You are on page 1of 10

A NEW EXAMINATION OF ECHOLOCATION CALLS OF SOME

NEOTROPICAL BATS (EMBALLONURIDAE AND MORMOOPIDAE)


MICHAEL J. O'FARRELL AND BRUCE W. Mn..LER

O'Farrell Biological Consulting, 2912 North Jones Boulevard, Las Vegas, NV 89108 (MO'F)
Wildlife Conservation Society, Gallon Jug, Belize (BWM)

In northwestern Belize, two emballonurids (Rhynchonycteris naso and Saccopteryx bili-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article-abstract/78/3/954/905406 by guest on 07 June 2019


neata) and three monnoopids (Pteronotus pamellii. P. davyi, and P. personatus) were
examined acoustically with the Anabat II bat detector and associated analysis system. R.
naso used calls consisting of a short constant frequency (CF) at 47 kHz and concave
downward frequency modulated (PM) sweep to ca. 40 kHz with two strong hannorucs.
Echolocation sequences of S. bilineata revealed a paired cadence of calls. Both signals in
a pair slowly increased in frequency, fonning a quasi-CF (QCF) component and tenninated
with a short, downward PM sweep. The initial call of a pair ranged from 44.5 to 46.6 kHz
and the second ranged from 46.6 to 48.7 kHz. P. parnellii used a long CF call at 63.5
kHz, with a tenninal downward PM sweep to ca. 54.5 kHz. P. davyi produced calls with
an initial short CF at 68.1 kHz, a downward PM sweep, and a tenninal short CF at 58.0
kHz. P. personatus emitted calls similar in shape to P. davyi, with the initial CF at 83.0
kHz and the terminal CF at ca. 68.0 kHz. Previous descriptions of echolocation calls for
these species compare favorably with those obtained in this study, but the Anabat equipment
generally revealed more variation and detail than previously described. The echolocation
calls described in this study provide precise vocal signatures for the accurate identification
of free-flying individuals of the species studied.

Key words: Pteronotus parnellii, Pteronotus davyi, Pteronotus personatus, Saccopteryx


bilineata, Rhynchonycteris naso, echolocation, Anabat n, vocal signatures

The recording and description of echo- ability to record individuals under field con-
location calls of some Neotropical bats be- ditions was still limited.
gan with the pioneering work of Griffin Early studies of Neotropical species of
(1953) and Griffin and Novick (1955). Al- emballonurids provided descriptions of so-
though data gathered were surprisingly nar calls of free-flying individuals in Trin-
good, technological limitations prevented idad and Costa Rica, but the foc;:us was on
recording under a variety of field condi- low-frequency communication calls (Brad-
tions. Thus, much of the early descriptions bury and Emmons, 1974; Bradbury and
of vocalizations were generated from cap- Vehrencamp, 1976). Orientation calls were
presented as oscilloscope tracings of sono-
tive bats. Bats of the families Emballonur-
gram envelopes or detailed verbal descrip-
idae and Mormoopidae tended to produce
tions. Improved equipment yielded more
high-intensity sounds that were easier to de-
detailed descriptions of design of echolo-
tect and record than the low-intensity, high- cation signals for embaIlonurids in Panama
ly directional sounds produced by bats in and Costa Rica (Barclay, 1983; Kalko,
the Phyllostomidae. Subsequent examina- 1995). The resolution of sonograms, how-
tion of some of these species. using im- ever, make visual comparisons among the
proved equipment, resulted in more defini- species difficult.
tive descriptions of echolocation calls Patterns of frequency-time structure
(Novick, 1963; Pye, 1973), although the based on constant-frequency (CF) and fre-

Journal of Mamf1Ullogy, 78(3):954--963, 1997 954


August 1997 O'FARRELL AND Mll..LER-ECHOLOCATION OF NEOTROPICAL BATS 955

quency-modulated (FM) components of Peninsula (Hartshorn, 1984). Within a 53,OOO-ha


echolocation calls have been described for tract of private property, a total of nine sites rep-
some Microchiroptera (SiImnons and Stein. resenting seven habitat types were sampled.
1980; Simmons et al., 1979). Variations in The general habitats being sampled by nets
and trap were monitored acoustically to provide
the frequency-time structure (e.g., frequen-
a library of vocalizations representative of the
cy bandwidth, duration of calls) have detectable bat fauna present. Emphasis was
proved useful in identification of species placed on monitoring the collecting devices in

