You are on page 1of 15

MIND, BRAIN, AND EDUCATION

Early Language Learning


and Literacy: Neuroscience
Implications for Education
Patricia K. Kuhl1

ABSTRACT—The last decade has produced an explosion human being to trigger their computation skills to learn
in neuroscience research examining young children’s early from exposure to language. These data are challenging brain
processing of language that has implications for education. scientists to discover how brains actually work—how, in
Noninvasive, safe functional brain measurements have now this case, computational brain areas and social brain areas
been proven feasible for use with children starting at birth. mature during development and interact during learning. The
In the arena of language, the neural signatures of learning can results also challenge educational scientists to incorporate
be documented at a remarkably early point in development, these findings about the social brain into teaching practices.
and these early measures predict performance in children’s Behavioral and brain studies on developing children indicate
language and pre-reading abilities in the second, third, and fifth that children’s skills, measured very early in infancy, predict
year of life, a finding with theoretical and educational import. their later performance and learning. For example, measures of
There is evidence that children’s early mastery of language phonetic learning in the first year of life predict language skills
requires learning in a social context, and this finding also between 18 and 30 months of age, and also predict language
has important implications for education. Evidence relating abilities and pre-literacy skills at the age of 5 years. Moreover,
socioeconomic status (SES) to brain function for language by the age of 5, prior to formal schooling, our studies show that
suggests that SES should be considered a proxy for the brain activation in brain areas related to language and literacy
opportunity to learn and that the complexity of language are strongly correlated with the socioeconomic status (SES)
input is a significant factor in developing brain areas related of the children’s families. The implication of these findings
to language. The data indicate that the opportunity to learn is that children’s learning trajectories regarding language are
from complex stimuli and events are vital early in life, and that influenced by their experiences well before the start of school.
success in school begins in infancy. In the next decade, neuroscientists, educators, biologists,
computer scientists, speech and hearing scientists, psycholo-
gists, and linguists will increasingly work together to under-
stand how children’s critical ‘‘windows of opportunity’’ for
Developmental studies suggest that children learn more and
learning work, what triggers their inception, and how learn-
learn earlier than previously thought. In the arena of language
ing can be encouraged once the optimal period for learning has
development, our studies show that children’s early learning
passed. The ultimate goal is to alter the trajectories of learning
is complex and multifaceted. For example, research shows
to maximize language and literacy skills in all children.
that young children rely on what has been called ‘‘statistical
To explore these topics, this review focuses on research in
learning,’’ a form of implicit learning that occurs as children
my laboratory on the youngest language learners—infants in
interact in the world, to acquire the language spoken in
the first year of life—and on the most elementary units of
their culture. However, new data also indicate that children
language—the consonants and vowels that make up words.
require a social setting and social interaction with another
Infants’ responses to the basic building blocks of speech
provide an experimentally accessible porthole through which
we can observe the interaction of nature and nurture in
1 Institute for Learning & Brain Sciences, University of Washington language acquisition. Recent studies show that infants’ early
Address correspondence to Patricia K. Kuhl, Institute for Learning &
processing of the phonetic units in their language predicts
Brain Sciences, University of Washington, Mailstop 357920, Seattle, future competence in language and literacy, contributing
WA 98195; e-mail: pkkuhl@u.washington.edu. to theoretical debates about the nature of language and

© 2011 the Author


128 Journal Compilation © 2011 International Mind, Brain, and Education Society and Blackwell Publishing, Inc. Volume 5—Number 3
Patricia K. Kuhl

emphasizing the practical implications of the research as well cognitive process (e.g., recognition of a semantic violation
as the potential for early interventions to support language and within a sentence or phrase). By placing sensors on a child’s
literacy. We are also beginning to discover how exposure to scalp, the activity of neural networks firing in a coordinated
two languages early in infancy produces bilingualism and the and synchronous fashion in open field configurations can be
effects of dual-language input on the brain. Bilingual children measured, and voltage changes occurring as a function of
inform debates on the critical period, with implications for cortical neural activity can be detected. ERPs provide precise
education, given the increasing number of bilingual children time resolution (milliseconds), making them well suited for
in the nation’s schools. studying the high-speed and temporally ordered structure of
In this review, I will also describe a current working human speech. ERP experiments can also be carried out in
hypothesis about the relationship between the ‘‘social brain’’ populations who cannot provide overt responses because of
and learning, and its implications for brain development. I age or cognitive impairment, such as children with autism
will argue that to ‘‘crack the speech code’’ infants combine a (Coffey-Corina, Padden, Estes, & Kuhl, 2011; Kuhl et al.,
powerful set of domain-general computational and cognitive 2005a). However, spatial resolution of the source of brain
skills with their equally extraordinary social skills (Kuhl, activation using EEG has limitations.
2007, 2011)—and explore what that means for theory and MEG is another brain imaging technique that tracks
practice. Moreover, social experience with more than one activity in the brain with exquisite temporal resolution.
language, either long-term experience as a simultaneous The SQUID (superconducting quantum interference device)
bilingual or short-term experience with a second language sensors located within the MEG helmet measure the minute
in the laboratory, is associated with increases in cognitive magnetic fields associated with electrical currents that are
flexibility, in adults (Bialystok, 1991), in children (Carlson & produced by the brain when it is performing sensory, motor,
Meltzoff, 2008), and in young infants (Conboy, Sommerville, or cognitive tasks. MEG allows precise localization of the
& Kuhl, 2008; Conboy, Sommerville, Wicha, Romo, & Kuhl, neural currents responsible for the sources of the magnetic
2011). Experience alters the trajectory of development in the fields. Cheour et al. (2004) and Imada et al. (2006) used
young brain. new head-tracking methods and MEG to show phonetic
I have suggested that the social brain—in ways we have discrimination in newborns and in infants during the first year
yet to understand—‘‘gates’’ the computational mechanisms of life. Sophisticated head-tracking software and hardware
underlying learning in the domain of language (Kuhl, 2007, enables investigators to correct for infants’ head movements,
2011). The assertion that social factors gate language learning and allows the examination of multiple brain areas as infants
may explain not only how typically developing children listen to speech (Imada et al., 2006). Travis et al. (in press)
acquire language, but also why children with autism exhibit used MEG to examine the spaciotemporal cortical dynamics
twin deficits in social cognition and language (see Kuhl, of word understanding in 12- to 18-month-old infants and
Coffey-Corina, Padden, & Dawson, 2005a for discussion). adults, and showed that both groups encode lexico-semantic
Moreover, this gating hypothesis may explain why social information in left frontotemporal brain areas, suggesting that
factors play a far more significant role than previously realized neural mechanisms are established in infancy and operate
in human learning across domains throughout our lifetimes across the lifespan. MEG (as well as EEG) techniques are
(Meltzoff, Kuhl, Movellan, & Sejnowski, 2009). completely safe and noiseless.
MRI provides static structural/anatomical pictures of the
brain, and can be combined with MEG and/or EEG. Structural
WINDOWS TO THE YOUNG BRAIN MRIs show anatomical differences in brain regions across the
lifespan, and have recently been used to predict second-
Rapid advances have been made in noninvasive techniques that language phonetic learning in adults (Golestani, Molko,
examine language processing in young children (Figure 1). Dehaene, LeBihan, & Pallier, 2007). Structural MRI measures
They include Electroencephalography (EEG)/Event-related in young infants identify the size of various brain structures
Potentials (ERPs), Magnetoencephalography (MEG), func- and these measures correlate with later language abilities
tional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), and Near- (Ortiz-Mantilla, Choe, Flax, Grant, & Benasich, 2010). When
Infrared Spectroscopy (NIRS). structural MRI images are superimposed on the physiological
ERPs have been widely used to study speech and activity detected by MEG or EEG, the spatial localization of
language processing in infants and young children (for brain activities recorded by these methods can be improved.
reviews, see Conboy, Rivera-Gaxiola, Silva-Pereyra, & Kuhl, fMRI is a popular method of neuroimaging in adults because
2008; Dehaene-Lambertz, Dehaene, & Hertz-Pannier, 2002; it provides high spatial-resolution maps of neural activity
Friederici, 2005; Kuhl, 2004). ERPs, a part of the EEG, reflect across the entire brain (e.g., Gernsbacher & Kaschak, 2003).
electrical activity that is time-locked to the presentation of Unlike EEG and MEG, fMRI does not directly detect neural
a specific sensory stimulus (e.g., syllables or words) or a activity, but rather the changes in blood-oxygenation that

