You are on page 1of 9

L

Language: Comprehension Introduction

Allison A. Steen and Elizabeth A. L. Successful execution of language processing


Stine-Morrow components depends to some extent on both men-
University of Illinois, Urbana, IL, USA tal mechanics, which are vulnerable with aging,
and knowledge, which has potential for
experience-based growth. Given this, it is not
Synonyms surprising that language comprehension can
show dynamic change over the life span. On the
Discourse memory; Language understanding; other hand, there is also growing evidence for the
Text memory; Text processing benefits of language proficiency in building cog-
nitive reserve. In this article, this bidirectional
relationship between aging and language is
reviewed by considering first the way in which
Definition
age-related changes in cognition can influence
language comprehension, and then briefly how
The comprehension of language in the domains of
literacy practices and engagement with language
both written and spoken communication requires
can influence cognition and health in late life.
the decoding of print or acoustic features of
speech to create mental representations of mean-
ing, events, and situations. In spite of the apparent
Effects of Aging on Language
ease with which it accomplished, language com-
Comprehension
prehension is a complex process requiring the
synchronization of a variety of cognitive compo-
Age differences in language comprehension are
nents (e.g., recognizing letters and words,
reflected in the multidirectional effects of age on
accessing and integrating word meanings to rep-
cognition. Declines in mechanics have been con-
resent ideas, and using existing knowledge to
ceptualized in terms of working memory, speed,
develop a representation of implied situations).
and interference control. Recent work has consid-
As such, language comprehension is interwoven
ered the role of interactions between mechanics
with core cognitive mechanisms, which show
and sensory processing. On the other hand,
multidirectional patterns of change as a function
growth in knowledge and literacy skills as a func-
of both primary aging processes and experience.
tion of engagement with reading can be a signif-
icant strength in adulthood. A foundation of
# Springer Science+Business Media Singapore 2016
N.A. Pachana (ed.), Encyclopedia of Geropsychology,
DOI 10.1007/978-981-287-080-3_220-1
2 Language: Comprehension

domain knowledge allows depth of comprehen- comprehension. Also, in spoken language, older
sion in specialized domains that is almost impos- adults make good use of prosodic contour (i.e., the
sible to achieve in any other way. Importantly, temporal patterning and changes in frequency that
experience-related growth in knowledge and lit- are characteristic of natural speech) to recognize
eracy skill opens pathways to compensation in syntactic structure, as well as the differential
terms of both processing mechanisms and in the emphasis of concepts.
way language input is regulated. On the other hand, a hallmark of language is
that it is generative, in the sense that we produce
Implications of Age-Related Declines sentences that have never been produced before
in Mental Mechanics and comprehend sentences that we have never
Many aspects of language comprehension are encountered before. Even though aging does not
unaffected by variation in mental mechanics and compromise the ability to use the rules that afford
are well preserved with healthy aging (Thornton generativity, these rules allow the construction of
and Light 2006; Wingfield and Stine-Morrow sentences that are very complex so as to tax men-
2000). In fact, older adults with developed literacy tal mechanics. Thus, age-related declines in
skills generally are at least as efficient as the mechanics may result in comprehension deficits
young in word recognition and in accessing mean- as the demands of language processing increase.
ings of individual words. Nor does aging appear to When older adults meet these demands so as to
impair the appreciation of basic sentence structure show good performance, they do so through the
(i.e., using syntactic form to determine how the allocation of extra effort. This has been demon-
meanings of individual words are connected to strated with a number of methods, including exag-
form ideas). For example, older adults are as gerated neural recruitment in imaging, increases
able as the young to quickly infer the particular- in reading time, and interestingly, with
ized meanings of words in context (e.g., that the pupillometry.
“insect” in The insect stung the professor is likely It is well established that the pupils dilate with
to be a bee or wasp and not a termite). Aging also increased cognitive demands. This phenomenon
does not impair the ability to distinguish between has been used to explore age differences in the
grammatical and ungrammatical sentences or to effort required to understand syntactically com-
use syntactic structure to infer semantic focus. plex sentences (Piquado et al. 2010). The contrast
This was demonstrated in a series of studies by between subject-relative (e.g., The doctor who
Price and Sanford (2012) who manipulated the was advised by the plumber went on vacation)
syntactic form of a sentence with clefting so as and the more complex object-relative (e.g., The
to put different elements in the foreground of doctor who advised the plumber went on vaca-
attention to examine whether there were age dif- tion) constructions has been well studied. Object-
ferences in using these cues for focus (e.g., The relative constructions are more difficult to
core idea John lost his daughter can be expressed understand for a number of reasons, including
with the subject clefted as, It was John who lost the violation of canonical subject-verb-object
his daughter, which puts the focus on John, or ordering, and the fact that the agent of the matrix
with the object clefted as, It was his daughter that clause (in this example, the doctor went on vaca-
John lost, which puts the focus on the daughter). tion) is also the object of the embedded clause
Reading times were faster for a continuation in (i.e., the plumber advised the doctor). Compre-
which the grammatical subject was matched to the hension is typically more error prone for object-
focused element (e.g., He had wandered off, in the relative sentences, especially for older adults.
former case; and She. . ., in the latter). Older adults Piquado et al. (2010) presented younger and
showed strong effects of focus, at least as robust as older adults with spoken sentences of these
those of the younger adults, demonstrating that forms in order to answer comprehension ques-
they were able to use linguistic structure to mod- tions afterwards. They found that for both age
ulate activation of concepts in language groups, pupil size increased as they listened to
Language: Comprehension 3

