You are on page 1of 25

622175

research-article2016
EROXXX10.1177/2332858415622175Grissom and ReddingDiscretion and Disproportionality

AERA Open
January-March 2016, Vol. 2, No. 1, pp. 1­–25
DOI: 10.1177/2332858415622175
© The Author(s) 2016. http://ero.sagepub.com

Discretion and Disproportionality: Explaining the Underrepresentation


of High-Achieving Students of Color in Gifted Programs

Jason A. Grissom
Christopher Redding
Vanderbilt University

Students of color are underrepresented in gifted programs relative to White students, but the reasons for this underrepresen-
tation are poorly understood. We investigate the predictors of gifted assignment using nationally representative, longitudinal
data on elementary students. We document that even among students with high standardized test scores, Black students are
less likely to be assigned to gifted services in both math and reading, a pattern that persists when controlling for other back-
ground factors, such as health and socioeconomic status, and characteristics of classrooms and schools. We then investigate
the role of teacher discretion, leveraging research from political science suggesting that clients of government services from
traditionally underrepresented groups benefit from diversity in the providers of those services, including teachers. Even after
conditioning on test scores and other factors, Black students indeed are referred to gifted programs, particularly in reading,
at significantly lower rates when taught by non-Black teachers, a concerning result given the relatively low incidence of
assignment to own-race teachers among Black students.

Keywords: gifted education, equity, teachers, race, school context

Substantial race disparities exist in student receipt of gifted lower achievement among Black and Hispanic students, for
education services in American schools. Data from the Office example, might contribute to underrepresentation in gifted
for Civil Rights (OCR) at the U.S. Department of Education programs, particularly in systems with narrower, achieve-
reveal that as of 2009, African American students constitute ment-centered definitions of giftedness (Ford, 1998). Lower
16.7% of the student population but just 9.8% of students in social and financial capital can mean that racial- and ethnic-
gifted programs. Similarly, Hispanic students constitute minority families may have less access to information about
22.3% of students but only 15.4% of students receiving gifted identification processes or to private psychologists or others
services (U.S. Department of Education, 2010). These dis- who can test them for giftedness outside the school
proportionalities have sparked concern among researchers (Mickelson, 2003). Furthermore, Black students are less
and the media not only because they represent potentially likely to attend schools with gifted programs. Data from the
unfair or inequitable treatment of minority students (Baker, Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten cohort
2013; Ford, 1998) but because studies have linked participa- (ECLS-K)—the data source for this study—show that 90%
tion in gifted programs to positive future outcomes, including of White, 93% of Hispanic, and 91% of Asian elementary
increased academic performance (Bhatt, 2009; Card & school students attend a school with a gifted program, com-
Giuliano, 2014; Delcourt, Cornell, & Goldberg, 2007; pared to only 83% of African American students. Within
Goldring, 1990; C. Kulik & Kulik, 1982; Rogers, 2007) and schools, students of color generally are less likely than White
improvements in such domains as motivation, self-efficacy, students to be identified even when they satisfy criteria for
engagement with learning, nonacademic self-concept, and gifted services (Ford, Grantham, & Whiting, 2008; McBee,
overall stress (e.g., C. Kulik & Kulik, 1982; J. Kulik & Kulik, 2006). Researchers have identified teacher discretion in the
1984; Marsh, Chessor, Craven, & Roche, 1995; Rogers, gifted assignment process as a potentially important con-
2007).1 Furthermore, disparities in gifted identification may tributor to this inequity. Because the process often begins
contribute to within-school segregation of students on the with teacher referral, classroom teachers can play a gate-
basis of race and ethnicity, with consequences for both non- keeping role in gifted assignments (Donovan & Cross,
White and White students (Darity & Jolla, 2009). 2002). Reliance on teacher referrals can disadvantage stu-
Scholars have attributed racial and ethnic disparities in dents of color if teachers hold lower expectations for them or
gifted program participation to a variety of factors. Generally are less likely to recognize giftedness in such students

Creative Commons CC-BY: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License (http://
www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further
permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-
at-sage).
Grissom and Redding

(Elhoweris, Mutua, Alsheikh, & Holloway, 2005; Ford & We use the ECLS-K to model student assignment to
Grantham, 2003). gifted services. The ECLS-K contains data on a nationally
To political scientists who study bureaucratic representa- representative cohort of students who were in kindergarten
tion—that is, the idea that who the providers of government during the 1998–1999 academic year, following up with
services are matters for how policy outputs are distributed them during first, third, fifth, and eighth grades. These
among client populations (Mosher, 1968)—contexts in data have a number of features that allow us to push the
which bureaucrats exercise substantial discretion and in research base forward substantially. First, the data are at
which race has high policy salience, as it does in education, the student level, allowing us both to model the movement
create conditions in which bureaucrat race often influences of individual students into gifted programs and to account
how populations benefit from government services. Research for a large number of important student covariates in the
in contexts as varied as the Farmers Home Administration, models, including student achievement in math and read-
social welfare agencies, and local law enforcement has ing. These data permit for a finer-grained examination of
found evidence that client populations of color receive more gifted assignment, demographic gaps in gifted assignment,
positive treatment from government agencies or organiza- and the factors that contribute to these demographic gaps
tions staffed by bureaucrats from their same racial or ethnic on a national scale than has been conducted in much prior
background (e.g., Theobald & Haider-Markel, 2009; Selden, work. Second, students are linked to their classroom teach-
1997). For example, Hindera’s (1993) study of the Equal ers and teachers’ characteristics, which permits us to test
Employment Opportunity Commission finds that district directly for interactions between student and teacher race
offices employing Black investigators are more likely to or ethnicity in predicting the movement of students into
pursue charges of employment discrimination on behalf of gifted programs.
Black complainants (and similarly for Hispanic investiga- We ask the following research questions. First, to what
tors and Hispanic complainants). Similarly, in a study of extent are African American and Hispanic elementary stu-
Florida traffic stop data, Close and Mason (2006) find that dents assigned to gifted programs at disproportionately
African American and Latino/a drivers are treated less puni- lower rates than White or Asian students? Second, to what
tively by officers of the same race/ethnicity than they are by extent is this disproportionality explained by other observ-
White officers. In the context of gifted assignment, bureau- able student characteristics, such as parental education or
cratic representation theory suggests that teachers of color achievement tests scores? Third, does assignment to an own-
are more likely than White teachers to exercise discretion on race teacher increase a non-White student’s probability of
behalf of students from their same racial or ethnic back- being assigned to gifted services?2
ground—and similarly for White teachers and White stu- Our study is motivated by prior work on racial/ethnic dis-
dents—such that students’ probabilities of being assigned parities in the composition of gifted programs and research
are higher with own-race teachers. Researchers have, in fact, on bureaucratic representation, which emphasizes how
provided suggestive evidence of such a relationship, finding diversity in the providers of public services, including teach-
that Black and Hispanic students are better represented in ers, matters for ameliorating those disparities (see Grissom,
gifted-and-talented programs in schools that employ larger Kern, & Rodriguez, 2015; Grissom, Rodriguez, & Kern, in
numbers of Black and Hispanic teachers (e.g., Nicholson- press). The next two sections discuss prior research in these
Crotty, Grissom, & Nicholson-Crotty, 2011; Rocha & areas. We then describe the data and methodological
Hawes, 2009). approaches to answering our research questions before pre-
These studies, however, have relied exclusively on aggre- senting our results. The final section provides a discussion of
gate (school-level) data on both student and teacher demo- the implications of the results for policy, practice, and future
graphics, limiting the research in a number of key ways. work in this domain.
First, reliance on school-level data to model an individual
phenomenon—such as race-related discretion in student
Student Race, Teacher Discretion, and Assignment to
referrals to gifted programs—raises significant concerns
Gifted Programs
about the potential for aggregation bias or ecological falla-
cies. Second, and relatedly, use of data on schools rather Researchers have recognized racial and ethnic dispropor-
than students means that important student-level determi- tionalities in gifted services for some time. In 1998, surveys
nants of gifted assignment, including measures of student from the Office for Civil Rights found that 6.2% of students
academic ability, cannot be considered or controlled for in were placed in gifted programs. This fraction includes nearly
the models. Third, modeling the presence of students in pro- 10% of Asian students, 7.5% of White students, 3.6% of
grams rather than the assignment process itself prevents Hispanic students, and 3.0% of Black students (Donovan &
drawing conclusions about what factors lead different kinds Cross, 2002). By 2009, the fraction of students in gifted pro-
of students to transition into gifted services. grams had increased slightly to 6.6%, as had the fractions of

2
Discretion and Disproportionality

students in underrepresented groups; the fraction of gifted groups of students even within the same school or class-
students who were Black was 3.9%, and Hispanic, 4.6% room. For example, racialized teacher perceptions may
(U.S. Department of Education, 2010). Despite these lead teachers to misinterpret Black or Hispanic students’
improvements, however, substantial disproportionalities in behavior because of different cultural backgrounds; what a
gifted assignment by race and ethnicity persist. teacher may attribute to precocity for one student may be
Numerous studies have explored the underrepresentation considered disruptive behavior for another (Ferguson,
of African American students in particular in an attempt to 1998). Furthermore, rigorous or valid assessment tools
elucidate contributing factors (Anguino, 2003; Harris, may not be mandated or utilized, and teachers often are
Brown, Ford, & Richardson, 2003; Kearns, Ford, & Linney, not trained on strategies for identifying gifted students
2004). Ford (1998) outlines several categories of factors (Donovan & Cross, 2002), increasing the influence of
that influence the disproportionate assignment of Black and teacher discretion in the screening and referral process.
Hispanic students to gifted programs. The first set of factors
points to educational inequities associated with under-
Linking Teacher Race/Ethnicity to Assignment to
achievement among students from traditionally underserved
Gifted Services
racial/ethnic groups, which, in turn, reduce the probability
of gifted identification. For example, students of color sys- The major role for teacher discretion in the screening
tematically are taught by less qualified, less effective teach- and referral processes for gifted assignments has prompted
ers (e.g., Clotfelter, Ladd, & Vigdor, 2005, 2010; DeMonte scholars to consider possible school-based mechanisms
& Hanna, 2014). Tracking (Oakes, 1990) and ability group- that influence assignment to gifted programs. A sizeable
ing (Gamoran, Nystrand, Berends, & LePore, 1995) sepa- body of research in the fields of political science and public
rate students by academic and nonacademic factors in ways administration has used the theory of bureaucratic repre-
that can disadvantage the learning opportunities of students sentation to examine how the composition of the school
of color. workforce may help explain patterns in minority student
The other factors Ford (1998) identifies pertain to representation in gifted programs across schools. This
the processes schools use for identification and screening research, however, has been less visible to education schol-
of gifted students and the personnel who determine assign- ars (Grissom et al., 2015). Normative work on the “repre-
ment. Identification often begins with a classroom teacher’s sentative bureaucracy” argues that workers in public
use of checklists, rating scales, informal recommenda- institutions, including schools, should reflect societal
tions, and cognitive assessments to document a student’s diversity—that is, be descriptively representative—not
academic capability and potential and ends with a referral only to have legitimacy with the public but to help ensure
for further evaluation (Donovan & Cross, 2002). Next, that client populations are treated equitably (Kingsley,
teachers or other school staff formally evaluate students 1944; Lim, 2006; Mosher, 1968). In this literature, descrip-
using tools based on the district or state’s definition of tive representation has most often been discussed in terms
giftedness. In all but one state, the main criteria for gifted- of race or ethnicity—unsurprising given a long history of
ness is academic performance (Donovan & Cross, 2002), discriminatory treatment of racial- and ethnic-minority
underscoring the importance of taking student academic populations by government in the United States and else-
achievement into account in predicting a students’ proba- where—but also in terms of gender, religion, sexual iden-
bility of gifted assignment. Yet states increasingly have tity, and other characteristics (Kennedy, 2013). A large
embraced broader understandings of giftedness as well, body of empirical research has demonstrated that public
employing a “multiple criteria method” that emphasizes agencies composed of more racially and ethnically repre-
such factors as student creativity, artistic ability, or leader- sentative workforces distribute policy outputs more equita-
ship. Scholars and advocates have supported the transition bly among minority and nonminority client populations
to a more holistic evaluation because of the potentially (e.g., Hindera, 1993; Meier, Stewart, & England, 1989;
detrimental impact on gifted identification of lower scores Nicholson-Crotty et al., 2011; Selden, 1997; Theobald &
on cognitive assessments for African American and Haider-Markel, 2009).
Hispanic students (Joseph & Ford, 2006). The use of such The mechanisms linking descriptive representation to the
criteria, however, also provides teachers with greater dis- distribution of policy outputs are less clear, however. Much
cretion in assignment. As all states rely on teacher refer- of the literature has focused on so-called active representa-
rals and input in assignment, teacher perceptions may tion, the idea that minority bureaucrats—or organizations
influence outcomes at numerous points in the process with larger numbers of minority bureaucrats—behave differ-
(Nicholson-Crotty et al., 2011). To the extent that teacher ently in ways that produce benefits for minority client popu-
perceptions of students are affected by race or ethnicity, lations. A necessary condition for active representation to
this discretion may lead to unequal treatment of different occur is bureaucratic discretion: Bureaucrats must have

