You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/265905729

Interrelations of language and cognitive development

Chapter · January 2014

CITATIONS READS
27 10,853

1 author:

Gedeon O Deák
University of California, San Diego
81 PUBLICATIONS   1,997 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

'Your surprise or mine?' EEG signatures of prediction error and outcome value in a dyadic turn-taking game View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Gedeon O Deák on 22 September 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Copyright © 2014 SAGE Publications. Not for sale, reproduction, or distribution.

284 Interrelationship of Language and Cognitive Development (Overview)

Pinker, Steven. Learnability and Cognition. Cambridge, psychologists, and educators: How does language affect
MA: MIT Press, 1989. cognition? How, for example, does language processing
Quine, Willard V. O. Word and Object. Cambridge, MA: facilitate attention, learning, memory, and reasoning?
MIT Press, 1960. Both questions raise an ancillary question about
whether specific cognitive or learning abilities evolved
on the coattails of language evolution or whether lan-
guage emerged as a coevolutionary by-product of
Interrelationship of hominin cognitive capacities and social structures. The
former implies that some general cognitive abilities,
Language and Cognitive such as music, are evolutionarily subordinate to lan-
Development (Overview) guage ability. The latter suggests that language, music,
mathematics, and writing systems are diverse products
For decades, researchers have debated and investi- of a set of cognitive and sociocultural traits common to
gated the relationship between language and cog- humans. However, this question is a subject of specula-
nitive development, especially in infancy and early tion and not amenable to direct investigation.
childhood. Modular perspectives posit that language The following sections review, first, how cognitive
development is controlled by specialized mecha- capacities relate to language development (broadly
nisms, much like the olfactory system evolved to construed) and, second, how language development
detect, learn, and process airborne particles. In this supports learning and cognition.
perspective, language learning might be quite inde-
pendent of other cognitive abilities. By contrast, con- Theoretical and Historical Trend
structivist and biologically based perspectives tend Linguistics from the 1950s to the 1980s mostly fol-
to emphasize the progressive, experience-dependent lowed Noam Chomsky’s assertion that language is
emergence of complex skills, including language. a specialized or modular faculty. This hegemony,
These theories postulate that domain-general cogni- though still represented by some child-language
tive capacities and processes are recruited to develop researchers, has gradually yielded to evidence that lan-
language. The frameworks make distinct predictions: guage processing is cognitive processing of language
Modular theories expect language-specific learning information and that language learning is continuous
processes and products. Constructivist and neuro- with learning of other sorts of information, such as
constructivist approaches expect language-learning gestures or sound patterns. There remains consider-
processes and products to show deep commonalities able debate about what sorts of processes might be
with nonlinguistic learning. specialized for language and how. Cases of children
A profound challenge in adjudicating between showing early sensitivity to nonobvious syntactic or
these views is that many capacities and skills change phonological constraints, with no apparent nonlin-
with age: Perceptual sensitivities change with practice, guistic parallel, support the idea of specialization.
everyday experiences provide a ballooning data set for However, in these cases it is possible that, even if one
inductive inference and pattern detection, and incre- cannot readily identify nonlinguistic analogues of the
mental practice leads to improvement of all sorts of constraint, children could equally well learn invented,
actions and cognitive skills. Another challenge is that logically equivalent, nonlinguistic patterns. This pos-
methods and instruments for measuring linguistic sibility is seldom tested, however.
and nonlinguistic cognitive skills are completely dif- Modeling studies have, in recent decades, pro-
ferent between infancy and early childhood and also vided increasing evidence that linguistic patterns
between early childhood and late childhood and ado- and principles are learnable by cognitive agents that
lescence. Thus, behavioral data cannot easily be com- are imbued with only general learning mechanisms.
pared across ages. Nevertheless, there is ample evi- Numerous studies have investigated whether simple
dence of robust relations between language abilities artificial systems, ranging from simple neural net-
and cognitive development, dating from the earliest works to embodied robots, can acquire simulated,
research on child language in the 1970s. simplified systems of quasi-linguistic symbols. The
A distinct but complementary question has been learning mechanisms in these studies represent a
addressed for over a century by anthropologists, variety of approaches including machine learning,

To cite: Deák, G.O. (2014). Interrelations of language and cognitive development.


