You are on page 1of 9

COSMETIC

Size and Perception of Facial Features with


Selfie Photographs, and Their Implication in
Rhinoplasty and Facial Plastic Surgery
Mark P. Pressler, M.D.

Background: Patients increasingly use photographs taken with a front-facing


Mikaela L. Kislevitz, B.S.N., smartphone camera—“selfies”—to discuss their goals with a plastic surgeon. The
R.N., M.D. purpose of this study was to quantify changes in size and perception of facial fea-
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/plasreconsurg by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdtwnfKZBYtws= on 03/29/2022

Justin J. Davis, M.D. tures when taking a selfie compared to the gold standard of clinical photography.
Bardia Amirlak, M.D. Methods: Thirty volunteers took three series of photographs. A 12-inch and
Dallas, Texas 18-inch series were taken with a front-facing smartphone camera, and the
5-foot clinical photography series was taken with a digital single-lens reflex
camera. Afterward, subjects filled out the FACE-Q inventory, once when view-
ing their 12-inch selfies and once when viewing their clinical photographs.
Measurements were taken of the nose, lip, chin, and facial width.
Results: Nasal length was, on average, 6.4 percent longer in 12-inch selfies com-
pared to clinical photography, and 4.3 percent longer in 18-inch selfies com-
pared to clinical photography. The alar base width did not change significantly
in either set of selfies compared to clinical photography. The alar base to facial
width ratio represents the size of the nose in relation to the face. This ratio
decreased 10.8 percent when comparing 12-inch selfies to clinical photography
(p < 0.0001) and decreased 7.8 percent when comparing 18-inch selfies to clini-
cal photography (p < 0.0001).
Conclusions: This study quantifies the change in facial feature size/percep-
tion seen in previous camera-to-subject distance studies. With the increasing
popularity of front-facing smartphone photographs, these data allow for a
more precise conversation between the surgeon and the patient. In addition,
the authors’ findings provide data for manufacturers to improve the societal
impact of smartphone cameras.  (Plast. Reconstr. Surg. 149: 859, 2022.)
CLINICAL QUESTION/LEVEL OF EVIDENCE: Diagnostic, III.

P
lastic surgeons use clinical photography discussion of surgical goals and impact postsurgi-
to communicate with patients about their cal patient satisfaction.5,6
surgical goals. In addition, it is a tool for There is a noted relationship between the
exchanging clinical and academic information increase in selfie photographs and an increase in
with other surgeons.1,2 Standardization of lens rhinoplasty requests, particularly among younger
settings and pose are necessary to compare pho- patients.5 Plastic surgeons should seek to under-
tographs.1 Patients commonly bring in aspira- stand both the psychosocial and the technical
tional or self-obtained photographs to the office considerations of this link. The distortion due
or online consultation to discuss surgical goals.3,4 to selfie photographs has also been shown to
Photographs using a front-facing smartphone negatively affect an individual’s self-esteem.7 The
camera (i.e., selfies) have increased in popular- advent of social media has led to an increase in
ity and, despite surgeons’ efforts to use standard requests for plastic surgery from patients due to
studio photography, have become part of the increased awareness of their looks, a trend that
has been increasing since 2014.8,9

From the Department of Plastic Surgery, University of Texas


Southwestern. Disclosure: This research did not receive any spe-
Received for publication March 12, 2020; accepted July 13, cific grant from funding agencies in the public, com-
2021. mercial, or not-for-profit sectors. The authors have no
Copyright © 2022 by the American Society of Plastic Surgeons financial interests to report.
DOI: 10.1097/PRS.0000000000008961

www.PRSJournal.com 859
Copyright © 2022 American Society of Plastic Surgeons. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery • April 2022

