You are on page 1of 8

Sensory and Affective Pain Discrimination After Inhalation of Essential Oils

JEFFREY J. GEDNEY, PSYD, TONI L. GLOVER, MA, RN, AND ROGER B. FILLINGIM, PHD
Objective: The purpose of this investigation was to examine the effects of olfactory absorption of two commonly used therapeutic
essential oils on sensory and affective responses to experimentally induced pain. Methods: A sex-balanced (13 men and 13 women)
randomized crossover design was used to obtain pre- and posttreatment change scores for quantitative sensory ratings of contact
heat, pressure, and ischemic pain across separate inhalation treatment conditions using essential oil of lavender, essential oil of
rosemary, and distilled water (control). Subjective reports of treatment-related changes in pain intensity and pain unpleasantness
were obtained for each condition using a visual analog scale. We interpret our findings with respect to the separate dimensions of
sensory and affective processing of pain. Results: Analyses revealed the absence of changes in quantitative pain sensitivity ratings
between conditions. However, retrospectively, subjects’ global impression of treatment outcome indicated that both pain intensity
and pain unpleasantness were reduced after treatment with lavender and marginally reduced after treatment with rosemary,
compared with the control condition. Conclusion: These findings suggest that aromatherapy may not elicit a direct analgesic effect
but instead may alter affective appraisal of the experience and consequent retrospective evaluation of treatment-related pain. Key
words: aromatherapy, sensory testing, pain intensity, pain unpleasantness.

PANAS ⫽ Positive Affect and Negative Affect Scale; RIA ⫽ barium enema suspension, compared with an equal number of
radioimmunoassay; STAI ⫽ State Trait Anxiety Index; VAS ⫽ patients (n ⫽ 70) receiving barium suspension without pep-
visual analog scales. permint oil (10). Woolfson and Hewitt randomized 36 inten-
sive care unit patients to one of three groups: massage with
INTRODUCTION
lavender, massage without lavender, or rest only (21). Treat-

T he use of complementary therapies as adjuncts to tradi-


tional Western medical practice has significantly in-
creased in the United States over the past 2 decades. Rising
ment consisted of 20 minutes of foot massage twice a week for
5 weeks. A significantly greater number of patients in the
lavender massage group demonstrated decreased heart rate,
health care costs and the increasing acceptance of holistic compared with the other two groups. These results suggest
approaches to health care have led to acceptance and use of that oil of lavender reduces physiological arousal above that
previously marginalized therapeutic modalities by both the provided by massage alone or rest only. Similar conclusions
general patient population and healthcare professionals. Aro- were obtained by Buckle, who recorded emotional and behav-
matherapy is the fastest growing complementary therapy (1). ioral stress levels during a randomized double-blind trial using
Aromatherapy includes the inhaled, absorbed, or ingested use two different species of lavender massaged for 20 minutes on
of essential oil extracts from plants and flowers for prophy- 2 consecutive days into the extremities and forehead of two
lactic medical care or active treatment. A growing body of groups of post– cardiac bypass patients (N ⫽ 28) (22). A
clinical and laboratory studies report beneficial effects of significant difference in self-reported stress between the two
essential oils on physiological and psychological processes in
groups was attributed to the differential therapeutic effect
both animals and humans (2–20). Presently, two experimental
between the two species of lavender oil, and not to the effect
animal studies demonstrate physiological effects of essential
of massage because this was common to all treatment condi-
oils. Buchbauer and colleagues showed that under standard-
tions.
ized laboratory conditions, male and female mice displayed a
The relaxant effect of oil of lavender has also been dem-
significant decrease in activity on inhalation of oil of lavender
onstrated in experimental studies with humans. Diego and
(20). In a second experiment, a hyperactive state was induced
colleagues used standardized measures of electroencephalo-
by injection of caffeine, after which inhalation of oil of lav-
gram (EEG) activity, alertness, and mood for 40 adults after
ender produced a sedative effect, significantly reducing the
inhalation of either oil of lavender or oil of rosemary (con-
mice’s motility to nearly their baseline level. In the second
sidered a stimulating odor) (9). Subjects were given simple
published study, Lis-Balchin and colleagues topically applied
math computations before and after the therapy. Subjects who
oil of lavender to an in vitro preparation of guinea-pig ileum
received the lavender aroma reported a significantly improved
after electrical stimulation induced spasm and observed a
mood, displayed a significant improvement on math compu-
significant decrease in spasmodic action (19).
tation accuracy, and exhibited a significant increase in EEG
Three well-designed clinical studies have been published
frontal alpha power, suggesting increased drowsiness. In con-
supporting the benefits of aromatherapy for general medical
trast, the rosemary aroma group showed a decrease in EEG
management. Sparks and colleagues demonstrated that pep-
permint oil significantly reduced colon spasm when added to frontal alpha power and beta power, suggesting increased
alertness. Moss and colleagues also used essential oils of
lavender and rosemary in a study of cognitive performance
From the Division of Public Health Services and Research, University of and mood in which 144 volunteers were randomly assigned to
Florida College of Dentistry, Gainesville, FL.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Roger Fillingim, 1600 one of three groups: lavender, rosemary, or control (23).
SW Archer Road, Box 100404, Gainesville, FL 32610. E-mail: Subjects were deceived as to the genuine aim of the study and
rfillingim@dental.ufl.edu were asked to complete a computerized cognitive assessment
Received for publication October 23, 2003; revision received March 26,
2004. battery. Visual analog mood questionnaires were completed
DOI: 10.1097/01.psy.0000132875.01986.47 before exposure to each condition and after completion of the

