You are on page 1of 6

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/249542306

Variation in the echolocation calls of the hoary bat (Lasiurus cinereus): Influence
of body size, habitat structure, and geographic location

Article  in  Canadian Journal of Zoology · September 1999


DOI: 10.1139/cjz-77-4-530

CITATIONS READS

115 1,826

3 authors, including:

David Steve Jacobs


University of Cape Town
143 PUBLICATIONS   3,742 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The role of echolocation in communication in a high duty cycle echolocating bat Rhinolophus clivosus (Chiroptera Rhinolophidae): An experimental approach View project

Variation in echolocation call intensities and detection ranges of bat assemblages across an environmental gradient. View project

All content following this page was uploaded by David Steve Jacobs on 08 December 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Color profile: Disabled
Composite Default screen

530

Variation in the echolocation calls of the hoary


bat (Lasiurus cinereus): influence of body size,
habitat structure, and geographic location
Robert M.R. Barclay, James H. Fullard, and David S. Jacobs

Abstract: The echolocation calls of bats vary according to the task being undertaken by the individual. Within species,
there is evidence for geographic, habitat, and individual variation, although it is often difficult to separate these factors
and rule out variation in recording or analysis techniques. We studied the variation in echolocation call design of the
hoary bat (Lasiurus cinereus) by analyzing calls, recorded in the same manner, from free-flying bats at one site in
Manitoba and at four sites in the Hawaiian islands. Sites varied in terms of the proximity of the bats to vegetation. As
predicted, individuals from the larger subspecies (Lasiurus cinereus cinereus; Manitoba) used lower frequency calls
than did the smaller individuals in Hawai′i. Within the same habitat type (open or closed), there was evidence for
differences in echolocation call design between populations on different islands. On the island of Hawai′i, bats at a
single site used shorter higher frequency calls when foraging within the vegetation than when foraging in the open,
again as predicted. However, bats foraging in different sites used calls with the opposite characteristics to those
predicted on the basis of the openness of the site. Although there are several possible explanations for this, we suggest
that prey encounter rate and the ability of bats to augment acoustic prey detection with visual information may be
important.
Résumé : Les cris d’écholocation des chauves-souris varient selon leur fonction. Au sein d’une espèce, la variation est
tributaire du lieu géographique, de l’habitat et de l’individu, bien qu’il soit difficile de distinguer ces facteurs et
impossible de rejeter l’hypothèse d’une variation générée par les techniques d’enregistrement ou d’analyse. Nous avons
étudié la variation dans le système des cris d’écholocation de la Chauve-souris cendrée (Lasiurus cinereus) par analyse
de cris enregistrés de la même façon chez des chauves-souris libres à un site au Manitoba et à quatre sites dans les
îles Hawai′i. Les différences entre les sites résidaient surtout dans la distance séparant les chauves-souris de la
végétation. Tel que prédit, les individus de la plus grosse sous-espèce (Lasiurus cinereus cinereus, Manitoba) utilisaient
des cris de fréquence plus basse que les individus plus petits d’Hawai′i. Dans un même type d’habitat (ouvert ou
fermé), les systèmes de cris d’écholocation variaient chez les populations de différentes îles. Sur l’île d’Hawai′i, les
chauves-souris à un site utilisaient des cris plus courts et de plus haute fréquence lorsqu’ils chassaient dans la
végétation que lorsqu’ils chassaient en terrain ouvert, tel que prédit. Cependant, les chauves-souris chassant à d’autres
sites utilisaient des cris aux caractéristiques opposées à celles prévues dans un habitat ouvert. Bien qu’il y ait plusieurs
explications possibles à ce paradoxe, nous croyons que la fréquence des rencontres avec des proies et la capacité des
chauves-souris e compléter leur système de détection acoustique par des signaux visuels sont sans doute des facteurs
importants.