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article-abstract/78/3/954/905406 by guest on 07 June 2019


(Fenton and Bell, 1981) and assessment of an attempt to provide recordings of bats just pri-
intraspecific variation with respect to habi- or to capture. In such cases, initial echolocation
tat and behavior (Kalko and Schnitzler, calls could be linked directly to a known spe-
1993; Obrist, 1995). Although the frequen- cies. However, this was not always possible and
cy-time structure has proved effective as a provided only a small portion of the potential
tool for identification of species, little stan- vocal repertoire of that species.
dardized information exists for most spe- Several methods to obtain echolocation calls
cies, particularly for Neotropical species. of known species were used after capture. For
species that were abundant, individuals were re-
The range of bat detectors available and
leased near the point of capture at times when
the subsequent analysis and display of
activity of bats was absent. Some released bats
echolocation calls vary (Fenton, 1988). circled the area and provided a series of vocal-
Cost of equipment and quality of output are izations that appeared to be representative of
equally variable and generally are prohibi- nonnal flight. Some individuals of abundant spe-
tive for many workers. The recent avail- cies and all individuals of less-conunon species
ability of the inexpensive and field-portable were released in a solid-ceiling, lathe-sided en-
Anabat II bat detector and analysis system closure (12.50 by 6.75 m). Recordings were ob-
provides greater flexibility in obtaining re- tained for known individuals in unrestrained
cordings of echolocation calls of free-flying flight within the enclosure.
bats. The detector output of all incoming vocali-
zations of bats was directed through the Anabat
The purpose of the present paper is to
provide a new examination of design of
n ZCA Interface Module, processed with asso-
ciated software, and displayed. on a laptop com-
echolocation calls using the frequency-time puter. Thus, the frequency-time structure of sig-
structure display from the Anabat II system nals could be examined as they were being pro-
for some species of emballonurid and mor- duced. Vocal sequences were selected and saved
moopid bats that have been studied and de- directly on the hard drive for future examination.
scribed in other terms in the past. 11ris in- As described, this system allowed instantaneous
formation will assist in providing a baseline monitoring of vocalizations of bats with equal
for establishing standardized vocal signa- ease in the field or laboratory setting and elim-
tures for the identification of free-flying inated possible distortions of sound associated
bats of the species examined. with use of tape recorders. Taxonomic nomen-
clature for all taxa follows Koopman (1993).
MATERIALS AND METIIODS
During 7-18 February 1995, we sampled the RESULTS
bat fauna in the region around Gallon Jug in the Emballonuridae.-Vocal signatures of
northwestern comer of Belize using convention- two members of this family were positively
al mist nets, a double-frame harp trap, and
identified. Rhynchonycteris naso was a
acoustic sampling with an Anabat II bat detector
(Titley Electronics, Ballina, Australia). North-
common species associated with streams
western Belize is comprised of Subtropical and the vegetated edges of various water
Moist Forests, following the Holdridge Life sources. It was found roosting under a small
Zone System (Holdridge, 1967), and is similar wooden pier. supporting a water pump and
to the broad-leaved forests of the Peten in north- delivery system, at the edge of Laguna Ver-
ern Guatemala and much of Mexico's Yucatan de, a small natural lake and on the sides of
956 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 78, No.3

..............
': ~
"\
, '.
; :~

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article-abstract/78/3/954/905406 by guest on 07 June 2019


".
. .
-,;....:. .. .:. -i-.;,;
", " ,

....
'

• <0 ..
TIME (mSec)
80 ' 00 ". ,<0

FIG. I.-Frequency-time display of a vocal sequence produced by Rhynchonycteris naso from


Belize. The bottom screen is the same vocal sequence, incorporating a feeding buzz, with an expanded
frequency range to better illustrate the second harmonic. The time between calls on both screens is
compressed to allow more calls per screen.