Psychosomatic Medicine 66:599 – 606 (2004) 599


0033-3174/04/6604-0599
Copyright © 2004 by the American Psychosomatic Society
J. J. GEDNEY et al.

cognitive assessment battery. They found that lavender pro- METHODS


duced a decrease in performance of attention and working A randomized crossover design was used to test for changes in objective
memory capacity and reaction times compared with controls. and subjective reports of pain sensitivity after inhalation of commercially
Rosemary enhanced memory performance but reduced mem- available essential oil of lavender (Lavandula angustifolia) and essential oil of
rosemary (Rosmarinus officinalis) (Elizabeth VanBuren Aromatherapy, Santa
ory reaction times compared with controls. In terms of posttest Cruz, CA), species most commonly used in clinical and research applications.
mood ratings, the control group was significantly less content Purity of the oils was documented by the distributor using mass spectrometry
than both the rosemary and lavender groups. Other studies and gas chromatography. Distilled water was used as a control. The three
have similarly reported improved memory performance (2,3) sessions were separated by 48 to 96 hours and were identical with the
exception of the treatment condition and completion of additional question-
and alertness (7) after olfactory exposure to different odors.
naires during the first session. Quantitative pain measures included contact
Very few studies have investigated the effects of aroma- heat (threshold, tolerance, and temporal summation of heat pain), pressure
therapy in the treatment and management of pain (5,6,8,24). pain threshold (trapezius and masseter), and ischemic pain threshold and
Gobel and colleagues used a crossover design to compare tolerance (circulatory occlusion of the upper arm) pain tasks. Procedures were
treatment effect between inhaled peppermint, acetamino- identical to those previously reported in the published pain literature (25).
Subjective measures consisted of subjects’ self-reported change in sensory
phen, peppermint plus acetaminophen, and placebo for intensity and pain unpleasantness for each condition.
headache pain in 41 adult males (11). Peppermint and A male and female experimenter were present during each session (6,26 –
acetaminophen were equally effective in reducing headache 29). To control for hormonal fluctuation believed to influence pain sensitivity
pain and both were significantly better than placebo. Gobel (30), all sessions were conducted during the follicular phase (between days 4
and 10 after menses) (31). To minimize measurement error, the same exper-
et al. topically administered preparations of peppermint oil,
imenter obtained measures during each of the three sessions, with male and
eucalyptus, and ethanol to the forehead and temples of 32 female experimenters randomized between subjects.
healthy males, using a double-blind, randomized, crossover
design (12). Pre- and postmanipulation measures included Subjects
temporal electromyogram (EMG) activity, mood state, and Twenty-six healthy, nonsmoking and unmedicated adult subjects (13 men
and 13 premenopausal women) were recruited from a pool of subjects who
sensitivity to experimental pain. Significant results in-
completed a similar experimental pain protocol within the past 18 months.
cluded decreased emotional irritation, decreased resting These subjects were thus familiar with the quantitative sensory testing appa-
EMG, and diminished ischemic pain only for those prepa- ratus and methodology, which reduced novelty effects. Subjects were blinded
rations containing peppermint oil. Marchand and Arsenault to the study’s hypotheses and treatment conditions. The study protocol con-
investigated the effects of pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral formed to the Institutional Review Board of the University of Florida. Sub-
jects were paid $75 for completion of the study. All subjects completed the
odors on mood and pain ratings (6). Twenty men and 20 study.
women individually rank ordered a series of common odors
(eg, orange water, vinegar, baby oil) as pleasant, unpleas- Session Baseline
ant, or neutral. They then completed a series of 3-minute On arrival, subjects provided informed consent and were comfortably
seated in a reclining chair in a sound-attenuated room. Subjects then com-
hand immersions in a hot circulating bath (46 – 48°C) while
pleted psychological measures assessing affectivity (Positive Affect and Neg-
continually smelling one of the three previously selected ative Affect Scale; PANAS (32), measures of trait and situational anxiety
odors (most pleasant, most unpleasant, and closest to neu- (State Trait Anxiety Index; STAI) (33), and present mood, using the Profile
tral). Pain intensity and pain unpleasantness ratings were of Mood States (34). Demographics and health history information and a
reported every 15 seconds during each trial. Women per- questionnaire assessing the subject’s familiarity with and attitudes about
complementary and alternative therapy modalities were completed during the
ceived significantly less pain intensity and pain unpleas- first session.
antness during inhalation of the pleasant odor relative to the
unpleasant or neutral odors. Pain intensity and unpleasant- Experimental Pain Tasks
ness did not differ across odor conditions for men. These Thermal Pain Threshold, Tolerance, and Temporal
findings suggest a sex-dependent effect of pleasant odor on Summation Procedures
perceived pain intensity and unpleasantness. It remains to Thermal pain stimuli were delivered using a computer-controlled Medoc
be seen whether therapeutic essential oils produce similar Thermal Sensory Analyzer (TSA-2001, Ramat Yishai, Israel), which is a
peltier-element-based stimulator using a 3 ⫻ 3-cm thermode. The thermode
effects. was placed on the right dorsal forearm. Its position was altered slightly
The purpose of this investigation was to examine the ef- between trials in order to avoid either sensitization or habituation of cutaneous
fects of olfactory absorption of two commonly used therapeu- receptors. The maximum permitted temperature was 52°C. Subjects could
tic essential oils on sensory and affective responses to exper- terminate the heat stimulus at any point by pressing a control device button or
by verbal command.
imentally induced pain. We hypothesized that inhalation of
Heat pain threshold and tolerance were obtained three times each. On
essential oil of lavender and rosemary, both essential oils with initiation of each trial, the thermal contact increased in temperature at a rate
purported analgesic properties (1,8) but opposite effects on of 0.5°C/second from a baseline temperature of 32°C. For pain threshold,
affective arousal, would decrease pain in response to noxious subjects were instructed to press a control button “when the heat first becomes
heat, pressure, and ischemic stimuli. Second, we hypothesized painful, when you first feel pain.” For pain tolerance, subjects were instructed
to press the button “when you no longer feel able to tolerate the pain.”
subjective reports of pain intensity and pain unpleasantness to Temporal summation of thermal pain was assessed using a series of 10
be less after separate inhalation treatment sessions with both consecutive 0.5-second-long heat pulses, with an interpulse interval of 2.5
essential oil of lavender and rosemary. seconds. Separate series of 10 trials with target temperatures of 49°C and

