You are on page 1of 20

Effects of Materiality, Risk,

and Ethical Perceptions on


Fraudulent Reporting by
Financial Executives William E. Shafer

ABSTRACT. This paper examines fraudulent finan- Introduction


cial reporting within the context of Jones’ (1991)
ethical decision making model. It was hypothesized The integrity of the financial reporting process
that quantitative materiality would influence judg- in the U.S. has recently come under intense crit-
ments of the ethical acceptability of fraud, and that icism. On several occasions, former Securities
both materiality and financial risk would affect the and Exchange Commission (SEC) Chairman
likelihood of committing fraud. The results, based on
Arthur Levitt asserted that registrants’ reported
a study of CPAs employed as senior executives,
provide partial support for the hypotheses. Contrary
financial results are often intentionally manipu-
to expectations, quantitative materiality did not lated to meet market expectations (e.g., Levitt,
influence ethical judgments. ANCOVA results based 1998, 2000). Levitt’s accusations were the latest
on participants’ estimates of the likelihood that a in a long line of charges of wrongdoing on the
“typical CPA” would manipulate reported results part of accountants and their independent
indicated that both materiality and risk significantly auditors (e.g., Public Oversight Board, 1993,
influenced the likelihood of fraud, but that the per- 1994; Schuetze, 1994). However, the frequency
ceived morality of the action did not. In contrast, and intensity of the recent allegations has
results based on participants’ self-reported behavior prompted writers in the financial press to openly
indicated that materiality and the perceived morality question the reliability of the entire financial
of the action would influence the likelihood of fraud, reporting process (e.g., Bartlett, 1998; Loomis,
but that financial risk would not. Regardless of the
1998).
measure used for the likelihood of fraud, the results
indicate that financial executives continue to be
One “earnings management” technique that
influenced by quantitative materiality when misstate- has provoked the ire of the SEC is the rational-
ments are clearly material on qualitative grounds. ization of intentional financial statement mis-
statements on the grounds that they do not
exceed traditional materiality thresholds.1 The
perception that such “abuses of materiality” are
commonplace led to the recent issuance of SEC
Staff Accounting Bulletin 99 (SEC, 1999). This
William E. Shafer is professor of accounting at the statement emphasized that exclusive reliance on
Graziadio School of Business and Management, quantitative measures of materiality is inappro-
Pepperdine University. His primary research interest is
priate, and that intentional misstatements of
ethics and professionalism in accounting. His research has
appeared in a variety of academic and professional
financial results may violate federal securities
journals, including AUDITING: A Journal of laws, even if the amounts fall below traditional
Practice & Theory, Accounting Horizons, materiality thresholds. The statement also
Accounting Auditing & Accountability Journal, expressed the view that it is not reasonable to
Journal of Business Ethics, Journal of record misstatements or not to correct known
Accountancy, and The CPA Journal. misstatements, even if quantitatively immaterial,

Journal of Business Ethics 38: 243–262, 2002.


© 2002 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands.
244 William E. Shafer

as part of an ongoing effort of the company’s reporting decisions, and in the case of intentional
senior management to manipulate or manage earnings manipulations, the acceptability of these
reported earnings. decisions may never be independently evaluated
Staff Accounting Bulletin 99 contained no unless the misstatements are detected by the
new concepts or rules regarding materiality. Its company’s auditors.2
purpose was to remind registrants and their In light of the recent SEC allegations, a better
auditors of the meaning and proper use of the understanding of financial executives’ propensity
materiality concept (SEC, 1999). Obviously, the to rationalize earnings manipulations on the
concept was never intended as a vehicle for the grounds of immateriality is clearly needed. The
intentional manipulation of financial statements. current study contributes to extant knowledge of
Many accounting issues arise for which the this phenomenon in several ways. It introduces
“correct” answer is not known at the time of Jones’ (1991) theory of moral intensity to the
financial statement preparation, e.g., estimates of literature on earnings manipulation, and argues
the realizable value of inventories or collectibility that this theory may help explain the prevalence
of receivables. The concept of materiality allows of earnings management in practice. It is also the
the use of reasonable estimates in such cases as a first study to examine the effects of quantitative
matter of practicality; otherwise, timely finan- materiality on financial executives’ propensity to
cial reporting could not occur. The SEC appar- engage in intentional manipulations of earnings.
ently felt that use of the concept in practice had Finally, it extends recent research on materiality
degenerated into game-playing, although the to the context of financial statement fraud, and
only evidence offered to support this view was demonstrates that even fraudulent financial
anecdotal in nature (see Levitt, 1998, 2000). reporting schemes may often be rationalized on
Many studies in the accounting literature have the grounds of immateriality. The findings of this
addressed the issue of earnings manipulation or study support the SEC’s recent charges regarding
“earnings management”, but most of these abuses of materiality, but also call into question
studies have relied on archived financial data and the feasibility of regulating this type of behavior.
focused exclusively on economic incentives,
rather than addressing this issue from an ethical
decision making perspective. The few studies that Literature review and hypothesis
have adopted an ethical perspective have focused development
primarily on managers’ or shareholders’ percep-
tions of the moral acceptability of earnings A long tradition of research on the issue of
manipulations, and have failed to offer a theo- intentional earnings manipulations or “earnings
retical explanation of why such manipulations management” exists in the accounting literature.
seem to be prevalent in practice. These studies Healy and Wahlen (1999, p. 368) recently
have also not focused specifically on the effects defined earnings management as managers using
of materiality on earnings manipulation. “judgment in financial reporting and in struc-
Several recent studies in the auditing litera- turing transactions to alter financial reports to
ture have examined the effects of quantitative either mislead some stakeholders about the
materiality on financial reporting judgments. underlying economic performance of the
However, these studies have been limited to the company or to influence contractual outcomes
effects of materiality on the disposition of errors that depend on reported accounting numbers.”
that were either based on subjective accounting Most previous studies of earnings management
estimates or “honest mistakes” on the part of have focused on economic incentives for this
management, rather than cases of financial state- type of behavior, such as the presence of man-
ment fraud. These studies have also been agement bonuses that are tied to reported
restricted to the study of independent auditors income, and have relied on archival studies of
rather than company managers or executives. published financial statements.3 Only a limited
Financial executives often make accounting and number of studies have considered earnings
Effects on Materiality, Risk, and Ethical Perceptions 245

management from an ethical decision making these manipulations (e.g., writing inventory
perspective, and most of these studies have been values down) can be rationalized on the basis of
limited to assessing the perceived morality or professional judgment, others (e.g., arranging for
ethical acceptability of the behavior. delayed invoicing for services already received)
Studies of ethical perceptions usually make a constitute financial statement fraud, and there is
distinction between two basic types of earnings a clear disconnect between the reported
management: (1) operating decisions, which accounting numbers and the underlying
involve the use of discretion in establishing the economic events. Thus, the findings of these
structure or timing of financial transactions; (2) studies suggest that intentional or fraudulent
accounting decisions, which involve judgment in earnings management through accounting
the selection among alternative accounting manipulations are generally viewed as unaccept-
methods or revisions of accounting assumptions able.
or estimates. In related studies, Merchant and his Studies of ethical attitudes toward earnings
colleagues found that managers generally management have generally neglected the issue
consider it ethical to intentionally manipulate of materiality. However, in one of the Bruns and
earnings through operating decisions such as Merchant (1990) scenarios the dollar amount of
deferring expenditures and engaging in year-end the misstatement was manipulated on a within-
sales promotions (Bruns and Merchant, 1990; subjects basis. Although the effect of this manip-
Merchant and Rockness, 1994). In contrast, ulation was statistically significant (Merchant and
respondents tended to view the intentional Rockness, 1994), the difference in respondents’
manipulation of accounting methods as uneth- ethical judgments for the large and small manip-
ical, although there was a great deal of variation ulations was actually quite small. In light of the
in their attitudes. Both Fischer and Rosenzweig demand effects associated with within-subjects
(1995) and Kaplan (2001) also found that manipulations, it appears that the effects of the
earnings management through operating deci- dollar amount of intentional earnings manipula-
sions was viewed as significantly more ethical tions on judgments of their ethical acceptability
than accounting manipulations. remains an open question.
The reason for these differences in perceptions Several recent studies have investigated the
is not difficult to understand. Analysis of the cases effects of materiality on auditors’ willingness to
used in these studies reveals that the operating allow aggressive financial reporting.5 Shafer et al.
earnings management schemes involved initiatives (1999) and Ketchand et al. (1999) studied the
such as running year-end sales promotions to effects of the dollar amount of financial statement
boost revenue, deferring expenses such as main- errors, as well as certain qualitative variables such
tenance and advertising until future years, and as the perceived likelihood of harm to financial
selling off excess assets in order to realize a statement users, on the financial reporting
profit.4 Although these types of management judgments of practicing auditors. Both studies
decisions may be motivated by the desire to found that the dollar amount of misstatements
manipulate earnings, they cannot be construed as had a significant influence on auditors’ judgments
fraudulent, and the reported accounting numbers of the ethical acceptability of acquiescing in
in these cases will accurately reflect the under- clients’ aggressive reporting schemes. Libby and
lying economic events. In contrast, the Kinney (2000) examined the effects of analysts’
accounting manipulations involved schemes such consensus earnings forecasts and proposed
as arbitrarily writing inventory values up or changes to professional auditing standards on
down, prepaying future years’ expenses and audit managers’ willingness to allow a quantita-
recording them as expenses in the current year, tively immaterial misstatement to go uncorrected.
and making arrangements with an outside con- This study found that auditors were less likely
sulting firm to delay invoicing the company for to require correction of immaterial errors if those
services rendered until the next year in order to errors would cause the company’s earnings to fall
delay recognition of the expense. While some of below the analyst-forecasted targets. The results
246 William E. Shafer