the sentences, especially so when they were trying tasks. Working memory measures included
to comprehend the object-relative constructions. assessments of simultaneous storage and manipu-
Consistent with the idea that syntactic complexity lations of verbal and visuospatial information.
is more demanding of attention for older adults, Inhibition processes were operationalized using
they showed an exaggerated increase in pupil size a number of tasks requiring resistance to interfer-
when listening to object-relative sentences. ence (e.g., intrusions in working memory). Youn-
Caplan and colleagues (2011) investigated ger and older adults read a series of narratives
speed and accuracy of sentence comprehension followed by comprehension questions for each
under varying levels of syntactic complexity. text. These questions were answered under one
Sentences also varied in plausibility (e.g., It was of two conditions, text-present or text-absent,
the movie that terrified the child because it such that the texts were either available when
showed a monster versus It was the child that participants answered the comprehension probes
nourished the food in the pantry). Participants or not. In addition, half of the comprehension
self-paced the presentation word by word, and questions probed specific details from the text
reading times were recorded as an online measure (i.e., facts explicitly stated in the text), and the
of comprehension as the sentence unfolded. After other half were inferential in nature (i.e., relying
each sentence, subjects indicated whether the sen- on inferences necessary for text coherence). The
tence was plausible or implausible as a measure of best-fitting model was one in which the effects of
comprehension accuracy. Both younger and older processing speed and inhibition on text compre-
adults were less able to distinguish plausible and hension were mediated by working memory, with
implausible sentences when they were more syn- working memory as the proximal cause of age
tactically complex, with an exaggeration of this differences in text comprehension. A further
effect for older adults. To some extent, older model comparison examining the statistical fit of
adults also took differentially more time to pro- this model separately for text-present and text-
cess the syntactically complex forms. absent conditions suggested that age and mechan-
There are competing theories regarding the ics accounted for more variance in the text-absent
nature of declines in mental mechanics and how condition than the text-present condition. Interest-
these mechanisms affect language processing ingly, age differences in text comprehension were
with aging. Working memory is the capacity for moderated by condition such that older adults
mental computation and simultaneous storage of performed equally as well as the younger adults
the products of these computations and is partic- when the text was present during the comprehen-
ularly important for comprehension and learning. sion probe. Together these results suggest, first,
Processing speed, on the other hand, is the effi- that working memory is a core cognitive mecha-
ciency with which these computations are com- nism responsible for age differences in text com-
puted. Finally, inhibition is the ability to ignore prehension, but that working memory itself has
contextually irrelevant information. Aging has dissociable components that contribute to com-
been shown to be associated with decreased work- prehension. Second, this study suggests that
ing memory capacity, reduced processing speed, demands on working memory from comprehen-
and failure to inhibit, all of which have conse- sion are less related to online processing than they
quences for language processing. are to the maintenance of information in memory.
In the Caplan et al. study, reading time mea- Finally, evidence from this study suggests that
sures were negatively correlated with both work- age-related differences in text comprehension
ing memory and processing speed. Borella may be minimal in typical conditions in everyday
et al. (2011) examined the differential impact of life, in which text is available as it is used for
processing speed, inhibition, and working mem- particular tasks.
ory on text comprehension using structural equa- There are both negative and potentially posi-
tion modeling. In this study, processing speed was tive implications of inhibition failure for text com-
measured using the letter and pattern comparison prehension. In a study by Thomas and Hasher
4 Language: Comprehension

