You are on page 1of 12

Original Article

Sleep Differences Among Children With Autism Spectrum


Disorders and Typically Developing Peers: A Meta-analysis
Marilisa G. Elrod, MD, PhD, Bradley S. Hood, MD

ABSTRACT: Objective: Sleep problems such as difficulties in sleep initiation, nighttime awakening, and
shortened sleep time are often subjectively reported in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD).
However, results of objective studies have been mixed. Our goal was to evaluate the existing data from
objective measures using a systematic approach to identify and describe the differences in sleep parameters
by comparing total sleep time (TST), sleep latency (SL), and sleep efficiency (SE) in children with ASD with
those of typically developing (TD) peers. Methods: Studies that used objective measures such as actigraphy or
polysomnography (PSG) to describe the sleep parameters of TST, SL, and SE in children with ASD compared
with children with TD were identified. A meta-analysis was performed for the 10 studies that met inclusion
criteria with evaluation of differences in means using random effects models. A total of 343 children with
ASD and 221 children with TD were included. Assessments for sources of heterogeneity and publication bias
were undertaken. Results: TST for children with ASD was on average 32.8 minutes less per day (95% con-
fidence interval [CI]: 16.6–49.0 minutes) than their TD peers. Average SL was 10.9 minutes longer (95% CI:
6.7–15.0 minutes), and average SE was 1.9% less (95% CI: 0.7%–3.1%) than their TD peers. Notable het-
erogeneity was found within studies for TST, and mild heterogeneity was found for SE. Concurrent intellectual
disability was a moderator of TST. Children with ASD and normal intelligence had a small and nonsignificant
decrease in TST as compared with TD peers, whereas those with ASD and intellectual disability (ID) had
a significant decrease in TST as compared with TD peers. The magnitude of the difference in mean SL and SE
increased as compared with TD peers as age increased. Studies that used PSG and those that did not include
children on medications were more likely to report mean decreases in SE. Conclusions: Children with ASD
have small but measurable objective differences in their sleep parameters that are consistent with subjective
reporting. Children with ASD have shorter TST, longer SL periods, and decreased SE as compared with TD
peers. Concurrent ID, medication use, method of data collection, and age of subjects significantly moderated
these results. The decrease in TST in children with ASD and normal intelligence was not significant as
compared with TD peers, suggesting that ID may help explain the shortened TST in children with ASD.
(J Dev Behav Pediatr 36:166–177, 2015) Index terms: autism, sleep, sleep parameters, sleep latency, sleep efficiency, total sleep time, polysomnography,
actigraphy.

S leep problems in children with autism spectrum dis-


order (ASD) are ubiquitously reported in the literature.1–5
Parents of children with ASD have been shown to have
higher levels of stress than both parents of typically de-
Poor sleep can be significantly impairing for a child with veloping (TD) children, and those with other dis-
ASD and his or her caregivers. Associations have also abilities.8 This increased stress has been linked to the
been shown between sleep problems and intensified severity of behavioral symptoms and their children’s
symptoms of autism and between sleep problems and poor sleep.9,10 Also, parents of children with autism have
decreased daytime cognitive and adaptive functioning.6,7 been shown to have poorer sleep quality and more
fragmented sleep than other parents.11,12
From the Department of Developmental-Behavioral Pediatrics, Madigan Army There have been many attempts to quantify the prev-
Medical Center, Tacoma, WA. alence of sleep abnormalities and characterize sleep
Received August 2014; accepted December 2014. behaviors and sleep quality in this population. However,
Disclosure: The authors declare no conflict of interest. many of these studies have been subjective and often
Supplemental digital content is available for this article. Direct URL citations lacked validated sleep questionnaires or strict diagnostic
appear in the printed text and are provided in the HTML and PDF versions of this
article on the journal’s Web site (www.jdbp.org).
criteria for autism.1,13 In general, parent-reported sleep
The views expressed are those of the authors and do not reflect the official policy
problem rates collected by survey are found to range from
of the Department of the Army, the Department of the Navy, the Department of 50% to 80% in children with ASD compared with 9% to
Defense, or the U.S. Government. 50% for comparison groups.14–26 The most common sleep
Address for reprints: Marilisa G. Elrod, MD, PhD, Department of Developmental- complaints are related to bedtime resistance, sleep initia-
Behavioral Pediatrics, Madigan Army Medical Center, ATTN: MCHJ-CLP-D, Joint
Base Lewis McChord, WA 98431; e-mail: marilisa.g.elrod.mil@mail.mil.
tion, nighttime awakening, and shortened sleep time.19
Early morning and night waking and shortened total sleep
Copyright Ó 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.
duration have been reported.18,22,27 However, in a report