also indicated that recent changes in professional Jones’ theory of ethical decision making
auditing standards that mandate communications
regarding uncorrected errors to a company’s audit According to Jones (1991), all phases of the
committee did not increase the likelihood of ethical decision making process will be influ-
error correction if such correction would cause enced by the extent of issue-related moral imper-
the company to miss its earnings targets. ative present in a given situation. Jones’ theory,
These findings all suggest that auditors are which is presented in Figure 1, adopts the classic
quite likely to allow immaterial errors to go four-component model of ethical decision
uncorrected, as charged by the SEC. However, making proposed by Rest (1986). This model,
these studies used cases involving errors that were which has been widely adopted in the accounting
based on subjective judgments or honest over- literature (e.g., Wright et al., 1998; Ketchand et
sights by company management, rather than al., 1999; Shafter et al., 1999), decomposes the
financial statement fraud. It is hardly surprising process into four logical components: (1) recog-
that auditors can be persuaded to acquiesce in nition of an issue, (2) making a moral or ethical
immaterial earnings management schemes judgment, (3) establishing behavioral intentions,
involving accounting issues that are in the “gray and (4) engaging in actual behavior.
area” or that involve subjectively determined The first component of the model involves
accounting estimates. After all, in these cases recognizing the moral implications of an issue,
legitimate differences of opinion often exist, and identifying the alternative courses of action that
any potential error should have a minimal impact are available, and assessing the consequences of
on financial statement users. Fraudulent reporting those actions (Rest, 1986). Once the moral
is a much more egregious offense, because it implications of an issue have been properly
involves clear intent on the part of management recognized, an ethical judgment will be made,
to deceive financial statement users. Libby and which identifies the morally correct course of
Kinney (2000) suggested that future studies of the action. After identifying one course of action as
effects of materiality on financial reporting the morally correct one, a person must weigh
should examine cases involving management their moral values in relation to other values in
intent to manipulate earnings, since the effects of order to establish behavioral intentions. For
materiality under these circumstances may be example, if a person values advancement in their
different. Accordingly, the current study sought career more than they value ethical behavior,
to extend research on abuses of materiality to the
context of financial statement fraud.
The materiality of a potential financial state-
ment misstatement is a situational, or issue-con-
tingent variable. To examine the effects of this
type of variable on ethical decision-making
processes, a model is needed that explicitly
focuses on the characteristics of moral issues,
rather than other potential variables such as char-
acteristics of the decision makers. Jones’ (1991)
theory of moral intensity provides such a focus,
and also appears to have particular relevance for
the study of abuses of materiality. Accordingly,
this model was adopted as a theoretical frame-
work for the current paper.

Figure 1. Jones’ ethical decision making model.


Effects on Materiality, Risk, and Ethical Perceptions 247

they may compromise their moral principles. 1995), sexual harassment (Bowes-Sperry and
Even if a person resolves to do what is judged as Powell, 1999), and whistle-blowing (Singer et al.,
morally correct, they may not possess the perse- 1998). Empirical studies have also found support
verance or resolve to follow through on their for the influence of moral intensity on ethical
intentions. Thus, the model makes an explicit decision making processes, although the specific
distinction between moral intentions and actual components of moral intensity that influence
behavior. decisions have varied among studies.
As indicated in the figure, the moral intensity Morris and MacDonald (1995) concluded
of an issue is hypothesized to affect all compo- based on early empirical studies that two dimen-
nents of the ethical decision making process. sions of moral intensity, magnitude of conse-
Jones (1991) recognized six components of the quences and social consensus, tend to be more
moral intensity construct: (1) magnitude of con- significant than the others. Their results, based
sequences, (2) probability of effect, (3) temporal on student responses to three ethical dilemmas,
immediacy, (4) social consensus, (5) proximity, generally supported this contention. Singer
and (6) concentration of effect. Magnitude of con- (1996) and Singer et al. (1998) also found that
sequences refers to the sum of the harms or magnitude of consequences and social consensus
benefits resulting from a given action. A higher had the most significant effects on observers’
magnitude of consequences results in greater judgments of actions with ethical implications.
moral intensity. Probability of effect refers to the Davis et al. (1998) tested the effects of three
likelihood of the consequences actually occur- components of moral intensity (magnitude of
ring. When the probability of actual harm is consequences, social consensus, and proximity)
relatively low, the moral intensity of the scenario in a cross-cultural study of judgments relating to
will also be low. The temporal immediacy of a questionable human resource management prac-
moral action refers to the length of time between tices. The results indicated that social consensus
the action and the onset of its consequences. had the strongest effect on judgments. Mixed
According to Jones (1991), people discount the results were reported for the effects of proximity,
effects of future consequences; thus, for a given while the effects for magnitude of consequences
magnitude of consequences, events that will were not significant. In a study that manipulated
occur in the future will be less morally intense all six components of moral intensity on a
than events that are imminent. Social consensus within-subjects basis. Frey (2000) found that
refers to the extent of agreement that an action magnitude of consequences, social consensus, and
is ethical or unethical. Greater consensus leads to probability of effect had the most significant
greater moral intensity.Proximity refers to the effects on business executives’ decisions relating
degree of affinity between the decision maker to two ethical scenarios. On the other hand,
and the victims or beneficiaries of the act in Singhapakdi et al. (1996) found that all six com-
question. Greater affinity with either the victims ponents of moral intensity had a significant
or beneficiaries of the act should increase the influence on the ethical perceptions of marketing
likelihood that the moral agent will act on these professionals.
parties’ behalf. The final component of the moral Overall, these findings seem to indicate sub-
intensity construct, concentration of effect, is an stantial support for the influence of the magni-
inverse function of the number of people affected tude of consequences and social consensus on
by an action of a given magnitude. Higher con- ethical decision making, but less support for the
centration of effect leads to greater moral inten- effects of the other components of moral inten-
sity. sity. Obviously, not all the components of moral
Moral intensity has been offered as a poten- intensity will be relevant to all ethical decision
tial explanation of attitudes and behaviors relating making scenarios ( Jones, 1991); thus, variations
to a wide variety of ethical issues in business, among studies that examine different decision
including software piracy (Logsdon et al., 1994), contexts would be expected. Consequently,
environmental pollution (Morris and McDonald, application of the moral intensity concept to
248 William E. Shafer