trees lining the edge of Chan Chich Creek. was revealed by changing the frequency
Because this species roosts in relatively scale (0-160 kHz; bottom screen of Fig. I).
well-lighted habitat and initiates flight be- The zero-crossings analysis used by the An-
fore dark. positive visual identification was abat system forfeits harmonic information
possible. Several evenings of monitoring (c. Corben, pers. comm.). The signal dis-
concentrated foraging activity of R. naso re- played on the screen represents the domi-
vealed a consistent repertoire of signals nant harmonic. If the strengths of two har-
(Fig. 1). Search and orientation calls dem- monics are about equal, the harmonic being
onstrated an initial short CF at 47 kHz fol- displayed may alternate between the two
lowed by a concave PM sweep to ca. 40 values. Thus, the harmonic displayed de-
kHz (Table 1). Sometimes the initial CF pends on a variety of factors such as rela-
portion was preceded by a discernible, I tive strength of emitted sounds, atmospher-
kHz up sweep. Similarly, the terminal FM ic attenuation, direction relative to the de-
portion varied, occasionally extending tector, and the frequency response of the de-
down to ca. 35 kHz. The CF portion varied tector. However, in the case of R. nasa, the
in length (4-6 msec) during search and ori- important fact for identification purposes is
entation behavior and then decreased pro- that a second harmonic will be apparent for
gressively through the pursuit phase. most call sequences.
The generally used frequency scale (0- The CF at 47kHz was constant during
80 kHz; top screen of Fig. 1) masked the flight at feeding grounds. However, individ-
presence of harmonic information, which uals startled at the roost generally flew
August 1997 O'FARRELL AND MILLER-ECHOLOCATION OF NEOTROPICAL BATS

TABLE I.-Characteristics of calls of free-flying emballonurid bats from Belize; n = number of


individual search-orientation calis, one standard error is given in parentheses.

Minimum Maximum
Duration of call Time between frequency frequency
Species n (msec) calls (kHz) (kHz)

Saccopteryx bilineata
First call 40' 6.7 55.8 44.5 46.6

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article-abstract/78/3/954/905406 by guest on 07 June 2019


(0.21) (1.85) (0.21) (0.18)
20' 5.9 58.7 42.1 45.5
(0.83) (5.20) (0.61) (0.37)
41,41, 6OC, respectively 9.4 85.8 45.1
(0.21) (3.18) (0.07)
Second call 40' 6.6 82.5 46.6 48.7
(0.15) (3.09) (0.19) (0.17)
23' 5.4 92.5 44.8 47.6
(0.58) (16.00) (0.46) (0.22)
38, 38, 57<, respectively 9.0 52.0 47.1
(0.28) (1.50) (0.08)
Rhynchonycteris naso 41" 6.7 60.8 40.2 47.2
(0.12) (2.99) (0.53) (0.06)
90, 82, 7OC, respectively 5.0 53.5 102.5
(0.08) (2.20) (D.ll)
80' 100
• This study.
b Ban:lay (1983); time belween calls converted from intercall interval.
e Kalko (1995).

d Bradbury and Vehrencamp (1976).