[Traduit par la Rédaction] Barclay et al. 534

Introduction of different species (Aldridge and Rautenbach 1987; Nor-


berg and Rayner 1987; Fenton 1990; Barclay and Brigham
Microchiropteran bats use a variety of echolocation call 1991). Large species have high wing loading, which requires
designs. Among aerial insectivorous species, echolocation that individuals fly rapidly to stay aloft, and means that they
call variation is related to the body size and foraging habitat are relatively unmaneuverable (Norberg and Rayner 1987).
They thus tend to forage in open areas. Such bats require
Received May 25, 1998. Accepted December 18, 1998. long-range prey detection and can achieve this by using
intense low frequency echolocation calls (Barclay and Brig-
R.M.R. Barclay.1 Department of Biological Sciences,
University of Calgary, Calgary, AB T2N 1N4, Canada.
ham 1991). Smaller species can fly more slowly and are
J.H. Fullard. Department of Biology, Erindale College, more maneuverable. They can thus forage in closer proxim-
University of Toronto, Mississauga, ON L5L 1C6, Canada. ity to vegetation and can use higher frequency calls which,
D.S. Jacobs.2 Department of Zoology, University of Hawai′i although they do not provide as large a prey-detection range,
at Manoa, 2538 The Mall, Honolulu, HI 96822, U.S.A. do provide greater information regarding the structural detail
1 of targets and are better for detecting small prey (Simmons
Author to whom all correspondence should be addressed
(e-mail: barclay@ucalgary.ca). and Stein 1980; Barclay and Brigham 1991).
2
Present address: Department of Zoology, University of Cape While between-species comparisons support the body size –
Town, Private Bag, Rondebosch 7700, Cape Town, South habitat clutter hypothesis regarding the evolution of varia-
Africa. tion in echolocation call design (Barclay 1986; Aldridge and
Can. J. Zool. 77: 530–534 (1999) © 1999 NRC Canada

J:\cjz\cjz77\cjz-04\Z99-008.vp
Thursday, September 09, 1999 4:36:28 PM
Color profile: Disabled
Composite Default screen