particular decision contexts should be based on ditional definitions of fraud usually incorporate
careful consideration of its relevance for that a materiality test. For example, the National
context. In the case of intentional earnings Association of Certified Fraud Examiners defines
manipulations that fall below traditional materi- fraud as “the intentional, deliberate, misstate-
ality thresholds, there appears to be strong con- ments or omission of material facts, or accounting
ceptual support for the relevance of the moral data, which is misleading and, when considered
intensity concept. with all the information made available, would
The magnitude of consequences appears to cause the reader to change or alter his or her
have particular relevance in this scenario, because judgment or decision” (NACFE, 1993, p. 12,
by definition the magnitude of consequences for emphasis added). According to this definition,
immaterial manipulations will be small or negli- fraudulent financial reporting ceases to be fraud
gible. Since materiality is defined as the when the amounts involved fall below users’
minimum amount that would make a difference materiality thresholds. The SEC attempted to
in financial statement users’ decision processes, counter this notion by asserting in Staff
it seems likely that managers and accountants will Accounting Bulletin 99 that intentional or fraud-
rationalize misstatements that fall below normal ulent earnings manipulations may violate federal
materiality thresholds on the grounds that they securities laws even if they are immaterial.
will not have a significant adverse impact on However, this argument has also met with con-
users. The perceived prevalence of this line of siderable resistance. For instance, Fang and Jacobs
reasoning recently prompted former SEC (2000) asserted that the SEC’s “invention of
Chairman Arthur Levitt to counter-argue that, if immaterial fraud” will allow the circumvention
“immaterial” earnings manipulations are in fact of plain statutory language and years of case law
inconsequential, managers would not go to such requiring that fraud be material. This discussion
great lengths to perpetrate them (Levitt, 1998). illustrates that the SEC has encountered consid-
There are also reasons to believe there may be erable difficulty in its attempts to enlist support
a low degree of social consensus regarding the for the condemnation of abuses of materiality.
immorality of small misstatements. If, as former This further implies a relatively low degree of
SEC Chairman Levitt has suggested, a financial consensus that such behavior is inappropriate.
reporting culture has developed in the U.S. in Other components of the moral intensity
which recording small intentional misstatements construct also appear relevant to the study of
is commonplace, accountants may feel there is a earnings manipulation. In the context of earnings
consensus that such behavior is an acceptable management, the probability of effect may be
business practice. Theories of ethical decision viewed as the joint probability of users relying
making commonly recognize the influence of on the financial statements, and harm occurring
cultural or social norms on judgments (e.g., as a result of that reliance. A number of factors
Ferrell and Gresham, 1985; Hunt and Vitell, may serve to reduce the perceived probability of
1991, 1986). If accountants feel that small effect, such as the availability of alternative
earnings manipulations are commonplace and sources of information about the company.6 Also,
culturally acceptable (low consensus of as the dollar amount of an intentional misstate-
immorality), they should be more likely to view ment decreases, the probability that users will
them as ethical. make harmful decision (e.g., to buy or sell shares)
Accounting scholars have even openly ques- as a result of the misstatement should also
tioned the validity of the SEC’s condemnation of decrease, resulting in a relatively low estimated
immaterial manipulations. Both DeChow and probability of harm for quantitatively immate-
Skinner (2000) and Libby and Kinney (2000) rial misstatements.
argued that immaterial earnings misstatements The lack of proximity between accountants
appear to be within the guidelines of Generally and those harmed by earnings manipulations also
Accepted Accounting Principles, thus ques- portends a low level of moral intensity.
tioning the SEC’s position on this issue. Even tra- Particularly in the case of public companies, the
Effects on Materiality, Risk, and Ethical Perceptions 249

potential harm will often be inflicted on a large judgments. The explicit separation of ethical
group of anonymous stakeholders, which makes judgments and behavior in the Rest (1986) and
it easier to dismiss the moral implications of the Jones (1991) models is based on recognition of
action. In the case of quantitatively immaterial the fact that practical considerations may lead to
misstatements, the fact that the potential harm behaviors that are inconsistent with a person’s
will be widely distributed suggests that the conception of what is morally right and wrong.
amount of harm to most individuals will be For example, the model presented in Figure 1
minimal. This low concentration of effect will explicitly acknowledges the potential influence
also reduce the perceived moral intensity of of organizational factors on ethical intentions and
earnings management. behavior. Jones (1991) recognized that factors
such as group dynamics, organizational authority,
and socialization processes may influence moral
Hypothesis development intentions and behavior. Theories of ethical
decision making in organizations also commonly
Jones’ (1991) theory suggests that the moral recognize the influence of other practical con-
imperative associated with quantitatively imma- straints on ethical intentions and behavior. For
terial misstatements will be relatively low. By instance, the Hunt-Vitell model recognizes that
definition, the potential magnitude of conse- factors such as the probability and desirability of
quences for such misstatements should be small consequences, and the importance of the stake-
or negligible. Since materially thresholds repre- holders involved will affect ethical intentions and
sent the minimum misstatements necessary to behavior (Hunt and Vitell, 1991, 1986). When
influence users’ judgments, the perceived likeli- practical considerations are taken into account, it
hood of harm (probability of effect) resulting appears that the likelihood of small intentional
from misstatements below such thresholds should misstatements may be rather high.
also be low. If accountants feel that small earnings As previously argued, the perceived moral
manipulations are commonplace in the current intensity of immaterial manipulations should be
financial reporting culture, as suggested in recent low, and a low level of moral intensity portends
SEC communications, they are also likely to feel a high likelihood of engaging in questionable
that there is at least some degree of social con- behaviors. For example, when the potential
sensus that such misstatements are less than misstatements are quantitatively immaterial, the
egregious. The lack of close proximity between perceived magnitude of consequences and prob-
auditors and anonymous stakeholders of public ability of harm may be negligible. In such cases,
companies, as well as the widely diffused nature the impact of practical considerations such as
of any potential consequences, should also serve organizational pressures will be considerable. The
to reduce the moral intensity of immaterial perceived probability of detection and punish-
misstatements. Consequently, materiality should ment for immaterial misstatements should also be
influence the perceived ethical acceptability of rather small. Thus, when an accountant or finan-
earnings manipulations, even if they are inten- cial executive is placed under organizational
tional or fraudulent in nature. This reasoning is pressure to manipulate financial results by
reflected in the following hypothesis: amounts that are quantitatively immaterial,
he/she will often be in a position of weighing
Hypothesis 1: Financial executives will judge direct and immediate personal benefits (e.g.,
financial statement fraud as more (less) cessation of pressure, job retention, higher raises
ethical when the dollar amounts involved or bonuses) against a low probability of harm to
are quantitatively immaterial (material). a large group of relatively anonymous financial
statement users. Couple this with a low proba-
Economic factors such as materiality should bility of detection and punishment, and it is easy
have a more pronounced effect on ethical to understand why “abuses of materiality” may
intentions and actual behavior than on ethical be common in practice. The Shafer et al. (1999)
250 William E. Shafer