Encyclopedia of Language Development (pp. 284-291). P. Brooks & V. Kampe, Eds. SAGE
Copyright © 2014 SAGE Publications. Not for sale, reproduction, or distribution.

Interrelationship of Language and Cognitive Development (Overview) 285

simple Hebbian learning, recurrent networks, genetic cortex plays an important role in generating semanti-
algorithms, Bayesian and other probability-based cally appropriate speech, and some right-hemisphere
algorithms, reinforcement learning models, Hid- regions are important for processing pragmatic infor-
den Markov models, and others. These studies have mation. However, perinatal stroke studies also show
contributed to a growing consensus that biologically that these language abilities can become functionally
inspired learning systems can, from limited experi- allocated to atypical cortical areas. Thus, there is some
ence, induce the abstract patterns in language. Such pluripotentiality of cortical tissue for language func-
work challenges the traditional hegemony of linguis- tions, suggesting that developmental learning pro-
tic modularity. However, any simulation must be cesses, not a priori properties of the infant brain, yield
evaluated in terms of (1) the assumptions manifested cerebral organization of language faculties.
in the model, (2) the structure of the quasi-linguistic
input corpus, and (3) the biological and psychological How Language Development Relies on
plausibility of the learning process. Often, these fac- Cognitive Development: Congenital Specificity
tors limit the conclusions that can be drawn from the of Speech–Sound Processing?
results. Nevertheless, some studies have provided pro- It has been hypothesized that human audition is evo-
vocative proof of possibility—that is, results indicat- lutionarily adapted for language. In fact, neonates dis-
ing that a simple, unspecialized, unsupervised system criminate changes in human speech sounds. However,
can readily acquire patterns once believed by linguists neonates also discriminate differences in the pitch,
to be unlearnable without specialized linguistic con- amplitude, and timing of nonspeech sounds. These
straints. acoustic features are important in phoneme discrimi-
nation. It is unclear whether infants are more sensitive
Neural Specialization for Language Learning? to these features in speech sounds than nonspeech
There are expanding efforts to explore how neural sounds. It is true that young infants prefer listening to
resources might become specialized or dedicated to human speech than to nonspeech sounds matched for
language processing. For example, it was initially sug- some basic acoustic properties. The basis of this pref-
gested from electroencephalographic (EEG) evidence erence is unknown, but it might rest partly on prena-
that 1-year-olds’ brains had not yet undergone corti- tal exposure to maternal speech, despite the acoustic
cal regionalization (i.e., specialization of certain areas) filtering of speech through the uterine aqueous envi-
for word knowledge. Most adults show reliable, maxi- ronment. Notably, prenatal auditory learning is not
mal processing of words by parts of the left inferior limited to speech; there is some limited evidence that
frontal and superior anterior temporal cortex. Early neonates respond differently to nonvocal music heard
studies of 1-year-olds suggested that hearing words repeatedly during pregnancy than to novel music.
activated widely distributed, bilateral areas of cortex. Thus, there is no compelling evidence that infants’
However, methods at that time did not permit good earliest auditory responses are specifically adapted to
localization of cortical activity sources from EEG speech stimuli.
data. A more recent study using magnetoencepha-
lography (MEG) revealed left fronto-temporal corti- Early Learning of Speech Patterns
cal specialization for word processing as early as 14 By midway through the first year, infants are sensitive
months. This suggests that whatever processes cause to a variety of native-language speech patterns. These
cortical specialization for word processing begin by include native phonemes (consonants and vowels),
an infant’s first birthday. This does not explain how sequences of phonemes, patterns of word stress, and
regional specialization emerges. However, the cortex prosodic markers of speech boundaries. For example,
in this region is not congenitally (i.e., at birth) special- Thai-learning infants divide bilabial stop consonants
ized for word learning: Infants who lose this region into three phoneme categories based on continuous
of cortex to perinatal (i.e., around birth) stroke can differences in voice onset time (VOT), that is, the time
eventually develop largely normal language, suggest- from vocal fold vibration to exhalation). English-
ing that other cortical tissue is plastic enough to take learning infants, by contrast, divide the VOT con-
over word-learning and word-retrieval functions. tinuum into two categories (/b/ and /p/). Also, Ger-
Adults show cortical specialization for a broader man-learning infants expect words to have a primary
range of language. For example, left inferior parietal (i.e., trochaic) stress pattern rather than a secondary
Copyright © 2014 SAGE Publications. Not for sale, reproduction, or distribution.