away from the initial roosting site in a loop jority of the call can be categorized as a
to a nearby spot. Recordings made during quasi-CF (QCF) component (Kalko and
this behavior revealed an unusual sequence Schnitzler, 1993).
of calls. Repetitive sequences of 8-12 calls Most echolocation sequences demon-
began with QCF-FM calls progressively in- strated an alternating cadence of paired
creasing in frequency (e.g., 39-28, 41-29, calls (Fig. 2). The paired calls began with
43-29, 45-34, 45-34, 45-34, 45-38, 48- a signal ranging from ca. 43-45 kHz
39, 45-39 kHz). Although exact frequen- (low-frequency pulse; LF), and the next
cies seemed to vary somewhat, the pattern was ca. 46-48 kHz (high-frequency pulse;
was constant. HF). As a vocal sequence progressed from
Saccopteryx bilineata was in all habitats search-orientation through pursuit, the sig-
sampled based on acoustic sampling and nals decreased in duration and became
identification of vocal signatures. We cap- horseshoe-shaped. The paired relationship
'tured this species in forest trails, which of a lower- followed by higher-frequency
could be effectively blocked with nets or call continued through entire sequences.
the harp trap. Initially, individuals released No difference was noted between dura-
in the trails provided no reference calls. tion of LF and HF calls of a pair (Table I).
Subsequently, release in the enclosure re- The paired nature of LF and HF pulses was
vealed the use of highly distinctive vocali- apparent based on time between calls. Time
zations (Fig. 2). Design of pulses of search hetween calls (TBC) for the first call re-
and orientation calls incorporated an in- flects the time between LF and HF pulses,
creasing frequency over most of the call, and TBC for the second call is the time be-
tenninating in a short down sweep. The ma- tween HF and succeeding LF pulses. The
958 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 78. No.3

70

"

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article-abstract/78/3/954/905406 by guest on 07 June 2019


" r- -. ... :.'. . ~\ , ..
,, ~ . .

"
10

• 20
" 60
TIME (msec)
80
. ... . . ..
100
" '

'20 .
...... , .
,
FIG. 2.-Frequency-time display of a vocal sequence produced by Saccopteryx bilineata from
Belize. The top screen begins with orientation-search calls ranging into pursuit calls. The bottom
screen is a continuation of the same sequence tenninating in a feeding buzz. The time between calls
on both screens is compressed to allow more calls per screen.

null hypothesis that TBe are equally spaced within the Vespertilionidae and Molossidae,
regardless of low- or high-frequency char- P. pamellii was distinctive in that no other
acteristics is rejected (t = 8.65; dj. = 39; variation in calls was observed.
P < 0.(01). Pteronotus davyi also was in all habitats
Mormoopidae.-Vocal signatures of sampled. Recording sessions with this spe-
three species were positively identified. cies in the enclosure were more successful
Pteronotus pamellii was in all habitats than with other species, because it tended
sampled and was a common species cap- to fly consistent laps producing signals
tured. It produced a distinctive echolocation identical to those gathered in the field.
call comprised of an initial short PM up Design of echolocation calls consisted of
sweep, a long CF component at 63.5 kHz. a short CF at ca. 68 kHz, a downward FM
and a long downward PM sweep to ca. 54.5 sweep. and a tenninal short CF component
kHz (Fig. 3; Table 2). During vocal se- at ca. 58 kHz (Fig. 4; Table 2). Under con-
quences, no feeding buzzes were detected. trolled conditions, no variation was noted.
only a shortening of the call duration. Five Under field situations, some signals in a se-
sequences (n = 48 individual calls) of bats quence appeared to contain only the FM
in apparent pursuit of prey yielded mean and tenninal CF components. This was
duration of 12.7 msec (SE ~ 0.10) and probably an artifact of direction of signal
mean time between calls of 30.0 msec (SE and attenuation of higher frequencies rather
== 1.02). Of all bats we have tested to date than actual missing signal components.
(M. J. O'Farrell, in litt.), including species Recognition of these incomplete signals is
August 1997 O'PAR.:RbLL AND MILLER-ECHOLOCATION OF NEOTROPICAL BATS 959

¥"
1!.
'~ \
,\ \,
>-"
u
z
w
"a"
...il!

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article-abstract/78/3/954/905406 by guest on 07 June 2019


" : .. '

n. 140
0
" 40 10 BO
TIME (msec)
100

FIG. 3.-Frequency-time display of a vocal sequence produced by Pteronotus pamellii from Belize.
The time between calls is compressed to allow more calls per screen.

important for identification purposes. Few Under field situations, some signals in a se-
terminal-pursuit sequences were observed; quence appeared to contain only the FM
these showed rapid pulses composed only and terminal CF components and, occasion-
of the FM component. ally, a lower harmonic element.
Pteronotus personatus was associated
with streams and was most abundant at the DISCUSSION