and Ketchand et al. (1999) studies also found loss to the stakeholders involved, higher levels
that the dollar amount of a financial statement of moral intensity should decrease the perceived
misstatement had a strong impact on professional morality of aggressive reporting, as well as the
auditors’ estimates of the likelihood that they likelihood of such reporting occurring. The
would allow aggressive reporting by a client. findings strongly supported this contention.
SEC officials have recently suggested that the Potential bankruptcy poses a similar threat to
manipulation of financial results has become stakeholders, by increasing both the likelihood
commonplace among publicly traded companies, and imminence of potential losses.
particularly when the amounts fall below Additional support also exists for the influence
traditional materiality thresholds (e.g., Levitt, of risk on aggressive financial reporting judg-
1998, 2000). However, neither the validity of this ments. Theories of ethical decision making in
assertion nor the extent to which materiality organizations recognize the intuitive proposition
affects the likelihood of fraud by financial exec- that the higher the probability of punishment for
utives has been documented. A demonstration unethical behavior, the lower the likelihood that
that quantitative materiality influences the like- managers will compromise moral principles (e.g.,
lihood of fraudulent reporting would suggest that Ferrell and Gresham, 1985; Trevino, 1986; Hunt
financial executives are not appropriately con- and Vitell, 1986). SEC investigations are often
sidering qualitative factors when making judg- triggered by company bankruptcy or other
ments about earnings manipulations. Accordingly, financial difficulties (Beasley et al., 1999).
the following hypothesis is proposed: Consequently, the likelihood that intentional
earnings manipulations will be detected and sanc-
Hypothesis 2: Financial executives will be tions administered should be higher for com-
more (less) likely to commit financial state- panies with greater financial risk.
ment fraud when the dollar amounts Theories of auditor independence have also
involved are quantitatively immaterial recognized the potential influence of risks asso-
(material). ciated with litigation and other sanctions on
auditors’ reporting judgments (e.g., Shockley,
Another factor that should influence the per- 1982). In fact, several studies have assumed that
ceived moral intensity of financial statement fraud auditors’ willingness to acquiesce in aggressive
is the degree of risk associated with the company. reporting schemes essentially involves a trade-off
Companies with higher financial risk face a between the risk of client loss on the one hand,
higher likelihood of bankruptcy and consequent and the risk of litigation or other adverse con-
losses to shareholders; accordingly, the perceived sequences on the other (e.g., Farmer et al., 1987;
probability of effect should be higher. A higher Trompeter, 1994; Hackenbrack and Nelson,
likelihood of imminent bankruptcy should also 1996). Shafer et al. (1999) explicitly modeled
increase the temporal immediacy of the poten- auditors’ ethical intentions as a function of the
tial losses to stakeholders, further increasing the perceived likelihood of various sanctions being
perceived moral intensity of the action. These imposed, such as negative peer review effects,
arguments are supported by the results of the professional disciplinary actions, and adverse lit-
Ketchand et al. (1999) study, in which the igation judgments. Their findings indicated that
intended use of financial statements was manip- the perceived risk of punishment affected the
ulated on a between-subjects basis. In one likelihood that auditors would allow aggressive
version of the case, the statements were to be reporting. Thus, higher levels of risk should
distributed to a bank for routine credit mainte- increase both the moral intensity of fraudulent
nance purposes, while in another version they reporting and the perceived likelihood of pun-
were to be used to establish the value of the ishment for such actions. Both of these effects
company pursuant to a pending buyout. should decrease the likelihood of fraud, as
Ketchand et al. argued that in the case of reflected in the following hypothesis.7
imminent transactions that pose a high risk of
Effects on Materiality, Risk, and Ethical Perceptions 251

Hypothesis 3: Financial executives will be a going concern. In contrast, the Low Risk case
significantly more (less) likely to commit indicated that, since the implementation of its
financial statement fraud when the finan- proprietary technology, the company had
cial risk associated with the company is captured a significant share of the market. The
lower (higher). Low Risk version stipulated that the company
had experienced a strong upward trend in
revenue and earnings for the past three years, and
Methodology expected increases in market share and prof-
itability in the future. This version made no
The data for this study were obtained by mailing
mention of a going concern modification in the
instruments to a sample of practicing CPAs
company’s audit report.
employed in industry. This section describes the
In the High (Low) Materiality condition, the
research instrument and participants.
fraud scheme involved the acceleration of the
recognition of $2 000 000 ($200 000) of revenue,
Instrument which would have overstated revenue by approx-
imately 25 (2.5) percent, and overstated the
The research instrument included (1) a cover pretax earnings or understated the pretax loss
letter, (2) a financial reporting case, and (3) a sup- (depending upon the risk condition) by approx-
plemental data sheet. The case, which is illus- imately 38 (3.8) percent. Thus, the amounts
trated in Appendix 1, was based on the financial involved in the Low Materiality condition were
statements of an actual company that was sanc- below traditional materiality thresholds, while in
tioned by the SEC for fraudulent financial the High Materiality condition they clearly
reporting and subsequently declared bankruptcy. exceeded such thresholds. The case was designed
Risk and materiality were manipulated on a in this manner to test whether participants would
between-subjects basis, resulting in a 2 × 2 exper- be willing to rationalize the acceptability of an
imental design. earnings manipulation on the basis of quantita-
In the High Risk condition, the reported tive immateriality.
financial position and results of operations of the The cases were reviewed by three CPAs who
company were similar to those reported by the were employed as Chief Financial Officers
actual company prior to the discovery of the (CFOs) of publicly held companies, who all
fraud scheme by the SEC. In the Low Risk indicated that the case seemed to address an
condition, the financial statements were adjusted important current issue, and that the case was
to reflect a significantly stronger financial position realistic, given practical constraints on the length
and results of operations. Both the High and Low of the survey instrument. The case was also
Risk versions indicated that since its incorpora- pretested on 27 members of an MBA night class,
tion, the company had been primarily engaged which resulted in minor changes being made to
in process and product technology development, clarify certain issues.
and had developed a unique proprietary process To assess the likelihood of fraudulent finan-
that it believed to be a technological break- cial reporting, participants were asked to provide
through. In the High Risk condition, the case their best estimate of the probability that (1) a
further indicated that since the implementation typical CPA employed in a similar position, and
of this process two years before, the company had (2) they personally would acquiesce in the
failed to capture a significant market share, and proposed premature revenue recognition scheme.
most of its sales had been to two principal cus- Responses were provided on eleven-point Likert
tomers. The High Risk version also indicated scales anchored on “0 percent” and “100
that the company had suffered recurring oper- percent.” Previous studies in accounting have
ating losses and that their audit report for the past assumed that responses regarding an “average” or
two years had been modified to indicate sub- “typical” CPA provide a more accurate estimate
stantial doubt about their ability to continue as of what participants would do personally, due to
252 William E. Shafer

social desirability effects (e.g., Cohen et al., their company. The gender composition of the
1995). Although it’s true that responses regarding sample is not surprising, in light of the fact that
other CAPs are probably more likely to be honest a strong majority of CPAs occupying higher-level
than responses regarding participants’ own positions in the U.S. are male (Doucet and
behavior, the current study does not assume that Hooks, 1999). The majority of participants had
responses regarding the “typical CPA” necessarily not earned a graduate degree, and had no pro-
reveal subjects’ personal propensity to commit fessional certifications other than the CPA
fraud. Rather, the two measures are treated as credential. The average respondent was 46 years
separate dependent variables for purposes of old and had approximately 22 years of profes-
analysis, and it is recognized that both estimates sional accounting experience.
probably understate subjects’ honest assessment
of the likelihood of fraud.
To test the effectiveness of the manipulation TABLE I
of risk, respondents were asked to (1) rate the Demographic profile of participants
degree of financial risk associated with the
Number Percent
hypothetical company; and (2) estimate the like-
lihood that the company would not continue as Sample size 138
a going concern. The financial risk estimates a
Gender:
were provided on an eleven-point scale anchored
Male 120 88.2%
on “low risk” and “high risk”, while the going Female 16 11.8
concern estimates were provided on an eleven-
point scale anchored on “0 percent” and “100 Position:
Financialb 103 76.3
percent.” To check the materiality manipulation,
Operationsc 32 23.7
participants rated the materiality of the misstate-
ments resulting from the proposed premature Company type:
revenue recognition scheme on an eleven-point Public 85 62.5
Nonpublic 51 37.5
scale anchored on “immaterial” and “highly
material.” Respondents also provided ethical Highest degree:
judgments of the hypothetical CPA’s actions on Bachelors 91 66.9
an eleven-point scale anchored on “clearly Masters or aboved 45 33.1
ethical” and “clearly unethical.” Certifications:
CPA only 126 92.6
CPA plus Otherse 10 7.4
Participants Age:
Mean 46
Instruments were mailed to a random sample of Standard deviation 8.0
500 AICPA members whose membership infor- Professional experience (years):
mation indicated that they were employed as Mean 22
senior executives in commerce or industry. After Standard deviation 7.9
a follow up mailing, a total of 138 usable a
responses were obtained, providing a response In all cases in which numbers do not sum to 138,
rate of approximately 28 percent. A comparison it is due to missing values.
b
Includes job titles Chief Financial Officer,
of the responses to the audit case and demo-
Controller, and Vice-President of Finance.
graphic data for the early and late respondents c
Includes job titles Chief Executive Officer,
indicated no significant differences. A demo- President, and Chief Operating Officer.
graphic profile of participants is provided in d
Includes two subjects with a J.D. and one subject
Table I. As the data indicate, approximately 88 with a Ph.D.
percent of the respondents were male, and 76 e
Includes CMA, CIA, CFE, and other miscellaneous
percent served as the chief financial executive of certifications.
Effects on Materiality, Risk, and Ethical Perceptions 253