Volume 5—Number 3 129


Early Language Learning and Literacy

Fig. 1. Four techniques now used extensively with infants and young children to examine their responses to linguistic signals (from Kuhl &
Rivera-Gaxiola, 2008).

occur in response to neural activation. Neural events happen in the brain as well as total blood volume changes in various
in milliseconds; however, the blood-oxygenation changes that regions of the cerebral cortex using near-infrared light. The
they induce are spread out over several seconds, thereby NIRS system can determine the activity in specific regions
severely limiting fMRI’s temporal resolution. Few studies have of the brain by continuously monitoring blood hemoglobin
attempted fMRI with infants because the technique requires level. Reports have begun to appear on infants in the first two
infants to be perfectly still, and because the MRI device years of life, testing infant responses to phonemes as well as
produces loud sounds making it necessary to shield infants’ longer stretches of speech such as ‘‘motherese’’ and forward
ears. fMRI studies allow precise localization of brain activity versus reversed sentences (Bortfeld, Wruck, & Boas, 2007;
and a few pioneering studies show remarkable similarity in Homae, Watanabe, Nakano, Asakawa, & Taga, 2006; Peña,
the structures responsive to language in infants and adults Bonatti, Nespor, & Mehler, 2002; Taga & Asakawa, 2007).
(Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2002, 2006). As with other hemodynamic techniques such as fMRI, NIRS
NIRS also measures cerebral hemodynamic responses in typically does not provide good temporal resolution. However,
relation to neural activity, but utilizes the absorption of light, event-related NIRS paradigms are being developed (Gratton
which is sensitive to the concentration of hemoglobin, to & Fabiani, 2001a,b). One of the most important potential uses
measure activation (Aslin & Mehler, 2005). NIRS measures of the NIRS technique is possible co-registration with other
changes in blood oxy- and deoxy-hemoglobin concentrations testing techniques such as EEG and MEG.

130 Volume 5—Number 3


Patricia K. Kuhl

LANGUAGE EXHIBITS A ‘‘CRITICAL PERIOD’’ FOR 2000; although see Birdsong, 1992; Snow & Hoefnagel-Hohle,
LEARNING 1978; White & Genesee, 1996). However, only a few studies
have examined language learning in both children and adults.
A stage-setting concept for human language learning is the Snow and Hoefnagel-Hohle (1978) made such a comparison
graph shown in Figure 2, redrawn from a study by Johnson and report that English speakers learning Dutch in Holland
and Newport (1989) on English grammar in native speakers over a 1-year period learned it most quickly and efficiently if
of Korean learning English as a second language. The graph they were between 12 and 15 years of age, rather than younger
as rendered shows a simplified schematic of second language or older, which is a very intriguing result. More second-
competence as a function of the age of second language language learning studies are needed that vary the age of the
acquisition. learner, and assess both the speed of initial learning as well as
Figure 2 is surprising from the standpoint of more general the final level of proficiency.
human learning. In the domain of language, infants and One important point is that not all aspects of language
young children are superior learners when compared to exhibit the same temporally defined critical ‘‘window’’ as
adults, in spite of adults’ cognitive superiority. Language that shown in Figure 2. The developmental timing of critical
is one of the classic examples of a ‘‘critical’’ or ‘‘sensitive’’ periods for learning phonetic, lexical, and syntactic levels
period in neurobiology (Bruer, 2008; Johnson & Newport, of language vary, although studies cannot yet document the
1989; Knudsen, 2004; Kuhl, 2004; Newport, Bavelier, & precise timing at each individual level. Studies in typically
Neville, 2001). developing monolingual children indicate, for example, that
Does the ‘‘critical period’’ diagram mean that it is impossible an important period for phonetic learning occurs prior to the
to learn a new language after childhood? Both anecdotal and end of the first year, whereas syntactic learning flourishes
experimental evidence support the idea that the answer to between 18 and 36 months of age. Vocabulary development
this question is ‘‘No.’’ A new language can be learned at ‘‘explodes’’ at 18 months of age. One goal of future research is to
any age, but most agree that the level of expertise will identify the optimum learning periods for phonological, lexical,
differ from that of a native speaker if exposure to the new and grammatical levels of language, so that we understand
language occurs after puberty. After puberty, mastery of the how they overlap and differ. This in turn will assist in the
pronunciation and mastery of the grammar is unlikely to be development of novel methods that improve second-language
identical to that of a native speaker, although word learning learning at all ages.
does not appear to be as sensitive to age and remains good Given the current state of research, there is widespread
throughout life. agreement that we do not learn equally well over the lifespan.
Scientists generally agree that the ‘‘critical period’’ learning Theoretical work is therefore focused on attempts to explain
curve is representative of data across a wide variety of second- the phenomenon. What accounts for adults’ inability to
language learning studies (Bialystok & Hakuta, 1994; Birdsong learn a new language with the facility of an infant? One
& Molis, 2001; Flege, Yeni-Komshian, & Liu, 1999; Johnson & of the candidate explanations was Lenneberg’s hypothesis
Newport, 1989; Mayberry & Lock, 2003; Neville et al., 1997; that development of the corpus callosum affected language
Weber-Fox & Neville, 1999; Yeni-Komshian, Flege, & Liu, learning (Lenneberg, 1967; Newport et al., 2001). More recent
hypotheses take a different perspective. Newport raised
a ‘‘less is more’’ hypothesis, which suggests that infants’
limited cognitive capacities actually allow superior learning
of the simplified language spoken to infants (Newport, 1990).
Work in my laboratory led me to advance the concept
of neural commitment, the idea that neural circuitry and
overall architecture develops early in infancy to detect the
phonetic and prosodic patterns of speech (Kuhl, 2004; Zhang,
Kuhl, Imada, Kotani, & Tohkura, 2005; Zhang et al., 2009).
This architecture is designed to maximize the efficiency of
processing for the language(s) experienced by the infant.
Once established, the neural architecture arising from French
or Tagalog, for example, impedes learning of new patterns that
do not conform. I will return to the concept of the critical
period for language learning, and the role that computational,
Fig. 2. The relationship between age of acquisition of a second cognitive, and social skills may play in accounting for the
language and language skill (adapted from Johnson & Newport, relatively poor performance of adults attempting to learn a
1989). second language.

