You are on page 1of 338

Effects of Online Social Networking Use on Inter-personal and Psychological

Well-being among Students in China: A Longitudinal Study

LI, Jibin

A Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfilment

of the Requirements for the Degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

in

Public Health

The Chinese University of Hong Kong

May 2016
ProQuest Number: 10300438

All rights reserved

INFORMATION TO ALL USERS


The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript
and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed,
a note will indicate the deletion.

ProQuest 10300438

Published by ProQuest LLC ( 2016 ). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author.

All rights reserved.


This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code
Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC.

ProQuest LLC.
789 East Eisenhower Parkway
P.O. Box 1346
Ann Arbor, MI 48106 - 1346
Thesis Assessment Committee

Professor WONG Chi Sang Martin (Chair)

Professor SU Xuefen (Thesis Supervisor)

Professor MO Kit Han Phoenix (Thesis Co-supervisor)

Professor LAU Tak Fai Joseph (Thesis Co-supervisor)

Professor MAK Wing Sze Winnie (Committee Member)

Dr. IP Patrick (External Examiner)


Abstract (English)

Effects of Online Social Networking Use on Inter-personal and Psychological

Well-being among Students in China: A Longitudinal Study

LI, Jibin

Doctor of Philosophy in Public Health

The Chinese University of Hong Kong (May 2016)

Introduction

Depression is the most widely reported psychiatric disorder among adolescents

worldwide. Social networking use as a new phenomenon has gained exponential

popularity, along with increased levels of high Internet penetration. Internet addiction

(IA) overall has been linked with higher level of depressive symptoms in many

cross-sectional studies. However, the causal direction is still under-researched due to

limitations of the cross-sectional study design, especially in the area of social

networking addiction (‘SNA’). Moreover, the impacts of social networking use

intensity on psychosocial and mental health have been raised as a public health

concern, but remain under-researched particularly among adolescents. Current

cross-sectional studies reveal a mixed association due to the study design and lack of

validated instruments used. The mechanisms underlying the relationship between

social networking use and depression have been minimally explored. Therefore,

conducting longitudinal studies among adolescents is highly warranted.

Objectives

This study consisted of two phases. The phase I cross-sectional study aimed to

i
develop and validate an instrument measuring social networking use intensity among

adolescents in Guangzhou, China. The phase II longitudinal study aimed to (1)

estimate the prevalence and incidence of depression, ‘SNA’, and IA among

adolescents in China; (2) explore the causal directions between Internet addictive

behaviors (‘SNA’ in specific and IA in general) and depression; (3) investigate the

predictive effects of intensity and emotional connection related to social networking

use on depression, by using the scales validated in the phase I study; (4) investigate

the impacts of social networking use intensity on psychosocial outcomes, including

friendship quality, perceived social support, self-esteem, adolescent-parents conflict

and social non-confidence; (5) explore the underlying mechanisms of psychosocial

factors and Internet addictive behaviors for the association between social networking

use intensity and depression among adolescents.

Methods

In the phase I validation study, 910 participants who were social networking

users were investigated in junior middle schools throughout Guangzhou, China. In the

phase II nine-month longitudinal study, 4,237 students who were social networking

users were investigated both at baseline and follow-up. Statistical methods including

univariate analyses, multilevel regression models, and mediation and moderation

analyses were used in this study.

Results

Among the longitudinal sample, the prevalence of depression, ‘SNA’, and IA

were respectively 24.57%, 13.69% and 6.56% at baseline, and the corresponding

incidence were 23.37, 12.80 and 6.77 per 100-person-years respectively during the

nine-month period. Longitudinal analyses showed bi-directional relationships between

ii
‘SNA’/IA and depression among adolescents.

The developed instrument, SNAIS, showed acceptable psychometric properties.

Two subscales were identified: SFUI and EFUI. The adjusted results showed that

SFUI and EFUI scores at baseline significantly and positively predicted incident

depression (AOR=1.017 for SFUI score and AOR=1.046 for EFUI score), while the

prediction of emotional connection on incident depression was not statistically

significant.

By using changes in scores during the longitudinal period as variables, increased

social networking use intensity was positively associated with increased perceived

social support (both SFUI and EFUI) and friendship quality (EFUI only). Additionally,

increased social networking use intensity was also positively associated with

increased social non-confidence (both SFUI and EFUI) and adolescent-mother

conflict (SFUI only).

Mediational analyses indicated that changes in addiction (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA)

scores and changes in psychosocial factors (i.e. increased social non-confidence and

adolescent-mother conflict) altogether mediated 78.77% of the effect of change in

SFUI score on change in depressive symptoms. The effect of change in EFUI on

change in depressive symptoms was mediated completely through the paths of change

in addiction (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA) scores and change in social non-confidence score.

Furthermore, the results of the mediational analyses revealed an interesting finding

that change in friendship quality and change in perceived social support negatively

suppressed the impact of change in social networking use intensity on change in

depressive symptoms, but the effect size was relatively small.

iii
Conclusion

Depression and Internet addictive behaviors are present in epidemic proportions

among adolescents, and the two issues are highly co-occurred. Social networking use

is harmful for adolescents’ psychosocial and mental well-being. The results of this

study identified underlying mechanisms and provided robust insight for designing

effective intervention programs to prevent depression among adolescents. Future

longitudinal studies with three or more waves of observations are needed to further

confirm these findings. Translational research is warranted to use this information to

form effective intervention programs.

iv
Abstract (Chinese)

社交網絡使用強度對青少年人際和社會心理健康影響的前瞻性隊列

研究

李濟賓

公共衛生 哲學博士

香港中文大學 二零一六年五月

背景

青少年人群中,抑鬱症的流行現況非常嚴峻。隨著互聯網技術的發展和普及,

社交網絡作為一種新興的網絡活動受到青少年的廣泛使用。多數橫斷面研究報道

認為網絡成癮可能會導致抑鬱症。但是,網絡成癮(包括社交網絡成癮)與抑鬱

症之間的因果關聯仍不清。此外,對於社交網絡使用強度對青少年社會心理健康

的影響的研究甚少。由於缺少有效的測量工具,目前為數不多的幾篇橫斷面研究

給出了矛盾的結果。社交網絡使用強度對抑鬱症影響的機制亦不清楚。尚無在該

人群中開展的前瞻性隊列研究。

目的

橫斷面研究的主要目的是研製適用於中國初中生的社交網絡使用強度測量

量表,并考核其信度和效度。前瞻性隊列研究的目的包括:1)估計抑鬱症,一

般網絡成癮和社交網絡成癮的患病率和發病率;2)探討網絡成癮與抑鬱症的因

果關聯;3)採用橫斷面研製的量表,調查社交網絡使用強度和社交網絡情感依

v
賴對抑鬱症的前瞻性影響;4)探討社交網絡使用強度對青少年社會心理健康的

影響,包括朋友質量,感知的社會支持,自尊,親子衝突和社交不自信;4)探

討社會心理因素和網絡成癮在青少年社交網絡使用強度和抑鬱症關聯中的機製

作用。

方法

本研究由橫斷面調查和前瞻性隊列研究兩部分組成。橫斷面研究調查了 910

名廣州初中生社交網絡使用者。前瞻性隊列研究調查了 4237 名廣州初中生社交

網絡使用者。統計分析方法包括单因素分析、多水平模型、中介效應和交互效應

分析等。

結果

抑鬱症、社交網絡成癮和一般網絡成癮的基線患病率分別為 24.57%、13.69%

和 6.56%。抑鬱症、社交網絡成癮和一般網絡成癮的發病率分別為 23.37、12.80

和 7.7 每 100 人年。多水平模型顯示一般網絡成癮以及社交網絡成癮與抑鬱症的

因果關聯是雙向的。

橫斷面研究中研製的初中生社交網絡使用強度測量量表具有較好的信度和

效度,因子分析顯示有兩個分量表:社交功能使用強度和娛樂功能使用強度。分

析顯示,社交功能使用強度和娛樂功能使用強度基線得分是抑鬱症發病率的危險

因素(社交網絡使用強度的 AOR=1.017,娛樂功能使用強度的 AOR=1.046)


,而

社交網絡情感依賴得分與抑鬱症發病率的關聯無統計學意義。

採用基線調查與隨訪調查間量表得分的改變量作為分析變量顯示,社交網絡

使用強度得分的改變量與感知的社會支持(包括社交功能使用強度和娛樂功能使

vi
用強度)和朋友質量(只有娛樂功能使用強度)的得分改變量成顯著正相關。同

時,社交網絡使用強度與社交不自信(包括社交功能使用強度和娛樂功能使用強

度)和親自衝突-母親(只有社交功能使用強度)的得分改變量成顯著正相關。

中介效應分析顯示,在社交功能使用強度得分改變量對抑鬱症得分改變量的

影響中,一般網絡成癮改變量、社交網絡成癮改變量、社交不自信得分改變量和

親自衝突-母親得分改變量四個變量對其的中介效應達 78.77%。同時,一般網絡

成癮改變量、社交網絡成癮改變量和社交不自信得分改變量三個變量完全中介了

娛樂功能使用強度得分改變量對鬱症得分改變量的影響。此外,朋友質量和感知

的社會支持得分改變量在社交網絡使用強度改變量和抑鬱症得分改變量的關聯

中表現出抑製作用。

結論

抑鬱、網絡成癮行為等心理健康問題和危險行為在中國青少年中的流行非常

嚴重。抑鬱症和網絡成癮行為之間的影響表現為雙向的。社交網絡使用不利於青

少年社會心理健康的發展。本研究的結果(如中介變量)將有助於制定高效的干

預研究策略以便有效預防和降低青少年人群中抑鬱症的水平。未來,需要更多具

有三次及以上觀察次數的前瞻性隊列研究來進一步研究社交網絡使用對青少年

健康的影響。同時,利用本研究的結論開展相應的轉化性的干預研究。

vii
Acknowledgement

My Ph.D. study in the Chinese University of Hong Kong has been like a

marathon full of challenges, frustrations, expectations, and excitement. I will carry

these invaluable experiences with me throughout my whole life and research career in

the future. As my study in CUHK is coming to a close, I would like to take this

opportunity to greatly thank all of those people who unconditionally provided help

and support in various ways during my academic journey.

Firstly, I would like to give my deepest thanks to my supervisors, Prof. Xuefen

Su, Prof. Phoenix Kit Han Mo and Prof. Joseph Tak Fai Lau. They encourage, support,

and push me to achieve different works, which led to the completion of this final

research thesis. They always walked me through all of the stages of thesis research,

from literature reviews, to extracting research topic, writing a proposal, contacting

study sites, assisting with field work, data analysis, to the final draft. Without their

consistent support and illuminating instructions, my Ph.D. study could not have been

achieved well. Their patience and support have made it possible for me to challenge

difficulties with strong confidence. Their hands-on supervision made me a much

stronger and motivated critical thinker and a better writer in scientific research.

During the study period, I am impressed and motivated by Prof. Lau’s wisdom, vision,

and passion for scientific research. I will always be grateful for their time and

guidance provided by my mentors.

Secondly, I would like to give my sincere gratitude to Director Jie Tang and Mr.

Zuguo Qin from the Institute of Health Education in Guangdong province, as well as

Director Jincheng Mai and Ms. Yuxia Chen from the Center for Health Promotion of

viii
primary and secondary schools in Guangzhou, for their strong support and

coordination for school contact and fieldwork of this research. I would also like to

thank all teachers of the junior middle schools and all student helpers for their

assistance and help in the field survey and data collection. I would also like to thank

all participants for their voluntary participation and cooperation. This study could not

have been done smoothly and successfully without their generous cooperation.

Thirdly, I deeply appreciate the help and support from my team members, Dr.

Zixin Wang, Dr. Jinhua Li, Dr. Chunrong Li, Ms. Wangnan Cao, Ms. Xueying Yang,

Mr. Mason Lau and Ms. Danielle Walden. I would also like to give my thanks to my

classmates. Their support and companionship made my three-year study enjoyable

and fruitful.

Fourthly, I would like to thank Ms. Wayser, Graceanne R for her help in editing

language of this thesis.

In addition, I would like to give my hearty thanks to professors, colleagues,

schoolmates and all teaching staff at the Jockey Club School of Public Health and

Primary Care, the Chinese University of Hong Kong, for their great support and help.

Lastly, but most importantly, my greatest gratitude goes to my beloved family

for their loving consideration and great confidence in me throughout the years. I thank

my beloved wife, Lifen Feng, for her love and support. My deepest gratitude also goes

to my parents and parents-in-law for loving and caring for me. My beloved daughter,

Yuxi Li, who came into my life during my Ph.D. study period, is the sunshine in my

life. We will always grow together.

ix
Table of Contents

Abstract (English) ......................................................................................................... i


Abstract (Chinese) ....................................................................................................... v
Acknowledgement .....................................................................................................viii
Table of Contents ......................................................................................................... x
List of Tables .............................................................................................................xvii
List of Figures ............................................................................................................. xx
Abbreviations ..........................................................................................................xxiii
Chapter 1 Introduction ................................................................................................ 1
1.1 Background of the study ...................................................................................... 1
1.1.1 Prevalence and adverse consequences of depression among adolescents .... 1
1.1.2 Increased high penetration of Internet and social networking use ................ 2
1.1.3 Prevalent Internet addictive behaviors .......................................................... 2
1.1.4 Online social networking use as a new threat for adolescents ...................... 4
1.1.5 Knowledge gaps related to Internet and social networking use among
adolescents .................................................................................................... 5
1.2 Overall study design .......................................................................................... 10
1.3 Aims ................................................................................................................... 10
1.4 Structure of the thesis......................................................................................... 12
Chapter 2 Literature review ...................................................................................... 16
2.1 Depression epidemic among adolescents ........................................................... 16
2.1.1 High disease burden caused by depression ................................................. 16
2.1.2 Depression epidemic among adolescents worldwide ................................. 17
2.1.3 Depression epidemic among adolescents in China ..................................... 18
2.2 Internet use worldwide and in China ................................................................. 18
2.3 Online social networking use among adolescents ............................................. 20
2.3.1 Online social networking definition............................................................ 20
2.3.2 Online social networking functions/activities ............................................. 21
2.3.3 High prevalent online social networking use among adolescents .............. 23
2.4 Internet addiction, social networking addiction and depression ........................ 24
2.4.1 Concept and classification of Internet addictive behaviors ........................ 24

x
2.4.2 Internet addiction epidemic......................................................................... 25
2.4.3 Social networking addiction epidemic ........................................................ 25
2.4.4 Relationship between IA and depression .................................................... 26
2.4.5 Relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression .............................................. 29
2.5 Impacts of social networking use on adolescents’ psychosocial status and
depression ........................................................................................................... 30
2.5.1 Assessment for social networking use intensity.......................................... 30
2.5.2 Association between social networking use and depression ....................... 35
2.5.3 Associations between social networking use and psychosocial status........ 38
2.6 Associations between social networking use and Internet addictive behaviors . 41
2.7 A summary of research gaps found in the literatures ......................................... 42
2.8 Statistical methodological considerations for longitudinal study ...................... 43
2.8.1 Causal direction testing in the longitudinal data ......................................... 43
2.8.2 Mediational effects and modeling specification ......................................... 46
2.8.3 Moderation effects and statistical strategies ............................................... 52
Chapter 3 Participants and methods ........................................................................ 53
3.1 Study design ....................................................................................................... 53
3.2 Study site ............................................................................................................ 54
3.3 A summary of rationale to traget junior middle school students........................ 56
3.4 Phase I: Validation study .................................................................................... 57
3.4.1 Participants and sampling ........................................................................... 57
3.4.2 Data collection procedure ........................................................................... 58
3.4.3 Measurement ............................................................................................... 60
3.4.4 Statistical Analysis ...................................................................................... 63
3.5 Phase II: A nine-month longitudinal study......................................................... 64
3.5.1 Participants and Sampling........................................................................... 64
3.5.2 Data collection ............................................................................................ 66
3.5.3 Measurements ............................................................................................. 68
3.5.4 Quality control ............................................................................................ 72
3.5.5 Statistical strategies ..................................................................................... 73
Chapter 4 Description of the independent and dependent variables in the
longitudinal sample .................................................................................................... 75
4.1 Objectives .......................................................................................................... 75