Noyes and Jenkins noted faces look more con- camera. Twelve inches was chosen as an estimate
vex when the lens is closer to the subject (0.2 to 3 of a selfie taken with the elbow bent at 90-degree
m in their example) and flatter from afar.10 Studies angle. Eighteen inches was chosen as an estimate
have found subjects are rated as less trustworthy, of a selfie at arm’s length, with the wrist bent at a
competent, or attractive in photographs with 90-degree angle. Five feet was chosen as the stan-
shorter focal lengths.11,12 Perspective distortions dard camera-to-subject distance used in clinical
(where closer objects are perceived larger than dis- photography at University of Texas Southwestern.
tant ones) of facial photographs have been stud- Both the smartphone and the digital single-lens
ied, finding focal length and camera-to-subject reflex camera were on a wheeled camera mount,
distance to be a source of these distortions.12,13 The and the height of the lens was at eye level (Fig. 1).
front-facing camera in a smartphone has a very The camera-to-subject distance was measured by a
short focal length, and the pictures it captures have ruler from the tip of the nose to the lens of the
a very short camera-to-subject distance. A study by camera. Each set included a front, a left oblique,
Ward et al. found the width of the nose seems 30 and a right oblique photograph. The oblique pho-
percent larger in photographs 12 inches from the tographs were taken according to a University of
face compared to photographs 5 feet away.6 Texas Southwestern Department of Plastic Surgery
This study aimed to further quantify the dis- rhinoplasty protocol, lining up the dorsum of the
tortion of selfies compared to gold standard clini- nose with the medial canthus of the eye.
cal photography. In addition, this study aimed to Participants were instructed to hold a ruler
test the difference in self-perceived aesthetics with next to their face, at a facial depth halfway between
the FACE-Q inventory. the tragus and the lateral canthus, to standardize
a “life-size” scale used for measurements (Figs. 2
PATIENTS AND METHODS and 3).19,20 The lighting of the studio was standard-
ized, using four overhead fluorescent photogra-
Participants and Equipment phy lights (temperature of 5000 Kelvin) placed
The Institutional Review Board at University at each corner. This setup created a homogenous
of Texas Southwestern, Dallas, Texas, approved light exposure and avoided shadowing.
this study. This study recruited participants by Measurements were taken using the Adobe
means of word of mouth from April to May of Photoshop CS6 Ruler Tool (Adobe, Inc., San Jose,
2019. Participants were included if they were adults Calif.) built into the software. A scale was obtained
capable of following study directions. Photographs by measuring 1  cm on the ruler (Figs.  2 and 3),
of participants were taken in a clinical photogra- establishing the number of pixels that correspond
phy studio. These photographs were immediately to 1  cm in each photograph. Measurements
uploaded onto a network computer. Participants (Table  1) were carefully placed both at soft-tis-
first looked at their selfies and completed the sue anatomical points and at transitions between
FACE-Q inventory. Afterward, participants filled out light and shadow, maximizing reproducibility
the same inventory for their clinical photographs. between each set of photographs. Clinical pho-
An Apple XR (Apple, Inc.; Cupertino, Calif.) tography was used as the gold standard set of
was used (encrypted per institutional standards) images. Therefore, any change in measurements
for selfie photographs. The front-facing camera on for either the 12-inch or 18-inch set of selfies was
this smartphone has a focal length of 2.87 mm.14 defined as “distortion.”
The clinical photographs were taken with a Nikon
digital single-lens reflex camera, using an AF-S DX FACE-Q
NIKKOR 18-200  mm ED VRII lens set at a focal The front and left oblique photographs
length of 50  mm (Nikon, Inc., Minato, Tokyo, were viewed by participants immediately after
Japan).15 A 50-mm focal length has been cited as their photographs. They filled out two FACE-Q
an approximation of human sight.16–18 inventories,21,22 the nose and facial appearance.
Participants filled out these inventories twice,
Pictures and Photoshop first for 12-inch selfies and then for clinical
Participants were seated with their back photographs.
straight and wore a neutral facial expression in the
studio. Three sets of three photographs were taken Statistics
(using a shutter remote clicker) at 12 inches and Each measurement was compared between
18 inches with the front-facing smartphone cam- sets of photographs (12-inch versus clinical pho-
era and 5 feet using the digital single-lens reflex tograph and 18-inch versus clinical photograph)

860
Copyright © 2022 American Society of Plastic Surgeons. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Volume 149, Number 4 • Implications of Selfies in Rhinoplasty

Fig. 1. This figure shows the setup for both selfies and clinical photography. Twelve-inch, 18-inch, and 5-feet distances were mea-
sured with a ruler from the tip of the nose to the camera. The lens was adjusted to be at eye level for both types of photographs.
Participants held a ruler next to their face as straight as possible to allow for accurate measurements.