166 | www.jdbp.org Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics


Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
by Schreck and Mulick28 using parental survey methods in articles were reviewed in detail. Eight were excluded
a population of children aged 5 to 12 years, overall sleep because they contained adult subjects, 2 did not use
duration was found to be similar between children with a control group of TD peers, 2 looked at longitudinal data
ASD and children with TD. Hours of sleep per night were from previously studied groups, and 1 reported sleep
also similar between groups of children with TD, children parameters that were determined by parental report.
with autism, and children with Asperger’s disorder in One was excluded as a result of the “Leave One Out”
a report by Polimeni et al.16 analysis. Ten studies were ultimately selected for in-
Objective data using actigraphy and polysomnography clusion in the meta-analysis. Refer to flow diagram in
(PSG) have shown mixed results.13,21,29,30 Some authors Figure 1 for details.
have suggested that differences between parental report
and objective findings may represent parental over- Moderating Factors
sensitivity to sleep problems with the increased demands Several factors were identified a priori as having po-
of caring for a child with ASD.31 Other studies in- tential to explain variability between the results of the
vestigated the relationship between objective and sub- previous studies. These were (1) inclusion of children in
jective measures of sleep with evidence of concordance the autism group with intellectual disability, (2) inclusion
between the 2.2,32,33 of children in either group using potentially sleep alter-
This systematic review and meta-analysis aims to ing medications, (3) inclusion of children in either group
summarize the objective information regarding the dif- with a seizure disorder, (4) method of data collection,
ferences in sleep parameters in children with ASD that is, actigraphy versus polysomnography (PSG), (5)
compared with their TD peers by focusing on measures age of study participants, and (6) year of publication.
that capture the most often described complaints of Intellectual Disability
prolonged sleep latency (SL), decreased total sleep time Intellectual disability (ID) is hypothesized to be in-
(TST), and poor sleep efficiency (SE) with frequent and dependently associated with alterations in sleep macro-
prolonged awakenings. Efforts are made to provide structure.34 Reviews of the literature in the past have
clinically relevant estimates of differences in sleep estimated the prevalence of ID in the population of
parameters between children with ASD and TD peers. children with autism to be 50% to 70%.35 However,
recent changes in diagnostic criteria to include all per-
vasive developmental disorders into a single diagnosis of
METHODS autism spectrum disorder likely renders these previous
Search Strategy for Identification of Studies figures an overestimate, as many children with normal
Three separate search engines were used to identify intelligence were previously given diagnoses of Asperg-
candidate studies, including PubMed, SCOPUS, and Google er’s disorder or pervasive developmental disorder not
Scholar using the search terms “Autism,” “sleep,” “sleep otherwise specified rather than autism spectrum disor-
duration,” “sleep problems,” and “Asperger Syndrome.” der (ASD). The most recent figures from the Centers for
The ancestry method was used, and the use of general Disease Control based on 2010 data estimate the preva-
search engines helped to identify gray literature. Inclusion lence of ID in children with ASD at 31%.36
criteria were determined to be the following: published in Sleep Altering Medications
English, published since 1994 (during the time the DSM-IV It is well known that psychotropic medications alter
was in use), include children, contain 1 control group of sleep patterns. Many children with ASD are treated with
typically developing (TD) peers, include measures of total stimulants, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, and
sleep time (TST) and/or sleep latency (SL) and/or sleep atypical neuroleptics. The most common stimulant used,
efficiency (SE) according to standard definitions, and use methylphenidate, is known to prolong SL and decrease
methods to determine these measures, which were ob- nighttime awakenings in children.37 Effects of SSRIs on
jective (such as actigraphy or PSG) instead of or in addition sleep architecture of children is extrapolated from adult
to surveys from individuals or caregivers. The following studies, in which SSRI use was associated with varied
definitions were used and confirmed as consistent for each effects, including prolonged SL and decreased SE.38 An-
study: TST— the time in minutes from sleep onset to the tiepileptic medications also cause various perturbations
end of sleep minus the time spent in wakefulness after of sleep architecture, which are compounded by the
initial sleep onset; SL— the time in minutes from bed- alterations that can be caused by the underlying seizure
time to the start of sleep; Sleep Efficiency (SE)— the disorder. Atypical neuroleptics are known to cause
percentage of time from sleep onset to the end of sleep sedation and therefore may shorten SL.39
spent in a sleep state as opposed to a wakeful state. Method of Data Collection
After removing duplicates, 201 abstracts were PSG is the current gold standard in measuring sleep
screened for inclusion. Ninety-nine articles were further macrostructure, but it is expensive and cumbersome.
scrutinized. Of these, 75 were excluded based on study Wrist actigraphy measures wrist movement to detect
design (e.g., case series, descriptive studies, review arti- sleep and wake states and is better tolerated as data are
cles, or other study designs that did not include objective collected in the patient’s normal sleeping environment
measures of sleep). The full texts of the remaining 24 where multiple nights of recordings can be collected.

Vol. 36, No. 3, April 2015 Copyright Ó 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 167

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Figure 1. Identification of independent studies for inclusion in the meta-analysis.

Although actigraphy has become increasingly common older. For 1 SD increase in IQ, prevalence of seizure dis-
in pediatric studies in the recent decade, there is still orders dropped by 47%.41 Seizure activity and sleep are in-
a lack of standard scoring rules. Both methods use tricately related. It has been established that epilepsy can
a procedure in which the sleep period is divided into cause measurable alteration in sleep structure to include
time segments, called epochs, and each epoch is cate- longer SL, poor SE, and a higher number of awakenings.42
gorized as wakefulness or sleep according to either Age of Study Participants
expert opinion or set parameters. Actigraphy has been As children develop, sleep patterns change. This is
shown to have a high accuracy in classifying epochs as most dramatic in the infant and toddler age ranges when
sleep across all pediatric age groups and devices when rapid development occurs but continues into adoles-
compared with PSG; unfortunately, a significant weak- cence. A recent meta-analysis of objective sleep param-
ness is its poor ability to detect epochs of motionless eters for ages 5 to 18 years by Ohayon et al aiming to
wakefulness interspersed among epochs of sleep.40 This establish normative sleep values for children and adults
may overestimate TST and SE because both depend on found that TST decreased over the childhood period only
the identification and quantification of wakefulness when measured on school days. Otherwise, TST
during the total sleep duration. remained consistent until the end of adolescence. SL and
Seizure Disorder SE did not seem to significantly change across childhood
Epilepsy is prevalent in children with ASD and is asso- and adolescence.43
ciated with decreased IQ and increased age. According to Year of Publication
a recent study by Viscidi et al, prevalence is 12% for children Although the diagnostic criteria of DSM-IV remained
of 2 to 17 years with ASD, but 26% for those of 13 years and constant throughout the time period queried, diagnostic

168 Alterations in Sleep Parameters in Children with Autism Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
practices did not. A recent Danish study described the summary measures for TST, SL, and SE, and coding of the
incidence of new cases of autism from 1995 to 2010 and moderator variables as described above.
reported an increase in incidence across all diagnostic All means and SDs for study groups consisting of
subtypes (but especially those with normal IQ) that was children with ASD were combined using a pooled SD
most pronounced in individuals from 14 to 20 years of calculated as if the combined group had never been di-
age.44 This reflects the general trend of increased di- vided for each individual article.47 For example, studies
agnosis that is believed to be partially because of in- by Goldman et al and Malow et al had separated the
creased clinical awareness for the disorder. This more children with ASD into 2 groups: good sleepers and poor
sensitive diagnostic approach would result in a larger sleepers. In each of these studies, the summary statistics
spectrum of severity of the cases of autism in the later for the poor and good sleeper group parameters were
studies’ samples. Additionally, the quality of the studies combined and compared with the control group
may have changed over the time period queried as parameters. This was possible because all subgroups
researchers improved on earlier studies or better tech- were formed after initial recruitment had occurred. See
nology became available. Table 1 for the study groups that were combined and
included in the analysis.
Methodologic Quality Assessment One actigraphy study had separated weekday from
The authors independently reviewed the methodol- weekend data.24 The data were recombined into meas-
ogy of each of the studies and coded all moderator var- ures including both weekend and weekday results
iables. Sources of potential bias were evaluated and commensurate with other studies included in the meta-
reported with decision of whether to include the study analysis. One study performed both actigraphy and PSG
in the pooled analysis made by the evaluators based on for the study and control groups.32 Only the data from
these assessments per the Preferred Reporting Items for PSG were used as this is the accepted gold standard and
Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses statement.45 A actigraphy was performed only on a subset of children
consensus approach was used with differences resolved. who also underwent PSG.
No quality score was assigned according to recom-
mendations of Sanderson et al46 for the assessment of Statistical Analyses
observational studies as there is no consensus on how to Calculation of Mean Differences
weight various aspects of the evaluation to generate The differences in means between sleep parameters
a meaningful summary measure that can be included in in the study group of children with ASD and TD were
the statistical analysis.47 examined as the primary outcome measures to better
interpret results and assess clinical significance
Data Extraction (X ASDi 2X TDi , for i 5 TST, SL, SE).53 SDs were not as-
Data for each study included demographics of the sumed to be equal in the calculation of pooled variance.
study and control groups, diagnoses of the study and Standardized mean differences in the form of Hedges’ g
control and groups, sample sizes for each group, were also calculated and reported. Hedges’ g was