xi
4.2 Statistical methods ............................................................................................. 75
4.3 Results ................................................................................................................ 76
4.3.1 Background characteristics of the entire sample at baseline assessment .... 76
4.3.2 Attrition analysis for loss to follow-up ....................................................... 77
4.3.3 The longitudinal sample (n=4237) .............................................................. 77
4.4 Discussions ........................................................................................................ 85
4.4.1 Internet addiction and online social networking among adolescents.......... 85
4.4.2 High prevalence and incidence of depression among adolescents ............. 88
4.4.3 Natural remission from depression, ‘SNA’, and IA status .......................... 89
Chapter 5 Directionality for the causal relationships between Internet/social
networking addiction and depressive symptoms................................................... 107
5.1 Background ...................................................................................................... 107
5.1.1 Relationship between IA and depression .................................................. 107
5.1.2 Relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression ............................................ 107
5.1.3 Knowledge gaps ........................................................................................ 108
5.2 Objectives ........................................................................................................ 109
5.3 Statistical analysis ............................................................................................ 111
5.3.1 Subsamples ............................................................................................... 111
5.3.2 Modeling methods .................................................................................... 112
5.4 Results .............................................................................................................. 115
5.4.1 Causal relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression.................................. 115
5.4.2 Causal relationship between IA and depression........................................ 118
5.5 Summary .......................................................................................................... 120
5.5.1 Causal relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression.................................. 120
5.5.2 Causal relationship between IA and depression........................................ 121
5.6 Discussion ........................................................................................................ 122
5.6.1 Depression as predictor of incident ‘SNA’/IA .......................................... 122
5.6.2 ‘SNA’/IA as predictors of incident depression.......................................... 124
5.6.3 The bidirectional relationship ................................................................... 125
5.6.4 Implications on prevention ....................................................................... 125
5.6.5 Implications for future research ................................................................ 127
5.6.6 Strength and limitation.............................................................................. 128
Chapter 6 Validation of the Social Networking Activity Intensity Scale............. 140

xii
6.1 Background ...................................................................................................... 140
6.2 Objectives ........................................................................................................ 142
6.3 Sample and Statistical method ......................................................................... 142
6.3.1 Sample used in this chapter....................................................................... 142
6.3.2 Statistical method ...................................................................................... 143
6.4 Results .............................................................................................................. 144
6.4.1 Social-demographic characteristics .......................................................... 144
6.4.2 Factor structures ........................................................................................ 144
6.4.3 Ceiling and floor effects of SNAIS and its two subscales ........................ 145
6.4.4 Item analysis ............................................................................................. 145
6.4.5 Reliability.................................................................................................. 145
6.4.6 Correlation between SNAIS, EC, ‘SNA’, and IA ..................................... 145
6.4.7 Relationship between SNAIS and participants’ characteristics of social
networking use.......................................................................................... 146
6.5 Discussion ........................................................................................................ 146
Chapter 7 Intensity and emotional connection of social networking use as
predictors of incident depression ............................................................................ 153
7.1 Background ...................................................................................................... 153
7.2 Objectives ........................................................................................................ 155
7.3 Statistical methods ........................................................................................... 156
7.4 Results .............................................................................................................. 156
7.4.1 Background factors associated with incident depression.......................... 156
7.4.2 Prediction of baseline SFUI, EFUI, and EC score on incident depression
.................................................................................................................. 157
7.4.3 Moderation effects of EC on the relationships between SFUI, EFUI and
incident depression ................................................................................... 157
7.5 Discussion ........................................................................................................ 158
Chapter 8 Mechanisms underlying the relationship between social networking
use intensity and depressive symptoms: the roles of psychosocial factors.......... 164
8.1 Background ...................................................................................................... 164
8.2 Objectives ........................................................................................................ 165
8.3 Part I: Testing mediation and suppression effects ............................................ 166
8.3.1 Some examples of mediation and suppression effect for the relationships
between Internet use and mental health outcomes ................................... 166

xiii
8.3.2 Mediation and suppression hypotheses for the associations between change
in social networking use intensity and change in depressive symptoms .. 167
8.3.3 Implications of testing the mediation and suppression hypotheses .......... 178
8.3.4 Statistical analysis for mediation and suppression effects ........................ 179
8.3.5 Results of mediation and suppression effects ........................................... 182
8.3.6 Summary for the mediation and suppression effects ................................ 189
8.4 Part II: Testing moderation effects ................................................................... 190
8.4.1 Moderation effects of psychosocial factors on the association between social
networking use intensity and depressive symptoms ................................. 190
8.4.2 Statistical methods for testing moderation effects .................................... 199
8.4.3 Results ....................................................................................................... 200
8.5 Discussion ........................................................................................................ 202
8.5.1 Associations of social networking use intensity on psychosocial factors . 202
8.5.2 Mediation effects of change in social non-confidence and adolescent-mother
conflict ...................................................................................................... 205
8.5.3 Suppression effect of change in perceived social support and friendship
quality ....................................................................................................... 206
8.5.4 Implications............................................................................................... 207
8.5.5 Moderation effects .................................................................................... 208
8.5.6 Limitations ................................................................................................ 208
Chapter 9 Relationship between social networking use intensity, addiction,
psychosocial factors and depression ....................................................................... 222
9.1 Background ...................................................................................................... 222
9.2 Objectives ........................................................................................................ 225
9.3 Statistical methods ........................................................................................... 227
9.3.1 Linear regression analysis ......................................................................... 227
9.3.2 Path analysis for indirect effect model ...................................................... 227
9.4 Results .............................................................................................................. 228
9.4.1 Associations between ΔSFUI/ΔEFUI and ΔIA/ΔSNA ............................. 228
9.4.2 Associations between ΔIA, ΔSNA and ΔCES-D....................................... 229
9.4.3 Mediation effects via ΔIA and ΔSNA for the association between ΔSFUI and
ΔCES-D .................................................................................................... 229
9.4.4 Mediation effects via ΔIA and ΔSNA for the association between ΔEFUI and
ΔCES-D .................................................................................................... 231

xiv
9.4.5 Path analysis for indirect effect model via ΔIA and ΔSNA ...................... 234
9.4.6 Combined mediation effects of change in addiction and psychosocial factors
altogether on the associations between ΔSFUI (or ΔEFUI) and ΔCES-D 235
9.4.7 Summary for mediation effects of all mediation models .......................... 238
9.5 Discussion ........................................................................................................ 239
9.5.1 Associations between change in social networking use intensity and change
in Internet addictive behaviors ................................................................. 239
9.5.2 Associations between change in ‘SNA’, change in IA and change in
depressive symptoms ................................................................................ 241
9.5.3 Mediation effects for the association between change in social networking
use intensity and change in depressive symptoms via change in ‘SNA’ and
change in IA ............................................................................................. 242
9.5.4 Path model for indirect effects via ‘SNA’ and IA ..................................... 243
9.5.5 Combined mediation effects via Internet addictive behaviors and
psychosocial factors altogether................................................................. 244
9.5.6 Limitations ................................................................................................ 245
9.5.7 Conclusion ................................................................................................ 246
Chapter 10 Conclusions ........................................................................................... 256
10.1 Summary and interpretation of key findings ................................................. 256
10.1.1 Depression and Internet addictive behaviors epidemic among adolescents
.................................................................................................................. 256
10.1.2 Causal directions between Internet addictive behaviors and depression 257
10.1.3 A validated scale for assessing social networking use intensity ............. 258
10.1.4 The impact of social networking use intensity on depression ................ 258
10.1.5 Impact of social networking use intensity on psychosocial status .......... 259
10.1.6 Underlying mechanisms for the relationship between social networking use
intensity and depression ........................................................................... 259
10.2 Implications of the study ................................................................................ 261
10.2.1 Intervention programs for reducing depressive symptoms ..................... 261
10.2.2 Intervention programs for preventing Internet addictive behaviors ....... 262
10.2.3 Collaboration and integration of Internet addictive behaviors and
depression intervention ............................................................................. 263
10.2.4 Assessment for social networking use intensity...................................... 264
10.2.5 Methodological considerations in longitudinal causal-effect models ..... 265
10.3 Limitations of the study ................................................................................. 266

xv
10.3.1 Study design ............................................................................................ 266
10.3.2 Sampling ................................................................................................. 267
10.3.3 Causal inference for the relationships between ’SNA’/IA and depression
.................................................................................................................. 267
10.3.4 Measures and reporting bias ................................................................... 268
10.4 Recommendations for future research ........................................................... 270
10.4.1 Longitudinal study with three or more observations .............................. 270
10.4.2 Mining online social networking content ............................................... 271
10.4.3 Research on benefits of proper online social networking use................. 272
10.4.4 Considerations for other measures in future direction ............................ 272
10.4.5 Moderation effects of gender and personality for the effects of online social
networking use intensity on psychosocial and mental health among
adolescents ................................................................................................ 273
10.4.6 Intervention studies using social networking as an innovative avenue .. 273
10.5 Overall remarks .............................................................................................. 274
Reference .................................................................................................................. 276
Appendix I ................................................................................................................ 296
Appendix II ............................................................................................................... 302

xvi
List of Tables

Table 2-1 Instruments measuring online social networking use .................................. 34


Table 2-2 Summary of the literature review for the association between social
networking use and depression .................................................................... 37
Table 3-1 Psychometric properties of the scales used in the longitudinal study.......... 74
Table 4-1 Background characteristics of the participants of the entire sample at baseline
(n=5365) ....................................................................................................... 91
Table 4-2 Attrition analysis for loss to follow-up ........................................................ 92
Table 4-3 Background characteristics of participants among the longitudinal sample
and the subsamples ...................................................................................... 93
Table 4-4 Comparision of background characteristics between those with SNA and
those without SNA at baseline (n=4237) ..................................................... 94
Table 4-5 Comparision of background characteristics between those with IA and those
without IA at baseline (n=4237) .................................................................. 95
Table 4-6 Comparision for the mean differences of baseline IA and SNA scores between
four groups (n=4237) ................................................................................... 96
Table 4-7 Prevalence of depression, ‘SNA’, and IA at baseline and follow-up by gender
in the longitudinal sample (n=4237) ............................................................ 97
Table 4-8 Age- and gender-specific incidence of depression among participants who
were non-depressed at baseline (n=3196) .................................................... 98
Table 4-9 Age- and gender-specific incidence of ‘SNA’ among participants who were
classified as non-‘SNA’ at baseline (n=3657) .............................................. 99
Table 4-10 Age- and gender-specific incidence of IA among participants who were
classified as non-IA at baseline (n=3959) .................................................. 100
Table 4-11 Age- and gender-specific remission from depression among participants
who were depressed at baseline (n=1041) ................................................. 101
Table 4-12 Age- and gender-specific remission from ‘SNA’ among participants who
were classified as ‘SNA’ at baseline (n=580)............................................. 102
Table 4-13 Age- and gender-specific remission from IA among participants who were
classified as IA at baseline (n=248) ........................................................... 103
Table 4-14 Frequency distribution of change in status of ‘SNA’, IA, and depression in
subsample Ia~IIIa ....................................................................................... 104
Table 4-15 Score changes of social networking related scales among longitudinal
sample (n=4237) ........................................................................................ 105
Table 4-16 Score difference of interpersonal and psychosocial scales between baseline
and follow-up among longitudinal sample (n=4237)................................. 106
Table 5-1 Background factors as predictors of new incident depression among
participants who were non-depressed at baseline (n=3196) ...................... 131
Table 5-2 Background factors as predictors of new incident ‘SNA’ among participants
who were non-‘SNA’ at baseline (n=3657) ................................................ 132

xvii
Table 5-3 Predictive effect of baseline ‘SNA’ on incident depression among participants
who were non-depressed at baseline (n=3196) .......................................... 133
Table 5-4 Predictive effect of baseline depression on incident ‘SNA’ among participants
who were non-‘SNA’ at baseline (n=3657) ................................................ 133
Table 5-5 Predictive effect of change in ‘SNA’ status over time on new incident
depression among participants who were non-depressed at baseline (n=3196)
.................................................................................................................... 134
Table 5-6 Predictive effect of change in depression status over time on new incident
‘SNA’ among participants who were non-SNA at baseline (n=3657) ....... 134
Table 5-7 Background factors as predictors of new incidence IA among participants
who were non-IA at baseline (n=3959) ...................................................... 135
Table 5-8 predictive effect of baseline IA status on new incident depression among
participants who were non-depressed at baseline (n=3196) ...................... 136
Table 5-9 predictive effect of baseline depression on incident IA among participants
who were non-IA at baseline (n=3959) ...................................................... 136
Table 5-10 Predictive effect of change in IA status over time on incident depression
among participants who were non-depressed at baseline (n=3196) .......... 137
Table 5-11 Predictive effect of change in depression over time on incident IA among
participants who were non-IA at baseline (n=3959) .................................. 137
Table 5-12 Summary of the causal relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression....... 138
Table 5-13 Summary of the causal direction between IA and depression ................. 139
Table 6-1 Factor loadings and path loading estimated by exploratory and confirmatory
factor analyses for the Social Networking Activity Intensity Scale (SNAIS)
.................................................................................................................... 149
Table 6-2 Item analysis of the SNAIS (n=910).......................................................... 150
Table 6-3 Correlations among SNAIS, emotional connection, social networking
addiction and Internet addiction (n=910) ................................................... 151
Table 6-4 Mean score differences of SNAIS, SFUI, and EFUI by groups (n=910) .. 152
Table 7-1 Predictive effects of baseline SFUI, EFUI, and EC scores on incident
depression by multilevel logistic regression (n=3196) .............................. 162
Table 7-2 Testing the moderation effects of emotional connection on the relationships
between SFUI, EFUI, and incident depression (n=3196) .......................... 163
Table 8-1 Summary of the mediation/suppression hypotheses to be tested and the
rationales based on the literature................................................................ 168
Table 8-2 Background variables associated with change in depressive symptoms in
longitudinal sample (n=4237) .................................................................... 210
Table 8-3 Associations of change in SFUI and EFUI on change in depressive symptoms
by multilevel linear regression models (n=4237) ...................................... 211
Table 8-4 Associations between change in SFUI, EFUI and change in psychosocial
variables by multilevel linear regression (n=4237) ................................... 212
Table 8-5 Association of change in psychosocial scale scores on change in depressive
symptoms by multilevel linear regression (n=4237) ................................. 213
Table 8-6 Testing the mediation effects of change in psychosocial scale scores on the

xviii
association between △SFUI and △CES-D by multilevel linear regression
(n=4237) ..................................................................................................... 214
Table 8-7 Summary for the mediation effects of change in psychosocial scale scores on
the association between △EFUI and △CES-D by multilevel linear regression
(n=4237) ..................................................................................................... 215
Table 8-8 Testing the suppression effects of change in psychosocial variables on the
association between △SFUI and △CES-D (n=4237) ................................. 216
Table 8-9 Testing the suppression effects of change in psychosocial scale scores on the
association between △EFUI and △CES-D by multilevel linear regression
(n=4237) ..................................................................................................... 217
Table 8-10 Summary of interaction effects of change in psychosocial variables on the
association between △SFUI and △CES-D (n=4237) ................................. 218
Table 8-11 Summary of interaction effects of change in psychosocial variables on the
association between △EFUI and △CES-D (n=4237)................................. 220
Table 9-1 Associations of ΔSFUI and ΔEFUI on ΔIA/ΔSNA among longitudinal
sample (n=4237) ........................................................................................ 248
Table 9-2 Associations between change in ‘SNA’, change in IA and change in CES-D
among longitudinal sample (n=4237) ........................................................ 249
Table 9-3 Hierarchical regression models for testing mediation effects of ΔIA and
ΔSNA on the association between ΔSFUI and ΔCES-D among longitudinal
sample (n=4237) ........................................................................................ 250
Table 9-4 Hierarchical regression models for testing mediation effects of ΔIA and
ΔSNA on the association between ΔEFUI and ΔCES-D among longitudinal
sample (n=4237) ........................................................................................ 251
Table 9-5 Summary for all significant mediators in single mediator models for the
association between change in SFUI (or change in EFUI) and change in
CES-D ........................................................................................................ 252
Table 9-6 Multivariate linear regression models for testing the joint mediation effects of
addiction and psychosocial factors on the associations between
ΔSFUI/ΔEFUI and ΔCES-D among longitudinal sample (n=4237).......... 253
Table 9-7 Summary of total, direct, and combined mediation effects of multiple
mediator models on change in depressive symptoms (n=4237) ................ 254
Table 9-8 Summary for mediation effects (% mediated) of all mediation models in
Chapter 8 and Chapter 9 ............................................................................ 255

