You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/259738471

Remembering Intergroup Conflict

Chapter · January 2012


DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199747672.013.0008

CITATIONS READS
29 725

2 authors:

Rezarta Bilali Michael Ross


New York University University of Waterloo
39 PUBLICATIONS   857 CITATIONS    112 PUBLICATIONS   13,626 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Doctoral thesis View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Rezarta Bilali on 16 January 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

CH A PT E R

Remembering Intergroup Conflict


8
Rezarta Bilali and Michael A. Ross

Abstract
Philosopher George Santayana is probably best known today for his aphorism, “Those who cannot
remember the past are condemned to repeat it.” Variations of this quote appear frequently when
countries, ethnic groups, or individuals engage in conflicts over economic, legal, or territorial issues.
The version of history provided by one’s own side is depicted as a true account of the past, whereas
contrasting versions provided by one’s opponents are deemed to be lies. In the current chapter,
we examine bases of conflicting narratives of the past. We document how individual- and group-
level factors (e.g., needs, goals, motives) shape historical memories, and how historical memories
exacerbate conflict. In the final section of the chapter, we consider how memories can be used
promote reconciliation rather conflict.
Key Words: Intergroup conflict, historical memories, historical injustices, harm-doing, victimization,
apologies, reconciliation

Individuals often suppose that they know the memories are embedded and reinforced in the images
history of the ethnic, religious, and national groups people encounter in their everyday lives. Important
to which they belong. Their historical memories historical figures and events are featured in coins,
include details and interpretations of past events that banknotes, street names, and public monuments.
they did not personally experience. These historical The combination of various forms of remembering
memories are to group identity what personal mem- makes historical memory resistant to extinction.
ories are to individual identity. Historical memory is Periodic commemorations celebrate important days
the glue that connects group members, events, and in each group or nation’s history providing group
ideas through time and space. Regardless of their members with a particular version of the past.
accuracy, historical memories provide the bases of Beyond these overarching societal influences,
people’s understanding of the origins, story, and cognitive and motivational factors influence how
characteristics of a group (Billig, 1995; Halbwachs, members of particular groups attend to, process, and
1950/1980). Historical memories help establish retrieve the available historical information (Blatz &
and support individuals’ beliefs about their group’s Ross, 2009). In fashioning stories of the past, both
uniqueness and standing relative to other groups. ordinary people and professional historians go
There are many sources of historical memory, beyond the simple cataloging of major events and
including societal influences conveyed through his- dates. As the term “story” implies, historical memo-
tory books, education, TV, films, novels, and plays ries are narratives that include appraisals of individu-
(Bar-Tal, 2000; Schumann, Akiyama, & Knauper, als and groups, as well as inferences about how they
1999; see also Iyer & Blatz, this volume). Historical acted and why. Even the narratives of professional

123

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 123 3/8/2012 5:45:53 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

historians are fictions to varying degrees. Often because memory is prescriptive as well as descriptive
dependent on inconsistent or incomplete sources, (Halbwachs, 1950/1980; Mead, 1929). Historical
professional historians forge meaningful stories by memories shape people’s current beliefs, foster col-
imposing their own interpretations and coherence lective action, and maintain and intensify group
on the information available (Trouillot, 1995). conflicts. In an ethnographic study of Hutu refugees
Different versions of a group’s history serve the who escaped the 1972 genocide in Burundi, Malkki
interests of different groups. The transmission of his- (1995) demonstrated how conflict memories can
torical memories is an ongoing process of negotiation shape beliefs. Malkki compared refugees’ historical
among ideological and interest groups to promote narratives to Bible stories. Refugees’ historical mem-
their favored interpretations of the past. For instance, ories included strong moral lessons and implica-
in some states in the United States there are ideo- tions. After telling a story, people would discuss its
logical and religious disputes over the depiction of moral implications as well as the lessons it provided
American history in school texts. Dominated by con- for the present and future (Malkki, 1995). Hutu
servative Christians, the Texas Board of Education refugees used their historical narratives of conflict
recently changed the state curriculum to provide a to educate people about the world, both past and
more religious and Christian orientation to American present.
history (McKinley, 2010). The revised curriculum Historical memories inform not only the under-
specifies that the Founding Fathers intended to build standing of the present, but also influence expec-
a Christian nation and downplays the contributions tations for the future. Historical experiences serve
of Thomas Jefferson, who advocated the separation to identify one’s current foes and allies, as well as
of church and state. Whatever one thinks of such identify and interpret current and future threats
amendments, the changes illustrate the importance of (Liu & Hilton, 2005). Memories of past griev-
historical memory. Groups attempt to use the teach- ances alert group members to potential threats and
ing of history to advance their beliefs and values. dangers. Historical victimization can also result in
Members of the same religious, ethnic, or the development of a siege mentality (Bar-Tal &
national groups typically access the same sources Antebi, 1992), a belief that much of the rest of the
for their historical knowledge. Consequently, group world opposes the ingroup. People endorsing this
members often possess overlapping memories of mentality perceive the ingroup to be vulnerable in
history that can differ in content, appraisals, and an extremely dangerous world. In this sense, histori-
interpretations from the shared memories of other cal victimization serves as a lesson for the ingroup,
groups. When different groups possess conflicting so that past tragedies can be avoided in the future.
narratives of the same events, they often regard their Stories of historical grievances can bring about
own side’s historical narrative as accurate and accuse moral obligations for the present and future. For
their opposition of distorting or ignoring history. instance, the defeat of Prince Lazar in the battle
This intergroup disparity in accounts of the past is of Kosovo in 1389 is the most important event
particularly evident in recollections of conflict, the in Serbian historical memory. To many Serbs, the
focus of the current chapter. defeat at the battle of Kosovo affirms that their
We identify the main characteristics of histori- group has been historically victimized. The lessons
cal memories of intergroup conflict and examine they derive from the battle of Kosovo include the
the influence of historical memories in perpetuating need for solidarity, group cohesion, and protective
intergroup conflicts. We document how individual- actions. Another example is the memory of the
and group-level factors (e.g., needs, goals, motives) battle of Masada, which portrays the last Jewish
shape historical memories, and how historical mem- resistance toward Romans after the fall of Jerusalem
ories exacerbate conflict. We focus less on accuracy, in a.d. 73. As defeat became inevitable, the Jewish
which can be difficult to determine, and more on fighters in the Masada fortress committed mass sui-
the content and functions of people’s stories of the cide to avoid capture. This event signifies the impor-
past. In the final section of the chapter, we consider tance of group members’ commitment and sacrifice
how memories might be used to promote reconcili- for the group (Schwartz, Zerubavel, & Barnett,
ation rather than conflict. 1986).
Decades and even centuries after their group’s
Historical Memory and Intergroup Conflict victimization, memories of the incident can evoke
The study of historical memory contributes to strong emotional reactions among group mem-
an understanding of intergroup conflict, in part, bers. Often, the historical trauma is relived during