Rio Bravo. Design of echolocation calls Seven characteristic aspects of echolo-


was similar to that of P. davy;. consisting cation calls of bats have been described
of a short CF at 83 kHz, a downward FM (Simmons and Stein, 1980); constant fre-
sweep, and a teoninal short CF component quency, frequency modulation, sequence of
at 68 kHz (Fig, 5; Table 2), However, the components, duration, harmonics, ampli-
initial CF component was shorter, and the tude, and adaptive variations. Frequency
teoninal CF element tended to be longer characteristics (CF, FM), duration, and the
than signals of P. davyi. Under controlled pattern of change in frequency over time
conditions, calls were of shorter duration appear to be important for identifying spe-
with little or no tenninal CF component; cies of free-flying bats (Fenton and Bell,
lower harmonic elements were common. 1981). The presence of multiple harmonics

TABLE 2.-Characteristics of calls of free-flying mornwopid bats from Belize; n = number of


individual search-orientation calls. one standard error is given in parentheses.

Minimum Maximum
Duration of call Time between frequency frequency
Species n (msec) calls (kHz) (kHz)

Pteronotus davyi 35' 5.5 62.2 58.0 68.1


(0.13) (3.38) (0.20) (0.17)
3.1b 63 78
Pteronotus personatus 25' 5.7 48.3 68.0 83.0
(0,14) (1.50) (0) (0,21)
3.2b 76 106
Pteronotus pamellii 30' 30.4 61.9 54.5 63.5
(0.50) (2.69) (0.17) (0)
25" 56 64
• This study.
• Novick (963).
JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 78. No.1

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article-abstract/78/3/954/905406 by guest on 07 June 2019


10

.oo
• 20 60 10
TIME (msec)
100 12.

FrG. 4.-Frequency-time display of a vocal sequence produced by Pteronotu$ davyi from Belize.
The time between calls is compressed to allow more calls per screen.

has received much attention when display- behavior through pursuit and capture of
ing frequenc y-time characteristics (Fenton prey-Simmons et aI., 1979) or between in-
and Bell, 1981; MacDonald et aI., 1994; dividuals (e.g., change in design of call
Simmons and Stein, 1980; Simmons et aI., with a shift in foraging strategy or use of
1979). Variations in echolocation calls oc- microhabitat-Faure and Barclay, 1994;
cur at the individual level (i.e., progression Simmons et aI., 1978; Simmons and
in shape of call from orientation and search O'Farrell, 1977).

"
30

'00

. ..\. \..\,\. \. \.\.\.\..\\\;\


\..\..,,\..\
.. '. .. ,
..
.oo
• 20 10 10
TIME (msec)
100 .20

FlG. 5.- Frequency-time display of a vocal sequence produced by Pteronotus personatus from
Belize. The bottom screen is the same vocal sequence with an expanded frequency range. The time
between calls on both screens is compressed to allow more calls per screen.
August 1997 O'FARRELL AND MILLER-ECHOLOCATION OF NEOTROPICAL BATS 961

Zero-crossing analysis allows examina- scribed a frequency pattern with a lazy Z


tion of all aspects of echolocation calls ex- shape incorporating a short CF and an FM
cept for amplirude and hannonic infonna- sweep terminating in a short CF. Pye (1973)
tion. Although simultaneous examination of described a similar call that sometimes had
all harmonics present is not possible. some the terminal CF of ca. 1 rnsec. Calls from
harmonic infonnation is available if two Panamanian individuals demonstrated three
strong harmonics exist and there is an al- harmonics with an initial short CF at ca. 75

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article-abstract/78/3/954/905406 by guest on 07 June 2019