(2012), older and younger adults read stories emphasized with a pitch accent, older (but not
containing irrelevant information, cued with a younger) adults showed a reduction in the accent
change in font, and were explicitly told to ignore boost, which Fraundorf et al. called the other-
this information. Later, subjects were given a accent penalty. They attributed this other-accent
memory task for a short list of words, half of penalty to the lower working memory capacity of
which were the distracting words from the stories. older adults. The other-accent penalty was also
Older adults were more disrupted in reading by found for younger adults with relatively low
the presence of distraction and were also more working memory scores, supporting the idea that
likely to recall the distracting words than new the age differences were due to working memory
words in the memory task. When participants declines that limit the ability to focus on multiple
were explicitly told that some of the words in the concepts in discourse.
recall task were from the stories they had just read, Collectively, this literature suggests that many
younger adults then recalled more of the aspects of language comprehension are preserved
distracting words and even outperformed older with aging (e.g., word recognition and basic syn-
adults in this recall task. Thus, these results are tactic and semantic operations). However,
consistent with the view that younger adults not age-related changes in mental mechanics can
only have more efficient inhibition mechanisms impact language comprehension across text type
but also have more cognitive control over when to (sentence, discourse) and modality (print, speech)
engage these mechanisms than older adults and in more difficult conditions.
that this plays a role in language comprehension.
Even though older listeners can take good Implications of Age-Related Decline
advance of cues from prosody to distinguish in Sensory Processing
important and unimportant concepts in speech, Whether it involves listening to speech or reading
mechanics can place limits on this ability as print, language comprehension depends on the
well. For instance, pitch accents (i.e., a change in perception of accurate sensory input. While
tone to cue the emphasis of a word or phrase word recognition processes are generally auto-
relative to other words in the text) have been matic among those with well-developed literacy
shown to boost memory in both younger and skills, when the sensory information is unclear,
older adults (Fraundorf et al. 2012); this is decoding the surface form can require attentional
known as an accent boost. In Fraundorf resources to the extent that comprehension may be
et al. (2012), older and younger adults listened to compromised. In other words, decoding and
brief stories that included the mention of two semantic processing draw on the same pool of
contrast sets, for example, about some British attentional resources. When the surface form is
and French scientists searching for endangered clear (e.g., print that is legible and appropriately
monkeys in Malaysia and Indonesia. In the target sized, speech with well-articulated phonology and
sentence that resolved the story, one element of good prosody), decoding is accomplished easily
each contrast set (i.e., British vs. French; Malaysia so that working memory resources can be devoted
vs. Indonesia) was selected (e.g., The French to constructing a representation of the meaning.
spotted a monkey in Indonesia). Pitch accent was Challenges to signal fidelity – because of noise in
systematically manipulated so as to stress one the environment, deficits in sensory processing, or
(e.g., The FRENCH spotted a monkey in a combination of both – can compromise lan-
Indonesia, or The French spotted a monkey in guage comprehension.
INDONESIA), both (e.g., The FRENCH spotted A number of recent studies support these
a monkey in INDONESIA), or neither element. claims in both the auditory and print domains.
Memory was tested with a fill in the blank task When individuals read text in visual noise (i.e.,
(e.g., The _____ spotted a monkey in ________). text degradation imposed by embedding the text
Both younger and older adults showed an accent in a visually complex environment), patterning of
boost. However, when both words were reading times can be used to show that relatively
Language: Comprehension 5

