between Participants
cravings or negative affect and risk for relapse among a sample of This study was approved by the authors’ institutional review
men and women in concurrent alcohol and tobacco treatment. In board. Participants were 101 male (n ⫽ 66) and female (n ⫽ 35)
regard to illicit substances, studies using EMA consistently dem- patients entering intensive outpatient treatment for substance de-
onstrate that increases in cravings are associated with increased pendence at Cornerstone of Recovery in Louisville, Tennessee. All
temptations to use and actual relapse to heroin and cocaine (Marhe patients completed a 4-week inpatient treatment prior to beginning
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

et al., 2013; Waters, Marhe, & Franken, 2012), ecstasy (Hopper et outpatient treatment and were drug-free at the beginning of out-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

al., 2006), and any substance (Johnson, Barrault, Nadeau, & patient treatment based on urine testing conducted by the facility.
Swendsen, 2009). In the only known study to examine both crav- Standard treatment lasts 8 weeks and emphasizes the 12-step
ings and negative affect, Epstein et al. (2009) found that negative recovery model. Inclusion criteria were (a) at least 18 years old; (b)
affect and cravings (“tempted to use”) were related to increased met criteria for dependence on alcohol or an illicit substance; and
risk for relapse to cocaine use, and feeling worried was related to (c) ability to read and speak English. Participants were randomized
relapse to heroin use. Moreover, related research on smoking to the EMA (n ⫽ 51) and Non-EMA (n ⫽ 50) groups. One person
lapses demonstrates that negative affect is associated with in- (female) dropped out before completing the handheld computer
creased risk for smoking lapses (e.g., Shiffman et al., 2007), training, leaving a final sample of 50 patients in each group for
especially rapid increases in negative affect (e.g., Shiffman & data analysis.
Waters, 2004).
Taken together, studies examining distal factors yield inconsis-
tent results, whereas studies assessing proximal factors suggest a Baseline and Follow-Up Measures
strong association between negative affect, cravings, and risk for The substance use module of the Structured Clinical Interview
relapse. The inconsistency across distal and proximal factors sug- for DSM–IV (SCID-P; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1995)
gests the need for a more nuanced approach to examining the assessed diagnostic criteria for Axis I substance use disorders.
relapse process. To the extent that proximal factors are operating Stress was assessed with the Life Experiences Survey (Sarason,
in episodes of relapse, they should be linked to distal risk factors Johnson, & Siegel, 1978), which assesses the stressfulness of life
according to Witkiewitz and Marlatt (2004, 2007). For example, events occurring in the past 6 months. Social support was assessed
individuals with poor coping skills (distal factor) may have limited using the Social Support Questionnaire (Sarason, Levine, Basham, &
resources to manage increases in cravings or negative affect (prox- Sarason, 1983), which measures availability of, and satisfaction
imal factors), which may increase risk of relapse. However, there with, social supports. A modified version of the Family Tree
is a paucity of data examining the confluence of distal and prox- Questionnaire (Mann, Sobell, Sobell, & Pavan, 1985) was used to
imal factors on relapse. The previously described EMA studies assess family history of alcohol and drug abuse. General self-
demonstrate that EMA technology can be invaluable for assessing efficacy was assessed with the Self-Efficacy Scale (Sherer et al.,
the process of relapse, but a number of limitations greatly reduce 1982). Fourteen coping styles were assessed with the Brief COPE
their generalizability. Namely, these studies did not assess distal (Carver, 1997). All measures evidenced acceptable psychometric
risk factors, did not include a non-EMA control group to rule out properties.
the possibility of reactivity to assessment, and assessed risk for Two and 4 months following baseline, participants completed
relapse that occurred usually within the first week to 1 month of measures of alcohol (Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test;
treatment. The literature clearly needs research examining the AUDIT; Saunders, Aasland, Babor, De La Fuente, & Grant, 1993)
influence of proximal and distal risk factors that may lead to and drug use (Drug Use Disorders Identification Test; DUDIT;
relapse. Stuart, Moore, Kahler, & Ramsey, 2003) to assess the occurrence
To elucidate the relapse process, this study examined the extent of relapse in the non-EMA group and to compare relapse rates
to which EMA measures of cravings and positive and negative across groups. Specifically, this allowed us to examine whether
affect predicted relapse over a 4-month period among men and any reactivity to assessment that may occur from using the hand-
women entering intensive outpatient substance abuse treatment. It held computers impacted relapse rates. The AUDIT and DUDIT
was hypothesized that increases in cravings and affect would include items that assessed use of any substance since the prior
predict onset of relapse. Furthermore, it was hypothesized that assessment.
individuals who report poor coping skills, low self-efficacy and
social support, and report greater stress and history of family
Ecological Momentary Assessment
substance abuse are at elevated risk for relapse when experiencing
increases in cravings and affect. Because little is known about the The electronic diary assessments were obtained using the HP
potential treatment effects of using EMA measures, we random- iPAQ 110 handheld computer and SnapSurvey Professional
ized participants to two groups— one who used a handheld com- Edition® software package. Participants in the EMA group were
puter to complete electronic diary entries and one who just com- prompted three times daily on a quasi-random basis (once during
EMA ASSESSMENT OF RELAPSE 621