Table 1. Description of Studies Using Actigraphy or Polysomnography to Assess Sleep Parameters in Children with Autism and Typically Developing
Controls
Sample Nights Mean Subjects Intellectual Seizure
ASD Study Sizes Used in Age, Taking Disability Disorder
First Author Country Group (ASD/TD) Method Analysis yr SD Medications Included Included
Hering et al31 Israel PS 8/8 ACT 3 8.0 2.7 Yes Yes Yes
Elia et al48 Italy ASD 13/5 PSG 1 8.9 2.2 No Yes No
Allik et al 24
Sweden AS/HFA 32/32 ACT 7 10.9 1.3 No No No
Malow et al33 USA PS 1 GS 19/9 PSG 1 6.7 2.0 No No No
Bruni et al 49
Italy AS 1 ASD 18/12 PSG 1 12.4 3.0 No Yes No
Miano et al2 Italy ASD 16/18 PSG 1 9.8 2.7 No Yes No
Goodlin-Jones USA ASD 68/69 ACT 7 3.6 0.9 Yes Yes No
et al13
Goldman et al32 USA PS 1 GS 42/16 PSG 1 ACT 2 6.2 1.9 No No No
Souders et al50 USA ASD 57/37 ACT 10 7.4 2.0 Yes Yes Yes
Buckley et al51 USA ASD 60/15 PSG 1 4.6 1.8 Yes Yes Yes
Giannotti et al52,a Italy REG 1 NREG 40/12 PSG 1 5.4 2.9 No Yes No
aThe Gianotti study was included in the systematic review but not in the formal meta-analysis because of statistical issues. ACT, actigraphy; AS, Asperger syndrome; ASD,
autism spectrum disorder; GS, good sleepers; HFA, high functioning autism; NREG, nonregressed autism; PS, poor sleepers; PSG, polysomnography; REG, regressed
autism; SD, standard deviation; TD, typically developing.

Vol. 36, No. 3, April 2015 Copyright Ó 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 169

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
selected to account for potential bias introduced with meta-analysis (Table 1). In addition to evaluating the re-
the inclusion of studies with small sample sizes.54 In- quired sleep parameters in the population of children
terpretation of Hedges’ g follows that of Cohen’s d with with ASD, 3 studies included an additional study group of
effect sizes of 0.2 considered small, 0.5 considered children with developmental delays or Fragile X.13,48,51
medium, and 0.8 considered large. Three studies also evaluated differences between chil-
Calculation and Analysis of Pooled Differences dren with autism and typical peers in nonrapid eye
in Means movement sleep microstructure in the form of cyclic
Each study’s differences in means were weighted alternating pattern.2,49,52 Other studies attempted to
according to the inverses of their variances including both correlate parental complaints with objective sleep
within and between-studies variance with random-effects data.2,31–33
models used to calculate the pooled differences in means. Giannotti et al52 stated that all parameters showed
Ninety-five percent confidence intervals (95% CIs) were positive skew and log transformed the data for analysis,
calculated for each study result and for the pooled estimates. although the authors reported raw means and SDs. This
Statistical analyses were performed using Comprehensive study was not ultimately included in the formal meta-
Meta-analysis (version 2.2; Biostat, Englewood, NJ) and R (R analysis because of significant influence on the parame-
Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria). ter mean differences found in the “leave-one-out” analysis.
Heterogeneity was assessed using the Cochran Q sta- This resulted in a total of 343 children with ASD and 221
tistic (a # .10) and the I2 statistic, which indicates the TD children included in the meta-analysis.
percentage of variation in the effect size estimate attrib- Recruitment
utable to heterogeneity rather than sampling error. An I2 Most studies used convenience sampling to generate
less than 30% indicates mild heterogeneity, whereas an I2 the index group, recruiting from subspecialty clinics.
greater than 50% suggests notable heterogeneity.55 Souders et al50 made an effort to randomly select indi-
Random-effects models were used in all analyses of viduals from their Regional Autism Center to provide
pooled mean difference in sleep parameters between a sample with which to estimate prevalence of sleep
children with ASD and TD to account for variability in disorders. Reporting of the method of recruitment of
methodology, setting, and populations. Random-effects control subjects was relatively poor overall with only 2
meta-regression analyses (using unrestricted maximum studies providing information detailing a randomized
likelihood estimation) and moderator analyses (using selection process while matching for age.31,50
mixed effects models) were performed to determine if
Age Considerations
the potential moderating factors described above could
The age of participants across studies ranged from 2
explain variability across studies. A post hoc power
years to 19 years of age. Most studies were performed
analysis for the categorical moderators between groups
using school-aged subjects with control children
differences was performed using the methods contained
matched for age. However, both Goodlin-Jones et al and
in Hedges and Pigott56 with the estimate of the non-
Buckley et al studied younger children and purposely
centrality parameter based on the mixed-model variance
chose controls that were 6 to 12 months younger than
of the group mean effect size estimates for each group
the index group to attempt to match for developmental
and a 5 .05. “Leave-one-out” analyses were conducted by
age rather than chronological age.13,51 Goodlin-Jones
iteratively deleting each study and calculating the result-
et al also included nap times for their young participants.
ing pooled mean difference.57
Analysis of Publication Bias Establishment of ASD Diagnosis
Publication bias was assessed visually using funnel Nine of the 11 studies performed diagnostic reas-
plots and also by using the commonly used regression sessment of the index group to ensure correct categori-
procedure to evaluate for asymmetry developed by Egger zation of children with ASD versus TD. Evaluations
et al.58 We adjusted for potential publication bias using included an Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale in 7 of
the trim-and-fill method by Duval and Tweedie.59 This the studies,13,32,33,49–52 the Autism Diagnostic Interview-
method evaluates the funnel plot for asymmetry with the Revised in 4 studies,13,49,51,52 the Child Autism Rating
assumption that this asymmetry indicates unpublished Scale in 2 studies,2,48 and the Asperger Syndrome Di-
studies in the area with fewer studies. The theoretically agnostic Scale in 1 study.50 The remaining 2 studies re-
missing studies are imputed, and the pooled estimate is lied on clinical diagnoses made before recruitment of the
recalculated including these studies. This adjusted result participants.24,31
can be used as a sensitivity analysis to assess the impact of Consideration of Intellectual Disability
the assumed publication bias on the pooled estimate.60 Some authors attempted to control for this con-
founder, whereas others specifically recruited subjects
with intellectual disability (ID) or global developmental
RESULTS delay for analysis. Only Buckley et al51 provided an av-
Description of the Studies Included erage nonverbal intelligence quotient (IQ) of 56.92 (SD
Eleven studies were selected for inclusion in the sys- 17.96) for their study participants with ASD. Goodlin-
tematic review, whereas 10 were selected for formal Jones et al13 provided an average score of 60.3 (SD