utilizing a paired t test. FACE-Q nose and facial Average age was 34.1 ± 11.3 years (range, 24.0 to
appearance inventory scores were also compared 62.3 years). Seven participants stated prior cos-
by a paired t test. The toolpak in Microsoft Excel metic injectables or surgery. There was no self-
(Microsoft, Inc., Redmond, Wash.) was used for reported history of mental illness.
statistical analysis.
Measurements
RESULTS Figure 4 shows the change in facial measure-
ments between 12-inch selfies, 18-inch selfies, and
Demographics clinical photography. Neither nasal width mea-
Thirty people, 23 women and seven men, surement (the alar base or alar wing) changed sig-
sat for photographs and rated their appearance. nificantly when comparing either 12- or 18-inch

Fig. 2. Twelve-inch selfie (left), 18-inch selfie (center), and 5-foot clinical photograph (right) are shown. This figure allows visualiza-
tion of the facial changes with different camera-to-subject distances. Note the narrower facial width in the selfie photographs,
which accentuates the size of the nose. The proportion of the length of the chin, relative to the length of the whole face, is smaller
in the selfie photographs. These selfie photographs, with their ultrawide-angle lenses and short camera-to-subject distances, cre-
ate a distortion where features closer to the camera (e.g., the nose) seem larger. This study aimed to quantify this visual phenom-
enon, utilizing the ruler next to the participant to standardize measurements.

861
Copyright © 2022 American Society of Plastic Surgeons. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery • April 2022

Fig. 3. An additional set of 12-inch selfie (left), 18-inch selfie (center), and 5-foot clinical photograph (right) is shown.

selfies to clinical photography (p values ranged and percent difference between sets of photo-
from 0.05 to 0.29). The five nasal length mea- graphs are listed in Table 2.
surements (Table 1) showed a 4.0 to 8.7 percent
increase in nasal length in the 12-inch selfies com- Reliability
pared to clinical photography (p values ranged All facial measurements of five participants
from 0.006 to <0.0001) and a 2.7 to 5.8 percent were repeated with a second investigator to assess
increase in nasal length in 18-inch selfies com- interrater reliability. Lin’s concordance correla-
pared to clinical photography (p values ranged tion coefficient was used to compare investiga-
from 0.006 to <0.0001). The length of the men- tors for each facial measurement independently.23
ton to labiale inferius was 11.9 percent shorter Correlation varied from 0.727 (alar base) to 0.995
in 12-inch selfies compared to clinical photog- (alar wing), as seen in Table 3.
raphy (p < 0.0001) and 8.8 percent shorter in
18-inch selfies compared to clinical photography
(p < 0.0001). The width of the face increased 10.3 DISCUSSION
percent in clinical photography compared to Plastic surgeons at this institution anecdotally
12-inch selfies (p < 0.0001) and increased 7.1 per- state that selfies not only distort facial features
cent in clinical photography compared to 18-inch (Figs.  2 and 3) but also complicate discussions
selfies (p < 0.0001). In addition to measuring the with patients about their goals. The purpose of
length of facial features in selfies, ratios were used this study was to quantify the change in facial fea-
to test proportionality, as shown at the bottom of ture size, proportionality, and perception seen in
Table 2. A full list of average measurement values selfies from various camera-to-subject distances.
Clinical photography was defined as the gold stan-
dard. Therefore, any difference between a set of
selfies and clinical photography was classified as
Table 1.  All Measurements for the Photographs of “distortion” in our study.6
Each Subject and the Utility of Each Measurement Třebický et al. took photographs of par-
Utility of ticipants with a digital single-lens reflex camera
Measurement Measurement (Nikon D90) using three different focal lengths.12
Nasal tip to trough of Cupid’s bow Upper lip length The authors used different camera-to-subject dis-
Alar base width Nasal width tances so the face would be the same size in all
Alar wing width Nasal width
Line of intercanthal distance to nasal tip Nasal length three pictures. Thus, the shorter focal lengths
Left medial canthus to left alar base Nasal length required the camera to be closer to the partici-
Left medial canthus to nasal tip Nasal length
Right medial canthus to nasal tip Nasal length pant. Other participants rated these different
Right medial canthus to right alar base Nasal length focal length photographs on attractiveness, find-
Menton to labiale inferius Length of lower ing the shortest focal length/shortest camera-
facial third,
length of chin to-subject distance was the least attractive.12 This
Facial width Standardization study also reported changes in perspective with
of nasal width shorter camera-to-subject distances, where facial