more attention is devoted to decoding and rela- abilities also have advantages in more efficient
tively less attention is devoted to semantic resolution of lexical ambiguities (Stites
processing, which results in poorer memory per- et al. 2013) and in using statistical properties of
formance (Gao et al. 2012). Older adults are more language to guide parsing (Payne et al. 2014).
impaired than younger adults at lower levels of Thus, lifelong habits of literacy experience can
visual noise suggesting that limits on mechanics help to preserve language processing and compre-
exacerbate sensory effects on comprehension. hension so as to trump declines in working
Similar phenomena have been demonstrated memory.
with speech comprehension. Van Engen and Domain-specific knowledge can also support
Peelle (2014) described three sources of acoustic language comprehension processes. Chin and col-
challenge: within the listener (e.g., hearing loss), leagues (2015) examined the differential effects of
from the environment (e.g., background noise), or health-specific domain knowledge and domain-
from the speaker (e.g., increase speech rate, pro- general verbal ability (measured as vocabulary)
sodic disruptions, unfamiliar dialect). Such acous- on reading comprehension among older adults.
tic challenge decreases the attentional resources Participants read sentences that were either related
available for higher-level language processes (i.e., to health topics or about other topics (but matched
comprehension of meaning) because they are allo- in readability), using a self-paced word-by-word
cated to lower-level decoding processes. Addi- reading paradigm. Based on reading time patterns,
tionally, this shift of attentional resources seems results showed that verbal ability was related to
to be moderated by age and working memory increased conceptual integration regardless of
capacity such that those with lower working mem- domain, but that health knowledge (controlling
ory capacity (e.g., older adults, and younger adults for verbal ability) was specifically related to
with lower WM) are more likely to show perfor- increased conceptual integration in health-related
mance deficits when sensory fidelity is sentences. Thus, domain-specific knowledge had
compromised than those with higher working effects on processing above and beyond that of
memory capacities. verbal ability to support comprehension and
memory for text.
Implications of Age-Related Knowledge
Growth Self-Regulation and Attention
Age affords time for experience, and among One implication of the effects of mechanics and
adults with good literacy skills and practices, crystallized abilities on comprehension is that
preservation or growth of crystallized ability and there can be wide variation in the way in which
print exposure is the norm. This “expertise” can the mental representation of meaning is
boost performance in text comprehension through constructed as language unfolds. In the early his-
several different pathways. In fact, among older tory of psycholinguistics, the emphasis was on
adults who have achieved strong literacy skills, language competence, how the rules of language
the effects of declines in mental mechanics on are used in an idealized way to construct the
comprehension and memory for text can be neg- language representation. Questions about varia-
ligible. For example, Payne et al. (2012) showed tion in comprehension as a function of abilities
the effects of working memory on sentence recall are ones about performance, how language is
were only found among older adults with rela- actually understood and interpreted in situ. It
tively low print exposure (an index of reading would be a mistake to think about abilities simply
experience). Based on word-by-word reading as fuel for the operation of language processing
times, they also showed that print exposure con- machinery that operates in an obligatory and uni-
ferred an advantage in the efficiency of word form way (e.g., performance does not simply
recognition processes and was also related to reflect incomplete computations defined by com-
greater effort allocated to semantic integration petence). Rather, as we have described above, the
processes. Older adults with higher verbal nature of the language representation is that it is
6 Language: Comprehension