xix
List of Figures

Figure 2-1 Top 10 causes of DALYs lost among adolescents by sex .......................... 17
Figure 2-2 Growth in Internet usage from 2005 to 2014 worldwide ........................... 19
Figure 2-3 Trend of Internet penetration among adolescents in China ........................ 19
Figure 2-4 Main history of online social networking development ............................. 21
Figure 2-5 Illustration of mediation/suppression effects ............................................. 48
Figure 2-6 Statistical strategy of mediation/suppression hypothesis testing based on
Baron and Kenny’s method .......................................................................... 50
Figure 2-7 Illustration of moderating effect ................................................................. 52
Figure 3-1 Study design flowchart ............................................................................... 54
Figure 3-2 Internet penetration rate in Guangzhou, Guangdong and national level .... 55
Figure 3-3 Location of the study site ........................................................................... 55
Figure 3-4 Flowchart of phase I data collection .......................................................... 59
Figure 3-5 Flowchart of participants in the longitudinal study.................................... 66
Figure 4-1 Illustration of the subsamples..................................................................... 78
Figure 5-1 Causal direction between ‘SNA’ and depression using approach I .......... 113
Figure 5-2 Causal direction between IA and depression using approach I ................ 113
Figure 5-3 Causal direction between ‘SNA’ and depression using approach II......... 114
Figure 5-4 Causal direction between IA and depression using approach II .............. 115
Figure 8-1 Hypothesis illustration for suppression effect of change in friendship quality
on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D 169
Figure 8-2 Hypothesis illustration for the suppression of change in perceived social
support on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in
CES-D ........................................................................................................ 171
Figure 8-3 Hypothesis illustration for the suppression of change in perceived social
support on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in
CES-D ........................................................................................................ 171
Figure 8-4 Hypothesis illustration for the mediation of change in adolescent-father
conflict on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in
CES-D ........................................................................................................ 173
Figure 8-5 Hypothesis illustration for the mediation of change in adolescent-mother
conflict on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in
CES-D ........................................................................................................ 173
Figure 8-6 Hypothesis illustration for the mediation of change in social non-confidence
on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D . 175
Figure 8-7 Hypothesis illustration for the suppression of change in self-esteem on the
association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D............ 177
Figure 8-8 Hypothesis illustration for the mediation of change in self-esteem on the
association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D............ 177
Figure 8-9 Illustrations for conceptual framework of change in psychosocial factors for

xx
the associations between △SFUI/△EFUI and △CES-D ............................ 181
Figure 8-10 Mediation effect of change in adolescent-mother conflict on the association
between change in SFUI and change in CES-D ........................................ 183
Figure 8-11 Mediation effect of change in social non-confidence on the association
between change in SFUI and change in CES-D ........................................ 184
Figure 8-12 Illustration for the mediation effect of △Social non-confidence on the
association between △EFUI and △CES-D ................................................ 185
Figure 8-13 Suppression effect of change in friendship quality on the association
between change in SFUI and change in CES-D ........................................ 186
Figure 8-14 Suppression effect of change in perceived family support on the association
between change in SFUI and change in CES-D ........................................ 187
Figure 8-15 Suppression effect of change in perceived friend support on the association
between change in SFUI and change in CES-D ........................................ 188
Figure 8-16 Suppression effect of change in perceived family support on the association
between change in EFUI and change in CES-D ........................................ 189
Figure 8-17 Suppression effect of change in perceived friend support on the association
between change in EFUI and change in CES-D ........................................ 189
Figure 8-18 Illustration of the poor-get-poorer hypothesis ........................................ 193
Figure 8-19 Illustration for modified poorer-get-poorer and buffering effect hypothesis
.................................................................................................................... 195
Figure 8-20 Hierarchical regression models to test the interaction effects between
change in SFUI (or change in EFUI) and change in psychosocial variables on
change in depressive symptoms ................................................................. 200
Figure 8-21 Graphical illustration for the interactions between change in SFUI/EFUI
and change in conflict with father on change in depressive symptoms3 ... 202
Figure 9-1 The hypothesized path model for indirect effects between △SFUI, △EFUI
and △CES-D via △SNA and △IA. △SFUI: Change in Social Function Use
Intensity; △EFUI: Change in Entertainment Function Use Intensity; △SNA:
Change in Social Networking Addiction; △IA: Change in Internet Addiction;
△CES-D: Change in depressive symptoms measured by Center for
Epidemiological Study Scale-Depression. “+” indicates a positive
association between two variables ............................................................. 226
Figure 9-2 Illustration for mediation effect of change in IA on the association between
change in SFUI and change in CES-D ....................................................... 230
Figure 9-3 Illustration for mediation effect of change in ‘SNA’ on the association
between change in SFUI and change in CES-D ........................................ 230
Figure 9-4 Illustration for mediation effect of change in ‘SNA’ and IA altogether on the
association between change in SFUI and change in CES-D...................... 231
Figure 9-5 Illustration for mediation effect of change in IA on the association between
change in EFUI and change in CES-D ...................................................... 232
Figure 9-6 Illustration for mediation effect of change in ‘SNA’ on the association
between change in EFUI and change in CES-D ........................................ 233
Figure 9-7 Illustration for mediation effect of change in ‘SNA’ and IA altogether on the

xxi
association between change in EFUI and change in CES-D ..................... 234
Figure 9-8 Path model for indirect effects between △SFUI, △EFUI and ΔCES-D via
△SNA and △IA. Paths shown in dashed lines represent non-significant
pathway at the 0.05 level. Standardized path coefficients (β) were presented
in the model. Model fit indexes: χ2 (2) =2.595 with p=0.273, CFI=1.000,
IFI=1.000 and RMSEA=0.008................................................................... 235
Figure 9-9 Illustration of combined mediation effect via change in IA, SNA, social
non-confidence and adolescent-mother conflict on the association between
change in SFUI and change in CES-D ....................................................... 237
Figure 9-10 Illustration of joint mediation effect via change in IA, SNA and social
non-confidence on the association between change in SFUI and change in
CES-D ........................................................................................................ 238

xxii
Abbreviations

CES-D Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression scale


CDI Children Depression Inventory
IA Internet Addiction
SNA Social Networking Addiction
SNUI Social Networking Use Intensity
SNAIS Social Networking Activity Intensity Scale
SFUI Social Function Use Intensity subscale
EFUI Entertainment Function Use Intensity subscale
EC Emotional Connection (to social networking)
DALYs Disability Adjusted Life Years
MDD Major Depressive Disorder
BDI-II Beck Depression Inventory-version II
GPIUS2 Generalized Problematic Internet Use Scale 2
SEM Structural Equation Model
ANOVA Analysis of variance
ANCOVA Analysis of Covariate
FIS Facebook Intensity Scale
SMUIS Social Media Use Integration Scale
EFA Exploratory Factor Analysis
CFA Confirmatory Factor Analysis
OR Odds Ratio
95% CI 95% Confidence Interval
SE Standardized Error
-2LL -2*Log (Likelihood)
df Degree of Freedom
NNFI Non-Normed Fit Index
CFI Comparative Fit Index
RMSEA Root Mean Square Error of Approximation

xxiii
Chapter 1 Introduction

1.1 Background of the study

1.1.1 Prevalence and adverse consequences of depression among adolescents

As a key developmental phase transitioning from childhood to adulthood, the

rapid changes in biological (e.g. puberty and sexual development), psychological (e.g.

personal identity development), and social development (e.g. intimate relationship

development and social autonomy) affect every aspect of adolescents’ development [1].

During this period, adolescents are at high-risk of suffering from diverse

psychological problems, especially along with increased penetration of Internet use [2].

Mental illness, depression in particular, is a worldwide challenge in public health

among adolescents. Due to the onset of puberty, depression is the most widely

reported psychiatric disorder among adolescents [3, 4], and it has been observed that the

risk of depression substantially increased upon entering into adolescence [5].

Depression, a major public health concern globally, is the leading cause of illness and

disability. Depression is associated with a variety of physical and psychological

problems [6], e.g. substance use, disordered eating, and suicidal ideation and attempts.

A high prevalence of depression has been reported among adolescents. Over 9% of

adolescents reported moderate/severe depression (CES-D≥24), and a one-year

incident rate of moderate/severe depression was estimated at 3% in U.S. [7] . The

prevalence of mild, moderate and severe depression (measured by the Children’s

Depression Inventory, CDI) was 14.6%, 15.3% and 12.4% respectively among middle

school students in Mainland China [8].

1
1.1.2 Increased high penetration of Internet and social networking use

Personal Internet use has increased dramatically over the past two decades.

Globally, the Internet penetration exponentially grew seven-fold from 6.5% to 43%

between 2000 and 2015 [9]. Internet penetration in China also significantly increased

from 6.5% in 2002 to 48.8% in 2015 [10, 11]. Adolescents have become one of the

fastest growing segments of Internet users. In China, Internet penetration among

adolescents sharply increased to 79.6% in 2014 [12].

Online social networking as a relatively new phenomenon has gained substantial

popularity among adolescents in recent years. This trend is mainly due to the rapid

advances in information and communication technology, which contribute to easy

accessibility of the Internet. Online social networking is defined as web-based

platforms that allow individuals to create their own personal profile and build a

network of connections with other users [13]. Eight in ten of U.S. teenage Internet users

were also social networking users in 2011 [14]. In Europe, it is estimated that 49% of

adolescents aged 11 to 12 years old, 73% of adolescents aged 13 to 14 years old, and

82% of adolescents aged 15 to 16 years old had their own online social networking

accounts in 2011 [15]. Prevalence data relating to social networking use among

adolescents is limited in China. One small survey reported that almost all sampled

college students (98%) are social networking users [16]. The high popularity of social

networking use among adolescents signifies that they have integrated such use into

their daily activities.

1.1.3 Prevalent Internet addictive behaviors

Along with high penetration and an increasing trend of excessive Internet use, IA

has raised as an important and noticeable public health challenge among

2
adolescents [17]. IA is defined as excessive Internet use that interferes with users’ daily

life, such as preoccupation and feeling restless or irritable when attempting to cut

back [18, 19]. As a condition of inappropriate and excessive Internet use, IA has

considerable negative influence on adolescents’ physical and psychological

development, leading to negative consequences (e.g. severe distress, depression) and

functional impairment [17, 20]. Davis categorizes IA into generalized and a more

specific forms [21], and such classification has been supported by other researchers [22,
23]
. It has highlighted the significance and necessity for distinguishing addiction to

specific Internet activities from IA in general [24]. ‘Social Networking Addiction

(SNA)’ in specific has been recently raised as a public health concern along with

excessive social networking involvement among adolescents. Research has revealed

that those people who are addicted to social networking experience symptoms similar

to those who suffer from addictions to substances and other behaviors [25]. ‘Social

Networking Addiction (SNA)’ is defined as “being overly concerned about social

networking use, to be driven by a strong motivation to log on to or use social

networking, and to devote much time and effort to social networking that impairs

other social activities, studies/jobs, interpersonal relationships, and/or psychological

health and well-being” [26]. Give that ‘SNA’ is a relatively new concern, and its

assessment and diagnosis is still in the way of development, we therefore put it in

quotation mark as an emphasis (i.e. ‘SNA’).

In recent years, a high prevalence of generalized IA has been reported globally.

The IA prevalence among adolescents ranged widely from 0.8% in Italy to 26.7% in

Hong Kong, due to different instruments and cut-off values used [27, 28]. The IA

prevalence among Chinese adolescents was up to 26.0% in 2011, while the proportion

3
of IA among adolescents aged 13-17 was even greater at 30.5%. Current studies

related to ‘SNA’ prevalence in specific usually involved small and non-representative

student samples by using various screening methods. Facebook studies reported an

‘SNA’ (Facebook addiction) prevalence of 1.6% in Nigerian undergraduate

students [29] and 8.6% in Peruvian undergraduate students [30]. The high prevalence of

‘SNA’ was also observed in Chinese college students by modified Young’s Internet

Addiction Test (24.0%~34.3%) [31, 32].

Internet addictive behaviors, IA in general and ‘SNA’ in specific, are strongly

associated with multiple physical, psychosocial and mental health problems among

adolescents, such as obesity, anxiety, substance use and depression [33, 34]. Therefore,

Internet addictive behaviors should be given priority in public health prevention

among adolescents.

1.1.4 Online social networking use as a new threat for adolescents

Online social networking use has profoundly changed the ways that people

communicate and interact with each other. Furthermore, online social networking

applications have brought about tremendous changes in terms of connectivity,

self-expression and daily activities in adolescents’ daily life. Some researchers have

associated online social networking use with several psychiatric disorders, such as

depressive symptoms, anxiety, and low self-esteem [35-37]. However, since online

social networking is a relatively new phenomenon, many questions regarding their

potential impact on psychosocial and mental health remain unanswered. Any future

research that confirms the connection between social networking use and psychiatric

problems would illustrate a serious public health concern.

4
1.1.5 Knowledge gaps related to Internet and social networking use among

adolescents

1.1.5.1 Unclear causal directions between Internet addictive behaviors (IA, SNA)

and depression

It would be beneficial for intervention programs if the causal directions and

magnitude of the relationships between Internet addictive behaviors and depression

among adolescents were clearly defined. However, most of the existing studies that

have investigated the association between IA and depression are cross-sectional in

nature. Due to limitations of the cross-sectional study design, the results cannot

provide definitive evidence for the causal inference between IA and depression. In

contrast, longitudinal studies are advantageous for detecting development or changes

in the characteristics of the target population over time.

Our literature review only found eight longitudinal studies that investigated the

longitudinal relationship between IA and depression among adolescents. Four of the

studies were conducted in Taiwan [38-41], two in Mainland China [42, 43], one in South

Korea [44] and one in Spain [45]. Of these eight longitudinal studies, four studies tested

the prediction of depression on IA, three studies tested the prediction of IA on

depression, and only one study tested the bidirectional relationship between IA and

depression. Unfortunately, these longitudinal studies yielded mixed results, and the

causal direction between IA and depression is still inconclusive. It may be

hypothetical that IA and depression among adolescents is likely to be bi-directional:

depressed adolescents might indulge in excessive Internet use as a means of

alleviating their negative mood, while the negative consequences of such excessive

Internet use (e.g. social isolation) would further exacerbate their level of depressive

5
symptoms [28, 46].

There are fewer studies that examined the causal relationship between ‘SNA’ in

specific and depression than there are for IA in general. Research related to ‘SNA’ and

depression is very scarce. Our literature review found only a few of cross-sectional

surveys that investigated the association between ‘SNA’ and depression among

adolescents [47, 48]. No longitudinal studies were found that explored the relationship

between ‘SNA’ and depression among adolescents.

Therefore, there is a dearth of longitudinal studies that test bi-directional

relationships between ‘SNA’/IA and depression among adolescents in the same study.

It is still unclear whether ‘SNA’/IA are causes or outcomes of depression, and

large-scale longitudinal studies are warranted for clarifying longitudinal relationships

between ‘SNA’/IA and depression, especially in Mainland China.