862
Copyright © 2022 American Society of Plastic Surgeons. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Volume 149, Number 4 • Implications of Selfies in Rhinoplasty

Fig. 4. This figure describes the variability of measurements between the three sets of photographs. The center line in the box
represents the median, and either border of the box represents the twenty-fifth and seventy-fifth percentiles. Nasal tip to trough of
Cupid’s bow was not statistically significant, nor was alar base or alar wing width. The five measurements of nasal length (Table 1)
were significant when comparing 12-inch selfies to clinical photographs and 18-inch selfies to clinical photographs, but not signifi-
cant between 12-inch and 18-inch selfies. Menton to labiale inferius and facial width were significant for each comparison. Table 2
lists the measurements, their p values, and the percent change between sets of photographs.

features closer to the camera seeming larger.12 The length of the face was also studied as
Based on these findings, our study should find a ratio. The length of the chin, from menton
facial features closer to the camera as larger (e.g., to labiale inferius, unexpectedly decreased 12
the nose), and participants should rate their percent in 12-inch selfies as opposed to clinical
12-inch selfies as less attractive than their clinical photography (p < 0.0001). Because of this unex-
photography. Our study found the five measure- pected finding, a comparison of the nasal length
ments for nasal length were 4.0 to 8.7 percent to chin length was done post hoc. The five nasal
longer in 12-inch selfies as opposed to clinical length measurements (Table  1) were averaged
photography (p values from 0.006 to <0.0001). for each participant and compared to chin length
Interestingly, nasal width measurements (i.e., as a ratio. The nasal length to chin length ratio
alar base and alar wing) were significantly larger was 17 percent larger in 12-inch selfies compared
only when expressed as a ratio with facial width to clinical photography (p < 0.0001). These dis-
(Table  2). This proportional change can be tortions were noticed by participants when they
explained by perspective distortions. Considering filled out the FACE-Q survey. There was a signifi-
the facial width alone decreased 10.5 percent in cant increase (9.1 percent, p = 0.003) in average
12-inch selfies as opposed to clinical photography FACE-Q nose score from 12-inch selfies to clinical
(p < 0.0001), these findings suggest the width of photography (Fig.  5). The average FACE-Q face
the face is a large contributor to the nose seem- score from 12-inch selfies to clinical photography
ing larger in short focal length photography, (5.7 percent, p = 0.06) trended toward statistical
namely, selfies. Table  2 shows all average mea- significance. Multiple participants stated, “my face
surements and percent differences for 12-inch looks more proportional in the clinical photo-
selfies, 18-inch selfies, and clinical photography. graph.” The increased nasal width to facial width

863
Copyright © 2022 American Society of Plastic Surgeons. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery • April 2022

Table 2.  Average Measurement Length of Each Feature and t Percent Change of Measurements between
Different Sets of Photographs with Their P Values*
12-Inch Selfie versus 18-Inch Selfie versus
18-Inch Clinical Clinical Photograph Clinical Photograph
12-Inch Selfie Photograph
Selfie (cm) (cm) (cm) % Change p % Change p
Nasal tip to trough of Cupid’s bow 1.86 1.84 1.85 −0.66 0.65 0.74 0.61
Alar base width 2.31 2.31 2.28 −1.44 0.29 −1.36 0.20
Alar wing width 3.78 3.71 3.67 −2.82 0.05 −0.97 0.26
Line of intercanthal distance to nasal tip 4.29 4.16 3.92 −8.69 <0.0001 −5.77 <0.0001
Left medial canthus to left alar base 4.30 4.23 4.11 −4.43 0.004 −2.91 0.003
Left medial canthus to nasal tip 4.55 4.43 4.22 −7.24 <0.0001 −4.75 <0.0001
Right medial canthus to nasal tip 4.60 4.48 4.24 −7.75 <0.0001 −5.20 <0.0001
Right medial canthus to right alar base 4.29 4.23 4.12 −4.01 0.006 −2.67 0.006
Menton to labiale inferius (length of chin) 3.23 3.32 3.61 11.95 <0.0001 8.80 <0.0001
Facial width 12.53 12.93 13.84 10.48 <0.0001 7.05 <0.0001
Alar base/facial width ratio 0.18 0.18 0.16 −10.84 <0.0001 −7.81 <0.0001
Alar wing/facial width ratio 0.30 0.29 0.27 −12.11 <0.0001 −7.53 <0.0001
Average nasal length 4.41 4.31 4.12
Nasal length/length of chin ratio 1.39 1.32 1.15 −16.98 <0.0001 −12.52 <0.0001
*A positive percent change signifies the facial feature is larger in clinical photographs; a negative percent change signifies the facial feature is
smaller. In addition to measuring the length of facial features in selfies, ratios were used to test proportionality. To test the proportion of nasal
width to facial width, the alar base and alar wing measurements were compared to the width of the face for each set of photographs. In addition,
the average of the five nasal length measurements (Table 1) was compared to the menton to labiale inferius measurement (a substitute for chin
length) to test how the middle and lower facial third change between selfies and clinical photographs. The average nasal length was calculated
from line of intercanthal distance to nasal tip, left medial canthus to left alar base, left medial canthus to nasal tip, right medial canthus to nasal
tip, and right medial canthus to right alar base. This was done to compare to length of chin as a ratio.