Volume 5—Number 3 131


Early Language Learning and Literacy

PHONETIC LEARNING the brain’s social understanding mechanisms which form


a human mirroring system for seamless social interaction
Perception of the phonetic units of speech—the vowels and (Hari & Kujala, 2009).
consonants that make up words—is one of the most widely What enables the kind of learning we see in infants for
studied linguistic skills in infancy and adulthood. Phonetic speech? No machine in the world can derive the phonemic
perception and the role of experience in learning can be studied inventory of a language from natural language input (Rabiner
in children at birth, during development as they are bathed & Huang, 1993), although models improve when exposed
in a particular language, in adults from different cultures, in to ‘‘motherese,’’ the linguistically simplified and acoustically
children with developmental disabilities, and in nonhuman exaggerated speech that adults universally use when speaking
animals. Phonetic perception studies provide critical tests to infants (de Boer & Kuhl, 2003). The variability in speech
of theories of language development and its evolution. An input is simply too enormous; Japanese adults produce both
extensive literature on developmental speech perception exists English r- and l-like sounds, exposing Japanese infants to
and brain measures are adding substantially to our knowledge both sounds (Lotto, Sato, & Diehl, 2004; Werker et al., 2007).
of phonetic development and learning (see Kuhl, 2004; Kuhl How do Japanese infants learn that these two sounds do not
et al., 2008; Werker & Curtin, 2005). distinguish words in their language, and that these differences
The world’s languages contain approximately 600 conso- should be ignored? Similarly, English speakers produce
nants and 200 vowels (Ladefoged, 2001). Each language uses Spanish b and p, exposing American infants to both categories
a unique set of about 40 distinct elements, phonemes, which of sound (Abramson & Lisker, 1970). How do American infants
change the meaning of a word (e.g. from bat to pat in English). learn that these sounds are not important in distinguishing
But phonemes are actually groups of non-identical sounds, pho- words in English? An important discovery in the 1970s was that
netic units, which are functionally equivalent in the language. infants initially hear all these phonetic differences (Eimas, 1975;
Japanese-learning infants have to group the phonetic units r Eimas, Siqueland, Jusczyk, & Vigorito, 1971; Lasky, Syrdal-
and l into a single phonemic category (Japanese r), whereas Lasky, & Klein, 1975; Werker & Lalonde, 1988). What we
English-learning infants must uphold the distinction to sep- must explain is how infants learn to group phonetic units into
arate rake from lake. Similarly, Spanish-learning infants must phonemic categories that make a difference in their language.
distinguish phonetic units critical to Spanish words (bano and
pano), whereas English-learning infants must combine them
into a single category (English b). If infants were exposed THE TIMING OF PHONETIC LEARNING
only to the subset of phonetic units that will eventually be
used phonemically to differentiate words in their language, An important discovery in the 1980s identified the timing of
the problem would be trivial. But infants are exposed to many a crucial change in infant perception. The transition from an
more phonetic variants than will be used phonemically, and early universal perceptual ability to distinguish all the phonetic
have to derive the appropriate groupings used in their specific units of all languages to a more language-specific pattern of
language. The baby’s task in the first year of life, therefore, is perception occurred very early in development—between 6
to make some progress in figuring out the composition of the and 12 months of age (Werker & Tees, 1984), and initial
40 odd phonemic categories in their language(s) before trying work demonstrated that infants’ perception of non-native
to acquire words that depend on these elementary units. distinctions declines during the second half of the first year of
Learning to produce the sounds that will characterize life (Best & McRoberts, 2003; Rivera-Gaxiola, Silvia-Pereyra,
infants as speakers of their ‘‘mother tongue’’ is equally & Kuhl, 2005b; Tsao, Liu, & Kuhl, 2006; Werker & Tees, 1984).
challenging, and is not completely mastered until the age Work in this laboratory also established a new fact: At the same
of 8 years (Ferguson, Menn, & Stoel-Gammon, 1992). Yet, by time that non-native perception declines, native-language
10 months of age, differences can be discerned in the babbling speech perception shows a significant increase. Japanese
of infants raised in different countries (de Boysson-Bardies, infants’ discrimination of English r-l declines between 8 and
1993), and in the laboratory, vocal imitation can be elicited by 10 months of age, whereas at the same time in development
20 weeks (Kuhl & Meltzoff, 1982). The speaking patterns we American infants’ discrimination of the same sounds shows an
adopt early in life last a lifetime (Flege, 1991). My colleague and increase (Kuhl et al., 2006) (Figure 3).
I have suggested that this kind of indelible learning stems from We argued that the increase observed in native-language
a linkage between sensory and motor experience; sensory phonetic perception represented a critical step in initial
experience with a specific language establishes auditory language learning (Kuhl et al., 2006). In later studies testing
patterns stored in memory that are unique to that language, the same infants on both native and non-native phonetic
and these representations guide infants’ successive motor contrasts, we showed a significant negative correlation
approximations until a match is achieved (Kuhl & Meltzoff, between discrimination of the two kinds of contrasts—as
1996). This ability to imitate vocally may also depend on native speech perception abilities increased in a particular

132 Volume 5—Number 3


Patricia K. Kuhl

Fig. 3. Effects of age on discrimination of the American English /ra-


la/ phonetic contrast by American and Japanese infants at 6–8 and Fig. 4. Idealized case of distributional learning is shown. Two
10–12 months of age. Mean percent correct scores are shown with women speak ‘‘motherese,’’ one in English and the other in Japanese.
standard errors indicated (adapted from Kuhl et al., 2006). Distributions of English /r/ and /l/, as well as Japanese /r/, are
tabulated. Infants are sensitive to these distributional cues and,
child, that child’s non-native abilities declined (Kuhl, Conboy, during the critical period, but only in a social context, plasticity is
Padden, Nelson, & Pruitt, 2005b). This was precisely what induced (modified from Kuhl, 2010).
our model of early language predicted (Kuhl, 2004; Kuhl et al.,
2008). In the early period, our data showed that although
adult speakers of English and Japanese produce both English
better discrimination on a native contrast predicts rapid
r- and l-like sounds, so it is not the mere presence of the sound
growth in later language abilities, better discrimination on non-
in language spoken to infants that accounts for learning, but
native contrasts predicts slower language growth (Kuhl et al.,
instead the patterns of distributional frequency of sounds
2005b, 2008). Better native abilities enhanced infants’ skills in
across the two languages. The idealized model of learning
detecting words and this vaulted them toward language,
is shown in Figure 4. When infants listen to English and
whereas better non-native abilities indicated that infants
Japanese, they take into account the distributional properties
remained in an earlier phase of development—sensitive to all
of the phonetic units contained in the two languages. Infants
phonetic differences. Infants’ ability to learn which phonetic
are sensitive to these distributional frequency differences
units are relevant in the language(s) they experience, while
in language input, and the distributional data affects their
decreasing or inhibiting their attention to the phonetic units
perception (Kuhl, Williams, Lacerda, Stevens, & Lindblom,
that do not distinguish words in their language, is the necessary
1992; Maye, Weiss, & Aslin, 2008; Maye, Werker, & Gerken,
step required to begin the path toward language—not auditory
2002; Teinonen, Fellman, Naatanen, Alku, & Huotilainen,
acuity, per se, as better sensory abilities would be expected to
2009). In fact, it has been shown that these distributional
improve both native and non-native speech discrimination.
differences are exaggerated in ‘‘motherese,’’ the prosodically
These data led to a theoretical model (Native Language
and phonetically stretched utterances that are near universal
Magnet, expanded, or NLM-e, see Kuhl et al., 2008 for details)
in language spoken to children around the world (Kuhl et al.,
which argues that an implicit learning process commits the
1997; Vallabha, McClelland, Pons, Weker, & Amano, 2007;
brain’s neural circuitry to the properties of native-language
Werker et al., 2007).
speech, and that this neural commitment has bi-directional
As illustrated in the idealized case (Figure 4), the
effects—it increases learning for patterns (such as words) that
distributions of English and Japanese differ: English motherese
are compatible with the learned phonetic structure, whereas
contains many English /r/ and /l/ sounds and very few of the
decreasing perception of non-native patterns that do not match
Japanese retroflex /r/ sounds, whereas the reverse is true for
the learned scheme.
Japanese motherese. A variety of studies show that infants pick
up the distributional frequency patterns in ambient speech,
WHAT ENABLES PHONETIC LEARNING BETWEEN whether they experience them during short-term laboratory
8 AND 10 MONTHS OF AGE? experiments or over months in natural environments, and that
this experience alters phonetic perception. Statistical learning
Children’s Computational Skills from the distributional properties in speech thus supports
An implicit form of computational learning, referred to as infants’ transition in early development from the ‘‘universal’’
‘‘statistical learning’’ (Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996), was state of perception they exhibited at birth to the ‘‘native’’ state
discovered to play a role in infants’ transition from universal of phonetic listening exhibited at the end of the first year
to language-specific listeners. For example, studies show that of life.