Barclay et al. 531

Rautenbach 1987; Fullard et al. 1991), within-species varia- ground, well below the canopy, and often within a few metres
tion in echolocation call design also occurs. Individuals alter of the trees surrounding the foraging areas. At Ocean View, we
the nature of their calls depending on the habitat (amount of recorded bats in two situations: as they flew over a 5–10 m
clutter) and the task at hand (Obrist 1995). Geographic vari- wide roadway and as they flew within the vegetation (′ohi′a
(Metrosideros polymorpha) forest) bordering the road (see also
ation between populations of the same species has also been
Jacobs 1996). We analyzed the recordings in the two Ocean View
documented (Thomas et al. 1987), although it is not clear sites separately so that we could examine variation in echolocation
whether such variation is due to body size or habitat differ- calls within the same population, but in two habitats differing in
ences between locations. A common problem in such com- amount of clutter. We predicted that bats foraging within the vege-
parisons has also been that recording and sound analysis tation would use shorter higher frequency calls.
methods have differed between studies, potentially adding to Based on the relationship between foraging habitat and echolo-
or obscurring natural variation in call features. cation call design, we predicted that bats recorded at the more open
The purpose of our study was to test predictions based on sites in Hawai′i (Koke′e and Bradshaw) would use lower frequency
the body size – habitat clutter hypothesis using a geographi- calls than would those recorded in the more cluttered sites (Kalopa
cally widespread species and a consistent recording and and Ocean View). Geographic variation between populations on
different islands would be evident in the calls made by bats at the
analysis protocol. To do this we studied the echolocation
two open sites (Bradshaw, Hawai′i, and Koke′e, Kaua′i).
calls of the hoary bat (Lasiurus cinereus) because of its wide
geographic distribution and variation in body size across
subspecies. Materials and methods
We recorded echolocation calls from all bats onto a Racal Store
Study species 4DS instrumentation tape recorder operated at 76 cm·s–1 using one
of three microphone systems: a Bruel and Kjaer (B and K) type
The hoary bat is found throughout much of North and South
4135 (¼ in.; 6.3 mm) condenser microphone and type 2204 mea-
America, as well as on the Hawaiian and Galapagos islands
suring amplifier (Koke′e, Kalopa), a Larson–Davis type 2520
(Jacobs 1996; McCracken et al. 1997; Shump and Shump 1982).
(¼ in; 6.3 mm) condenser microphone and type 2200C preampli-
Wherever it has been studied, L. cinereus is insectivorous and cap-
fier (Ocean View, Bradshaw), and a custom-made condenser micro-
tures prey in the air. It uses a relatively low frequency, frequency-
phone and power supply (Ocean View, Manitoba). The bandwidth
modulated echolocation call (Belwood and Fullard 1984; Barclay
for the microphones and tape recorder in all situations was 5–
1986; Obrist 1995; McCracken et al. 1997). However, there has
80 kHz (±2 dB). To verify the sensitivity characteristics of the cus-
been some debate (e.g., see McCracken et al. 1997) over the simi-
tom-made microphone in the Ocean View site, we simultaneously
larity or difference between the calls used by Lasiurus cinereus
recorded bats with this microphone and the Larson–Davis micro-
semotus in Hawai′i and those used by Lasiurus cinereus cinereus in
phone. Frequency analyses of the calls revealed no spectral devia-
North America. Belwood and Fullard (1984) considered that the
tions (±2 dB within the flat range) between the two systems.
two subspecies used similar calls, based on values for North Amer-
All echolocation calls were analyzed in the laboratories of J.H.F.
ica reported by Fenton and Bell (1981). Later, more detailed re-
using customized fast Fourier transform (FFT) spectral analyses
cordings, however, suggested that L. c. cinereus used calls that differ
software, that employs a Cooley–Tukey algorithm and has a
significantly from those of L. c. semotus (Barclay 1986). Again
minimum time and frequency resolution of 10 µs and 800 Hz (J.
differences in recording and analysis techniques or habitat may
Dawson, Queens University; personal communication) calibrated
have been involved. We thus analyzed calls recorded in the same
with a Nicholet 100A minianalyzer.
way from both subspecies.
As bats change the frequency and duration of their calls when
Lasiurus cinereus semotus in Hawai′i is considerably smaller
they search for, detect, and approach targets (Kick and Simmons
(female body mass, 18.0 g) and has a lower wing loading (9.9 N·m–2)
1984), we chose an arbitrary method to standardize which of the
than does L. c. cinereus from North America (34.0 g, 14.7 N·m–2;
calls that we analyzed. We only analyzed search phase calls, which
Jacobs 1996). We thus predicted that mainland bats would use
we defined as calls with at least 100 ms separating them from an-
lower frequency calls than the Hawaiian individuals.
other call. As these were recorded from wild bats, we could not
identify individuals and we have thus likely analyzed more than
Study sites one call per bat. We did, however, choose only one call from each
bat pass by the microphone. At all sites, at least six individuals
We recorded free-flying foraging L. cinereus at one site in Man- were present at any one time on a given night and we recorded
itoba, Canada, in 1983 and 1984 (see Barclay 1986 for details) and calls on more than one night at each site. Nonetheless, variation of
four sites in the Hawaiian islands: Koke′e State Park, Kaua′i calls within a site was undoubtedly less than if we had been able to
(1987); Kalopa State Park, Hawai′i (1987); Bradshaw Airforce analyze only one call per bat.
Base (= Pohakuloa; Jacobs 1996), Hawai′i (1992); and Ocean We measured a number of echolocation call variables. We used
View Estates, Hawai′i (1992). These sites varied in terms of the the frequency with the highest intensity as a reference and deter-
density of the vegetation and the proximity that the bats flew to mined higher and lower frequencies of calls at specific intensities
this clutter. We could thus test predictions based on hypothesized below the peak (Fullard et al. 1993). This eliminates the subjectiv-
variation in echolocation calls due to habitat type and geographical ity involved in attempts to determine the minimum and maximum
location. frequencies in a call, which are often difficult to measure owing to
In Manitoba, bats were recorded as they flew 5–15 m above the background noise. For each call, we measured duration in millisec-
ground (below tree canopy height) along a 5–10 m wide roadway onds, peak frequency in kilohertz (spectral peak of highest inten-
bordered by deciduous trees (Barclay 1986). Recordings at both sity), Low18 (lowest frequency, 18 dB below peak), Low6 (lowest
Koke′e and Bradshaw were of bats flying in the open around large frequency, 6 dB below peak), High18 (highest frequency, 18 dB
security lights. The bats flew 15–30 m above the ground and away above peak), and High6 (highest frequency, 6 dB above peak). All
from (>20 m) vegetation (see also Belwood and Fullard 1984; values were computed by one person (J.H.F.), who was unaware of
Jacobs 1996). We recorded bats at Kalopa and Ocean View as they the identity of any of the recordings. Statistical analysis was also
foraged in more cluttered situations. They flew 3–15 m above the conducted without knowledge of the locality. Call variables were
© 1999 NRC Canada

J:\cjz\cjz77\cjz-04\Z99-008.vp
Thursday, September 09, 1999 4:36:30 PM
Color profile: Disabled
Composite Default screen

532 Can. J. Zool. Vol. 77, 1999

Table 1. Characteristics of the echolocation calls of Lasiurus cinereus recorded from various locations.

