You are on page 1of 15

Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95 – 109

www.elsevier.com/locate/intmar

Do Price Charts Provided by Online Shopbots Influence Price Expectations


and Purchase Timing Decisions?
Wenzel Drechsler ⁎ & Martin Natter
Strothoff Chair of Retailing, University Frankfurt, Germany

Available online 22 March 2011

Abstract

Online price comparison sites (shopbots) like PriceGrabber.com are the most powerful tools for consumers to easily compare prices and find offers
for desired products. Besides providing distributions of actual prices in price comparison tables, shopbots like NexTag.com have recently introduced
price charts (line charts) displaying a product's full price history. Price charts should support consumers in forming expectations about future prices.
Nevertheless, it is currently unclear how price charts influence consumer price expectations and purchase decisions. The results of this study show that
the provision of past prices leads to strong adjustments of price expectations depending on price chart characteristics. In particular, the trend, variance and
range of past prices in the chart strongly affect price expectations and purchase timing decisions. Furthermore, in the case of a strong downward trend and
high variance in past prices, results show that nearly 50% of the total effect is caused by the visualization of the price history.
© 2011 Direct Marketing Educational Foundation, Inc. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Price expectations; Purchase timing; Shopbots; Price comparison sites; Information visualization

Introduction while on pricescan.com, it is called “price trend graph.” Most


frequently, such charts show the minimum prices for a product
Nielsen NetRatings (2007) shows that the ability to across different retailers over time (see Fig. 1).
efficiently compare offers is one of the most popular reasons Information on a product's price history is a source of
for consumers to shop on the Internet, as it is cited by 62% of external reference prices and should therefore stimulate
those surveyed. Thus, online price comparison sites (shopbots) consumer behavior (Kopalle and Lindsey-Mullikin 2003).
like PriceGrabber.com and YahooShopping.com are the most Consequently, price charts should support consumers when
powerful tools for consumers to easily compare prices and find forming expectations about future prices. Research shows that
offers for desired products. As such, shopbots reduce the cost of price expectations and purchase timing are conceptually related
search for information about products and facilitate better and (Danziger and Segev 2006). Price history information included
more efficient purchase decisions (e.g., Häubl and Trifts 2000; in the price chart could therefore enforce strategic buying
Trifts and Häubl 2003). behavior with respect to buying now or later. Since studies in
Besides providing distributions of actual prices for any behavioral finance show that some investors base their buy or
product in the form of price comparison tables, shopbots like sell decisions depending on certain chart patterns of past stock
NexTag.com, PriceScan.com, and Skinflint.co.uk have recently prices (Park and Irwin 2007), one might also expect that
introduced line charts displaying a product's full price history. visualization itself has an effect on consumer expectations and
NexTag.com, for instance, calls this feature “price history,” purchase timing decisions. Depending on price charts char-
acteristics and, therefore, price history, consumers potentially
form and/or adjust expectations about future prices, which in
⁎ Corresponding author at: Strothoff Chair of Retailing, Goethe University
turn influence purchase timing decisions.
Frankfurt, Department of Marketing. Grueneburgplatz 1, 60323 Frankfurt,
Germany.
Information on the most attractive product categories on
E-mail addresses: wenzel.drechsler@wiwi.uni-frankfurt (W. Drechsler), shopbot sites reveals that consumers use shopbots especially for
natter@wiwi.uni-frankfurt.de (M. Natter). finding the best offers in the category of consumer durables,
1094-9968/$ - see front matter © 2011 Direct Marketing Educational Foundation, Inc. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.intmar.2011.02.001
96 W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109

Fig. 1. Examples of shopbots with price charts.

particularly consumer electronics such as notebooks, TVs or study enhances knowledge about reference price effects on
digicams. This is because purchase timing is a critical decision, durable goods purchase timing (Mazumdar, Raj, and Sinha 2005).
especially for the most durable product purchases (Mazumdar, Third, the results of this study update current knowledge
Raj, and Sinha 2005). Hence, consumers who use shopbot sites concerning the relation between the presentation of information
that provide information about a product's price history can at shopbot sites and consumer price perceptions (Smith 2002).
gain easy access to information about a product's life cycle Finally, this study contributes to the field of research on visual
stage. Although consumers expect prices to decrease over a representation and decision-making (Lurie and Mason 2007).
product's life cycle especially in high-tech markets (Bridges, To test our hypotheses, we conducted experiments in which
Yim, and Briesch 1995), the price chart makes this information participants were asked to state their price expectations and
more apparent to consumers. purchase timing decisions after viewing a particular price chart
Understanding the response of consumers to price chart condition, which we had manipulated for our purposes. Further,
information should be relevant for shopbots, retailers and we also tested the effects of the same price histories on price
manufacturers. When price charts are perceived as relevant expectations and purchase timing when presented in a non-
information, shopbots could increase their popularity. Research graphical manner to explore the effects of the visualization.
shows that with respect to purchase timing, a change in price The results show that in general, the price charts induce strong
expectations has a strong influence on demand elasticities (Erdem reference price effects. Further, the trend, variance/volatility and
et al. 2005). Hence, retailer and manufacturer sales and profits are amount of price decline (i.e., range) shown in a chart exert a strong
potentially affected by consumer reactions to price charts. influence on price expectations. The trend and variance also
To the best of our knowledge, there is no study that analyzes exhibit a strong influence on purchase timing decisions. In general,
the effect of price history charts on consumer decision-making. results indicate that the strength of the impact of the chart
The introduction of a product's full price history visualized in characteristics increases with long-term price expectations.
a line chart introduces two types of information, namely, Finally, the results clearly demonstrate that the graphical
1) historical information about prices and 2) a graphical display representation itself enforces the impact of the price trend and
of this information; as such, it is currently unclear how price charts variance on consumer price expectations and purchase timing
influence consumer expectations and purchase decisions. decisions.
Based on the foregoing discussion, it is the aim of this study The remainder of this article is organized as follows. First,
to explore the effects of price charts on consumer expectations we discuss the related literature and derive the underlying
regarding durables prices and purchase timing decisions. In hypotheses. Second, we present the outline and results of Study 1,
particular, this study investigates whether the introduction of which analyzes the influence of price charts on price expectations
price charts (that is, price history) induces reference price effects and purchase timing. We then present Study 2, which assesses the
in terms of adjusting a consumer's price expectation. Further- effect of graphical representation. The last section summarizes the
more, this study analyzes the impact of price chart character- results, discusses managerial implications and presents avenues
istics on consumer price expectations and purchase timing and for further research.
disentangles the effects of reference price histories from effects
due to their visualization. Related Literature
This study contributes to several fields of research. First, with
regard to reference price research, this study extends the analysis Consumer Price Expectations for Durables
of the impact of external reference prices at a certain point in time
to an analysis of a whole series of external reference prices over Expected future prices are particularly important for all
time that are captured and ordered in a single source. Second, the product categories that experience significant price changes
W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109 97