286 Interrelationship of Language and Cognitive Development (Overview)

(iambic) pattern; French-learning infants do not have unexpectedness responses than adults who learned
the same expectation. Infants learn many such experi- the language earlier in response to hearing gram-
ence-driven distinctions within the first year. matical errors. This suggests that language process-
The mechanisms by which infants learn these dis- ing becomes faster with practice, even in childhood
tinctions are not well understood. However, there and adolescence. Studies of linguistic plasticity fur-
is some evidence that those mechanisms are not ther support the processing-speed hypothesis. Several
exclusively used for language learning. For example, studies have trained adolescents and adults with read-
although infants can rapidly learn that some pho- ing or language disabilities to perceive basic speech
neme sequences are more likely than others, they can sound distinctions. It is not obvious that phoneme
also learn that some tone sequences (i.e., two-note discrimination training should improve a complex
melodic motifs) or sequences of colored shapes are language process such as reading. Nevertheless, the
more common than others. Also, chinchillas (desert results suggest that phoneme discrimination-speed
rodents with unimpressive linguistic abilities) are, like training improves reading scores more than tradi-
humans, sensitive to phonemic differences based on tional reading interventions used in schools. Pre- and
VOT. Thus, language ability is not necessary for pho- post-studies of brain activity suggest that the former
neme discrimination. training causes persistent changes in brain metabo-
lism in regions most activated during reading and
Auditory Temporal Acuity other language-related processes. This shows that
Other studies have asked whether individual differ- experience-driven plasticity for language learning
ences in audition, such as the ability to perceive small persists into adulthood, and neural bases of auditory
changes in sounds, might contribute to individual language processing are not fixed.
differences in language learning. This question is rel- In sum, although there are limited studies of the
evant because some theorists had speculated that lan- neural bases of language specialization, existing data
guage impairment disorders are due to genetic abnor- suggest that cortical specialization begins within the
malities of a specialized language-learning module. first 14 months. This suggests that specialization for
However, many children and adults with language language processing follows similar processes and is
delays and reading disabilities show a lower-level similar to specialization for other kinds of skills and
auditory processing problem: specifically, hearing information (e.g., reading or math, which obviously
fast sound changes. Even infants who have a relative depend on specific experience). Also, individual dif-
with language impairment show reduced sensitivity ferences in auditory processing speed predict lan-
to fast tone changes. Possibly their auditory systems guage development. This suggests that speed of pro-
cannot update pitch information quickly enough to cessing low-level acoustic information is critical not
assimilate the fast-changing sound patterns in nor- only for speech perception, but also for the gradual
mal speech. More recent studies show differences in emergence of higher-level language comprehension,
cortical responses to simple sounds in typical versus and that phonological processing is plastic and train-
language-impaired children and in infants with or able even in adults.
without a language-impaired relative. The differences These conclusions indicate a relation of language
occur in brain responses to sounds that are processed and cognition. Notably because processing speed is
even when we are inattentive. Although the results a significant factor in many nonlinguistic tasks, and
are complex, they point to individual differences in because individual differences in processing speed
efficiency of sound processing within the primary correlate across a wide variety of tasks (linguistic and
or secondary auditory cortex. At one extreme, these nonlinguistic), general neural properties that contrib-
individual differences predispose children to language ute to general processing speed differences will influ-
impairment. ence the development of both linguistic and nonlin-
There is other evidence that efficiency (i.e., speed) guistic skills.
of auditory processing is critical for normal language
development. For example, both children and adult Working Memory in Language Learning
second-language learners have more difficulty under- and Processing
standing sped-up speech, and adults who learned Once children are old enough to form and process
a second language later in life show slower neural utterances (usually 18 to 30 months), limitations on
Copyright © 2014 SAGE Publications. Not for sale, reproduction, or distribution.