3
Grissom and Redding

direct or indirect influence over the distribution of policy scholars have used gifted representation as an outcome for
goods or services (Meier, 1993). These differential behaviors testing the tenets of bureaucratic representation theory in
are thought to be rooted in shared values and backgrounds numerous studies. Research has, in fact, provided suggestive
that may make minority bureaucrats more attuned to past dif- evidence of such a relationship, finding that Black and
ferential treatment or present needs of minority constituen- Hispanic students are better represented in gifted-and-tal-
cies (Keiser, Wilkins, Meier, & Holland, 2002; Meier, 1993). ented programs in schools that employ larger numbers of
They may, for example, exercise partiality on behalf of cli- Black and Hispanic teachers (e.g., Grissom et al., 2015, in
ents “like them” or advocate for changes in institutional pol- press; Grissom, Nicholson-Crotty, & Nicholson-Crotty,
icy or practices that make the institution more sensitive to 2009; Nicholson-Crotty et al., 2011; Meier & Stewart, 1992;
minority interests or emphasize fairness or equity more gen- Rocha & Hawes, 2009).
erally (Dolan & Rosenbloom, 2003; Meier, 1993). In the con- Although suggestive, previous studies linking teacher
text of gifted assignments, active representation might occur race or ethnicity to differential assignment of students to
when minority teachers recommend minority students for gifted services faces a key drawback: reliance on school-
gifted screening at higher rates or push for universal screen- level aggregate data. Essentially, these studies identify a
ing processes in their schools that help ameliorate gaps that correlation between the proportion of students from a given
might occur when minority student giftedness is recognized race or ethnic group and the proportion of teachers in the
less often through “subjective” channels. school from that group, adjusted for other school-level
As the bureaucratic representation literature has matured, covariates. Yet without individual data on students and their
it has begun to recognize other potential mechanisms linking teachers, these studies have been unable to model the
descriptive representation to policy outputs. For example, assignment process or account for important student-level
minority bureaucrats may be better positioned to push characteristics that may confound these relationships,
minority clients to increase effort or make other behavioral including student academic achievement. Our analysis steps
changes that improve their own outcomes. The presence of into these gaps.
minority teachers may produce role-modeling effects, for
example, that raise student performance or increase outward
Data and Methods
expression of talent in ways that increase the probability of
gifted assignment. To this point, several prior studies have The data in this study come from the restricted-use version
demonstrated that Black and Hispanic students perform bet- of the ECLS-K, a nationally representative sample of 21,260
ter on tests when taught by own-race teachers (e.g., Aaronson, kindergarteners in the 1998–1999 school year followed by
Barrow, & Sander, 2007; Dee, 2004). the National Center for Educational Statistics (NCES)
Perhaps even more important, representation scholars through eighth grade, gathering data on them at intervals
posit that the client population itself may be more willing to (Tourangeau et al., 2006). This study uses data collected in
engage with the organization in beneficial ways because of the elementary grades—that is, data collected in the spring of
the presence of bureaucrats with whom they can more easily kindergarten and first, third, and fifth grades—when most
communicate or identify (Meier & Nicholson-Crotty, 2006). gifted students are identified. We further restrict the analysis
In schools, the cultural capital of White middle-class parents sample to public schools that report having a gifted program
aids in facilitating relationships with teachers and school in either reading or math.3 Schools without gifted programs
personnel and connecting them to information about school were excluded to avoid conflating variation in student assign-
processes, providing them with an advantage over non- ment with other between-school factors, such as school
White families in influencing their children’s schooling out- resources, that might predict the absence of gifted services.
comes (Lareau, 1987; Lareau & Horvat, 1999). The presence Across the survey waves, between 33% and 38% of pub-
of non-White teachers may help non-White parents over- lic schools report having no gifted program, which limits the
come those disadvantages by, for example, helping parents analysis sample to a maximum of 10,640 students in kinder-
feel more comfortable advocating for their children’s needs garten, 9,120 in first grade, 8,250 in third grade, and 7,000 in
or making it more likely that they tap into school informa- fifth grade. Missing data further reduced the available sam-
tion networks. In the context of gifted assignment, a non- ple sizes as the ECLS-K data collection progressed. These
White parent may feel more at ease requesting from a missing data can be attributed variously to parental refusal to
non-White teacher that his or her child be screened or seek- further participate in the study, missing assessment data, and
ing out advice or information from that teacher about how to incomplete data from classroom teachers or school person-
obtain gifted services. nel.4 Given these sample size reductions, we utilized multi-
Because of the numerous potential mechanisms linking ple imputation.5 For all estimates, we used longitudinal
descriptive representation in the teacher workforce to stu- weights provided by NCES in all analyses to recover popu-
dent representation in gifted programs, it is perhaps unsur- lation estimates. The largest analytic sample contains 14,280
prising that political scientists and public administration observations.

4
Discretion and Disproportionality

Measuring Assignment to Gifted Programs analysis (Pollack, Narajian, Rock, Atkins-Burnett, & Hausken,
2005).9 We standardize scores within year.
The dependent variable for this analysis is the student’s
Student characteristics include the child’s race and eth-
assignment to a gifted program in either reading or mathe-
nicity, gender, a scale measure of socioeconomic status
matics. This variable is measured through teachers’ response
(SES), a measure of the parent’s report of the child’s health
to the question, “Does this child receive instruction and/or
status at entry to kindergarten, and the child’s age in
related services in any of the following types of programs in
months as of September of 1998, approximately the start
your school day?” Teachers designated whether or not a stu-
of kindergarten. The SES measure is computed by NCES
dent was in a gifted-and-talented program in reading or
and combines mother’s and father’s education level, status
math.6 To simulate the assignment process, we assume that a
of mother’s and father’s occupation, and household
student first identified for gifted services at time t does not
income, imputing missing values and standardizing and
begin receiving them until time t + 1. Thus, our analysis con-
averaging across measures (Tourangeau et al., 2006). The
siders the probability of being assigned to gifted services in
child health measure is a subjective measure rated on a
the next survey period, conditional on not being assigned in
5-point scale (excellent to poor), which we reverse-coded
the present period.7 For example, we label a student as having
and standardized.
been assigned in kindergarten if he or she was not identified
Teacher characteristics include race/ethnicity, years of
as gifted during kindergarten data collection but was identi-
experience in the teacher’s current school, and indicators for
fied in the first-grade data collection. Similarly, a student is
whether or not the teacher has a master’s degree or is certi-
labeled as having been identified in first grade if he or she
fied. Classroom characteristics include class size and class-
was not gifted in first grade but was in third grade and as
room averages for the percentages of Black, Hispanic, and
identified in third grade if he or she was not in gifted in third
Asian students plus mean student SES.10 These classroom-
grade but was as of fifth grade. A consequence of this speci-
level variables rely on having sufficient respondents within
fication is the uncertainty of conditions in second or fourth
a classroom to accurately reflect the classroom composition.
grade that may have also influenced student assignment that
In the initial survey years, when respondents were more
are not accounted for given the lack of survey data for these
highly concentrated in classrooms, these measures likely
periods.
have greater validity. Finally, school characteristics include
indicators for whether the school is suburban or urban (with
Student and Teacher Race and Ethnicity rural as the omitted category); region of the country (four
Central to research on representation in the bureaucracy regions; East is the omitted category); school enrollment
is shared demographic characteristics between clients and size; the fraction of Black, Hispanic, and Asian students
street-level bureaucrats—in this case, students and teach- within a school; school free or reduced-price lunch (FRPL)
ers. ECLS-K provides categorical information on student rate; and the school average test performance. Accounting
racial and ethnic identification from the parent interview for school average achievement in particular is important if
data in the initial year of data collection. Information on teachers’ or others’ perceptions of a student’s performance
the race and ethnicity for the student’s classroom teacher and qualification for gifted services is relative to other stu-
was collected from teacher survey self-reports during each dents in that setting (Hibel, Farkas, & Morgan, 2010). A cor-
survey period. Because elementary school students are relation matrix for all control variables appears in Appendix
very likely to be in self-contained classes, the potential for Table A1.
mismeasurement from having a student assigned to mul-
tiple teachers was not of large concern. To verify that stu- Regression Analysis
dents in the study indeed were in self-contained classes,
we examined the frequency with which different teachers To analyze the relationship between assignment to
completed either the dedicated math or reading teacher gifted programs and student–teacher race congruence, a
survey and found that in kindergarten and first and third series of models are estimated that consider the probability
grades, this happened less than 1% of the time in each that a student will be assigned to a gifted program in the
year.8 next period for all students not currently in a gifted pro-
gram, with controls for student, teacher, and school charac-
teristics. Equation 1 shows the general form of these
School and Teacher Characteristics models:
ECLS-K provides extensive information pertaining to the
students’ academic performance, family background, and Pr(gifted)ijt+1 = β0 + β1Rijt + β2Cit + β3Tit + β4Sit + γt + ϵit, (1)
classroom and school context. We use the criterion-referenced
composite scale scores for the math and reading achievement where Cit is a vector of the child’s characteristics for student
tests, which have been vertically equated for longitudinal i in year t, Tit is a vector of characteristics of the student’s

5
Grissom and Redding

teacher and classroom, and Sitis a vector of school character- student, teacher/classroom, and school factors that may be
istics in a given year. A wave fixed effect γt is included to associated with assignment to gifted programs. Two-sided t
control for time-specific correlates of assignment to gifted tests for differences in means were conducted comparing
services. The dependent variable is a binary indicator set White students with African American, Hispanic, and Asian
equal to 1 if a student is assigned to gifted services by the students, respectively.
next survey period and 0 if not, conditional on not currently There are numerous notable differences across groups.
being assigned. The vector R includes an indicator for First, in any given survey wave, an average of 5.3% of
whether the student is racially/ethnically congruent with his White students in schools with gifted programs are assigned
or her teacher plus a series of interactions between the con- to gifted services, compared to 2.2% of Black students,
gruence indicator and student race to test whether racial/eth- 3.5% of Hispanic students, and 6.2% of Asian students.13
nic congruence is differentially associated with gifted We also see differences in other student characteristics by
assignment by student demographic group. Because of con- race and ethnicity. Black students perform nearly half a
cerns that racial/ethnic congruence and same-year test scores standard deviation lower than White students on the read-
may be endogenous if teacher race/ethnicity affects student ing assessment (0.14 vs. –0.30) and nearly two thirds of a
academic performance (e.g., Dee, 2004), our preferred standard deviation lower on the math assessment (0.21 vs.
model includes lagged test score rather than the current-year –0.41). Hispanic students score slightly less than half a
score in C, though using the current-year score results in standard deviation lower than White students in reading
substantively similar conclusions (see Appendix Tables A2 and math. There are no statistically significant differences
and A4). All models are estimated using logistic regression in the reading and math test performance of Asian students
with standard errors clustered at the student level to account and White students. Differences for SES are similar. Both
for multiple observations of the same student across time. Black and Hispanic students are half a standard deviation
A limitation of this approach is that it does not take into lower on the scale measure of SES.
account unobserved school characteristics, such as the Patterns in teacher race and ethnicity show much differ-
school’s gifted referral and evaluation process, that may ent likelihoods of exposure to a same-race teacher in ele-
affect a student’s probability of being assigned to gifted ser- mentary school for students of different backgrounds. As
vices. A typical strategy for accounting for such factors is to Table 1 shows, 91% of teachers in the sample are White,
include a school fixed effect, which would estimate the so it is unsurprising that White students are much more
impact of teacher race congruence or other factors on gifted likely than other students to find themselves in a class-
assignment by comparing assignment patterns across class- room with a race-congruent teacher. In fact, for White stu-
rooms within the same school. Unfortunately, the infre- dents, the rate of teacher race congruence is 95%. In
quency of gifted assignment in the data—and, for the race contrast, for Black students in elementary school, race
congruence analysis, the lack of variation in teacher race congruence with one’s teacher in occurs only about 22%
across classrooms—makes a school fixed-effect approach of the time, and for Hispanic students, 20% of the time.
infeasible.11 Asian students have the lowest rate of congruence, only
9%. These patterns bolster our interest in testing whether
assignment to an own-race teacher is associated with dif-
Descriptive Analysis
ferential gifted assignment.
Descriptive statistics for the main variables we consider We also examine differences by teacher qualifications,
are shown in Table 1. The first row shows what fraction of including teaching experience in the school, degree level,
students were assigned to gifted services for that grade who and certification status. Teachers of White students have
had not previously been assigned, with the next two rows been in their schools the longest, on average; followed by
showing assignment in reading and math separately. The Asian, Hispanic, and Black students, whose teachers have
fraction increases at each grade level but totals approxi- approximately a year and a half fewer years in their
mately 5% of students (4% each in reading and math).12 schools than those of White students. White and Asian stu-
Figure 1 breaks gifted assignment down by race/ethnicity dents’ teachers also have higher educational attainment.
and grade. Consistent with prior research, White and Asian Approximately 40% of these teachers have a master’s degree
students are assigned to gifted services at higher rates than or above, compared to 33% for Black students and 34% for
Black and Hispanic students at each grade level. Hispanic students. Black and Hispanic students are also less
likely to be taught by a fully certified teacher. Asian students
are enrolled in slightly larger classes than White students, on
Comparing Gifted Assignment and Potential Predictors by
average. Black and Hispanic students’ peers are of signifi-
Student Race
cantly lower SES relative to White students.
In Table 2, we provide a comparison by race and ethnicity The bottom section of the table compares school charac-
first of proportion in gifted programs and then of potential teristics by student race and ethnicity. Black, Hispanic, and