over time like consumer durables (Ofir and Winer 2002). Since a varying time horizon for past performance charts induces a
price decreases over the product life cycle are fairly common significant influence on a trader's investment decisions.
among durable products, they are widely anticipated by Especially with regard to investment decisions to sell or buy,
purchasers of durable products (Balachander and Srinivasan investors follow stock price trends typically visualized in price
1998). Marketing literature suggests that consumers form charts. Indeed, many investors practice technical analysis of
expectations regarding a product's attributes (most notably price chart patterns (Leigh et al. 2002), which presumably
price) based on historical patterns for the attributes of the identifies patterns in price charts that may offer an indication of
product category; they then incorporate these expectations in whether a trend is likely to continue or terminate (Park and
their purchase decisions (Bridges, Yim, and Briesch 1995). Irwin 2007). Of course, such a possibility is in principle ruled
There is evidence that consumers form forward-looking out by the random walk assumption, and so many researchers
expectations that affect consumer durable purchases (Winer are quite skeptical of these ideas. However, practitioners use
1985). Consumers expect price declines due to experience curve them routinely for trading (Caginalp and Balenovich 1996), and
effects and plan or delay their purchases accordingly (Doyle and so the price chart can be regarded as standard information for
Saunders 1985). This discussion suggests that due to prior investors. Typically, technical analysis is operationalized
experiences, consumers develop mental schemas or a set of through trading rules of the following form: “If chart pattern
expectations about a product category (Sujan, Bettman, and X is identified then buy/sell within/after the next N trading
Sujan 1986). Erdem et al. (2005), for instance, find that in the days” (Leigh et al. 2002). This means that by using chart
case of PCs, consumers generally expect a steady-state rate of information, investors try to maximize their profits and hence
price decline. Further, they find that consumers seem to expect the economic value of their transactions.
mean reversion in price declines; i.e., if the decline over the past Likewise, in the case of durable products, consumers also try
few months was greater (or less) than normal, then consumers to maximize the economic value of their transaction, since they
expect a lesser (greater) price decline over the next few months. typically make a trade-off between buying now or later when
However, since durables have longer interpurchase times the price level has reached a certain level. In particular, they try
than frequently purchased packaged goods (FPPG) consumers to maximize transaction utility. This transaction utility increases
are normally less informed about the development and changes when the price meets or falls below an expected price (Darke
of attribute configuration, technology, and price level of a and Chung 2005). Hence, price charts on a product's price
durable. The information acquired during prior purchase history should also support consumers when forming future
occasions is therefore less salient in the formation of price price expectations and when making purchase decisions.
expectations for a durable product than it is for a FPPG However, there is no study available that provides insights
(Mazumdar, Raj, and Sinha 2005). With the introduction of on the influence of price charts on consumer price expectations
information about a product's price history displayed in a price and purchase decision for products that are priced according to
chart, consumers no longer have to rely on likely obsolete price their stage in the life cycle.
knowledge based on prior experience when forming price
expectations for purchase timing of durables. Therefore, it is Study 1
essential to investigate whether the introduction of price charts
induces reference price effects in terms of adjusting prior price Expected Effects and Hypotheses Development
expectations and how different chart patterns affect price
expectations and purchase timing. Reference Prices — Anchoring and Adjustment
According to adaptation-level and assimilation-contrast
Price Chart Information and Consumer Purchase Decisions theories, price information (or external reference prices) affects
consumer perceptions when it is judged acceptable or plausible
In the marketing literature, the influence of price history relative to the internal price standards of consumers (Monroe
visualized in a line chart and chart pattern characteristics on 2003; Urbany, Bearden, and Weilbaker 1988). These processes
consumer decisions is not discussed. also occur when consumers are exposed to a price that is outside
Research on behavioral finance suggests that investors base the expected range but still plausible. Instead of rejecting this
their investment decisions on specific stock price chart price information outright, consumers assimilate and reduce it
characteristics. Standard economic theory, however, assigns to a level more reasonable for the product category (Urbany,
little informative value to stock price charts due to the random Bearden, and Weilbaker 1988). Taken together, these points
walk assumption (Brealey, Myers, and Allen 2007). Accord- indicate that consumers who encounter new price information
ingly, historical price movements shown in a chart should not tend to update their prior (Yadav and Seiders 1998) price
predict how a stock will behave in the future. Nevertheless, expectations. Hence, we expect that consumers use price charts
research shows that some investors base their investment showing the development of past prices to update (adjust) their
decisions on specific stock price-chart characteristics. Muss- prior price expectations.
weiler and Schneller (2003) show that investors use salient
standards in price charts such as salient highs or lows for their Influence of Chart Characteristics
future stock price expectations and, thus, for investment A key feature of high-tech durables is the tendency for prices
decisions. Additionally, Benartzi and Thaler (1999) show that to fall quickly over time, creating an incentive to delay
98 W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109

purchases. The strength of this incentive depends on consumer trend becomes less obvious in a highly volatile price series. This
forecasts of how quickly prices will drop (Erdem et al. 2005). In leads to the following hypothesis:
the formation of price expectations for durables, consumers
especially use product histories if a trend exists (Mazumdar, H3. A high variance in prices displayed in a price chart
Raj, and Sinha 2005). In general, consumer expected prices for moderates the effect of the chart's trend on a) price expectations
time period t are then equal to the current price plus a fraction and b) purchase time.
reflecting the difference between this period's price and last
period's price (i.e., extrapolative expectations). This means that According to range and range–frequency theories (Parducci
consumers update their price expectations by factoring in a price 1965; Volkmann 1951), consumers judge actual prices not only
trend observed from prior prices (Mazumdar, Raj, and Sinha by their location within the distribution of other prices but also
2005). by the perceived range at the time of judgment. Range theory
Therefore, we expect that consumers who systematically postulates that consumer price perceptions depend on a
process chart information use trend characteristics (that is, weak comparison of a market price to the endpoints of an evoked
vs. strong downward trends) as a salient standard (i.e., chart price range. Janiszewski and Lichtenstein (1999) show that in a
characteristic) to form their expectations and make their high-range price situation in which the market price is nearer the
decisions. If consumers notice a strong decrease in price from lower endpoint, consumers judge the market price as more
one week to the next, they may expect even lower prices in the attractive as compared to a low-range situation. In the context of
future and decide to acquire the product only if a certain price is price charts, we accordingly expect to find an impact of the
reached (Kalyanaram and Little 1994). In the case of a weak range of the historical prices on price expectations and purchase
decrease in which the trend of past prices levels off (i.e., long- timing. In particular, we propose that the difference between the
term stationarity of price series), consumers instead may first and last price in the chart (that is, the range) affects price
forecast no further price decreases such that postponing does expectations because it establishes a benchmark for the
not seem worthwhile. These points lead to the following evaluation of the actual price. If the range between the first
hypothesis: and last prices in the price history increases, consumers should
evaluate the last price (i.e., actual price) more favorably, as it
indicates a higher gain within a certain time frame. Due to the
H1. Price charts with a strong downward price trend compared
assumption of extrapolative expectations, consumers should
to price charts with a weak downward trend lead to a) a
consequently also expect higher rates of price decline in the
downward shift in price expectations and b) a postponement of
future. Thus, this leads to the following hypothesis:
the purchase time.
H4. Price charts with a high range of past prices compared to
Besides trend characteristics, we expect that the variance in a price charts with a low range of past prices lead to a) a
chart (i.e., price fluctuations from one point to another) is downward shift in price expectations and b) a postponement of
another important chart characteristic. Research shows that the purchase time.
consumers might use the variance of observed prices as a
heuristic for price search behavior (Darke, Chaiken, and
Freedman 1995). The underlying idea is that the more variance Methodology
consumers observe in prices, the more likely they will be to
continue to search. They defer their purchase decision in the To test the hypotheses, we conducted experiments in which
belief that further searching will pay off. Kalwani and Yim participants are asked to state their price expectations and
(1992) also provide empirical evidence that consumers expect purchase timing decisions after viewing a particular price chart
prices to strongly decrease in light of frequently-changing condition that we manipulated.
prices. This leads to the following hypothesis:
Study Design Manipulations
H2. Price charts with a high variance in past prices compared to As indicated in the introductory section, consumers use
price charts with a low variance in past prices lead to a) a shopbots most frequently for gathering information about prices
downward shift in price expectations and b) a postponement of for durables, especially consumer electronics. In order to
the purchase time. determine the appropriate product category for the experiment,
we therefore conducted a pre-test with 63 participants. Given the
Besides expecting direct effects of the trend and variance on overall goal of this study, the aim of the pre-test was to identify a
price expectations and purchase timing, we further assume that durable product category for which purchase timing plays an
these two chart characteristics are not perceived independently. important role. Therefore, the participants rated on a 7-point scale
That is due to the fact that determining a trend in a set of past the importance of purchase timing for the three most popular
prices requires a consumer to interpolate between a large number electronic product categories on Internet shopbots, namely,
of data points (Vessey 1991). If, however, the variance of these notebooks, MP3-Player/IPods and digicams. Results show that
data points increases, it becomes more difficult to interpolate in differences in mean across all three categories are significant at
order to infer the underlying trend correctly. Put differently, the p b .05, with the highest score for the notebook category
W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109 99