ternation of energy between harmonics as kHz and a terminal FM component to ca.
for R. nasa (Pig. I). It has yet to be estab- 60 kHz for the third harmonic, estimated
lished that the loss of this infonnation from from the graphic display. In our study,
echolocation calls is important with respect search and orientation calls of unenclosed
to identifying the species emitting the calls. individuals contained the initial short CF
Of the species examined in the present component plus a consistent terminal CF
srudy, P. pamellii has been the most exten- that was a major portion of each call (Fig.
sively investigated, primarily due to abun- 5). Individuals within an enclosure pro-
dance, relative ease in obtaining specimens, duced calls of shorter duration, lacking the
and the use of a long CF-FM, otherwise terminal CF, and the consistent presence of
only known for bats in the Rhinolophidae. a lower harmonic. Bats recorded in the pre-
Novick (1963) provided a detailed descrip- vious srudies were apparently in enclosed
tion of design of pulses and noted that ear- siruations, which may account for the gen-
lier equipment provided less than satisfac- erally missing terminal CF. However, the
tory results. His verbal description and that initial short CF was a minimum of 13 kHz
illustrated by Simmons and Stein (1980) are lower than that found by Novick (1963) and
identical to that found in this study (Pig. 3), ca. 8 kHz higher than that reported by Sim-
although two harmonics were described by mons et aI. (1979; Table 2). Similarly, the
Novick (1963) and three were illustrated by terminal CF was a minimum of 4 kHz low-
Simmons and Stein (1980). Novick's er (Novick, 1963) and ca. 8 kHz higher
(1963) description of the second harmonic (Simmons et aI., 1979) than previously re-
with the CF at ca. 64 kHz corresponds to ported. The differential in frequency ranges
our findings (Table 2). Duration of pulses may be due to a variety of factors including
compare generally with those given by the type of equipment used in earlier stud-
Novick (1963). A more detailed examina- ies, flight conditions, and geographic vari-
tion of changes in duration of pulse during ation.
approach and pursuit noted an increase in Novick (1963) described an initial short
the CF portion from 20 msec at 4 m from CF-FM call with two harmonics for P. dav-
the target to 28-32 msec 1.5-2.5 m from yi in Mexico. We found this species in Be-
the target and then a rapid decline to 16-8 lize to use orientation-search calls that were
msec within 0.5 m of the target (Novick and a short-CF, PM, and short-CF, Both CP
Vaisnys, 1964). We observed several pur- components were absent during the tenni-
suit phases with the CF portion of the sig- nal pursuit phase. Some calls in a sequence
nals ranging from 15 to 11 msec. lacked the initial CF component. The initial
Echolocation calls of Pteronotus person- and terminal CF components were 9 and 4
atus have been examined from Mexico kHz lower, respectively, than Novick's
(Novick, 1963, 1965) and from Panama (1963) values (Table 2). As with the pre-
(Simmons et aI., 1979). Calls from Mexican ceding species, differences may be due to
individuals showed two to four harmonics, equipment, flight conditions, and geograph-
shorter duration, and higher minimum and ic variation.
maximum frequencies than found in the Infonnation on echolocation calls of Sac-
present study (Table 2). Novick (1965) de- copteryx and Rhynchonycteris is more var-
962 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 78, No.3

ied. In Trinidad (Bradbury and Emmons, tion of prey and an FM component for eval-
1974), calls of S. bilineata primarily fea- uating position in relation to clutter. How-
tured low-frequency communication com- ever, her study failed to show the use of an
ponents, but did show several orientation or FM component in calls of Saccopteryx, im-
search calls with an apparent upsweep to ca. plying the same conclusion as Barclay
60 kHz. A more detailed verbal description (1983). We found no prominent FM com-
for this species in Costa Rica described ponent in the calls of this species (Fig. 2).

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article-abstract/78/3/954/905406 by guest on 07 June 2019