Site Openness Duration (ms) Low18 (kHz) Low6 (kHz) Peak (kHz) High6 (kHz) High18 (kHz)
Bradshaw, Hawai′i Open 7.38 (1.78)a 26.15 (1.78)a 27.78 (1.91)a 29.81 (2.66)a 34.74 (4.84)a 45.46 (6.17)a
Koke′e, Kaua′i Open 7.69 (1.98)a 25.51 (1.29)b 27.02 (1.41)b 27.88 (1.70)b 32.04 (2.52)b 41.69 (4.72)b
Ocean View, Hawai′i Closed 9.63 (1.28)b 23.86 (1.27)c 25.26 (1.21)c 26.21 (1.16)c 29.57 (2.19)c 37.77 (3.46)c
Kalopa, Hawai′i Closed 10.27 (1.35)b 24.17 (1.18)c 25.79 (1.17)c 26.80 (1.14)c 32.28 (2.44)b 43.56 (3.65)a,b
Manitoba, Canada Intermediate 9.77 (2.52)b 18.30 (1.93)d 19.35 (2.06)d 20.11 (2.19)d 21.93 (3.28)d 27.73 (9.34)d
Note: Sample size was 100 calls for each site. Openness refers to our subjective ranking of how close the bats were flying to the vegetation and other
objects. Values are given as the mean with SD in parentheses. Within columns, means followed by different letters are significantly different (ANOVA with
Tukey’s test, p < 0.05).

Table 2. Comparison of echolocation call characteristics of Lasiurus cinereus flying in the open versus in the vegetation at Ocean
View, Hawai′i.
Duration (ms) Low18 (kHz) Low6 (kHz) Peak (kHz) High6 (kHz) High18 (kHz)
Open 8.92 (1.45)*** 24.72 (1.79)** 26.19 (1.76)*** 27.43 (2.24)*** 30.98 (3.18)ns 39.61 (5.08)**
Vegetation 5.46 (2.31)*** 25.90 (3.05)** 27.93 (2.22)*** 29.24 (2.80)*** 32.60 (4.35)ns 42.36 (6.31)**
Note: Sample size was 55 calls in the open and 69 calls in the vegetation. Values are given as the mean with SD in parentheses. Samples were tested
for differences using either two-tailed t tests or Mann–Whitney tests. ***, p < 0.001; **, p < 0.01; ns, p > 0.05.

analyzed for differences among locations using ANOVA and being longer, calls made in the open had a narrower average
Tukey’s tests. Calls made at Ocean View in the vegetation and in bandwidth (14.9 kHz) compared to those used in the vegeta-
the open were compared using t tests, or Mann–Whitney tests tion (16.46 kHz).
when variances were unequal (F ratio test). All statistical tests
used α = 0.05.