1.1.5.2 Lack of longitudinal studies related to social networking use

(1) Validated instrument for assessing social networking use intensity

In order to sufficiently assess levels of social networking use intensity and its

impact on adolescents’ psychosocial and mental health outcomes (i.e. depression),

fully validated instruments for such assessment are a prerequisite. To date, there have

been no satisfactory measures to estimate social networking use intensity. Most of the

existing studies measured social networking behaviors only by simple items, such as

weekly frequency of use and daily time spent on social networking use [36, 49, 50]. These

simple questions usually perform poorly, are unreliable, and also cannot adequately

cover the full spectrum of complex activities related to social networking use.

Actually, adolescents might conduct diverse activities for different purposes on online

6
social networking platforms.

There are a few existing scales (mainly focusing on Facebook, e.g. Facebook

Intensity Scale [51]) aiming to assess the social networking use intensity, but they are

not fully validated and psychometrically weak in measuring social networking use

intensity. The existing scales are also potentially not suitable in Chinese cultural

context. Therefore, there is a lack of acceptable instruments to assess social

networking use intensity that take diverse social networking activities and functions

into account.

(2) Impacts of social networking use intensity on depression

Currently, the health impact of social networking use among adolescents is still

controversial. Some studies have associated social networking use with a high level of

depressive symptoms [35, 37], while other studies found no association between social

networking use and depression [52, 53]. All of these studies are based on a

cross-sectional design and were conducted in western countries (e.g. the United

States). Again, longitudinal studies are highly warranted. The present study is

expected to fill these knowledge gaps.

(3) Mechanisms underlying the relationship between social networking use

intensity and depressive symptoms

It is also important to understand the mechanisms underlying the relationship

between social networking use intensity and depression (e.g. potential mediation,

moderation, or suppression effect). Knowing this information would be helpful for

designing effective intervention programs to reduce the impact of social networking

use on depressive symptoms among adolescents. However, there is still a lack of

7
clarity on how and why social networking use affects the levels of depressive

symptoms among adolescents.

Psychosocial factors

Given that adolescents use online social networking mainly for social interaction

and communication purposes (e.g. seeking friends and social support, maintaining and

establishing friendship) [54, 55], it is possible that social networking use intensity may

have significant impacts on adolescents’ psychosocial status. Friends-related status

(friendship quality, perceived social support), family-related status (adolescent-parents

conflict), and intrapersonal-related status (social non-confidence and self-esteem) may

all be impacted by social networking use intensity. Some cross-sectional studies have

investigated the associations between social networking use and some of these

psychosocial factors among adolescents [36, 56-58]. First, it is established that

adolescents’ offline friends largely overlap with their online social networking friends,

and they mainly use social networking to connect with people they known from an

offline context [59]. It is generally believed that social networking use is potentially

beneficial for a sense of friendship quality and perception of social support [16, 60].

Second, adolescents’ preoccupation in social networking use might interfere with their

daily family activities, and causes less time interacting with family members and

greater conflict with parents [58, 61]. However, some parents may use social networking

to maintain closer contact with their children. The impact of social networking use on

adolescent-parents relationship remains unclear. Third, intensive online social

networking use inevitably decreases offline social activities. Due to insufficient

offline interactions, confidence in social occasions may be weakened. Although social

confidence is important for adolescents’ development, there are presently no studies

8
that have investigated the influence of social networking use on social confidence as a

construct among adolescents. Fourth, self-esteem as an important protective factor for

adolescents’ health development has also been researched in a social networking

context among adolescents. However, the results are mixed, with some studies

reporting a positive association between social networking use and self-esteem [62, 63]

and other studies reporting a negative associations [36, 64]. Therefore, the impact of

social networking use intensity on psychosocial factors among adolescents needs to be

further investigated, especially in longitudinal studies.

Moreover, the protective effects of high level of friendship quality, high level of

perceived social support, and high level of self-esteem on depression, in addition to

the harmful effects of social non-confidence and adolescent-parents conflicts on

depression, have been well researched and documented in previous studies among

adolescents. Therefore, the aforementioned psychosocial factors may have the ability

to partially explain how and why social networking use intensity impacts depression

in adolescents. However, there is a lack of longitudinal studies to explore the roles of

these psychosocial factors on the relationship between social networking use intensity

and depressive symptoms among adolescents, i.e. mediation, suppression or

moderation effects.

Internet addictive behaviors

Higher intensity of social networking use has significantly associated with

increased addictive tendency to Internet/social networking use [48, 65], and such

addictive use, in turn, causes adolescents’ mental health problems (e.g. depression).

However, no studies have estimated the extent to which the impact of social

networking use intensity on depressive symptoms is attributable to addictive

9
Internet/social networking use (i.e. mediation effects). Such pathways via Internet

addictive behaviors (i.e. IA, SNA) are likely to be present, but have not been

investigated in previous studies.

In summary, there is a dearth of longitudinal studies that examine psychosocial

factors and Internet addictive behaviors (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA) altogether to explain the

mechanisms underlying the relationship between social networking use intensity and

depressive symptoms.

1.2 Overall study design

This thesis consists of two phases. The phase I study is a validation study, in

which an instrument measuring social networking use intensity was developed

according to a standard scale development process, and then formally evaluated its

psychometric properties. Using the validated instrument, a nine-month longitudinal

study was conducted to explore the longitudinal relationships between social

networking use, ‘SNA’, IA and depression, as well as the potential underlying

mechanisms for the relationship between social networking use intensity and

depression among adolescents in China.

1.3 Aims

There are several objectives in this study, including:

(1). This study aims to estimate the depression, ‘SNA’ and IA prevalence, as well as

respective incidence in a longitudinal study sample of adolescents who are social

networking users in China. Such information is important to expanding our

understanding of the depression, ‘SNA’ and IA epidemic among adolescents over

10
time in China.

(2). This study aims to explore the causal directions between ‘SNA’/IA and

depression among adolescents who are social networking users. In specific, two

causal directions were investigated: (a) For the causal directions from ‘SNA’/IA

to incident depression, the predictions of baseline ‘SNA’/IA status on incident

depression were firstly tested (modeling approach I), and then the predictions of

changes in ‘SNA’/IA status over time (i.e. persistent non-‘SNA’/IA, remission

from ‘SNA’/IA, persistent ‘SNA’/IA, incident ‘SNA’/IA) on incident depression

were conducted (modeling approach II); (b) vice versa, for the causal directions

from depression to incident ‘SNA’/IA, the similar two modeling approaches were

conducted.

(3). This study aims to develop and validate an instrument for robustly assessing

social networking use intensity among adolescents in China. This validation study

was applied as a cross-sectional sample that differed from the longitudinal sample.

The psychometric properties and factor structure of the Social Networking

Activity Intensity Scale (SNAIS) were thoroughly examined.

(4). Using the scale developed in the validation study, this study aims to investigate

the prediction of social networking use intensity on incident depression among

adolescents in China. In addition, given that emotional connection is an important

construct related to social networking use and has not been investigated in

previous studies, its prediction and interaction effect with social networking use

intensity on depression were also investigated.

(5). Last but not least, this study aims to explore the mechanisms underlying the

11
relationship between change in social networking use intensity and change in

depressive symptoms. Specifically, it explores (a) the potential mediation,

suppression and moderation effects of change in psychosocial factors in the

relationship; (b) the potential mediation effects of change in Internet addictive

behaviors (‘SNA’ and IA) in the relationship; (c) the magnitude of combined

mediation effects of significant psychosocial mediators and Internet addictive

behaviors in the relationships. No such studies have previously been conducted

for these research purposes in China or worldwide.

1.4 Structure of the thesis

Chapter 1 is a brief introduction to the background, rationale, overall study design,

objectives, and thesis structure of the study.

Chapter 2 provides a thorough literature review on relevant topics of the thesis. It

includes the following topics: (1) The depression epidemic and disease burden. (2)

Increased high Internet penetration rates worldwide and in China. (3) The definition

of online social networking, related functions/activities, as well as the high prevalence

among adolescents. It was found that there is a dearth of data related to social

networking use among adolescents in China. (4) Research related to IA and ‘SNA’

concepts and their corresponding epidemics, as well as a summary of the

cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships between ‘SNA’/IA and depression. (5)

The literature review also summarized the measurements for assessing social

networking use. (6) The association between social networking use and depression, as

well as the associations between social networking use and psychosocial factors,

including friendship quality, perceived social support, self-esteem, adolescent-parents

conflict, and social non-confidence, are summarized. These psychosocial variables

12
served as underlying mechanisms for exploring for the relationship between social

networking use intensity and depressive symptoms (i.e. potential mediation,

moderation or suppression). (7) The associations between social networking use and

Internet addictive behaviors (‘SNA’ and IA) are summarized. Lastly, topics related to

longitudinal data analysis methods are summarized.

Chapter 3 describes the participants and methodology of the thesis in details. The

setting of study site, study design, participants, sampling method, data collection and

procedure, quality control, measurements of the phase I validation study and phase II

longitudinal study are described. The psychometric properties (i.e. Cronbach’s α) of

the scales that were used in this study are also reported in this chapter.

Chapter 4 presents the age-and gender-specific prevalence and incidence of

depression, ‘SNA’ and IA in the longitudinal sample. Meanwhile, natural remission

rates from depression, ‘SNA’ and IA over time were also estimated in the longitudinal

sample. In addition, descriptive statistics (i.e. gender-specific central tendency and

variance) of social networking-related scales and psychosocial scales are also

presented.

Chapter 5 thoroughly explores the causal directions between IA in general, ‘SNA’ in

specific and depression in the longitudinal sample. Two modeling approaches were

applied: the first modeling approach uses baseline measures to predict incidence, and

the second modeling approach uses changes in status of independent variables

between baseline and follow-up to predict incidence.

Chapter 6 presents the validation results of the developed instrument (SNAIS) in the

phase I study, which was used to assess the level of social networking use intensity.

13
There were no similar instruments available in China. The SNAIS was a short and

easy tool to implement. Two subscales, Social Function Use Intensity (SFUI) and

Entertainment Function Use Intensity (EFUI), with satisfactory psychometric

properties were identified.

Chapter 7 investigates the longitudinal predictive effects of baseline social

networking indicators (SFUI, EFUI, and EC) on incident depression, and explores the

moderation effects of EC on the associations between baseline SFUI, EFUI and

incident depression in the longitudinal sample.

Chapter 8 explores the mediation, suppression and moderation effects of change in

psychosocial factors, including friendship quality, perceived social support,

self-esteem, adolescent-parents conflict, and social non-confidence, on the

associations between change in social networking use intensity (SFUI and EFUI) and

change in depressive symptoms based on the longitudinal sample.

Chapter 9 explores the relationship between change in social networking use intensity,

change in Internet addictive behaviors (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA), and change in depressive

symptoms among adolescents by the longitudinal sample. The mediation effects of

change in ‘SNA’ and IA on the association between change in social networking use

intensity and change in depressive symptoms are also assessed. In addition, the

magnitude of joint mediation effects of significant psychosocial factors and Internet

addictive behaviors (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA) for the association between change in social

networking use intensity and change in depressive symptoms are explored and

summarized.

Chapter 10 is the conclusion section. Key findings of the thesis are firstly summarized

14
and interpreted. The implications of the findings, limitations of the study and

recommendations for future research are discussed in details.

15
Chapter 2 Literature review

2.1 Depression epidemic among adolescents

2.1.1 High disease burden caused by depression

Depression is a common mental disorder that is characterized by depressed mood,

loss of interest or pleasure, decreased energy, feelings of guilt or low-worth, disturbed

sleep or appetite, feelings of tiredness, and poor concentration [66]. Depression often

occurs during adolescence and young adulthood and substantially impairs an

individual’s ability to function at work or school or cope with daily life. Especially

when long-lasting and with moderate or severe intensity, depression may become a

serious public health problem.

It is estimated that depressive disorder accounted for 40.5% (95%CI:

31.7%~49.2%) of the Disability Adjusted Life Years (DALYs) caused by mental and

substance use disorders, and the highest proportion of total DALYs occurred in people

aged 10 to 29 years [67]. It is the first cause of DALYs among adolescents (Figure

2-1) [68]. The effects of depression are considerable and associated with a range of

negative outcomes (i.e. suicide, substance use) [69].

16
Figure 2-1 Top 10 causes of DALYs lost among adolescents by sex

2.1.2 Depression epidemic among adolescents worldwide

A community-based epidemiological survey in 10 countries reported that lifetime

prevalence of depression ranged from approximately 3% in Japan to 16.9% in the

United States, with most countries falling in the range of 8% to 12% [70]. As one of the

most frequently diagnosed psychiatric disorders among adolescents globally, it is

estimated that depression affects 1% to 6% of adolescents each year worldwide [4].

One recent systematic review summarized that the lifetime prevalence of major

depressive disorder (MDD) ranged from 1.1% to 14.6%, with one-year prevalence of

MDD ranging from 0.8% to 13.0% [71]. In Iranian adolescents, the estimated

prevalence of depression ranged from 13.1% to 55.4% based on different instruments

used for measurement [72]. In Chilean adolescents, the prevalence rate of clinical

depression is a markedly high 35% for girls and 28% for boys [73].

17
2.1.3 Depression epidemic among adolescents in China

Since the early 1990’s, the level of depression has significantly increased across

birth cohorts among Chinese adolescents [74]. China has been on the road towards

rapid modernization and social environmental change in recent years. Surrounded by

unprecedented competitive social and academic environments, adolescents have been

exposed to increasing amounts of stress (such as school examinations, family conflicts,

poor peer relationships, etc.), and reported a high prevalence rate of depression. For

instance, a cross-sectional study in twelve middle school students reported a high

depression prevalence of 42.3%, in which the prevalence of mild, moderate and

severe depression was 14.6%, 15.3% and 12.4% respectively [8]. The depression

prevalence in Shandong province was 16.9% [75] and 22.9% in Hunan province [76].

The depression prevalence among adolescents in Yunnan was 22.81%, in which the

prevalence among adolescents aged 11-13, 14-15, and 16-19 years were 20.17%,

24.71%, and 24.92% respectively [77].

2.2 Internet use worldwide and in China

Internet use has become an indispensable part of adolescents’ daily lives. It

provides an easy and immediate way for them to explore information and

communicate with other people across the world. Globally, Internet use has

substantially grown in the past two decades, with the world penetration of Internet use

sharply increasing from 0.4% in 1993 to 40.4% in 2014 [78] (Figure 2-2).

18
100 Developed

Individuals using the Internet (%)


90 World
Developing 82.2
80
70
60
50 43.4
40
30 35.3
20
10
0
2001 2003 2005 2007 2009 2011 2013 2015*
∗ 2015data are estimated. Data taken from International
Source: ITU World Telecommunication /ICT Indicators database

Figure 2-2 Growth in Internet usage from 2005 to 2014 worldwide

The Internet penetration in Mainland China has also dramatically increased in

recent years. It is estimated that there are 668 million Internet users in China by the
[11]
end of June 2015, accounting for nearly 49% of total population . Moreover, the

Internet penetration among adolescents in China sharply increased from 54.4% in

2009 to 79.6% in 2014 according to a recent official report from China Internet

Network Information Center (Figure 2-3) [12].

100 million
3 2.77 100%
2.56
2.32 2.35
2.12 80%
1.95 79.6%
2 71.8%
64.4% 66.4% 60%
60.1%
54.4%
40%
1
20%

0 0%
2009 2010 2011 2012 2013 2014

No. Penetration rate

Figure 2-3 Trend of Internet penetration among adolescents in China

19
2.3 Online social networking use among adolescents

2.3.1 Online social networking definition

Since the launch of the first recognizable online social networking platform

(Sixdegrees.com) in 1997, diverse online social networking services have been created

and are used by millions of people on the Internet globally (Figure 2-4). Online social

networking has rapidly become an important medium for social interaction and has

profoundly changed the ways that people communicate with each other during the

past two decades, especially among adolescent and young adults [79]. However, there

is neither one generally accepted term nor well-established definition for online social

networking. Numerous terms exist in the literatures (e.g. social networking sites,

online social networking, or social networking services), however, these terms are

often used synonymously. Boyd and Ellison [13] used the term “social network sites”

and defined them as Internet-based services that allow individuals to 1) construct a

public or semi-public profile; 2) identify a list of other users with whom a connection

is shared; 3) view and traverse their list of connections as well as those made by

others within the system. However, the definition provided by Boyd and Ellison is too

broad. Schneider et al used the term of online social networking and defined it as

web-based online communities among people with common interests, activities,

backgrounds, and/or friendships that allow users to upload profiles (text, images, and

videos) and interact with others in numerous ways [80]. Online social networking is

more egocentrically structured in that the users are the center of his/her own

community. In the present study, the term online social networking is used.