170 Alterations in Sleep Parameters in Children with Autism Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
18.2) for the group of children with ASD on the Mullen identity was reported as sleep technologist in 1 study,32
Scales of Early Learning (a cognitive ability measure). sleep technologist with sleep physician review in 1
Medication Use study,33 physician in 2 studies,2,49 and qualified sleep
Most studies excluded children taking psychotropic physician in 2 studies.51,52
or sleep-altering medications or required subjects to be Evaluation for Baseline EEG Abnormalities
off of medications for an extended period of time (at All but 3 of the studies excluded children with seiz-
least 2 weeks) before collection of sleep data. However, ures.31,50,51 Most studies excluded children on the basis
3 of the studies did include children on medications at of medical history, but the 4 studies performed in Italy
the time of data collection. Hering et al31 reported 1 required both normal neuroimaging and a normal EEG
child in the autistic group on carbamazepine. Goodlin- for inclusion.2,48,49,52
Jones et al13 did not exclude children taking medications
but did not report a list of medications taken. Souders Leave-One-Out Analysis
et al did not require children to be medication free and Sensitivity analyses indicated that only the Giannotti
included children on medications to treat allergies, study unduly influenced the pooled mean differences of
asthma, and gastrointestinal complaints, as well as stim- the sleep parameters studied between the group of
ulant medications, SSRIs, atypical neuroleptics, anti- children with autism and the group of typical controls
convulsants, and medications to aid sleep such as (Table, Supplemental Digital Content 1, http://links.lww.
melatonin and clonidine. The use of these medications com/JDBP/A73). On visual inspection of histograms and
was significantly higher in the ASD group as compared funnel plots, it was a clear outlier. Therefore, it was not
with the TD cohort.50 In the study by Buckley et al, included in the formal meta-analysis.
subjects were also allowed to continue any medications
including the night of the PSG recording. Only 1 child in Magnitude of Mean Differences and Main Effect Sizes
the TD group took an antihistamine, whereas 11 of the Forest plots for each outcome measure along with
children in the ASD group took SSRIs, atypical neuro- summary measures based on random effects models are
leptics, anticonvulsants, and medications to aid sleep.51 presented in Figure 2A–C. Study design and quality
Measurement Procedures seemed to improve with time. Therefore, the studies
Actigraphy was performed using various commer- were ordered by year of publication to observe for
cially available actigraphs with different softwares. Two a possible trend.
studies specifically used an actigraph with software that Table 2 provides summary information for each out-
used the Sadeh algorithm, which is the algorithm with come’s pooled analysis to include the number of studies
normative data on the pediatric population.31,50 PSG was included in the analysis, the pooled weighted differences
performed in sleep laboratories except for 1 study, in means and Hedge’s g with 95% confidence intervals,
where it occurred on an inpatient pediatric ward.51 and Cochran’s Q and I2 values for evaluation for hetero-
The “first-night effect” in PSG, where a patient’s first 2 geneity. In general, all pooled measures were statistically
consecutive nights of recordings in the sleep laboratory significant with effect sizes ranging from the small to
fundamentally differ because of accommodation to sur- medium range. Statistically significant heterogeneity of
roundings and testing, has been well documented in results was found in TST but not for SL or SE. I2 values
children and adults and more recently in children with indicated mild heterogeneity among the studies for sleep
autism.61 For this reason, all but 2 of the studies using efficiency and notable heterogeneity among the studies
PSG excluded the first night from analysis.32,51 for TST.
Some authors experienced difficulties in sleep moni-
toring in the population of children with ASD due to Impact of Moderator Variables for Total Sleep Time
sensory issues and intolerance to change and new The relationship between autism and TST was mod-
settings. Hering et al31 were unable to collect data on erated by ID. Studies that included children with ID in
one-third of their original sample because of subjects the index group showed a pooled difference in means in
removing the actigraph from their wrists due to sensory TST of 245.0 minutes (95% CI: 262.6 to 228.6) from
intolerance. Souders et al50 piloted a new method of the typical controls. However, the remaining 3 studies
collecting actigraphy data by placing the actigraph in that excluded children with ID showed a much smaller
a soft cotton pocket hidden in the sleeve of a fitted and nonsignificant difference in TST between the group
pajama top. of children with autism and the control group of 27.3
PSG monitoring differed between the studies but at minutes (95% CI: 221.5 to 7.0) (Table 3).
a minimum included electroencephalogram (ranging Method of data collection and inclusion of children on
from 3 to 21 leads), electrooculogram, and electromyo- psychotropic medication or with seizure disorders were
gram. Only 2 studies included pulmonary monitoring to not found to be moderators. The continuous variables of
detect sleep disordered breathing.32,33 Other studies mean subject age (slope [95% CI]: 20.26 [26.27 to
excluded children with reported snoring.49,52 Scorers of 5.758]; p 5 .933) and publication year (slope [95% CI]:
the PSGs were reported as blinded to subject’s study 1.53 [23.61 to 6.67]; p 5 .560) were also not found to be
group in 4 of the 7 studies using PSG.32,33,51,52 Scorer moderators, although confidence intervals were wide.

Vol. 36, No. 3, April 2015 Copyright Ó 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 171

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Figure 2. Forest plots for mean differences and combined mean differences between groups of children with autism spectrum disorders (AUT) and
typically developing peers based on random effects models for (A) total sleep time (TST), (B) sleep latency (SL), and (C) sleep efficiency (SE). Studies are
arranged in chronological order according to date of publication. Study-specific mean differences are denoted by black boxes with 95% confidence
intervals (95% CI) shown as black lines. Box sizes are proportional to individual study weights. The combined mean differences are represented by black
diamonds, where diamond widths correspond to 95% CI bounds.