Interrelationship of Language and Cognitive Development (Overview) 287

working memory will affect comprehension, pro- sorting based on colors when these properties call for
duction, and learning. In preschool and school-aged opposing responses). Some researchers have suggested
children, verbal working memory predicts word learn- that young children’s difficulties with task-switching
ing and reading comprehension. Interpretation of tasks (e.g., switching errors) are due to their difficulty
this correlation is difficult, however. Verbal working inhibiting prior responses. Others have suggested that
memory seems to be a specialized resource for lan- their difficulties relate to an inability to represent mul-
guage processing, as proposed by Alan Baddeley. How- tiple nested rules. However, growing evidence indi-
ever, there is some evidence that working memory for cates that cue-processing demands, including working
musical sequences and for nonnative speech correlates memory for the current cue, comprehension of the
with language abilities including word learning. Inter- cue, and speed of cue processing all predict children’s
estingly, musical sequence processing by children acti- performance on task-switching tests.
vates overlapping brain regions and elicits similar EEG Several other studies have not found strong relations
phenomena as some language processing tasks. How- between executive functions and language processes.
ever, there are few studies of how learning and mem- For example, many studies assess children’s inhibi-
ory for linguistic sequences is distinct from learning tory processes with alternate-naming tasks, wherein
and memory of nonlinguistic sequences. In sum, there children must reverse naming associations (e.g., say
is some relation between working memory resources day when shown a sun picture or night when shown
and language abilities, including language learning, a moon picture). Although these tasks are convenient
but the nature of the relation is not well specified. and they elicit age differences, there is no evidence
that the results relate to receptive language ability or,
Cognitive Control for Language Learning indeed, that they relate robustly to other tests of execu-
There has been recent interest in how language devel- tive functions. Thus, inhibition of lexical associations
opment relates to cognitive control or executive func- is not a clear predictor of other language or cognitive
tions, including inhibitory control, selective and stra- skills. In sum, although there is abundant circumstan-
tegic attention, cognitive flexibility or switching the tial evidence of relations between cognitive control and
mental set, and working memory control and coher- language development, there is not yet a coherent pat-
ence. These processes are critical for problem solv- tern of evidence or comprehensive theoretical account.
ing, planning, and reasoning. However, their role in
language processing, for example, understanding and Issues of Interpretation
producing discourse, was until recently unexplored. A general limitation of the aforementioned evidence
Although several findings suggest relations between is that much of it is correlational. The problem is that,
executive functions and developing language skills, in psychological research, good things go together:
the data are not cohesive. A pervasive but seldom Positive traits or outcomes are usually correlated. For
acknowledged problem is that most executive func- example, although verbal IQ (vIQ) subscale scores are
tion tasks recruit language skills. Those skills include more strongly correlated with each other than with
word and sentence comprehension, pragmatics, and performance IQ (pIQ) subscales, vIQ and pIQ scores
discourse processing, and sometimes knowledge of are nevertheless moderately correlated in children as
written symbols. Few studies have adequately con- well as adults. There is also evidence that processing
trolled for the language-processing demands of speed in infants—even visual processing speed—pre-
executive function tasks. Another problem is that the dicts later language development. Such evidence calls
executive functions themselves are poorly defined or to mind the historical idea of g, a single global factor
measured in unspecified ways across tests. of intelligence. Whether or not that concept is valid, it
Despite these problems, there are suggestive lines is nevertheless the case that a small number of cogni-
of evidence. One suggests that children’s flexibility in tive variables might explain some variability in a wide
switching between tasks depends on their ability to array of language skills as well as executive function
represent and update changing task cues or instruc- and other cognitive tasks.
tions. In task-switching tests, participants must attend
to and understand alternating cues or commands to Specialized Word-Learning Processes
switch from one response criterion to another oppos- A contentious question is whether children have spe-
ing criterion (e.g., from sorting cards based on shape to cialized word-learning biases. There were claims in
Copyright © 2014 SAGE Publications. Not for sale, reproduction, or distribution.