600 Psychosomatic Medicine 66:599 – 606 (2004)


AROMATHERAPY AND PAIN DISCRIMINATION
52°C were delivered to the right ventral forearm. At each pulse peak, subjects procedures were as follows: a) All standards and controls provided were
rated the intensity of the stimulus using a verbal rating scale, with anchors of diluted 1:10 with the cortisol assay dilution buffer. This provided a working
0 (no pain) and 100 (most intense pain imaginable). A rating of 20 was standard curve range of (0.05– 6.00 ␮g/dl); b) All standards, controls and
defined as “barely painful.” unknowns were assayed in duplicate at 100 ␮l; c) Cortisol (I125) reagent and
cortisol antiserum complex were added in 100-␮l volumes to all appropriate
Pressure Pain Procedure tubes; and d) All samples were incubated overnight at room temperature. All
other RIA procedures followed the protocol provided in the DSL-2000
Pressure pain threshold was measured using a digital, handheld, clinical
cortisol RIA kit. With the above modifications, this provided an assay system
grade pressure algometer (Jtech, Heber City, UT). Pressure was applied at the
that had a sensitivity of 0.05 ␮g/dl (at 90% binding) and an intra-assay CV
rate of 0.5 kg/second to the right upper trapezius and the right masseter, using
a 0.5-cm2 probe. Subjects were instructed to respond verbally when “the and interassay CV of 3.95% and 4.94%, respectively.
pressure first becomes painful, when you first feel pain.” Three trials were
completed for each site. Postsession Questionnaire
After completing each session’s posttreatment experimental pain proce-
Ischemic Pain Procedure dures, subjects completed a postsession evaluation questionnaire. Using
100-mm visual analog scales (VAS), subjects were asked to rate whether the
Ischemic pain was assessed using the submaximal effort tourniquet pro-
session’s odor was relaxing (0-mm anchor) or stimulating (100-mm anchor).
cedure (35). The ischemic pain task consisted of elevating the right arm above
Ratings of 50 mm indicated a neutral rating for each VAS. Also, subjects rated
heart level for 30 seconds. Circulation was then occluded using a standard
whether the session’s aroma increased pain intensity and pain unpleasantness
blood pressure cuff inflated to 240 mm Hg. The arm was then lowered and the
(0-mm anchor) or decreased pain intensity and pain unpleasantness (100-mm
subject completed 20 hand-grip exercises of 2-second duration at 4-second
anchor). Finally, subjects rated the intensity of the odor using a 100-mm VAS
intervals at 50% of their maximum grip strength. Subjects were instructed to
with anchors of “not noticeable” (0 mm) and “intense” (100 mm).
report when they first felt pain (pain threshold) and when the pain became
intolerable (pain tolerance), and the time required to reach these two end-
points was recorded. Every 30 seconds after the first report of ischemic pain,
Analysis
subjects rated in alternate order their ischemic pain intensity and unpleasant- Between-session differences for baseline anxiety, affectivity, and post-
ness using a 0 to 20 combined verbal–numerical box scales (36). The treatment evaluation questionnaire data were tested using a series of one-way
procedure was terminated when one of the following occurred: a rating of 20 analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Wilcoxon Z for related, nonparametric
was obtained, on request by the subject, or after 15 minutes. samples. A series of 2 Time (pretreatment, posttreatment) ⫻ 3 Condition
(control, lavender, rosemary) repeated-measures ANOVA were used to test
Treatment Condition for time-by-condition interaction effects on sensory test scores, cardiovascular
measures (systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, heart rate) and
Treatment consisted of the randomized application of 5 drops of essential
salivary cortisol. Main effects for sex were tested by entering sex as a
oil of lavender, rosemary, or distilled water to a 2 ⫻ 2-inch cotton gauze. The
between-subjects factor for each ANOVA. Bonferroni adjustments were used
gauze was attached to the front of the subject’s garment, approximately 12
to correct for post hoc pairwise comparisons. Two measures were derived
inches below their nose. Subjects were directed to breathe normally while
from the temporal summation procedure. First, the average of all 10 pain
they sat quietly in a recliner for 10 minutes. The gauze remained in place
ratings were computed. Then, the maximum increase was calculated as the
while subjects then began the posttreatment experimental pain procedures.
difference between the ratings of first heat pulse and the maximum rating
This procedure reflects a modification of that used by others (8) by increasing
from the remaining pulses. Average ratings of ischemic pain intensity and
the application dose and duration of exposure (24).
unpleasantness were computed by calculating the mean of all ratings for each
subject. For subjects who terminated the task before the time limit, their last
Salivary Cortisol rating was carried forward for all future time points.
Saliva samples for determination of cortisol were obtained to coincide
with the end of the resting period, the end of treatment, and the midpoints of RESULTS
the pre- and posttreatment pain tasks. Samples were collected 25 minutes after Baseline
each of these time points, thus coinciding with the peak cortisol concentration
present in the saliva (37). Samples were collected using an absorbent cotton No differences in baseline measures of salivary cortisol, STAI, or
pellet (Salivette, Sarstedt Inc., Newton, NC) and frozen at ⫺20°C. Before PANAS emerged between the three treatment conditions (p val-
assay, saliva was extracted from the Salivettes by thawing and then centrif- ues ⬎ .1), demonstrating the absence of a session order effect.
ugation (Beckman refrigerated centrifuge, Model J-6B) at 3000 RPM at 4°C Accordingly, order was not controlled in the analyses.
for 20 minutes. Salivary cortisol levels were determined using the DSL-2000
cortisol radioimmunoassay (RIA) kit (Diagnostic Systems Laboratories, Inc., Physiological Responses
Webster, TX) with modifications. The modifications increased the sensitivity
of the cortisol assay and provided a reliable assay system to detect salivary Tables 1 and 2 show that the Time ⫻ Condition interaction
cortisol in all samples provided. Modifications of the standard cortisol RIA was not significant for cortisol or for any of the cardiovascular