Impact of Moderator Variables for Sleep Latency found to be a moderator (Table 3). The meta-regression
Inclusion of children with ID, on psychotropic medi- of mean subject age on difference in mean SL showed an
cations, or with seizure disorders was not found to be increase in difference in mean SL as age increased (slope
moderators. Method of data collection was also not [95% CI]: 1.51 [0.13 to 2.90]; p 5 .032). Publication year

172 Alterations in Sleep Parameters in Children with Autism Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Table 2. Pooled Differences in Means and Effect Sizes with Homogeneity Statistics for Sleep Variables
Difference in Means Hedges g Heterogeneity
With 95% CI With 95% CI p Qdf p I2
Total sleep time (minutes, k 5 10) 232.8 (249.0 to 216.6) 20.55 (20.82 to 20.28) ,.001 28.39 .001 68.2
Sleep latency (minutes, k 5 9) 10.9 (6.7 to 15.0) 0.41 (0.18 to 0.63) ,.001 8.98 .355 9.7
Sleep efficiency (%, k 5 10) 21.9 (23.1 to 20.7) 20.28 (20.45 to 20.10) .003 12.99 .165 30.5
Pooled difference in means and Hedges g were calculated using random effects models. Heterogeneity statistics for differences in means were calculated using fixed
effects models. CI, confidence interval; df, degrees of freedom; k, number of studies included.

(slope [95% CI]: 21.72 [23.61 to 0.16]; p 5 .07) was not Publication Bias
found to be related to difference in means but The funnel plots of each parameter (in which each
approached statistical significance. study’s effect size was plotted against the standard error)
were inspected for asymmetry. Marked asymmetry was
Impact of Moderator Variables for Sleep Efficiency noted in the case of SE suggesting that studies with
Method of data collection was found to be a modera- negative findings may not have been published. This was
tor. Pooling of the studies that used actigraphy did not confirmed by the regression intercept approach. The
result in a statistically significant difference in means in trim-and-fill adjusted pooled mean difference (95% CI)
SE between children with ASD and TD peers, whereas with 3 imputed studies was found to no longer be sta-
those who used PSG yielded a pooled difference in tistically significant at 21.2 (22.6 to 0.2). This indicates
means of 23.5% (95% CI: 25.3 to 21.6; p , .001). In- that results for sleep efficiency were sensitive to the
clusion of children on psychotropic medications also effects of possible publication bias (Figure, Supplemental
was a moderating factor for difference in mean SE. Digital Content 2, http://links.lww.com/JDBP/A74 and
Studies that included children taking medications did not Table, Supplemental Digital Content 3, http://links.lww.
show a significant pooled difference in mean SE, com/JDBP/A73).
whereas those that excluded children on medication did
(22.9%, 95% CI: 24.3 to 21.4; p ,.001). Inclusion of DISCUSSION
children with ID and with seizure disorders was not This systematic review found that, on average, chil-
found to be moderators (Table 3). dren with ASD had shorter TST, longer SL, and lower SE
The meta-regression of mean subject age on differ- than their TD peers. Standardized effect sizes were small
ence in mean SE showed an increase in magnitude of to medium. Although these differences are not extreme,
difference in mean SE as age increased (slope with 95% they are noticeable to parents, who have frequently
CI: 20.32 [20.62 to 20.02]; p 5 .034). Publication year reported such differences to clinicians and in studies
(slope with 95% CI: 0.11 [20.28 to 0.51]; p 5 .566) was including parental sleep questionnaires. This amount of
not found to be related to difference in means. sleep disturbance may be enough to alter parents’ sleep
quality and add stress and also exacerbate the behavioral
Post Hoc Power Analysis of the Comparison of symptoms of autism.
Subgroups Within Categorical Moderators The meta-analysis was performed with the goal of
Power, in this case, can be thought of as the sensi- making the results as generalizable as possible to the
tivity to detect differences between subgroups if they population of children with ASD by including all di-
exist. This has no bearing on the tests where a signifi- agnoses within the spectrum and using random effects
cant result has been determined such as the difference models. Despite the limitations of the moderator analysis
between the subgroups of children with ASD and ID and because of small sample sizes and a small number of
those with ASD without ID. However, it provides con- available studies resulting in low power for some com-
text for the comparisons that were not found to be parisons, the results are believed to be consistent with
significant. To interpret the nonsignificant tests in the the subjective reporting of the underlying changes in
moderator analysis as evidence that effects do not differ sleep parameters as experienced by parents. In fact, the
across subgroups, it should be clear that power is suf- improved power of the combined analysis allowed the
ficient to detect meaningful differences. The power confirmation of what had been subjectively reported in
analysis is presented in Table 4. Two choices of the underpowered studies used in this analysis. For example,
smallest meaningful difference are presented with evi- the parents in Miano et al reported prolonged SL, shorter
dence of variable power. Power for any moderator TST, and more interrupted sleep patterns. Only TST was
analysis is expected to be lower than that for the main found to be significantly reduced in the underpowered
effect tested. However, it is noted that across all sleep objective arm of the study.2 In the small study (n 5 8
parameters, the test of SZD as a moderator was children with ASD) by Hering et al, where parental
underpowered. reporting of nighttime arousals and early morning

Vol. 36, No. 3, April 2015 Copyright Ó 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 173