paired cruising pulses with the first pulse Past examinations of echolocation sig-
ranging from 47 to 50 kHz and the second nals from R. naso contained a high-fre-
from 50 to 53 kHz (Bradbury and Vehren- quency CF component (Table 1; Bradbury
camp, 1976). This alternating pattern of and Vehrencamp, 1976; Kalko, 1995). The
pulses was first described by Pye (1973), described CF component was followed by
but calls were characterized as CF in nature. a concave downward FM sweep. Bradbury
There was also the indication that pulses and Vehrencamp (1976) noted the presence
from bats in Trinidad were ca. 5 kHz lower. of two harmonics with the greatest energy
The quantitative information from Panama in the second. We found R. naso, in Belize,
and Costa Rica (Barclay, 1983; Kalko, to have two strong harmonics. although the
1995) agree well with that from the present first appeared dominant (Fig. 1). Systems
study (Table I). using zero-crossing analysis, such as Ana-
Pye (1973) noted a frequency difference bat II, perceives the first harmonic as con-
of 2.5-3 kHz between the alternating CF taining the greatest energy. The discrepancy
calls of S. bilineata. He hypothesized that between past and present studies may re-
the LF calls were used for discrimination of flect diffe~nces in sensitivity of equipment.
targets, and the HF calls were used for nav- The vocal sequences for disturbed roosting
igation based on the presence of only LF individuals, demonstrating a progressive in-
calls were found during interception of tar- crease in frequency, has not been described
gets. We found continued alternation of before.
calls during the approach phase (Fig. 2), but It is clear that the Anabat n bat detector
the feeding buzz appeared to be only LF provides detailed structural information of
calls. Similarly, only HF calls were found echolocation calls and that the calls of the
unpaired, suggesting use for orientation or species examined allow for accurate iden-
search for distant targets and supporting the tification of free-flying individuals. The
above hypothesis. However, calls we re- zero-crossings analysis provides an output
corded were not CF, rather a QCF signal that has more detail and clarity than sound
consisting of an extended up sweep and spectrograms obtained with other equip-
shorter down-sweep portion similar to those ment (e.g., Fenton and Bell, 1981; Mac-
signals described by Barclay (1983). As Donald et a1., 1994). Harmonic and ampli-
signals shortened during pursuit, the signals tude infonnation are lost with this analytic
became more horseshoe-shaped. but the approach, but neither has been proven to be
paired nature of initial LF followed by an important for use in identification of spe-
HF component remained for all but the ter- cies. In fact, harmonic displays may add too
minal feeding buzz (Fig. 2). much detail and obscure differences that
Barclay (1983) presented a compelling would allow identification.
argument that the paired QCF signals may Establishment of such vocal signatures
provide a wider bandwidth, thus providing will aid in the ability to perfonn rapid and
more detailed acoustic infonnation allow- accurate inventories, establish activity pat-
ing use of cluttered habitat types. Kalko terns, use of habitat, and other aspects of
(1995) concluded that the four emballon- the behavioral ecology of bat communities
urids in her study used CF-QCF for detec- (Kalko, 1995). However, it is imperative to
August 1991 O'FARRELL AND MILLER-ECHOLOCATION OF NEOTROPICAL BATS 963

establish the range of variation inherent in profile. pp. 88-93, in Trejos. San Jose, Costa Rica,
151 pp.
the vocal repertoire of each species. Geo- HOLDRIDGE, L. R. 1967. Life zone ecology. Revised
graphic variation, temporal shifts in forag- edition. Tropical Science Center. University of San
ing strategies, and intraspecific and inter- Jose, San Jose, Costa Rica, 206 pp.
KALKO, E. K. V. 1995. Echolocation signal design.
specific influences in patterns of calls need foraging habitats and guild structure in six Neotrop-
to be addressed. It also is important to es- ical sheath-tailed bats (Emballonuridae). Pp. 259-
tablish the limitations of the equipment and 273, in Ecology, evolution and behaviour of bats (P.
A Racey and S. M. Swift, eds.). The Zoological

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jmammal/article-abstract/78/3/954/905406 by guest on 07 June 2019