Discussion
Results
The echolocation calls of the free-flying hoary bats that
As we predicted, there were significant differences in all we recorded varied from location to location in most, but not
call characteristics for bats recorded in different locations all cases, in accordance with our predictions based on varia-
(F[4,495] > 51.0, p < 0.0001 in each case; Table 1). The calls tion in body size and habitat structure. Our prediction that
of bats from Manitoba were significantly different in all fre- the larger subspecies from the mainland would use lower
quency variables from all other calls. They were of a lower frequency calls compared with those used by the Hawaiian
frequency in each case, peak frequency (the most reliable bats is supported by our data. Hoary bats from Manitoba
measure) being 20.1 compared with 26.2–29.8 kHz for the used significantly lower frequency calls compared with the
various Hawaiian populations. The Manitoba calls also had a bats we recorded at all the Hawaiian sites. This confirms
shallower frequency sweep (average bandwidth 9.4 vs. 13.9– earlier analyses (Barclay 1986; Obrist 1995) and qualitative
19.4 kHz). Call duration was no different from that of the assessments (Fenton et al. 1983) from other North American
closed habitat populations on the Hawaiian islands. For repre- sites, and corrects the misconception that the two subspecies
sentative sonographs of calls from Manitoba and Hawai′i, use similar calls (Belwood and Fullard 1984; McCracken et
see Barclay (1986) and Belwood and Fullard (1984), respec- al. 1997).
tively. North American hoary bats are almost twice as heavy as
Within Hawai′i, bats foraging in the two closed sites on their Hawaiian counterparts and have a significantly higher
Hawai′i (Ocean View and Kalopa) used similar calls (Ta- wing loading (Jacobs 1996). This should mean that North
ble 1). Duration and three of the five frequency variables American individuals must fly faster to stay aloft and are
were not significantly different. Calls at Kalopa started at less maneuverable than Hawaiian individuals (Norberg and
higher frequencies, although this is often the least reliable Rayner 1987). Hawaiian hoary bats do fly in more cluttered
measurement as atmospheric attenuation is greater at higher habitats than do North American individuals (Barclay 1985;
frequencies (Griffin 1971). Jacobs 1996). The greater prey-detection range afforded by
Calls from bats recorded at the two open sites (Bradshaw lower frequency calls (Barclay and Brigham 1991) should
and Koke′e) on the different islands were significantly dif- be less important to Hawaiian bats, which can react more
ferent in all frequency components (Bradshaw calls were quickly to prey detected at short range. The improved detec-
higher), but were not different in duration (Table 1). Calls at tion of small prey, afforded by higher frequency calls, may
these two sites were, however, more similar to one another have been an important selection factor in the evolution of
than they were to those recorded at the two closed sites. Bats the echolocation system of the smaller Hawaiian subspecies.
at the open sites had significantly shorter higher frequency Lasiurus cinereus semotus is believed to have originated
calls. from North American stock and has been present on the is-
As predicted, bats flying within the vegetation at Ocean lands for up to several million years (Morales and Bickham
View used significantly shorter higher frequency calls than 1995). It is interesting that another island population of
did those foraging in more open spaces (Table 2). Despite hoary bats, on the Galapagos islands, is also composed of

© 1999 NRC Canada

J:\cjz\cjz77\cjz-04\Z99-008.vp
Thursday, September 09, 1999 4:36:33 PM
Color profile: Disabled
Composite Default screen