TABLE 1. Salivary Cortisol Concentration (␮g/dl) Obtained at Baseline and Immediately Posttreatment, Reported for Sex by Treatment
Condition (Standard deviation reported in parentheses)

Time ⫻
Sex main
Control Lavender Rosemary condit.
effect
interaction

Baseline Treatment Baseline Treatment Baseline Treatment F(1,23) p F(1,24) p

Men .183 (.091) .109 (.036) .168 (.075) .120 (.046) .188 (.117) .153 (.113) 0.53 .59 ⬍.01 .97
Women .170 (.062) .139 (.091) .163 (.063) .122 (.056) .181 (.056) .149 (.070)

Psychosomatic Medicine 66:599 – 606 (2004) 601


J. J. GEDNEY et al.
TABLE 2. Cardiovascular (CV) Measures (mm Hg and bpm) Obtained at Baseline and Immediately Posttreatment, Reported for Sex by
Treatment Condition

Time ⫻
Sex main
Control Lavender Rosemary condit.
effect
CV measure interaction

Baseline Treatment Baseline Treatment Baseline Treatment F(1,23) p F(1,24) p

SBP
Men 128.0 (12.5) 122.7 (11.6) 124.0 (14.9) 121.3 (12.1) 118.2 (13.0) 118.2 (13.0) 1.18 .32 10.46 ⬍.01
Women 109.0 (10.8) 108.8 (11.7) 107.5 (9.8) 107.6 (9.1) 107.6 (9.1) 107.4 (10.0)
DBP
Men 75.9 (13.2) 74.0 (12.8) 73.0 (10.8) 76.6 (10.8) 72.5 (10.4) 68.3 (9.4) 0.03 .97 0.69 .42
Women 72.4 (6.6) 73.2 (9.6) 70.5 (7.9) 70.5 (8.7) 71.1 (7.0) 74.1 (7.1)
HR
Men 71.2 (10.1) 69.2 (10.4) 66.7 (9.2) 68.0 (9.5) 71.0 (10.2) 69.2 (10.3) 0.07 .94 0.30 .59
Women 70.9 (7.2) 70.6 (10.0) 74.2 (11.0) 73.9 (10.7) 73.1 (8.5) 72.0 (8.7)

a
A significant main effect for sex was obtained for SBP. Standard deviations are reported in parentheses.
SBP ⫽ systolic blood pressure; DBP ⫽ diastolic blood pressure; HR ⫽ heart rate.

TABLE 3. Heat Threshold, Tolerance, and Pressure Pain Procedures Reported for Sex by Treatment Conditiona

Time ⫻
Sex main
Control Lavender Rosemary condit.
effect
Sensory test interaction

Pre-Tx Post-Tx Pre-Tx Post-Tx Pre-Tx Post-Tx F(1,23) p F(1,24) p

Heat threshold (°C)


Men 45.0 (2.0) 44.8 (1.6) 45.0 (2.8) 45.0 (2.6) 45.0 (2.4) 44.8 (2.4) 0.10 .91 2.26 .15
Women 43.7 (2.1) 43.8 (2.5) 44.0 (1.7) 43.7 (1.4) 43.9 (2.1) 44.1 (2.1)
Heat tolerance (°C)
Men 50.0 (1.3) 49.4 (1.4) 50.0 (1.5) 49.6 (1.3) 49.8 (1.2) 49.7 (1.3) 0.59 .56 6.69 .02
Women 48.5 (1.6) 48.2 (1.7) 48.6 (1.9) 48.3 (1.3) 48.7 (1.1) 48.2 (1.4)
Pressure, masseter (kg/cm2)
Men 3.6 (1.1) 3.5 (1.1) 3.5 (0.8) 3.6 (0.9) 3.1 (0.8) 3.1 (0.9) 0.17 .80 13.63 ⬍.01
Women 2.5 (0.6) 2.5 (0.7) 2.5 (0.7) 2.4 (0.8) 2.6 (0.6) 2.6 (0.8)
Pressure, trapezius (kg/cm2)
Men 7.8 (1.8) 7.2 (1.7) 7.6 (1.6) 7.4 (1.6) 6.3 (2.1) 6.2 (2.1) 0.23 .79 10.44 ⬍.01
Women 4.7 (1.6) 4.8 (1.5) 4.7 (1.7) 4.4 (1.7) 5.2 (1.8) 5.0 (2.1)

a
Significant main effects for sex were obtained for Heat Tolerance and Masseter and Trapezius Pressure. Significant main effects of time emerged for Heat
Tolerance and Pressure Pain Threshold at the Trapezius. Standard deviations are reported in parentheses.