every 4-hr block between 9:00 a.m. and 9:00 p.m.) to respond to Table 1
questions about cravings and affect (independent variables) and Baseline Participant Characteristics (N ⫽ 100)
substance use (dependent variable). For each prompt, participants
were asked to rate, from 1 ⫽ none to 5 ⫽ very much, their current Characteristic
craving level for each of the major classes of drugs (alcohol, Age (M, SD) 33.16 (9.94)
marijuana, cocaine, etc.), and their level of positive affect (happy, Sex (%)
excited, determined, energetic, and enthusiastic) and negative af- Males 66.00
fect (angry, irritable, jittery, afraid, nervous, scared, and hostile), Females 34.00
Race (%)
adapted from the PANAS-X (Watson & Clark, 1994). Though not Caucasian 93.00
a PANAS-X item, the term “stressed” was also included in the African American 3.00
measure of negative affect, given past research documenting the Asian American 1.00
association between stress and relapse (e.g., Sinha, 2001). Partic- Other 3.00
Education (%)
ipants also indicated whether they used any substances (yes/no)
Grade/high school 24.00
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

since the last prompt (presented on the screen as a list of common Attended college 42.00
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

substances). Responses were sent via wireless encrypted commu- College degree 20.00
nication from the handheld computers to a stand-alone secure Graduate degree 14.00
computer. Employment (%)
Unemployed/disability 57.00
Employed part time 8.00
Procedures Employed full time 35.00
Annual income (%)
Patients were informed about the study by treatment staff during ⬍$25,000 38.00
standard intake sessions and were invited to visit a member of the $25,001–$50,000 24.00
research team. Following informed consent, participants were ad- $50,001–$100,000 28.00
ministered the SCID-P substance use module and asked to com- ⬎$100,000 10.00
plete baseline questionnaires. They returned at a designated time
with the completed packet and were randomized to the EMA and
non-EMA groups. Participants in the EMA group were trained to group (38%) was not significantly different than the non-EMA
use a handheld computer to complete diary entries three times each group (32%). Of the 19 patients who relapsed in the EMA group,
day for 4 months. All participants were asked to return 2 and 4 12 reported relapse via the handheld computers, and these reports
months following baseline to complete follow-up questionnaires were used in all subsequent analyses. Analyses including and
and address any questions about study participation. Participants in excluding the remaining seven patients who reported relapse only
the non-EMA group did not receive a handheld computer but on the AUDIT or DUDIT at follow-up were virtually identical, so
completed all measures. All participants earned $25 for completing these participants were retained in all analyses.
the baseline and follow-up assessments ($75 total). Participants in
the EMA group received $1.00 for each on-time (completed within Proximal Predictors of Relapse
1 hr of prompting) diary entry and an additional $7 for each week
of on-time responding to at least 75% of the prompts. To test our first hypothesis, that increases in cravings and affect
would predict onset of relapse, we used a lagged two-level model
Results analysis using HLM 6 (Raudenbush, Bryk, & Congdon, 2004).
Analyses examined the extent to which cravings and affect re-
Descriptive Statistics ported on a given diary entry predicted relapse on the subsequent
diary entry in both separate and simultaneous analyses (length of
For the overall sample, the average age was 33.2 years (SD ⫽ time between reports of relapse via diary entries and the preceding
9.9; range ⫽ 18 to 58), with the majority being Caucasian (93%; diary entry was no more than 12 hr). The association between
see Table 1 for sample characteristics). Although relatively fewer cravings, negative affect, and substance use following first use of
females participated in the study, they were fairly equally repre- a substance are not included in these models. Cravings and affect
sented across the EMA (n ⫽ 19) and non-EMA (n ⫽ 15) groups. were person-centered at Level 1 and the individual means were
Among the sample, 47% received an alcohol dependence diagnosis entered on the intercept at Level 2, which was allowed to vary
and 83% received a drug dependence diagnosis (cannabis, 24%; randomly across people.
cocaine, 28%; other stimulants, 16%; opioids, 41%; sedatives, Results showed that, controlling for average levels of cravings,
26%; hallucinogens, 5%; other, 8%; and polydrug dependence individuals who reported a sudden increase in cravings were nearly