proportion and the increased nasal length to chin selfie be known. Our study’s nasal length and nasal
length proportion corroborate the perspective dis- width measurement data, coupled with the lower
tortion findings by Třebický et al.12 The increased FACE-Q nose score when viewing selfies, connects
nose attractiveness rating suggests that facial width the well-established negative feelings toward self-
and chin length are contributors to self-perception ies25,26,28–32 with the face shape change noted from
of the nose, in addition to the nose itself. short camera-to-subject distance.10,11,27 The restora-
A set of 18-inch selfies was chosen to represent tion of psyche is an important tenant in plastic and
a photograph taken with the smartphone held reconstructive surgery. The American Academy of
at full arm extension, a common pose for self- Facial Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery annual
ies. This set was taken to see how the distortion survey has noted an interaction between selfies
changes from previously studied 12-inch photo- and rhinoplasty requests multiple times in recent
graphs6 compared to a common selfie pose. The years.8,9,24 Surgeons may find it ideal to avoid any
reduction in distortion from an 18-inch selfie (a selfie-based discussion, intuitively seeing the pho-
selfie at arm’s length) as compared to a 12-inch tographs distort facial features. However, quantify-
selfie, as seen in Table  2, suggests there may be ing how selfies distort photographs is important
an optimal distance for minimizing distortion. It to better understand how these negative feelings
can guide the surgeon and the patient in their dis- influence a patient’s psyche, their choice in pursu-
cussion of surgical goals should the distance of a ing plastic surgery, and their outcome.

Table 3.  Interrater Reliability for Each Facial Measurement and Second Investigator Duplicated Measurements
of Five Participants
Average Measurement and Variance (cm)
Investigator 1 Investigator 2 Lin’s CCC
Nasal tip to trough of Cupid’s bow 1.83 ± 0.11 1.92 ± 0.07 0.852
Alar base width 2.34 ± 0.14 2.22 ± 0.07 0.727
Alar wing width 3.68 ± 0.18 3.70 ± 0.16 0.995
Line of iIntercanthal distance to nasal tip 4.08 ± 0.34 4.01 ± 0.30 0.945
Left medial canthus to left alar base 4.19 ± 0.15 4.16 ± 0.15 0.979
Left medial canthus to nasal tip 4.34 ± 0.38 4.27 ± 0.32 0.945
Right medial canthus to nasal tip 4.45 ± 0.26 4.35 ± 0.23 0.923
Right medial canthus to right alar base 4.21 ± 0.15 4.18 ± 0.17 0.974
Menton to labiale inferius 3.19 ± 0.15 3.19 ± 0.15 0.993
Facial width 12.99 ± 0.72 12.93 ± 0.69 0.991
Lin’s CCC, Lin’s concordance correlation coefficient.

864
Copyright © 2022 American Society of Plastic Surgeons. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Volume 149, Number 4 • Implications of Selfies in Rhinoplasty

Fig. 5. This graph shows the change in average FACE-Q scores for the participants’ nose and facial appearance. Participants per-
ceived their noses more favorably in clinical photographs than in 12-inch selfies (p = 0.003). There was no significant change in
perception of facial appearance (p = 0.06).