(MNotebook = 6.06, SDNotebook = .83). Consequently, we use the To create a realistic shopbot environment for all treatment
notebook category in the main study. groups with price charts and a control group (that is, without price
The time horizon of the price charts used in the experiment was chart), we further constructed a price comparison table with
set to three months, since a typical lifecycle of a notebook model current prices for 10 competing retailers. In this price comparison
is only about six months (Guide, Muyldermans, and Van table, the minimum price of 850€ corresponds to the last price in
Wassenhove 2005). The time horizon of three months seemed the chart. Compared to the treatment groups, the control group
appropriate because it neither indicates that the notebook is at the only received the price comparison table of actual prices without
end of its life cycle, nor that it was recently introduced, which information on the notebook's past prices. Other potentially
could cause the majority of consumers to wait for better prices. available information, such as shipping fees and retailer ratings,
The experimental design encompasses price chart manipula- were kept constant. This latter information was only provided for
tions in terms of the actual downward trend (strong vs. weak), creating a more realistic experimental environment. Hypothetical
variance (high vs. low) and price range (i.e., the rate of price decline brands for the notebook, shopbot (namely, cheaper.com), and
(high vs. low)), resulting in a 2 × 2 × 2 between-subjects design. The retailers (listed in the price comparison table) were used to
trend manipulations are given by the two most representative chart minimize the potential participant's tendency to base their
patterns of notebook past prices on price comparison sites. decisions on brand and retailer images or previous experiences
The procedure to assign realistic price levels and price patterns (Kwon and Schumann 2001). Furthermore, the overall quality of
to the different chart conditions was as follows. First, we inferred the notebook and retailers were rated as very good to avoid the
information about the most popular notebooks in the market and effect of quality uncertainties.
their prices from Amazon's top 100 selling notebooks. We
calculated a mean sales price as a rank-weighted sales price of the Experimental Procedure
top 100 notebooks. Calculation of the average notebook price Participants were asked to imagine that they have a desktop
delivers a mean price of MPrice = 828.66€ (SDPrice = 478.59€). computer they bought several years ago that is currently working.
Based on this result, we assigned 850€ as the actual lowest price They were further told that this computer is technically out of date
to the notebook in the experiment. Second, we ensured that price and too inflexible and that they are thinking about buying a
declines follow realistic levels and patterns. Different shopbots notebook in order to make their work more efficient. The aim of
show price declines for notebooks within a three-month period framing the story this way was to prevent participants from feeling
ranging from 10 to 30%. For the low-range condition, we pressure that an immediate purchase was necessary.
therefore assigned 1000€ as a start price corresponding to a 15% In particular, the participants were told that they have decided
price decline. Furthermore, 1150€ was the price assigned to the to buy a specific notebook because of its convincing price
high-range condition corresponding to a 26% price decline. The performance ratio. After providing them with the average offline
intervals on the horizontal axis of the charts were adjusted price of this notebook they were exposed to additional
accordingly, holding all else equal. Examples for this manipula- information about the desired notebook's current prices at a
tion in the low-range condition are presented in Fig. 2. shopbot website and ask to make their final purchase timing

Fig. 2. Example charts.


100 W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109

decision. To eliminate effects caused by financial constraints, To test the impact of chart characteristics on the level of price
participants were provided with a budget of 1000€, which expectation and purchase timing, another measure was
exceeded the actual lowest price of the notebook (850 €). calculated that relates future price expectations to the current
In total, 531 people participated in an online experiment and price of 850€:
were randomly assigned to one of nine conditions, including
eight treatment groups plus one control group. In particular, a 2
(trend) × 2(variance) × 2(range) between-subject design PEt after
PEt = ; with t = 4; 8; 12 weeks 2Þ
(N = 427) was employed such that each treatment was exposed 850
to a price comparison table of current prices and one of the
manipulated price charts with the notebook's price history. The Since our experimental framing indicates that the notebook
control group (N = 104) only received the price comparison has already been on the market 12 weeks, then 12 weeks into
table of current prices. the future would typically correspond to the end of its life cycle
In the treatment groups, participants were asked twice to state (i.e., 6 months). We therefore denote price expectations over the
their price expectations concerning the notebook's minimum next 12 weeks as long term for notebooks.
price development at price points of 4, 8 and 12 weeks later. This Purchase timing was measured with the question “Given the
question first appeared after participants saw the price comparison information from cheaper.com, when would you most likely
table alone; it appeared second after being exposed to the price buy this notebook?” Responses were anchored at a weekly scale
comparison table and the price chart together. Finally, participants from 1 = “now” up to 14 = “after 12 weeks.”
were told to think about the optimal time to purchase the
notebook. In the control group, participants indicated their price
Results
expectations and planned purchase timing directly after seeing the
price comparison table. After participating in the experiment,
Manipulation Checks. To assess whether the levels of trend
participants filled out an online questionnaire. In order to control
and variance in the charts were actually perceived as different, the
for individual differences between participants, we measured
participants in each treatment group were asked to rate the slope of
important covariates that might affect price expectations and
the trend and the level of price fluctuations in the price chart on a
purchase timing.
7-point scale from 1 = low to 7 = high. Results of a 2 × 2 analysis
In particular, we controlled for participant usage and/or
of variance indicate that participants significantly perceive
experience with shopbots and the information they use on
differences in slope (Mstrong trend = 5.57 vs. Mweak trend = 4.61, F
shopbots (e.g., price chart used = 1, not used = 0). In total, 79% of
(1,426) = 131,9, p b .01) and variance (Mlow variance = 3.26 vs.
participants indicated that they regularly use shopbots for finding
Mhigh variance = 4.42, F(1,426) = 103,94, p b .01) as intended.
offers, whereas 31% of these shopbot users also use price chart
Furthermore, we find no significant interaction between our
information.
independent variables on the perceived level of variance (F
Regarding consumer psychographics (see Appendix), we
(1,427) = 2.58, p N .10). For the trend, however, results show a
control for deal proneness and notebook expertise as literature
significant interaction (F(1,427) =16.56, p b .01), indicating that
indicates that they are related to price expectations, timing
the level of trend is not perceived independently from the level of
decisions and information processing (Biswas and Sherrell,
variance.
1993; Lurie and Mason 2007; Martinez and Montaner, 2006;
To check the relevance of price charts for purchase timing
Rao and Sieben 1992). Finally, we control for the individual's
decisions, participant perceptions of the relevancy of the
evaluation of the current notebook price, as this might affect
shopbot information were assessed with three 7-point Likert-
study results. In particular, participants should indicate the
type scale statements (Mason et al. 2001). Higher scores
perceived expensiveness of the current notebook price.
indicate that participants perceived the information provided as
more relevant information (coefficient alpha = .90). With respect
Key Measures
to purchase timing, participants in the price chart condition
As indicated in the previous section, the treatment groups,
perceived the information provided by the shopbot as more
which are provided with price charts, were asked twice about their
relevant (MChart = 4.49) as compared to the control group, who
price expectations regarding the minimum price in the future. In
received the price comparison table only (MNo Chart = 3.83,
particular, they were asked about the expected future minimum
t = 3.85, p b .01). This first result reveals that the price chart is a
prices of the notebook in the upcoming 4, 8 and 12 weeks
useful tool for consumers who are deciding when to buy a
(PEt before). Updates of these three price estimates were obtained
product.
after participants received additional information about the
notebook's price history (PEt after). Reference price effects are
measured in terms of the adjustment of price expectations (ΔPEt) Price Expectations — Anchoring and Adjustment
in response to the chart as follows: ANOVAs and pairwise comparisons between all eight
treatment groups show no significant differences in price
expectations measured prior to the exposition to the chart
PEt before−PEt after information (PEtbefore). Results also show no significant
ΔPEt = ; with t = 4; 8; 12 weeks 1Þ
PEt before differences between the control group's price expectations
W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109 101