288 Interrelationship of Language and Cognitive Development (Overview)

the 1980s and 1990s that children and even infants part of what is learned. In children and adults, exper-
do have such biases. These claims were buttressed tise within a domain typically entails learning finer-
by findings that suggested that young children learn grained distinctions, suggesting that word learning
words significantly faster than other kinds of infor- will focus on subordinate labels and rare words or
mation. However, more recent evidence has eroded words for atypical categories.
that idea. Children from 1 to 4 years of age learn novel Very little is known about how conceptual knowl-
spoken words from very few exposures, but they also edge relates to accuracy and richness of word mean-
learn novel gestures, melodic patterns, facts, and pic- ings. An old debate concerned toddlers’ over-extension
tograms from few exposures. In a recent study, chil- of labels (e.g., calling all men daddy). One theory is
dren age 3 to 5 years learned words not faster but actu- that this was due to conceptual blurring—that is, not
ally slower than facts and pictorial symbols, partly, distinguishing between subtypes (e.g., different men).
it seems, because they require several repetitions to Although toddlers probably do not subdivide highly
form a representation of novel phonological strings similar and less-familiar subtypes (e.g., squirrels and
(lexemes). Moreover, children did not apply biases in gophers or falcons and hawks), it is unlikely that over-
inferring meaning—so-called taxonomic and mutual extension is mostly due to conceptual limitations.
exclusivity biases—more to novel words than to sen- Some over-extension errors are apparently due to
tences or pictograms. pragmatics: Because toddlers’ productive vocabulary is
Another set of studies in the 1990s suggested that so limited, they may use some word they can produce
words selectively draw infants’ attention to objects that has a similar meaning to whatever they wish to
or categories of objects or that infants preferentially label. It is unlikely that a toddler who calls unfamiliar
expect words to refer to categories. However, subse- men daddy cannot discriminate her father from other
quent studies showed that stimuli such as melodic men. Thus, over-extension does not necessarily indi-
tones similarly draw infants’ attention and that chil- cate conceptual blurring. Less clear is whether some
dren readily generalize facts and pictorial symbols, as words are learned before others simply because some
well as words, to novel categories. concepts are easier to understand than others. There
Finally, much attention has been paid to infants’ are arguments that various kinds of words, like color
ability to utilize social contextual cues to facilitate word words, number words, or verbs in general, are learned
learning. However, infants utilize the same cues to learn later because they offer some general conceptual or
and interpret nonverbal events. For example, infants, perceptual difficulty in acquiring the related concept.
by 18 months, assume that whatever an adult was look- At some level, this point is trivial: For example, sum
ing at when he or she said a word is the referent of that is easier to understand than second derivative, and
word. This suggests that 1-year-olds are learning about more people comprehend the former than the latter.
the social context of people’s language use. However, In young children, however, these differences might be
infants also use adults’ gaze direction to redirect their more pervasive and less obvious. For example, children
own attention, to form emotional associations with know few superordinate words (vehicle, action, and
objects, and to learn how to use objects. Thus, infants shape). Superordinate words might be harder to learn
use these social cues for a variety of functions, suggest- because they are so abstract. Yet, superordinate words
ing a general kind of utilitarian cue-learning function also are fairly infrequent in the words children hear,
(e.g., learning to attend to whatever social events are so it is not clear that the late acquisition of superordi-
useful). In sum, it is unclear what, if any, learning pro- nate words is strictly due to children’s conceptual dif-
cesses are specialized for word learning. ficulty with abstractness. Moreover, some words that
arguably could be very confusing indeed, like deictic
Conceptual Knowledge and Language Growth pronouns (you, me, this, and that), are used correctly
A clear connection between cognition and language is by 2-year-olds. Thus, it is not clear that conceptual dif-
the accrual of words related to new conceptual knowl- ficulty determines when children learn a word.
edge. In some studies, vocabulary is actually used as
a measure of content knowledge in a given domain. How Language Development Facilitates
In studies of children acquiring expert knowledge, Cognition: Does the Lexicon Structure Perception?
learning domain-specific words (e.g., names for types A centuries-old question is how language influences
of birds) has been both a measure of expertise and perception of the world. The hypothesis that our
Copyright © 2014 SAGE Publications. Not for sale, reproduction, or distribution.