Social media sites, such as Instagram, began clinical photography. This distortion is corrobo-
to test a new interface without the “like” button to rated with nasal length and nose to chin length
combat these negative feelings.33 Thus, the interac- measurements. Perspective distortions are more
tion between selfie distortion and psyche is impor- pronounced in selfies, specifically at the lateral
tant for smartphone and social media companies border of the cheek and mandible. This contrib-
to better understand the societal impact of these utes to the face looking narrower and deeper. In
prevalent selfies. This article begins to quantify contrast, the face in clinical photography looks
distortions colloquially seen by individuals and, more flat and proportional (Figs.  2 and 3). This
using FACE-Q, test their effect on an individual’s visual distortion is corroborated by the facial width
opinion of their image. This can lead to a larger measurements and the nasal width to facial width
discussion about the unwanted psychological ratios of this study. This study begins to connect
impact of distorted selfie images by psychologists, the perspective changes seen visually in Figures 2
policy makers, and smartphone manufacturers. and 3 to the measurements in Table 2. This con-
Perspective distortions of facial photographs nection can begin to quantify why selfie photogra-
(where closer objects seem larger than distant phy looks different from professional photography
ones) are more pronounced with shorter camera- and can begin to facilitate a more precise discus-
to-subject distance and help explain our find- sion of outcomes between surgeons and patients.
ings.12,13 This study chose to place the lens of both Ward et al. standardized the bizygomatic width
cameras at eye level, and, therefore, the nose is of all photographs before calculating nasal distor-
closer to the camera than the chin. This perspec- tion.6 Instead, our study defined the facial width
tive distortion is more dramatic for a lens at 12 using landmarks. We chose the distance between the
or 18 inches from the face when compared to hairline drawn through the lateral canthi, because
a lens set 5 feet from the face. This can be seen the hairline and the skin immediately anterior pro-
in Figures 2 and 3, where the nose is larger than vided an easily distinguishable line of contrast. Our
the chin in a selfie but more proportional in study expressing the width of the nose as a ratio to

865
Copyright © 2022 American Society of Plastic Surgeons. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery • April 2022