multifaceted, and these different facets can be but required more effort (as measured by
constructed to varying levels of fidelity. rereading) to resolve them.
One view of age differences in self-regulation Collectively, this literature suggests that it is
of language is that older adults show more shallow not that language comprehension is generally
or “good enough” processing in comprehending more superficial with age. Rather, that there is a
language. For example, Christianson and col- shift in how resources are allocated among the
leagues (2006) showed that when encountering different levels of analysis (Stine-Morrow
sentences with temporary ambiguities, such as et al. 2008).
While Anna dressed the baby played in the crib, Behavioral research tends to show that older
both younger and older adults are likely to incor- adults are differentially facilitated in word recog-
rectly answer “yes” to the question, Did Anna nition processes by context in both the auditory
dress the baby? with older adults even more vul- and print modalities. This is particularly interest-
nerable to this error. According to the “good ing in light of findings from electrophysiological
enough” account, as the sentence unfolds, the studies suggesting that meaning is slower to come
initial interpretation that Anna dressed the baby online for older adults. Event-related brain poten-
is maintained in memory even after the interpre- tials (ERPs) are a time-sensitive measure of brain
tation that the baby played in the crib is encoded activation in response to a particular stimulus
(in fact, participants are also likely to say that the (e.g., word) widely used to examine online com-
baby played in the crib). Christianson et al. argued prehension. In particular, the N400 is a negative
that rather deriving meaning strictly based on the ERP component occurring about 400 ms after
rules of language, we use heuristics to create stimulus onset. Because its amplitude is very sen-
meaning representations that are consistent with sitive to the predictability from context, the N400
world knowledge. Thus, even though the coexis- is taken as an index of very early effects of context
tence of these two meanings is not allowed by the on lexical processing. Younger adults show
sentence structure, we maintain them because it is reduced N400s for words predictable from the
easy to create a mental model of the situation. prior sentence context as well as words that do
Because older adults tend to rely on knowledge- not fit the context but are from the same semantic
based heuristics, they are more likely to use such category (e.g., To make the hotel look more like a
“good enough” processing. tropical resort, they planted rows of palms/pines).
In fact, language comprehension is often shal- This is taken as evidence that younger adults
low. As any middle schooler who has snagged actively predict in language comprehension so
someone with the problem of the plane crash on that items related to the target are activated. How-
the border between the USA and Canada can tell ever, older adults show only an N400 reduction
you, when you pose the question of where one for expected endings (e.g., palms). This suggests
should bury the survivors, most people will puzzle that older adults are not using predictive
over the border issue without realizing they are processing. A recent study examined age differ-
being asked about burying living people. ences in predictive processing by manipulating
Daneman et al. (2006) measured eye movements constraint and expectancy of sentence final
as younger and older adults read and attempted to words. Wlotko and colleagues (2012) measured
solve problems containing such anomalies the N400 effect for target words preceded by
(embedded among problems that did not). There strong (e.g., At night the old woman locked the
were no age differences in the probability of notic- door) and weak (e.g., Mr. Smith enjoyed showing
ing the anomalies (i.e., young and old were people his newly installed stereo) sentence con-
equally likely to say that the survivors should text. Although both younger and older adults
not be buried). Based on the eye movement showed reduced N400s for expected endings
record, Daneman et al. concluded that older adults when contextual constraint was strong, only
were actually quicker in detecting the anomalies, younger adults showed reduced N400s for weakly
constrained endings. These results suggest that
Language: Comprehension 7

although older adults are equally proficient at Education, Literacy, and Longevity
conceptual integration (as shown by There is increasing interest in educational attain-
age-equivalent performance in the strongly ment and literacy as predictors of longevity and
constraining condition), younger adults are more cognitive health (Sisco et al. 2015; Spittle
likely to use contextual information for predictive et al. 2015). This body of research has grown in
purposes. Collectively, age-comparative studies recent years and has shown that controlling for
using ERPs suggest that older adults (in general) demographic factors known to contribute to life
do not engage in predictive processing in the same expectancy (e.g., sex, ethnic group, income), edu-
way as younger adults. However, older adults cational attainment is a reliable predictor of life
with high verbal ability show predictive expectancy, more so than perhaps more obvious
processing comparable to the young. predictors of longevity such as cholesterol level or
The nature of comprehension is that the ideas frequency of health checkups. According to Spit-
given by the text are integrated and interpreted in tle et al., if all Americans attained a high school
light of existing knowledge, but this balance diploma, annual mortality would be reduced by
changes with age to give priority to knowledge- about 240,000 persons. However, measures of
based interpretation (over direct computation of educational attainment (e.g., years of formal edu-
meaning). At the same time, the ability to com- cation) are of many varieties and do not take into
pute the meaning from the text as an integrated account the quality of educational experience. One
set of ideas is preserved with aging, though the hypothesis for why education has such powerful
way in which effort is regulated to achieve this effects on lifelong cognition is that it is a proxy for
may change. Older readers have to allocate more literacy, with evidence that, in fact, print exposure
effort to integrate ideas and may engage integra- is a better predictor of cognitive health than educa-
tion processes more frequently as they read in tion level and can delay the onset of dementia.
order to offset the demands of integrating larger Language is, of course, important for many
segments of text (Stine-Morrow and Payne aspects of life in predominantly literate societies
2015). To some extent, segmentation itself is a such as in the USA. But perhaps surprisingly,
procedural skill that is intact into very late life, studies comparing low-literacy adults to those
though integration processes may become less with well-developed literacy skills have revealed
effective with late-life cognitive declines significant behavioral and anatomical conse-
(Payne and Stine-Morrow 2016). quences of literacy for cognition and neural sub-
Self-regulation, then, plays a large role in the strates, which have the potential to impact late-life
preservation of language comprehension with health. Often, with comparisons of high- and
aging. This is seen in both greater reliance on low-literacy samples in the USA, it is difficult to
knowledge-based processing and in text-based disentangle the effects of literacy from the social
processing that accommodate working memory and economic factors associated with literacy illit-
limits. eracy. However, a number of studies in recent
years have focused on populations in which illit-
eracy is not necessarily a consequence of poverty
or socioeconomic stressors that have their own
Effects of Language on Aging
significant effects on cognition and health (e.g.,
children in Columbia who were conscripted into
An emerging body of literature suggests that over
service during civil war at the expense of early
time a mentally stimulating lifestyle can create
schooling and literacy acquisition; older daugh-
cognitive and neural reserve that may delay
ters in the Algarve region of Portugal, who by
age-related cognitive decline, with some evidence
tradition were given childcare responsibilities
supporting the idea that language experience may
while younger siblings went to school to receive
impact cognitive health.
literacy instruction). Such natural experiments
have shown that illiterates have impoverished
8 Language: Comprehension