Table 1 Table 2
A priori price expectations in euros (PEt before). Adjusted price expectations.
4 weeks 8 weeks 12 weeks Δ Price expectations (ΔPEt )
Mean 823 795 756 Chart version N Week 4 Week 8 Week 12
Std. 42 54 74
Range: low
N = 427. 1) ST_LV 52 −.038*** −.073*** −.086***
(−6.68) (− 7.11) (− 5.02)
2) WT_LV 51 .005 .020** .048**
(0.72) (2.06) (3.26)
and the treatment groups' prior expectations. Hence, partici-
3) ST_HV 50 −.015** −.032*** −.049***
pants seem to have homogeneous expectations about the future (−2.46) (− 3.28) (− 3.86)
development of notebook prices. In particular, participants seem 4) WT_HV 50 .014* .021* .048***
to expect a monthly steady-state rate of price decline of 3.8%. (1.72) (1.70) (2.79)
Table 1 shows the mean expected prices for the next 4, 8 and
Range: high
12 weeks.
5) ST_LV 56 −.037 −.077*** −.106***
After the treatment groups were exposed to the price chart, (−5.45) (− 6.05) (− 6.78)
nearly all groups adjusted their price expectations. Table 2 6) WT_LV 61 .016 .022** .033***
reports the percentage change in price expectations (ΔPEt) for (1.42) (1.96) (2.32)
the eight different chart version groups and shows whether this 7) ST_HV 55 −.026*** −.049*** −.075***
(−2.65) (− 4.75) (− 5.42)
change significantly deviates from zero.
8) WT_HV 52 −.023*** −.029*** −.028**
Results clearly demonstrate that the different price charts (−3.12) (− 3.08) (− 2.21)
lead to adjustments of price expectations in both directions. For
***p b .01, **p b .05, *p b .1, t-values in parantheses.
instance, in the conditions including the strong downward trend Notes:
(versions 1, 3, 5 and 7), participants significantly lower their ST (WT) = strong (weak) downward trend.
price expectations for all three point estimates. The strongest HV (LV) = high (low) variance.
downward adjustments take place for the long-term price
expectations (i.e., 12 weeks) ranging from − 4.9% (version 3) to
− 10.6% (version 5). In contrast, the low-trend condition leads different price expectation measures for the upcoming 4, 8 and
to an adjustment in the other direction. In particular, participants 12 weeks are related to each other as confirmed by the Pearson
of the chart versions 2, 4, and 6 raised their prices expectations, correlation coefficients (ranging from .79 to .93, p b .01), we
especially for weeks 8 and 12. have to control for this correlations (Tabachnik and Fidell,
The results clearly show that the availability of price charts 2007). Hence, we analyse the data by MANCOVA through a
induce reference price effects in terms of adjusting price multivariate generalized linear model (GLM).
expectations, which stresses the chart's relevance for purchase In addition to the price chart characteristics, two covariates
decisions. Additional analyses that compare the price expecta- (namely, notebook expertise and price chart usage = 1/0) are
tions (PEt) of the different treatment groups with those of the included in the model estimation of each price expectation
control group point in the same direction. For instance, control measure. In particular, we use the following underlying model
group comparisons reveal the strongest differences for the long- structure to estimate the GLM:
term price expectations (i.e., 12 weeks). As compared to the
control group, chart version groups 1, 3, 5, and 7 show PEt = αt0 + αt1 Range + αt2 Trend + αt3 Variance
significantly (p b .05) lower price expectations ranging from + αt4 Trend × Variance ðt = 4; 8; 12Þ
− 5.2% (version 3) to − 9.3% (version 3). Furthermore,
participants of the chart version groups 2, 4 and 6 show higher + αt5 NotebookExpert + αt6 ChartUser + εt;PEt
price expectations (p b .10) than the control group. Hence, these ð1Þ
results indicate that consumers adjust their prior price expecta-
tions depending on chart patterns. Results of the MANCOVA part indicate significant
multivariate main effects for the range (Wilks' Lambda = .98,
Influence of Price Chart Characteristics F(3) = 2.92, p b .05), the trend (Wilks' Lambda = .83, F(3) =
In this section we test hypotheses H1 to H4, i.e., whether 28.10, p b .01) and the interaction between trend and variance
different charts lead to different price expectations and purchase (Wilks' Lambda = .98, F(3) = 2.90, p b .05) on expected prices.
timing decisions. Therefore, we first analyze the influence of Further, the variable chart-user shows a significant multivariate
different chart characteristics on price expectations (PEt) for main effect (Wilks' Lambda = .98, F(3) = 2.76, p b .05).
weeks 4, 8, and 12. In a second step, we test the effects of price To get a more differentiated picture of the impact of different
charts characteristics on participant purchase timing decisions chart characteristics on the three different price expectation
while accounting for price expectations. measures we report the parameter estimates of the GLM in
Table 3.
Effects of Price Chart Characteristics on Price Expecta- As expected, estimation results in Table 3 show that the
tions. Since the three dependent variables, i.e., the three direct effects of a high range and a strong downward trend
102 W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109

Table 3 chart characteristics and covariates according to (Bhattacharjee


Influence of price chart characteristics on price expectations. et al. 2007)
Price expectations (PEt) 0 1
β0 + β1 Range + β2 Trend + β3 Variance + β4 Trend × Variance
Week 4 Week 8 Week 12
ϕ = exp@ 4 A
+ β5 PEt + ∑ β5 + m Covariates + ε
Coefficient St.- Coefficient St.- Coefficient St.- m=1
error error error
ð3Þ
Chart characteristics
Range −.01*** .005 −.02*** .007 −.03*** .009 This model allows us to test hypotheses H1b to H4b, which
Trend −.04*** .007 −.08*** .009 −.11*** .013
propose direct effects of price chart characteristics on the time
Variance −.01* .007 −.02* .010 −.03** .013
Trend ∗ variance .02* .010 .04*** .014 .05*** .018 of purchase. Prior research indicates that price expectations and
purchase timing are conceptually related (Kalwani et al. 1990).
Covariates Therefore, we control for price expectations by estimating the
Notebook .00 .002 .00 .002 .00 .003 model for each of the three different price expectation measures
expertise PEt (t = 4, 8, 12 weeks). In addition to the covariates included in
Price chart user .00 .006 .01 .008 .02* .011
Constant −.07** .008 −.11*** .012 −.18*** .016 the price expectations models, we control for general deal
R2 (Adj.) .11(.10) .18(.17) .22(.21) proneness and the perceived expensiveness of the notebook's
***p b .01, **p b .05, *p b .1.
current price.
In line with previous research on purchase timing decisions,
we assume that the baseline survivor function S0(t) follows a
Weibull distribution (Helsen and Schmittlein 1993; Seetharaman
and Chintagunta 2003).1 For the estimation, the AFT model in
significantly lower participants' price expectations (p b .01).
Eq. (2) is put into the log-linear form with respect to purchase time
Further, the negative effect of a high variance of past prices
t (Bradburn et al. 2003). The estimation results are presented in
seems to increase from week 4 (p b .10) and week 8 (p b .10) to
Table 4.
week 12 (p b .05). Hence, these results provide strong support
A positive coefficient indicates that increasing values of the
for hypotheses H1a, H2a and H4a. Further, the results show a
respective independent variable lead to a delay of the purchase.
strong significant positive interaction (p b .01) between the trend
By exponentiation of the coefficients, we further obtain the time
and variance for price expectations in weeks 8 and 12 and a
ratio which can be interpreted as a deferral factor (DF) in our
marginal effect for week 4 (p b .10). This finding supports
context (Bradburn et al. 2003). DF indicates whether the chart
hypothesis H3a, which states that the variance in a chart
characteristics lead to a delay of the purchase (DF N 1) as
moderates the influence between the trend and price expecta-
compared to the respective reference group. Results for Model 1
tions. Overall, the results indicate that the magnitude of effects
with price expectations for four weeks as a control variable
increases from week 4 to week 12. This means that the price
reveal strong significant effects for the trend and variance
chart characteristics particularly influence long-term price
(p b .01), showing that a strong downward trend and a high
expectations.
variance of price charts increase the probability that participants
would defer their purchase. In particular, the probability to defer
Effects of Price Chart Characteristics on Purchase Timing.
increases by 32% (DF = 1.32) in response to a strong trend and
The response in the questionnaire concerning the independent
by 34% (DF = 1.34) in response to a high variance. Hence,
variable purchase timing allowed for the option of not buying
hypotheses H1b and H2b are supported. These findings also
the notebook within the upcoming twelve weeks. Indeed, 45
largely hold for Models 2 and 3 (with price expectations for
(10.5%) participants stated they would buy the notebook after
8 and 12 weeks, respectively). However, the effect of the trend
this time frame. Hence, our dependent variable is right
seems to decrease from Model 1(p b .01) to Model 3 (p b .10). At
censored, which implies the need for an event history modeling
the same time, the effect of the price expectations increases.
approach to assess the influence of price chart characteristics on
This result indicates that the variance in the chart is the main
purchase timing (Helsen and Schmittlein 1993).
driver of purchase timing decisions, whereas the direct effect
Following Bayus (1998), who analyzes purchase behavior
of trend weakens when long-term price expectations are
for personal computers, we use a parametric survival model. In
considered for purchase timing. In particular, higher long-
particular, an accelerated failure time (AFT) model is used to
term price expectations for week 12 favor earlier notebook
estimate the effects of chart characteristics on purchase time t.
Generally, we model the effects of the different chart
characteristics and covariates on purchase time t as 1
We also tested other distributions to specify the baseline survivor function
(e.g., exponential and log-logistics). Likelihood-ratio tests and the Bayesian
S ðt Þ = S0 ðϕt Þ ð2Þ information criterion (BIC) confirm that the Weibull distribution best describes
where the survivor function S(t) gives the probability that a the underlying purchase timing process. Further, the shape parameter θ of the
Weibull distribution is highly significant (p b .01) and larger than 1 in all
participant would buy the notebook after some specified models, indicating that the Weibull distribution is more appropriate to describe
purchase time t. S0(t) is the baseline survivor function and ϕ the underlying purchase timing process as compared to, for instance, an
is termed acceleration factor which depends on the different exponential distribution.
W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109 103