124 remembering intergroup conflict

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 124 3/8/2012 5:45:54 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

commemorations of the ingroup’s past. The reen- Burundi, each group blamed the outgroup for pro-
actment of past victimization through rituals elic- voking the violence, and depicted their ingroup’s
its emotions that are shared by ingroup members violent actions as self-defense.
(Liu & László, 2007). Such emotions include grief, Emphasizing prior victimization serves to legiti-
shame, rage, fear, anger, and sometimes humiliation mize current conflicts and justify negative actions
(Frijda, 1997; Rice & Benson, 2005). During ongo- against outgroup members (Vollhardt, this volume),
ing intractable conflicts, these emotions contribute even when the targeted outgroup did not partici-
to perpetuating cycles of violence (Bar-Tal, 2000). pate in the original victimization. In a recent study
For instance, feelings of anger and humiliation of this phenomenon, psychologists asked Israeli par-
(Lickel, Miller, Stenstrom, Denson, & Schmader, ticipants about Israel’s war (2008–2009) in Gaza.
2006; Pick, 2001) are associated with a desire for In one condition participants were reminded of the
retribution (see also Lickel, this volume). Holocaust prior to answering questions about the
conflict in Gaza. Participants who were reminded of
Mobilizing Collective Action and the Holocaust were more likely to justify Palestinian
Justifying Ingroup Aggression civilian casualties. Reminders of the Holocaust
Historical memories play a role in mobilizing increased justification for Palestinian civilian casu-
collective action. Reviving and revoking historical alties specifically among Israeli participants who
memories has been crucial for waging wars, build- endorsed right-wing ideologies (Hirschberger &
ing nations, as well as inspiring resistance and civil Pyszczynski, in press).
rights movements (Apfelbaum, 2000). For exam- Similarly, Wohl and Branscombe (2008) exam-
ple, Edward Said (2000) attributed the failure of ined how remembering the historical victimization
Palestinian national project to the failure to create of the ingroup influenced emotional reactions to
a powerful historical narrative that would bring aggression against a current adversary. American
Palestinians together around a common goal. By Jews who were reminded of the Holocaust reported
contrast, he attributed the establishment of the less collective guilt for the plight of Palestinians than
state of Israel to the creation of a strong histori- did American Jews who were not provided with this
cal narrative that matched the goals of the Zionist reminder. Similarly, Americans reported less collec-
movement (Said, 2000). tive guilt for their group’s harm-doing in Iraq after
Historical memories are often used by elites to they were reminded of either the Japanese attack on
justify intergroup conflict (Berlin, 1979; Mack, Pearl Harbor or the 2001 terrorist attacks on the
1983; Ramanathapillai, 2006). Historical memories World Trade Center.
of violence can contribute to current conflicts by Apparently, historical memory does not simply
evoking threat and fear. By affirming that the adver- increase enthusiasm for retribution against a prior
sary is dangerous, memories of conflict elicit mis- enemy. Instead, reminders of ingroup victimization
trust and delegitimize the adversary (Bar-Tal, 2007; seemed to legitimize an ingroup’s actions against
Bar-Tal & Hammack, this volume). Such historical any enemy and decrease sympathy for outgroup
memories might drive groups to engage in preemp- suffering.
tive wars (strike first) to protect themselves from the
threatening outgroup (Staub, 1998). Group mem- Disparate Memories of Conflict
bers are likely to perceive their group’s aggression as Psychological research at the individual level
a response to provocation (Bandura, 1990; Staub, has revealed that victims emphasize the negative
1989) and regard aggression in response to provoca- and lasting consequences of the harm and perpe-
tion as justifiable (Dodge & Coie, 1987; Wohl & trators’ responsibility for the act. Victims often
Reeder, 2004). neglect to mention, however, potential mitigating
In a study of historical memories of Armenian circumstances that might have contributed to the
massacres at the beginning of the 20th century, transgression. By contrast, perpetrators minimize
Bilali, Tropp, and Dasgupta (in press, Study 1) found the consequences of their actions, focus on mitigat-
that Turkish participants viewed Turkish aggression ing circumstances, and depict victims as overreact-
as a response to provocation by Armenians. Similar ing (e.g., Baumeister, Stillwell, & Wotman, 1990;
findings were observed in a study examining histori- Baumeister & Catanese, 2001).
cal memories of conflict between Tutsis and Hutus Similar differences in perpetrator and victim per-
in Burundi (Bilali et al., in press, Study 2). When spectives were evident in the hearings of the Truth
asked about the history of the violent conflict in and Reconciliation Commission in South Africa

bil ali, ross 125

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 125 3/8/2012 5:45:54 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

(see Hamber, this volume). Perpetrators tended to social disadvantages of the victim group can be linked
present their transgression as unrelated to previous to the original injustice (Brooks, 1999; Matsuda,
or subsequent assaults, whereas victims tended to 1987; Starzyk & Ross, 2008). Contemporary mem-
depict the incident as part of a continued series of bers of previously victimized groups are more likely
assaults against them. Also, while admitting that to perceive a link between past mistreatment and
their acts were morally wrong, perpetrators justified current suffering than are members of the nonvic-
their violence; in contrast, victims described the timized majority (Ross, Banfield, & Blatz, 2011).
violence as incomprehensible (Kraft, 2009). In turn, members of a nonvictimized majority
Parallel differences in perpetrator and victim are more likely to perceive members of a victim-
perspectives occur for group-based harms. Groups ized group as personally responsible for their fate,
and individuals tend to focus on the harm inflicted thus legitimizing the current system (Kay, Jost, &
on the ingroup while minimizing the harm they Young, 2005) and avoiding experiencing collec-
inflict on others (Pratto & Glasford, 2008). The tive guilt (Branscombe & Doosje, 2004; Wohl,
media focuses primarily on the suffering of the Branscombe, & Klar, 2006).
ingroup while downplaying the outgroup’s misery
(e.g., Fishman & Marvin, 2003). Emphasizing the Cleansing and Glorifying History to
ingroup’s own suffering and victimization rather Maintain Positive Group Identity
than its hostile actions fosters a favorable and moral Why do conflicting groups develop very disparate
image of the ingroup (Nadler & Saguy, 2004; Noor, memories of conflicts? One answer is that historical
Brown, & Prentice, 2008). memory forms an important part of a group’s iden-
As this focus on ingroup suffering occurs on both tity. Group identity is enhanced if, on looking back,
sides of a conflict, each group perceives itself as vic- people are proud of their group’s actions (Volpato &
timized. Interestingly, an emphasis on past victim- Licata, 2010). But all groups have engaged in at least
ization is observed not only among groups that have some actions that seem shameful from the perspec-
been the main victims of violence (e.g., Hutus in tive of current societal standards and group ideals.
Burundi), but also among groups that are judged to Perpetrator groups can strive to maintain favorable
be the main perpetrators of violence by third-party social identities by constructing pasts consistent
observers (e.g., Turks in the context of the massacres with their preferences. To support positive views of
against Armenians). For instance, in Bilali et al.’s their groups they may erase or alter accounts of past
(in press) study of Turks’ historical memories of harms. They may also dehumanize the victims. We
Armenian massacres, Turkish participants acknowl- discuss each of these responses in turn.
edged the suffering of Armenians. However, they
emphasized accounts of Turks’ victimization during Silencing
the Balkan Wars (which preceded the massacres of To preserve a favorable image of their group in
Armenians), and also produced narratives of Turks’ the face of shameful episodes, groups may remem-
suffering at the hand of Armenians. The amplifica- ber the past in ways that eliminate humiliating
tion and focus on the ingroup’s suffering serve to events altogether from their historical narratives.
legitimize the harm done to Armenians. Fitzgerald (1979) analyzed American school his-
Such preoccupation with ingroup victimiza- tory texts. Until the civil rights era in the 1960s,
tion often leads to a competition regarding which texts presented slaves as appearing “magically” at
group has suffered more harm (Nadler & Saguy, some unspecified time and disappearing after the
2004; Noor, Brown, & Prentice, 2008). This com- Civil War (p. 83). Textbook writers failed to men-
petition may have implications for reconciliation tion that slaves were forcibly abducted from their
(see Nadler, this volume). In a series of studies of families and homes.
conflicts in Chile and Northern Ireland, Noor et al. Silencing of shameful historical episodes is also
(2008) found that the more individuals competed evident in Canadian textbooks. Backhouse (1999,
over which group has suffered more, the less likely p. 278) commented on the “stupefying innocence”
they were to forgive the outgroup for the past harms of Canadians about the history of racism in their
committed, and the less willing they were to recon- country. She attributed the ignorance of Canadians
cile with the outgroup. to the lack of discussion of racism in educational
Members of victim and perpetrator groups have curricula and the media. This silencing seems
also different views regarding the presence or absence designed to help Canadians maintain an image of
of “privity”—whether the ongoing economic or themselves as fair and tolerant.