Volume 5—Number 3 133


Early Language Learning and Literacy

From a theoretical standpoint, brain plasticity for speech listened to four different native speakers of Mandarin during
could be described as a statistical process. Infants build 12 sessions scheduled over 4–5 weeks. The foreign-language
up statistical distributions of the sounds contained in the ‘‘tutors’’ read books and played with toys in sessions that
language they hear, and at some point these distributional were unscripted. A control group was also exposed for 12
properties become stable—additional language input does sessions but heard only English from native speakers. After
not cause the overall statistical distribution of sounds to infants in the experimental Mandarin exposure group and
change substantially and, according to NLM-e, this would the English control group completed their sessions, they were
cause the organism to become less sensitive to language tested with a Mandarin phonetic contrast that does not occur
input. This change in plasticity with experience may be due in English. Both behavioral and ERP methods were used. The
to a statistical process: when experience produces a stable results indicated that infants showed a remarkable ability to
distribution of sounds, plasticity is reduced. Hypothetically, learn from the ‘‘live-person’’ sessions—after exposure, they
a child’s distribution of the vowel ‘‘ah’’ might stabilize when performed significantly better on the Mandarin contrast when
she hears her one-millionth token of the vowel ‘‘ah,’’ and this compared to the control group that heard only English. In fact,
stability could instigate a closure of the critical period. On they performed equivalently to infants of the same age tested
this account, plasticity is independent of time, and instead in Taiwan who had been listening to Mandarin for 10 months
dependent on the amount and the variability of input provided (Kuhl et al., 2003).
by experience. The NLM-e account lends itself to testable The study revealed that infants can learn from first-time
hypotheses, and empirical tests of the tenets of the model natural exposure to a foreign language at 9 months, and
are underway. Studies of bilingual infants, reviewed later in answered what was initially the experimental question: Can
this chapter, provide one example of an empirical test of the infants learn the statistical structure of phonemes in a new
model. In the case of bilingualism, increased variability in language given first-time exposure at 9 months of age? If infants
language input may affect the rate at which distributional required a long-term history of listening to that language—as
stability is achieved in development. In other words, NLM-e would be the case if infants needed to build up statistical
proposes that bilingual children stay ‘‘open’’ longer to the distributions over the initial 9 months of life—the answer
effects of language experience, and therefore that bilingual to our question would have been no. However, the data
children will show distinct patterns in early development, clearly showed that infants are capable of learning at 9 months
different from their monolingual peers (‘‘Bilingual Language when exposed to a new language. Moreover, learning was
Learners’’ section). From a statistical standpoint, bilingual durable. Infants returned to the laboratory for their behavioral
children may achieve a stable distribution of the sounds in their discrimination tests between 2 and 12 days after the final
two languages at a later point in development when compared language-exposure session, and between 8 and 33 days for
to monolingual children, therefore exhibiting greater plasticity their ERP measurements. No ‘‘forgetting’’ of the Mandarin
than monolingual children at the same age. Experience drives contrast occurred during the 2- to 33-day delay.
plasticity. We were struck by the fact that infants exposed to
Mandarin were socially very engaged in the language sessions
and began to wonder about the role of social interaction
The Social Component: A Possible Trigger for Critical in learning. Would infants learn if they were exposed to
Period Onset? the same information in the absence of a human being,
New studies suggested that infants’ computational abilities say, via television or an audiotape? If statistical learning is
alone could not account for phonetic learning. Our studies sufficient, the television and audio-only conditions should
demonstrated that infant language learning in complex produce learning. Infants who were exposed to the same
natural environments required something more than raw foreign-language material at the same time and at the same
computation. Laboratory studies testing infant phonetic and rate, but via standard television or audiotape only, showed
word learning from exposure to complex natural language no learning—their performance equaled that of infants in the
demonstrated limits on statistical learning, and provided control group who had not been exposed to Mandarin at all
new information suggesting that social brain systems are (Figure 5).
integrally involved and, in fact, may be necessary to trigger Thus, the presence of a human being interacting with
natural language learning (Kuhl, Tsao, & Liu, 2003; Conboy the infant during language exposure, while not required for
& Kuhl, 2011). simpler statistical-learning tasks (Maye et al., 2002; Saffran
The new experiments tested infants in the following way: et al., 1996), is critical for learning in complex natural
At 9 months of age, when the initial universal pattern of language-learning situations (Kuhl et al., 2003). Using the
infant perception is changing to one that is more language- same experimental design, this work has been extended to
specific, infants were exposed to a foreign language for the Spanish and included measures beyond Kuhl et al. (2003);
first time (Kuhl et al., 2003). Nine-month-old American infants these studies demonstrated that infants not only learn Spanish

134 Volume 5—Number 3


Patricia K. Kuhl

(a)

(b)

Fig. 5. The need for social interaction in language acquisition is shown by foreign-language learning experiments. Nine-month-old infants
experienced 12 sessions of Mandarin Chinese through (a) natural interaction with a Chinese speaker (left) or the identical linguistic
information delivered via television (right) or audiotape (not shown). (b) Natural interaction resulted in significant learning of Mandarin
phonemes when compared with a control group who participated in interaction using English (left). No learning occurred from television
or audiotaped presentations (middle). Data for age-matched Chinese and American infants learning their native languages are shown for
comparison (right) (adapted from Kuhl et al., 2003).

phonemes (Conboy & Kuhl, 2011) but also Spanish words et al., 2005a; Kuhl, 2007). The broader role of sociocultural
they were exposed to during the language-exposure sessions context on language learning is also illustrated in studies
(Conboy & Kuhl, 2010). Moreover, our work demonstrates focusing on language and brain in children from families with
that individual differences in infants’ social behaviors during low SES (see Raizada, Richards, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2008;
the Spanish exposure sessions is significantly correlated to the Stevens, Lauinger, & Neville, 2009).
degree to which infants learn both phonemes and words, as The model we have developed indicates that perhaps it
indicated by the relationship between social behaviors during is the interaction between computational skills and social
the sessions and brain measures documenting learning post- cognition that opens plasticity for language learning. Infants
exposure (Conboy, Brooks, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, submitted). have computational skills from birth (Teinonen et al., 2009).
These studies show that infants’ computational abilities The fact that effects of linguistic experience on phonetic
are enabled by social interaction, a situation mirrored in perception are not observed until 8 months of age is of interest
neurobiological studies on vocal communication learning in to theory. We speculated that infants might require 8 months
other species, such as birds (Doupe & Kuhl, 2008). The of listening to build up reliable statistical distributions of the
notion that social interaction acts as a ‘‘gate’’ for infants initial sounds contained in the language they experienced. This idea
language learning has important implications for children with in fact was the impetus for the Kuhl et al. (2003) investigation,
autism that we are beginning to explore (see Kuhl, 2011; Kuhl which examined whether infants could learn from first-time