phonological processing, visual attention, abstract recognition and use of syntactic cues to guide
reasoning, semantic fluency, and memory com- semantic processing are largely intact. On the
pared to their literate counterparts. Literacy acqui- other hand, there are a number of ways in which
sition also enhances cortical responses to language complexity can tax attentional demands
two-dimensional objects and words and supports so as to compromise comprehension.
the development of verbal working memory capac- With the rise of neuroscience, there are provoc-
ity. Furthermore, results from neuroscience ative puzzles that arise in reconciling findings
research (e.g., functional and structural imaging) between imaging and behavioral data. For exam-
suggest that literacy reorganizes cortical networks ple, older adults typically show exaggerated con-
to change the brain’s response to both spoken and textual facilitation in behavioral data, suggesting
written language, with greater selectivity of activa- that the build up of message-level semantics can
tion (Huettig and Mishra 2014). However, as supporting decoding processing. Electrophysio-
Huettig and Mishra (2014), in most cultures, liter- logical data, however, often show the reverse
acy acquisition is tightly woven with schooling, so (e.g., Federmeier et al. 2010), in particular less
that it is a challenge to disentangle effects of liter- predictive processing from sentence context with
acy from educational experience more broadly. age. Thus, it will be important for future work to
develop a theoretical account of age differences in
Bilingualism how different sorts of processes come on line in
Further evidence for language impacting cogni- language processing, so as to accommodate both
tion is a considerable body of work suggesting behavioral and neuroscience data.
that bilingualism (or even better, multilingualism) Literacy is a form of expertise that is often
confers advantages for adult cognition. Early taken for granted in cognitive aging research,
bilinguals (i.e., those who learned a second lan- and adults who have not developed these skills
guage early in life and continue to use both lan- are typically underrepresented in research sam-
guages) show reduced declines on measures of ples. However, a nontrivial proportion of adults
executive control as they age and show a delay in the USA have literacy skills inadequate to the
in the onset of symptoms for dementia (Bialystok demands of everyday life, and they are especially
et al. 2012). The roots of these effects are still vulnerable to declines in cognitive health in later
being explored, but likely derive from multiple life (National Research Council 2012). More
mechanisms. First, proficiency in a second lan- needs to be known both about the effects of liter-
guage is enhanced among those with strong skill acy skill as a moderator of cognitive aging and as
in the first language, so it is possible that the potential pathway to resilience.
bilingual advantage is grounded in part in early
literacy acquisition. More importantly, in manag-
ing multiple linguistic codes, only one of which is Cross-References
typically appropriate at a given time, bilinguals
are routinely engaging suppression mechanisms ▶ Age-Related Change in Abilities
in ordinary communication. Thus, bilingualism ▶ Aging and Inhibition
entails routine practice in attentional control that ▶ Aging and Language: Production and
may contribute to cognitive health in late life. Communication
▶ Cognitive Changes: Normal and Age-Related
▶ Cognitive Control and Self-Regulation
Conclusions and Future Directions ▶ Crystallized Intelligence
▶ Intelligence
Language comprehension is a highly active form ▶ Knowledge
of mental engagement. Older adults with ▶ Language: Naming
well-developed literacy skills have a number of ▶ Sensory Effects on Cognition in Late Life
advantages that support comprehension. Word ▶ Working Memory in Older Age
Language: Comprehension 9