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Table 3. Categorical Moderator Analysis
Within Group
Heterogeneity Effect of Moderator
2
Moderator No. of Studies D (95% CI) p Qdf Within p I Qdf Between p
Total sleep time (min)
ID
Included (240/164) 7 245.0 (262.6 to 228.6) ,.001 12.46 .054 51.6 11.51 .001
Excluded (93/57) 3 27.3 (221.5 to 7.0) .316 1.02 .599 0.0
Medications
Yes (193/129) 4 235.7 (258.0 to 213.4) .002 6.93 .076 56.3 0.11 .810
No (140/92) 6 231.7 (256.1 to 27.3) .011 20.05 .001 75.0
Method
Actigraphy (165/146) 4 222.1 (238.3 to 25.8) .008 5.63 .131 46.8 1.61 .201
PSG (168/75) 6 242.9 (270.5 to 215.4) .002 20.05 .001 74.9
SZD
Included (125/60) 3 234.5 (275.6 to 6.6) .100 6.62 .038 69.5 0.011 .925
Excluded (208/161) 7 232.3 (251.5 to 213.1) .001 21.76 .001 72.4
Sleep latency (min)
ID
Included (232/156) 6 10.2 (3.4 to 17.1) .003 8.35 .139 16.5 0.291 .588
Excluded (93/57) 3 12.9 (6.0 to 19.9) ,.001 0.12 .974 0.0
Medications
Yes (187/121) 3 6.6 (22.1 to 15.2) .136 4.82 .090 58.4 2.221 .136
No (140/92) 6 14.6 (8.6 to 20.5) ,.001 1.65 .902 0.0
Method
Actigraphy (157/138) 3 10.3 (4.7 to 15.9) ,.001 3.52 .175 42.6 0.161 .691
PSG (168/75) 6 12.4 (3.8 to 20.9) .005 5.25 .390 4.1
SZD
Included (117/52) 2 6.1 (211.0 to 23.2) .482 3.01 .716 66.3 0.271 .603
Excluded (208/161) 7 10.8 (6.1 to 15.6) ,.001 5.96 .436 0.0
Sleep efficiency (%)
ID
Included (240/164) 7 22.2 (24.1 to 20.2) .028 12.16 .060 50.3 0.011 .939
Excluded (93/57) 3 22.1 (23.7 to 20.5) .012 0.22 .907 0.0
Medications
Yes (193/129) 4 20.6 (21.8 to 0.6) .314 3.03 .385 1.3 5.641 .018
No (140/92) 6 22.9 (24.3 to 21.4) ,.001 4.15 .533 0.0
Method
Actigraphy (165/146) 4 20.9 (22.0 to 0.2) .113 3.33 .350 8.7 5.491 .019
PSG (168/75) 6 23.5 (25.3 to 21.6) ,.001 3.95 .571 0.0
SZD
Included (125/60) 3 21.0 (23.9 to 1.9) .498 3.02 .229 32.3 0.581 .446
Excluded (208/161) 7 22.2 (23.7 to 20.8) .003 9.66 .142 37.6
A mixed effects method was used with random effects models used to combine studies within each subgroup, and fixed effects models used to combine groups and yield
the overall effect. Sample sizes for the group of children with autism/typically developing peers are provided for each subgroup. CI, confidence interval; D, pooled
difference in means; DF, degrees of freedom; ID, intellectual disability; min, minutes; PSG, polysomnography; SZD, seizure disorder.

awakenings were significantly different in parents of stated that “Parental oversensitivity to sleep disturbances
children with ASD as compared with TD controls but of the autistic children may explain this phenomenon.”31
was not confirmed by actigraphic monitoring, it was Although more data are needed to confirm the size of the

174 Alterations in Sleep Parameters in Children with Autism Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Table 4. Posthoc Power for Categorical Moderator Analysis heterogeneity between studies were SL and TST on
Power to Detect a Difference Between nonschool days.43 The difference in mean SL between
Subgroups children with ASD and TD peers seems to be greater in
the school aged children compared with the preschool-
Total sleep time 30 min 40 min
aged children. This effect seems to be stable in the
ID 0.756 0.943 school years. This could reflect the high prevalence of
Medications 0.428 0.659 bedtime resistance in the preschool-aged population
Method 0.451 0.688 estimated at nearly 30% tapering off to 13% by middle
SZD 0.254 0.408 school, which could obscure underlying differences in
SL in the younger ages.63 Age also was related to the
Sleep latency 10 min 15 min
difference in SE, with the magnitude of the mean dif-
ID 0.519 0.854
ference increasing as the subjects’ ages increased.
Medications 0.464 0.801 Method of data collection was found to moderate the
Method 0.462 0.819 differences in mean SE but not the differences in mean
SZD 0.198 0.382 TST. This is an interesting finding as both of these
Sleep efficiency 2% 3% parameters rely on the identification and measurement
of sleep epochs within episodes of sleep, which is
ID 0.337 0.636
a known weakness of actigraphy. This may be explained
Medications 0.565 0.889
by the lower power to detect meaningful differences in
Method 0.449 0.784 the moderator analyses for method of data collection and
SZD 0.228 0.444 TST. The point estimates of the pooled mean differences
Power calculated using the mixed-model variances of the group mean effect size for TST in the actigraphy versus the PSG subgroups
estimates for each group and a 5 .05. ID, intellectual disability; min, minutes; provide some evidence that actigraphy may be inferior at
SZD, seizure disorder.
detecting the underlying difference, although it did not
reach the level of statistical significance.
differences between the sleep parameters of children The variable approach to inclusion of children on
with ASD as compared with TD peers, this meta-analysis medications only moderated the difference in mean SE.
substantiates the existence of the differences that have Studies that included children on medications were less
been subjectively reported. likely to report significant mean differences in SE.
The amount of heterogeneity among the studies in the However, for TST and SL, medications seemed to have
measurement of TST was high and at least partially re- little effect. Indeed, the study by Souders et al50 did not
lated to the decision to include children with ID. Taylor show normalization of sleep for the children on medi-
et al7 have shown a correlation between sleep duration cations to treat sleep-related concerns. They described
and full scale, and verbal IQ scores in that sleep duration the use of sleep medications rather as a predictor of
decreases with IQ. Therefore, by selecting children for sleep disturbances as these children’s sleep parameters
the study group with normal IQ results, one may also be were more affected than the other children not taking
selecting those who have more normal sleep duration. medications. Parents of these children perceived the
Although the number of studies that did not include medications as helping and improving the quality of their
children with intellectual disabilities was small, the lives. This again is evidence that parents are sensitive to
pooled mean difference in TST was not statistically sig- even minor changes in their children’s quality of sleep
nificant (27.3 minutes; 95% CI: 221.5 to 7.0; p 5 .316). such as slightly improved SE. It also may speak to the
Note that for SL and SE, the presence of ID was not difficulty in pharmacologically targeting what may be
a moderating factor. These findings are also consistent intrinsic biological or genetic abnormalities that result in
with data from studies of adults with autism and normal alteration in neuronal pathways in ASD.64
intelligence where TST was not different from matched It has been hypothesized that hyperarousal related to
controls, but SL was prolonged and SE was decreased.62 increased anxiety and perseverative symptoms of autism
Thus, a child or an adult with ASD would take longer to is to blame for the difficulties in sleep initiation and
fall asleep and then also to achieve the same TST as their maintenance in people with autism. However, in adults,
TD peers because of decreased SE. The interaction be- Limoges et al62 showed changes in sleep parameters
tween IQ, autism, and decreased TST requires additional without subjective complaints and without an increase
evaluation that was not possible with the limited data in cortisol levels. This may point to a more fundamental
available. alteration in neurobiology as a cause of the differences in
Very little heterogeneity between studies was found SL and SE. The persistence of the sleep alterations into
in the measurement of difference in SL. There seemed to the adult years also supports this.
be little effect if it was measured by actigraphy or PSG. The results of this meta-analysis should be inter-
This is consistent with the meta-analysis that described preted in the context of its limitations. First, the final
normative sleep values across the life span, where the meta-analyses were conducted with 10 studies with
only measures in children that did not show significant relatively small sample sizes. Although it was