Approximately 62 percent of the participants TABLE II


worked for public companies, while the Responses to case: Estimated likelihood of
remaining 38 percent worked for private com- committing fraud
panies or other types of organizations.8 Since the
case dealt with an SEC registrant, preferably Risk
respondents should have had public company
Low High Pooled
experience. However, of the 51 participants who
worked for nonpublic companies, 31 (60 percent) Panel A – Typical CPA:
reported that they had prior experience either
Materiality:
working for or auditing public companies. Thus,
only 20 respondents had no previous public Lowa, b 63% 42% 53%
company experience.9 There were no statistically (70) (24) (53)
significantly differences between the mean n = 35 n = 35 n = 70
responses of subjects employed by public and High 32% 23% 28%
nonpublic companies; consequently, all subjects (19) (12) (17)
were pooled for purposes of analysis. n = 36 n = 32 n = 68
Pooled 47% 33% 40%
(53) (22) (41)
Results n = 71 n = 67 n = 138
Panel B – Participants:
Responses to case and preliminary analyses
Materiality:
A summary of respondents’ estimates of the like- Low 16% 10% 13%
lihood of committing fraud is provided in (22) (6) (16)
Table II. The data generally indicate clear dif- n = 35 n = 35 n = 70
ferences between both the Low and High Risk High 6% 7% 6%
and the Low and High Materiality conditions. (5) (4) (4)
The data also reflect a large disparity between n = 36 n = 32 n = 68
participants’ likelihood assessments for the typical Pooled 11% 8% 10%
CPA and for themselves. For example, in the (16) (6) (12)
Low Risk, Low Materiality condition, respon- n = 71 n = 67 n = 138
dents felt there was a 63 percent chance that the
typical CPA would commit fraud in the face of Notes:
a
Reported numbers are mean responses. Numbers
organizational demands, but only admitted to a
in parentheses represent standard deviations.
16 percent chance that they personally would b
All responses were provided on an eleven-point
succumb to such pressure. The large disparities scale where 0 = “0 percent” and 10 = “100 percent”,
between responses to these two questions suggest and converted to percentages.
that subjects’ responses regarding their own cor-
ruptibility were affected by a social desirability
bias, which is not surprising given the sensitive bility even when the amounts were highly sig-
nature of this issue. nificant. The existence of such pessimistic atti-
Participants’ estimates of the likelihood of the tudes toward CPA integrity suggests that earnings
typical CPA committing fraud provide cause for manipulation may indeed be a relatively common
concern. Across both risk conditions, respon- phenomenon.
dents felt there was a 53 percent probability that Responses to the manipulation checks are
their peers would succumb to organizational summarized in Table III. Estimates of the finan-
pressures to fraudulently manipulate reported cial risk associated with the hypothetical
financial results when the amounts involved were company show marked differences between the
relatively small, and almost a 30 percent proba- Low and High Risk conditions. A one-way
254 William E. Shafer

TABLE III
Other case responses

Materiality

Low High

Low Risk High Risk Low Risk High Risk

Manipulation checks:
Financial riska, b 5.07 8.40 5.53 8.19
(1.67) (0.99) (2.50) (1.61)
Going concern riskc 3.34 5.76 4.24 5.64
(2.10) (2.63) (2.35) (2.53)
Materialityd 5.63 6.89 9.07 9.03
(2.58) (2.85) (0.59) (0.79)
Ethical judgmentse 9.03 9.16 9.39 9.38
(0.98) (0.69) (0.54) (0.44)

Notes:
a
Reported numbers are mean responses. Numbers in parentheses represent standard deviations.
b
Responses were provided on an eleven-point scale where 0 = “low risk” and 10 = “high risk”.
c
Responses were provided on an eleven-point scale where 0 = “0 percent” and 10 = “100 percent”.
d
Responses were provided on an eleven-point scale where 0 = “immaterial” and 10 = “highly material”.
e
Responses were provided on an eleven-point scale where 0 = “clearly ethical” and 10 = “clearly unethical”.

ANOVA with perceived financial risk as the who received the High Risk case felt that the
dependent variable and manipulated risk as the misstatement was more material. This result is
independent variable indicated that the effects not surprising, since materiality judgments
of the manipulation were significant at the should reflect qualitative considerations such as
0.0001 level. Although the differences are not as the likelihood of harm to financial statement
large, a one-way ANOVA also indicated that the users. However, in the High Materiality condi-
effect of the risk manipulation on the estimated tion, risk had no appreciable effect, as subjects
likelihood that the company would not continue almost uniformly agreed that the misstatement
as a going concern was significant at the 0.01 was highly material.
level. These results indicate that the manipula- Judgments in the Low Materiality condition
tion of risk was effective. reflected less consensus among participants, as
A one-way ANOVA indicated that the effect indicated by the relatively high standard devia-
of the materiality manipulation on perceived tions of the responses. From a normative stand-
materiality was also significant at the 0.0001 level. point, it could be argued that any intentional
Because the materiality ratings appeared to reflect misstatement of financial statements is highly
an interaction between materiality and risk, a material, regardless of the dollar amount (SEC,
two-way ANOVA with perceived materiality as 1999). According to this logic, the misstatement
the dependent variable and manipulated risk and in the Low Materiality condition should also
materiality as the factors was also ran. This model have been rated as highly material by the survey
indicated that the main effect for materiality participants. The fact that the ratings in the Low
remained significant at the 0.0001 level, while Materiality condition were above the midpoint
the risk/materiality interaction was also signifi- of the scale indicates that, overall, subjects did
cant at the 0.05 level. This result reflects the fact feel that the misstatement was material on
that, in the Low Materiality condition, subjects qualitative grounds, even though the effects on
Effects on Materiality, Risk, and Ethical Perceptions 255

the financial statements were below conventional that a typical CPA employed in a similar position
materiality thresholds. However, the lack of would have committed the fraud described in the
consensus in the Low Materiality condition case. In the second model, the dependent
reflects the fact that many subjects failed to variable was the estimated likelihood that par-
acknowledge the qualitative significance of the ticipants themselves would have committed the
intentional earnings manipulation, and fraud. The results of both models are presented
approached the materiality decision primarily in Table IV. Both models included risk and mate-
from a quantitative standpoint. riality as between-subjects variables. Because the
Univariate ANOVA and regression models Jones (1991) model of ethical decision making
were used to test for possible effects of various recognizes the influence of moral judgments on
demographic factors on participants’ responses to ethical decision making, both models also
the case. Univariate ANOVA models indicated included ethical judgments as a covariate. Due to
that neither gender, job title (financial vs. oper- the significant interactive effect of risk and
ating position), nor education level (bachelors vs. materiality on participants’ materiality judgments
masters) had a significant effect on responses. discussed in the preceding section, the interac-
Linear regression models also revealed that tion of risk and materiality was also included in
neither age nor years of experience affected the ANCOVA models.
participants’ responses. Based on the lack of sig- The results in Panel A of the table indicate that
nificant influence of demographic factors, these both risk and materiality had a significant effect
variables were excluded from subsequent analyses. on the estimated likelihood of a typical CPA
committing fraud, which provides support for
Hypotheses 2 and 3. Ethical judgments did not
Hypothesis tests
TABLE IV
Table III also presents participants’ mean ethical ANCOVA results
judgments regarding the hypothetical CPA’s
actions. The high means and low standard devi- F-value Significance
ations of these responses indicate that most levela
subjects viewed the action as clearly unethical.
There was also very little variation in responses Panel A – Typical CPA:
across the four experimental conditions, which Between-subjects effects:
suggests that neither materiality nor risk had a Risk 4.7 0.033
significant effect on ethical judgments. To Materiality 11.30 0.001
formally address Hypothesis 1, an ANOVA Risk ∗ Materiality 0.7 0.410
model was ran with ethical judgments as the Covariates:
dependent variable and materiality as the inde- Ethical judgments 1.8 0.183
pendent variable. The effects of materiality on Model R2 00.14
ethical judgments did not approach significance, Panel B – Participants:
suggesting that financial executives view even
Between-subjects effects:
small intentional earnings manipulations as Risk 1.7 0.191
unethical. Of course, these results should be Materiality 5.3 0.022
interpreted with caution, since self-reported Risk ∗ Materiality 2.5 0.117
ethical judgments regarding a currently sensitive
Covariates:
issue may be subject to a significant social desir- Ethical judgments 28.80 0.000
ability bias. Model R2 00.26
Hypotheses 2 and 3 were tested using analysis
of covariance (ANCOVA) models. Two separate Notes:
models were estimated. In the first model, the a
Reported significance levels are based on one-tailed
dependent variable was the estimated probability tests.
256 William E. Shafer