Volume 5—Number 3 135


Early Language Learning and Literacy

foreign-language exposure at 9 months of age. Our results Only a few studies have addressed the timing of the per-
verified that 9-month-old infants did not require 8 months of ceptual transition in bilingual infants and results have been
listening to learn from experiencing a new language—they mixed, perhaps due to differences in methodology, differences
learned after less than 5 hr of exposure to a new language, but in the amount of language exposure to the two languages
only in a social context. in individual bilingual participants, and the specific char-
These data raise the prospect that infants’ social skills acteristics of the languages and speech contrasts studied.
which develop at about this time—their ability to track Some studies have reported that bilingual infants show differ-
eye movements, achieve joint visual attention, and begin ent developmental patterns when compared to monolingual
to understand others’ communicative intentions—serve as infants. Bosch and Sebastián-Gallés (2003a) compared 4-, 8-
a plasticity trigger. Social understanding might be the ‘‘gate’’ and 12-month-old infants from Spanish monolingual house-
that initiates plasticity for phonetic learning in human infants holds, Catalan monolingual households, and Spanish-Catalan
(Kuhl, 2011). There is a neurobiological precedent for a social bilingual households on a vowel contrast that is phonemic in
interaction plasticity trigger in songbirds: it is well established Catalan but not in Spanish (/ε/ vs. /e/). Their results showed
that a more natural social setting prompts learning and that that 4-month-old infants discriminated the vowel contrast
manipulating social factors can either shorten or extend the but that at 8 months of age only infants exposed to Catalan
optimum period for learning (Knudsen, 2004; Woolley & succeeded. Interestingly, the same group of bilinguals regained
Doupe, 2008). The possibility of a social interaction plasticity their ability to discriminate the speech contrast at 12 months
trigger in humans raises many new questions, and also has of age. The authors reported the same developmental pat-
implications for developmental disabilities (see Kuhl, 2011 for tern in bilingual infants in a study of consonants (Bosch &
discussion). Sebastián-Gallés, 2003b) and interpreted the results as evi-
dence that different processes may underlie bilingual versus
monolingual phoneme category formation (at least for speech
Bilingual Language Learning sounds with different distributional properties in each of the
According to our modeling, bilingual language learners would two languages). Sebastián-Gallés and Bosch (2009) repro-
be expected to follow the same principles as monolingual duced this pattern of results in two vowels (/o/ vs. /u/), which
learners—both computational and social factors influence the are common to and contrastive in both languages. However,
period of plasticity. Nonetheless, we argue that this process Sebastián-Gallés and Bosch also tested bilingual and mono-
might result in a developmental transition that occurs at a lingual Spanish/Catalan infants in their ability to discriminate
later point in time for bilingual infants than for monolingual a second pair of vowels that is common to and contrastive in
infants learning either language. We have argued that infants both languages, but acoustically more salient (i.e., /e/ vs. /u/).
learning two first languages simultaneously would remain Eight-month-old bilinguals were able to discriminate this
‘‘open’’ to experience for a longer period of time because they acoustically distant contrast. The authors interpreted these
are mapping language input in two forms, each with distinct data as supporting the idea that differences may exist in mono-
statistical distributions. Social experience often links the lingual and bilingual phonetic development and that factors
statistical distributions for particular languages to individual in addition to the distributional frequency of phonetic units
social partners, and thus perhaps assists infants in separating in language input, such as lexical similarity, may play an
the statistics of one language from another. If this reasoning is important role.
correct, a longer period of time may be required to begin Other investigations have found that bilingual infants
to close the critical period in bilinguals because infants discriminate phonetic contrasts in their native languages
must receive sufficient data from both languages to reach on the same timetable as monolingual infants. For example,
distributional stability. This in turn depends on factors such Burns, Yoshida, Hill, and Werker (2007) tested consonant
as the number of people in the infant’s environment producing discrimination using English and French sounds at 6–8, 10–12,
the two languages in speech directed toward the child, and and 14–20 months in English monolingual and English–French
the amount of input each speaker in the infant’s environment bilingual infants. As expected, 6- to 8-month-old English
provides. It would be highly adaptive for bilingual infants monolingual infants discriminated both contrasts, whereas 10-
to remain perceptually ‘‘open’’ for a longer period of time. to 12- and 14- to 20-month-old English monolingual infants
Social interaction would play a role as well, in that people discriminated only the English contrast. In bilingual infants, all
in the bilingual child’s home often speak in their preferred age groups were able to discriminate both contrasts. Sundara,
language. This social information may help infants assign the Polka, and Molnar (2008; see also Sundara & Scutellaro, in
statistical properties of language input to different languages. press) produced similar findings.
If an infant’s mother speaks English and the child’s father We conducted the first longitudinal study of English–Span-
speaks Japanese, social information could allow the child to ish bilingual infants combining a brain measure of discrimina-
mentally separate the properties of the two languages. tion for phonetic contrasts in both languages (ERPs) in 6- to

136 Volume 5—Number 3


Patricia K. Kuhl

9- and 10- to 12-month-old infants, with concurrent measures LANGUAGE, COGNITION, AND BILINGUAL LANGUAGE
of language input to the child in the home, and follow-up EXPERIENCE
examination of word production in both languages months
later (Garcia-Sierra et al., in press). The study addressed three Specific cognitive abilities, particularly the executive control
questions: Do bilingual infants show the ERP components of attention and the ability to inhibit a pre-potent response
indicative of neural discrimination for the phonetic units of (cognitive control), are affected by language experience. Bilin-
both languages on the same timetable as monolingual infants? gual adult speakers are more cognitively flexible given novel
Is there a relationship between brain measures of phonetic problems to solve (Bialystok, 1999, 2001; Bialystok & Hakuta,
discrimination and the amount of exposure to the two lan- 1994; Wang, Kuhl, Chen, & Dong, 2009), as are young school-
guages? Is later word production in the infants’ two languages aged bilingual children (Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008). We have
predicted by early ERP responses to speech sounds in both recent evidence that cognitive flexibility is also increased in
languages, and/or the amount of early language exposure to infants and young children who have short-term or long-
each of the two languages. term exposure to more than one language. In the short-term
As predicted, bilingual infants displayed a developmental Spanish exposure studies described earlier, a median split
pattern distinct from that of monolingual infants previously of infants based on their post-exposure brain measures of
tested using the same stimuli and methods (Rivera-Gaxiola, Spanish-language learning revealed that the best learners (top
Klarman, Garcia-Sierra, & Kuhl, 2005a; Rivera-Gaxiola et al., half of the distribution) have post-exposure cognitive flexi-
2005b). Our studies indicated that bilingual infants show bility scores that are significantly higher than infants whose
patterns of neural discrimination for their (two) native Spanish-language learning scores put them in the lowest half
languages at a later point in time when compared to of the distribution (Conboy, Sommerville, & Kuhl, 2008). In a
monolingual infants (Garcia-Sierra et al., in press). The data second experiment, we linked brain measures of word recog-
suggest that bilingual infants remain ‘‘open’’ to language nition in both languages to cognitive flexibility scores in 24- to
experience longer than monolingual infants, a response that 29-month-old bilingual children—those with enhanced brain
is highly adaptive to the increased variability in the language measures in response to words in both their languages show
input they experience. the highest cognitive flexibility scores (Conboy et al., 2011).
We also showed that neural discrimination of English These data provide further evidence that experience shapes
and Spanish in these bilingual infants was related to the the brain; bilingual adults and children have advanced skills
amount of exposure to each language in the home—infants when coping with tasks that require the ability to ‘‘reverse
who heard more Spanish at home had larger brain responses the rules’’ and think flexibly. These skills could aid various
to Spanish sounds, and the reverse was found in infants forms of learning in school, and educators should be aware of
who heard more English at home. Finally, we hypothesized a them. At the same time, the cognitive flexibility induced by
relationship between the early brain measures and later word experience with two languages may play a role in bilingual
production, as well as relationships between early language children remaining open to language experience longer in
exposure and later word production. Both hypotheses were development, which changes the timetable of learning.
confirmed. Children who were English dominant in word Further research is needed to examine in detail the implica-
production at 15 months had shown relatively better early tions for bilingual language learning and literacy—studies on
neural discrimination of the English contrast, as well as bilingual learning have just begun and will be a strong focus
stronger early English exposure in the home. Similarly, children for neuroscience and education in the next decade.
who were Spanish dominant in word production at 15 months
had earlier shown relatively better neural discrimination
of the Spanish contrast and stronger Spanish exposure in EARLY LANGUAGE LEARNING PREDICTS LATER
the home. LANGUAGE SKILLS
Taken as a whole, the results suggest that bilingual
infants tested with phonetic units from both of their native Early language learning is a complex process. Our working
languages stay perceptually ‘‘open’’ longer than monolingual hypothesis is the following: Infants computational skills,
infants—indicating perceptual narrowing at a later point in modulated by social interaction, open a window of increased
time, which is highly adaptive for bilingual infants. The results plasticity at about 8 months of life. Between 8- and 10-month-
reinforce the view that experience alters the course of brain old monolingual infants show an increase in native-language
structure and function early in development. We also show phonetic perception, a decrease in non-native phonetic
that individual differences in infants’ neural responses to perception, and remain open to phonetic learning from a
speech, as well as their later word production, are influenced new language that can be induced by social experience with
by the amount of exposure infants have to each of their native a speaker of that language (although not via a standard TV
languages at home. experience). The complexity of learning in this early phase