Vol. 36, No. 3, April 2015 Copyright Ó 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 175

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
sufficiently well powered for the main effect estima- REFERENCES
tion, it was not well powered for the moderator analysis 1. Sivertsen B, Posserud MB, Gillberg C, et al. Sleep problems in
as is common in meta-analyses. The small number of children with autism spectrum problems: a longitudinal
studies also precludes the evaluation of more sophisti- population-based study. Autism. 2012;16:139–150.
cated models and interactions between moderators. 2. Miano S, Bruni O, Elia M, et al. Sleep in children with autistic
Second, the small sizes of some of the studies make spectrum disorder: a questionnaire and polysomnographic study.
Sleep Med. 2007;9:64–70.
the assumption of normality tenuous. Meta-analytic
3. Baker E, Richdale A, Short M, et al. An investigation of sleep
methods rely on the principle of the central limit the- patterns in adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum
orem where the distribution of the mean of the mean disorder compared with typically developing adolescents. Dev
differences approaches the normal distribution. This Neurorehabil. 2013;16:155–165.
convergence occurs irrespective of the distribution of 4. Anders TF, Iosif AM, Schwichtenberg AJ, et al. Six-month sleep-
wake organization and stability in preschool-age children with
the underlying distributions from which the mean dif-
autism, developmental delay, and typical development. Behav
ferences originated. However, this is not as robust for Sleep Med. 2011;9:92–106.
small sample sizes in individual studies. Many of the 5. Ming X, Walters AS. Autism spectrum disorders, attention
studies with small sample sizes used nonparametric deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and sleep disorders. Curr Opin Pulm
statistics as not to assume a normal distribution (al- Med. 2009;15:578–584.
though they all reported means and SDs). Regardless, 6. Schreck KA, Mulick JA, Smith AF. Sleep problems as possible
predictors of intensified symptoms of autism. Res Dev Disabil.
the results which were most heavily weighted in the 2004;25:57–66.
analysis were not unduly influenced by the smaller 7. Taylor MA, Schreck KA, Mulick JA. Sleep disruption as a correlate
studies with larger SDs. We used a small sample size to cognitive and adaptive behavior problems in autism spectrum
correction in our estimation procedure. We also re- disorders. Res Dev Disabil. 2012;33:1408–1417.
moved 1 study that reported skewness and would have 8. Hastings R, Kovshoff H, Ward N, et al. Systems analysis of stress and
positive perceptions in mothers and fathers of pre-school children
had a significant impact on the size of the pooled dif-
with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35:635–644.
ferences in means reported, although not the in- 9. Tomanik S, Harris GE, Hawkins J. The relationship between
ference. It is difficult to determine if skewness affected behaviours exhibited by children with autism and maternal stress.
the remainder of the studies, as this was not reported. J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2004;29:16–26.
The best way to evaluate this and to fully combine the 10. Doo S, Wing YK. Sleep problems of children with pervasive
developmental disorders: correlation with parental stress. Dev Med
available data would be to collate individual participant
Child Neurol. 2006;48:650–655.
data to alleviate the reliance on reported summary 11. Meltzer LJ. Brief report: sleep in parents of children with autism
measures. spectrum disorders. J Pediatr Psychol. 2008;33:380–386.
Third, there was a large amount of heterogeneity be- 12. Cotton S, Richdale A. Brief report: parental descriptions of sleep
tween studies in the parameter of TST likely related to problems in children with autism, Down syndrome, and Prader–
the variable inclusion of children with ID. It would be Willi syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2006;27:151–161.
13. Goodlin-Jones BL, Tang K, Liu J, et al. Sleep patterns in preschool-
interesting to evaluate the results based on average IQ of age children with autism, developmental delay, and typical
participants in the group of children with autism. How- development. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47:
ever, most studies did not provide this information. Au- 930–938.
tism may only slightly decrease TST, whereas ID may be 14. Honomichl RD, Goodlin-Jones BL, Burnham M, et al. Sleep patterns
the most important factor either in combination with of children with pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev
Disord. 2002;32:553–561.
ASD or alone. More research is needed to understand the
15. Richdale AL, Schreck KA. Sleep problems in autism spectrum
interaction between ID and ASD and their effect on sleep disorders: prevalence, nature, & possible biopsychosocial
parameters. The pooled difference in mean TST between aetiologies. Sleep Med Rev. 2009;13:403–411.
the group of children with ASD and their TD peers 16. Polimeni MA, Richdale AL, Francis AJ. A survey of sleep problems
should be interpreted with this heterogeneity in mind. in autism, Asperger’s disorder and typically developing children.
J Intellect Disabil Res. 2005;49:260–268.
Finally, possible publication bias was evident in the
17. Miano S, Ferri R. Epidemiology and management of insomnia in
reporting of SE. It is difficult to draw inference from this children with autistic spectrum disorders. Paediatr Drugs. 2010;
isolated finding as there was little evidence of possible 12:75–84.
publication bias for TST or SL, and these parameters 18. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. Variables related to sleep problems in
were derived from the same 10 studies. It cannot be children with autism. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2009;3:931–941.
ignored that the imputed studies by the trim-and-fill 19. Krakowiak P, Goodlin-Jones B, Hertz-Picciotto I, et al. Sleep
problems in children with autism spectrum disorders,
method did change the inference showing that our developmental delays, and typical development: a population-
measure of change in SE may not be robust to publica- based study. J Sleep Res. 2008;17:197–206.
tion bias. 20. Liu X, Hubbard JA, Fabes RA, et al. Sleep disturbances and
correlates of children with autism spectrum disorders. Child
Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2006;37:179–191.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 21. Wiggs L, Stores G. Sleep patterns and sleep disorders in children
The authors would like to thank Dr Sean Meagher, MD, Dr with autistic spectrum disorders: insights using parent report and
Christine Erdie-Lalena MD, Dr Bonnie Geneman, MD, Mr Michael actigraphy. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2004;46:372–380.
Elrod, MA, and Ms Kathleen Knorr, LICSW for their thoughtful 22. Richdale AL, Prior MR. The sleep/wake rhythm in children with
reading of the article. autism. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1995;4:175–186.