have a significant impact on participants’ esti- significant probability that company executives
mates of the likelihood that their peers would will succumb to organizational pressures to fraud-
commit fraud. Panel B presents the results for ulently misstate reported financial results, partic-
participants’ self-reported behavior. The effects ularly when the amounts are quantitatively
of materiality were again significant, although the immaterial.
risk factor was not. Thus, the results of the study Participants felt that the likelihood of a typical
provide consistent support for the effects of executive perpetrating a financial statement fraud
quantitative materiality on the propensity to would be strongly influenced by the dollar
commit fraud, but mixed results for the effects of amount of the misstatement and the financial risk
financial risk. In contrast to the results reported associated with the company. Participants’ esti-
in Panel A, the effects of the perceived morality mates of the likelihood that they would person-
of the action had a highly significant effect on ally commit fraud were much lower than their
the self-reported likelihood of fraud. estimates for a representative CPA, but were also
The interaction of risk and materiality was not clearly influenced by the dollar amount of the
significant in either of the models tested. misstatement. Interestingly, although the per-
Although no interaction effects were explicitly ceived risk associated with the company signifi-
hypothesized, they were included in the cantly influenced estimates of peer behavior, the
ANCOVA models due to the fact that the risk effects of risk on participants’ self-reported like-
and materiality manipulations had a significant lihood of fraud were not significant. It is also
interactive effect on participants’ materiality interesting to note that judgments of the ethical
judgments. Specifically, higher levels of risk acceptability of fraudulent reporting significantly
increased perceived materiality in the low mate- influenced the self-reported likelihood of fraud
riality condition, but not in the high materiality as would be predicted by the Rest (1986) and
condition. This result implies that under condi- Jones (1991) models, but did not influence the
tions of low materiality, the effects of risk on estimated likelihood of fraud by participants’
ethical intentions should have been greater. This peers.
is consistent with the pattern of responses This pattern of results is consistent with the
reported in Table II for self-reported intentions, presence of a social desirability response bias;
but not for estimates of peer intentions. The thus, the self-reported results should be inter-
interactive effect of manipulated risk and mate- preted with caution. When answering regarding
riality on materiality judgments was only mar- their own intentions, participants may have
ginally significant (0.05 level); therefore, the deemphasized the rational consideration of risk
effect may have simply been too subtle to sig- effects, and simply provided what was perceived
nificantly affect behavioral intentions. as the socially desirable response. This explana-
tion is generally consistent with the observed
results, which indicate low self-reported proba-
Discussion and suggestions for future bilities of fraud, and a highly significant influence
research of ethical judgments on personal intentions.
Participants’ responses regarding their own
Contrary to expectations, the results of the behavior generally reflect the position that
current study indicate that financial executives because fraud is highly unethical they would be
view intentional earnings manipulations as highly unlikely to consider it under any condition, par-
unethical, even when the amounts involved fall ticularly if the effects were material. In contrast,
below traditional materiality thresholds. Across responses regarding peer behavior seem to reflect
both materiality conditions, there was a high the view that a typical financial executive would
degree of consensus among the study participants essentially ignore ethical considerations but
that the premature recognition of revenue was would consider the risk involved, and that there
morally unacceptable. However, despite these is a significant likelihood that he/she would
moral reservations, participants felt there was a commit fraud even if the amounts were material.
Effects on Materiality, Risk, and Ethical Perceptions 257

The fact that materiality was significant in both Thus, despite intense efforts on the part of the
models attests to the robustness of materiality SEC to discourage “abuses of materiality”, the
effects on fraudulent financial reporting, i.e., even findings of the current study indicate that such
when participants answered in a socially desirable behavior may still be prevalent in practice. Even
fashion, they acknowledged the influence of in a clear case of financial statement fraud, par-
materiality on their judgments. Future research ticipants felt there was a greater than 50 percent
in this area should attempt to more effectively chance (across both risk conditions) that a typical
address the issue of social desirability response CPA employed as a financial executive would
bias, e.g., by explicitly measuring and controlling succumb to organizational pressure for earnings
for subjects’ proclivity to respond in a socially manipulation when the amounts fell below
desirable fashion. traditional materiality thresholds. The likelihood
The findings of this research have several of earnings management through less egregious
implications. First, the results raise general means, such as the manipulation of accounting
concerns regarding the likelihood of executives estimates, is probably significantly greater than in
yielding to organizational pressures for the the case of fraud.
manipulation of financial results. Across all risk These findings raise questions regarding the
and materiality conditions, the estimated likeli- ability of regulatory authorities such as the SEC
hood that participants’ peers would commit fraud to control immaterial manipulations of reported
was approximately 40 percent. Theories of ethical financial results, and add to previously expressed
decision making recognize that one of the key doubts regarding the wisdom of the SEC’s recent
predictors of unethical behavior is an employee’s emphasis on this issue. Fang and Jacobs (2000)
perceptions of what their peers or other organi- argued that the SEC’s attempt to “micromanage”
zational members would do (Ferrell and financial disclosures by prohibiting immaterial
Gresham, 1985; Hunt and Vitell, 1986, 1991; misstatements is economically unsound, because
Trevino, 1986). If peer perceptions have a similar increased vigilance in detecting and correcting
influence on financial executives’ behavior, the small errors will increase accounting and auditing
results of the current study suggest that the like- costs in exchange for questionable benefits. Fang
lihood of their acquiescence in earnings manip- and Jacobs (2000) also argued that, because legal
ulation schemes may be significant. This definitions of fraudulent financial reporting
observation appears to be consistent with the incorporate materiality tests, it is doubtful that
spate of recent criticisms by regulators, and prohibitions of immaterial financial statement
suggests that there may be systemic weaknesses manipulations will be legally enforceable. If these
in the current financial reporting system. assertions are correct, it is not likely that the
The results also suggest that risk and materi- SEC’s condemnation of small manipulations will
ality continue to influence judgments in a situ- be effective, since it will not carry the threat of
ation in which, according to accounting legal liability.
regulators, they should have no influence. The Additional insights into the effectiveness of
SEC has recently asserted that conventional attempts to regulate earnings manipulations could
concepts of materiality do not apply in situations be obtained through the study of the perceived
involving intentional manipulations of earnings certainty and severity of disciplinary actions or
(Levitt, 1998; SEC, 1999). However, it appears sanctions resulting from such behavior. Research
that economic considerations such as materiality in law and sociology has investigated the effects
still have a strong impact on the likelihood that of both formal (e.g., litigation, fines, license sus-
financial executives will commit fraud. Whether pension) and informal (negative publicity, loss of
the probability of fraud was measured by subjects’ respect of peers) sanctions on the likelihood of
estimates that a typical CPA or they personally committing a variety of crimes, including cor-
would manipulate earnings, the likelihood was porate and white collar crimes (e.g., Braithwaite
significantly higher when the dollar amounts and Makkai, 1991; Makkai and Braithwaite,
could be rationalized as immaterial. 1994; Elis and Simpson, 1995). Recent studies
258 William E. Shafer

in the auditing literature have also addressed the policy decisions. For instance, if future studies
effectiveness of sanctions as deterrents to aggres- reveal that the perceived threat of sanctions for
sive financial reporting. For example, Shafer et immaterial misstatements is negligible, the SEC
al. (1999) investigated the effectiveness of several will be faced with a choice of either intensifying
formal sanctions, such as peer review and legal its disciplinary efforts or abandoning costly
liability, in controlling aggressive reporting attempts to regulate such behavior, as suggested
among professional auditors. Similar studies of by Fang and Jacobs (2000).
financial executives could provide useful input for

Appendix 1: Experimental cases

High risk, high materiality condition:

Bob Jenkins is a licensed CPA and has been employed as the CFO of Photek, Inc. for the past year and a half.
Photek develops, manufactures, and markets high-performance film-coated glass used in such products as
computer screens, photocopiers, and projection televisions. The company was incorporated in 1994, and from
its inception through 1997 was primarily engaged in the development of process and product technology, with
limited commercial production. The company has successfully developed a unique, proprietary process for
applying thin film coatings to glass and other products that it believes represents a fundamental technological
breakthrough. However, since the implementation of this technology in early 1998 they have not captured a
significant share of the market for this type of process, and the majority of their sales have been to two prin-
cipal customers.
Photek’s common stock was initially registered with the SEC in May 2000, and is quoted on the NASDAQ.
It is now January, 2001, and the company is preparing for an audit of their annual financial statements for the
year ended December 31, 2000, which will be included in their Form 10K filed with the SEC. Summarized
financial information for Photek as of and for the three years ended December 31, 2000 follows:

(Unaudited)
1998 1999 2000

Balance Sheet:
Current assets $1,569,010 $8,374,559 $6,068,392
Property and equipment 5,779,044 8,394,457 23,235,573
Other assets 31,782 262,170
$7,379,836 $17,031,186 $29,303,965

Current liabilities $2,936,994 $12,234,619 $6,081,206


Long term liabilities 633,691 915,340 793,376
Common stock 7,993,400 8,005,460 29,191,905
Retained earnings (deficit) (4,184,249) (4,124,233) (6,762,522)
$7,379,835 $17,031,186 $29,303,965

Income Statement:
Revenue $1,097,683 $4,035,382 $8,063,848
Cost of sales 1,219,954 2,458,226 6,232,346
Gross (loss) profit (122,271) 1,577,156 1,831,502
Operating expenses 1,489,084 1,173,424 4,375,750
Interest expense 74,688 343,716 94,041
Net (loss) income $(1,686,043) $60,016 $(2,638,289)
Effects on Materiality, Risk, and Ethical Perceptions 259

As indicated above, the company has suffered operating losses and has a large accumulated deficit as of the end
of the current year, and a net loss for the year. The company incurred substantial charges for process and
product development during the year ended December 31, 2000 (reported as Operating expenses), in an effort
to develop other technologies that may prove profitable in the future. The company’s audit report has also been
modified for the past two years to indicate substantial doubt regarding their ability to continue as a going concern.
After seeing the unaudited results presented above for the year ended December 31, 2000, the company’s founder
and CEO, Jim Munitz, became very concerned about the company’s poor operating results and the failure to
meet market analysts’ forecasts. Consequently, he proposed a plan to improve reported performance. The plan
would involve backdating sales invoices and shipping documents for a large number of sales made during the
first quarter of 2001, so that the sales could be recognized in the year 2000. One of the company’s freight carriers
has agreed to backdate their bills of lading to correspond with the company’s shipping documents. Munitz
directed Jenkins to accelerate the recognition of approximately $2,000,000 in revenue for the year ended
December 31, 2000. This plan would increase gross profit and reduce the reported net loss by approximately
$1,000,000. Jenkins agreed to implement the plan, because he was concerned about losing his job if he refused
to comply with the CEO’s request.

Low risk, low materiality condition:

Bob Jenkins is a licensed CPA and has been employed as the CFO of Photek, Inc. for the past year and a half.
Photek develops, manufactures, and markets high-performance film-coated glass used in such products as
computer screens, photocopiers, and projection televisions. The company was incorporated in 1994, and from
its inception through 1997 was primarily engaged in the development of process and product technology, with
limited commercial production. The company has successfully developed a unique, proprietary process for
applying thin film coatings to glass and other products that it believes represents a fundamental technological
breakthrough. Since the implementation of this technology in early 1998, they have captured a significant share
of the market for this type of process.
Photek’s common stock was initially registered with the SEC in May 2000, and is quoted on the NASDAQ.
It is now January, 2001, and the company is preparing for an audit of their annual financial statements for the
year ended December 31, 2000, which will be included in their Form 10K filed with the SEC. Summarized
financial information for Photek as of and for the three years ended December 31, 2000 follows:

(Unaudited)
1998 1999 2000

Balance Sheet:
Current assets $2,569,010 $8,374,559 $6,068,392
Property and equipment 4,779,044 8,394,457 23,235,573
Other assets 31,782 262,170
$7,379,836 $17,031,186 $29,303,965

Current liabilities $2,936,994 $11,046,680 $6,081,206


Long term liabilities 633,691 915,340 793,376
Common stock 4,993,399 4,993,399 19,715,327
Retained earnings (deficit) (1,184,249) 75,767 2,714,056
$7,379,835 $17,031,186 $29,303,965
260 William E. Shafer

Income Statement:
Revenue $2,097,683 $5,035,382 $8,063,848
Cost of sales 1,129,954 2,458,226 4,232,346
Gross (loss) profit 967,729 2,577,156 3,831,502
Operating expenses 789,084 973,424 1,099,172
Interest expense 74,688 343,716 94,041
Net (loss) income $103,957 $1,260,016 $2,638,289

As indicated above, the company has experienced a strong upward trend in revenue and earnings over the past
three years, and is expected to increase its market share and profitability in the future. Nevertheless, after seeing
the unaudited results presented above for the year ended December 31, 2000, the company’s founder and CEO,
Jim Munitz, was concerned about the company’s failure to meet market analysts’ forecasts. Consequently, he
proposed a plan to improve the reported performance.
The plan would involve backdating sales invoices and shipping documents for a large number of sales made
during the first quarter of 2001, so that the sales could be recognized in the year 2000. One of the company’s
freight carriers has agreed to backdate their bills of lading to correspond with the company’s shipping
documents. Munitz directed Jerkins to accelerate the recognition of approximately $200,000 in revenue for the
year ended December 31, 2000. This plan would increase gross profit and pretax income by approximately
$100,000. Jenkins agreed to implement the plan, because he was concerned about losing his job if he refused
to comply with the CEO’s request.

Notes and Messier (1982). For more recent perspectives see,


e.g., Carpenter and Dirsmith (1992) and Carpenter
1
Materiality is usually defined as the magnitude of et al. (1994).
6
error in a company’s financial statements that would If accountants or auditors subscribe to the efficient
influence the judgment of informed users of those markets hypothesis, they should assume that the prob-
statements (SEC, 1999). ability of effect approaches zero, i.e., earnings manip-
2
The significant role played by company executives ulations should be transparent and ineffectual in an
in the financial reporting process is evident when one efficient market.
7
considers that the majority of SEC Accounting and We acknowledge that in practice, higher levels of
Auditing Enforcement Releases (AAERs) name either financial risk may also have the opposing effect of
the Chief Financial Officer or Chief Executive Officer making management more “desperate” to improve
as a participant in fraudulent reporting schemes. For reported results, which may lead to added pressure on
example, in the sample of AAERs examined in the financial executives for aggressive reporting. However,
recent COSO study, CFOs or CEOs were named in holding the level of pressure constant, risk should
83 percent of the cases (Beasley et al., 1999). decrease the propensity for fraudulent reporting.
8
3
See Healy and Wahlen (1999) for a recent review Two participants were employed by nonprofit
of this literature. organizations.
9
4
Bruns and Merchant (1990), Merchant and It appears that a disproportionate number of
Rockness (1994), and Fischer and Rosenzweig (1995) respondents had current or previous experience
all used the same set of earnings manipulation working for publicly-traded companies. This is
scenarios, which were originally developed by Bruns probably attributable to the fact that the case dealt
and Merchant (1990). Kaplan (2001) also used similar with a public company, and the cover letter described
scenarios. the project as a study of issues recently raised by the
5
Previous studies have investigated the effects of SEC. However, any possible bias in favor of public
many variables on auditors’ materiality judgments, but company respondents was not considered a cause for
few studies have investigated the effects of materi- concern, since these individuals should have been
ality on aggressive financial reporting. For a review of more qualified to respond to the case.
early empirical studies on materiality, see Holstrum
Effects on Materiality, Risk, and Ethical Perceptions 261