Table 4
Influence of price chart characteristics on purchase timing.
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Coefficient St.-error DFa Coefficient St.-error DFa Coefficient St.-error DFa
Chart characteristics
Range −.01 .068 0.99 −.03 .068 0.97 −.04 .068 0.96
Trend .27*** .095 1.32 .22** .098 1.24 .18* .099 1.19
Variance .29*** .095 1.34 .29*** .095 1.33 .27*** .094 1.32
Trend ∗ variance −.23* .134 0.79 −.20 .134 0.82 −.18 .134 0.83

Price expectation
PE4 −.12 .651 0.89 – – – – – –
PE8 – – – −.85* .483 0.43 – – –
PE12 – – – – – – −.94** .369 0.39

Covariates
Notebook expertise −.07*** .025 0.93 −.07*** .024 0.93 −.08*** .024 0.93
Deal proneness .07*** .022 1.07 .07*** .022 1.07 .07*** .022 1.07
Perceived expensiveness .10*** .026 1.10 .10*** .026 1.10 .09*** .026 1.10
Price chart user −.25*** .079 0.78 −.24*** .079 0.79 −.22*** .079 0.80
Constant 1.70*** .183 5.45 1.67 .182 5.31 1.65*** .182 5.19
θ (shape parameter) 1.56*** .068 1.57*** .068 1.57*** .068
LL (χ2) − 502.26 (58.23***) − 503.86 (55.04***) − 500.42 (61.92***)
***p b .01, **p b .05, *p b .1.
a = Deferral factor = exp(coeff.).

purchases (p b .05). This finding indicates that participants do put differently, they potentially trigger strategic buying behavior.
not expect strong price decreases in the future, which implies Comparisons of the treatment groups and control groups confirm
that longer waiting is not worthwhile (Kalyanaram and Little significant differences in price expectations and purchase timing.
1994).
Model 1 shows a marginal significant negative effect Study 2
(p b .10) of the interaction term between trend and variance on
purchase timing. Hence, a high variance of past prices reduces The Impact of Visualization
the strong direct effect of the trend, which increases the As discussed in the introductory section, the chart itself not
probability to buy earlier (hypothesis H3b). only includes price information but also represents this
To obtain a complete picture of the effects of the price charts, information in a graphical manner. Studies in behavioral
we re-estimated the above described survival models by finance show that the visualization of past prices itself leads
including the reference group without price charts. In particular, to a change in investment decisions. Hence, it is necessary to
we entered dummy variables for each of the chart version assess the impact of the graphical representation of price
groups, while the control group served as the reference histories on price expectations and purchase timing decisions to
category. Estimation results show that all price chart groups, fully understand the effects of price charts provided by
except for those exposed to chart version 2 and chart version 5 shopbots. The aim of Study 2 is therefore to disentangle the
with low trend low variance, respectively, would significantly effects caused by reference price histories from effects caused
defer their purchase time as compared to the control group by their visualization.
(p b .05). This additional analysis underlines the effects of In general, charts are spatial problem representations, since
strong decreasing trends and high variance of past prices on they present spatially-related information. Charts are expected
purchase timing decisions. to be both faster and more accurate decision sources than, for
instance, tables (Vessey 1991). According to Larkin and Simon
Summary Study 1 (1987), charts preserve explicit information about the topolog-
Results of Study 1 clearly show that a price chart of a ical and geometric relations among the components of the
product's price history is perceived as relevant information for problem; that is, they emphasize information about relation-
purchase timing decisions. Further, this information induces ships in data. Seminal research reports empirical evidence in
strong reference price effects in terms of adjusting consumer support of the notion that line charts especially facilitate the
price expectations. Participants lower or raise price expectations recall of trends (Vessey 1991; Washburne 1927). The tabulated
depending on chart characteristics. representation of data, in contrast, facilitates the recall of
In particular, a strong downward trend, high variance and a specific amounts. Therefore, line charts are preferred whenever
high range of past prices lead to lower price expectations with a it is important for consumers and/or investors to quickly and
strongest impact on long-term price expectations. Further, the easily recognize characteristics of data such as trends, volatility
trend and variance cause participants to postpone their purchase; and functional relations. Thus, we expect that in particular,
104 W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109