Interrelationship of Language and Cognitive Development (Overview) 289

perceptions are indelibly shaped by our language, his- understanding the word shape might help a child learn
torically associated with the linguists Edward Sapir oval from a sentence like Eggs are shaped liked ovals. In
and Benjamin Lee Whorf, has been a topic of renewed short, to the extent that children can use contextual
interest among cognitive linguists. Developmental semantic elements within networks of meaning, both
studies have most vigorously investigated this ques- classifiers and related nouns and predicates might help
tion with respect to spatial predicates. These words children acquire new words and word meanings.
vary across languages: for example, in situations
where English speakers would use on (e.g., __ a table, Does Verb Syntax Affect Event Construals?
__ your finger, __ your head, and hanging __ the wall), Any event can be described any number of ways,
Korean, Dutch, or Mandarin speakers would use dif- and verbs and their inflectional elements (e.g., tense
ferent words. Other languages use the same predicate markers) allow speakers to place a different perspec-
for relations that English divides into on or in. Does tive, or construal, on an event. Nevertheless, different
the division of spatial relations by predicates affect languages have different prototypical ways to capture
how children come to perceive those relations? Stud- event information in verb phrases. For example, Span-
ies suggest that, well before they are fluent, infants and ish tends to encode the path of motion in movement
toddlers tend to generalize (i.e., perceive as similar) verbs, whereas German and English tend to encode
spatial relations based on the spatial predicate assign- manner of motion in verbs (e.g., jogging versus loping)
ments in their native language. For example, Korean- and use prepositions or other satellite morphemes to
learning toddlers begin to notice differences between encode path. Adults are sensitive to these distinctions,
events that English-learning children do not readily although children seem to slowly learn them: For
notice as these events are classified by different spatial example, English- and Spanish-learning 2- or 3-year-
predicates in Korean but not in English. olds do not differ much in their construal of novel
Aside from a handful of studies such as these, most verbs (i.e., generalizing path or manner). The problem
research on how language affects perception or cogni- does not seem to be that children cannot detect dif-
tion has tested adults or, increasingly, preschool-aged ferences in path and manner of movement in events.
children. Several studies have investigated classifiers: Rather, it might be that because the languages are only
inflectional markings or words (often articles) that probabilistically different in this regard, it takes several
imply, often loosely, noun categories. Mandarin, for years for children to notice the distinction. In short,
example, has classifiers for superordinate categories of children notice various possible relations among their
animal types, artifact types, things with certain shape language patterns and various distinctions in the
features, things found in different set sizes (e.g., pairs), world, but how long it takes them to make these asso-
and so on. Children learn their language’s classifiers by ciations might depend on the strength or predictive
3 to 4 years, and researchers have investigated whether regularity of the semantic relations.
classifiers affect their perception—for example, their
expectations about meanings of novel nouns fol- Language Routines and Cultural Learning
lowing a known classifier. Results suggest that some Cognition is not an individual, solipsistic achievement
familiar and conceptually clear classifiers might guide but a process of distributed learning in an environ-
children’s attention to category-relevant features and ment of other people, objects, and culturally structured
affect their inferences about the objects denoted by events. Speech acts in social events reflect how inter-
the classifier. However, it is not clear that these effects acting individuals or groups conceptualize and priori-
are pervasive. That is, if children have no information tize those objects and events. Children learn patterns
about a word’s meaning except a classifier that com- of social interaction based on how language is used.
monly refers to long, thin things (e.g., the Mandarin For example, middle-class parents talk differently to
tiao4), it stands to reason that children (or any ratio- preschool daughters and sons about past events, often
nal agent) would use that cue to select a long, thin test framing similar events differently, for example, using
object rather than, say, a round thing or a bag-like more emotion-state descriptors with girls (e.g., “You
thing, both of which take other classifiers. However, felt sad about that, didn’t you?”). Later, girls and boys
in languages with few classifiers, like English, super- may use emotion words differently. One interpreta-
ordinate words might serve similar roles, though they tion is that adults’ sociocultural beliefs about gender
are syntactically distinct from classifiers. For example, inform conversations with children and are assimilated
Copyright © 2014 SAGE Publications. Not for sale, reproduction, or distribution.