6
Table 1
Mean Sample Characteristics, by Grade Level

Characteristic Kindergarten First grade Third grade All years

Students assigned to gifted 0.03 0.03 0.08 0.05


  Assigned to gifted in reading 0.03 0.03 0.07 0.04
  Assigned to gifted in math 0.03 0.02 0.06 0.04
Race congruence  
  White Student × White Teacher 0.65 0.63 0.62 0.63
  Black Student × Black Teacher 0.02 0.03 0.03 0.03
  Hispanic Student × Hispanic Teacher 0.02 0.03 0.04 0.03
  Asian Student × Asian Teacher <0.01 <0.01 <0.01 <0.01
Student characteristics  
  Reading test (standardized) −0.05 0.02 −0.01 −0.00
  Math test (standardized) 0.02 0.06 −0.01 0.03
 Female 0.49 0.50 0.50 0.49
 White 0.68 0.66 0.65 0.66
 Black 0.15 0.14 0.15 0.15
 Hispanic 0.14 0.17 0.18 0.17
 Asian 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.03
 SES −0.05 −0.09 −0.13 −0.10
  Parent’s health rating −0.03 −0.03 −0.03 −0.02
  Age in months at start of kindergarten 66.46 66.58 66.43 66.36
Teacher/classroom characteristics  
 White 0.93 0.91 0.89 0.91
 Black 0.05 0.07 0.07 0.06
 Hispanic 0.03 0.04 0.05 0.04
 Asian 0.01 0.02 0.01 0.01
  Teaching experience (in current school) 9.66 9.08 9.84 9.59
  Master’s degree 0.38 0.38 0.43 0.39
 Certified 0.93 0.93 0.92 0.93
  Class size 21.01 20.36 21.18 20.80
  Class mean SES −0.06 −0.09 −0.11 −0.09
  Class percentage Black 0.15 0.14 0.14 0.15
  Class percentage Hispanic 0.14 0.16 0.17 0.16
  Class percentage Asian 0.05 0.05 0.04 0.04
School characteristics  
 Urban 0.30 0.23 0.26 0.27
 Rural 0.25 0.29 0.30 0.27
 Northeast 0.12 0.13 0.11 0.12
 Midwest 0.22 0.22 0.24 0.23
 South 0.48 0.44 0.46 0.46
 West 0.18 0.21 0.19 0.19
  School size (100s) 5.61 5.62 5.42 5.52
  Fraction White students 0.69 0.67 0.66 0.66
  Fraction Black students 0.15 0.15 0.14 0.15
  Fraction Hispanic students 0.11 0.13 0.15 0.13
  Fraction Asian students 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.03
  School percentage FRPL 0.34 0.32 0.35 0.34
  School mean test score 29.65 49.41 95.92 57.48
Observations 7,190 5,910 5,130 15,130

Note. Estimates adjusted using Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten cohort probability weights. Public schools with gifted programs only.
Sample sizes rounded in accordance with National Center for Education Statistics nondisclosure rules. SES = socioeconomic status; FRPL = free or reduced-
price lunch.

7
Figure 1.  Students assigned to gifted programs, by survey year and student race.

Asian students are, on average, more likely to attend larger, The second column adds a control for student scores on
urban schools and schools with higher FRPL percentages. the ECLS-K math and reading assessments, both of which,
Black and Hispanic students attend schools with lower mean unsurprisingly, are strongly positively associated with gifted
test scores, on average. assignment. The addition of these variables drastically
reduces the assignment gap between White students and
Explaining Disproportionalities in Assignment to Gifted Hispanic students and, to a lesser extent, Black students. In
Programs fact, the gap between White and Hispanic students all but
disappears and is statistically indistinguishable from zero,
Next, we turn to predicting the probability of gifted suggesting that differences in assessment scores explain the
assignment using student, teacher, classroom, and entire White–Hispanic differential. Also, the White–Asian
school characteristics. In Table 3, we document racial gap in column 2 is no longer statistically significant. Notably,
and ethnic disproportionalities in a logistic regression however, even when controlling for math and reading
format and examine how other factors explain gifted knowledge or ability, Black students are still significantly
assignment and the racial/ethnic differences we observe. less likely to be assigned to gifted services. Based on this
Gifted assignment in this table is operationalized as the model, the predicted probability of assignment for Black
student is assigned to gifted services in either reading or students is approximately half the probability for White stu-
math. dents. The addition of controls for student sex, SES, health,
The first column includes only race and ethnicity vari- and age at kindergarten entry (column 3) does little to change
ables, where White students are the base category. The the results. Student SES is strongly positively associated
results show that Asian students are more likely than White with likelihood of assignment, but the inclusion of these
students to be assigned to gifted services, while Hispanic additional variables closes the Black–White gap only slightly
students are less likely, and Black students are less likely further, suggesting that factors beyond observable student
still. The odds of being assigned to a gifted program are background characteristics are responsible for explaining
66% lower for Black students than for White students. For the Black–White gap in gifted assignment.
Hispanic students, the odds of assignment to gifted are Columns 4 and 5 add further controls for teacher, class-
47% lower than for White students. The odds of assign- room, and school characteristics. These additions lead to
ment for Asian students are 44% higher than for White several observations. First, the gap in gifted assignment
students. between Black students and students of other racial and

8
Table 2
Comparing Student, Teacher, Classroom, and School Characteristics by Student Race and Ethnicity

Characteristic White Black Hispanic Asian

Student characteristics  
  Students assigned to gifted 0.053 0.022*** 0.035*** 0.062*
   Students assigned to gifted in reading 0.050 0.021*** 0.034*** 0.058*
   Students assigned to gifted in math 0.043 0.017*** 0.029*** 0.054
  Reading test (standardized) 0.14 −0.30*** −0.34*** 0.17
  Math test (standardized) 0.21 −0.41*** −0.30*** 0.12
 Female 0.49 0.47 0.52 0.54*
 SES 0.05 −0.42*** −0.45*** 0.01
  Parent’s health rating 0.07 −0.17*** −0.24*** −0.11**
  Age in months at start of kindergarten 66.64 66.31 65.75*** 65.42***
Teacher/classroom characteristics  
 White 0.95 0.76*** 0.89*** 0.85***
 Black 0.03 0.22*** 0.04*** 0.04
 Hispanic 0.01 0.02 0.20*** 0.02**
 Asian 0.01 0.01 0.03*** 0.09***
  Teaching experience (in current school) 10.18 8.53*** 8.25*** 8.65***
  Master’s degree 0.41 0.33*** 0.34*** 0.39
 Certified 0.94 0.91*** 0.91*** 0.94
  Class size 20.76 20.79 20.90 21.38**
  Class mean SES 0.03 −0.36*** −0.36*** −0.00*
  Class percentage Black 0.05 0.68*** 0.05** 0.06
  Class percentage Hispanic 0.06 0.05 0.68*** 0.11***
  Class percentage Asian 0.03 0.02* 0.05*** 0.57***
School characteristics  
 Urban 0.19 0.40*** 0.43*** 0.35***
 Rural 0.35 0.12*** 0.20*** 0.20***
 Northeast 0.14 0.07*** 0.07*** 0.12
 Midwest 0.29 0.07*** 0.12*** 0.23***
 South 0.42 0.81*** 0.33*** 0.25***
 West 0.15 0.05*** 0.49*** 0.39***
  School size (100s) 5.31 5.61*** 6.09*** 5.80***
  Fraction White students 0.80 0.40*** 0.42*** 0.52***
  Fraction Black students 0.09 0.49*** 0.08 0.12**
  Fraction Hispanic students 0.07 0.07 0.46*** 0.18***
  Fraction Asian students 0.02 0.02 0.05*** 0.18***
  School percentage FRPL 0.26 0.53*** 0.46*** 0.35***
  School mean test score 56.63 50.98*** 54.9*** 57.82**
Observations 9,400 1,840 2,380 1,060

Note. Estimates adjusted using Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten cohort probability weights. Public schools with gifted programs only.
Statistical significance based on a t test comparing White students to the other groups. Sample sizes rounded in accordance with National Center for Educa-
tion Statistics nondisclosure rules. SES = socioeconomic status; FRPL = free or reduced-price lunch.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

ethnic backgrounds remains when characteristics of gap larger than if only student characteristics are included.
schools are included. In the fully saturated model, the odds In terms of predicted probabilities with all other variables
of assignment to gifted are 50% lower for Black students at their means, White students have a predicted probability
than for White students. If anything, accounting for Black of assignment of 6.2%, whereas Black students have
and Hispanic students’ teacher, classroom, and school only a 2.8% probability, a statistically and substantively
characteristics makes the point estimates of the assignment important difference. Second, most teacher and classroom