126 remembering intergroup conflict

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 126 3/8/2012 5:45:54 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

Such cleansing and glorification of history is was unthinkable. Slaves were supposedly satisfied
perhaps particularly common with regard to violent with their conditions, unable to organize them-
conflicts and military events. In war, individuals selves, incapable of imagining or desiring free-
engage in behaviors that would be unthinkable in dom, and perhaps most important, incapable of
peace. At times, groups deal with wartime events defeating sophisticated, well-armed French forces.
that violate their norms and values by refusing to Despite all of these supposed disadvantages, the
think or talk about them. In recent years, Japanese Haitian slaves defeated the French by using supe-
government officials announced that they would rior tactics. Confronting the impossible, contem-
eliminate references to how their army forced civil- porary European observers derived explanations
ians to commit mass suicide during the Battle of for the rebellion that better fit their expectations
Okinawa. Japanese officials also proclaimed that (e.g., French forces suffered from yellow fever). In
they would stop discussing their army’s involvement the aftermath of the rebellion, silencing occurred
in the creation of the “comfort women” system (Trouillot, 1995). Details of the rebellion were
(Onishi, 2007). omitted from Western histories.

Altering the Past Dehumanization


Sometimes groups modify rather than suppress People sometimes defend the ingroup and the
unthinkable events. US military officials recently status quo by dehumanizing victims or subordinate
provided a fictionalized account of the death in groups. For instance, people can legitimize earlier
Afghanistan of Pat Tillman, an ex-NFL football aggressive acts against an outgroup by viewing its
player who was depicted as a hero in the media for members as subhuman (see Bar-Tal & Hammack,
quitting a lucrative career to fight for his country. this volume, for an extended discussion).
According to media reports after his death, Tillman Castano and Giner-Sorolla (2006) presented
died heroically under enemy fire (Herbert, 2007; American participants with a passage that empha-
Krakauer, 2009). In reality, a fellow American sol- sized that white settlers had killed large numbers of
dier accidentally killed Tillman. An official inquiry Native Americans either deliberately through mili-
later revealed that high-ranking military officials tary campaigns or inadvertently through the spread
were responsible for attempting to cover up details of contagious diseases. Participants evidenced
of Tillman’s death and distributing the false story to stronger dehumanization of Native Americans
the media (Krakauer, 2009). when the killing was deliberate. Apparently, par-
The cleansing of history occurs not only at the ticipants relied on dehumanization to protect the
group level by elites, but also at the individual image of their ingroup when its members deliber-
level. Social identity theory is particularly relevant ately caused great harm. Because standards of fair-
to understanding responses at the individual level ness and morality don’t apply to the same degree to
(Tajfel & Turner, 1986). According to social iden- subhuman species (Castano & Giner-Sorolla, 2006;
tity theory, people derive their self-concepts, in part, Clayton & Opotow, 2003; Kelman, 1973; Leyens,
from their membership in social groups. Their self- et al., 2000; Opotow, 1990; Hafer & Olson, 2003),
evaluations reflect their assessments of the groups aggression toward them is not perceived as a serious
to which they belong. Because people are gener- moral violation.
ally motivated to regard themselves favorably, they In addition to legitimizing past violence, dehu-
are also motivated to view their groups favorably manization of the outgroup renders violence against
(see Roccas & Elster, this volume). Social identity it more likely today. When outgroup members are
theory does not directly address historical memo- described as vermin or insects, destroying the out-
ries, but the implications are clear. People should group becomes a moral imperative. For instance, in
prefer favorable historical portrayals of their groups Rwanda, the hate radio during the genocide called
because they can then think well of themselves. for the killing of the cockroaches—the dehuman-
Group members are thus motivated to reinterpret izing label applied to Tutsis (Melvern, 2006). Each
or silence events that could reflect poorly on their war seems to generate a list of dehumanizing labels
group and by extension, themselves (Baumeister & that serve to legitimize the killing of enemy soldiers
Hastings, 1997). and civilians.
Trouillot (1995) reported relevant findings with When outgroups are dehumanized, perpetrator
respect to the Haitian Slave rebellion in 1791. For groups may acknowledge rather than deny harms
contemporary European observers, a slave rebellion and glorify rather than disparage the aggressors.

bil ali, ross 127

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 127 3/8/2012 5:45:54 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

In the aftermath of the war in Serbia, Ratko Mladic ways that may solidify the boundaries dividing the
and Radovan Karadzic who ordered the massacres ingroup and the outgroup.
of thousands of people in Bosnia were celebrated as Motivations relevant to the cleansing of and
national heroes and had children named after them glorification of history also involve processes of
(Ramet, 2007). social dominance. Social dominance theory (SDT,
Sidanius & Pratto, 1999) suggests that humans have
Social Psychological Motives Underlying a general tendency to form and maintain group-
Cleansing of and Glorification of History based hierarchies. Individuals and institutions
Social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1986) adopt social myths and ideologies that legitimize
proposes that people’s motivation to enhance their and enhance these hierarchies (hierarchy-enhancing
group’s history should be related to the strength ideologies) (Sidanius, Laar, Pratto, & Levin, 2004).
of their association with a group (Baumeister & SDT implies that people would prefer interpreta-
Hastings, 1997). Consistent with this hypothesis, tions of the past that legitimize structural hierar-
Sahdra and Ross (2007) found that, relative to low chies. Not surprisingly, the tendency to maintain
identifiers, individuals who identified highly with hierarchy-enhancing ideologies and beliefs may be
their ethnic group recalled fewer incidents of inter- stronger among members of dominant groups who
group violence and hatred committed by ingroup benefit from their higher social status, than among
members. In an investigation of Americans’ histori- members of subordinate groups who are disadvan-
cal memories of Pearl Harbor attacks, Bilali (2010) taged by their status (see also Cohrs, this volume).
reported that participants for whom American In the context of New Zealand, Sibley, Wilson,
identity was central to their self-concept reported and Robertson (2007) observed that members of
more anger toward Japan, more sympathy toward the majority group (Pakeha) who were high in the
American victims, and perceived Pearl Harbor desire for group-based dominance were more likely
attacks as more important to US history. Higher to negate Pakeha’s responsibility for historical injus-
identity centrality also predicted attributions of tices inflicted on Maoris (the indigenous people of
more responsibility to Japan and less responsibility New Zealand). In turn, the refutation of historical
to the United States for the Pearl Harbor attacks. responsibility served as a legitimizing myth justifying
In considering group identity, it is possible to the social inequality between Pakeha and Maori.
distinguish glorifying a group from feeling attached In contrast, subordinate groups sometimes tell
to it. The glorification dimension of ingroup iden- historical stories that seem designed to undermine the
tification, not group attachment per se, predicts legitimacy of current hierarchies. For instance, one of
denial of ingroup responsibility for wrongdoing the first tasks of the women’s movement in the 1970s
and legitimization of past ingroup harms (Roccas, was to highlight the history of women’s important
Klar, Liviatan, 2006; Leidner, Castano, Zaiser, & contributions to society (Apfelbaum, 2000). Similarly,
Giner-Sorolla, 2010; see also Roccas & Elster, this Alex Haley’s (1976) novel Roots, which was allegedly
volume). In a study of historical representations of based on the genuine history of his own family, had
American Thanksgiving, Kurtis, Adams, and Yellow a significant impact in expanding the meaning and
Bird (2010) demonstrated a bidirectional link understanding of African Americans’ experience of
between ingroup glorification and historical memo- slavery and led to the reevaluation of slavery as a con-
ries. High glorifiers preferred accounts of American sequence of white domination (Apfelbaum, 2000).
Thanksgiving that omitted violence against indig- These stories challenged the essentialist perspectives
enous peoples; in turn, accounts that failed to men- underlying sexist and racist ideologies that viewed
tion the violence led to increased glorification of the gender and race inequalities as natural. The accuracy
United States. of Haley’s historical scholarship has been questioned
The reciprocal relation between identity and (e.g., Mills & Mills, 1984), but our point is not that
memory is also evident in research on memories stories of the past provided by dominant or subordi-
for severe forms of violence against the ingroup, nate groups are necessarily accurate. Instead, we focus
such as genocide, or slavery or unexpected defeats. on the use of historical stories to bolster or under-
Recollecting such events enhances ingroup solidarity mine the status quo.
and identity (Devine-Wright, 2003; Irwin-Zarecka,
1994). Recollections of past victimization increase Promoting Reconciliation
people’s perceptions of a shared common fate with Historical memory poses one of the most chal-
other group members (Vollhardt, this volume) in lenging obstacles to peaceful resolution of conflicts.