References ‘An Investigation of the Impact of Economic and


Organizational Factors on Auditor Independence’,
Barlett, S.: 1998, ‘Who Can You Trust?’, Business Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 7(1), 1–14.
Week (October 5), 133–162. Ferrell, O. C. and L. G. Gresham: Summer 1985, ‘A
Beasley, M. S., J. V. Carcello and D. R. Hermanson: Contingency Framework for Understanding
1999, Fraudulent Financial Reporting 1987–1997: An Ethical Decision Making in Marketing’, Journal of
Analysis of U.S. Public Companies (Committee of Marketing 49, 87–96.
Sponsoring Organizations of the Treadway Fischer, M. and K. Rosenzweig: 1995, ‘Attitudes of
Commission, Jersey City, NJ). Students and Accounting Practitioners Concerning
Bowes-Sperry, L. and G. N. Powell: 1999, ‘Observers’ the Ethical Acceptability of Earnings Manage-
Reactions to Social-sexual Behavior at Work: An ment’, Journal of Business Ethics 14, 433–444.
Ethical Decision-making Perspective’ Journal of Frey, B. F.: 2000, ‘The Impact of Moral Intensity on
Management 25(6), 779–809. Decision Making in a Business Context’, Journal
Braithwaite, J. and T. Makkai: 1991, ‘Testing an of Business Ethics 26, 181–195.
Expected Utility Model of Corporate Deterrence’, Hackenbrack, K. and M. Nelson: January 1996,
Law and Society Review 25(1), 7–39. ‘Auditors’ Incentives and Their Application of
Bruns, W. J. and K. A. Merchant: August 1990, ‘The Financial Accounting Standards’, The Accounting
Dangerous Morality of Managing Earnings’, Review 71(1), 43–59.
Management Accounting 72(2), 22–25. Healy, P. M. and J. M. Wahlen: 1999, ‘A Review of
Carpenter, B. W. and M. W. Dirsmith: 1992, ‘Early the Earnings Management Literature and Its
Debt Extinguishment Transactions and Auditor Implications for Standard Setting’, Accounting
Materiality Judgments: A Bounded Rationality Horizons 13(4), 365–383.
Perspective’, Accounting, Organizations, and Society Holstrum, G. L. and W. F. Messier, Jr.: 1982, ‘A
17(8), 709–739. Review and Integration of Empirical Research on
Carpenter, B. W., M. W. Dirsmith and P. P. Gupta: Materaility’, Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory
1994, ‘Materiality Judgments and Audit Firm 2(1), 45–63.
Culture: Social-behavioral and Political Perspec- Hunt, S. D. and S. J. Vitell: Spring 1986, ‘A General
tives’, Accounting, Organizations, and Society 19(4/5), Theory of Marketing Ethics’, Journal of
355–380. Macromarketing 6, 5–16.
Cohen, J., L. Pant and D. Sharp: 1995, ‘Measuring Hunt, S. D. and S. J. Vitell: 1991, ‘The General
the Ethical Awareness and Ethical Orientation of Theory of Marketing Ethics: A Retrospective and
Canadian Auditors’, Behavioral Research in Revision’, in N. C. Smith and J. A. Guelch (eds.),
Accounting (Supplement), 98–119. Ethics in Marketing (Irwin, Homewood, IL), pp.
Davis, M. A., N. B. Johnson and D. G. Ohmer: 1998, 775–784.
‘Issue-contingent Effects on Ethical Decision Jones, T. M.: April 1991, ‘Ethical Decision Making
Making: A Cross-cultural Comparison’, Journal of by Individuals in Organizations: An Issue-contin-
Business Ethics 17, 373–389. gent Model’, Academy of Management Review 16(2),
DeChow, P. M. and D. J. Skinner: 2000, ‘Earnings 366–395.
Management: Reconciling the Views of Kaplan, S. E.: 2001, ‘Ethically Related Judgments by
Accounting Academics, Practitioners, And Observers of Earnings Management’, Journal of
Regulators’, Accounting Horizons 14(2), 235–250. Business Ethics 32, 285–298.
Doucet, M. S. and K. L. Hooks: 1999, ‘Toward an Ketchand, A. A., R. E. Morris and W. E. Shafer:
Equal Future’, Journal of Accountancy 187(6), 71–76. 1999, ‘An Analysis of the Role of Moral Intensity
Elis, L. A. and S. S. Simpson: 1995, ‘Informal in Auditing Judgments’, Research on Accounting
Sanction Threats and Corporate Crime: Additive Ethics 5, 249–269.
Versus Multiplicative Models’, Journal of Research in Levitt, A.: September 28, 1998, The Numbers Game.
Crime and Delinquency 32(4), 399–424. Speech delivered at the New York University
Fang, K. C. and B. Jacobs: 2000, ‘Clarifying and Center for Law and Business.
Protecting Materiality Standards in Financial Levitt, A.: May 10, 2000, Renewing the Covenant with
Statements: A Review of SEC Staff Accounting Investors. Speech delivered at the New York
Bulletin 99’, The Business Lawyer 55 (May), University Center for Law and Business.
1039–1064. Libby, R. and W. R. Kinney, Jr.: 2000, ‘Does
Farmer, T., L. Rittenberg and G. Trompeter: 1987, Mandated Audit Communication Reduce
262 William E. Shafer

Opportunistic Corrections to Manage Earnings to 1999, ‘The Effects of Formal Sanctions on Auditor
Forecasts?’, The Accounting Review 75(4), 383–404. Independence’, Auditing: A Journal of Practice &
Logsdon, J. M., J. K. Thompson and R. A. Reid: Theory 18 (Supplement), 85–101.
1994, ‘Software Piracy: Is It Related to Level of Shockley, R.: 1982, ‘Perceptions of Audit
Moral Judgment?’, Journal of Business Ethics 13, Independence: A Conceptual Model’, Journal of
849–857. Accounting, Auditing, and Finance 2, 126–143.
Loomis, C. J.: 1999, ‘Lies, Damned Lies, and Singer, M. S.: 1996, ‘The Role of Moral Intensity
Managed Earnings’, Fortune (August 2), 75–92. and Fairness Perception in Judgments of Ethicality:
Makkai, T. and J. Braithwaite: 1994, ‘The Dialectics A Comparison of Managerial Professionals and the
of Corporate Deterrance’, Journal of Research in General Public’, Journal of Business Ethics 15,
Crime and Delinquency 31(4), 347–373. 469–474.
Merchant, K. A. and J. Rockness: 1994, ‘The Ethics Singer, M. S., S. Mitchell and J. Turner: 1998,
of Managing Earnings: An Empirical Investigation’, ‘Consideration of Moral Intensity in Ethicality
Journal of Accounting and Public Policy 13, 79–94. Judgments: Its Relationship with Whistle-blowing
Morris, S. A. and R. A. McDonald: 1995, ‘The Role and Need-for-cognition’, Journal of Business Ethics
of Moral Intensity in Moral Judgments: An 17, 527–541.
Empirical Investigation’, Journal of Business Ethics Singhapakdi, A., S. J. Vitell and K. L. Kraft: 1996,
14, 715–726. ‘Moral Intensity and Ethical Decision-making of
National Association of Certified Fraud Examiners Marketing Professionals’, Journal of Business Research
(NACFE): 1993, Cooking the Books: What Every 36, 245–255.
Accountant Should Know about Fraud. Course No. Trevino, L. K.: 1986, ‘Ethical Decision Making in
92-5401, Self study workbook. Organizations: A Person-situation Interactionist
Public Oversight Board: 1993, In the Public Interest: Model’, Academy of Management Review 11(3),
A Special Report by the Public Oversight Board of the 601–617.
SEC Practice Section, AICPA (POB, Stamford, CT). Trompeter, G.: 1994, ‘The Effect of Partner
Public Oversight Board: 1994, Strengthening the Compensation Schemes and Generally Accepted
Professionalism of the Independent Auditor: Report to Accounting Principles on Audit Partner Judgment’,
the Public Oversight Board of the SEC Practice Section, Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 13(2),
AICPA from the Advisory Panel on Auditor 56–68.
Independence (POB, Stamford, CT). Wright, G., C. Cullinan and D. Bline: 1998,
Public Oversight Board: 2000, The Panel on Audit ‘Recognizing Ethical Issues: The Joint Influence of
Effectiveness: Report and Recommendations (POB, Ethical Sensitivity and Moral Intensity’, Research on
New York). Accounting Ethics 4, 29–52.
Rest, J.: 1986, Moral Development: Advances in Research
and Theory (Praeger, New York).
Schuetze, W:. 1994, ‘A Mountain or a Molehill?’,
Accounting Horizons 8(1), 69–75. Graziadio School of Business and Management,
Securities and Exchange Commission: 1999, Pepperdine University,
Materiality in Financial Statements: SEC Staff 400 Corporate Pointe Drive,
Accounting Bulletin 99 (SEC, Washington, DC). Culver City, CA 90230,
Shafter, W. E., R. E. Morris and A. A. Ketchand: U.S.A.

You might also like