9
Grissom and Redding

characteristics are uncorrelated with the probability of a and ethnicity. We show results both for assignment to either
student’s assignment to gifted programs, at least when student reading or math and for assignment to reading or math
characteristics are included in the models. One exception is separately.
that students in classrooms with Black teachers are more The takeaway from Table 5 is that, consistent with the
likely to be assigned, highlighting that further examination predictions from bureaucratic representation theory,
of teacher race effects is warranted. Finally, several school assignment to a Black teacher partially ameliorates the dis-
characteristics are associated with assignment probability, parity in gifted assignment probability between Black stu-
including school racial/ethnic composition; students in dents and students from other groups, particularly for
schools with larger fractions of Black and Hispanic stu- assignment in reading. For the most saturated model for
dents have higher assignment probabilities, while those in reading assignment (column 6), the odds ratios suggest that
schools with larger Asian populations have lower assign- Black students with a Black teacher are significantly more
ment probabilities. likely to be assigned to gifted services than Black students
Table 4 shows these same results, this time separating without Black teachers. The predicted probability of
assignment to gifted programs in reading from assignment assignment to gifted programs in reading for Black stu-
to gifted programs in math. Making this distinction gener- dents in classrooms with Black teachers is 6.2%, compared
ally does not change the interpretations in Table 3, though to 2.1% for Black students with non-Black teachers, hold-
it does add some nuance. The gifted-assignment gap for ing other variables at their means. In other words, all else
Black students is large and evident in both subjects, even equal, Black students are predicted to be assigned to gifted
when accounting for the full set of control variables. We services 3 times more often in classrooms with Black
also find that Asian students are more likely than White teachers than with non-Black teachers. Significant evi-
students to be assigned to gifted services in math (but not dence of a correlation between gifted assignment and race
reading) than other factors would predict. This finding is congruence is not found for White, Hispanic, or Asian stu-
consistent with evidence of positive stereotyping of Asian dents. When we limit to assignment in math only, we do
students as quantitatively gifted and with evidence that not find evidence of a race congruence association for any
Asian parents are more likely to participate in gifted iden- student subgroup.
tification processes, among other possible explanations As one check on these results, Appendix Table A4 con-
(Kitano, 2011). Most other variables in the model have the trols for test scores in the current year. Although we prefer
same estimated association with reading assignment as the model controlling for the lagged test score because of
with math assignment in the full models. concerns that the current test score is endogenous to assign-
In summary, large disparities exist in gifted assignment ment, making this change strengthens the case for an asso-
by race and ethnicity in elementary schools with gifted pro- ciation between race congruence and gifted assignment for
grams, and neither math and reading assessment scores nor Black students.
other background characteristics fully account for the dis- Given the significant associations for the presence of a
proportionately low assignment of Black students, in partic- race-congruent teacher for Black students in Table 5, in
ular. Nor are the disparities explained by the kinds of schools Figure 2 we plot the predicted probabilities from this
Black students attend. There is some suggestion that the race model for Black students in race-congruent and race-
of the teacher to which a student is assigned does predict a incongruent classrooms compared to White students with
student’s assignment probability, an issue into which we and without a same-race teacher across the score distribu-
next delve further. tion (averaging math and reading) from the median to the
100th percentile. The likelihood of assignment to gifted
Results for Teacher–Student Race Congruence and services increases for all students as their test scores
Assignment to Gifted Programs increase. White students are assigned at higher probabili-
ties throughout the distribution, but assignment probabili-
Table 5 considers the question of whether assignment to ties for Black students with Black teachers are similar to
an own-race teacher matters for a student’s probability of those of White students. In contrast, Black students with
assignment to gifted services, focusing specifically on non-Black teachers are predicted to be assigned to gifted
Black students as the group identified as underrepresented programs at consistently lower probabilities than the three
in the preceding analyses. As in Tables 3 and 4, a variety of other groups of students depicted.
models with different combinations of control variables are
shown, though control variables are omitted from the table
Discussion and Conclusions
for brevity (full results are shown in Appendix Table A3).14
The addition in Table 5 is an indicator for whether the stu- As Konstantopoulos, Modi, and Hedges (2001) argue in
dent is race congruent with his or her teacher and a series their study of gifted students, managing gifted students’
of interactions between race congruence and student race intellectual talent “is essential to maintaining high national

10
Table 3
Predicting Assignment to Gifted Programs

Characteristic (1) (2) (3) (4) (5)

Student characteristics  
  Black student 0.34** 0.57** 0.63* 0.44** 0.50**
(−5.80) (−2.83) (−2.29) (−3.17) (−2.84)
  Hispanic student 0.53** 0.96 1.07 0.84 0.89
(−4.91) (−0.27) (0.44) (−0.81) (−0.51)
  Asian student 1.44* 1.25 1.20 1.11 1.19
(2.31) (1.35) (1.05) (0.46) (0.76)
  Reading test score (standardized) 1.78*** 1.72** 1.73** 1.75**
  (10.14) (9.32) (9.14) (8.93)
  Math test score (standardized) 1.97*** 1.90** 1.93** 1.95**
  (11.20) (10.11) (9.81) (9.73)
  Female student 0.99 0.99 1.01
  (−0.06) (−0.08) (0.12)
  Socioeconomic status 1.38** 1.58** 1.58**
  (4.69) (5.03) (4.99)
  Student health 0.95 0.98 0.98
  (−0.91) (−0.37) (−0.42)
  Age in months as of start of kindergarten 0.99 0.99 0.99
  (−0.70) (−0.60) (−0.94)
Teacher/classroom characteristics  
  Black teacher 1.72** 1.44†
  (2.60) (1.71)
  Hispanic teacher 0.96 0.78
  (−0.15) (−0.79)
  Asian teacher 1.20 1.29
  (0.53) (0.71)
  Teaching experience (in current school) 1.00 1.00
  (0.14) (0.29)
  Master’s degree 0.96 1.01
  (−0.39) (0.13)
 Certified 1.03 0.99
  (0.14) (−0.03)
  Class size 1.01 1.01
  (0.40) (0.51)
  Class percentage Black students 0.83 0.91
  (−1.47) (−0.61)
  Class percentage Hispanic students 1.49 0.47†
  (1.26) (−1.85)
  Class percentage Asian students 1.42 0.73
  (1.14) (−0.65)
School characteristics  
 Urban 1.12
  (0.85)
 Rural 0.92
  (−0.56)
 Midwest 1.46*
  (2.18)

(continued)

11
Table 3 (continued)
Characteristic (1) (2) (3) (4) (5)

 South 1.17
  (0.91)
 West 1.13
  (0.62)
  School size 0.96
  (−1.49)
  School FRPL rate 1.06
  (0.16)
  Fraction Black students 5.29**
  (3.38)
  Fraction Hispanic students 4.09*
  (2.15)
  Fraction Asian students 0.21†
  (−1.66)
  School mean test score 1.00
  (−0.02)
Observations 14,280 14,280 14,280 14,280 14,280

Note. Coefficients reported as odds ratios. Estimates adjusted using Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten cohort probability weights. Public
schools with gifted programs only. The t statistics, in parentheses, are based on robust standard errors clustered at the child level. FRPL = free or reduced-
price lunch.

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01.

Table 4
Predicting Assignment to Gifted Programs, by Subject

  Reading Math

Characteristic (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10)
Student characteristics  
  Black student 0.36** 0.61* 0.67† 0.39** 0.45** 0.36** 0.62* 0.66† 0.51* 0.53*
(−5.26) (−2.42) (−1.91) (−3.41) (−3.11) (−4.67) (−2.12) (−1.75) (−2.26) (−2.11)
  Hispanic student 0.54** 0.98 1.08 0.84 0.90 0.46** 0.93 1.03 0.67 0.71
(−4.47) (−0.13) (0.47) (−0.73) (−0.45) (−4.61) (−0.36) (0.13) (−1.55) (−1.25)
  Asian student 1.46* 1.24 1.18 1.16 1.23 1.65** 1.59 1.53* 1.73† 1.83*
(2.27) (1.19) (0.92) (0.61) (0.87) (2.76) (2.22) (2.04) (1.95) (2.10)
  Reading test score 1.81** 1.74** 1.76** 1.78** 1.75** 1.71** 1.75** 1.82**
(standardized)   (9.83) (8.94) (8.80) (8.55) (8.65) (8.23) (8.28) (8.67)
  Math test score 1.96** 1.91** 1.93** 1.95** 1.94** 1.87** 1.88** 1.92**
(standardized)   (10.58) (9.66) (9.33) (9.21) (9.33) (8.30) (8.01) (8.33)
  Female student 1.06 1.06 1.09 0.88 0.84 0.84
  (0.54) (0.52) (0.71) (−0.96) (−1.23) (−1.24)
 SES 1.35** 1.58** 1.58** 1.31** 1.59** 1.58**
  (4.05) (4.69) (4.66) (3.24) (4.06) (3.98)
  Student health 0.95 0.98 0.98 0.98 0.99 1.01
  (−0.78) (−0.28) (−0.28) (−0.35) (−0.12) (0.15)
  Age in months as of 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.98 0.99 0.98
start of kindergarten   (−0.67) (−0.46) (−0.81) (−0.85) (−0.69) (−0.84)
Teacher/classroom characteristics  
  Black teacher 1.85** 1.51+ 1.85** 2.32** 1.81* 2.32**
  (2.89) (1.90) (2.89) (3.50) (2.40) (3.50)

(continued)

12
Table 4 (continued)
  Reading Math

Characteristic (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10)

  Hispanic teacher 1.05 0.82 1.05 0.70 0.54 0.70


  (0.17) (−0.60) (0.17) (−0.83) (−1.26) (−0.83)
  Asian teacher 1.26 1.31 1.26 0.58 0.60 0.58
  (0.66) (0.72) (0.66) (−1.09) (−0.97) (−1.09)
  Teaching experience (in 1.00 1.00 1.00 0.99 0.99 0.99
current school)   (0.18) (0.32) (0.18) (−0.78) (−0.70) (−0.78)
  Master’s degree 0.96 1.03 0.96 0.93 1.00 0.93
  (−0.33) (0.22) (−0.33) (−0.51) (−0.00) (−0.51)
 Certified 1.02 0.99 1.02 1.45 1.46 1.45
  (0.09) (−0.04) (0.09) (1.49) (1.52) (1.49)
  Class size 0.99 1.00 0.99 1.02 1.02 1.02
  (−0.33) (−0.17) (−0.33) (0.86) (0.80) (0.86)
  Class mean SES 0.81 0.92 0.81 0.73* 0.91 0.73*
  (−1.55) (−0.57) (−1.55) (−2.00) (−0.51) (−2.00)
  Class percentage Black 1.85† 0.50 1.85† 1.05 0.24** 1.05
students   (1.86) (−1.55) (1.86) (0.12) (−2.68) (0.12)
  Class percentage 1.44 0.73 1.44 1.92† 0.86 1.92†
Hispanic students   (1.12) (−0.60) (1.12) (1.72) (−0.22) (1.72)
  Class percentage Asian 1.07 1.19 1.07 0.96 1.07 0.96
students   (0.18) (0.41) (0.18) (−0.08) (0.14) (−0.08)
School characteristics  
 Urban 1.09 0.91
  (0.60) (−0.57)
 Rural 0.92 0.80
  (−0.52) (−1.05)
 Midwest 1.35 1.40
  (1.57) (1.63)
 South 1.22 1.27
  (1.07) (1.19)
 West 1.12 1.09
  (0.52) (0.33)
  School size 0.95+ 0.96
  (−1.94) (−1.02)
  School FRPL rate 1.13 1.30
  (0.32) (0.58)
  Fraction Black students 5.98** 6.51**
  (3.32) (3.02)
  Fraction Hispanic 4.50* 4.11
students   (2.15) (1.51)
  Fraction Asian students 0.20 0.25
  (−1.63) (−1.06)
  School mean test score 1.00 0.97*
  (0.07) (−2.48)
Observations 14,230 14,230 14,230 14,230 14,230 12,070 12,070 12,070 12,070 12,070

Note. Coefficients reported as odds ratios. Estimates adjusted using Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten cohort probability weights. Public
schools with gifted programs only. The t statistics, in parentheses, are based on robust standard errors clustered at the child level. SES = socioeconomic
status; FRPL = free or reduced-price lunch.

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01.