128 remembering intergroup conflict

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 128 3/8/2012 5:45:54 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

Divergent interpretations of history evoke hatred For instance, former US President Bill Clinton
and produce violence (Kriesberg, 2004). Because of refused to apologize for slavery, citing potential
this destructive potential, for a long time, historical hostile responses from white Americans (Brooks,
concerns have been intentionally ignored in conflict 1999). Public opinion polls support Clinton’s con-
resolution approaches. More recently, research and cerns: In a recent poll, 53% of white Americans
practice in the field of reconciliation (e.g., Brooks, opposed a federal apology for slavery (Polling
1999; Lederach, 1997; Minow, 2002) suggest that Report, 2008). Public opinion on government
addressing, rather than ignoring the past, is crucial apologies is malleable, however. After governments
to reconciliation (see also Hamber, this volume). apologize for historical injustices, public support for
We discuss two approaches to confronting the past: the apology tends to rise dramatically (Schumann
Public government apologies for historical injustices & Ross, 2010). This shift in public opinion proba-
and more direct interventions aimed at fostering bly occurs for a number of reasons (e.g., the content
reconciliation for recent or ongoing conflicts. of the apology stresses the innocence of the victims
and the value of even a belated apology), but it is
Government Apologies for Historical also consistent with system justification theory (Jost
Injustices & Banaji, 1994). Members of the nonvictimized
We have detailed numerous ways in which peo- majority can support the current system by justify-
ple hide, deny, and justify past harms committed ing a lack of an apology before one is offered and
by members of their group. However, governments subsequently justifying an apology after it occurs. In
sometimes respond to ingroup harm-doing by any event, the data suggest that governments need
acknowledging government responsibility for earlier not be overly concerned about the possibility of a
injustices. Although relatively rare, such acknowl- majority backlash against apologies for historical
edgment has become more common in recent injustices.
decades, as evidenced by the increasing number of Government apologies tend to follow a certain
public government apologies for wrongs committed time course. Most obviously, they often occur in
decades or even centuries earlier (Blatz, Schumann, bunches. The Canadian apology for the Japanese
& Ross, 2009; Brooks, 1999; Schumann & Ross, internment occurred within weeks of a similar
2010; see also Iyer & Blatz, this volume). For exam- US apology; the Australian apology for the stolen
ple, the United States and Canada apologized for generations followed the Canadian apology for
the internment of American and Canadian Japanese similar actions by slightly more than a year; and
during World War II, Australia apologized for the apologies by seven American states for slavery
forcibly removing Aboriginal children from their occurred within a year of each other after almost a
families between 1915 and 1967, seven American century and a half without any formal state apolo-
state legislatures apologized for their state’s role in gies. It seems likely that the grouping of apologies
African American slavery (Blatz et al., 2009), and for similar injustices is not due to chance. Earlier
most recently Mississippi Governor Haley Barbour apologies for similar injustices probably increase
apologized to civil rights workers who were assaulted political and moral pressures for an apology from
and imprisoned in 1961 (retrieved online from the victimized groups and their allies in other coun-
Commercial Appeal, May 25, 2011). tries or states, provide governments with models for
When do governments acknowledge past harms how to apologize (including what to offer by way
rather than deny or justify them? Schumann and of material compensation, if anything), and perhaps
Ross suggested that governments are more likely to increase a government’s courage by demonstrating
acknowledge and apologize for historical injustices that apologies do not appear to have a serious politi-
when victimized groups or their allies have political cal downside.
clout, the majority of voters do not strongly oppose Analyzing a set of government apologies,
an apology, and some but not too much time has Schumann and Ross (2010) found that the median
passed. The first factor is political, but the remain- interval between the end of the injustice and gov-
ing ones have psychological components and are ernment apologies was 59 years. There are probably
worthy of further comment. a variety of reasons for the delay. Societal attitudes
Politicians sometimes hesitate to acknowledge need to change so that discriminatory actions that
government supported historical harms committed were once viewed by the majority as socially, mor-
against minorities because they fear the opposition ally, and legally justified (e.g., African American
of many members of the nonvictimized majority. slavery, Japanese internment, forced participation in

bil ali, ross 129

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 129 3/8/2012 5:45:54 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