20
In China

Figure 2-4 Main history of online social networking development

2.3.2 Online social networking functions/activities

Although the nature and nomenclature of online social networking varies around

different platforms/services, most of their core functions are fairly consistent. Along

with technological development of the Internet, online social networking has

combined different types of Internet functions. The core of online social networking

consists of personalized user-oriented profiles, messaging/comments, sharing, and

game functions. A profile is a personal webpage that contains an individual’s

identifying information (i.e. name, photo, date of birth, school, hometown, and

contact information) and interests. A profile provides users a place to express their

individual personalities and hobbies, display online friend lists, and messages from

friends. They can share their lives intimately with their social network online through

their personal profiles. Messaging or commenting (e.g. private/public instant

21
messaging and chatting) is another core function provided by online social networking

services, which can facilitate social interaction with other people. On the message

board (or wall), users can send messages, update their self-status, and post photo or

links. Moreover, users can comment on such messages and respond to comments from

their friends. Messaging and commenting functions are popular features of online

social networking. Additionally, individuals can actively share content (i.e. photos,

videos, music, and news feed) among their friend network through online social

networking. In addition, many online social networking services have implanted

entertainment functions and activities, such as online games and movies.

In summary, individuals can use online social networking for social interaction,

for sharing information about oneself, or for entertainment purposes. Online social

networking users may use personal websites for self-presentation, instant messaging

for communication, message boards for discussion and photo sharing, and gaming for

entertainment. Online social networking has transformed the traditional face-to-face

communication to a new, one-to-many or even many-to-many interactions in a virtual

community.

Adolescents use social networking for different purposes. It has been reported

that adolescents mainly use online social networking for the purposes of social

connection, relationship maintenance, shared identities, photographs, content sharing,

social network surfing and status updates [81-84]. The most common social networking

activities used by adolescents include sending messages/chatting with friends,

reading/responding to notes/messages, reading comments/posts on their profiles,

browsing friends’ pages/profiles/walls, writing comments on friends’ pages/posting

on other people’s walls, and status updating [14, 54, 85]. Moreover, around one fourth of

22
sampled adolescents indicate that they use online social networking mainly for

entertainment purposes (i.e. playing online games, watching videos, listening to

music) [55].

2.3.3 High prevalent online social networking use among adolescents

2.3.3.1 Prevalent online social networking use worldwide

Popularity of online social networking has exponentially increased among

adolescents in recent years. As the most popular social networking service [86],

Facebook has more than one billion active users worldwide and it has been estimated

that this number would significantly increase in the future. In the United States, a

national survey among youth in 2007 by the Pew Internet and American Life Project

reported that around 41% of adolescents aged 12 to 13 years old and 61% of

adolescents aged 14 to 17 years old were online social networking users [55]. In 2012,

more than 70% of U.S. teens were social media users [87]. In Europe, reports from

Livingstone et al in 2011 showed that 49% of adolescents aged 11 to 12 years old,

73% of adolescents aged 13 to 14 years old, and 82% of adolescents aged 15 to 16

years old had their own online social networking accounts [15]. In Canada, an online

survey reported that 85% of college students possessed a Facebook account, and

moreover, 79% of them spent 10~60 minutes on Facebook daily [85]. Besides, it is

reported that more than half of adolescents (51%) checked their online social

networking profiles more than once a day, and more than one fifth (22%) of

adolescents checked their social networking profiles even more than 10 times a

day [88].

23
2.3.3.2 Prevalent online social networking use in China

The China Internet Network Information Center reported in 2012 that 48.8% of

Internet users also used online social networking in Mainland China, while 28.8% of

these social networking users were between 10 and 19 years old [89, 90]. One recent

small survey in Chinese college students (n=337) reported that almost all sampled

college students (98%) used Qzone, 18% used Renren.com, and 7% used both [16].

Qzone is one of the most popular types of online social networking services. It was

launched by Tencent company in 2005 and allows users to write blogs, keep diaries,

send out photos, listen to music, and watch videos. Meanwhile, Renren.com is called

the "Chinese Facebook" as most of its functions are just the same as those on

Facebook. A survey among Chinese college students reported that more than

three-fifths of participants spent 15~60 minutes per day on using social networking,

and over 12% of participants spent more than one hour on social networking use

daily [91]. No data was found pertaining to middle school students.

2.4 Internet addiction, social networking addiction and depression

2.4.1 Concept and classification of Internet addictive behaviors

Based on Davis classification for IA, there are two distinct forms of IA, namely

generalized IA and specific IA [22, 92]. Generalized IA refers that individual use of

Internet as a whole in a compulsive manner, and is defined as excessive Internet use

that interferes with daily life such as preoccupation and feeling restless or irritable

when attempting to cut back [19]. In contrast, specific IA refers to problematic use of

distinct online activities (e.g. online gaming addiction, online gambling addiction).

Previous studies have highlighted the importance of distinguishing between

24
generalized and specific IA [22].

There is an increased trend to distinguish specific IA from generalized IA in

recent research. Excessive use of online social networking raises concerns related to

‘social networking addiction’ (SNA) [93], which is considered a subtype of IA in

general [94]. Previous studies indicate that ‘SNA’ incorporates the experiences of the

classic addictive symptoms like other behavioral addiction [25, 95-97], including mood

modification (engagement in online social networking leads to a favorable change in

emotional states), salience (behavioral, cognitive and emotional preoccupation with

online social networking use), tolerance (increasing use of online social networking

over time), withdrawal symptoms, conflict (interpersonal and intra-psychic problems

because of online social networking use), and relapse.

2.4.2 Internet addiction epidemic

Internet addiction (IA) in general has drawn important attention and has become

a serious public health issue among adolescents. A meta-analysis of 31 nations across

seven world regions estimated a global IA prevalence of 6% [95% CI: 5.1%~6.9%],

the Middle East with the highest prevalence of 10.9%, and Asia (including Mainland

China, Hong Kong, India and South Korea) at 7.1% [98]. In China, the IA prevalence

among adolescents dramatically increased to 26.0% in 2011 from 14.1% in 2009,

while proportion of IA among adolescents aged 13~17 was markedly high at 30.5% in

2011. A representative sample among college students in Taiwan also reported a high

prevalence of IA (15.3%) [99].

2.4.3 Social networking addiction epidemic

Social networking addiction (‘SNA’) in specific is a recent public health concern

25
along with the phenomenon of excessive social networking use. It has been reported

that about one tenth (9.78%) of college students self-perceived with Facebook

addiction [55]. Another study among Irish adolescents reported that about 33% of

participants felt that they overused online social networking [100]. However, one study

among Nigerian undergraduate students reported a relatively low percentage (1.6%)

of Facebook addiction [29], while a survey among undergraduate students in Peru

reported that the prevalence of Facebook dependence was around 8.6% [30]. Moreover,

three studies modified the Young’s Internet Addiction Test to measure ‘SNA’. Wu et al

reported 12% of probable problematic social networking use among Chinese

smart-phone users [101], while 24.0%~34.3% of sampled Chinese college students

were classified with ‘SNA’ [31, 32]. ‘SNA’ has therefore become a significant public

health issue, especially with an exponentially increasing availability of new

technologies (such as smartphones, laptops).

2.4.4 Relationship between IA and depression

The positive association between IA and depression has been reported in a

substantial amount of cross-sectional studies [102, 103]. Although the association

between IA and depression has been repeatedly reported and reviewed, it is still

unclear as to whether IA is a cause or consequence of depression among adolescents.

It is important to clearly understand causality to develop effective interventions and

treatments for both disorders. Our literature review found eight longitudinal studies

among adolescents that investigated the longitudinal relationship between IA and

depression. From this literature, three possible causal directions were described to

explain the relationship. First, it posits that depression as a psychiatric disorder

contributes to and/or deteriorates the symptoms and course of Internet addiction

26
development. Second, that Internet addiction leads to or contributes to the

development of depression. Third, that the relationship between IA and depression

might be bidirectional and may form a vicious circle.

For the causal direction from depression to IA, Davis’ cognitive-behavior

theoretical model proposes that pre-existing psychopathology (i.e. depression, social

anxiety, and substance dependence) is the diathesis and a distal necessary cause of

symptoms of Internet addictive behaviors. Along with this theoretical model, it has

been argued that the presence of depressive symptoms predisposes individuals to

develop Internet addictive behaviors. There are four existing longitudinal studies that

estimated the predictive effect of depression/depressive symptoms on IA, including

one in South Korea and three in Taiwan. Two studies used baseline depression to

predict incident IA, including the study of Cho SM et al in South Korea which

reported that children who had been classified with anxious/depressive symptoms

during childhood had increased risk of IA in adolescence during the 7-year follow-up

period [44], and a two-year prospective study in Taiwan adolescents showing that

pre-existing depression significantly predicted the occurrence of IA [39]. Another study

in Taiwanese high school students found that baseline depression and its increase in

CES-D score during the one-year follow-up period significantly predicted the

incidence of IA [38]. However, the Ko CH et al one-year longitudinal study in Taiwan

found that there was no significant difference in baseline depressive scores between

the incident IA group and the non-IA group [40]. Therefore, these four longitudinal

studies revealed a mixed prediction of depression on IA among adolescents.

Alternatively, according to the displacement hypothesis theory proposed by

Kraut et al [61], excessive Internet use declines with social involvement, then

27
deteriorates the symptoms of psychiatric disorders (i.e. depression, loneliness). Three

longitudinal studies tested the causal direction from IA to depression among

adolescents, including two conducted in Mainland China and one in Taiwan. For

instance, a longitudinal study among Chinese high school students reported that

participants with IA at baseline were 2.5 times more likely to develop depression

during the nine-month follow-up period [43]. Another one-year longitudinal study in

Mainland China reported that a significant increase in depressive scores was observed

between the before and after time periods of participants becoming addicted to the

Internet[42]. A prospective study in Taiwan also demonstrated that adolescents in the

incident IA group had a larger increase in CES-D score compared to the non-addiction

group at the one-year follow-up [41]. It should be noted, however, that research studies

delving into this topic area are quite limited in number. It should be noted that there is

a dearth of such studies delving this topic.

For the bi-directional relationship between IA and depression, Davis notes that

maladaptive cognitions (i.e. self-doubt, low self-efficacy, and negative self-appraisal)

and dysfunctional behaviors (i.e. compulsive Internet use that leads to negative

outcomes, denying about Internet use, and using the Internet to escape from emotional

problems) of pathological Internet use would intensify and continue to produce

negative outcomes over time, resulting in a diminished sense of self-worth and

increased social withdrawal. As symptoms of IA worsen, they would exacerbate

existing psychopathologies, resulting a vicious dysfunctional cycle [17, 21]. To our

knowledge, only one recent one-year longitudinal study among Spanish adolescents

analyzed the bi-directional relationships between depressive symptoms and four

components (i.e. preference for online social interaction, use of the internet for mood

28
regulation, deficient self-regulation and manifestation of negative outcomes) of IA.

The aforementioned components of IA were measured using Generalized Problematic

Internet Use Scale 2 (GPIUS2) in the study. Furthermore, the results from Structural

Equation Modeling revealed that depressive symptoms at baseline positively predicted

components of preference for online relationships, mood regulation, and negative

outcomes (three subscales of GPIUS2) one-year later, while, in turn, the only

component of negative outcomes (the subscale of negative outcomes) at baseline

positively predicted depressive symptoms at follow-up. Thus, the authors concluded

that depressive symptoms is a risk factor of the components of preference for online

social interaction and mood regulation, while the relationship between the component

of negative outcomes and depressive symptoms was reciprocal [45].

The relationship between IA and depression remains controversial as researchers

continue to debate over the causal direction. In particular, there is a dearth of research

to theoretically test a bi-directional relationship between IA and depression among

adolescents. It is still unclear as to whether IA is a cause or a consequence of

depression, or even if a bi-directional relationship exists between IA and depression.

Large-scale longitudinal studies are highly warranted.

2.4.5 Relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression

‘SNA’ may have potential detrimental effects on mental health and other

psychological well-being. Some studies have identified that ‘SNA’ is associated with

several types of health outcomes, including emotional problems (i.e. depression,

anxiety, and poor self-esteem), relational problems (i.e. family conflict, loss of friends,

and relationship dissatisfaction), and other health-related problems (i.e. sleep

problems and poorer sleep quality, somatic symptoms) [26, 104].

29
Research related to ‘SNA’ and depression is very scarce. Only a few

cross-sectional surveys investigated the association between ‘SNA’ and depression

among adolescents, one of which was conducted among Turkish students and reported

that severe depression is significantly associated with ‘SNA’ (specifically Facebook

addiction) [48]. Among Taiwanese college students, it was also found that depressive

symptoms were positively associated with Facebook addiction [47]. One

cross-sectional survey among general Facebook users reported a positive association

between depression and Facebook addiction [105]. Another study reported that

compulsive social networking users scored a higher level of depressive mood than

those non-compulsive users [106]. However, research addressing influences of ‘SNA’

on mental health, especially depression, is still limited.

In summary, there are no longitudinal studies exploring the causal relationship

between ‘SNA’ in specific and depression among adolescents. A knowledge gap hence

exists.

2.5 Impacts of social networking use on adolescents’ psychosocial status and

depression

2.5.1 Assessment for social networking use intensity

2.5.1.1 Using general questions to estimate social networking use intensity

The majority of the studies assessed social networking use behaviors by using

investigator-developed simple or single-item measures. Researchers often estimated

social networking use behaviors by frequency of use [62, 107], number of Facebook

friends [60], and average daily time spent on online social networking [35, 108]. These

simple questions often fail to assess complex constructs and may involve sizable

30
measurement errors. The functions and diverse activities of online social networking

(i.e. instant messaging, comments, sharing personal photo/information, and playing

on-line games with friends), have not been taken into account, which poses an issue

since the natures of activities on online social networking, rather than the frequency of

online social networking use, may have direct effects on adolescents’ psychosocial

well-being [62].

2.5.1.2 Instruments related to social networking use intensity

There are a few studies that have developed specific scales for their research

involving social media use. However, these measures tend to focus only on a single

social networking platform (mainly Facebook), and are often not fully validated.

These scales include the Facebook Intensity Scale (FIS) [51], Facebook

questionnaire [85], Social Media Use Integration Scale (SMUIS) [109], and the Facebook

social connectedness scale [110] (See Table 2-1).

The Facebook Intensity Scale (FIS) is developed by Ellison et al [51] in 2007.

After that, it became a commonly used instrument in the literature [36, 111, 112]. The FIS

aims to measure the extent to which an individual is emotionally connected to

Facebook and integrated its use into his/her daily activities. It includes two parts: the

first part includes six items with a five-point Likert scale (5=strongly agree,

1=strongly disagree) to measure individual’s attitudes towards Facebook (a sample

item is “Facebook is part of my everyday activity”), and the second part includes two

items that are self-reported assessments for Facebook behaviors (i.e. the number of

Facebook friends and the amount of time spent on Facebook in the past week). For the

FIS, Ellison et al reported the internal reliability, but not mention other psychometric

properties. The application of the FIS is inconsistent in the literature. Some

31
researchers argue that the two items for Facebook behaviors (i.e. the number of

Facebook friends and the amount of time spent on Facebook in the past week)

estimated different constructs from emotional connections, and the six attitude-related

items were used as a separate construct to evaluate the extent to which an individual is

emotionally connected to social networking [36].

[85]
The Facebook questionnaire, developed by Ross et al , is another scale that

has been used by other studies. The Facebook questionnaire included the six

attitudinal items from FIS along with one additional item created by the authors

(“How satisfied are you with your Facebook, overall?”), and a number of items

assessing frequency of use for basic features of Facebook. The principle component

factor analysis revealed a two-factor solution, including Attitude (7 items, α=0.83)

and Online Sociability Functions (5 items, α=0.74) (see Table 2-1).