13
Table 5
Predicting Assignment to Gifted Programs With Student–Teacher Race Congruence

Reading or math assignment Reading assignment Math assignment

Variable (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)

Black student 0.38** 0.29** 0.31** 0.35** 0.24** 0.26** 0.56 0.47 0.51
(−2.65) (−3.16) (−3.03) (−2.70) (−3.43) (−3.24) (−1.29) (−1.59) (−1.45)
Hispanic student 0.90 0.74 0.77 0.82 0.69 0.72 1.10 0.79 0.83
(−0.36) (−0.87) (−0.77) (−0.59) (−1.06) (−0.90) (0.25) (−0.55) (−0.41)
Asian student 1.06 1.02 1.05 0.95 0.97 1.01 1.78 1.87 1.96
(0.16) (0.05) (0.13) (−0.16) (−0.08) (0.01) (1.38) (1.37) (1.49)
Black teacher 1.28 1.19 1.01 1.31 1.18 1.00 2.29* 2.19* 1.76
(0.82) (0.55) (0.03) (0.84) (0.51) (−0.01) (2.18) (2.05) (1.48)
Hispanic teacher 0.93 0.90 0.75 1.07 1.02 0.83 0.87 0.82 0.69
(−0.21) (−0.30) (−0.76) (0.19) (0.07) (−0.49) (−0.29) (−0.42) (−0.70)
Asian teacher 1.10 1.09 1.13 1.09 1.08 1.08 0.47 0.48 0.52
(0.23) (0.20) (0.26) (0.19) (0.18) (0.15) (−0.91) (−0.91) (−0.80)
Race congruence 0.87 0.88 0.87 0.80 0.81 0.81 1.15 1.16 1.15
(−0.48) (−0.44) (−0.47) (−0.75) (−0.70) (−0.68) (0.39) (0.40) (0.39)
Black Student × 3.26† 3.26† 3.02† 3.90* 3.88* 3.48† 1.13 1.19 1.09
Race Congruence (1.79) (1.79) (1.67) (1.96) (1.96) (1.79) (0.15) (0.22) (0.11)
Hispanic Student × 1.55 1.40 1.33 1.58 1.42 1.35 1.43 1.20 1.08
Race Congruence (0.78) (0.59) (0.48) (0.80) (0.61) (0.49) (0.45) (0.23) (0.09)
Asian Student × 1.00 0.96 1.27 1.28 1.25 1.65 1.42 1.49 2.13
Race Congruence (0.00) (−0.06) (0.32) (0.33) (0.29) (0.64) (0.31) (0.35) (0.65)
Reading test score 1.71** 1.72** 1.74** 1.73** 1.74** 1.76** 1.70** 1.71** 1.77**
(standardized) (9.26) (9.37) (9.28) (8.84) (8.99) (8.86) (8.06) (8.24) (8.62)
Math test score 1.91** 1.92** 1.92** 1.92** 1.93** 1.93** 1.88** 1.89** 1.93**
(standardized) (10.17) (10.04) (9.80) (9.73) (9.61) (9.34) (8.34) (8.26) (8.50)
Observations 14,280 14,280 14,280 14,230 14,230 14,230 12,070 12,070 12,070

Note. Coefficients reported as odds ratios. All models include student controls. Models 2, 5, and 8 add teacher and classroom controls; Models 3, 6, and 9
add school controls (not shown). Estimates adjusted using Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten cohort probability weights. Public schools with
gifted programs only. The t statistics, in parentheses, are based on standard errors clustered at the child level.

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01.

standards of achievement in education, international com- likely to receive gifted services in subsequent years, partic-
petitiveness, and labor market efficiency” (p. 344). This goal ularly in reading. The lower likelihood of assignment for
of fostering academic growth among academically talented high-achieving Black students in classrooms with non-
youth hinges on the proper identification of gifted students. Black teachers diverts gifted services from the very students
Yet our results show that identification of gifted students who may benefit the most from such programs (Card &
depends, in part, on factors having little to do with student Giuliano, 2014). Representative bureaucracy theory sug-
performance or ability that lead students to be assigned dis- gests multiple mechanisms that might produce such a pat-
proportionately on the basis of race and ethnicity. Taking tern. One explanation is that teachers exercise discretion in
student math and reading assessment scores into account, student referral, diagnosis, or selection along racial/ethnic
which school a student attends and the characteristics of the lines in ways that contribute to patterns of disproportional-
classroom to which a student is assigned, including the race ity in assignment. Teachers may perceive potential gifted-
of the classroom teacher, each partially explain students’ ness differently in students from other-race groups because
gifted assignment probabilities and, to some degree, the of differences in backgrounds or biases in their judgments
observed gaps among students of different demographic or expectations (Gershenson, Holt, & Papageorge, 2015;
groups. Grissom et al., 2015). Alternatively, students may perform
In particular, we uncover evidence that Black students in or behave differently in the presence of an own-race teacher
classrooms with non-Black teachers are systematically less in ways that make giftedness easier to identify, or students

14
Figure 2.  Predicted assignment to gifted services for Black and White students.

or parents may engage differently in the gifted assignment Ingersoll & May, 2011; Quiocho & Rios, 2000; Villegas
process itself in the presence of own-race teachers (Grissom & Irvine, 2010). The evidence presented here adds to this
et al., 2015; Lim, 2006). For example, Black parents may list, suggesting that greater teacher diversity may help
feel more comfortable reaching out to a Black teacher to ameliorate racial gaps in student assignment to gifted pro-
suggest that a child be tested or may be more responsive grams. Given these results, future work might investigate
when that teacher reaches out to encourage the family to whether these patterns may also hold for other student out-
sign the child up for gifted services. An important task for comes with similar gaps, including discipline or referral to
future research is to explore these mechanisms. Nevertheless, special education services.
this finding that Black students—even with the same assess- Of course, if teacher discretion is indeed the primary
ment scores in schools with similar characteristics—are less mechanism underlying the race congruence result, policy-
likely to be assigned to gifted services, at least in reading, makers and education leaders need not wait for greater
when taught by a non-Black teacher raises serious concerns teacher workforce diversity to address the issue we high-
in the context of an educational system in which approxi- light. In special education, as a result of three decades of
mately 80% of Black elementary school students are taught legal challenges, districts have made strides in minimizing
by other-race teachers. the inequitable exercise of discretion through the adoption
That Black students are overwhelmingly taught by non- of less biased identification and placement systems, includ-
Black teachers reflects a lack of diversity in the teacher ing enhanced training for teachers on identification and the
workforce. Although 43% of the public school student pop- use of norm-referenced instruments (Zhang & Katsiyannis,
ulation are students of color, non-White teachers make up 2002). Evidence suggests that such processes help equalize
only about 17% of the teaching workforce (Bireda & Chait, the representation of Black students in special education in
2011; Grissom et al., 2015), and this discrepancy may be elementary school (Hibel et al., 2010; Morgan, Farkas,
growing as the schools become more diverse (Boser, 2014). Hillemeier, & Maczuga, 2012). Similar steps could be
Scholars have cited numerous justifications for policies taken to formalize the processes for gifted identification to
aimed at diversifying teaching demographically, including form what Joseph and Ford (2006) refer to as “nondiscrim-
role-modeling benefits for students of color, greater likeli- inatory assessment.” They describe a process that draws
hood of culturally relevant instruction, and lower teacher upon a variety of sources of student data and ensures that,
churn in schools with large concentrations of disadvan- rather than a single individual making assignment deci-
taged teachers (Bireda & Chait, 2011; Boser, 2014; sions, assignment teams using culturally sensitive

15
Grissom and Redding

assessments are engaged in evaluation. School systems variables in the models, also highlights an avenue for addi-
might also move toward universal gifted screening proce- tional explanatory research.
dures to reduce the role of teacher discretion in placement Our study faces a number of limitations. First, we lack
processes. In fact, universal screening may help address data on teachers’ actual identification, referral, and diagno-
other potential sources of disproportionalities associated sis behaviors and instead approximate this complex pro-
with differences in referral rates, such as parental advo- cess with information on whether the student moves from
cacy. Evidence suggests that shifting to universal screening not receiving gifted services to receiving them in consecu-
procedures indeed increases identification rates for non- tive survey periods. This approximation is more problem-
White students (Card & Giuliano, 2015). atic in some years because the ECLS-K did not collect data
Teacher preparation and professional development may in second and fourth grades. Second, data limitations pre-
be particularly important avenues for reducing racial dis- vent us from more fully accounting for unobserved charac-
parities at the teacher referral stage. Research has docu- teristics that might bias our results. For example, it could
mented that White preservice teachers often bring a dearth be that motivated parents both push for their children to be
of cross-cultural experience and knowledge with which to assigned to same-race teachers and advocate for them to be
negotiate how to teach students of color (Cochran-Smith, tested for gifted classification. If so, we risk attributing the
2000; Ladson-Billings, 1999; Sleeter, 2001), particularly impact of parental involvement to the presence of a race-
for Black students (Ladson-Billings, 2000). Addressing the congruent teacher. We attempt to account for a number of
need to train culturally responsive teachers, one approach student, teacher, and school characteristics to avoid these
has been to focus on specific strategies that inform how sources of bias, but larger data sets of the type maintained
implicit biases may affect decision making and behavior. for state administrative purposes could permit other model-
Research from a number of domains, including the legal ing strategies, such as the inclusion of student or teacher
profession, health care, law enforcement, and professional fixed effects, that would provide more convincing esti-
sports, suggests that explicit training to increase awareness mates. An additional data limitation is that the ECLS-K
of prejudice or stereotypes may reduce the unintentional data contain no measures of student aptitude. We include
racial bias from decisions made when the level of discre- math and reading achievement scores in our models, but if
tion over outcomes is high (e.g., Burgess, van Ryn, Dovidio, schools aim to target gifted services at high-aptitude stu-
& Saha, 2007; Correll et al., 2007; Greenwald, Oakes, & dents, regardless of past achievement, these scores may not
Hoffman, 2003; Kang et al., 2012; Plant & Peruche, 2005; be sufficient. Rather than interpreting our results as defini-
Pope, Price, & Wolfers, 2013). Studies in K–12 and higher tive evidence that teacher discretion contributes to dispro-
education similarly indicate that training around diversity portionalities in student gifted assignment, we interpret
can reduce racial bias in participants’ perceptions (Denson, them as suggesting new avenues for further research to bet-
2009; Engberg, 2004). In the context of gifted education, ter understand a complex set of social and educational
training for teachers could emphasize strategies aimed at processes.
identifying giftedness among racially or ethnically diverse Future studies could take advantage of administrative
students and identification approaches that are not culture- data sets that permit a more precise modeling of the gifted
blind (Ford, Moore, & Scott, 2011). assignment process. In particular, studies that use more
We emphasize, however, that teacher–student race con- robust district or state administrative data could utilize
gruence only partially explains the apparent underassign- variation in referral, identification, and evaluation mech-
ment of Black students to gifted programs that remains anisms across schools and differences in students’ deci-
even after student test scores; other background character- sions to take advantage of gifted services following
istics, such as SES; and classroom and school characteris- nomination, which may also differ by student characteris-
tics are taken into account. Unlike the underrepresentation tics, to isolate which stages contribute to the underrepre-
of Hispanic students relative to White students, which is sentation of students of color. Qualitative exploration of
largely explained by differences in reading and math the practices that schools and their teachers engage in
achievement scores, the Black–White gap in gifted assign- during assignment or how parents interface with schools
ment cannot be fully explained by the relatively large num- could elucidate other factors that influence student place-
ber of control variables included in our models. The ment in gifted programs. Future work could also be
persistence of this gap points to a need for additional extended to retention in gifted programs. As gifted pro-
research into the structural, social, and cultural contribu- grams themselves do not always embrace the diverse cul-
tors to differences in assignment even for high-achieving tures of their students (Ford, 1998; Ford et al., 2008),
Black students. The finding that Asian students are over- research could address the connection between retention
represented in gifted programs relative to other students, and underrepresentation, and the roles of diverse teachers
particularly in math, even after including extensive control in student retention.

16
Appendix

Table A1
Correlation Matrix for Control Variables

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15

  1. Gifted assignment —  
  2. Black child −0.06 —  
  3. Hispanic child −0.05 −0.20 —  
  4. Asian child 0.05 −0.11 −0.15 —  
  5. Test score 0.26 −0.08 −0.07 0.06 —  
(standardized)
  6. SES 0.18 −0.16 −0.29 0.03 0.13 —  
  7. School percentage −0.06 0.26 0.30 0.01 −0.08 −0.47 —  
FRPL
  8. Fraction Black −0.01 0.64 −0.12 −0.04 −0.07 −0.17 0.38 —  
students
  9. Fraction Hispanic −0.02 −0.14 0.68 0.02 −0.06 −0.28 0.40 −0.16 —  
students
10. Fraction Asian 0.02 −0.08 0.04 0.48 0.07 0.03 0.02 −0.07 0.07 —  
students
11. School mean test 0.16 −0.06 −0.05 0.05 0.94 0.06 −0.08 −0.08 −0.06 0.07 —  
score
12. Class mean SES 0.13 −0.17 −0.32 0.05 0.12 0.75 −0.59 −0.22 −0.37 0.04 0.09 —  
13. Class percentage −0.04 0.79 −0.17 −0.09 −0.07 −0.16 0.31 0.81 −0.16 −0.10 −0.07 −0.22 —  
Black
14. Class percentage −0.04 −0.17 0.83 −0.08 −0.07 −0.29 0.34 −0.14 0.82 0.05 −0.06 −0.38 −0.21 —  
Hispanic
15. Class percentage 0.05 −0.10 −0.08 0.74 0.06 0.05 0.02 −0.05 0.04 0.63 0.06 0.07 −0.12 −0.09 —
Asian

Note. SES = socioeconomic status; FRPL = free or reduced-price lunch.