native residential schools) are viewed as abhorrent guilt and shame (Branscombe & Doosje, 2004;
and illegal by more modern standards. The change Branscombe & Miron, 2004; Doosje, Branscombe,
in attitudes toward the discriminatory actions is Spears, & Manstead, 1998; Heider, 1958). These
typically accompanied by a shift in attitudes toward emotions can occur even when historical injustices
the minority group, such that its members are no occurred decades or centuries earlier (Branscombe,
longer viewed as subhuman and beyond the scope Doosje, & McGarty, 2002; Iyer, Leach, & Crosby,
of justice. Second, with the passage of time, govern- 2003). The experience of guilt predicts increased
ment and legal officials who initiated, endorsed, or support for reparations for past harms (e.g., Doosje
supervised the injustice are no longer in positions et al., 1998; Zebel, Doosje, & Spears, 2004). For
of power, and those now in charge can dissociate example, Iyer et al. (2003) demonstrated that guilt
themselves from the injustice and experience less about white Americans’ racial discrimination pre-
psychological need to defend it. As well, with the dicted support for affirmative actions to compensate
passage of sufficient time, most of the original direct African Americans for slavery.
victims of the injustice will have died. Material Conversely, the experience of shame is associ-
compensation to individuals is typically directed ated with actions aimed at defending the ingroup’s
only at direct victims of injustice, not their descen- image rather than ameliorating the damage inflicted
dants (Brooks, 1999; Starzyk, Blatz, & Ross, 2009). on an outgroup (Branscombe, Slugoski, & Kappen,
This was the case for example, for US and Canadian 2004; Lickel, Schmander, & Barquissau, 2004). For
government reparations offered to individuals for example, in a study of Italian colonialism, Mari,
the Japanese internment. If most of the original vic- Andrighetto, Gabbiadini, Durante, and Volpato
tims are dead, there is likely to be less demand for (2010) found that shame for the violence perpetrated
material compensation and any compensation offer against the colonized people predicted reparation
should be relatively inexpensive. Because a delayed strategies that attempted to enhance Italy’s public
apology is less likely to be costly to government and image, whereas guilt predicted reparation strategies
taxpayers, it should face less opposition. The other designed to aid immigrant African workers.
side of the coin is that if the delay is too long the While research on members of perpetrator groups
injustice may seem like ancient history to members has focused mostly on emotional responses to his-
of both the victimized and perpetrator groups and torical injustices, research on victimized groups has
a present-day apology will seem inappropriate and tended to examine the effects of how the injustices
unnecessary. There is no gold standard, however, as are framed. For example, Wohl and Branscombe
to what constitutes “too long.” (2005) assessed the effects of depicting the Holocaust
Proponents of government apologies argue that as a general human tendency toward violence rather
apologies promote intergroup reconciliation as well than German violence against Jews. As expected,
as allow victim groups to heal and put the harms when Germans and Jews were depicted as part of
behind them (e.g., Brooks, 1999; Minow, 2002). an inclusive group (i.e., humans—a superordinate
Despite the growing enthusiasm for public govern- category) as compared to two distinct social groups
ment apologies, there is little research that directly (Germans vs. Jews), Jewish participants were more
assesses their effectiveness. To this point, the lim- willing to forgive Germans for the past atrocities.
ited amount of research is equivocal. For example, In related research, Vollhardt (2009) argued that
Brown, Wohl, and Exline (2008) found that govern- people understand their group’s past victimization
ment apologies for past injustices increased victims’ by relating it to other groups’ experiences. When
forgiveness, whereas Philpot and Hornsey (2008) the past victimization of the ingroup is perceived
reported that apologies had no effect on forgiveness. as unique and distinct, victimized groups show an
The appropriate question is not whether govern- increased tendency to seek revenge. However when
ment apologies are effective, but rather what kinds the ingroup’s experience is construed as similar to
of apologies are beneficial and for what historical the experiences of other groups, participants from
injustices and outcomes (e.g., forgiveness, trust, lik- various victimized groups demonstrated a desire to
ing, acceptance, etc.). Research by psychologists on help other victim groups around the world, and were
emotional responses to injustices and on the effects more willing to engage in collective action on behalf
of different ways of framing of injustices provides of other victims (see also Vollhardt, this volume).
some potential answers. The effectiveness of government apologies in pro-
Individuals who regard members of their group moting intergroup reconciliation may thus depend,
as responsible for past transgressions may experience in part, on the type of emotion (shame versus guilt)

130 remembering intergroup conflict

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 130 3/8/2012 5:45:54 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

that apologies generate or assuage among mem- collection through private interviews with witnesses
bers of the perpetrator group, and how they lead and victims, data analysis, documentation, and dis-
members of the victimized group to construe the semination of the findings through public hearings
injustice. An analysis of the content of previous and reports help to achieve this goal (Freeman &
government apologies would suggest that govern- Hayner, 2003). This process is important as it docu-
ments attempt to avoid eliciting either shame or ments the causes and patterns of violence, includ-
guilt in current members of the perpetrator group ing the role of individuals and institutions in the
(Schumann & Ross, 2010). Instead governments violence (Dimitrijevic, 2006).
often strive to engender pride among members of As with any other regime, the new system with
both perpetrator and victim groups. Moreover, the its focus on reconciliation needs historical accounts
apologies seem to focus on the horror and unique- that legitimize the new norms and relations between
ness of the injustice, rather than its universality. All groups. Truth commissions endorse a clear moral
of the apologies, however, emphasize the innocence attitude toward the past (Dimitrijevic, 2006). For
of the victims and some point to the important con- the new historical narrative to be successful, soci-
tributions of both members of the victimized group ety members should be convinced of its veracity
and the perpetrator group to the greater society. In (Zerubavel, 1994). According to Gibson (2004),
doing so, government apologies may promote feel- South Africa’s Truth Reconciliation Commission
ings of belonging to a common group among mem- (TRC) was successful in achieving this goal.
bers of both the victimized and perpetrator groups Gibson’s (2004) research revealed that the truth
(e.g., Wohl & Branscombe, 2005). Although this about apartheid promulgated by the TRC included
approach entails both strengths and limitations (see three central elements: (1) apartheid was a crime
Dovidio, Saguy, West, & Gaertner, this volume), against humanity, (2) both sides committed gross
such feelings should enhance reconciliation and offenses and human rights abuses, and (3) apartheid
lead group members to focus less on their tumultu- was criminal because of the actions of both indi-
ous history and more on their shared future. viduals and institutions (pp. 72–74). By revealing
the misdeeds that both sides committed, the TRC
Reconciling Groups Engaged in Recent had people question the morality of their side and
and Ongoing Conflicts: Transitional challenged the “good versus evil” view of the con-
Justice Measures flict (Gibson, 2004). However, the process of seek-
Reconciliation marks a transition from a violent ing the truth about the past, particularly in contexts
to a peaceful society by promoting peaceful coex- of extreme forms of violence, might also have unin-
istence between previous enemies. Because each tended consequences (see Hamber, this volume).
group blames the other for its own violent acts Brouneus (2010) in a study of the effects of truth-
(Staub & Bar-Tal, 2003), a change in each group’s telling in the Gacaca trials (the traditional truth and
beliefs about past conflict and about each other is reconciliation commissions in Rwanda) found that
important for long-term reconciliation (Bar-Tal, witnessing the Gacaca trials led to negative psycho-
2000; Nadler, this volume). Seeking a common logical outcomes such as increased depression and
narrative about the past has become a core ele- PTSD symptoms.
ment of reconciliation as it is viewed as necessary Additionally, projects on teaching history
for healing the wounds of the past (Gibson, 2004). have been developed in various regions around
To achieve these goals, many countries undergoing the world. For instance, the Joint History Project
regime change have set up transitional justice mea- (Koulouri, 2005) critically examines historical
sures (e.g., prosecution of perpetrators, compensa- texts in Southeast Europe, trains teachers to over-
tion of victims, or truth-seeking mechanisms) to come nationalistic depictions of history, and aims
address past wrongs (Hamber, this volume; Hayner, to develop a joint history for the whole region.
2000). Numerous historical and truth commissions In Israel, social psychologists have worked with
have been established around the world to investi- Palestinian and Israeli teachers to develop a joint his-
gate and document past transgressions and abuses tory textbook, which depicts both groups’ historical
(Freeman & Hayner, 2003). An important goal of narratives (Adwan & Bar-On, 2004). These proj-
these commissions is to provide a historical account ects endorse two different approaches in address-
that reduces intergroup biases at different stages of ing history. The first attempts to produce a shared
memory construction: fact creation, fact assembly, memory, whereas the latter aims to have each group
and fact retrieval (see Trouillot, 1995). Rigorous data acknowledge the other’s narrative. Should groups in

bil ali, ross 131

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 131 3/8/2012 5:45:54 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