measures (p values ⱖ .32), suggesting the absence of neu- tolerance and greater windup (heat pulse) pain relative to men.
roendocrine or autonomic responses to treatment. With the Main effects of time emerged for several pain measures (Heat
exception of systolic blood pressure, the main effect for sex Tolerance, Trapezius Pressure, Ischemic Threshold and 49°C
was likewise not significant, demonstrating that men and and 52°C Maximum Heat Ratings), with all effects except
women responded similarly and consistently within and be- Ischemic Threshold in the direction of greater pain sensitivity
tween sessions (systolic blood pressure: p ⫽ .02, all others: during the postassessment.
p ⬎ .31).
Posttreatment Questionnaires
Experimental Pain Procedures Table 5 and Figure 1 present data from the postsession
Tables 3 and 4 show that the Time ⫻ Condition interaction questionnaire. A significant difference in the strength of odor
was significant only for the 49°C Heat Pulse, calculated using between the control condition and each of the two essential
the pain rating change across the trial. Scores differed between oils was reported (p values ⬍ .01). The Lavender treatment,
the Lavender and Rosemary conditions (p ⫽ .01), but not but not the Rosemary treatment was rated as marginally more
between the Control and aroma treatment conditions. Signif- stimulating than the control condition (p values ⫽ .06 and
icant main effects for sex were obtained for Heat Tolerance, 0.78, respectively). Retrospectively, subjects indicated greater
Masseter and Trapezius Pressure and 49°C and 52°C Heat reductions in pain intensity and pain unpleasantness after
Pulse (maximum rating change across the trial) (p values ⱕ treatment with lavender, compared with the control condition
.02). Consistently, women reported lower heat and pressure evaluations. Specifically, men reported diminished pain inten-

602 Psychosomatic Medicine 66:599 – 606 (2004)


AROMATHERAPY AND PAIN DISCRIMINATION
TABLE 4. Heat Pulse and Ischemic Pain Procedures Reported for Sex by Treatment Conditiona

Time ⫻
Sex main
Control Lavender Rosemary condit.
effect
Sensory test interaction

Pre-Tx Post-Tx Pre-Tx Post-Tx Pre-Tx Post-Tx F p F p

Heat pulse, 49°C max. rating


Men 24.7 (8.4) 26.1 (8.3) 27.3 (8.0) 30.5 (11.3) 25.7 (11.3) 23.8 (9.5) 5.10 .01 7.05 .01
Women 41.7 (23.5) 48.7 (24.5) 39.7 (24.0) 49.6 (26.5) 37.4 (16.5) 39.1 (16.9)
Heat pulse, 49°C rating change
Men 5.3 (5.3) 5.8 (4.9) 6.5 (4.3) 6.4 (6.3) 6.1 (3.6) 6.1 (4.4) 0.01 .99 2.16 .15
Women 10.7 (13.4) 9.1 (13.4) 9.8 (9.7) 9.3 (9.7) 11.3 (10.7) 10.5 (8.4)
Heat pulse, 52°C max. rating
Men 44.8 (18.6) 49.8 (24.2) 49.1 (19.8) 52.3 (18.5) 45.4 (19.7) 47.7 (21.4) 0.53 .59 9.37 .01
Women 73.4 (25.8) 79.0 (25.1) 71.3 (25.8) 74.9 (27.9) 69.2 (21.4) 78.3 (25.7)
Heat pulse, 52°C rating change
Men 12.9 (11.4) 15.5 (9.3) 19.2 (14.0) 19.4 (18.5) 18.8 (13.6) 18.2 (15.8) 0.36 .70 0.01 .93
Women 17.5 (15.9) 13.7 (13.4) 17.7 (20.0) 15.4 (16.9) 25.8 (13.3) 18.5 (13.9)
Ischemic, intensity
Men 12.0 (5.6) 11.4 (6.0) 10.9 (5.3) 11.0 (5.8) 12.0 (5.3) 11.3 (5.7) 1.16 .32 0.35 .56
Women 10.0 (5.4) 9.9 (5.9) 10.7 (5.6) 10.4 (5.7) 10.9 (4.9) 10.3 (5.6)
Ischemic, unpleasantness
Men 12.4 (5.3) 11.6 (5.7) 11.3 (4.9) 11.3 (5.6) 11.9 (4.8) 11.7 (5.4) 0.33 .72 0.03 .86
Women 11.5 (5.4) 11.4 (5.6) 12.3 (5.9) 12.0 (6.1) 11.7 (5.0) 11.6 (5.6)
Ischemic, threshold (seconds)
Men 141 (078) 193 (185) 164 (124) 202 (182) 150 (113) 190 (190) 1.88 .17 0.65 .43
Women 242 (237) 247 (240) 233 (252) 220 (236) 219 (193) 237 (228)
Ischemic, tolerance (seconds)
Men 611 (252) 659 (229) 705 (240) 712 (223) 651 (239) 709 (217) 1.06 .36 0.22 .64
Women 642 (266) 646 (273) 620 (276) 601 (264) 630 (288) 637 (288)

a
A significant time ⫻ condition interaction was obtained for heat pulse, 49°C (maximum rating), where pain rating differences were obtained between the
lavender and rosemary treatment conditions. Significant main effects for sex were obtained for heat pulse, 49°C and 52°C (maximum rating). Significant main
effects of time emerged for maximum ratings at 49°C and 52°C and for Ischemic Threshold. Standard deviations are reported in parentheses.