176 Alterations in Sleep Parameters in Children with Autism Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics
Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
23. Gail Williams P, Sears LL, Allard A. Sleep problems in children with 44. Jensen CM, Steinhausen HC, Lauritsen MB. Time trends over 16
autism. J Sleep Res. 2004;13:265–268. years in incidence-rates of autism spectrum disorders across the
24. Allik H, Larsson JO, Smedje H. Sleep patterns of school-age children lifespan based on nationwide Danish register data. J Autism Dev
with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:1808–1818.
Disord. 2006;36:585–595. 45. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, et al. The PRISMA statement for
25. Patzold LM, Richdale AL, Tonge BJ. An investigation into sleep reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that
characteristics of children with autism and Asperger’s Disorder. evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration.
J Paediatr Child Health. 1998;34:528–533. PLoS Med. 2009;6:e1000100.
26. Johnson CR, Turner KS, Foldes EL, et al. Comparison of sleep 46. Sanderson S, Tatt ID, Higgins JP. Tools for assessing quality and
questionnaires in the assessment of sleep disturbances in children susceptibility to bias in observational studies in epidemiology:
with autism spectrum disorders. Sleep Med. 2012;13:795–801. a systematic review and annotated bibliography. Int J Epidemiol.
27. Hoshino Y, Watanabe H, Yashima Y, et al. An investigation on sleep 2007;36:666–676.
disturbance of autistic children. Folia Psychiatr Neurol Jpn. 1984; 47. Higgins JPT, Deeks JJ. Chapter 7: selecting studies and collecting
38:45–51. data. In: Higgins JPT, Green S, eds. Cochrane Handbook for
28. Schreck KA, Mulick JA. Parental report of sleep problems in Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 (Updated
children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:127–135. 2011). Oxford, UK: The Cochrane Collaboration; 2011. Available
29. Allik H, Larsson JO, Smedje H. Insomnia in school-age children with at: http://handbook.cochrane.org/. Accessed May 13, 2014.
Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. BMC Psychiatry. 48. Elia M, Ferri R, Musumeci SA, et al. Sleep in subjects with autistic
2006;6:18. disorder: a neurophysiological and psychological study. Brain Dev.
30. Øyane NMF, Bjørvatn B. Sleep disturbances in adolescents and 2000;22:88–92.
young adults with autism and Asperger syndrome. Autism. 2005;9: 49. Bruni O, Ferri R, Vittori E, et al. Sleep architecture and NREM
83–94. alterations in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome.
31. Hering E, Epstein R, Elroy S, et al. Sleep patterns in autistic Sleep. 2007;30:1577–1585.
children. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29:143–147. 50. Souders MC, Mason TBA, Valladares O, et al. Sleep behaviors and
32. Goldman SE, Surdyka K, Cuevas R, et al. Defining the sleep sleep quality in children with autism spectrum disorders. Sleep.
phenotype in children with autism. Dev Neuropsychol. 2009;34: 2009;32:1566–1578.
560–573. 51. Buckley AW, Rodriguez AJ, Jennison K, et al. Rapid eye movement
33. Malow BA, Marzec ML, McGrew SG, et al. Characterizing sleep in sleep percentage in children with autism compared with children
children with autism spectrum disorders: a multidimensional with developmental delay and typical development. Arch Pediatr
approach. Sleep. 2006;29:1563. Adolesc Med. 2010;164:1032–1037.
34. Miano S, Bruni O, Elia M, et al. Sleep phenotypes of intellectual 52. Giannotti F, Cortesi F, Cerquiglini A, et al. Sleep in children with
disability: a polysomnographic evaluation in subjects with Down autism with and without autistic regression. J Sleep Res. 2011;20:
syndrome and Fragile-X syndrome. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119: 338–347.
1242–1247. 53. Cummings P. Arguments for and against standardized mean
35. Matson JL, Shoemaker M. Intellectual disability and its relationship differences (effect sizes). Arch Adolesc Med. 2011;165:592–596.
to autism spectrum disorders. Res Dev Disabil. 2009;30: 54. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical Methods for Meta–Analysis.
1107–1114. Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1985.
36. Baio J. Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders in Children Aged 55. Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quanitfying heterogeneity in a meta-
8 Years: Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring analysis. Stat Med. 2002;21:1539–1558.
Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2010. MMWR Surveill Summa. 56. Hedges LV, Pigott TD. The power of statistical tests for moderators
2014;63:6. in meta-analysis. Psychol Methods. 2004;9:426.
37. Sangal RB, Owens J, Allen AJ, et al. Effects of atomoxetine and 57. Greenhouse JB, Iyengar S. Sensitivity analysis and diagnostics. In:
methylphenidate on sleep in children with ADHD. Sleep. 2006;29: Cooper HM, Hedges L, eds. The Handbook of Research Synthesis.
1573. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation; 2009.
38. Jindal RD, Friedman ES, Berman SR, et al. Effects of sertraline on 58. Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, et al. Bias in meta-analysis
sleep architecture in patients with depression. J Clin detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315:629–634.
Psychopharmacol. 2003;23:540–548. 59. Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot–based
39. McConville BJ, Sorter MT. Treatment challenges and safety method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-
considerations for antipsychotic use in children and adolescents analysis. Biometrics. 2000;56:455–463.
with psychoses. J Clin Psychiatry. 2003;65:20–29. 60. Peters JL, Sutton AJ, Jones DR, et al. Performance of the trim and fill
40. Meltzer LJ, Montgomery-Downs HE, Insana SP, et al. Use of method in the presence of publication bias and between-study
actigraphy for assessment in pediatric sleep research. Sleep Med heterogeneity. Stat Med. 2007;26:4544–4562.
Rev. 2012;16:463–475. 61. Buckley A, Wingert K, Swedo S, et al. First night effect analysis in
41. Viscidi EW, Triche EW, Pescosolido MF, et al. Clinical a cohort of young children with autism spectrum disorder. J Clin
characteristics of children with autism spectrum disorder and co- Sleep Med. 2013;9:67–70.
occurring epilepsy. PLoS One. 2013;8:e67797. 62. Limoges É, Mottron L, Bolduc C, et al. Atypical sleep architecture
42. Eriksson SH. Epilepsy and sleep. Curr Opin Neurol. 2011;24: and the autism phenotype. Brain. 2005;128:1049–1061.
171–176. 63. Archbold KH, Pituch KJ, Panahi P, et al. Symptoms of sleep
43. Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, et al. Meta-analysis of disturbances among children at two general pediatric clinics.
quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy J Pediatr. 2002;140:97–102.
individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human 64. Cortesi F, Giannotti F, Ivanenko A, et al. Sleep in children with
lifespan. Sleep. 2004;27:1255–1274. autistic spectrum disorder. Sleep Med. 2010;11:659–664.

Vol. 36, No. 3, April 2015 Copyright Ó 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. 177

Copyright © 2015 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like