without alcohol, 20%). A total of 7,618 of 18,000 random prompts 14 times more likely (OR ⫽ 13.81, 95% CI [1.09, 175.54]) to
were answered (M ⫽ 152.36 [42.3%], SD ⫽ 96.35, Mdn ⫽ 139, relapse than individuals who did not experience an increase in
range ⫽ 12 to 333 out of 360 possible). Compliance rates for cravings, t(49) ⫽ 2.07, p ⬍ .05 (effect size ⫽ 1.45). Results
completing the 2- and 4-month follow-ups were 83% and 67%, showed that after controlling for average levels of negative
respectively. affect, individuals who reported a sudden increase in negative
affect were somewhat more likely (OR ⫽ 1.13, 95% CI [.99, 1.28])
EMA Methods and Relapse
to relapse than individuals who did not experience an increase in
Defining relapse as any use of substances, 35% of the partici- negative affect, t(49) ⫽ 1.99, p ⫽ .051. No effects were found for
pants relapsed during the study. The rate of relapse for the EMA positive affect. However, after controlling for affect, individuals
622 MOORE, SEAVEY, RITTER, MCNULTY, GORDON, AND STUART

who reported a sudden increase in cravings were over 3 times more than controls. Age moderated the association between cravings and
likely (OR ⫽ 3.49, 95% CI [1.84, 6.64]) to relapse than individuals relapse, wherein older individuals were significantly more likely to
who did not experience an increase in cravings, t(49) ⫽ 3.91, p ⬍ relapse when reporting a sudden increase in cravings than controls.
.001 (effect size ⫽ .69). Results further showed that individuals Finally, gender moderated the association between cravings and
who reported lower average cravings were more likely to relapse relapse, wherein women were 3.36 times more likely to relapse
following a sudden increase in cravings than were individuals who when reporting a sudden increase in cravings compared with men.
reported higher average cravings, t(48) ⫽ ⫺2.12, p ⬍ .05. Nega-
tive and positive affect were not associated with relapse after Discussion
controlling for cravings and subsequent analyses excluded affect.
The findings of this study significantly extend the relapse liter-
Effects of Distal Factors on Moderating the ature by being the first to demonstrate that cravings emerged as the
only unique temporal predictor of relapse, and that people with
Craving/Relapse Association
lower average levels of cravings are at increased risk for relapse
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Analyses using HLM examined whether distal factors (entered when they experience a sudden spike in cravings. The lack of an
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

as grand-centered continuous variables, except for gender, which association between cravings and relapse among individuals with
was dummy-coded, such that 0 ⫽ male and 1 ⫽ female) exerted an high average levels of cravings may be due to a ceiling effect. This
influence on relapse. None of the distal factors (including age and study is also the first to find that cravings lead to relapse for
gender as exploratory factors) predicted relapse. However, a num- different people in different ways, with greatest risk when those
ber of significant cross-level interactions indicated that distal fac- experiencing an increase in cravings are women, older individuals,
tors moderated the association between cravings and relapse (see individuals with a family history of drug abuse, and individuals
Table 2). Three coping styles interacted with cravings to increase who used distraction and disengagement or rarely used acceptance
risk for relapse. Specifically, individuals who reported greater use as coping strategies, and those who tended to experience relatively
of coping via distraction and behavioral disengagement (e.g., giv- lower levels of cravings on average. This study highlights the
ing up attempt to cope), and less use of coping via acceptance, importance of considering the benefits of tailoring treatment to
were significantly more likely (ORs ⫽ 2.3 to 6.5) to relapse when address the needs of individuals who may be at greatest risk of
reporting a sudden increase in cravings than controls. Further, relapse during periods of heightened cravings. It also suggests that
family history of drug abuse evidenced a trend effect, wherein patients may benefit from incorporating greater emphasis on help-
individuals who reported a greater history of family drug abuse ing patients improve coping skills. This is consistent with the
were somewhat more likely to relapse (OR ⫽ 2.60; p ⫽ .053; ambivalence model of craving in which affect and craving are
effect size ⫽ .52) when reporting a sudden increase in cravings critical factors in exacerbating or attenuating approach inclinations