Barclay et al. 533

small individuals (17 g) that use calls similar in design to vative. Finally, it is possible that the light poles at Bradshaw
those used in Hawai′i (29–22 kHz; McCracken et al. 1997). acted as clutter (see Kalko and Schnitzler 1993). This seems
On the Hawaiian islands, hoary bats foraging in the same unlikely to have affected the calls to the degree that we
habitat used similar call designs. Bats foraging in the rela- found, however, as the bats at Bradshaw still foraged well
tively closed sites on Hawai′i (Ocean View and Kalopa) used away from the poles (>10 m) most of the time.
calls that were not significantly different in four of the six Bats flying parallel to linear habitat edges, such as those
variables we measured. Again the least reliable variables at Ocean View, may perceive the habitat as being unclut-
(High6 and High18) were different. Likewise, calls from the tered in front of them (E. Kalko, personal communication).
two open sites were more similar to one another than they In this case, individuals may use narrower band calls than
were to calls from either of the closed sites. expected. However, this would not explain the fact that calls
Between islands, there was some evidence for slight, but at Ocean View were more narrow and lower in frequency
statistically significant differences in call design. For exam- than in those used at Bradshaw.
ple, there were statistically significant frequency differences Different prey density at the open and closed sites is one
between the calls recorded at the open sites on Kaua′i and possible factor that could have influenced call design. If
Hawai′i. The sites were similar in that the bats at both for- concentrations of insects attracted to the lights at both open
aged high above the ground near bright lights and well away sites meant that prey density was higher than at the closed
from vegetation or other obstacles. Thus habitat differences sites, foraging bats would encounter prey at a greater rate
are unlikely to have influenced call design. Geographic dif- and at a shorter range. In this case, shorter calls might be
ferences in echolocation call structure of bats have been used to eliminate pulse–echo overlap and higher frequencies
documented before (Thomas et al. 1987). It is not known to could be used, as maximizing prey-detection range would be
what degree the populations of L. c. semotus on the various less critical. Prey density, however, is actually lower at
Hawaiian islands are isolated, but our results suggest that Bradshaw than at Ocean View (Jacobs 1993). On this basis
there may be slight echolocation call divergence. alone, encounter rates at Bradshaw should have been lower
Our analyses of the calls from bats foraging in different and longer range calls more beneficial.
habitats are somewhat contradictory. As predicted based on Prey size, bat flight speed, and mode of prey detection
the relationship between habitat clutter and echolocation call may help explain the results. The insect fauna at Bradshaw
design (Aldridge and Rautenbach 1987; Fenton 1990), bats is dominated by large moths and the bats there prey heavily
at Ocean View used shorter higher frequency calls when for- on them. At Ocean View, a variety of small insects make up
aging in the vegetation than when foraging in the open. the fauna and the diet of bats (Jacobs 1993). Despite the low
Shorter calls may be necessary to avoid pulse–echo overlap prey density at Bradshaw, the large target size and greater
(Kalko and Schnitzler 1989) in situations where the bats fly echo strength combined with the greater flight speed of the
close to obstacles, and higher frequency calls, giving more bats may mean that the detection rate was actually higher
detailed information regarding small targets, can be used at Bradshaw than at Ocean View. Indeed, our qualitative
where long-range prey detection is not a priority. assessment of the recordings indicated that Bradshaw had a
While the above data from a single site fit the predictions, higher rate of terminal feeding buzzes (i.e., the rapid series
bats foraging at different sites behaved exactly opposite to of calls produced as a bat attacks an insect; Griffin 1958)
the predictions. Bats foraging at the two open sites used than did Ocean View, despite a lower insect density. In addi-
shorter higher frequency calls than did those foraging at the tion, we suggest that hoary bats at Bradshaw (and Koke′e)
two closed sites. This is despite the fact that hoary bats at may detect and initially track large moths visually in the
the open sites in Hawai′i fly faster and are less maneuverable bright lights. If so, echolocation may only be used to track
than bats at the closed sites (Jacobs 1996), and should thus prey during the latter stages of the approach, thereby ex-
have benefited from longer range calls. Although the echolo- plaining the shorter duration and higher frequency calls.
cation call differences were relatively small (approximately Video analysis with simultaneous echolocation call record-
2 ms and 2 kHz), they were consistent. ings might help elucidate the interaction of visual and acous-
Explaining this apparent contradiction is difficult. Brad- tic prey detection, and explain echolocation call variation
shaw and Ocean View provide the best comparison as the that is otherwise perplexing. In the end, however, corre-
sites are on the same island and were studied at the same lational studies such as ours will need to be supported by ex-
time and with the same equipment, and data on bat behav- perimental studies testing the various explanations.
iour and insect abundance exist for both (Jacobs 1993). It is
unlikely that bats at Bradshaw and Ocean View were drawn
from different populations with different echolocation call Acknowledgements
designs, given the relatively small size of the Big Island and
the ability of bats to move over large distances. Indeed, there We thank the many people who assisted with the field and
is genetic evidence that bats move between the study sites laboratory work including L. Freed, C. Stone, D. Stone, Q.
(D.S. Jacobs, unpublished data). Differences in recording Tomich, and C. Belanger, as well as the 150th Air National
conditions are also an unlikely explanation. Although bats at Guard (Koke′e State Park) and those at the University of
Bradshaw were often further from the microphone than bats Manitoba Field Station. Comments and suggestions by E.
at Ocean View, this would only serve to bias the recordings Kalko and an anonymous reviewer significantly improved
at Bradshaw in favour of low frequencies, owing to the at- the manuscript. Funding for our studies was provided by the
mospheric attenuation of high frequencies (Griffin 1971). Audubon Society (Hawai′i chapter), the Hawaiian Division
Thus if anything, the differences we observed were conser- of Forestry and Wildlife, U.S. Fish and Wildlife, the Na-

© 1999 NRC Canada

J:\cjz\cjz77\cjz-04\Z99-008.vp
Thursday, September 09, 1999 4:36:35 PM
Color profile: Disabled
Composite Default screen