facial width is essentially the same standardization instructions for patients to send in photographs
process as fixing a bizygomatic width for all pic- before and after surgery to minimize the distor-
tures. The advantage of width described as a ratio tions. The effect of selfies is unavoidable in other
is it does not require any alteration of photographs circumstances, such as patients sharing their own
(e.g., changing the size of the images to equalize preoperative and postoperative pictures on social
the bizygomatic width), nor does it require any in- media. Social media can drive online traffic toward
person anthropometric measurements. In addition, or away from a surgeon’s practice. Therefore, an
Ward et al. did not use a front-facing smartphone understanding of these distortions is necessary
camera to take their 12-inch photographs.6 to have these unavoidably public pictures judged
There are limitations to our study. Participants without bias.
were instructed to look at the camera lens, and
camera lenses were placed at facial level per pho- CONCLUSIONS
tographic standards described by DiBernardo et
This is the first study to use a smartphone cam-
al.1 This largely mitigated error brought on by
era to investigate camera-to-subject distance and
a person’s pose. Despite our efforts to keep the
the distortion selfies create.6,10,12 It begins to quan-
head stable, participants could have tilted their
tify the distortion seen with selfies at two different
head up or down slightly, introducing some mea-
distances as compared to standard clinical pho-
surement error of the chin or length of the nose.
tography. As the popularity of selfies increases, it
Also, using a participant-held ruler as a measure-
is crucial to understand how they distort facial fea-
ment scale introduces some error if the ruler tilts
tures and how patients use them to communicate.
between sets of photographs. Another limitation
Further study will elucidate how changes between
is this study’s use of only one smartphone, con-
different photography settings further contribute
sidering the continuously updating smartphone
to distortions and perceptions in plastic surgery.
market. The FACE-Q and NOSE-Q inventories
were administered only for the 12-inch selfies and Bardia Amirlak, M.D.
clinical photographs. This was done to limit the Department of Plastic Surgery
time requirement from participants, but it does University of Texas Southwestern
1801 Inwood Road
not allow this study to compare the perception Dallas, Texas 75390
of 18-inch selfies to clinical photographs. Finally, bardia.amirlak@utsouthwestern.edu
although there was a decreasing trend in average Instagram: dramirlak
FACE-Q face scores from 12-inch selfies to clinical
photographs (5.7 percent, p = 0.06), this did not
PATIENT CONSENT
achieve statistical significance. A larger sample
size could potentially solidify this difference. Patients provided written informed consent for the
Selfies are taken at a variety of angles and use of their images.
in different lighting to find the most flattering
pose.34 In addition, there are a multitude of filters REFERENCES
that also flatter.35,36 Future studies could utilize lat-
1. DiBernardo BE, Adams RL, Krause J, Fiorillo MA, Gheradini
eral and oblique photographs to further describe G. Photographic standards in plastic surgery. Plast Reconstr
the distortion a selfie brings to the face and to Surg. 1998;102:559–568.
the body. In addition, selfies could be captured 2. Yavuzer R, Smirnes S, Jackson IT. Guidelines for standard pho-
before and after facial plastic surgery, such as a tography in plastic surgery. Ann Plast Surg. 2001;46:293–300.
face lift or rhinoplasty, to study the effect of surgi- 3. Elliott A. “I want to look like that!”: Cosmetic surgery and
celebrity culture. Cultural Sociology 2011;5:463–477.
cal interventions through the lens patients utilize 4. Pastorek NJ. The female beautiful face. JAMA.
the most. Comparing selfies and clinical photo- 2017;317:1198–1200.
graphs to three-dimensional photography will also 5. Özgür E, Muluk NB, Cingi C. Is selfie a new cause of increas-
be pursued in future studies. Understanding the ing rhinoplasties? Facial Plast Surg. 2017;33:423–427.
changes in facial features from a selfie is impor- 6. Ward B, Ward M, Fried O, Paskhover B. Nasal distortion in
short-distance photographs: The selfie effect. JAMA Facial
tant not only in rhinoplasty but also in other facial Plast Surg. 2018;20:333–335.
procedures. The surgeon can have informed dis- 7. Chang L, Li P, Loh RSM, Chua THH. A study of Singapore ado-
cussions with the patient based on selfies during lescent girls’ selfie practices, peer appearance comparisons,
preoperative and postoperative consultation if and body esteem on Instagram. Body Image 2019;29:90–99.
they understand the inherent distortions with self- 8. American Academy of Facial Plastic and Reconstructive
Surgery. Selfie trend increases demand for facial plastic
ies. In addition, variability of photographs from surgery. Available at: https://www.aafprs.org/media/press_
patients can be reduced by having standardized release/20140311.html. Accessed June 17, 2019.

866
Copyright © 2022 American Society of Plastic Surgeons. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Volume 149, Number 4 • Implications of Selfies in Rhinoplasty