Table A2
Modeling Student Assignment to Gifted Services Using Current Year’s Math and Reading Achievement Test Scores
Reading Math

Characteristic (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10)
Student characteristics  
  Black student 0.36** 0.74 0.79 0.48** 0.53* 0.36** 0.77 0.80 0.57† 0.63
(−5.26) (−1.52) (−1.13) (−2.76) (−2.50) (−4.67) (−1.19) (−0.95) (−1.87) (−1.61)
  Hispanic student 0.54** 1.10 1.19 0.82 0.88 0.46** 0.99 1.07 0.67 0.71
(−4.47) (0.60) (1.13) (−0.85) (−0.56) (−4.61) (−0.07) (0.33) (−1.54) (−1.26)
  Asian student 1.46* 1.53* 1.49† 1.31 1.40 1.65** 1.80** 1.75** 1.87* 1.98*
(2.27) (2.12) (1.93) (1.23) (1.56) (2.76) (2.90) (2.74) (2.30) (2.50)
  Reading test score 2.51** 2.41** 2.42** 2.58** 2.33** 2.28** 2.29** 2.54**
(standardized)   (13.78) (12.73) (12.67) (13.48) (10.53) (10.30) (10.07) (10.98)
  Math test score 2.31** 2.25** 2.27** 2.39** 2.45** 2.35** 2.39** 2.55**
(standardized)   (10.11) (9.55) (9.52) (10.21) (8.44) (7.93) (7.96) (9.07)
  Female student 1.08 1.09 1.08 0.91 0.90 0.88
  (0.72) (0.77) (0.67) (−0.70) (−0.80) (−1.00)

(continued)

17
Table A2 (continued)

Reading Math

Characteristic (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10)

 SES 1.27** 1.48** 1.45** 1.25** 1.47** 1.41**


  (3.25) (4.35) (4.06) (2.68) (3.53) (3.15)
  Student health 0.97 0.98 1.00 0.97 0.98 1.00
  (−0.55) (−0.38) (−0.04) (−0.51) (−0.38) (−0.02)
  Age in months as of 1.00 1.00 1.00 0.99 0.99 0.99
start of kindergarten   (0.02) (0.02) (−0.30) (−0.48) (−0.55) (−0.81)
Teacher/classroom characteristics  
  Black teacher 1.80** 1.50† 2.22** 1.82*
  (2.78) (1.82) (3.41) (2.44)
  Hispanic teacher 1.17 0.98 1.01 0.81
  (0.62) (−0.09) (0.04) (−0.60)
  Asian teacher 1.04 1.21 0.52 0.66
  (0.10) (0.48) (−1.20) (−0.72)
  Teaching experience 1.00 1.00 0.99 0.99
(in current school)   (0.44) (0.32) (−0.77) (−1.01)
  Master’s degree 1.00 1.06 1.01 1.06
  (0.03) (0.50) (0.07) (0.43)
 Certified 0.98 1.00 1.42 1.47†
  (−0.09) (−0.01) (1.51) (1.65)
  Class size 1.00 1.01 1.02 1.02
  (−0.03) (0.25) (0.62) (0.75)
  Class mean SES 0.77* 1.00 0.74* 1.07
  (−2.03) (0.01) (−2.02) (0.41)
  Class percentage 1.77† 0.64 1.26 0.39†
Black students   (1.81) (−1.03) (0.64) (−1.90)
  Class percentage 1.72† 1.05 2.12* 1.22
Hispanic students   (1.70) (0.10) (2.06) (0.31)
  Class percentage 1.20 1.55 0.85 1.27
Asian students   (0.49) (0.94) (−0.37) (0.45)
School characteristics  
 Urban 1.20 1.05
  (1.38) (0.32)
 Rural 0.87 0.82
  (−0.84) (−0.99)
 Midwest 1.30 1.32
  (1.46) (1.50)
 South 1.24 1.34
  (1.25) (1.56)
 West 1.06 0.99
  (0.27) (−0.04)
  School size 0.94* 0.95
  (−2.08) (−1.45)
  School FRPL rate 1.23 1.15
  (0.68) (0.37)
  Fraction Black 3.38* 3.23*
students   (2.58) (2.12)
  Fraction Hispanic 2.49 2.56
students   (1.40) (1.07)

(continued)

18
Table A2 (continued)

Reading Math

Characteristic (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10)

  Fraction Asian 0.18† 0.14†


students   (−1.91) (−1.86)
  School mean test 0.97** 0.94**
score   (−2.69) (−5.55)
Observations 15,060 15,060 15,060 15,060 15,060 12,880 12,880 12,070 12,070 12,070

Note. Coefficients reported as odds ratios. Estimates adjusted using Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten cohort probability weights. Public
schools with gifted programs only. The t statistics, in parentheses, are based on robust standard errors clustered at the child level. SES = socioeconomic
status; FRPL = free or reduced-price lunch.

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01.

Table A3
Predicting Assignment to Gifted Programs With Student–Teacher Race Congruence (Full Results)

Reading or math assignment Reading assignment Math assignment

Variable (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)

Black student 0.38** 0.29** 0.31** 0.35** 0.24** 0.26** 0.56 0.47 0.51
(−2.65) (−3.16) (−3.03) (−2.70) (−3.43) (−3.24) (−1.29) (−1.59) (−1.45)
Hispanic student 0.90 0.74 0.77 0.82 0.69 0.72 1.10 0.79 0.83
(−0.36) (−0.87) (−0.77) (−0.59) (−1.06) (−0.90) (0.25) (−0.55) (−0.41)
Asian student 1.06 1.02 1.05 0.95 0.97 1.01 1.78 1.87 1.96
(0.16) (0.05) (0.13) (−0.16) (−0.08) (0.01) (1.38) (1.37) (1.49)
Black teacher 1.28 1.19 1.01 1.31 1.18 1.00 2.29* 2.19* 1.76
(0.82) (0.55) (0.03) (0.84) (0.51) (−0.01) (2.18) (2.05) (1.48)
Hispanic teacher 0.93 0.90 0.75 1.07 1.02 0.83 0.87 0.82 0.69
(−0.21) (−0.30) (−0.76) (0.19) (0.07) (−0.49) (−0.29) (−0.42) (−0.70)
Asian teacher 1.10 1.09 1.13 1.09 1.08 1.08 0.47 0.48 0.52
(0.23) (0.20) (0.26) (0.19) (0.18) (0.15) (−0.91) (−0.91) (−0.80)
Race congruence 0.87 0.88 0.87 0.80 0.81 0.81 1.15 1.16 1.15
(−0.48) (−0.44) (−0.47) (−0.75) (−0.70) (−0.68) (0.39) (0.40) (0.39)
Black Student × 3.26† 3.26† 3.02† 3.90† 3.88† 3.48† 1.13 1.19 1.09
Race Congruence (1.79) (1.79) (1.67) (1.96) (1.96) (1.79) (0.15) (0.22) (0.11)
Hispanic Student × 1.55 1.40 1.33 1.58 1.42 1.35 1.43 1.20 1.08
Race Congruence (0.78) (0.59) (0.48) (0.80) (0.61) (0.49) (0.45) (0.23) (0.09)
Asian Student × 1.00 0.96 1.27 1.28 1.25 1.65 1.42 1.49 2.13
Race Congruence (0.00) (−0.06) (0.32) (0.33) (0.29) (0.64) (0.31) (0.35) (0.65)
Reading test score 1.71** 1.72** 1.74** 1.73** 1.74** 1.76** 1.70** 1.71** 1.77**
(standardized) (9.26) (9.37) (9.28) (8.84) (8.99) (8.86) (8.06) (8.24) (8.62)
Math test score 1.91** 1.92** 1.92** 1.92** 1.93** 1.93** 1.88** 1.89** 1.93**
(standardized) (10.17) (10.04) (9.80) (9.73) (9.61) (9.34) (8.34) (8.26) (8.50)
Female student 0.99 0.99 0.99 1.05 1.06 1.06 0.86 0.86 0.85
(−0.13) (−0.09) (−0.12) (0.47) (0.55) (0.52) (−1.07) (−1.11) (−1.20)
SES 1.39** 1.58** 1.58** 1.37** 1.59** 1.59** 1.32** 1.56** 1.55**
(4.87) (5.28) (5.28) (4.25) (5.02) (5.01) (3.36) (3.97) (3.97)
Student health 0.95 0.95 0.97 0.95 0.96 0.98 0.97 0.98 1.00
(−0.95) (−0.87) (−0.59) (−0.81) (−0.71) (−0.39) (−0.38) (−0.29) (0.06)

(continued)

19
Table A3 (continued)

Reading or math assignment Reading assignment Math assignment

Variable (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)

Age in months 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.98 0.98 0.98
as of start of (−0.67) (−0.70) (−0.94) (−0.63) (−0.68) (−0.89) (−0.83) (−0.88) (−1.02)
kindergarten
Teacher characteristics  
 Teaching 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 0.99 0.99
experience (in   (0.35) (0.49) (0.36) (0.51) (−0.83) (−0.76)
current school)
  Master’s degree 0.96 1.00 0.95 1.00 0.97 1.02
  (−0.41) (−0.02) (−0.40) (0.04) (−0.24) (0.15)
 Certified 1.10 1.07 1.09 1.06 1.56† 1.57†
  (0.51) (0.38) (0.43) (0.32) (1.81) (1.82)
Classroom characteristics  
  Class size 1.01 1.01 1.00 1.00 1.01 1.02
  (0.46) (0.51) (0.01) (0.12) (0.48) (0.53)
  Class mean SES 0.81† 0.88 0.78† 0.87 0.76† 0.96
  (−1.67) (−0.89) (−1.85) (−0.89) (−1.85) (−0.28)
  Class percentage 1.59 0.62 1.90† 0.65 1.30 0.33*
Black students   (1.47) (−1.14) (1.95) (−0.96) (0.69) (−2.15)
  Class percentage 1.43 0.81 1.47 0.79 1.89† 1.00
Hispanic students   (1.22) (−0.45) (1.21) (−0.46) (1.72) (−0.01)
  Class percentage 1.08 1.27 0.96 1.14 0.83 1.11
Asian students   (0.23) (0.61) (−0.10) (0.32) (−0.42) (0.19)
School characteristics  
 Urban 1.25† 1.21 1.04
  (1.78) (1.42) (0.27)
 Rural 0.92 0.92 0.85
  (−0.52) (−0.49) (−0.81)
 Midwest 1.40* 1.31 1.28
  (2.03) (1.51) (1.28)
 South 1.18 1.24 1.28
  (1.02) (1.22) (1.27)
 West 1.12 1.12 1.04
  (0.59) (0.54) (0.14)
  School size 0.96 0.95† 0.96
  (−1.46) (−1.86) (−1.29)
  School FRPL rate 1.27 1.41 1.41
  (0.82) (1.14) (0.92)
  Fraction Black 3.85** 4.26** 5.01**
students   (2.80) (2.88) (2.77)
  Fraction Hispanic 3.20† 3.52† 3.20
students   (1.86) (1.88) (1.27)
  Fraction Asian 0.19* 0.18† 0.13†
students   (−2.01) (−1.87) (−1.81)
  School mean test 1.01 1.01 0.97*
score   (0.47) (0.49) (−2.46)
Observations 14,280 14,280 14,280 14,230 14,230 14,230 12,070 12,070 12,070

Note. Coefficients reported as odds ratios. Estimates adjusted using Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten cohort probability weights. Public
schools with gifted programs only. The t statistics, in parentheses, are based on robust standard errors clustered at the child level. SES = socioeconomic
status; FRPL = free or reduced-price lunch.

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01.