trend and variance in a price history are more visible in a line- wise comparisons also show no significant differences between
chart than in a table presentation. Accordingly, we expect that the treatment (tables, charts) and control (no price history)
trend and variance exert stronger effects on price expectations groups. Hence, participants again express homogeneous price
and purchase timing when visualized in a price chart. The expectations before they are exposed to the price history.
influence of the price range, however, should not be affected by Corresponding to Study 1, we estimate MANCOVA through a
its presentation format since the start- and endpoints are easily multivariate generalized linear model (GLM) to investigate the
observable independent of the presentation format. Overall, this influence of the price history characteristics on price expectations
discussion leads to the following hypothesis: (after respondent's were exposed to the table conditions). The
results show multivariate main effects for for the range (Wilks'
H5. The visualization of the price trend and price variance in a
Lambda = .97, F(3) = 4.26, p b .01) and the trend (Wilks' Lamb-
line chart as compared to the presentation in a table leads to a) a
da = .93, F(3) = 10.01, p b .01). Furthermore, the parameter
stronger downward shift in price expectations and b) a
estimates of the GLM confirm that a strong downward trend
postponement of the purchase time.
and a high range of past prices (p b .01) shift down all three price
expectation measures (weeks 4, 8, and 12). However, compared
to the chart condition in Study 1, the results show no significant
Methodology
effects of the variance and the interaction effect between variance
To assess the influence of the visualization of a product's
and trend on price expectations. Furthermore, estimating the
past prices in a line chart, we gathered additional data in an
corresponding survival models for purchase timing reveals only
online experiment from N = 399 people who were exposed to the
marginal negative directs effects (p b .10) for the range in Model 2
experimental setting and questionnaire used in Study 1. The
and Model 3. Together, these results indicate that the visualization
major difference is that the notebook's price history was
itself might have an influence on the strength of the effects in
presented in a table instead of a chart. Similar to Study 1, 81%
Study 1. This seems particularly plausible because no direct
of the participants indicated that they regularly use shopbots for
effects of the price history characteristics on purchase timing are
searching for offers. In addition, 38% of these shopbot users use
apparent in the table condition, whereas the trend and range still
price chart information provided by these shopbots.
have an influence on price expectations. Further, the results show
To guarantee fair comparison between both studies, bi-weekly
no significant effect of the variance on price expectations
prices were shown in the table. This interval was chosen because it
indicating that this characteristic is difficult to recognize in a
corresponds to the intervals on the horizontal axis in the chart
table. Accordingly, we explicitly test hypothesis H5, i.e., the
conditions from Study 1. Everything else (i.e., dependent and
effect of the graphical representation (visualization) on price
independent variables) were measured in the same way as in
expectations and purchase timing in the next section.
Study 1. Fig. 3 shows two examples with the prices corresponding
to the chart versions 1 (that is, low range, strong trend and low
The Impact of Price History Visualization on Price
variance) and chart version 2 (that is, low range, weak trend and
Expectations and Purchase Timing
low variance).
Since we employed a between subjects design in Study 1 and
Study 2 we are able to directly assess the influence of the
Results visualization effect itself. In particular, we re-estimate the
In this section we first report the empirical results of table models presented in Study 1 by incorporating participants from
conditions. Second, we explicitly test hypothesis H5, i.e., we both studies (N = 826). Hence, we introduce the visualization as
show how the visualization itself (chart vs. table) affects price an additional factor (1 = chart, 0 = table) into the analysis.
expectations and purchase timing. Examining the interaction effects between price history
characteristics and whether they are presented in a chart or a
The Influence of Tabulated Price Histories on Price table should reveal the effect of the visualization. With respect
Expectations and Purchase Timing to the chart characteristics the MANCOVA part of the
As in Study 1, participants show no significant differences in estimation procedure shows multivariate effects only for the
their a priori price expectations (PEtbefore). ANOVA and pair range (Wilks' Lambda = .98, F(3) = 4.32, p b .01) and the trend
(Wilks' Lambda = .96, F(3) =10.40, p b .01). In addition, the
covariate chart-user exhibits a multivariate main effect (Wilks'
Lambda = .98, F(3) = 5.75, p b .01).
However, results of the GLM part in (Table 5) reveal deeper
insights into the effect of the visualzation on the three price
expectation measures (PEt).
Estimation results show that the visualization of price history
characteristics as a chart increases the effects of a strong
downward trend for PE8 and PE12 (Chart ∗ Trend; p b .05). This
effect is stronger for long-term price expectations (week 12)
than for short-term expectations (week 4 and week 8). Thus,
Fig. 3. Example tables. hypothesis H5a is in generally supported with regard to the trend
W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109 105

Table 5
Influence of the visualization of past prices on price expectations.
Price Expectations (PEt)
Week 4 Week 8 Week 12
Coefficient St.-Error Coefficient St.-Error Coefficient St.-Error
Price history characteristics
Range −.01** .005 −.02*** .007 −.03*** .009
Trend −.02*** .008 −.05*** .010 −.07*** .013
Variance .00 .008 .00 .010 −.01 .013
Trend ∗ variance .00 .011 .01 .014 .02 .019

Visualization
Chart vs. table .01 .008 .01 .011 .02 .015
Chart ∗ range .00 .007 .00 .010 .01 .013
Chart ∗ trend −.02 .010 −.03** .014 −.04** .018
Chart ∗ variance −.02 .010 −.02 .014 −.02 .018
Chart ∗ trend ∗ variance .02 .015 .03 .020 .03 .026

Covariates
Notebook expertise .00 .001 .00 .002 .00 .002
Price chart user .00 .004 .01* .006 .02*** .007
Constant −.03*** .008 −.06*** .011 −.09*** .000
R2(Adj.) .08(.07) .15(.13) .15(.17)
***p b .01, **p b .05, *p b .1.

effect. As expected, the effect of the price range (p b .05) is on purchase timing decisions estimated by use of a survival
independent of visualization. Similarly, the results show no model, as in Study 1.
significant direct and indirect effects of the variance on price The survival models in Table 6 show that the direct effects of
expectations, indicating that this effect vanishes under the non- trend and variance are not significant, which indicates that they
graphical condition. Table 6 shows the effects of visualization are mainly driven by the way they are presented to participants.

Table 6
Influence of the visualization of past prices on purchase timing.
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Coefficient St.-error DFa Coefficient St.-error DFa Coefficient St.-error DFa
Price history characteristics
Range −.11 .072 .89 −.13* .072 .87 −.15** .072 .86
Trend −.06 .101 .94 −.08 .101 .92 −.10 .101 .91
Variance −.08 .101 .93 −.07 .101 0.93 −.07 .100 .93
Trend ∗ variance .10 .142 1.10 .08 .142 1.09 .09 .141 1.09

Visualization
Chart vs. table −.29*** .112 0.75 −.28** .112 .75 −.27** .112 .76
Chart ∗ range .11 .099 1.11 .12 .099 1.12 .11 .099 1.12
Chart ∗ trend .34** .139 1.40 .31** .139 1.36 .29** .139 1.33
Chart ∗ variance .38*** .141 1.46 .37*** .140 1.44 .36** .140 1.43
Chart ∗ trend ∗ variance −.33* .199 .72 −.29 .199 .75 −.27 .199 .76

Price expectation
PE4 −.21 .455 .81 – – – – – –
PE8 – – – −.89** .362 .41 – – –
PE12 – – – – – – −.92*** .278 .40

Covariates
Notebook expertise −.08*** .017 .93 −.08*** .017 .92 −.08*** .017 .92
Deal proneness .06*** .017 1.07 .07*** .017 1.07 .06*** .017 1.07
Perceived expensiveness .12*** .019 1.13 .12*** .019 1.12 .12*** .019 1.12
Price chart user −.17*** .057 .84 −.17*** .057 .85 −.15*** .057 .86
Constant 1.89*** .141 6.65 1.85*** .141 6.35 1.83*** .141 6.22
θ (shape parameter) 1.50*** .047 1.51*** .047 1.51*** .047
LL (χ2) − 1006.86 (100.03***) − 1003.83 (106.35***) − 1001.21 (111.58***)
***p b .01, **p b .05, *p b .1.
a = Deferral factor = exp(coeff.).
106 W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109

Indeed, these results confirm that in all three survival models, the i.e., a large amount of price decline, leads to a downward shift in
visualization of the trend (strongly decreasing) and variance price expectations. Further, the influence of price trends is
(high) as a chart increases the tendency to defer the purchase moderated by the variance of the past price series. Overall, the
(interaction terms; p b .05). Hence, hypothesis H5b is supported. results indicate that the strength of the impact of chart
Similar to Study 1, we again find that the variance exerts the characteristics increases with long-term price expectations.
strongest impact on purchase timing decisions. Study 2 shows that the previously discussed effects can partly be
Fig. 4 shows that in the case of a price chart with a strong attributed to the visualization of price history. The visualization
downward trend, the survival model (model 3)predicts that especially exhibits strong effects on consumers' purchase timing
participants would in general defer their purchase by 41.1 % as decisions.
compared to the control group without a price history. Further, the In particular, for a strong downward trend, 16.9 percentage
predictions show that 16.9 percentage points can be attributed to the points of the total effect of the price history is caused by
visualization. In the case of a price chart with a high variance, this visualization, which corresponds to 41.1% of the total effect.
effect becomes much stronger. In particular, 20.3 percentage points Under the condition of high variance, this effect is much stronger
of the total effect can be attributed to the visualization, which is and reaches 20.3 percentage points, which is nearly 50% of the
nearly the half of the total effect. Hence, 50% of the total effect is total effect. Hence, the trend and variance of the price charts are
caused by the price history information and 50% by the the most important chart characteristics.
visualization.
Implications
Summary Study 2
Study 2 reveals that the negative impact of the strong From a consumer's perspective, the results of this study show
downward trend of a notebook's past prices on price expectations that price chart information is especially valuable in situations in
can partly be attributed to its visualization. Furthermore, the which the price history indicates a stronger future decrease and
visualization of the trend and variance exhibits a strong influence high variance of prices than consumers would have expected.
on a consumer's purchase timing decisions. This means that not When charts indicate that prices will further decline, this
only the information about past prices influence consumer information is incorporated into consumer expectations and
decision-making but also the graphical presentation itself. helps them to maximize the transaction utility, i.e., to predict
prices and to purchase when the price meets or falls below the
Discussion future expected price. Hence, from a consumer's perspective
price, charts provided by shopbots are valuable information that
General Findings serves as an anchor to support consumer purchase decisions. For
this reason, the price chart feature is positively evaluated by, for
This research investigates whether price charts regarding a instance, the press when comparing and recommending different
product's price history induce reference price effects and how shopbot sites (SmartMoney 2010). Based on the above discus-
chart characteristics affect consumer price expectations and sion, it is obvious that from a shopbot's perspective, the provision
purchase timing decisions. The results of Study 1 show that the of price charts is worthwhile because it is perceived as containing
price chart is perceived as highly relevant information with valuable information from a consumer's and a public policy
which to form price expectations and to plan purchase time. maker's perspective. Hence, we recommend that shopbots
We find that charts illustrating a product's price history induce provide price charts in order to increase their popularity.
strong reference price effects. This means that consumers adjust From a manufacturer's and retailer's perspective, however,
their prior price expectations according to chart characteristics. consumer reactions to the price chart information can have a serious
Like investors, consumers especially follow price trends in impact on sales and profits. This is because consumers adjust their
predicting prices and planning purchase timing. Besides a strong reference prices depending on the price chart pattern. On the one
downward trend and high variance, a high range of past prices, hand, price charts can help to uphold price expectation levels, but