TABLE 5. Subjective Ratings of Aromatherapy Treatment, Reported by Treatment Condition: Paired-Samples T-Tests Reveal a Significant
Effect of Lavender (and Marginal Effect for Rosemary) on Perceived Intensity, and a Significant Effect of Lavender on Perceived Unpleasantness

Control– Lavender–
Control Lavender Rosemary Control–Rosemary
Lavender Rosemary
Odor ratinga
Mean Mean Mean
t pⱕ t pⱕ t pⱕ
(SD) (SD) (SD)

Strength of odor (0 ⫽ not noticeable, 7.0 (13.2) 61.3 (18.3) 65.9 (22.1) 11.5 .01 12.7 .01 0.7 .48
100 ⫽ strong)
Property of odor (0 ⫽ stimulating, 47.2 (7.9) 36.3 (26.9) 45.2 (24.5) 2.0 .06 0.3 .78 1.4 .17
100 ⫽ relaxing)
Odor effect on perceived pain intensity 51.3 (3.2) 56.2 (7.8) 55.8 (10.3) 2.9 .01b 2.0 .06 0.6 .53
(0 ⫽ increase, 100 ⫽ decrease)
Odor effect on perceived pain unpl. 50.8 (2.2) 56.4 (9.7) 54.1 (14.9) 2.7 .01c 1.1 .30 1.0 .31
(0 ⫽ increase, 100 ⫽ decrease)

a
Ratings based on a 100-mm visual analog scale. The midpoint (50 mm) is neutral for the last three rating scales. Standard deviations are reported in parentheses.
b
Men: Wilcoxon z ⫽ 2.14, p ⫽ .03; Women: Wilcoxon z ⫽ 1.68, p ⫽ .09.
c
Men: ns; women: Wilcoxon z ⫽ 2.08, p ⫽ .04.

sity (Wilcoxon z ⫽ 2.14, p ⫽ .03), whereas women reported and posttreatment psychophysical pain responses along with
diminished pain unpleasantness (Wilcoxon z ⫽ 2.08, p ⫽ .04). retrospective evaluations of analgesic effects were examined.

DISCUSSION Effect of Aromatherapy on Physiological Response


This study employed quantitative sensory testing to mea- The absence of pre- posttreatment differences indicates that
sure the analgesic effect of inhalation treatment using essential our treatment conditions did not elicit a detectable neuroen-
oil of lavender and essential oil of rosemary. Changes in pre- docrine or autonomic response. The absence of a cortisol sex

Psychosomatic Medicine 66:599 – 606 (2004) 603


J. J. GEDNEY et al.

Figure 1. Bar charts reporting subject ratings of odor strength and effect of treatment on pain intensity and pain unpleasantness. Significant differences were
obtained for pain intensity and pain unpleasantness between the Lavender and Control conditions, while a moderate difference for pain intensity was obtained
between the Rosemary and Control condition (Error bars ⫽ SEM).

main effect indicates that both men’s and women’s neuroen- most distressing would have been less likely to volunteer for
docrine responses were equal at the end of the 10-minute quiet another laboratory pain experiment.
resting baseline and treatment periods. Though not signifi- To our knowledge, this is the first time that quantitative
cantly different (with the exception of systolic blood pres- sensory testing with healthy subjects has been used to test
sure), cardiovascular sex effects were in the expected direc- for possible analgesic effects of inhaled essential oils. In
tion. the only other published experimental pain report, Gobel et
al. topically administered four different test preparations to
Effect of Aromatherapy on Quantitative Sensory the forehead and temples of 32 healthy males, using a
Testing double-blind, randomized, crossover design (12). Test
The results of quantitative sensory testing revealed a sig- preparations included combinations of peppermint oil, eu-
nificant pre–post treatment difference across experimental calyptus, and ethanol. Pre- and postmanipulation measures
conditions only for the 49°C Heat Pulse task, when ratings included temporal EMG activity, mood state, and sensitiv-
were calculated as the difference between the first and last of ity to experimental pain. Pain stimuli were selected to simulate
the 10 pulses. It is noteworthy that this apparent treatment tension-type headache symptoms. Stimuli included ischemic
effect was obtained between the Lavender and Rosemary pain (pressure cuff placed around the cranium), mechanical
treatment conditions, and not between the Control and either pressure pain (pressure algometer applied to the scalp and
essential oil treatment condition. Thus, the absence of control middle phalanx of the middle finger), and thermal pain (con-
matched changes in pain response after inhalation of essential tact thermistor applied to the forehead). After treatment with
oils indicates that the treatment condition we employed pro- peppermint oil, subjects reported decreased emotional irrita-
duced no analgesic effects. As reported by others (6), main sex tion and demonstrated decreased resting EMG. Ischemic pain,
effects were observed for several of the pain measures (Heat but not thermal or mechanical pain ratings were significantly
Tolerance, 49°C and 52°C Heat Pulse and Masseter and reduced. It is possible that the topical application, versus the
Trapezius Pressure). All women in this study were scheduled inhalation used in our study, was responsible for the pain-
during the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle (4 –9 days reducing effects of their intervention. It remains to be seen
after the onset of menses). It is possible that controlling for how the topical application of essential oils may affect quan-
cycle phase eliminated potential sex differences for the other titative sensory testing.
pain measures (4). Also, all subjects in this study had partic-
ipated in a previous experimental pain investigation; therefore, Retrospective Reports of Treatment Effects
reduced novelty effects may have diminished any sex differ- Retrospective evaluation of the effects of aromatherapy
ences. Moreover, a selection bias may have attenuated sex suggest that when asked to self-report any perceived change in
differences, because those subjects who found the pain stimuli pain sensitivity after treatment, subjects reported that they