20
Table A4
Student Gifted Assignment and Race Congruence, Controlling for Current Year’s Test Score

Reading or math assignment Reading assignment Math assignment

Variable (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)

Black student 0.43* 0.34** 0.38* 0.40* 0.28** 0.33** 0.70 0.59 0.64
(−2.31) (−2.77) (−2.51) (−2.37) (−3.05) (−2.75) (−0.81) (−1.16) (−0.98)
Hispanic student 0.89 0.68 0.74 0.84 0.63 0.70 1.08 0.73 0.79
(−0.37) (−1.11) (−0.86) (−0.56) (−1.26) (−0.97) (0.21) (−0.73) (−0.55)
Asian student 1.22 1.08 1.17 1.12 1.05 1.13 1.96† 2.04 2.14†
(0.58) (0.23) (0.46) (0.32) (0.12) (0.35) (1.68) (1.61) (1.72)
Black teacher 1.24 1.14 1.01 1.27 1.15 1.00 2.36* 2.21* 1.82
(0.71) (0.44) (0.02) (0.75) (0.43) (−0.01) (2.35) (2.18) (1.61)
Hispanic teacher 0.78 0.75 0.67 0.90 0.87 0.76 0.74 0.70 0.62
(−0.72) (−0.80) (−1.05) (−0.30) (−0.41) (−0.73) (−0.64) (−0.75) (−0.93)
Asian teacher 0.97 0.94 1.10 0.94 0.91 1.02 0.42 0.42 0.52
(−0.07) (−0.14) (0.20) (−0.14) (−0.21) (0.04) (−1.07) (−1.08) (−0.80)
Race congruence 0.82 0.84 0.85 0.76 0.78 0.80 1.15 1.14 1.14
(−0.69) (−0.63) (−0.57) (−0.92) (−0.86) (−0.76) (0.39) (0.38) (0.37)
Black Student × 3.73* 3.74* 3.46† 4.35* 4.33* 3.90* 1.04 1.14 1.14
Race Congruence (1.99) (1.99) (1.88) (2.12) (2.12) (1.97) (0.05) (0.16) (0.16)
Hispanic Student × 2.45 2.09 1.84 2.42 2.06 1.82 2.07 1.69 1.40
Race Congruence (1.61) (1.32) (1.08) (1.54) (1.27) (1.03) (0.98) (0.70) (0.44)
Asian Student × 1.43 1.33 1.62 1.84 1.72 2.12 2.20 2.23 3.08
Race Congruence (0.50) (0.40) (0.68) (0.82) (0.73) (0.99) (0.73) (0.75) (1.01)
Reading test score 2.26** 2.27** 2.42** 2.40** 2.42** 2.58** 2.28** 2.30** 2.55**
(standardized) (12.26) (12.33) (13.28) (12.56) (12.67) (13.46) (10.07) (10.06) (10.97)
Math test score 2.36** 2.38** 2.50** 2.25** 2.27** 2.38** 2.34** 2.38** 2.54**
(standardized) (10.58) (10.55) (11.30) (9.48) (9.46) (10.16) (7.86) (7.88) (8.98)
Student Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
characteristics
Teacher and class No Yes Yes No Yes Yes No Yes Yes
characteristics
School No No Yes No No Yes No No Yes
characteristics
Observations 15,110 15,110 15,110 15,060 15,060 15,060 12,880 12,880 12,880

Note. Coefficients reported as odds ratios. Estimates adjusted using Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten cohort probability weights. Public
schools with gifted programs only. The t statistics, in parentheses, are based on robust standard errors clustered at the child level.

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01.

Table A5
Assignment to Gifted Programs and Race Congruence (Dependent Variable = Gifted Assignment in Same Year)

Reading assignment Math assignment

Variable (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

Black student 0.63 0.47* 0.48* 0.58† 0.38* 0.43*


(−1.53) (−2.17) (−2.14) (−1.75) (−2.40) (−2.15)
Hispanic student 1.30 1.19 1.23 1.09 1.00 1.06
(1.05) (0.67) (0.74) (0.32) (−0.01) (0.18)

(continued)

21
Table A5 (continued)

Reading assignment Math assignment

Variable (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

Asian student 1.24 1.41 1.63 1.24 1.57 1.81


(0.77) (1.02) (1.40) (0.76) (1.33) (1.63)
Black teacher 1.21 1.14 0.99 1.21 1.13 0.92
(0.70) (0.48) (−0.04) (0.63) (0.39) (−0.26)
Hispanic teacher 1.09 1.04 0.81 1.39 1.31 0.95
(0.29) (0.13) (−0.70) (0.93) (0.78) (−0.14)
Asian teacher 0.64 0.65 0.77 0.59 0.62 0.81
(−1.15) (−1.10) (−0.66) (−1.09) (−0.97) (−0.41)
Race congruence 0.89 0.89 0.98 0.79 0.80 0.86
(−0.51) (−0.49) (−0.07) (−0.93) (−0.92) (−0.58)
Black Student × Race Congruence 1.06 0.99 0.97 0.92 0.88 0.92
(0.10) (−0.01) (−0.05) (−0.15) (−0.23) (−0.13)
Hispanic Student × Race 1.10 1.03 0.80 1.22 1.18 0.89
Congruence (0.22) (0.07) (−0.51) (0.43) (0.34) (−0.23)
Asian Student × Race Congruence 0.49 0.49 0.45 0.37 0.40 0.38
(−0.56) (−0.56) (−0.63) (−0.94) (−0.89) (−1.00)
Student characteristics Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Teacher and class characteristics No Yes No No Yes No
School characteristics No No Yes No No Yes
Observations 21220 21220 21220 18970 18970 18970

Note. Coefficients reported as odds ratios. Estimates adjusted using Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten cohort probability weights. Public
schools with gifted programs only. The t statistics, in parentheses, are based on robust standard errors clustered at the child level.

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01.

Acknowledgments  4. To avoid double-counting students who exit and reenter


gifted services at different time points, we include students in our
Mimi Engel and Donna Ford provided helpful feedback on earlier
analysis only the first time they are assigned to a gifted program.
drafts of this manuscript.
These students are effectively “dropped” from subsequent years,
resulting in a small further reduction in sample size. Finally, to
Notes avoid overweighting students who were assigned to gifted programs
  1. Using a regression discontinuity (RD) design, a recent study in multiple survey waves, we count students as being assigned to
in a single district by Bui, Craig, and Imberman (2014) found gifted services only the first time they are assigned.
evidence of only small impacts of gifted assignment on student   5. Multiple imputation was run using the suite of multiple imputa-
achievement. Using a similarly rigorous design, Card and Giuliano tion analysis commands (mi) in Stata 13. Ten data sets were generated.
(2014) found evidence that gifted effects are larger for Black and We used the multivariate normal imputation method with a burn-in of
Hispanic students than for other students. Other RD evidence sug- 10,000 and a burn-between of 2,500. Initial values are obtained using a
gests that gifted assignment increases the probability that parents Markov chain Monte Carlo procedure with 5,000 iterations.
keep their children in the district, suggesting that parents are valu-   6. In the third- and fifth-grade surveys, an additional response
ing outcomes for students that may not be picked up by standard- category was added to indicate whether or not the school had a
ized test scores (Davis, Engberg, Epple, Sieg, & Zimmer, 2010). gifted program.
  2. Although “Hispanic” is an ethnicity rather than a race, for   7. Student assessments were conducted beginning in the end
simplicity we use the terms own race, same race, and race congru- of March, after field supervisors, interviewers, and assessors com-
ence to refer to matching between students and teachers on either pleted training throughout February and March (Tourangeau et al.,
the race or the ethnicity dimension. 2006). School surveys were distributed at a similar time. When we
  3. To ascertain whether or not a school had a gifted program instead operationalize assignment as occurring in the same year
in either reading or math, we relied on two indicators. First, we (i.e., the outcome indicates whether or not a student was assigned
marked a school as having a gifted program if teachers reported in a given year, conditional on not being assigned in the prior sur-
any students as participating in a gifted program. Second, if the vey period), we find many of the same relationships with student
administrator reported that children participate in a gifted and tal- background characteristics (e.g., socioeconomic status) and class
ented program at this school, we report a school as having a gifted and school characteristics but no relationship with teacher race/eth-
program. nicity or with congruence (see Appendix Table A5).

22
Discretion and Disproportionality

  8. Fifth-grade teacher characteristics are not relevant because Bireda, S., & Chait, R. (2011). Increasing teacher diversity:
we use fifth-grade data only to capture whether a student not in Strategies to improve the teacher workforce. Washington, DC:
gifted services in third grade was assigned in fifth grade. Center for American Progress. Retrieved from http://american-
 9. The Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten progress.org
cohort math and reading assessments are tests of math and reading Boser, U. (2014). Teacher diversity revisited: A new state-by-state
knowledge and skills in typical content areas taught in elementary analysis. Washington, DC: Center for American Progress.
school. The tests have very good reliability and validity proper- Retrieved from http://americanprogress.org
ties, information about which can be found in National Center for Bui, S. A., Craig, S. G., & Imberman, S. A. (2014). Is gifted educa-
Education Statistics (2002). tion a bright idea? Assessing the impact of gifted and talented
10. We also considered including class mean test score per- programs on students. American Economic Journal: Economic
formance as a covariate. We discovered, however, that this vari- Policy, 6(3), 30–62.
able was correlated at more than 0.9 with both individual student Burgess, D., van Ryn, M., Dovidio, J., & Saha, S. (2007). Reducing
achievement and school mean achievement, so we did not include racial bias among health care providers: lessons from social-
it in the models reported here. Including it, however, has little effect cognitive psychology. Journal of General Internal Medicine,
on most primary coefficients of interest. 22(6), 882–887.
11. To illustrate, consider the kindergarten sample. Of the 1,560 Card, D., & Giuliano, L. (2014). Does gifted education work? For
schools we include, only 90 assigned more than one sampled stu- which students? (Working Paper No. 20453). Cambridge, MA:
dent to gifted services by first grade. Of these 90, only 50 showed National Bureau of Economic Research. Retrieved from http://
variation in the race of assigned students, and only 10 were stu- www.nber.org/papers/w20453
dents assigned with and without a race-congruent teacher. Such Card, D., & Giuliano, L. (2015). Can universal screening increase
small cell sizes create similar challenges for the use of student or the representation of low income and minority students in gifted
teacher fixed effects. education? (Working Paper No. 21519). Cambridge, MA:
12. These statistics refer to the percentage of students assigned National Bureau of Economic Research. Retrieved from http://
to gifted services in schools with gifted programs. Among all www.nber.org/papers/w21519
schools, 4% of students are assigned to gifted services (3% each in Close, B.R., & Mason, P. L. (2006). After the traffic stops:
reading and math). Officer characteristics and enforcement actions. BE Journal of
13. If we do not limit on schools with gifted programs, these Economic Analysis & Policy, 6(1), 1–43.
percentages are approximately 4% of White students, 2% of Black Clotfelter, C. T., Ladd, H. F., & Vigdor, J. (2005). Who teaches
students, 3% of Hispanic students, and 6% of Asian students. whom? Race and the distribution of novice teachers. Economics
14. The full results in Appendix Table A3 show that, in math, of Education Review, 24(4), 377–392.
being in a school with high average test scores makes it less likely Clotfelter, C. T., Ladd, H. F., & Vigdor, J. L. (2010). Teacher
that a student is assigned to gifted services. This evidence is consis- credentials and student achievement in high school: A cross-
tent with findings in Hibel, Farkas, and Morgan (2010) that special subject analysis with student fixed effects. Journal of Human
education placement shows evidence of a “frog pond” effect, in Resources, 45(3), 655–681.
which high average achievement in the school increases the likeli- Cochran-Smith, M. (2000). Blind vision: Unlearning racism in
hood that a student is placed in special education. We attempted to teacher education. Harvard Educational Review, 70(2), 157–
investigate further by interacting individual math score with school 190.
average math score and found that the interaction indeed was sta- Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., Wittenbrink, B., Sadler, M. S., &
tistically significant, though in the opposite direction of what we Keesee, T. (2007). Across the thin blue line: Police officers and
expected; namely, the pattern in the coefficients suggested that the racial bias in the decision to shoot. Journal of Personality and
probability that a high-achieving student was identified as gifted Social Psychology, 92(6), 1006–1023.
increased in relatively high-achieving schools. This pattern would Darity, W. A., Jr., & Jolla, A. (2009). Desegregated schools with
be worth examining further in future work. Our other main coef- segregated education. In C. Hartman & G. D. Squires (Eds.),
ficients of interest were not substantively affected by inclusion of The integration debate: Futures for American cities (pp. 99–
this interaction. 117). New York, NY: Routledge.
Davis, B., Engberg, J., Epple, D. N., Sieg, H., & Zimmer, R.
References (2010). Evaluating the gifted program of an urban school
Aaronson, D., Barrow, L., & Sander, W. (2007). Teachers and stu- district using a modified regression discontinuity design (No.
dent achievement in the Chicago public high schools. Journal w16414). Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic
of Labor Economics, 25(1), 95–135. Research.
Anguino, L. (2003). Underrepresentation of minority students in Dee, T. S. (2004). Teachers, race, and student achievement in a
gifted and talented education. Multicultural Education, 11(1), randomized experiment. Review of Economics and Statistics,
32–34. 86(1), 195–210.
Baker, A. (2013, January 12). Gifted, talented and separated: In one Delcourt, M. A. B., Cornell, D. G., & Goldberg, M. D. (2007).
school, students are divided by gifted label—and race. The New Cognitive and affective learning outcomes of gifted elementary
York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com school students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 51, 359–381.
Bhatt, R. (2009) The impacts of gifted and talented education DeMonte, J., & Hanna, R. (2014). Looking at the best teachers
(SSRN Working Paper No. 09-11). Retreived from http:// and who they teach. Washington, DC: Center for American
papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=1494334. Progress.