Volume 5—Number 3 137


Early Language Learning and Literacy

is not trivial, and that complexity might explain why our that infants showed excellent skills in detecting phonetic
laboratory studies show wide individual differences in the differences in native-language sounds, the better their later
early phonetic transition. An important question, especially for performance in measures of language and pre-literacy skills
practice, was suggested by these data: Is an individual child’s (Cardillo Lebedeva & Kuhl, 2009).
success at this early transition toward language indicative of These results are theoretically interesting and also highly
future language skills or literacy? relevant to early learning practice. These data show that
We began studies to determine whether the variability the initial steps that infants take toward language learning
observed in measures of early phonetic learning predicted are important to their development of language and literacy
children’s language skills measured at later points in years later. Our data suggest as well that these early
development. We recognized that it was possible that the differences in performance are strongly related to experience.
variability we observed was simply ‘‘noise,’’ in other words, Our studies reveal that these early measures of speech
random variation in a child’s skill on the particular day discrimination, which predict future language and literacy,
that we measured that child in the laboratory. We were are strongly correlated to experience with ‘‘motherese’’
therefore pleased when our first studies demonstrated that early in development (Liu, Kuhl, & Tsao, 2003). Motherese
infants’ discrimination of two simple vowels at 6 months of exaggerates the critical acoustic cues in speech (Kuhl et al.,
age was significantly correlated with their language skills 1997; Werker et al., 2007), and infants’ social interest in speech
at 13, 16, and 24 months of age (Tsao, Liu, & Kuhl, 2004). is, we believe, important to the social learning process. Thus,
Later studies confirmed the connection between early speech talking to children early in life, reading to them early in life,
perception and later language skills using both brain (Rivera- and interacting socially with children around language and
Gaxiola et al., 2005a; Kuhl et al., 2008) and behavioral (Kuhl literacy activities creates the milieu in which plasticity during
et al., 2005b) measures on monolingual infants, and with the critical period can be maximized for all children.
bilingual infants using brain measures (Garcia-Sierra et al., in There is increasing evidence that children raised in families
press). Other laboratories also produced data that indicated with lower SES show deficits in language measured either
strong links between the speed of speech processing and later behaviorally or in brain studies (for extensive review, see
language function (Fernald, Perfors, & Marchman, 2006) and Raizada & Kishiyama, 2010). In one of the first studies
between various measures of statistical learning and later of 5-year-old children combining behavioral and brain
language measures (Newman, Ratner, Jusczyk, Jusczyk, & measures, Raizada et al. (2008) examined the associations
Dow, 2006). between standardized test scores of language, social cognition,
Recent data from our laboratory indicate long-term intelligence, SES, and fMRI-measured brain activity as the
associations between early measures of infants’ phonetic 5-year-old children worked on a rhyming task. The results
perception and future language and reading skills. The new showed correlations between SES, language performance, and
work measures vowel perception at 7 and 11 months and shows the degree of hemispheric specialization in Broca’s area, as
that the trajectory of learning between those two ages predicts measured by left-minus-right fMRI activation (Figure 6). The
the children’s language abilities and pre-literacy skills at the SES-Broca’s link remained highly significant after the effects of
age of 5 years—the association holds regardless of SES, as well the language scores were removed, indicating the relationship
as the level of children’s language skills at 18 and 24 months of cannot be attributed to both measures’ correlations with the
age (Cardillo Lebedeva & Kuhl, 2009). language scores. The study shows a correlational link, which
Infants tested at 7 and 11 months of age show three patterns of course we cannot assume to be causal.
of speech perception development: (1) infants who show The authors concluded that fMRI is a more sensitive
excellent native discrimination at 7 months and maintain measure of the development of Broca’s area than any of the
that ability at 11 months, the high–high group, (2) infants who behavioral tests; each behavioral score is a compound function
show poor abilities at 7 months but excellent performance of perception, cognition, attention, and motor control, whereas
at 11 months, the low–high group, and (3) infants who show fMRI probes Broca’s more directly. Thus, neuroimaging
poor abilities to discriminate at both 7 and 11 months of age, studies, especially early in development, may be able to provide
the low–low group. We followed these children until the age us with highly sensitive measures of competence.
of 5, assessing language skills at 18 months, 24 months, and We assumed that SES is not itself the variable driving
5 years of age. Strong relationships were observed between these effects on the brain—SES is likely a proxy for the
infants’ early speech perception performance and their later opportunity to learn. We learned in a follow-up study that
language skills at 18 and 24 months. At 5 years of age, SES could be removed from the equation if language input
significant relationships were shown between infants’ early itself was measured. The complexity of language input is the
speech perception performance and both their language skills more direct factor influencing development of brain areas that
and the phonological awareness skills associated with success code language. When measures of the complexity of maternal
in learning to read. In all cases, the earlier in development language were assessed across the entire sample of children in

138 Volume 5—Number 3


Patricia K. Kuhl

(a) (b)

Fig. 6. Relationship between left hemisphere specialization in Broca’s area (a) and SES in 5-year-old children (b) (from Raizada et al., 2008).

the study, we observed a correlation with structural measures and previous work has shown that social interaction can
of the brain in Broca’s area. These measures indicated that extend the ‘‘critical period’’ for learning in birds (Brainard
greater gray matter in the left hemisphere language areas was & Knudsen, 1998). Social factors play a role in learning
related to the complexity of maternal language in conversations throughout life. The prevalence, across age, of new social
between the mothers and their 5-year-old children. technologies (text messaging, Facebook, Twitter) indicates
In summary, our results suggested that language input to the a strong human drive for social communication. Technology
child—its complexity and diversity—was the factor affecting used for teaching is increasingly embodying the principles
brain development in the language areas, not SES per se. The of social interaction to enhance student learning (Koedinger
implication is that children’s brains literally depend on input & Aleven, 2007). A true understanding of the nature of the
for development. Although these results are correlational, we relationship between social interaction and learning requires
believe that the connection between experience with language more research, and the NSF-funded LIFE Center is focused
and brain development is potentially causal and that further on this question across domains and ages (see LIFE Web site:
research will allow us to develop causal explanations. http://life-slc.org).
The fact that young children use implicit and automatic
mechanisms to learn and that children’s brains are influenced
DISCUSSION by the ambient information they experience is a sobering
thought. Opportunities for learning of the right kind must
There are two important implications of these data. The first be available for children to maximize the unique learning
is that early language learning is highly social. Children do not abilities that their brains permit during the early period of
compute statistics indiscriminately. Social cues ‘‘gate’’ what life. Scientists of course do not yet know how much or what
and when children learn from language input. Machines are kinds of experience are necessary for learning—we cannot
not sufficient as instructors, at least in the early period and write an exact prescription for success. Nonetheless, the data
when standard machines such as television sets are used as on language and literacy indicate a potent and necessary
the instructor. Further studies are needed to test whether role for ample early experience, in social settings, in which
our work suggesting that language learning must be social complex language is used to encourage children to express
to ‘‘stick’’ applies to other learning domains—must cognitive themselves and explore the world of books. Further data will
learning or learning about numbers be social? refine these conclusions and allow us to develop concrete
Studies will also be needed to determine the ages at recommendations that will enhance the probability that all
which the social context conclusion applies—infants are children the world over maximize their brain development
predisposed to attend to people and do not learn from and learning.
TVs, but that conclusion does not apply to older children,
teenagers, or adult learners. What is it about social learning Acknowledgments —This study was supported by a grant
that is so important to the young? Animal models show from the National Science Foundation’s Science of Learning
that neurosteroids modulate brain activity during social Program to the LIFE Center (SBE-0354453), and by grants
interactions (Remage-Healey, Maidment, & Schlinger, 2008) from the National Institutes of Health (HD37954, HD55782,