Fig. 4. The impact of price history visualization on purchase timing.


W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109 107

on the other hand, they can accelerate anticipated price declines. transaction data in a real-world setting. Such a setting would
This problem is especially relevant for durable products with allow researchers to measure the actual effects of purchase
typical life cycle pricing patterns, since price expectations exhibit a postponement on conversion or click rates. Third, it would be
strong influence on price elasticities. Reference price effects interesting to incorporate the effect of price charts in demand
triggered by price chart information should therefore affect price models in order to derive optimal pricing decisions. When
elasticities in the market, which subsequently affect retailer and modeling reference prices through first-order exponential
manufacturer sales. The reason behind this rationale is that price smoothing, a systematic error occurs when a trend is present.
chart information is especially valuable for imitators and followers According to our findings concerning the relevance of price
in the adoption process of durables, since they normally buy at a trends, it would be interesting to model reference prices through
later stage and at a lower price level in a product life cycle. Hence, second-order exponential smoothing. Fourth, since we focus on
durables face increasing absolute price elasticities until the end of the most popular product category on shopbot sites that normally
their product life cycle (Parker and Neelamegham 1997). When experiences significant price decreases over time, we only used
price chart information leads to a change in price expectations, these declining price charts in the experiments. However, future
dynamics affect when manufacturers break even because chart research could extend our research by using categories with
information can lead to a shift in periodic sales due to increased price increases as well as decreases (e.g., airline tickets). Finally, it
strategic planning of the more price sensitive segment of followers would be interesting to investigate the effect of the price chart
in the market. Since this segment is normally responsible for the information on consumer replacement decisions, especially in
bulk of the purchases over the whole life cycle of a product, this technology markets (Gordon 2009).
problem also affects retailer margins because they often acquire
products at later times for a reduced price from the manufacturer.
Nevertheless, it is important to note that retailer competition in Acknowledgements
a shopbot setting especially drives price chart patterns. This is due
to the fact that retailers mostly compete on their rank in the price The authors gratefully acknowledge many helpful comments
comparison table of actual prices since it is possible to gain sales from the editors and the two anonymous reviewers.
by implementing a small price decrease (Iyer and Pazgal 2003;
Smith 2002). Hence, we recommend retailers to balance the effect
of constantly reducing prices at present with the long-term effect Appendix
caused by this strategy on sales and profit development.
In summary, the results of the study show that the initial price
expectations of participants are homogeneous and that depending
on chart characteristics, participants adjust their price expectations.
Coefficient
Manufacturers and retailers should infer from shopbot sites how
alpha
market price expectations develop over time and should incorporate
this information in their dynamic pricing strategy. However, as long Construct Scale items Source Study Study
1 2
as not all consumers use price history information about a product's
price history, there is still some uncertainty about the market price Deal I wait until there is an Roy (1994) .73 .71
proneness advertised sale before
expectations. Hence, it would be useful to make price history going to shop at a mall.
information available to all consumers in the market. I hunt around until I find a
real bargain.
Limitations and Future Research Notebook I regularly use notebooks. Roehm and .73 .77
expertise I am very familiar with Sternthal
notebooks. (2001)
In this study, we find significant effects of price charts on I would call myself a notebook
purchase timing and expected future prices. To isolate the chart expert.
effects, we eliminated dynamic effects such as consumer learning, Shopbot The information provided by Mason et al. .90 .90
brand choice, brand switching and shopbot and retailer switching, information the shopbot was relevant for (2001)
which could also be triggered by viewing the price chart. A first relevancy the purchase timing task.
The information that was
direction for future research would therefore be to assess the provided by the shopbot would
influence of this additional information on consumer behavior help me in making purchase
and on the profitability of shopbots, retailers and manufacturers. It timing decisions.
would also be worthwhile to identify product categories that link The information provided
the strengths of these effects. Further, one could extend future by the shopbot aided me
in completing the
research to another setting in which the supply of the product purchase timing task.
category is limited. For instance, Microsoft's Bing travel website Perceived The actual price Yoo, .85 .80
(http://www.bing.com/travel/) provides graphs of historical prices expensiveness of the notebook is high. Donthu, and
for flight tickets. The goal of this service is to predict air travel The actual price of the Lee (2000)
prices and offer the consumer advice about when to purchase a notebook is expensive.
ticket. Second, it would be useful to analyze a shopbot's Note: All scales ranged from 1 = “Totally disagree” to 7 = “Totally agree”.
108 W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109