23
Grissom and Redding

Denson, N. (2009). Do curricular and cocurricular diversity in gifted programs: Evidence from national data. Elementary
activities influence racial bias? A meta-analysis. Review of School Journal.
Educational Research, 79(2), 805–838. Harris, J. J., III, Brown, E. L., Ford, D. Y., & Richardson, J. W.
Dolan, J., & Rosenbloom, D. H. (2003). Representative bureau- (2003). African Americans and multicultural education: A pro-
cracy: Classic readings and continuing controversies. Armonk, posed remedy for disproportionate special education placement
NY: M. E. Sharpe. and underinclusion in gifted education. Education and Urban
Donovan, M. S., & Cross, C. T. (2002). Minority students in spe- Society, 36(3), 304–341.
cial and gifted education. Washigton, DC: National Academy Hibel, J., Farkas, G., & Morgan, P. L. (2010). Who is placed in
Press. special education? Sociology of Education, 83(4), 312–332.
Elhoweris, H., Mutua, K., Alsheikh, N., & Holloway, P. (2005). Hindera, J. J. (1993). Representative bureaucracy: Further evi-
Effect of children’s ethnicity on teachers’ referral and recom- dence of active representation in the EEOC district offices.
mendation decisions in gifted and talented programs. Remedial Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory, 3(4),
and Special Education, 26(1), 25–31. 415–429.
Engberg, M. E. (2004). Improving intergroup relations in higher Ingersoll, R., & May, H. (2011). Recruitment, retention, and the
education: A critical examination of the influence of edu- minority teacher shortage. Philadelphia, PA: University of
cational interventions on racial bias. Review of Educational Pennsylvania, Consortium for Policy Research in Education.
Research, 74(4), 473–524. Joseph, L. M., & Ford, D. Y. (2006). Nondiscriminatory assess-
Ferguson, R. F. (1998). Teachers’ perceptions and expecta- ment: considerations for gifted education. Gifted Child
tions and the Black–White test score gap. In C. Jencks & M. Quarterly, 50(1), 42–51.
Phillips (Eds.), The Black–White test score gap (pp. 217–317) Kang, J., Bennett, M., Carbado, D., Casey, P., Dasgupta, N.,
Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press. Faigman, D.,  . . . Mnookin, J. (2012). Implicit bias in the court-
Ford, D. Y. (1998). The underrepresentation of minority students room. UCLA Law Review, 59, 1124–1186.
in gifted education: Problems and promises in recruitment and Kearns, T., Ford, L., & Linney, J. A. (2004). African American
retention. Journal of Special Education, 32(1), 4–14. student representation in special education programs. Journal
Ford, D. Y., & Grantham, T. C. (2003). Providing access for cultur- of Negro Education, 74(4), 297–310.
ally diverse gifted students: From deficit to dynamic thinking. Keiser, L. R., Wilkins, V. M., Meier, K. J., & Holland, C. A.
Theory Into Practice, 42, 217–225. (2002). Lipstick and logarithms: Gender, institutional context,
Ford, D. Y., Grantham, T. C., & Whiting, G. W. (2008). Culturally and representative bureaucracy. American Political Science
and linguistically diverse students in gifted education: Review, 96(03), 553–564.
Recruitment and retention issues. Exceptional Children, 74(3), Kennedy, B.A. (2013). Sorting through: The role of representation
289–306. in bureaucracy. Journal of Public Administration Research and
Ford, D. Y., Moore, J. L., III, & Scott, M. T. (2011). Key theories Theory, 23(4), 791–816.
and frameworks for improving the recruitment and retention Kingsley, J. D. (1944). Representative bureaucracy. Yellow
of African American students in gifted education. Journal of Springs, OH: Antioch Press.
Negro Education, 80(3), 239–253. Kitano, M. K. (2011). Issues in research on Asian American gifted
Gamoran, A., Nystrand, M., Berends, M., & LePore, P. C. (1995). students. In J. A. Castellano & A. D. Frazier (Eds.), Special
An organizational analysis of the effects of ability grouping. populations in gifted education: Understanding our most able
American Educational Research Journal, 32(4), 687–715. students from diverse backgrounds (pp. 3–25). Waco, TX:
Gershenson, S., Holt, S. B., & Papageorge, N. W. (2015). Who Prufrock Press.
believes in me? The effect of student-teacher demographic Konstantopoulos, S., Modi, M., & Hedges, L. V. (2001). Who
match on teacher expectations (Upjohn Institute Working are America’s gifted? American Journal of Education, 109(3),
Paper 15-231). Kalamazoo, MI: W. E. Upjohn Institute for 344–382.
Employment Research. Kulik, C. L. C., & Kulik, J. A. (1982). Effects of ability group-
Goldring, E. B. (1990). Assessing the status of information on ing on secondary school students: A meta-analysis of evalua-
classroom organizational frameworks for gifted students. tion findings. American Educational Research Journal, 19(3),
American Journal of Education, 83(6), 313–326. 415–428.
Greenwald, A. G., Oakes, M. A., & Hoffman, H. G. (2003). Targets Kulik, J., & Kulik, C.-L. (1984, August). Effects of ability group-
of discrimination: Effects of race on responses to weapons ing on elementary school pupils: A meta-analysis. Paper pre-
holders. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(4), sented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological
399–405. Association, Toronto, Canada.
Grissom, J. A., Kern, E. C., & Rodriguez, L. A. (2015). The “repre- Ladson-Billings, G. (1999). Preparing teachers for diverse stu-
sentative bureaucracy” in education: Educator workforce diver- dent populations: A critical race theory perspective. Review of
sity, policy outputs, and outcomes for disadvantaged students. Research in Education, 24, 211–247.
Educational Researcher, 44(3), 185–192. Ladson-Billings, G. (2000). Fighting for our lives: Preparing teach-
Grissom, J. A., Nicholson-Crotty, J., & Nicholson-Crotty, S. ers to teach African American students. Journal of Teacher
(2009). Race, region, and representative bureaucracy. Public Education, 51(3), 206–214.
Administration Review, 69(5), 911–919. Lareau, A. (1987). Social class differences in family–school rela-
Grissom, J. A., Rodriguez, L. A., & Kern, E. C. (in press). Teacher tionships: The importance of cultural capital. Sociology of
and principal diversity and the representation of students of color Education, 60(2), 73–85.

24
Discretion and Disproportionality

Lareau, A., & Horvat, E. M. (1999). Moments of social inclusion Pope, D. G., Price, J., & Wolfers, J. (2013). Awareness reduces
and exclusion: Race, class, and cultural capital in family-school racial bias (NBER Working Paper 19765). Retrieved from
relationships. Sociology of Education, 72, 37–53. http://www.nber.org/papers/w19765
Lim, H. (2006). Representative bureaucracy: Rethinking substan- Quiocho, A., & Rios, F. (2000). The power of their presence:
tive effects and active representation. Public Administration Minority group teachers and schooling. Review of Educational
Review, 66, 193–204. Research, 70(4), 485–528.
Marsh, H. W., Chessor, D., Craven, R., & Roche, L. (1995). The Rocha, R. R., & Hawes, D.P. (2009). Racial diversity, represen-
effects of gifted and talented programs on academic self-con- tative bureaucracy, and equity in multiracial school districts.
cept: The big fish strikes again. American Educational Research Social Science Quarterly, 90(2), 326-344.
Journal, 32(2), 285–319. Rogers, K.B. (2007). Lessons learned about educating the gifted
McBee, M. T. (2006). A descriptive analysis of referral sources and talented: A synthesis of the research on educational prac-
for gifted identification screening by race and socioeconomic tice. Gifted Child Quarterly, 51(4), 382–396.
status. Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 17, 103–111. Selden, S. (1997). The promise of representative bureaucracy:
Meier, K. J. (1993). Representative bureaucracy: A theoretical diversity and responsiveness in a government agency. Armonk,
and empirical exposition. In J. Perry (Ed.), Research in public NY: M. E. Sharpe.
administration (pp. 1–35). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Sleeter, C. E. (2001). Preparing teachers for culturally diverse
Meier, K. J., & Nicholson-Crotty, J. (2006). Gender, representative schools: Research and the overwhelming presence of Whiteness.
bureaucracy, and law enforcement: The case of sexual assault. Journal of Teacher Education, 52(2), 94–106.
Public Administration Review, 66, 850–860. Theobald, N. A., & Haider-Markel, D. P. (2009). Race, bureau-
Meier, K. J., & Stewart, J. (1992). The impact of representa- cracy, and symbolic representation: Interactions between citi-
tive bureaucracies: Educational systems and public policies. zens and police. Journal of Public Administration Research and
American Review of Public Administration, 22(3), 151–171. Theory, 19(2), 409–426.
Meier, K. J., Stewart, J., & England, R. (1989). Race, class and Tourangeau, K., Nord, C., Le, T., Pollack, J. M., Atkins-Burnett, S.,
education. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press. & Hausken, E. G. (2006). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study,
Mickelson, R. (2003). When are racial disparities in education the Kindergarten Class of 1998–99 (ECLS-K): Combined user’s
result of racial discrimination? A social science perspective. manual for the ECLS-K fifth-grade data files and electronic
Teachers College Record, 105(6), 1052–1086. codebooks.Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education,
Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Hillemeier, M. M., & Maczuga, S. National Center for Education Statistics.
(2012). Are minority children disproportionately represented U.S. Department of Education. (2010). Civil rights data collection,
in early intervention and early childhood special education? 2009–2010: National and state estimations. Retrieved from http://
Educational Researcher, 41(9), 339–351. ocrdata.ed.gov/StateNationalEstimations/Projections_2009_10
Mosher, F. (1968). Democracy and the public service. New York, Villegas, A. M., & Irvine, J. J. (2010). Diversifying the teaching
NY: Oxford University Press. force: An examination of major arguments. Urban Review,
National Center for Education Statistics. (2002). Early Childhood 42(3), 175–192.
Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten Class of 1998–99 (ECLS-K), Zhang, D., & Katsiyannis, A. (2002). Minority representation in
psychometric report for kindergarten through first grade. special education: A persistent challenge. Remedial and Special
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. Education, 23(3), 180–187.
Nicholson-Crotty, J., Grissom, J. A., & Nicholson-Crotty, S.
(2011). Bureaucratic representation, distributional equity, and
Authors
democratic values in the administration of public programs.
Journal of Politics, 73(2), 582–596. JASON A. GRISSOM is an associate professor of public policy
Oakes, J. (1990). Multiplying inequalities: The effects of race and education at the Peabody College of Education and Human
social class, and tracking on opportunities to learn mathemat- Development, Vanderbilt University, PMB 414, 230 Appleton
ics and science. Santa Monica, CA: RAND. Place, Nashville, TN 37203. He conducts research in the areas
Plant, E. A., & Peruche, B. M. (2005). The consequences of race for of K–12 leadership and governance, teacher and principal
police officers’ responses to criminal suspects. Psychological effectiveness, educator labor markets, and representation in
Science, 16(3), 180–183. education.
Pollack, J. M., Narajian, M., Rock, D.A., Atkins-Burnett, S., & CHRISTOPHER REDDING is a doctoral candidate at the Peabody
Hausken, E.G. (2005). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study– College of Education and Human Development, Vanderbilt
Kindergarten Class of 1998–99 (ECLS-K), psychometric University, PMB 414, 230 Appleton Place, Nashville, TN 37203.
report for the fifth grade. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of His research focuses on teacher labor markets, teacher education
Education, National Center for Education Statistics. and development, and school improvement.

25

You might also like