604 Psychosomatic Medicine 66:599 – 606 (2004)


AROMATHERAPY AND PAIN DISCRIMINATION

experienced less pain intensity and pain unpleasantness after ceived pain. Therefore, given the minimal side effects of this
the Lavender treatment and a trend toward less pain intensity intervention, aromatherapy may be helpful in treatment set-
after the Rosemary treatment, relative to the Control condi- tings associated with both pain and heightened affective
tion. There was no clear sex-related trend in this self-report, arousal, such as dental care or the outpatient treatment setting.
although men reported diminished pain intensity and women
reported diminished pain unpleasantness in the Lavender con- REFERENCES
dition. Although the inhalation of the essential oils did not 1. Buckle J, Clinical aromatherapy in nursing. 2nd ed. New York: Arnold
elicit a detectable analgesic response, the use of these oils may Publishers; 1997.
have provided an additional pleasant sensory and affective 2. Ehrlichman H, Halpern JN. Affect and memory: effects of pleasant and
unpleasant odors on retrieval of happy and unhappy memories. J Pers Soc
stimulus, which could alter overall evaluation of the pain Psychol 1988;55:769 –79.
experience, thus influencing retrospective evaluation of the 3. Degel J, Koster EP. Odors: implicit memory and performance effects.
treatment effects. This interpretation is consistent with those Chem Senses 1999;24:317–25.
4. Lehrer J, Eckersberger C, Walla P, Potsch G, Deecke L. Ambient odor of
of others who have demonstrated aroma-mediated changes orange in a dental office reduces anxiety and mood in female patients.
in affect in both the clinical and laboratory setting Physiol Behav 2000;71:83– 6.
(4 – 6,9,18,22,24,38 – 40). The role of affectivity as an in- 5. Dale A, Cornwell S. The role of lavender oil in relieving perineal
discomfort following childbirth: a blind randomized clinical trial. J Adv
tegral component of the pain experience has been well Nurs 1994;19:89 –96.
documented (41– 47). From the results of the present study, 6. Marchand S, Arsenault P. Odors modulate pain perception: a gender-
it appears that the use of aromatherapy for the management specific effect. Physiol Behav 2002;76:251– 6.
7. Ilmberger J, Heuberger E, Mahrhofer C, Dessovic H, Kowarik D, Buch-
of acute pain may produce beneficial effects not through bauer G. The influence of essential oils on human attention. I: alertness.
direct physiological mechanisms, but by providing a com- Chem Senses 2001;26:239 – 45.
peting pleasant sensory and affective experience that can 8. Buckle J. Use of aromatherapy as a complementary treatment for chronic
pain. Alternative Ther 1999;5:42–51.
alter retrospective pain evaluation. 9. Diego MA, Jones NA, Field T, Hernandez-Reif M, Schanberg S, Kuhn C,
McAdam V, Galamaga R, Galamaga M. Aromatherapy positively affects
Limitations mood, EEG patterns of alertness and math computations. Int J Neurosci
1998;96:217–24.
Several limitations of this study deserve mention. First, 10. Sparks MJ, O’Sullivan P, Herrington AA, Morcos SK. Does peppermint
subjects in this study were familiar with sensory testing meth- oil relieve spasm during barium enema? Br J Radiol 1995;68:841–3.
ods through previous participation in another sensory study, 11. Gobel H, Fresenius J, Heinze A, Dworschak M, Soyka D. Effectiveness
of Oleum menthae piperitae and paracetamol in therapy of headache of
which is both a strength and a limitation. The benefit is that the tension type. Nervenarzt 1996;67:672– 81.
the variability associated with novelty effects was reduced; 12. Gobel H, Schmidt G, Soyka D. Effect of peppermint and eucalyptus oil
however, recruiting experienced subjects may have introduced preparations on neurophysiological and experimental algesimetric head-
ache parameters. Cephalalgia 1994;14:228 –34.
a selection bias, and a different pattern of results may have 13. Bengtsson S, Berglund H, Gulyas B, Cohen E, Savic I. Brain activation
emerged with naı̈ve participants. Another limitation is the during odor perception in males and females. Neuroreport 2001;12:
relatively small sample size; however, given the minimal 2027–33.
14. Hardy M, Kirk-Smith MD, Stretch DD. Replacement of drug treatment
effect sizes, the sample size required to detect significant for insomnia using ambient odour. Lancet 1995;346:701.
aromatherapy effects on psychophysical responses would be 15. Liu JH, Chen GH, Yeh HZ, Huang CK, Poon SK. Enteric-coated pep-
prohibitive. In addition, the aromatherapy intervention may permint-oil capsules in the treatment of irritable bowel syndrome: a
prospective, randomized trial. J Gastroenterol 1997;32:765– 8.
not have been potent enough to alter pain responses, and a 16. Saeki Y, Shiohara M. Physiological effects of inhaling fragrances. Int J
more intense and/or more prolonged aromatherapy treatment Aromather 2001;11:118 –25.
may have been more effective. Lastly, it is possible that 17. Saeki Y. The effect of foot-bath with or without the essential oil of
lavender on the autonomic nervous system: a randomized trial. Comple-
providing odor property ratings (ie, analog rating from stim- ment Ther Med 2000;8:2–7.
ulating to relaxing) immediately before the posttreatment pain 18. Edge J. A pilot study addressing the effect of aromatherapy massage on
ratings may have biased a response toward diminished pain mood, anxiety and relaxation in adult mental health. Complement Ther
Nurs Midwifery 2003;9:90 –7.
ratings. However, this seems unlikely as the subjects’ affec- 19. Lis-Balchin M, Hart S, Wan Hang, Lo B. Jasmine absolute (Jasminum
tive response to the odor was established before the posttreat- grandiflora L.) and its mode of action on guinea-pig ileum in vitro.
ment pain stimuli and postsession questionnaire. Phytother Res 2002;16:437–9.
20. Buchbauer G, Jirovetz L, Jager W, Plank C, Dietrich H. Fragrance
compounds and essential oils with sedative effects upon inhalation.
CONCLUSION J Pharm Sci 1993;82:660 – 4.
In this study, we demonstrated that inhalation of essential 21. Woolfson A, Hewitt D. Intensive aromacare. Int J Aromather 1992;4:
12– 4.
oil of lavender and essential oil of rosemary does not produce 22. Buckle J. Does it matter which lavender essential oil is used? Nurs Times
a detectable analgesic effect; however, subjects’ retrospective 1993;89:32–5.
evaluations of aroma-induced changes in pain intensity and 23. Moss M, Cook J, Wesnes K, Duckett P. Aromas of rosemary and
lavender essential oils differentially affect cognition and mood in healthy
pain unpleasantness suggested that they perceived some ben- adults. Int J Neurosci 2003;113:15–38.
efit of the intervention, especially for lavender. Thus, in a 24. Ching M. Contemporary therapy: aromatherapy in the management of
typical clinical evaluation protocol that relies on retrospective acute pain? Contemp Nurse 1999;8:146 –51.
25. Edwards RR, Fillingim RB. Age-associated differences in responses to
evaluations of treatment effectiveness, aromatherapy may pro- noxious stimuli. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2001;56:M180 –5.
duce a clinically relevant shift in the patient’s report of per- 26. Robinson ME, Riley JL 3rd, Myers CD, Papas RK, Wise EA, Waxenberg