9. American Academy of Facial Plastic and Reconstructive 25. McLean SA, Paxton SJ, Wertheim EH, Masters J.
Surgery. AAFPRS 2018 annual Survey reveals key trends in Photoshopping the selfie: Self photo editing and photo
facial plastic surgery. Available at: https://www.aafprs.org/ investment are associated with body dissatisfaction in adoles-
media/stats_polls/m_stats.html. Accessed June 17, 2019. cent girls. Int J Eat Disord. 2015;48:1132–1140.
10. Noyes E, Jenkins R. Camera-to-subject distance affects 26. Mills JS, Musto S, Williams L, Tiggemann M. “Selfie” harm:
face configuration and perceived identity. Cognition Effects on mood and body image in young women. Body
2017;165:97–104. Image 2018;27:86–92.
11. Bryan R, Perona P, Adolphs R. Perspective distortion from 27. Harper B, Latto R. Cyclopean vision, size estimation, and
interpersonal distance is an implicit visual cue for social presence in orthostereoscopic images. Presence: Teleoperators
judgments of faces. PLoS One 2012;7:e45301. and Virtual Environments 2001;10:312–330.
12. Třebický V, Fialová J, Kleisner K, Havlíček J. Focal length 28. Chua THH, Chang L. Follow me and like my beautiful self-
affects depicted shape and perception of facial images. PLoS ies: Singapore teenage girls’ engagement in self-presentation
One 2016;11:e0149313. and peer comparison on social media. Computers in Human
13. Kingslake R. Optics in Photography. Bellingham, Wash.: SPIE-- Behavior 2016;55:190–197.
The International Society for Optical Engineering; 1992. 29. Kleemans M, Daalmans S, Carbaat I, Anschütz D. Picture
14. Gallagher F. What is the focal length of an iPhone camera perfect: The direct effect of manipulated Instagram pho-
and why should I care? Available at: https://improvepho- tos on body image in adolescent girls. Media Psychology
tography.com/55460/what-is-the-focal-length-of-an-iphone- 2018;21:93–110.
camera-and-why-should-i-care/. Accessed June 17, 2019. 30. Wang Y, Fardouly J, Vartanian LR, Lei L. Selfie-viewing and
15. Nikon, Inc. AF-S DX NIKKOR 18-200mm f/3.5-5.6G ED
facial dissatisfaction among Chinese adolescents: A moder-
VR II tech specs. Available at: https://www.nikonusa.com/ ated mediation model of general attractiveness internaliza-
en/nikon-products/product/camera-lenses/af-s-dx-nikkor- tion and body appreciation. Body Image 2019;30:35–43.
18-200mm-f%252f3.5-5.6g-ed-vr-ii.html#tab-ProductDetail- 31. Barry CT, Doucette H, Loflin DC, Rivera-Hudson N,
ProductTabs-TechSpecs. Accessed June 17, 2019. Herrington LL. “Let me take a selfie”: Associations between
16. Belt AF. The Elements of Photography: Understanding and Creating self-photography, narcissism, and self-esteem. Psychology of
Sophisticated Images. 2nd ed. New York: Routledge; 2011. Popular Media Culture 2017;6:48–60.
17. Anthes B, Modrak RB. Reframing Photography: Theory and
32. Diefenbach S, Christoforakos L. The selfie paradox: Nobody
Practice. New York: Routledge; 2011. seems to like them yet everyone has reasons to take them.
18. Cooper EA, Piazza EA, Banks MS. The perceptual basis of An exploration of psychological functions of selfies in self-
common photographic practice. J Vis. 2012;12:8. presentation. Front Psychol. 2017;8:7.
19. Guyuron B. Precision rhinoplasty. Part I: The role of life-size 33. Martin N. Instagram may be getting rid of ‘likes’ on platform.
photographs and soft-tissue cephalometric analysis. Plast Available at: https://www.forbes.com/sites/nicolemar-
Reconstr Surg. 1988;81:489–499. tin1/2019/04/30/instagram-may-be-getting-rid-of-likes-on-
20. Guyuron B. Precision rhinoplasty. Part II: Prediction. Plast platform/#5036825931d1. Accessed April 30, 2019.
Reconstr Surg. 1988;81:500–505. 34. Improve Photography. 15 poses and tips for selfies. Available
21. Klassen AF, Cano SJ, Schwitzer JA, Scott AM, Pusic AL. at: https://improvephotography.com/19446/how-to-pose-
FACE-Q scales for health-related quality of life, early life for-a-selfie/. Accessed June 17, 2019.
impact, satisfaction with outcomes, and decision to have 35. Wolfson S. Snapchat photo filters linked to rise in cosmetic
treatment: Development and validation. Plast Reconstr Surg. surgery requests. The Guardian. Available at: https://www.
2015;135:375–386. theguardian.com/technology/2018/aug/08/snapchat-sur-
22. Klassen AF, Cano SJ, Scott A, Snell L, Pusic AL. Measuring gery-doctors-report-rise-in-patient-requests-to-look-filtered.
patient-reported outcomes in facial aesthetic patients: devel- Accessed June 17, 2019.
opment of the FACE-Q. Facial Plast Surg. 2010;26:303–309. 36. Chiu A. Patients are desperate to resemble their doctored
23. Lin LI. A concordance correlation coefficient to evaluate selfies. Plastic surgeons alarmed by ‘Snapchat dysmor-
reproducibility. Biometrics 1989;45:255–268. phia.’ The Washington Post. Available at: https://www.wash-
24. American Academy of Facial Plastic and Reconstructive
ingtonpost.com/news/morning-mix/wp/2018/08/06/
Surgery. AAFPRS annual survey unveils rising trends in facial patients-are-desperate-to-resemble-their-doctored-selfies-
plastic surgery. Available at: https://www.aafprs.org/media/ plastic-surgeons-alarmed-by-snapchat-dysmorphia/?utm_
press-release/20170125.html. Accessed June 17, 2019. term=.8d317be16328. Accessed June 17, 2019.

867
Copyright © 2022 American Society of Plastic Surgeons. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like