Table 2
Moderating Effect of Distal Factors on Associations Between Cravings and Relapse

Relapse
Distal Factors ⫻ Cravings t Odds ratio CI Effect size (d)

Age 2.08 1.24 1.01, 1.53 .12
Gender 4.58ⴱⴱⴱ 3.36 1.98, 5.71 .67
Stress ⫺0.51 0.96 0.81, 1.13 .02
Family history
Alcohol 0.58 1.29 0.53, 3.17 .14
Drugs 1.99† 2.57 0.99, 6.72 .52
Self-efficacy 0.14 1.02 0.81, 1.27 .01
Social support 0.70 3.68 0.09, 155.89 .72
Coping styles
Acceptance ⫺3.25ⴱⴱ 3.32 1.58, 6.94 .66
Active coping ⫺1.34 0.59 0.27, 1.30 .37
Behavioral disengagement 2.54ⴱ 2.35 1.20, 4.60 .47
Denial 0.76 1.46 0.53, 4.01 .21
Humor 0.07 1.04 0.31, 3.56 .02
Planning 0.05 1.03 0.33, 3.19 .02
Positive reframing ⫺0.39 0.88 0.47, 1.67 .07
Religion 0.20 1.09 0.47, 2.50 .05
Self-blame 0.97 2.12 0.45, 10.09 .42
Self-distraction 2.12ⴱ 6.54 1.11, 38.71 1.04
Substance use 1.02 1.40 0.72, 2.70 .19
Use of emotional support 0.61 1.50 0.40, 5.60 .22
Use of instrumental support ⫺0.18 0.81 0.07, 9.26 .11
Venting ⫺0.90 0.39 0.05, 3.22 .41
Note. CI ⫽ confidence interval.

p ⬍ .10. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .001.
EMA ASSESSMENT OF RELAPSE 623

to substance use cues (Breiner, Stritzke, & Lang, 1999; Schlauch, References
Gwynn-Shapiro, Stasiewicz, Molnar, & Lang, 2013). Thus,
Breiner, M. J., Stritzke, W. G. K., & Lang, A. R. (1999). Approaching
mindfulness-based additions to treatments may reduce relapse by
avoidance: A step essential to the understanding of craving. Alcohol
helping patients better manage affect and cravings via use of
Research & Health, 23, 197–206.
acceptance-based coping (e.g., Witkiewitz, Marlatt, & Walker, Carver, C. S. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too
2005). long: Consider the Brief COPE. International Journal of Behavioral
A major strength of this study was the use of handheld com- Medicine, 4, 92–100. doi:10.1207/s15327558ijbm0401_6
puters to assess proximal risk factors. The use of state-of-the-art Chung, T., & Maisto, S. A. (2006). Relapse to alcohol and other drugs use
technology afforded the opportunity to address limitations of ret- in treated adolescents: Review and reconsideration of relapse as a
rospective studies (i.e., retrospective recall bias) and to more change point in clinical course. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 149 –
precisely examine factors that immediately precede relapse with 161. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.11.004
minimal concern potentially reactivity to assessment would in- Cooney, N. L., Litt, M. D., Cooney, J. L., Pilkey, D. T., Steinberg, H. R., &
crease the relapse rate, especially given that we found no differ- Oncken, C. A. (2007). Alcohol and tobacco cessation in alcohol-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ence in relapse rates between groups. By demonstrating that pa- dependent smokers: Analysis of real-time reports. Psychology of Addic-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tive Behaviors, 21, 277–286. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.21.3.277