534 Can. J. Zool. Vol. 77, 1999

tional Geographic Society, Sigma Xi, and the Natural Sci- Griffin, D.R. 1971. The importance of atmospheric attenuation for
ences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. the echolocation of bats (Chiroptera). Anim. Behav. 19: 55–61.
Jacobs, D.S. 1993. Character release in the endangered Hawaiian
hoary bat, Lasiurus cinereus semotus. Ph.D. dissertation, Uni-
References versity of Hawai’i, Honolulu.
Jacobs, D.S. 1996. Morphological divergence in an insular bat,
Aldridge, H.D.J.N., and Rautenbach, I.L. 1987. Morphology, echo- Lasiurus cinereus semotus. Funct. Morphol. 10: 622–630.
location and resource partitioning in insectivorous bats. J. Anim. Kalko, E.K.V., and Schnitzler, H.-U. 1989. The echolocation and
Ecol. 56: 763–778. hunting behavior of Daubenton’s bat, Myotis daubentoni. Behav.
Barclay, R.M.R. 1985. Long- versus short-range foraging strategies Ecol. Sociobiol. 24: 225–238.
of hoary (Lasiurus cinereus) and silver-haired (Lasionycteris nocti- Kalko, E.K.V., and Schnitzler, H.-U. 1993. Plasticity in echo-
vagans) bats and the consequences for prey selection. Can. J. location signals of European pipistrelle bats in search flight:
Zool. 63: 2507–2515. implications for habitat use and prey detection. Behav. Ecol.
Barclay, R.M.R. 1986. The echolocation calls of hoary (Lasiurus Sociobiol. 33: 415–428.
cinereus) and silver-haired (Lasionycteris noctivagans) bats as Kick, S.A., and Simmons, J.A. 1984. Automatic gain control in the
adaptations for long- versus short-range foraging strategies and bat’s sonar receiver and the neuroethology of echolocation. J.
the consequences for prey selection. Can. J. Zool. 64: 2700–2705. Neurosci. 4: 2725–2737.
Barclay, R.M.R., and. Brigham, R.M. 1991. Prey detection, dietary McCracken, G.F., Hayes, J.P., Cevallos, J., Guffey, S.Z., and
niche breadth, and body size in bats: why are aerial insectivo- Carlos Romero, F. 1997. Observations on the distribution, ecol-
rous bats so small? Am. Nat. 137: 693–703. ogy, and behaviour of bats on the Galapagos Islands. J. Zool.
Belwood J.J., and Fullard, J.H. 1984. Echolocation and foraging Lond. 243: 757–770.
behaviour in the Hawaiian hoary bat, Lasiurus cinereus semotus. Morales, J.C., and Bickham, J.W. 1995. Molecular systematics of
Can. J. Zool. 62: 2113–2120. the genus Lasiurus (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae) based on re-
Fenton, M.B. 1990. The foraging behaviour and ecology of animal- striction-site maps of the mitochondrial ribosomal genes. J.
eating bats. Can. J. Zool. 68: 411–422. Mammal. 76: 730–749.
Fenton, M.B., and Bell, G.P. 1981. Recognition of species of insec- Norberg, U.M., and Rayner, J.M. V. 1987. Ecological morphology
tivorous bats by their echolocation calls. J. Mammal. 62: 233–243. and flight in bats (Mammalia; Chiroptera): wing adaptations,
Fenton, M.B., Merriam, H.G., and Holroyd, G.L. 1983. Bats of flight performance, foraging strategy and echolocation. Philos.
Kootenay, Glacier, and Mount Revelstoke national parks in Can- Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 316: 335–427.
ada: identification by echolocation calls, distribution, and biol- Obrist, M.K. 1995. Flexible bat echolocation: the influence of indi-
ogy. Can. J. Zool. 61: 2503–2508. vidual, habitat and conspecifics on sonar signal design. Behav.
Fullard, J.H., Koehler, C., Surlykke, A., and McKenzie, N.L. 1991. Ecol. Sociobiol. 36: 207–219.
Echolocation ecology and flight morphology of insectivorous Shump, K.A., Jr., and Shump, A.U. 1982. Lasiurus cinereus.
bats (Chiroptera) in south-western Australia. Aust. J. Zool. 39: Mamm. Species, 185: 1–6.
427–438. Simmons, J.A., and Stein, R.A. 1980. Acoustic imaging in bat so-
Fullard, J.H., Barclay, R.M.R., and Thomas, D.W. 1993. Echo- nar: echolocation signals and the evolution of echolocation.
location in free-flying Atiu swiftlets (Aerodramus sawtelli). J. Comp. Physiol. 135: 61–84.
Biotropica, 25: 334–339. Thomas, D.W., Bell, G.P., and Fenton, M.B. 1987. Variation in
Griffin, D.R. 1958. Listening in the dark. Yale University Press, echolocation call frequencies recorded from North American
New Haven, Conn. vespertilionid bats: a cautionary note. J. Mammal. 68: 842–847.

© 1999 NRC Canada

J:\cjz\cjz77\cjz-04\Z99-008.vp
Thursday, September 09, 1999 4:36:36 PM
View publication stats

You might also like