Volume 5—Number 3 139


Early Language Learning and Literacy

HD02274, DC04661). The review updates information in Kuhl Cardillo Lebedeva, G. C., & Kuhl, P. K. (2009). Individual differences
(2010). in infant speech perception predict language and pre-reading
skills through age 5 years. Paper presented at the Annual
Meeting of the Society for Developmental & Behavioral
REFERENCES Pediatrics. Portland, OR.
Cheour, M., Imada, T., Taulu, S., Ahonen, A., Salonen, J., & Kuhl, P. K.
Abramson, A. S., & Lisker, L. (1970). Discriminability along the (2004). Magnetoencephalography is feasible for infant assess-
voicing continuum: Cross-language tests. Proceedings of the ment of auditory discrimination. Experimental Neurology, 190,
International Congress of Phonetics Science, 6, 569–573. S44–S51.
Aslin, R. N., & Mehler, J. (2005). Near-infrared spectroscopy for Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Estes, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2011, May).
functional studies of brain activity in human infants: Promise, ERPs to words in toddlers with ASD predict behavioral measures
prospects, and challenges. Journal of Biomedical Optics, 10, 11009. at 6 years of age. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the
Best, C. C., & McRoberts, G. W. (2003). Infant perception of non- International Meeting for Autism Research, San Diego, CA.
native consonant contrasts that adults assimilate in different Conboy, B. T., Brooks, R., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (submitted).
ways. Language and Speech, 46, 183–216. Infants’ social behaviors predict phonetic learning.
Bialystok, E. (1991). Letters, sounds, and symbols: Changes in chil- Conboy, B. T., & Kuhl, P. K. (2010, November). Brain responses
dren’s understanding of written language. AppliedPsycholinguistics, to words in 11-month-olds after second language exposure.
12, 75–89. Poster presented at the American Speech-Language-Hearing
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in Association, Philadelphia, PA.
the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70, 636–644. Conboy, B. T., & Kuhl, P. K. (2011). Impact of second-language
Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and experience in infancy: Brain measures of first- and second-
cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press. language speech perception. Developmental Science, 14, 242–248.
Bialystok, E., & Hakuta, K. (1994). In other words: The science and Conboy, B. T., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Silva-Pereyra, J., & Kuhl, P. K.
psychology of second-language acquisition. New York: Basic Books. (2008). Event-related potential studies of early language
Birdsong, D. (1992). Ultimate attainment in second language processing at the phoneme, word, and sentence levels. In
acquisition. Language, 68, 706–755. A. D. Friederici & G. Thierry (Eds.), Early language development:
Birdsong, D., & Molis, M. (2001). On the evidence for maturational Vol. 5. Bridging brain and behavior, trends in language acquisition research
constraints in second-language acquisitions. Journal of Memory (pp. 23–64). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.
and Language, 44, 235–249. Conboy, B. T., Sommerville, J., & Kuhl, P. K. (2008). Cognitive
de Boer, B., & Kuhl, P. K. (2003). Investigating the role of infant- control skills and speech perception after short term second
directed speech with a computer model. ARLO, 4, 129–134. language experience during infancy. Journal of the Acoustical Society
Bortfeld, H., Wruck, E., & Boas, D. A. (2007). Assessing infants’ of America, 123, 3581.
cortical response to speech using near-infrared spectroscopy. Conboy, B. T., Sommerville, J. A., Wicha, N., Romo, H. D., & Kuhl,
NeuroImage, 34, 407–415. P. K. (2011, March–April) Word processing and cognitive
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003a). Simultaneous bilingualism control skills in two-year-old bilingual children. Symposium
and the perception of a language-specific vowel contrast in the presentation at the 2011 Biennial Meeting for the Society for
first year of life. Language and Speech, 46, 217–243. Research in Child Development, Montreal, Canada.
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003b). Language experience and Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dehaene, S., & Hertz-Pannier, L. (2002).
the perception of a voicing contrast in fricatives: Infant and adult Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science,
data. In D. Recasens, M. J. Solé, & J. Romero (Eds.), Proceedings 298, 2013–2015
of the 15th international conference of phonetic sciences (pp. 1987–1990). Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Hertz-Pannier, L., Dubois, J., Meriaux, S.,
Barcelona, Spain: UAB/Casual Prods. Roche, A., Sigman, M., et al. (2006). Functional organization
de Boysson-Bardies, B. (1993). Ontogeny of language-specific syllabic of perisylvian activation during presentation of sentences in
productions. In B. de Boysson-Bardies, S. de Schonen, P. Jusczyk, preverbal infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
P. McNeilage, & J. Morton (Eds.), Developmental neurocognition: (USA), 103, 14240–14245.
Speech and face processing in the first year of life (pp. 353–363). Doupe, A. J., & Kuhl, P. K. (2008). Birdsong and human speech:
Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer. Common themes and mechanisms. In H. P. Zeigler & P. Marler
Brainard, M. S., & Knudsen, E. I. (1998). Sensitive periods for visual (Eds.), Neuroscience of birdsong (pp. 5–31). Cambridge, UK:
calibration of the auditory space map in the barn owl optic Cambridge University Press.
tectum. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 3929–3942. Eimas, P. D. (1975). Auditory and phonetic coding of the cues for
Bruer, J. T. (2008). Critical periods in second language learning: speech: Discrimination of the /r–l/ distinction by young infants.
Distinguishing phenomena from explanation. In M. Mody & Perception and Psychophysics, 18, 341–347.
E. Silliman (Eds.), Brain, behavior and learning in language and reading Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech
disorders (pp. 72–96). New York: Guilford. perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306.
Burns, T. C., Yoshida, K. A., Hill, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). The Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (Eds.). (1992).
development of phonetic representation in bilingual and Phonological development: Models, research, implications. Timonium,
monolingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 455–474. MD: York Press.
Carlson S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006) Picking up speed
executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary
282–298. growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98–116.

140 Volume 5—Number 3


Patricia K. Kuhl

Flege, J. (1991). Age of learning affects the authenticity of voice onset Kuhl, P. K., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., & Dawson, G. (2005a).
time (VOT) in stop consonants produced in a second language. Links between social and linguistic processing of speech
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 89, 395–411. in preschool children with autism: Behavioral and electro-
Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., & Liu, S. (1999). Age constraints physiological evidence. Developmental Science, 8, 1–12.
on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-
41, 78–104. Gaxiola, M., & Nelson, T. (2008). Phonetic learning as a
Friederici, A. D. (2005). Neurophysiological markers of early language pathway to language: New data and native language magnet
acquisition: From syllables to sentences. TrendsinCognitiveScience, theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
9, 481–488. Society B: Biological Sciences, 363, 979–1000.
Garcia-Sierra, A., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Conboy, B. T., Romo, H., Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Padden, D., Nelson, T., & Pruitt, J. (2005b).
Percaccio, C. R., Klarman, L., et al. (in press). Bilingual language Early speech perception and later language development:
learning: An ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, Implications for the ‘critical period.’ Language Learning and
language input, and later word production. Journal of Phonetics. Development, 1, 237–264.
Gernsbacher, M. A., & Kaschak, M. P. (2003). Neuroimaging studies Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1982). The bimodal perception of
of language production and comprehension. Annual Review of speech in infancy. Science, 218, 1138–1141.
Psychology, 54, 91–114. Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). Infant vocalizations in response
Golestani, N., Molko, N., Dehaen, S., LeBihan, D., & Pallier, C. (2007). to speech: Vocal imitation and developmental change. Journal of
Brain structure predicts the learning of foreign speech sounds. the Acoustical Society of America, 100, 2425–2438.
Cerebral Cortex, 17, 575–582. Kuhl, P. K., & Rivera-Gaxiola, M. (2008). Neural substrates of
Gratton, G., & Fabiani, M. (2001a). Shedding light on brain function: language acquisition. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 511–534.
The event related optical signal. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S., &
357–363. Iverson, P. (2006). Infants show facilitation for native language
Gratton, G., & Fabiani, M. (2001b). The event-related optical signal: phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental
A new tool for studying brain function. International Journal of Science, 9, 13–21.
Psychophysiology, 42, 109–121. Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F.-M., & Liu, H.-M. (2003). Foreign-language
Hari, R., & Kujala, M. (2009). Brain basis of human social interaction: experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and
From concepts to brain imaging. Physiological Reviews, 89, social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National
453–479. Academy of Sciences (USA), 100, 9096–9101.
Homae, F., Watanabe, H., Nakano, T., Asakawa, K., & Taga, G. Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N., & Lind-
(2006). The right hemisphere of sleeping infant perceives blom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception
sentential prosody. Neuroscience Research, 54, 276–280. in infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255, 606–608.
Imada, T., Zhang, Y., Cheour, M., Taulu, S., Ahonen, A., & Kuhl, P. K. Ladefoged, P. (2001). Vowels and consonants: An introduction to the sounds
(2006). Infant speech perception activates Broca’s area: A of language. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
developmental magnetoenceohalography study. NeuroReport, 17, Lasky, R. E., Syrdal-Lasky, A., & Klein, R. E. (1975). VOT discrim-
957–962. ination by four to six and a half month old infants from
Johnson, J., & Newport, E. (1989). Critical period effects in second Spanish environments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 20,
language learning: The influence of maturation state on the 215–225.
acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biologicalfoundationsoflanguage. New York: Wiley.
21, 60–99. Liu, H.-M., Kuhl, P. K., & Tsao, F.-M. (2003). An association between
Knudsen, E. I. (2004). Sensitive periods in the development of the mothers’ speech clarity and infants’ speech discrimination skills.
brain and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1412–1225. Developmental Science, 6, F1–F10.
Koedinger, K. R., & Aleven, V. (2007). Exploring the assistance Lotto, A. J., Sato, M., & Diehl, R. (2004). Mapping the task for the
dilemma in experiments with cognitive tutors. Educational second language learner: The case of Japanese acquisition of /r/
Psychology Review, 19, 239–264. and /l/. In J. Slitka, S. Manuel, & M. Matties (Eds.), From sound
Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech to sense (pp. C181–C186). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 831–843. Mayberry, R. I., & Lock, E. (2003). Age constraints on first versus
Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain? second language acquisition: Evidence for linguistic plasticity
Developmental Science, 10, 110–120. and epigenesis. Brain and Language, 87, 369–384.
Kuhl, P. K. (2010, October). The linguistic genius of babies. A Maye, J., Weiss, D., & Aslin, R. (2008). Statistical learning in infants:
TED Talk retrieved from TED.com website: http://www. Facilitation and feature generalization. Developmental Science, 11,
ted.com/talks/lang/eng/patricia_kuhl_the_linguistic_genius_of_ 122–134.
babies.html Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to
Kuhl, P. K. (2011). Social mechanisms in early language acquisition: distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination.
Understanding integrated brain systems supporting language. Cognition, 82, B101–B111.
In J. Decety & J. Cacioppo (Eds.), Thehandbookofsocialneuroscience Meltzoff, A. N., Kuhl, P. K., Movellan, J., & Sejnowski, T. (2009).
(pp. 649–667). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Foundations for a new science of learning. Science, 17, 284–288.
Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Neville, H. J., Coffey, S. A., Lawson, D. S., Fischer, A., Emmorey, K.,
Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, V. L., et al. (1997). Cross- & Bellugi, U. (1997). Neural systems mediating American Sign
language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to Language: Effects of sensory experience and age of acquisition.
infants. Science, 277, 684–686. Brain and Language, 57, 285–308.