tients in treatment will provide frequent responses about sensitive
Epstein, D. H., Willner-Reid, J., Vahabzadeh, M., Mezghanni, M., Lin,
information, this study also suggests that handheld computers and
J. L., & Preston, K. L. (2009). Real-time electronic diary reports of cue
similar devices may be of great utility for primary or adjunctive exposure and mood in the hours before cocaine and heroin craving and
treatment delivery, especially for patients who might otherwise not use. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 88 –94. doi:10.1001/
seek treatment or have limited capabilities to attend treatment. archgenpsychiatry.2008.509
Although this study possessed a number of strengths, one lim- First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1995).
itation is that some EMA patients reported relapse on follow-up Structured clinical interview for DSM–IV Axis I disorders – Patient ed.
self-report measures but not on the handheld computers. This may (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric
have stemmed from fears about negative consequences associated Institute.
with reporting (e.g., treatment termination), leading them to report Holt, L. J., Litt, M. D., & Cooney, N. L. (2012). Prospective analysis of
relapse only after completing treatment. Future studies should use early lapse to drinking and smoking among individuals in concurrent
a calendar-based interview, frequent biochemical verification, and alcohol and tobacco treatment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 26,
collateral reports to assess any non-EMA reported relapses to more 561–572. doi:10.1037/a0026039
Hopper, J. W., Su, Z., Looby, A. R., Ryan, E. T., Penetar, D. M., Palmer,
accurately calculate the role of proximal variables on the risk for
C. M., & Lukas, S. E. (2006). Incidence and patterns of polydrug use and
relapse. Another limitation is the relatively low EMA compliance
craving for ecstasy in regular ecstasy users: An ecological momentary
rate. Studies of similar length and design obtained compliance assessment study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 85, 221–235. doi:
rates at or above 65% (e.g., Epstein et al., 2009; Holt et al., 2012). 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.04.012
These studies incorporated counselor awareness of compliance Johnson, E. I., Barrault, M., Nadeau, L., & Swendsen, J. (2009). Feasibility
rates, study discharge for low compliance, and/or repeated prompts and validity of computerized ambulatory monitoring in drug-dependent
when initial prompts were not answered. We considered these women. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 99, 322–326. doi:10.1016/j
approaches during the design phase but opted to use monetary .drugalcdep.2008.06.010
incentives to enhance compliance rather than methods that may Krahn, D. D., Bohn, M. J., Henk, H. J., Grossman, J. L., & Gosnell, B.
reduce confidentiality, add a potential punishment for noncompli- (2005). Patterns of urges during early abstinence in alcohol-dependent
ance, or risk burnout and reduced compliance from repeated subjects. The American Journal on Addictions, 14, 248 –255. doi:
prompts, respectively. However, use of these procedures may 10.1080/10550490590949424
Mann, R. E., Sobell, L. C., Sobell, M. B., & Pavan, D. (1985). Reliability
explain the higher compliance rate in these studies, and future
of a family tree questionnaire for assessing family history of alcohol
researchers should carefully consider the strengths and weaknesses
problems. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 15, 61– 67. doi:10.1016/0376-
of utilizing specific methods to improve compliance. 8716(85)90030-4
Another limitation, given the small sample, is that we lacked Marhe, R., Waters, A. J., van de Wetering, B. J. M., & Franken, I. H. A.
sufficient power to detect what may be a significant association (2013). Implicit and explicit drug-related cognitions during detoxifica-
between affect and relapse, and we lacked sufficient power to tion treatment are associated with drug relapse: An ecological momen-
examine whether risk factors lead to relapse in the same way for tary assessment study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
different substances. Future research with larger samples could 81, 1–12. doi:10.1037/a0030754
ascertain the extent to which these findings hold specifically for McKay, J. R., Franklin, T. R., Patapis, N., & Lynch, K. G. (2006).
alcohol and each drug class, and whether negative affect plays a Conceptual, methodological, and analytical issues in the study of re-
larger role in predicting relapse as has been found in prior research lapse. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 109 –127. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005
(e.g., Shiffman & Waters, 2004). However, it should be noted that .11.002
McKay, J. R., & Weiss, R. V. (2001). A review of temporal effects and
effect sizes confirmed the robustness of the results even within a
outcome predictors in substance abuse treatment studies with long-term
relatively small sample. Despite these limitations, results from this
follow-ups: Preliminary results and methodological issues. Evaluation
study support a growing body of research showing that cravings Review, 25, 113–161. doi:10.1177/0193841X0102500202
are strongly related to relapse. Results also show that this relation- Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., & Congdon, R. T. (2004). HLM: Hier-
ship is not always a direct one, and that distal factors play an archical linear modeling with the HLM/2L and HLM/3L programs.
important role. The treatments that may be most effective in Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International.
reducing relapse may need to be tailored and handheld devices Sarason, I. G., Johnson, J. H., & Siegel, J. M. (1978). Assessing the impact
may be useful tools for delivering these interventions. of life changes: Development of the Life Experiences Survey. Journal of
624 MOORE, SEAVEY, RITTER, MCNULTY, GORDON, AND STUART