Psychosomatic Medicine 66:599 – 606 (2004) 605


J. J. GEDNEY et al.
LB, Fillingim RB. Gender role expectations of pain: relationship to sex crine research: recent developments and applications. Psychoneuroendo-
differences in pain. J Pain 2001;2:251–7. crinology 1994;19:313–33.
27. Myers CD, Robinson ME, Riley JL 3rd, Sheffield D. Sex, gender, and 38. Baron RA. Environmentally induced positive affect: its impact on self-
blood pressure: contributions to experimental pain report. Psychosom efficacy, task performance, negotiation, and conflict. J Appl Soc Psychol
Med 2001;63:545–50. 1990;20:368 – 84.
28. Wise EA, Price DD, Myers CD, Heft MW, Robinson ME. Gender role 39. Robin O, Alaoui-Ismaili O, Dittmar A, Vernet-Maury E. Basic emotions
expectations of pain: relationship to experimental pain perception. Pain evoked by eugenol odor differ according to the dental experience. A
2002;96:335– 42. neurovegetative analysis. Chem Senses 1999;24:327–35.
29. Yousem DM, Maldjian JA, Siddiqi F, Hummel T, Alsop DC, Geckle RJ, 40. Wheeler-Robins JL. The science and art of aromatherapy. J Holist Nurs
Bilker WB, Doty RL. Gender effects on odor-stimulated functional 1999;17:5–17.
magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Res 1999;818:480 –7. 41. Price DD, Riley JL 3rd, Wade JB. Psychophysical approaches to the
30. Fillingim RB, Maixner W, Girdler SS, Light KC, Harris MB, Sheps DS, measurement of the dimensions and stages of pain. In: Melzack R, Turk
Mason GA. Ischemic but not thermal pain sensitivity varies across the DC, editors. Handbook of pain measurement. New York, NY: The
menstrual cycle. Psychosom Med 1997;59:512–20. Guilford Press; 2001. p. 53–75.
31. Pfleeger M, Straneva PA, Fillingim RB, Maixner W, Girdler SS. Men-
42. Gedney JJ, Logan HL, Baron RS. Predictors of short-term and long-term
strual cycle, blood pressure and ischemic pain sensitivity in women: a
memory of sensory and affective dimensions of pain. J Pain 2003;4:
preliminary investigation. Int J Psychophysiol 1997;27:161– 6.
47–55.
32. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief
measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc 43. Logan HL, Gedney JJ, Sheffield D, Xiang Y, Starrenberg E. Stress
Psychol 1988;54:1063–70. influences the level of negative affectivity following forehead cold pres-
33. Spielberger CD, Gorusch R, Lushene RE, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual sor pain. J Pain 2003;4:520 –9.
for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y1). Palo Alto, CA: Con- 44. Diagnostic criteria for functional gastrointestinal disorders. In: Drossman
sulting Psychologists Press; 1983. D, Corazziari E, Talley N, Thompson W, Whitehead WE, editors. The
34. McNair D, Lorr M, Droppleman LF. Profile of mood states manual. San functional gastrointestinal disorders. Lawrence, KS: Allen Press; 2000. p.
Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service; 1971. 659 – 69.
35. Moore PA, Duncan GH, Scott DS, Gregg JM, Ghia JN. The submaximal 45. Sheffield D, Logan H, Lutgendorf S, Haldeman R, Iverson K, Wade R,
effort tourniquet test: its use in evaluating experimental and chronic pain. Kirchner HL, Lubaroff D. Effects of stress and relaxation on capsaicin-
Pain 1979;6:375– 82. induced pain and flare response. Br J Health Psychol 2003;9 (in press).
36. Gracely RH, Kwilosz DM. The Descriptor Differential Scale: applying 46. Price DD. Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimen-
psychophysical principles to clinical pain assessment. Pain 1988;35: sion of pain. Science 2000;288:1769 –72.
279 – 88. 47. Janssen SA. Negative affect and sensitization to pain. Scand J Psychol
37. Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendo- 2002;43:131–7.

606 Psychosomatic Medicine 66:599 – 606 (2004)

You might also like