conflict work toward producing a shared memory, the negative impact of historical memory on indi-
or can they coexist peacefully while acknowledging viduals’ well-being and on intergroup relations. The
divergent memories? Such questions are difficult to large number of interventions around the world that
answer. Psychology can contribute to these debates address the past to promote peaceful coexistence
by assessing the impact of each approach on people’s between groups evidence similar goals. However,
attitudes and behaviors. the theoretical and empirical support behind such
Psychologists can also help design programs that efforts is lacking, and proof of their success is largely
effectively address historical memories of conflict in anecdotal (Barsalow, 2008; Mendeloff, 2004).
ways that lead to reconciliation, rather than perpet- Research is needed to examine the impact of such
uation of cycles of violence. For instance, Shnabel interventions on attitudes and behaviors. Because
and Nadler (2008) suggest that harm-doers and these interventions attempt to produce psychologi-
victims have different psychological concerns and cal and behavioral change, psychological theory and
needs: Victims need to restore their lost power and methods are particularly relevant to addressing such
status, whereas perpetrators need to restore their issues.
positive identity (see also Nadler, this volume). The potential contributions of psychology are
Similarly, Pratto and Glasford (2008) identified both methodological and theoretical. The major
psychological needs relevant to ingroup harm- strengths of the psychological approach include
doing and ingroup suffering. Addressing historical experimental methods, a careful attention to the
memories in ways that meet the needs of the victim measurement and statistical analysis of relevant
and harm-doing groups might be an important step variables, as well as theories that can prove useful
toward reconciliation. in explaining the content, functions, and effects of
historical memories.
Future Directions To study historical memory and reconciliation,
In this chapter, we outlined different ways in however, psychologists will need to go beyond tra-
which historical memory is biased by group goals and ditional methods (e.g., laboratory experiments,
motivation, including how past episodes are silenced, student samples) to incorporate less common meth-
glorified, or legitimized. We also demonstrated that odologies such as field experiments, qualitative
historical memories play multiple functions in inter- research, longitudinal research, and more represen-
group conflict: they serve to enhance group identity tative samples in real-world conflicts (see Paluck,
and group cohesion; sometimes, historical memories this volume). The study of historical memories calls
are activated purposively and used as legitimizing for a historically and socially contextualized social
myths to maintain or undermine a group’s position psychology (Pettigrew, 1991).
in society; other times, historical memories give
meaning to current events, provide expectations for Conclusions
the future, and motivate groups’ actions. Philosopher George Santayana is probably best
Great strides have been made in addressing the known today for his aphorism “Those who cannot
role of historical memory in conflict, yet the condi- remember the past are condemned to repeat it.” It
tions and the processes that modulate these differ- should now be clear that Santayana’s faith in the
ent outcomes and different functions of historical lessons to be derived from the past is often unwar-
memories are not well understood. An overarch- ranted. Santayana assumes a single, consensual
ing theoretical framework on the psychological past. When conflicting groups disagree about his-
underpinnings of historical memory is lacking. tory, they will draw divergent lessons from it. As a
Particularly, the interaction between the societal consequence, a focus on history can exacerbate and
and individual-level processes in the construc- perpetuate ongoing, intergroup conflicts. In such
tion of historical memories is not well understood. instances, one could argue that those who remem-
Developing a framework that links processes and ber the past are doomed to repeat it. At the same
concepts at different levels of analyses (individual, time, ignoring or silencing the past has proved to
group and societal), which could be used to make be ineffective or even destructive. Also, a focus on
predictions about the relationships among these the past can sometimes promote reconciliation and
variables is important. help groups avoid repetition of past mistakes as the
Investigations of the functions of historical Santayana quote implies.
memories can serve as the basis for developing and The question is not whether groups should
testing hypotheses about mechanisms that reduce remember their pasts. Historical memories are

132 remembering intergroup conflict

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 132 3/8/2012 5:45:54 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

inevitable, because they are vital to the psychol- Branscombe, N. R., Doosje, B., & McGarty, C. (2002).
ogy and understanding of group members. More Antecedents and consequences of collective guilt. In D. M.
Mackie & E. R. Smith (Eds.), From prejudice to intergroup
important questions concern how should groups emotions: Differentiated reactions to social groups (pp. 49–66).
remember and the types of remembering that could Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
potentially benefit intergroup relations. Branscombe, N. R., & Miron, A. M. (2004). Interpreting the
ingroup’s negative actions toward an outgroup: Emotional
References reactions to appraised harm. In L. Z. Tiedens & C. W. Leach
Adwan. S., & Bar-On, D. (2004). Shared History Project: A (Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 314–335). New York:
PRIME example of peace building under fire. International Cambridge University Press.
Journal of Politics, Culture, and Society, 17, 513–522. Branscombe, N. R., Slugoski, B., & Kappen, D. M. (2004). The
Apfelbaum, E. R. (2000). And now what, after such tribulations? measurement of collective guilt: What it is and what it is not.
Memory and dislocation in the era of uprooting. American In N. R. Branscombe & B. Doosje (Eds.), Collective guilt:
Psychologist, 55, 1008–1013. International perspectives (pp. 16–34). New York: Cambridge
Backhouse, C. (1999). Colour-coded: A legal history of racism in University Press.
Canada, 1900–1950. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Brooks, R. L. (Ed.). (1999). When sorry isn’t enough: The con-
Bandura, A. (1990). Mechanisms of moral disengagement. troversy over apologies and reparations for human injustice.
In R. Walter (Ed.), Origins of terrorism: Psychologies, ide- New York: New York University Press.
ologies, theologies, states of mind (pp. 161–191). New York: Brouneus, K. (2010). The trauma of truth telling: Effects of
Cambridge University Press. witnessing in the Rwandan Gacaca Courts on psychological
Barsalow, J. (2008). Managing memory: Looking to transitional health. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 54, 408–437.
justice to address trauma. In B. Hart (Ed.), Peacebuilding in Brown, R. P., Wohl, M. J. A., & Exline, J. J. (2008). Taking
traumatized societies (pp. 27–48). New York: University Press up offenses: Second-hand forgiveness and identification with
of America. targets of transgressions. Personality and Social Psychology
Bar-Tal, D. (2000). Shared beliefs in a society: Social psychological Bulletin, 34, 1406–1419.
analysis. Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage. Castano, E., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2006). Not quite human:
Bar-Tal, D. (2007). The sociopsychological foundations of Infrahumanization in response to collective responsibility for
intractable conflicts. American Behavioral Scientist, 50, intergroup killing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
1430–1453. 90(5), 805–818.
Bar-Tal, D., & Antebi, D. (1992). Beliefs about negative inten- Clayton, S., & Opotow, S. (2003). Justice and identity: Changing
tions of the world: A study of the Israeli siege mentality. perspectives on what is fair. Personality and Social Psychology
Political Psychology, 13, 633–645. Review, 7, 298–310.
Baumeister, R. F., & Hastings, S. (1997). Distortions of collec- Devine-Wright, P. (2003). A theoretical overview of memory and
tive memory: How groups flatter and deceive themselves. conflict. In E. Cairns & M. D. (Eds.), The role of memory in
In J. W. Pennebaker, D. Paez, & B. Rime (Eds.), Collective ethnic conflict (pp. 9–33). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
memory of political events: Social psychological perspectives (pp. Dimitrijevic, N. (2006). Justice beyond blame: Moral justifica-
277–293). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. tion of (the idea of ) a truth commission. Journal of Conflict
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A., & Wotman, S. (1990). Victim Resolution, 50, 368–382.
and perpetrator accounts of interpersonal conflict: Autobio- Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social information process-
graphical narratives about anger. Personality Processes and ing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s
Individual Differences, 59, 994–1005. peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,
Baumeister, R. F., & Catanese, K. R. (2001). Victims and 1146–1158.
perpetrators provide discrepant accounts: Motivated cog- Doosje, B., Branscombe, N. R., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R.
nitive distortions about interpersonal transgressions. In (1998). Guilty by association: When one’s group has a nega-
J. Forgas, K. Williams, & L. Wheeler (Eds.), The social mind tive history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25,
(pp. 274–293). New York: Cambridge University Press. 872–886.
Berlin, I. (1979). Nationalism: Past neglect and present power. Fishman, J., & Marvin, C. (2003). Portrayals of violence and
Partisan Review, 46, 344–361. group difference in newspaper photographs: Nationalism
Bilali, R. (2010). Differential effect of ingroup glorification and and media. Journal of Communication, 53, 32–44.
identity centrality on historical memories. Unpublished Fitzgerald, F. (1979). America revised: History schoolbooks in the
manuscript. twentieth century. New York: Little, Brown, and Company.
Bilali, R., Tropp, L. R., & Dasgupta, N. (in press). Attributions Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2004). Culture and language in the
of responsibility and perceived harm in the aftermath of mass emergence of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science,
violence. Peace & Conflict. Journal of Peace Psychology. 15, 573–577.
Billig, M. (1995). Banal nationalism. London: Sage. Freeman, M., & Hayner, P. B. (2003). Truth-telling. In
Blatz, C. W. & Ross, M. (2009). Historical memories. In D. Bloomfield, T. Barnes, & T. Huyse (Eds.), Reconciliation
P. Boyer and J. Wertsch (Eds.), Memory in mind and culture after violent conflict: A handbook (pp. 122–139). Stockholm,
(pp. 223–237). New York: Cambridge University Press. Sweden: International Idea for Democracy and Electoral
Blatz, C. W., Schumann, K., & Ross, M. (2009). Government Assistance.
apologies for historical injustices. Political Psychology, 30, Frijda, N. H. (1997). Commemorating. In J. W. Pennebaker,
219–241. D. Paez, & B. Rimé (Eds.), Collective memory of politi-
Branscombe, N. R., Doosje, B. (2004). Collective guilt: Inter- cal events: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 103–130).
national perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