Social Media Use Integration Scale (SMUIS) was objectively developed to

measure the social networking use integration of social behaviors and daily routines,

along with the importance of and emotional connection to social networking use [109].

The 10-item SMUIS included two subscales using six Likert points ranging from

strongly disagree to strongly agree, namely Social integration and emotional

connection subscale with 6 items and Integration into social routines subscale with 4

items by exploratory factor analysis and confirmatory factor analysis (Table 2-1). This

scale was developed based on a formal scale development procedure and conducted

formal validation analysis. The SMUIS has one item related to social routines that

overlapped with two items from FIS (i.e. “Facebook has become part of my everyday

activity and Facebook is part of my everyday activity”).

[110]
The Facebook Social Connectedness Scale was adapted from the Social

32
Connectedness Scale by revising to it specifically for the Facebook environment. It

consists of 20 items (10 positive and 10 negative) to reflect an individual’s positive

and negative feelings towards their Facebook environment. Examples of items include,

“I find myself actively involved in Facebook environment” (positive perception), and

“Even among my Facebook friends, there is no sense of brother/sisterhood” (negative

perception).

In summary, all of the aforementioned scales only examine one specific site (i.e.

Facebook) and mainly emphasized the integration and emotional connection to

Facebook. Additionally, all studies were conducted among college students (see Table

2-1). Currently, the majority of adolescents use multiple types of social media [14] and

involved multiple types of social networking activities (e.g. messaging with friends,

posting comments, status updating) [113]. Adolescents have potentially become the

prominent populations that use online social networking frequently.

33
Table 2-1 Instruments measuring online social networking use
Authors-year Sample Measures Response Items Reliability Methods
Ellison et al-2007 [51] Undergraduate Facebook Intensity Scale 5-point 8 items α=0.83 EFA
students (n=286)
Ross et al-2009 [85] Undergraduate Facebook questionnaire PCF
students (n=97) - Attitudes towards Facebook Attitude: 5-point 7 items Attitude: α=0.83
- Online sociability function OSF: 9-point 5 items OSF: α=0.74
Jenkins-Guarnieri MA et Undergraduate SMUIS 6-point SMUIS: α=0.91 EFA
al-2013 [109] students (n=552) - SIEC 6 items SIEC: α=0.89 CFA
- ISR 4 items ISR: α=0.83
Test-retest=0.80
Grieve et al-2013 [110] College students Facebook social connectedness 6-point 20 itemsα=0.92 EFA
(n=344)
Note: FIS=Facebook Intensity Scale; Attitude towards Facebook subscale includes 6 items from FIS of Ellison et al study and one additional
item composed by the author (How satisfied are you with your Facebook, overall?); Online sociability function subscale was composed of 5
questions relating to the frequency with which individuals engaged in different Facebook activities.
SMUIS: Social Media Use Integration Scale, SIEC=Social Integration and Emotional Connection subscale, ISR=Integration into Social Routines
subscale
EFA=Exploratory Factor Analysis; CFA=Confirmatory Factor analysis; PCA=Principle Component Analysis

34
2.5.2 Association between social networking use and depression

Some studies have linked online social networking use with several psychiatric

problems. The most in-depth mental health measures could be classified into two

panels, including (1) negative outcomes like depression/depressive symptoms [35, 37, 52,
53, 108, 110]
, social anxiety [16, 37, 58, 110] and loneliness [57, 58, 84, 111, 114], and (2) positive

outcomes such as satisfaction with life [16, 49, 60, 62, 110, 114, 115]. Most of these studies are

based on the cross-sectional study design and were conducted in western countries (i.e.

the United States) by focusing on Facebook, which is the most popular social

networking service worldwide. However, the relationships between social networking

use and mental health outcomes remain controversial and unclear. Our thesis would

apply depression as the outcome considering its high prevalence and disease burden

among adolescents.

The literature review found seven cross-sectional studies that investigated the

associations between diverse measures of social networking use and depressive

symptoms (Table 2-2), including two in the United States [52, 53], two in the

Philippines [37, 108], and one each in Australia [110], Serbia [35] and Belgium [116]. Five

studies were conducted in college/university students; while two of the studies, one in

Serbia and another in Belgium, were conducted among high school students. No

longitudinal study was found that investigated the association between social

networking use and depression among adolescents. In the aforementioned

cross-sectional studies, the results on the association between social networking use

and depression are inconsistent. For instance, three surveys found a non-significant

association between social networking use and depression [52, 53, 108], while two studies

reported that prolonged use of social networking (measured by amount of time spent

35
on social networking) was significantly correlated with high level of depression [35, 37].

However, the study in Australia reported that Facebook social connectedness was

significantly associated with lower level of depression [110]. The American Academy

of Pediatrics (APP) revealed a new term “Facebook depression”, that describes the

potential effects on mental health problems due to excessive amount of time spent on

social media (like Facebook), in which individuals affected by the condition will

subsequently begin to exhibit classic symptoms of depression [117].

Apart from the differences in the methodology of these studies, it is possible that

social networking use may have diverse impacts on different age groups, given that

most of the previous studies were conducted among older adolescents and young

adults (i.e. college students). There are many potential reasons why social networking

users have the tendency to suffer from depressive disorder. Contrastingly, there are

numerous factors that may influence already depressed individuals to begin using

online social networking. However, this issue has not yet been interpreted and

discussed sufficiently, and factors that contributed to heterogeneity of findings have

not yet been identified thoroughly. Considering the potentially different effects of

social networking use on depression between early and late adolescence, it is

necessary to further conduct related studies among early adolescents in particular.

Moreover, longitudinal studies with large-scale samples are warranted to estimate the

predictive effects of this new phenomenon on depression among adolescents.

In summary, all of the studies that investigated the association between social

networking use and depression are cross-sectional, and the results are inconsistent.

The impact of social networking use on adolescents’ depression is still under-

researched. Longitudinal studies are needed to robustly investigate the relationship.

36
Table 2-2 Summary of the literature review for the association between social networking use and depression
Author-year-country Design Subjects Measure for SNS use Main results
Frison and CSS 910 high school Active public Facebook use, - Passive Facebook use and active public
Eggermont-2015 [116] students Active private Facebook use Facebook use is positively associated with
Belgium Passive Facebook use depressed mood
Labrague L-2014 [37] CSS 76 college - Facebook intensity scale - Time spent on Facebook positively correlated
Philippines students - Number of Facebook friends with depression (r=0.23) and anxiety (r=0.26)
- Time spent on Facebook in a
typical day
Simonic TE et al-2014 [52] CSS 237 university - Facebook activity subscale - No direct association between Facebook use
United States students and depressive symptoms
- Moderating effect was observed: more frequent
Facebook use was associated with lower
depressive symptoms only for females with
high neuroticism
[108]
Datu JA et al-2013 CSS 200 college - Number of hours per week - No significant association
Philippines students aged
17~20
[110]
Grieve R et al-2013 CSS 274 university - Facebook social - Facebook facilitated social connections and
Australia students connectedness associated with lower depression (r=-0.27) and
anxiety (r=-0.21), and greater satisfaction with
life (r=0.30)
Jelenchick LA et CSS 190 - Average time spent on social - No association between social networking use
al-2013 [53] undergraduate networking per day based on and depressive symptoms
United States students experience sample method
one week
Pantic I, et al-2012 [35] CSS 160 high school - Daily time spent on social - Positive correlation was observed between time
Serbia students networking spent on social networking and depression
CSS: Cross-sectional study; r: correlation coefficient.

37
2.5.3 Associations between social networking use and psychosocial status

2.5.3.1 Association between social networking use and friendship quality

Establishing intimate relationships and emotional connection with others is an

important developmental skill during adolescence. Researchers suggest that

adolescents online and offline worlds are indeed connected and overlap [54]. They use

social networking mainly to maintain connections with offline friends and family

members [54, 59]. A previous study showed that 43% of sampled adolescents felt that

social networking use made them feel closer with their friends [59]. Another study

among adolescents aged 9 to 13 years old also showed that social networking users

reported a strong sense of belonging to their friendship group compared to non-users,

and a positive linear relationship between social networking use intensity and feelings

of belongingness among males was found [118]. However, one survey reported that the

majority (73%) of sampled participants thought that social networking use had not

made any difference to their relationships with friends, whereas around 20% felt that

social networking use made them closer with their friends, and on the contrary, a very

small proportion (2.5%) felt that it had negatively impacted their relationships [54]. A

few cross-sectional studies reported that social networking use was significantly

associated with enhanced friendship quality and intimacy [51, 110].

2.5.3.2 Association between social networking use and perceived social support

Perceived social support has emerged as a prominent concept that characterizes

social support as the cognitive appraisal of being reliably connected to others [119],

which refers to an individual’s belief in the availability of support. One study among

U.S. undergraduate students showed that the number of Facebook friends was

38
associated with increased perceived social support [60]. Another study among South

Korea undergraduate students reported that self-disclosure through online social

networking was positively associated with social support [115]. In general, social

networking use is considered to be beneficial for enhancing adolescents’ social

connection and perceived social support.

2.5.3.3 Association between social networking use and family relationships

During adolescence, young people strive for dependency and become

autonomous to establish their own identities. On the other hand, family still exerts a

potent influence and support on their development. Harmonious family relationships,

especially between adolescents and their parents, are important for adolescents’

development by providing emotional and instrumental support for adolescents. Such

support could assist adolescents to solve certain problems. Intra-familial conflicts,

particularly adolescents’ conflicts with their parents, are common during this period.

Adolescent-parents relationships have a strong impact on adolescents’ psychological

development. Engagement in online social networking potentially interferes with

adolescents’ family activities and reduces communication with parents [61]. Social

networking use among adolescents has been linked with greater parent-youth

conflicts [54] and less time spent with parents [120]. However, some parents may utilize

social networking to maintain greater contact with their children, and require them to

be connected through online social networking [121]. Hence, the association between

social networking use and adolescent-parents relationships remains controversial.

Future research into this area is warranted.

39
2.5.3.4 Association between social networking use and social non-confidence

Social confidence represents the extent to which an individual can disclose to,

relate to, and successfully interact with others on social occasions [122]. High levels of

social confidence are generally viewed as a protective factor that is associated with

good mental health and psychosocial outcomes among adolescents. Building

confidence to interact with others is an important aspect of adolescents’ development.

Social networking interaction potentially reduces face-to-face interaction offline.

Online interaction is substantially different from face-to-face interaction in terms of a

lack of non-verbal cues (i.e. voice tone, eye contact), and a reduced degree of social

presence [123]. Due to a reduced probability in face-to-face interactions in real life,

social confidence might be diminished. Although social confidence is an important

protective psychosocial factor for adolescents’ mental health, no existing study has

tested the impact of social networking use on social confidence as a construct among

adolescents.

2.5.3.5 Association between social networking use and self-esteem

Self-esteem refers to the evaluation of individuals’ overall sense of self-worth or

personal value. Self-esteem is an important protective psychosocial factor in

developing and maintaining mental health and overall quality of life among

adolescents. A low level of self-esteem is associated with numerous mental health

problems (e.g. depression) [124, 125]. Previous studies reported conflicting results

regarding the influence of social networking use on self-esteem. In Kalpidou et al’s

study [36], spending long time on Facebook was related with lower self-esteem.

However, some researchers argue that online social networking allows teenagers to

have enough time to control information that they want others to have about them and

40
enables users to present themselves in a selective way. These selective

self-presentations of themselves on online social networking influence their self-

impressions. A cross-sectional survey among Dutch teenagers reported a positive

association between social networking use and youth self-esteem [62]. It should be

noted that there is presently a dearth of studies that examine the relationship between

social networking use and self-esteem.

2.6 Associations between social networking use and Internet addictive behaviors

As a popular Internet activity, online social networking has been frequently and

excessively used by adolescents. One recent study among Chinese college students

reported that more than three-fifths of participants spent 15 to 60 minutes per day

using online social networking, and over 12% of participants spent more than one

hour on online social networking daily [91]. Another study reported that around 6.55%

of sampled participants spent more than 16 hours on social networking per week [106].

The characteristics of online social networking (i.e. anonymous, selective

self-disclosure, and convenient communication without the limitation by geography

and distance) could facilitate online social interaction, and then function as an

attractive factor for excessive Internet/social networking use. Keeping in touch with

friends was the greatest reason for high frequencies of social networking use [126].

Some studies have investigated the association between diverse measures of

social networking use and addictive behaviors and tendency. One survey among

college students reported that Internet addicts had higher Facebook use intensity (i.e.

frequency and minutes per day) as compared to non-addicts, and moreover, a

regression model revealed that time spent online and frequency of Facebook use were

41
significant predictors of IA [127]. A one-year longitudinal study reported that high

frequency of online social networking use significantly predicted compulsive Internet

use [65].

Similarly, a study from the Belgian general population reported that compulsive

social networking users spent considerably more time on these Internet applications

than other users [106]. Another survey in Turkish college students reported that students

who frequently used Facebook for social interaction reported a higher level of

Facebook addiction [48]. One recent cross-sectional study reported that daily time

spent on Internet use was significantly associated with Facebook addiction [105].

2.7 A summary of research gaps found in the literatures

There are several knowledge gaps found in the literature, including:

(1) The causal relationships between ‘SNA’/IA and depression remain controversial

and under-researched. There is a lack of longitudinal studies to test the two

predictive directions (‘SNA’/IA predicts depression and depression predict

‘SNA’/IA) in the same longitudinal study. Clarification for the causal directions

between ‘SNA’/IA and depression would be helpful for the development of

effective interventions.

(2) There have been no fully validated and acceptable instruments for measuring

social networking use intensity robustly, especially in Chinese context.

Instruments with good psychometric properties are highly needed, which would be

important to robustly estimate the impact of social networking use on adolescents’

mental health problems (e.g. depression).

42
(3) The impact of social networking use on depression among adolescents is

under-researched and mixed.

(4) Moreover, it is still unclear as to how and why social networking use intensity

impacts adolescents’ depression, i.e. underlying mechanisms. Literature reviews

imply that psychosocial factors (i.e. friendship quality, perceived social support,

self-esteem, social non-confidence, adolescent-parents conflict) and Internet

addictive behaviors (i.e. SNA, IA) have the potential to explain the association

between social networking use intensity and depression. However, the role of

these variables on the association between social networking use intensity and

depression is not investigated in previous studies.

2.8 Statistical methodological considerations for longitudinal study

2.8.1 Causal direction testing in the longitudinal data

The statistical methodologies in longitudinal observational studies that aim to

test longitudinal causal relationships are diverse and need to be addressed. In the

literature, there are at least six modeling approaches to analyze longitudinal

observational data in studying IA/depression or other outcome variables: (1) Baseline

independent variable to predict the dependent variable (i.e. incident depression or IA),

with or without controlling for the baseline level of the corresponding dependent

variable [43, 44, 128]; (2) Change in status of the independent variable (i.e. changing from

non-depression at baseline to depression at follow-up, changing from depression at

baseline to non-depression at follow-up) to predict the dependent variable [129, 130]; (3)

Score change over time of the independent variable to predict the dependent

variable [131]; (4) Baseline independent variable to predict the score change of the

43
dependent variable [132]; (5) Change in status of the independent variable to predict

score change of the dependent variable [133]; (6) Structural Equation Modeling

(SEM) [45, 134]. Thereinto, change in score is different from change in status, change in

scale score indicates the difference in score from baseline to follow-up, which is a

continuous variable, while change in status indicates the conversion of the

psychopathological condition over time (i.e. participants transitioned from

non-depression at baseline to depression at follow-up, or recovery from depression at

baseline to non-depression at follow-up), which is a categorical variable [130]. For the

aforementioned modeling approaches, the previous longitudinal studies involving IA

applied modeling approach 1 and the SEM approach.