Volume 5—Number 3 141


Early Language Learning and Literacy

Newman, R., Ratner, N. B., Jusczyk, A. M., Jusczyk, P. W., & Teinonen, T., Fellman, V., Naatanen, R., Alku, P., & Huoti-
Dow, K. A. (2006). Infants’ early ability to segment the con- lainen, M. (2009). Statistical language learning in neonates
versational speech signal predicts later language development: revealed by event-related brain potentials. BMC Neuroscience,
A retrospective analysis. Developmental Psychology, 42, 643–655. 10, 21.
Newport, E. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Travis, K. E., Leonard, M. K., Brown, T. T., Hagler, D. J., Jr., Curran, M.,
Cognitive Science, 14, 11–28. Dale, A. M., et al. (in press). Spaciotemporal neural dynamics of
Newport, E. L., Bavelier, D., & Neville, H. J. (2001). Critical think- word understanding in 12- to 18-month-old-infants. Cerebral
ing about critical periods: Perspectives on a critical period for Cortex.
language acquisition. In E. Dupoux (Ed.), Language, brain, and Tsao, F.-M., Liu, H.-M., & Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Speech perception in
cognitive development: Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler (pp. 481–502). infancy predicts language development in the second year of life:
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. A longitudinal study. Child Development, 75, 1067–1084.
Ortiz-Mantilla, S., Choe, M. S., Flax, J., Grant, P. E., & Benasich, A. A. Tsao, F.-M., Liu, H.-M., & Kuhl, P. K. (2006). Perception of native and
(2010). Association between the size of the amygdala in infancy non-native affricate-fricative contrasts: Cross-language tests on
and language abilities during the preschool years in normally adults and infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120,
developing children. NeuroImage, 49, 2791–2799. 2285–2294.
Peña, M., Bonatti, L., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (2002). Signal-driven Vallabha, G. K., McClelland, J. L., Pons, F., Werker, J.F., & Amano, S.
computations in speech processing. Science, 298, 604–607. (2007). Unsupervised learning of vowel categories from infant-
Rabiner, L. R., & Huang, B. H. (1993). Fundamentals of speech recognition. directed speech. ProceedingsoftheNationalAcademyofSciences(USA),
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. 104, 13273–13278.
Remage-Healey, L., Maidment, N. T., & Schlinger, B. A. (2008). Fore- Wang, Y., Kuhl, P. K., Chen, C., & Dong, Q. (2009). Sustained and
brain steroid levels fluctuate rapidly during social interactions. transient language control in the bilingual brain. NeuroImage, 47,
Nature Neuroscience, 11, 1327–1334. 414–422.
Raizada, R. D. S., Richards, T. L., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. Weber-Fox, C. M., & Neville, H. J. (1999). Functional neural subsys-
(2008). Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric special- tems are differentially affected by delays in second language
ization of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. immersion: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilinguals. In
NeuroImage, 40, 1392–1401. D. Birdsong (Ed.), Second language acquisition and the critical period
Raizada, R. D. S., & Kishiyama, M. (2010). Effects of socioeconomic hypothesis (pp. 23–38). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
status on brain development, and how cognitive neuroscience Werker, J. F., & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental frame-
may contribute to levelling the playing field. Frontiers in Human work of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Develop-
Neuroscience, 4, 3. ment, 1, 197–234.
Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Klarman, L., Garcia-Sierra, A., & Kuhl, P. K. Werker, J. F., & Lalonde, C. (1988). Cross-language speech percep-
(2005a). Neural patterns to speech and vocabulary growth tion: Initial capabilities and developmental change. Developmental
in American infants. NeuroReport, 16, 495–498. Psychology, 24, 672–683.
Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Silvia-Pereyra, J., & Kuhl, P. K. (2005b). Brain Werker, J. F., Pons, F., Dietrich, C., Kajikawa, S., Fais, L., &
potentials to native and non-native speech contrasts in 7- and Amano, S. (2007). Infant-directed speech supports phonetic
11-month-old American infants. Developmental Science, 8, 162–172. category learning in English and Japanese. Cognition, 103,
Saffran, J., Aslin, R., & Newport, E. (1996). Statistical learning by 147–162.
8-month old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928. Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech percep-
Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Bosch, L. (2009). Developmental shift in the tion: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year
discrimination of vowel contrasts in bilingual infants: Is the of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49–63.
distributional account all there is to it? Developmental Science, 12, White, L., & Genesee, F. (1996). How native is near-native? The issue
874–887. of ultimate attainment in adult second language acquisition.
Snow, C. E., & Hoefnagel-Hohle, M. (1978). The critical period for Second Language Research, 12, 233–265.
language acquisition: Evidence from second language learning. Woolley, S. C., & Doupe, A. J. (2008). Social context-induced song
Child Development, 49, 1114–1128. variation affects female behavior and gene expression. Public
Stevens, C., Lauinger, B., & Neville, H. (2009). Differences in the neu- Library of Science Biology, 6, e62.
ral mechanisms of selective attention in children from different Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Flege, J. E., & Liu, S. (2000). Pronuncia-
socioeconomic backgrounds: An event-related brain potential tion proficiency in the first and second languages of Kore-
study. Developmental Science, 12, 634–646. an–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3,
Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Molnar, M. (2008). Development of coro- 131–149.
nal stop perception: Bilingual infants keep pace with their Zhang, Y., Kuhl, P. K., Imada, T., Iverson, P., Pruitt, J., Stevens,
monolingual peers. Cognition, 108, 232–242. E. B., et al. (2009). Neural signatures of phonetic learning in
Sundara, M., & Scutellaro, A. (in press). Rhythmic distance between adulthood: A magnetoencephalography study. NeuroImage, 46,
languages affects the development of speech perception in bilin- 226–240.
gual infants. Journal of Phonetics. Zhang, Y., Kuhl, P. K., Imada, T., Kotani, M., & Tohkura, Y. (2005).
Taga, G., & Asakawa, K. (2007). Selectivity and localization of corti- Effects of language experience: Neural commitment to language-
cal response to auditory and visual stimulation in awake infants specific auditory patterns. NeuroImage, 26, 703–720.
aged 2 to 4 months. NeuroImage, 36, 1246–1252.

142 Volume 5—Number 3

You might also like