bil ali, ross 133

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 133 3/8/2012 5:45:55 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

Gibson, J. L. (2004). Does truth lead to reconciliation? Testing Leidner, B., Castono, E., Zaiser, E., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2010).
the causal assumptions of the South African truth and rec- Ingroup glorification, moral disengagement, and justice
onciliation process. American Journal of Political Science, 48, in the context of collective violence. Personality and Social
201–217. Psychology Bulletin, 36, 115–1139.
Hafer, C. L. & Olson, J. M. (2003). An analysis of empiri- Leyens, J. Ph., Paladino, M. P., Rodriguez, R. T., Vaes, J.,
cal research on the scope of justice. Personality and Social Demoulin, S., Rodriguez, A. P., & Gaunt, R. (2000). The
Psychology Review, 7, 311–323. emotional side of prejudice: The attribution of secondary
Halbwachs, M. (1950/1980). The collective memory. New York: emotions to ingroups and outgroups. Personality and Social
Harper Colophon. Psychology Review, 4, 186–197.
Haley, A. (1976). Roots: The saga of an American family. Garden Lickel, B., Miller, N., Stenstrom, D. M., Denson, T., &
City, NY: Doubleday Books. Schmader, T. (2006). Vicarious retribution: The role of col-
Hayner, P. (2000). Past truths, present dangers. The role of offi- lective blame in intergroup aggression. Personality and Social
cial truth seeking in conflict resolution and prevention. In Psychology Review, 10, 372–390.
P. C. Stern & D. Druckman (Eds.), International conflict reso- Lickel, B., Schmader, T., & Barquissau, M. (2004). The evo-
lution after the Cold War (pp. 338–387). Washington, DC: cation of moral emotions in intergroup contexts: The dis-
National Academies Press. tinction between collective guilt and collective shame. In
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. N. R. Branscombe & B. Doosje, (Eds.) Collective guilt:
New York: Wiley. International perspectives (pp. 35–55). New York: Cambridge
Herbert, B. (2007, April 30). Working the truth beat (Op-ed). University Press.
The New York Times, p. A21. Liu, J. H., & Hilton, D. J. (2005). How the past weighs on the
Herbert, T. L., & Dunkel-Schetter, C. (1992). Negative social present: Social representations of history and their role in iden-
reactions to victims: An overview of responses and their deter- tity politics. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 537–556.
minants. In L. Montada, S. H. Filipp, & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Liu, J. H, & László, J. (2007). A narrative theory of history
Life crises and experiences of loss in adulthood (pp. 497–518). and identity: Social identity, social representations, society
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. and the individual. In G. Moloney & Iain Walker (Eds.),
Hirschberger, G., & Pyszczynski, T. (2011). Killing with a clean Social representations and identity. Content, process, and power
conscious: Existential angst and the paradox of morality. In (pp. 85–107). New York: Palgrave-Macmillan.
M. Mikulincer & P. Shaver (Eds.), The social psychology of Mack, J. (1983). Nationalism and the self. Psychohistory Review,
morality: Exploring the causes of good and evil (pp. 331–348). 11, 47–69.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Malkki, L. H. (1995). Purity and exile: Violence, memory and
Irwin-Zarecka, I. (1994). Frames of remembrance: The dynamics of national cosmology among Hutu refugees in Tanzania. Chicago,
collective memory. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books. IL: University of Chicago Press.
Iyer, A., Leach, C. W., & Crosby, F. J. (2003). White guilt and Mari, S., Andrighetto, L., Gabbiadini, A., Durante, F., &
racial compensation: The benefits and limits of self-focus. Volpato, C. (2010). The shadow of the Italian colonial experi-
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 117–129. ence: The impact of collective emotions on intentions to help
Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyp- the victims’ descendants. International Journal of Conflict and
ing in system-justification and the production of false Violence, 4, 58–74.
consciousness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 1–27. Matsuda, M. J. (1987). Looking to the bottom: Critical legal
Kay, A. C., Jost, J. T., & Young, S. (2005). Victim derogation studies and reparations. Harvard Law Liberties–Civil Right
and victim enhancement alternate routes to system justifica- Law Review, 22, 362–397.
tion. Psychological Science, 16, 240–246. McKinley, J. C. (2010, March 12). Texas conservatives win cur-
Kelman, H. (1973). Violence without moral constraint: riculum change. New York Times. Retrieved from http://
Reflections of the dehumanization of victims and victimizers. www.nytimes.com.
Journal of Social Issues, 29, 25–61. Mead, G. H. (1929). The nature of the past. In J. J. Coss (Ed.),
Koulouri, C. (2005). Teaching modern Southeast European history: Essays in honor of John Dewey (pp. 235–242). New York: Holt.
Alternative educational materials. Thessaloniki, Greece: Center Mendeloff, D. (2004). Truth seeking, truth telling, and post-
for Democracy and Reconciliation for Southeastern Europe. conflict peacebuilding: Curb the enthusiasm. International
Kraft, R. N. (2009). The magnitude gap: Revealing differences Studies Review, 6, 355–380.
in recall between victims and perpetrators. In M. R. Kelley Melvern, L. (2006). Conspiracy to murder: The Rwandan genocide.
(Ed.), Applied memory (pp. 147–167). Hauppauge, NY: London: Verso.
Nova Science. Mills, B. G., & Mills, E. S. (1984). The genealogist’s assessment
Krakauer, J. (2009). Where men win glory: The odyssey of Pat of Alex Haley’s Roots. National Genealogical Society Quarterly,
Tillman. New York: Doubleday. 84, 35–49.
Kriesberg, L. (2004). Comparing reconciliation actions within Minow, M. (2002). Breaking the cycles of hatred. Princeton, NJ:
and between countries. In Y. Bar-Siman-Tov (Ed.), From Princeton University Press.
conflict resolution to reconciliation (pp. 81–110). New York: Nadler, A., & Saguy, T. (2004). Reconciliation between nations:
Oxford University Press. Overcoming emotional deterrents to ending conflicts
Kurtis, T., Adams, G., & Yellow Bird, M. (2010). Generosity between groups. In H. J. Langholtz & C. E. Westport (Eds.),
or genocide? Identity implications of silence in American The psychology of diplomacy (pp. 29–46). Westport, CT:
Thanksgiving commemorations. Memory, 18, 208–224. Praeger/Greenwood.
Lederach, J. P. (1997). Building peace: Sustainable reconciliation Noor, M., Brown, R., & Prentice, G. (2008). Precursors and
in divided societies. Washington, DC: United States Institute mediators of intergroup reconciliation in Northern Ireland: A
of Peace Press. new model. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 481–495.