Examples for the first approach as mentioned above include the Lam LT et al

study which was conducted in China and used baseline IA to predict incident

depression by fitting logistic regression models [43], and the Cho SM et al study which

was conducted in South Korea and used baseline depression to predict IA at follow-up

by simple logistic regression models [44]. In these studies, IA or depression was

generally measured at one point in time with the assumption that the condition

remains unchanged over time. This modeling method has the strength of providing an

explicitly temporal sequence for the effect between the independent and dependent

variables, meaning that the independent variable is definitely prior of the outcome,

which is one of the important criteria for causal inference. Therefore, we applied this

statistical approach in the present study.

However, the aforementioned modeling method does not take into account

remission from IA to non-IA during the study period (and remission from depression

to non-depression). Previous studies have reported that about half of participants

44
(49.5% [40] and 51.5% [135]) naturally remitted from IA during the one-year follow-up

period. Considering the potential remission effects, some studies have used the change

in status of independent variables over time for modeling specification, such as

classifying participants into four categories based on depression status at baseline and

follow-up, including (1) persistent non-depression (persistent non-depression both at

baseline and follow-up), (2) incident depression (incidence from non-depression at

baseline to depression at follow-up), (3) remission from depression (remission from

depression at baseline to non-depression at follow-up), and (4) persistent depression

(persistent depression both at baseline and follow-up). However, these studies mainly

related to the relationship between physical activity and depression. No longitudinal

studies aiming to investigate the relationship between IA and depression have applied

this modeling method. Therefore, we also applied this modeling approach to take into

account the status transition of the independent variable (e.g. remission from ‘SNA’,

IA or depression) over time.

Moreover, as previously discussed, some studies used the change in score from

baseline to follow-up (i.e. score of depressive symptoms at follow-up minus the score

at baseline) for modeling specification. However, in order to definitively define the

change in status for ‘SNA’, IA, and depression over time, we applied the modeling

approach with status change rather than score change. Besides, Structural Equation

Modeling (SEM) techniques have been increasingly used in medical statistics in

recent years. SEM has the advantage of testing the theoretical models involving

complex patterns of relationships among multiple predictors and dependent variables

simultaneously [136]. As a general rule, SEM is indicated when more than one

regression equation is necessary for statistical modeling of the phenomena under

45
investigation [137]. However, SEM is mainly used for continuous variables, and is still

in its development for handling categorical variables. It has been argued that SEM

should be used only when 1) it is truly interested in exploring a wide range of

different effects and pathways (i.e. direct and indirect effects) across an entire set of

variables for several different outcomes, and 2) it is being used principally for

exploratory and hypothesis-generating purposes [138]. Therefore, in this chapter, the

SEM is not applied as a statistical method for causal direction.

In summary, this study applied the first two aforementioned modeling

approaches when analyzing our longitudinal data, aiming to investigate the causal

directions between ‘SNA’/IA and depression from two angles – Approach I which has

predictive implications by the advantage of an explicitly temporal sequence and

Approach II which takes the changes in status of the independent variables (including

remissions from pathological states) into account (Chapter 5).

2.8.2 Mediational effects and modeling specification

2.8.2.1 Concepts of mediation and suppression effects

A mediator conceptually means a variable that has a position in the causal

sequence and could account for all or part of the relationship between an independent

variable and a dependent variable (Figure 2-5a) [139]. Statistically, the mediation effect

would reduce the magnitude of the relationship between an independent variable and

a dependent variable when the effect of a mediator is controlled for in the model [139,
140]
. The effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable is shown to

include two causal paths (Figure 2-5b): the direct impact of the independent variable

(path c’) and the impact of the mediator (path b). Moreover, there is a path from the

independent variable to the mediator (path a). For the mediation effect, indirect effect

46
of the independent variable on the dependent variable via the mediator has the same

sign of the direct effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable. For

example, one study among high school students in Italy investigated the associations

between self-esteem, preference for online social interaction and IA [141], and the

results showed that the negative association between self-esteem and IA was partially

mediated by preference for online social interaction. When controlling for preference

for online social interaction in the model, the regression coefficient of self-esteem on

IA significantly reduced from -0.711 to -0.411. This means that the negative effect of

self-esteem on IA is partially through the increase of preference for online social

interaction.

A suppressor is defined as a situation in which a third variable would increase the

predictive validity of an independent variable on a dependent variable by its inclusion

in a regression model [140, 142], thus the strength of the relationship between an

independent and a dependent variable is suppressed by a third variable. In the

statistical model (Figure 2-5b), if path a and path b mentioned above are fulfilled, but

when paths a and path b are controlled for in the same model, and a previously

significant relationship between the independent variable and dependent variable

significantly increased, a suppression effect should hence be considered. A

suppression analysis proposes that the indirect effect of the independent variable on

the dependent variable through a suppressor and the direct effect of the independent

variable on the dependent variable have different signs. From this perspective,

suppression is also known as inconsistent mediation [143]. For instance, Paulhus DL

and colleagues presented the suppression effect of self-esteem on the association

between narcissism and antisocial behavior, reporting that when adding self-esteem

47
into the regression model, the positive effect of narcissism on antisocial behavior

significantly increased from 0.21 to 0.32. A suppression effect from self-esteem is

hence present [144]. In detail, results from this study showed that narcissism is

positively associated with antisocial behavior (β=0.21), self-esteem was negative

associated with antisocial behavior (β=-0.27), and self-esteem and narcissism were

positively correlated. It showed that the direct effect of narcissism on antisocial

behavior is positive, and the indirect effect of narcissism on antisocial behavior

through self-esteem is negative. When these two effects are considered together, they

cancel each other out and result in a diminished total effect. Another cross-sectional

study examines whether the Medicaid suppresses racial disparities on health care

needs (including child’s need for professional care coordination, child’s need for

mental health services, and the family’s need for mental health services), and found

that race has a negative direct effect for all three outcomes, and Medicaid coverage

suppressed the racial disparities in reporting the need for services [145]. However, the

suppression effect was small, ranging from 0.011 to 0.017 for the three outcomes.

Independent c Dependent
Independent
variable variable
variable

Mediator/
Mediator/
Suppressor
Suppressor a b

Independent c’ Dependent
Dependent variable variable
variable
Total effect=c
Direct effect=c’; Mediation/suppression effect=ab
(a) Conceptual model (b) Statistical model
Figure 2-5 Illustration of mediation/suppression effects

48
2.8.2.2 Statistical requirements for testing mediation and suppression effects

A variable functions as a mediator when it meets the following requirements

from Baron and Kenny’s method [139]: (a) variations in levels of the independent

variable significantly account for variations in the presumed mediator (i.e. path a), (b)

variations in the mediator significantly account for variations in the dependent

variable (i.e. path b), and (c) when path a and path b are controlled for, a previously

significant relationship between the independent variable and the dependent variable

is significantly reduced or becomes non-significant. If these requirements are fulfilled,

the mediation effect is hence support. Based on the Baron and Kenny’s method, in

order to test for the mediation effect, three following regression equations should be

estimated (Figure 2-6): (1) the dependent variable is regressed on the independent

variable; (2) the mediator is regressed on the independent variable; (3) the dependent

variable is regressed on both the independent variable and the mediator. Separate

regression coefficients for each model should be estimated and tested. These three

regression equations provide the tests of the linkages of the mediational model.

To establish mediation, the following conditions must be fulfilled: (1) the

independent variable must affect the dependent variable in the first regression; (2) the

independent variable must be shown to affect the mediator in the second regression;

and (3) the mediator must affect the dependent variable in the third regression; (4) if

these conditions all hold in the predicted direction, the regression coefficient of the

independent variable on the dependent variable in the third model must be

significantly less than that in the first regression model. Complete mediation holds

true if the independent variable has no effect when the mediator is controlled in the

third regression model.

49
For suppression effect analysis, the first three aforementioned conditions are held.

It is the fourth condition that is used to distinguish the suppression from mediation.

Specifically, the regression coefficients of the independent variable on the dependent

variable in the third regression must be significantly larger than that in the first

regression model.

e (1)

Independent Step 1: c Dependent


variable X variable Y
Y = b 0(1) + cX + e (1)

e (2)

Intervening
variable M
Step 2: a Step 3: b
e (3)

Step 3: c’
Independent Dependent
variable X variable Y
M = b 0(2) + aX + e (2)
Y = b 0(3) + c ' X + bM + e (3)

Figure 2-6 Statistical strategy of mediation/suppression hypothesis testing based on


Baron and Kenny’s method

2.8.2.3 Testing mediation hypotheses for longitudinal data

Compared with cross-sectional data, longitudinal data could add potential

improvement for interpretation. However, longitudinal research introduces a number

of modeling options and additional considerations when testing longitudinal

mediational relationships. One of the advantages of using longitudinal data to evaluate

causal mediational processes comparing with using cross-sectional data is that

longitudinal data could examine associations within changes across waves of data (i.e.

within subjects) [146].

50
With regard to two-wave longitudinal data, there are mainly two mediational

modeling approaches as summarized in the literature [147, 148]. The first approach is the

raw change score approach. In this modeling approach, change scores are calculated

for each of independent, mediating, and dependent variables, and then these change

scores are used for mediation modeling through the traditional mediation strategy

(Baron and Kenny [139]). This modeling approach has been widely used in previous

longitudinal studies. For instance, a 16-week interval, two-wave longitudinal study

used the variables of change in consumption of food away from home, change in diet

quality/dietary intake and change in BMI/percent body fat to test the mediation effects

of change in diet quality/dietary intake on the relationships between change in

consumption of food away from home and change in BMI/percent body fat [149].

Another longitudinal study also applied the raw change score testing the mediation of

changes in HRV on the relationships between changes in smoking frequency and

changes in erectile tumescence using the Baron and Kenny’s strategy [150].

The second approach is the Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) approach. This

approach uses time one measurements as the covariates in the model with either raw

scores at time two or change scores as dependent variables. However, the ANCOVA

approach is appropriate in the case of an intervention effects in randomized controlled

trial, but is not recommended for observational studies [148, 151, 152].

Considering the advantage that the raw change score approach over time could

remove the influence of cross-sectional correlations between subjects and allow the

analysis to exclusively focus on within-subject effects [148], we applied the raw change

score modeling approach for the longitudinal mediational modeling analysis in the

present study (Chapter 8 and Chapter 9).

51
2.8.3 Moderation effects and statistical strategies

A moderator is a variable that affects the direction and/or strength of the

relationship between an independent variable and a dependent variable (Figure

2-7a) [139, 153]. In statistics, a moderation effect can be tested by using an interaction

term, whereby the effect of an independent variable on a dependent variable varies by

the level of another variable. There are three causal paths that feed into the outcome

variable (Figure 2-7b): the independent variable (path a), the moderator (path b) and

the interaction of the independent variable and the moderator (path c). The moderating

hypothesis is supported if the regression coefficient of the interaction term (path c)

onto the dependent variable is statistically significant. Moderation hypothesis testing

can signify how the association between an independent variable and a dependent

variable varies in terms of the level of the third variable.

Independent
variable a
Moderator
b Dependent
Moderator
variable
Independent Dependent
variable variable c
Predictor
×
Moderator

(a) Conceptual model (b) Statistical model

Figure 2-7 Illustration of moderating effect

52
Chapter 3 Participants and methods

3.1 Study design

Data of this thesis was obtained based on a two-phase study (see Figure 3-1).

Phase I, a cross-sectional validation study, aimed to develop and validate a new

instrument (named as Social Networking Activity Intensity Scale, SNAIS) for

measuring social networking use intensity by considering diverse social networking

activities and functions among Chinese junior middle school students.

By using instruments validated in the phase I study, we conducted a nine-month

longitudinal study (Phase II), aiming to objectively investigate the causal directions

between Internet addictive behaviors (‘SNA’ and IA) and depression, prediction of

social networking use intensity and emotional connection to social networking onto

depression, and also to test the potential underlying mechanisms for the association

between social networking use intensity and depression (i.e. mediation, suppression,

and moderation effects of psychosocial status and mediation effects of Internet

addictive behaviors).

53
Panel, literature review,
focus group, compile items

Pilot survey and scale revision � Validation analysis


Phase Ⅰ: - Construct validity: EFA/CFA
Validation survey - Content validity: Spearman correlation
- Test-retest reliability: ICC
Validation survey among junior high
- Internal reliability: Cronbach alpha
school students in Guangzhou

Revise and get the validated scales

Background variables Psychosocial scales

Baseline survey for junior high


school students

Phase Ⅱ: 9-month
Longitudinal study Data analysis
interval

Follow-up survey

Figure 3-1 Study design flowchart

3.2 Study site

The present study was conducted in Guangzhou, Mainland China. Guangzhou,

the capital of the Guangdong province, is located in the Pearl River Delta Economic

Zone in China with a total permanent population of 12.8 million in 2012, in which

there were 373,481 students in 354 junior middle schools. As one of the three

megacities in Mainland China, the Internet penetration rate was a high 71.7% among

the general population in 2010, which was more than two-fold of the national level

(Figure 3-2). Weekly time spent on the Internet was around 23.9 hours in Guangzhou

in 2010.

Geographically, administrative regions in Guangzhou consist of ten districts:

54
Yuexiu, Haizhu, Liwan, Tianhe, Baiyun, Huangpu, Huadu, Panyu, Nansha and

Luogan, and two counties: Conghua and Zengcheng. Based on geography and

educational development conditions, these districts and counties were divided into

three regions: Core region (Yuexiu, Liwan, Tianhe, and Haizhu), Suburb region

(Panyu, Huangpu, Baiyun, and Luogang), and Outer suburb region (Nansha, Huadu,

Cunhua and Zengcheng) (Figure 3-3)

80.0
70.0
60.0
50.0
40.0
30.0
20.0
10.0
0.0
2009 2010
Guangzhou 65.0 71.7
Guangdong 50.9 55.2
National 28.9 34.3

Figure 3-2 Internet penetration rate in Guangzhou, Guangdong and national level

Guangdong

Guangzhou

Figure 3-3 Location of the study site

55
3.3 A summary of rationale to traget junior middle school students

We targeted junior middle school students as our study population based on the

following considerations: (1) Adolescence represents the second fast growth spurt

after infancy [154]. Junior middle school students are an important subpopulation of

adolescents. As a key stage of early adolescence, the rapid physical (e.g. early

puberty), psychological (e.g. concrete thinking but early moral concepts) and social

(e.g. emotional seperation from parents, start of strong peer identification; early

exploratory behaviours) developemtal changes produce specific disease patterns,

unusual presentations of symptoms, unique communication and management

challenges, and they gradually establish identity and autonomy in the context of the

socialcultural environment [1, 155]. During this period, adolescents are at high-risk of

suffering from many potential psychological and mental health problems. Moreover,

adolescence has been increasingly recognised as a life period that poses specific

challenges for treating disease and promoting health. (2) The high prevalence of

depression among adolescents has been reported in previous studies. The rates of

depression rise sharply after entering puberty with immediate and long term risks [4].

It has been observed that risk of depression is low in childhood but increasing

substantially with adolescence [5], and the meaningful risk begins in the early

teens [156]. (3) Online social networking use has received considerable popularity

among adolescents in recent years. A large-scale school-based survey among students

in middle and high schools reported that majority (81.5%) of students are dialy users

of online social networking [157], and proportion of students who reported daily online

social networking use of more than 2 hours significantly increased with students’

grade [158]. (4) A few of cross-sectional studies (see our literature review in chapter 2

56
in detail) have investigated the detrimental effects of online social networking use on

mental health among post-secondary (college and university) students [52, 53]. However,

little is known about the effects of online social networking use on mental health in

middle and high school students. Therefore, from the perspective of early prevention,

it is necessary and important to investigate the effects of online social networking use

on mental health (i.e. depression) among junior middle school students.

3.4 Phase I: Validation study

3.4.1 Participants and sampling

For the validation study in phase I, eligible participants were students from grade

seven to grade nine in public secondary schools in Guangzhou who were willing to

voluntarily participate in the survey. We excluded the migrant and private secondary

schools, considering the potential differences of cultural and familial environments

from public schools.