290 Interrelationship of Language and Cognitive Development (Overview)

and reflected in children’s own descriptions. Language last 40 years, language development research has moved
becomes a mediating medium for socializing children’s steadily toward recognizing that language processing is
construal of events and shaping their autobiographi- cognition, language use is distributed cognition, and
cal memory. This interpretation, however, assumes understanding children’s capacity for language means
that adult conversational patterns are powerful causal understanding the development and recruitment of
forces, which has not been established. general learning and cognitive processes.

Learning Multiple Languages Gedeon O. Deák


There is evidence that bilingualism is associated with University of California, San Diego
better performance in other cognitive abilities. Some
studies have compared mono- and bilingual children See Also: Associative Processes; Auditory Processing
on executive function tasks. When socioeconomic and Language Impairments; Bilingualism: Effects of
(SES) status is controlled, some bilingual children per- Cognitive Development; Computational Models of
form better on these tasks. One theory is that bilingual Language Development; Domain Specificity; Event
children must regularly switch between languages or Perception and Language Development; Executive
rapidly activate different codes based on their inter- Functions and Language Development/Processing; Gender
locutor’s language, and this demand accelerates the Differences in Language Development; Labeling Effects
development of executive functions. Other evidence on Cognitive Development; Neonatal Speech Perception;
suggests that bilingual children perform better on tests Neural Basis of Language Development; Over-Extension
of understanding other people’s divergent beliefs. Per- and Under-Extension in Word Learning; Processing
haps bilingual children must frequently make infer- Deficits in Children With Language Impairment;
ences about other people’s meaning or communicative Semantic Development; Spatial Cognition and Language
intent, with the added demand of inferring what code Development; Verbal Working Memory.
they are using. Whatever the reason, learning multiple
languages might help children master complex cogni- Further Readings
tive skills slightly earlier than same-aged peers. How- Bowerman, Melissa and Stephen Levinson, eds. Language
ever, a general difficulty in interpreting these results Acquisition and Conceptual Development. Cambridge,
is that, in any community, there are multiple differ- UK: Cambridge University Press, 2001.
ences between monolingual and multilingual families. Clark, Eve. “How Language Acquisition Builds on
These include cultural practices (e.g., educational atti- Cognitive Development.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
tudes and how children are spoken to), SES, history of v.8 (2004).
experiences, and biological factors (e.g., diet, medical Deák, Gedeon O. “Hunting the Fox of Word Learning:
care, and genetic differences). Some studies control Why ‘Constraints’ Fail to Capture It.” Developmental
for SES, and this is important, but many other poten- Review, v.20 (2000).
tially important factors are left uncontrolled. Thus, the Deák, Gedeon O. and Alexis Toney. “Young Children’s
available evidence is suggestive, but it remains unclear Fast Mapping and Generalization of Words, Facts, and
exactly how significant and general are the cognitive Pictograms.” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
benefits conferred by learning two languages. v.115 (2013).
Endress, Ansgar D., Marina Nespor, and Jacques Mehler.
Conclusion “Perceptual and Memory Constraints on Language
A voluminous body of research points to many interre- Acquisition.” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, v.13 (2009).
lations between language development and cognition, Gogate, Lakshmi and George Hollich, eds. Theoretical and
including others not mentioned here. Available evi- Computational Models of Word Learning. Hershey, PA:
dence does not permit any uniform, simplistic conclu- IGI Global, 2013.
sions. Every language skill that has been systematically Maratsos, Michael. “Grammatical Acquisition.” In
investigated recruits general, nonlinguistic cognitive Handbook of Child Psychology, William Damon, ed.
capacities and processes. There remain many achieve- New York: Wiley and Sons, 1998.
ments of childhood language, however, that have not Pullum, Geoffrey K. and Barbara C. Scholz. “Empirical
been compared to nonlinguistic analogues. In these Assessment of Stimulus Poverty Arguments.” The
cases, no conclusion can be drawn. Regardless, in the Linguistic Review, v.18 (2002).
Copyright © 2014 SAGE Publications. Not for sale, reproduction, or distribution.