References Kwon, Kyoung-Nan and David W. Schumann (2001), “The Influence of


Consumer's Price Expectation on Value Perception and Purchase Intention,”
Advances in Consumer Research, 28, 316–22.
Balachander, Subramanian and Kannan Srinivasan (1998), “Modifying Larkin, Jill H. and Herbert A. Simon (1987), “Why a Diagram Is (sometimes)
Customer Expectations of Price Decreases for a Durable Product,” Worth Ten Thousand Words,” Cognitive Science, 11, 65–99.
Management Science, 44, 6, 776–86. Leigh, William, Naval Modani, Russell Purvis, and Tom Roberts (2002), “Stock
Bayus, Barry L. (1998), “An Analysis of Product Lifetimes in a Technologically Market Trading Rule Discovery Using Technical Charting Heuristics,”
Dynamic Industry,” Management Science, 44, 6, 763–75. Expert Systems with Applications., 23, 155–9.
Benartzi, Shlomo and Richard H. Thaler (1999), “Risk Aversion or Myopia? Lurie, Nicholas H. and Charlotte H. Mason (2007), “Visual Representation:
Choices in Repeated Gambles and Retirement Investments,” Management Implications for Decision Making,” Journal of Marketing 71, January,
Science, 45, 3, 364–82. 160–77.
Bhattacharjee, Sudip, Ram D. Gopal, Kaveepan Lertwachara, R. Marsden Martinez, Eva and Teresa Montaner (2006), “The Effect of Consumer's
James, and Rahul Telang (2007), “The Effect of Digital Sharing Psychographic Variables upon Deal-proneness,” Journal of Retailing and
Technologies on Music Markets: A Survival Analysis of Albums on Consumer Services, 13, 157–68.
Ranking Charts,” Marketing Science, 53, 9, 1359–74. Mason, Kevin, Thomas Jensen, Scot Burton, and Dave Roach (2001), “The
Biswas, Abhijit and Daniel L. Sherrell (1993), “The Influence of Product Accuracy of Brand and Attribute Judgments: The Role of Information
Knowledge and Brand Name on Internal Price Standards and Confidence,” Relevancy, Product Experience, and Attribute-Relationship Schemata,”
Psychology and Marketing, 10, 1, 31–46. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 29, 3, 308–18.
Bradburn, M.J., T.G. Clark, S.B. Love, and D.G. Altmann (2003), “Survival Mazumdar, Tridib, S.P. Raj, and Indrajit Sinha (2005), “Reference Price
Analysis Part II: Multivariate Data Analysis — An Introduction to Concepts Research: Review and Propositions,” Journal of Marketing, 69, October,
and Methods,” British Journal of Cancer, 89, 431–6. 84–102.
Brealey, Richard A., Stewart C. Myers, and Franklin Allen (2007), Principles of Monroe, Kent B. (2003), Pricing, Making Profitable Decisions. New York:
Corporate Finance. 8th.ed. New York: Irwin. McGraw Hill.
Bridges, Eileen, Chi Kin B. Yim, and Richard A. Briesch (1995), “A High-Tech Mussweiler, Thomas and Karl Schneller (2003), “What Goes Up Must Come
Product Market Share Model with Costumer Expectations,” Marketing Science, Down — How Charts Influence Decisions to Buy and Sell Stocks,” Journal
14, 1, 61–81. of Behavioral Finance, 4, 3, 121–30.
Caginalp, Gundaz and Donald Balenovich (1996), “Based Asset Flow in Technical Nielsen Netratings (2007), “What's Driving Online Holiday Shoppers this
Analysis and Securities Marketing,” Psychology and Marketing, 13, 4, 407–44. Year?,” (Accessed February 10, 2010 from http://www.nielsen-online.com/
Danziger, Shai and Ruthie Segev (2006), “The Effects of Informative and Non- pr/pr_071120.pdf.)
informative Price Patterns on Consumer Price Judgments,” Psychology and Ofir, Chezy and Russel S. Winer (2002), “Pricing: Economic and Behavioral
Marketing, 23, 6, 535–53. Models,” in Handbook of Marketing, Barton Weitz and Robin Wensley, eds,
Darke, Peter R., Shelly Chaiken, and Jonathan L. Freedman (1995), “Percentage London: Sage Publications Ltd., 267–81.
Discounts, Initial Price, and Bargain Hunting: A Heuristic–Systematic Approach Parducci, Allen (1965), “Category Judgment: A Range–Frequency Model,”
to Price Search Behavior,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 5, 580–6. Psychological Review 72, November, 407–18.
——— and Cindy M.Y. Chung (2005), “Effects of Pricing and Promotion on Park, Cheol-Ho and Scott H. Irwin (2007), “What Do We Know About the
Consumer Perceptions: It Depends on How You Frame it,” Journal of Profitability of Technical Analysis?" Journal of Economic Surveys, 27, 4,
Retailing, 1, 35–47. 786–826.
Doyle, Peter and John Saunders (1985), “The Lead Effects of Marketing Parker, Philip and Ramya Neelamegham (1997), “Price Elasticity Dynamics Over the
Decisions,” Journal of Marketing Research, 22, 54–65 (February). Product Life Cycle: A Study of Consumer Durables,” Marketing Letters, 8, 2,
Erdem, Tülin, Michael P. Keane, T. Sabri Öncü, and Judi Strebel (2005), “Learning 205–16.
about Computers: An Analysis of Information Search and Technology Rao, Akshay R. and Wanda A. Sieben (1992), “The Effect of Prior Knowledge
Choice,” Quantitative Marketing and Economics, 3, 3, 207–47. on Price Acceptability and the Type of Information Examined,” Journal of
Gordon, Brett R. (2009), “A Dynamic Model of Consumer Replacement Cycles Consumer Research, 19, 256–70.
in the PC Processor Industry,” Marketing Science, 28, 5, 846–67. Roehm, Michelle L. and Brian Sternthal (2001), “The Moderating Effect of
Guide, Jr.V., R. Daniel, Luc Muyldermans, and Luk N. Van Wassenhove Knowledge and Resources on the Persuasive Impact of Analogies,” Journal
(2005), “Hewiett-Packard company unlocks the value potential from time- of Consumer Research, 28, 2, 257–72.
sensitive returns,” Interfaces, 35, 4, 281–93. Roy, Abhik (1994), “Correlates of Mall Visit Frequency,” Journal of Retailing,
Häubl, Gerald and Valerie Trifts (2000), “Consumer Decision Making in Online 70, 2, 139–61.
Shopping Environments: The Effects of Interactive Decision Aids,” Seetharaman, P.B. and Pradeep K. Chintagunta (2003), “The Proportional
Marketing Science, 19, 1, 4–21. Hazard Model for Purchase Timing: A Comparison of Alternative
Helsen, Kristiaan and David C. Schmittlein (1993), “Analyzing Duration Times Specifications,” Journal of Business & Economic Statistics, 21, 3,
in Marketing: Evidence for the Effectiveness of Hazard Rate Models,” 368–82.
Marketing Science, 12, 4, 395–414. SmartMoney (2010), “Which Comparison Shopping Site Is Best?,” (Accessed
Iyer, Ganesh and Amit Pazgal (2003), “Internet Shopping Agents: Virtual Co- February 10, 2010 from http://www.smartmoney.com/spending/deals/
location and Competition,” Marketing Science, 22, 1, 85–106. which-comparison-shopping-site-is-best/.)
Janiszewski, Chris and Donald R. Lichtenstein (1999), “A Range Theory Smith, Michael D. (2002), “The Impact of Shopbots on Electronic Markets,”
Account of Price Perception,” Journal of Consumer Research, 25, 353–68. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 30, 4, 446–54.
Kalwani, Manohar U., Chi Kin Yim, Heikki J. Rinne, and Yoshi Sugita (1990), Sujan, Mita, James R. Bettman, and Harish Sujan (1986), “Effects of Consumer
“A Price Expectations Model of Customer Brand Choice,” Journal of Expectations on Information Processing in Selling Encounters,” Journal of
Marketing Research, 27, 3, 251–62. Marketing Research, 23, 4, 346–53.
——— and ——— (1992), “Consumer Price and Promotion Expectations: An Tabachnik, Barbara G. and Linda S. Fidell (2007), Experimental Design Using
Experimental Study,” Journal of Marketing Research 29, February, 90–100. ANOVA. Belmont: Thompson.
Kalyanaram, Gurumurthy and John D.C. Little (1994), “An Empirical Analysis Trifts, Valerie and Gerald Häubl (2003), “Information Availability and Consumer
of Latitude of Price Acceptance in Consumer Package Goods,” Journal of Preference: Can Online Retailers Benefit from Providing Access to Competitor
Consumer Research, 21, 3, 408–18. Price Information,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 1&2, 149–59.
Kopalle, Praveen K. and Joan Lindsey-Mullikin (2003), “The Impact of External Urbany, Joel E., William O. Bearden, and Dan C. Weilbaker (1988), “The Effect
Reference Price on Consumer Price Expectations,” Journal of Retailing, 79, of Plausible and Exaggerated Reference Prices on Consumer Perceptions
225–36. and Price Search,” Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 95–271.
W. Drechsler, M. Natter / Journal of Interactive Marketing 25 (2011) 95–109 109

Vessey, Iris (1991), “Cognitive Fit: A Theory-based Analysis of the Graphs Winer, Russell S. (1985), “A Price Vector Model of Demand for Consumer
versus Tables Literature,” Decision Sciences, 22, 2, 219–40. Durables: Preliminary Developments,” Marketing Science, 4, 1, 74–90.
Volkmann, John (1951), “Scales of Judgment and their Implication for Social Yadav, Manjit S. and Kathleen Seiders (1998), “Is the Price Right?
Psychology,” in Social Psychology at the Crossroads, John H. Rohrer and Understanding Contingent Processing in Reference Price Formation,”
Muzafer Sherif, eds. NewYork: Harper & Row, 273–94. Journal of Retailing, 74, 311–29.
Washburne, John N. (1927), “An Experimental Study of Various Graphic, Yoo, Boonghee, Naveen Donthu, and Sungho Lee (2000), “An Examination of
Tabular and Textual Methods of Presenting Quantitative Material,” Journal Selected Marketing Mix Elements and Brand Equity,” Journal of the
of Educational Psychology, 18, 6, 361–76. Academy of Marketing Science, 28, 2, 195–211.

You might also like