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 932–946. doi:10.1037/0022- Sinha, R. (2001). How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse?
006X.46.5.932 Psychopharmacology, 158, 343–359. doi:10.1007/s002130100917
Sarason, I. G., Levine, H. M., Basham, R. B., & Sarason, B. R. (1983). Stuart, G. L., Moore, T. M., Kahler, C. W., & Ramsey, S. E. (2003).
Assessing social support: The Social Support Questionnaire. Journal of Substance abuse and relationship violence among men court-referred to
Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 127–139. doi:10.1037/0022- batterers’ intervention programs. Substance Abuse, 24, 107–122. doi:
3514.44.1.127 10.1080/08897070309511539
Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., De La Fuente, J. R., & Grant, Waters, A. J., Marhe, R., & Franken, I. H. A. (2012). Attentional bias to
M. (1993). Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification drug cues is elevated before and during temptations to use heroin and
Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Per- cocaine. Psychopharmacology, 219, 909 –921. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-
sons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption–II. Addiction, 88, 791– 804. 2424-z
doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x Watson, D. L., & Clark, L. A. (1994). Manual for the Positive and
Schlauch, R. C., Gwynn-Shapiro, D., Stasiewicz, P. R., Molnar, D. S., &
Negative Affect Schedule (Expanded form). Unpublished manuscript,
Lang, A. R. (2013). Affect and craving: Positive and negative affect are
University of Iowa, IA.
differentially associated with approach and avoidance inclinations. Ad-
Witkiewitz, K., & Marlatt, G. A. (2004). Relapse prevention for alcohol
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

dictive Behaviors, 38, 1970 –1979. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.12.003


and drug problems: That was zen, this is tao. American Psychologist, 59,
Scott, C. K., Foss, M. A., & Dennis, M. L. (2005). Pathways in the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

224 –235. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.4.224


relapse-treatment-recovery cycle over 3 years. Journal of Substance
Abuse Treatment, 28, S63–S72. doi:10.1016/j.jsat.2004.09.006 Witkiewitz, K., & Marlatt, G. A. (2007). Modeling the complexity of
Sherer, M., Maddux, J., Mercadante, B., Prentice-Dunn, S., Jacobs, B., & post-treatment drinking: It’s a rocky road to relapse. Clinical Psychology
Rogers, R. (1982). Self-Efficacy Scale: Construction and validation. Review, 27, 724 –738. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.01.002
Psychological Reports, 51, 663– 671. doi:10.2466/pr0.1982.51.2.663 Witkiewitz, K., & Marlatt, G. A., & Walker, D. (2005). Mindfulness-
Shiffman, S., Balabanis, M. H., Gwaltney, C. J., Paty, J. A., Gnys, M., based relapse prevention for alcohol and substance use disorders.
Kassel, J. D., . . . Paton, S. M. (2007). Prediction of lapse from Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 19, 211–228. doi:10.1891/jcop
associations between smoking and situational antecedents assessed by .2005.19.3.211
ecological momentary assessment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 91,
159 –168. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.017
Shiffman, S., & Waters, A. J. (2004). Negative affect and smoking lapses: Received March 19, 2013
A prospective analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Revision received July 2, 2013
72, 192–201. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.192 Accepted July 8, 2013 䡲

You might also like