References and individual differences in binding during sentence


understanding: Evidence from temporary and global
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2012). syntactic attachment ambiguities. Cognition, 130,
Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. 157–173.
Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 240–250. Piquado, T., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Wingfield, A. (2010).
Borella, E., Ghisletta, P., & de Ribaupierre, A. (2011). Age Pupillometry as a measure of cognitive effort in youn-
differences in text processing: The role of working ger and older adults. Psychophysiology, 47(3),
memory, inhibition, and processing speed. Journal of 560–569.
Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 66B, 311–320. Price, J. M., & Sanford, A. J. (2012). Reading in healthy
Caplan, D., DeDe, G., Waters, G., Michaud, J., & aging: The influence of information structuring in
Tripodis, Y. (2011). Effects of age, speed of processing, sentences. Psychology and Aging, 27, 529–540.
and working memory on comprehension of sentences Sisco, S., Gross, A., Shih, R. A., Sachs, B. C.,
with relative clauses. Psychology and Aging, 26, Glymour, M. M., Bangen, K. J., . . . Manly, J. J.
439–450. (2015). The role of early-life educational quality and
Chin, J., Payne, B. R., Gao, X., Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., literacy in explaining racial disparities in cognition in
Morrow, D. G., Conner-Garcia, T., . . . & Murray, M. D. late life. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sci-
(2015). Memory and comprehension for health infor- ences, 70, 557–567.
mation among older adults: Distinguishing the effects Spittle, M. L., Riley, W. T., & Kaplan, R. M. (2015).
of domain-general and domain-specific knowledge. Educational attainment and life expectancy:
Memory, 23, 577–589. A perspective from the NIH Office of Behavioral and
Christianson, K., Williams, C. C., Zacks, R. T., & Social Sciences Research. Social Science and Medi-
Ferreira, F. (2006). Younger and older adults’ “good- cine, 127, 203–205.
enough” interpretations of garden-path sentences. Dis- Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., & Payne, B. R. (2015). Age
course Processes, 42, 205–238. differences in language segmentation. Experimental
Daneman, M., Hannon, B., & Burton, C. (2006). Are there Aging Research, 42(1), 83–96.
age-related differences in shallow semantic processing Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Miller, L. M. S., Gagne, D. D., &
of text? Evidence from eye movements. Discourse Hertzog, C. (2008). Self-regulated reading in adult-
Processes, 42, 177–203. hood. Psychology and Aging, 23, 131–153.
Fraundorf, S. H., Watson, D. G., & Benjamin, A. S. (2012). Stites, M. C., Federmeier, K. D., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L.
The effects of age on the strategic use of pitch accent in (2013). Cross-age comparisons reveal multiple strategies
discourse: A processing-resource account. Psychology for lexical ambiguity resolution during natural reading.
and Aging, 27, 88–98. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory
Gao, X., Levinthal, B. R., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. and Cognition, 39, 1823–1841. doi:10.1037/a0032860.
(2012). The effects of aging and visual noise on con- Thomas, R., & Hasher, L. (2012). Reflections of distraction
ceptual integration during sentence reading. Quarterly in memory: Transfer of previous distraction improves
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1833–1847. recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning
Huettig, F., & Mishra, R. K. (2014). How literacy acquisi- Memory and Cognition, 38, 30–39.
tion affects the illiterate mind: A critical examination of Thornton, R., & Light, L. L. (2006). Language compre-
theories and evidence. Language and Linguistics Com- hension and production in normal aging. In J. E. Birren
pass, 8, 401–427. & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of
National Research Council. (2012). Improving adult liter- aging (6th ed., pp. 261–287). New York: Academic.
acy instruction: Options for practice and research. van Engen, K. J., & Peelle, J. E. (2014). Listening effort
Washington, DC: National Academies Press. and accented speech. Frontiers in Human Neurosci-
Payne, B. R., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2016). Risk for ence, 8, Article 57, doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00577.
mild cognitive impairment is associated with semantic Wingfield, A., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2000). Language
integration deficits in sentence comprehension. Journal and speech. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.),
of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 71, 243–253. The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed.,
Payne, B. R., Gao, X., Noh, S. R., Anderson, C. J., & pp. 359–416). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2012). The effects of print Wlotko, E. W., Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (2012). To
exposure on sentence processing and memory in older predict or not to predict: Age-related differences in the
adults: Evidence for efficiency and reserve. Aging, use of sentential context. Psychology and Aging, 27,
Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19, 122–149. 975–988. doi:10.1037/a0029206.
Payne, B. R., Grison, S., Gao, X., Christianson, K.,
Morrow, D., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2014). Aging

You might also like