134 remembering intergroup conflict

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 134 3/8/2012 5:45:55 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRST-PROOF, 03/08/12, NEWGEN

Onishi, N. (2007, March 8). Denial reopens wounds of Japan’s policy: Replication and extension of a predictive model.
ex-sex slaves. The New York Times. Retrieved from http:// New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 36, 25–33.
www.nytimes.com Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An inter-
Opotow, S. (1990). Moral exclusion and injustice: An introduc- group theory of social hierarchy and oppression. Cambridge:
tion. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 1–20. Cambridge University Press.
Pettigrew, T. (1991). Toward unity and bold theory: Popperian Sidanius, J., Pratto, F., Van Laar, C., & Levin, S. (2004). A com-
suggestions for two persistent problems of social psychology. prehensive analysis of group-based oppression: Social domi-
In C. W. Stephan, W. G. Stephan, & T. Pettigrew (Eds.), The nance theory, its agenda and method. Political Psychology, 25,
future of social psychology: Defining the relationship between 845–880.
sociology and psychology (pp. 13–27). New York: Springer. Starzyk, K. B., Blatz, C. W. & Ross, M. (2009). Acknowledging
Philpot, C. R., & Hornsey M. J. (2008). What happens when and redressing historical injustices (pp. 463–479). In Jost, J.,
groups say sorry: The effect of intergroup apologies on their Kay, A. C., & Thorisdottir, H. (Eds.), Social and psychological
recipients. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, bases of ideology and system justification. New York: Oxford
474–487. University Press.
Pick, M. (2001). The myth of trauma/the trauma of myth: Myths Starzyk, K. B. & Ross, M. (2008). A tarnished silver lining:
as mediators of some long-term effects of war-trauma. Peace Victim suffering and support for reparations. Personality and
and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 7, 201–226. Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 366–380.
Pratto, F., & Glasford, D. (2008). How needs can motivate inter- Staub, E. (1989). The roots of evil: The origins of genocide and other
group reconciliation in the face of intergroup conflict. In A. group violence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Nadler, T. E. Malloy, & J. D. Fisher (Eds.), The social psy- Staub, E. (1998). Breaking the cycle of genocidal violence:
chology of intergroup reconciliation (pp. 117–144). New York: Healing and reconciliation. In J. Harvey (Ed.), Perspectives
Oxford University Press. on loss (pp. 231–241). Washington, DC: Taylor and Francis.
Ramanathapillai, R. (2006). The politicizing of trauma: A case Staub, E., & Bar-Tal, D. (2003). Genocide, mass killing and
study of Sri Lanka. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace intractable conflict: Roots, evolution, prevention and rec-
Psychology, 12, 1–18. onciliation. In D. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jarvis (Eds.),
Ramet, S. P. (2007). The denial syndrome and its consequences: Handbook of political psychology (pp. 710–755). New York:
Serbian political culture since 2000. Communist and Post- Oxford University Press.
Communist Studies, 40, 41–58. Tajfel, H. & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of
Roccas, S., Klar, Y., & Liviatan, I. (2006). The paradox of group- intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.),
based guilt: Modes of conflict identification, conflict vehe- Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago:
mence, and reactions to the in-group’s moral violations. Nelson-Hall.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 698–711. Trouillot, M. (1995). Silencing the past: Power and the production
Ross, M., Banfield, J. C., & Blatz, C. W. (2011). Responding to of history. Boston: Beacon Press.
historical injustices: Does group membership trump conserva- Vollhardt, J. R. (2009). Victim consciousness and its effects on inter-
tism? Manuscript under review. group relations: A double-edged sword? (Unpublished doctoral
Sahdra, B., & Ross, M. (2007). Group identification and his- dissertation). University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
torical memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, Volpato, C., & Licata, L. (2010). Introduction: Collective mem-
284–295. ories of colonial violence. International Journal of Conflict
Rice, C. A. & Benson, J. (2005). Hungering for revenge: The and Violence, 4, 4–10.
Irish Famine, the Troubles and shame-rage cycles, and their Wohl, M., & Branscombe, N. (2008). Remembering historical vic-
role in group therapy in Northern Ireland. Group Analysis, timization: Collective guilt for current ingroup transgressions.
38, 219–235. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 988–1006.
Said, E. (2000). Invention, memory, and place. Critical Inquiry, Wohl, M. J. A., & Branscombe, N. R. (2005). Forgiveness and
26, 175–192. collective guilt assignment to historical perpetrator groups
Schumann, H., Akiyama, H., & Knauper, B. (1999). Collective depend on level of social category inclusiveness. Journal of
memories of German and Japanese about the past half-cen- Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 288–303.
tury. Memory, 6, 427–454. Wohl, M. J. A., Branscombe, N. R., & Klar, Y. (2006). Collective
Schumann, K., & Ross, M. (2010). The antecedents, nature and guilt: Emotional reactions when one’s group has done wrong or
effectiveness of political apologies for historical injustices. been wronged. European Review of Social Psychology, 17, 1–37.
In D. R. Bobocel, A. C. Kay, M. P. Zanna, & J. M. Olson Wohl, M. J. A., & Reeder, G. D. (2004). When bad deeds are
(Eds.), The Ontario symposium: Vol. 11. The psychology of forgiven: Judgments of morality and forgiveness for inter-
justice and legitimacy (pp. 299–324). New York: Psychology group aggression. In J. P. Morgan (Ed.), Focus on aggression
Press. research (pp. 59–74). New York: Nova Science Publishers.
Schwartz, B., Zerubavel, Y., & Barnett, B. M. (1986). The recov- Zebel, S., Doosje, B., & Spears, R. (2004). It depends on your
ery of Masada: A study in collective memory. Sociological point of view: Implications of perspective-taking and national
Quarterly, 27, 147–64. identification for Dutch collective guilt. In N. R. Branscombe
Shnabel, N., & Nadler, A. (2008). A needs-based model of rec- & B. Doosje (Eds.), Collective guilt: International perspectives
onciliation: Satisfying the differential emotional needs of (pp. 148–168). New York: Cambridge University Press.
victim and perpetrator as a key to promoting reconciliation. Zerubavel, Y. (1994). The historic, the legendary, and the incred-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(1), 116–132. ible: Invented tradition and collective memory in Israel. In
Sibley, C. G., Wilson, M. S., & Robertson, A. (2007). Differ- J. R. Gillis (Ed.). Commemorations: The politics of national
entiating the motivations and justifications underlying identity (pp. 105–126). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
individual differences in Pakeha opposition to bicultural Press.

bil ali, ross 135

08_Tropp_Chapter 08.indd 135 3/8/2012 5:45:55 PM


View publication stats

You might also like