Intervention for Child Language Impairments 291

Tomasello, Michael, ed. The New Psychology of Language: born with a language acquisition device that enables
Cognitive and Functional Approaches to Language children to recognize the frames and constructs of
Structure. Vol. 1. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, 1998. languages (the principles and parameters) such that a
Yang, Charles. “Computational Models of Language child can identify what is common across languages
Acquisition.” Handbook of Generative Approaches and what is specific to their own language, enabling
to Language Acquisition: Studies in Theoretical the child to establish the relevant word-order (syn-
Psycholinguistics, v.41 (2011). tactic) patterns and ways of encoding morphological
markers, such as plural, tense, and possession, for their
input language. The theory holds that children do not
respond to variability in the input but need only a few
Intervention for Child exemplars in order to fine-tune the relevant principles
and parameters. In this view, the language faculty is
Language Impairments a domain-specific ability—the language-learning
faculty is modular and not related to other cognitive
Child language impairment (CLI) is a term covering a abilities. The child learns the language by mapping the
diverse range of developmental impairments in child- input vocabulary onto language rules.
hood. The DSM-V A02 definition is that a diagnosis Children vary in language ability because of deficits
of language impairment is made when a child has in learning the rules of language. Application of this
language abilities in one or more areas that are below theory to language intervention results in a focus on
age expectations. Although children with neurodevel- learning syntax, including word order and morphology.
opmental disorders such as autism spectrum disorder Therapy is highly structured to focus the child on rec-
or Williams syndrome may present with features of ognition of correct versus incorrect syntactic construc-
language impairment, these are comorbid with other tions. The idea is that once the child has worked out the
developmental problems. A developmental language relevant parameter (e.g., the correct word order), then
impairment in the absence of neurological, physi- the target rule is generalized across the child’s language
ological, emotional, social, or cognitive conditions is system. Language learning is rule based, and therapy
commonly known as a specific language impairment requires that the child learn the right rule.
(SLI). The term used here is child language impair- The social-interactionist theory claims that lan-
ment. This entry broadly focuses on language inter- guage development is driven by the child’s need to
vention for children with the most common charac- become an effective communicator and that language
teristics of a language impairment involving grammar development is closely linked to other cognitive pro-
and morphology and reduced ability with vocabulary. cesses used in nonverbal communication, joint atten-
The nature of intervention for CLI is diverse, varying tion, and goal achievement. The child and signifi-
according to theoretical perspective and the resulting cant others, such as parents, engage in shared social
purpose of and goals for the intervention. Contem- interactions that determine the rate and route of lan-
porary intervention approaches tend to converge on guage development. Children vary in language ability
a common set of core principles that are appropriate because able language users in the child’s environment
for young children. vary in the learning experiences they provide for the
child. Application of this theory to language interven-
Theoretical Perspectives tion results in contexts in which learning is deliberately
CLI is founded on the clinician’s beliefs about language incremental. Learning can be effectively controlled by
development and language learning. L. Abbeduto and the parents’ use of scaffolding, with effective scaffold-
D. Boudreau, as well as G. H. Poll, outline the three ing requiring that the parent is aware of the level or
most widely held views of language development. stage that the child is currently at in order to pitch the
These are the nativist approach, the social-interaction- input language that occurs within shared activities at
ist approach, and the emergentist approach. Adoption a level slightly higher than that used by the child. This
of any one of these theoretical perspectives generates a ensures that the language environment is not too com-
particular view of the purposes, goals, and strategies of plex for the child but is sufficiently advanced to show
intervention. The nativist approach, advocated most the child how increased communicative needs can be
strongly by Noam Chomsky, claimed that children are met by increased language complexity.

View publication stats

You might also like