the manner in which acoustic sampling is Society of London. Clarendon Press, Oxford, United
conducted. Properly performed. standard- Kingdom, 421 pp.
ized, acoustic sampling will yield signifi- KALKa, E. K. V., AND H.-U. SCHNITZLER. 1993. Plas-
ticity in echolocation signals of European pipisrrelle
cant information for free-flying bats. Ana- bats in search flight: implications for habitat use and
bat appears to be a powerful tool for ac- prey detection. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiol-
complishing such work. ogy, 33:415-428.
KOOPMAN, K. F. 1993. Order Chiroptera. pp. 137-
241, in Mammal species of the world: a taxonomic
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS and geographic reference. Second ed. (D. E. Wilson
and D. M. Reeder. eds.). Smithsonian Institution
We thank C. M. Miller for field assistance. C. Press, Washington, D.C .• 1206 pp.
Corben, M. B. Fenton, E H. Emmerson, and J. MACDONALD, K., E. MATSUI, R. STEVENS, AND M. B.
A. Simmons provided helpful comments on an FENToN. 1994. Echolocation calls and field identi-
fication of the eastern pipistrelle (Pipistrellus subfia-
earlier version of the manuscript. lbis study was vus: Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae). using ultrasonic
supported by the Wildlife Conservation Society. bat detectors. Journal of Manunalogy, 75:462-465.
NOVICK. A. 1963. Orientation in Neotropical bats. II.
LITERATURE CITED Phyllostomatidae and Desmodontidae. Journal of
Mammalogy, 44:44-56.
BARCLAY, R. M. R. 1983. Echolocation calls of em- - - - . 1965. Echolocation of flying insects by the
ballonurid bats from Panama. Journal of Compara- bat, Chilonycteris psilotis. Biological Bulletin, 128:
tive Physiology, A. Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral 297-314.
Physiology, 151:515-520. NOVICK, A, AND J. R. VAISNYS. 1964. Echolocation
BRADBURY, J. W.• AND L H. EMMONS. 1974. Social of flying insects by the bat, Chilonycteris parnellii.
organization of some Trinidad bats. I. Emballonuri- Biological Bulletin. 127:478-488.
dae. Zeitschrift fUr Tierpsychologie, 36:137-183. OBRlST, M. K. 1995. Flexible bat echolocation: the
BRADBURY. J. W.. AND S. L. VEHRENCAMP. 1976. So- influence of individual, habitat and conspecifics on
cial organization and foraging in emballonurid bats. sonar signal design. Behavioral Ecology and Socio-
I. Field studies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiol- biology. 36:207-219.
ogy, 1:337-381. hE. J. D. 1973. Echolocation by constant frequency
FAURE, P. A •• AND R. M. R. BARCLAY. 1994. Substrate- in bats. Periodicum Biologorum, 75:21-26.
gleaning versus aerial-hawking: plasticity in the for- SIMMONS, J. A, AND M. J. O'FARRELL. 1977. Echo-
aging and echolocation behaviour of the long-eared location by the long-eared bat, Plecotus phyllotis.
bat, Myotis evotis. Journal of Comparative Physiol- Journal of Comparative Physiology, A. Sensory,
ogy A. Sensory. Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 122:201-214.
174:651-660. SIMMONS, J. A, AND R. A. STEIN. 1980. Acoustic im-
aging in bat sonar: echolocation signals and the evo-
FENTON, M. B. 1988. Detecting. recording, and ana-
lution of echolocation. Journal of Comparative
lyzing vocalizations of bats. pp. 91-104, in Ecolog-
Physiology, A. Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral
ical and behavioral methods for the study of bats (1:
Physiology, 135:61-84.
H. Kunz, ed.). Smithsonian Institution Press, Wash-
SIMMONS, J. A, M. B. FENTON, AND M. J. O'FARRELL.
ington, D.C., 533 pp.
1979. Echolocation and pursuit of prey by bats. Sci-
FENTON, M. B., AND G. P. BELL. 1981. Recognition of
ence, 203:16--21.
species of insectivorous bats by their echolocation
SIMMONS. J. A .. ET AL. 1978. Echolocation by free-
calls. Journal of Mammalogy, 62:233-243.
tailed bats (Tadarickl). Journal of Comparative Phys-
GRIFFIN, D. R. 1953. High frequency sounds of trop-
iology, A. Sensory. Neural, and Behavioral Physi-
ical bats. The Anatomical Record, 117:567.
ology. 125:291-299.
GRIFFIN, D. R., AND A. NOVICK. 1955. Acoustic ori-
entation of Neotropical bats. Journal of Experimen- Submitted 4 March 1996. Accepted 4 December 1996.
tal Zoology. 130:251-300.
HARTSHORN. G. 1984. Belize country environmental Associate Editor was Earl G. Zimmennann.

You might also like