The survey was conducted in October of 2013, and a stratified cluster sampling

method was used to select participants. First, two areas were selected from

Guangzhou, including one urban and one rural country. Second, two public secondary

schools, one from the urban area and one from the rural area, were conveniently

selected. The students in the selected classes from the two schools were voluntarily

recruited to participate in the validation survey.

During this period, a total of 1088 students voluntarily completed the

questionnaire. As a measure of quality control, 73 cases with at least one scale having

more than 20% of its items being missing (SNAIS, emotional connection to social

networking, ‘SNA’, and IA) were excluded from data analysis. The remaining 1015

57
questionnaires had only 0.11% to 3.74% of item responses with missing values, which

were imputed by using the modal value of the other items of the same scale. Finally,

data from 910 (89.7%) of the 1015 students who were social networking users

(defined as those currently possessing at least one social networking account) were

used in the validation analysis.

3.4.2 Data collection procedure

Assisted by teachers of the selected schools and the Institute of Health Education

in Guangdong province, leaflets with the significance and procedures of the study

were approached to the selected school principals and students simultaneously several

days before the data collection. School consent and permission to administer the

in-school survey was approved by school principals prior to data collection.

An anonymous structured questionnaire was self-administered by the participants

in the absence of teachers in classroom settings, and supervised by a well-trained field

worker from our research team. Information on the study’s background and purpose

and confidentiality of the study was printed in the cover page of the questionnaire.

The nature of voluntary participation was also clearly announced by the field workers,

and students were informed that they have the right to terminate their participation in

the study at any time. Verbal informed consent was also obtained from participants

before they filled out the questionnaires. After distributing the questionnaires, field

workers stood by for enquiries and supervised the students to complete the

questionnaires. The questionnaires were checked for completeness and collected

immediately one-by-one by the field workers upon students’ submissions. All

questionnaires were sealed in prepared envelopes by our field workers in the presence

of students in the classroom, and names of the school and class, field workers, as well

58
as the date of data collection were wrote down on the envelopes as a means of quality

control. During the data collection period, field workers were also supervised by a

researcher in our team. Additionally, to gauge test-retest reliability, 114 students from

the urban secondary school were invited to complete the same questionnaire after two

weeks.

No incentive was given to students. The study was approved by the Survey and

Behavioral Research Ethics Committee of the Chinese University of Hong Kong. The

flowchart of phase I data collection is presented in Figure 3-4.

Eligible secondary schools

Informed consent from school principals

Two public schools selected


(one urban and one rural)

Students in the selected classes completed the


surveys
73 surveys
excluded
1015 valid surveys
Response rate=93.3%

910 online social networking users for


validation analysis

114 participants retested after 2 weeks

Figure 3-4 Flowchart of phase I data collection

59
3.4.3 Measurement

The questionnaire for the phase I validation study is included in Appendix I.

3.4.3.1 Development of online Social Networking Activity Intensity Scale (SNAIS)

A literature review on online activities and related measures on social networking

use was conducted [54, 55, 84, 114], and a pool of 38 items was extracted. A panel,

consisting of a behavioral scientist, two psychologists and an epidemiologist, held

several meetings, and eliminated some overlapping items while combined others with

similar meanings. The 14 items hence remained were written as questions: “How often

had you performed the following online social networking activities in the last month?”

The item response scale included six categories: 0 (never), 1 (few), 2 (occasional), 3

(sometimes), 4 (often) and 5 (always). The 14-item scale was then pilot tested among

85 students from a selected secondary school in the urban area (52.9% were grade

seven and 47.1% were grade eight students). A total of 77 students were identified as

social networking users. In the pilot study, the average item response rate was 98.3%

(ranging from 96.1% to 98.7%), and the item mean ranged from 1.1 to 3.8, suggesting

satisfactory acceptance and variance. All students had a sufficient understanding of

the items and found the questions not difficult to answer. Four additional students

answered an open-ended question on whether there were any other types of online

social networking activities unmentioned. Answers obtained included “chatting with

strangers”, “online shopping”, and “reading/searching materials”. These suggested

activities, however, overlapped with the current 14 items (table 6-1 in Chapter 6). In

order to further refine the existing items and response scale, individual interviews

were conducted among 20 students (10 from seventh grade and 10 from eighth grade)

by the researchers of our team to further test the feasibility and content of the scale.

60
The interviewees expressed that the 14 items captured the common functions and

activities of online social networking use and were suitable to the Chinese cultural

context. Based on their comments, we removed the item response category of

“occasional” to improve the discrimination of the response options. Thus a 5-point

scale was used with a higher score indicating higher intensity of social networking

use.

3.4.3.2 Other measures

3.4.3.2.1 Social-demographic variables

All participants were asked about their gender, grade, parental educational

attainment, and smartphone ownership.

3.4.3.2.2 Characteristics related to social networking use

Participants were asked whether they currently possess any social networking

accounts. Those providing an affirmative answer to the question were asked about

their experiences with social networking use, including the duration of social

networking use, number of days per week using social networking on average,

amount of time spent on social networking on a typical day, and number of online

social networking friends.

3.4.3.2.3 Emotional connection to social networking (EC)

Emotional connection to social networking was measured by six-attitudinal items

adapted from the Facebook Intensity Scale in the Ellison et al study [51]. The word

“Facebook” was replaced with “online social networking”. It was translated to

English and then translated back to Chinese. The original items were translated from

61
English into Chinese, and then translated back to English by two bilingual researchers.

Five-point Likert scales were used, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly

agree). Higher scores indicated higher levels of emotional connection to social

networking. A sample item was “Online social networking is part of my everyday

activity”. In our sample, an exploratory factor analysis revealed a one-factor solution

for this scale, explaining 60.0% of the total variance, and the factor loadings of the six

items ranged from 0.73 to 0.85. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was 0.87, and the

test-retest intra-class correlation coefficient was 0.78.

3.4.3.2.4 Social Networking Addiction (SNA)

‘SNA’ was measured by a modified version of the Facebook Addiction Scale [48],

which included eight items describing addictive symptoms (i.e. cognitive and

behavioral salience, conflict with other activities, euphoria, loss of control,

withdrawal, and relapse and reinstatement). Similarly, the word “Facebook” was

replaced with “online social networking”, and translation and back-translation

processes were implemented by two bilingual researchers. Response categories rated

from 1 (not true) to 5 (extremely true), thus the total score of the scale would range

from 8 to 40, with a higher score indicating a higher level of addictive tendency to

social networking. A sample item was “I have difficulties in focusing on my academic

work due to my social networking use”. In this study, an exploratory factor analysis of

this scale showed a one-factor solution that explained 51.2% of the total variance,

with high factor loadings ranging from 0.64 to 0.77. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient

was 0.86, and the test-retest intra-class correlation coefficient was 0.84.

62
3.4.3.2.5 Internet addiction (IA)

IA was measured by the 8-item Young’s Diagnostic Questionnaire (YDQ), which

involved “yes/no” response categories. Participants who provided five or more “yes”

answers were classified as cases of IA [19, 159]. The YDQ is easy to administer. The

scale has commonly been used in the Chinese student population and showed

acceptable validity and reliability [160, 161]. In our sample, the exploratory factor

analysis revealed a one-factor solution for this scale that explained 37.8% of the total

variance, and the factor loadings ranged from 0.46 to 0.69. The scale showed good

internal reliability (Cronbach’s α=0.73).

3.4.4 Statistical Analysis

Data were double-entered and the two datasets were compared using the EpiData

software (EpiData for Windows; The EpiData Association, Odense, Denmark). The

confirmatory factor analysis was conducted using LISREL 8.70, while other data

analyses were performed using Statistical Analysis System software (SAS 9.3 for

Windows; SAS Institute Inc., NC). Statistical significance was defined by a two-tailed

p value <0.05.

The detailed statistical methods used were described in the Chapter 6.

63
3.5 Phase II: A nine-month longitudinal study

3.5.1 Participants and Sampling

For the nine-month longitudinal study, participants were grade seven and grade

eight students from public junior middle schools in Guangzhou, China. We excluded:

(1) Grade nine students based on the consideration that they need to prepare for the

high school entrance examination and would leave their schools at the end of the

academic year. Therefore, there would be practical difficulties following-up with

these students later on; (2) Migrant and private junior middle schools due to potential

differences in cultural and familial environments; (3) those unwilling to participate in

the study. The participants enrolled in the longitudinal study differed from those in the

validation study in phase I.

A stratified cluster sampling method was applied to recruit participants.

1) One district (or country) was conveniently selected from three regions

(Core region, Suburb region and Outer suburb region), respectively.

2) Three junior middle schools were conveniently selected from each selected

district/county. In total, nine secondary schools were selected.

3) Students in the grades seven and eight in the selected schools were all

voluntarily recruited for baseline and subsequent follow-up surveys.

The baseline survey was conducted between March and April 2014, while the

follow-up survey was conducted during December 2014 to January 2015. After the

sampling procedures were applied, 5472 students completed the survey at baseline

(n=2645 in grade seven, 48.34%; n=2872 in grade eight, 51.66%), and 5527 students

64
were investigated at follow-up (Figure 3-5).

As a measure of quality control, we inspected the data for missing values. An

invalid response was defined as those with any scales having more than 25% missing

values. As a result of this process, 107 participants at baseline and 134 participants at

follow-up were excluded from the study. The response rates were 98.04% at baseline

and 97.58% at follow-up. For the remaining data of the 5365 participants at baseline

and 5393 participants at follow-up, the mean percentages of missing values per item

were 0.27% (range=0.00 to 1.19%) at baseline and 0.42% (range=0.00 to 3.15%) at

follow-up. The missing values were imputed by the mean values of the other items

from the same scale.

Among 5365 students investigated at baseline, 4951 participants (92.28%)

reported that they were social networking users, while 4905 out of 5393 (90.95%)

participants were social networking users at follow-up. After the matching process,

4871 participants were investigated at both baseline and follow-up. The 4237

participants who were social networking users at both baseline and follow-up

constituted the longitudinal sample (Figure 3-5).

65
Baseline Follow-up
Baseline Follow-up
5472 students were 5527 students were
investigated from 9 schools investigated from 9 schools
107 students were 134 students were
excluded due to excluded due to
incomplete data incomplete data

5365 data were valid 5393 data were valid

4951 students were 414 students did not use 4905 students were 487 students did not use
social networking users social networking social networking users social networking

4871 students were investigated


at both baseline and follow-up

4515 students were social 356 students were non-


networking users at baseline users at baseline

4237 students provided data 278 students who were users at 212 students who were 144 students who were
related to social networking at baseline did not provide data related non-users at baseline non-users at both
both baseline and follow-up to social networking at follow-up became users at follow-up baseline and follow-up

4237 in baseline follow-up


constituted longitudinal sample

Figure 3-5 Flowchart of participants in the longitudinal study

3.5.2 Data collection

The data collection procedures were similar to those used in the phase I study.

We collaborated with the Center for Health Promotion of primary and secondary

school in Guangzhou, which has a strong relationship with the secondary schools in

Guangzhou. As the collaboraters of our research, they provided strong support and

coordinations for school contact in the fieldwork. No incentives were provided to the

Center. School consent and permission for the in-school survey were approved by

school principals before the survey was administered. Information on the study’s

background and purpose and confidentiality of the study was printed on the cover

66
page of the questionnaire. The nature of voluntary participation was clearly mentioned

in an announcement and students were informed that they had a right to terminate

their participation in the study at any time. Verbal informed consent was also obtained

from participants before the survey.

Field workers were university students (upperclassmen in Year 4 and 5) majoring

public health at Sun Yat-sen University in Guangzhou. They all received

comprehensive training to conduct field work by the research team from the Chinese

University of Hong Kong (CUHK). After the training, they were also supervised in

the field by a researcher from CUHK during the data collection period.

The well-structured questionnaire used in the longitudinal study was designed by

a panel consisting of epidemiologists and psychologists. The questionnaire was pilot

tested among 46 junior middle school students in grade seven for the feasibility and

acceptability before conducting a baseline survey. In accordance with the feedback

from the pilot test, the questionnaire was then finalized by the panel for the baseline

survey and subsequent follow-up. Both surveys used similar questionnaires.

Since an anonymous approach was used to maximize protection of personal

information of the participants, students were asked to write down multiple codes on

the cover page of the questionnaire to assist with correctly matching the two surveyed

questionnaires completed by each student at baseline and follow-up. The codes

included the last four digits of participants’ home telephone number, last four digits of

both parental mobile phone numbers, last four digits of their identity card, participants’

date of birth, and the last letter of self and both parental spell-name. It was thought

that the use of such codes would make it difficult to identify the specific students, thus

maximizing the confidentiality and privacy of participants.

67
No incentives were given to students. As a feedback, a simple report of the

survey (e.g. including research background, methogology, characteristics of Internet

and online social networking use, prevalence of depression and IA) was provided to

the specific surveyed schools seperately. The study was approved by the Survey and

Behavioral Research Ethics Committee of the Chinese University of Hong Kong.

3.5.3 Measurements

The questionnaire for the longitudinal study is enclosed in Appendix II.

3.5.3.1 Background variables

All participants were asked about their gender, grade, both parental education

levels, family financial situation, living arrangement with parents, self-reported

academic performance and perceived study pressure at baseline.

3.5.3.2 Social networking-related scales

The measures for social networking use include online Social Networking

Activity Intensity Scale (two subscales: EFUI and EFUI), Emotional Connection to

social networking (EC) and ‘Social Networking Addiction (SNA)’ scale. These three

scales have been well-validated in the phase I study and were measured at both

baseline and follow-up. In the longitudinal sample, the three scales showed acceptable

reliability. For SNAIS and its two subscales (SFUI and EFUI), the Cronbach’s α were

0.87, 0.88, and 0.60 at baseline, and 0.89, 0.89, and 0.62 at follow-up respectively.

For EC, the Cronbach’s α were 0.88 at baseline and 0.89 at follow-up. For ‘SNA’, the

Cronbach’s α were 0.86 at baseline and 0.89 at follow-up. (Table 3-1)

Since there is no established cutoff value for the ‘SNA’ scale, participants scoring

68
in the top 10% of scale scores would be defined as addicted to social networking. This

classification strategy has been used in previous studies [162, 163].

3.5.3.3 Internet addiction (IA)

IA was measured using the 8-item Young’s Diagnostic Questionnaire (YDQ) at

both baseline and follow-up. The YDQ was detailed and descripted in the phase I

validation study. In the longitudinal sample, the Cronbach’s α were 0.66 at baseline

and 0.74 at follow-up, showing acceptable reliability (Table 3-1).

3.5.3.4 Psychosocial variables

3.5.3.4.1 Friendship quality

Friendship quality was measured using a peer-relationship subscale of Children

and Adolescent Quality of Life Scale [164], developed by Tong Ji Medical College of

Huazhong University of Science and Technology in China, and widely used in

Chinese adolescents[165]. The peer-relationship subscale includes 6 items with

four-point Likert scale, and a sample item is “Is your friend care for you?” The total

score ranges from 6 to 24, with a higher score indicating a higher quality of friendship.

In the longitudinal sample, the Cronbach’s α were 0.82 at baseline and 0.83 at

follow-up, showing good reliability.

3.5.3.4.2 Perceived social support

Perceived social support was measured by the 12-item Multidimensional Scale of

Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) [166]. The Chinese version of MSPSS revealed two

subscales: perceived family support (4 items) and perceived friend support (8

items) [167]. A sample item was “I get the emotional help and support I need from my

69
family”. Each item was rated on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1=very

strongly disagree to 7=very strongly agree, with higher scores indicating higher levels

of perceived social support. In the longitudinal sample, the Cronbach’s α were 0.93

for perceived friend support and 0.90 for perceived family support at baseline, while

they were 0.95 and 0.91 respectively at follow-up, showing good internal reliability.

3.5.3.4.3 Adolescent-parents conflict

Adolescent-parents conflict were assessed using the adolescent-parents conflict

questionnaire from the Fang XY et al study [168]. The questionnaire assessed seven

aspects of the adolescent-parents conflict, including academic, housework, spending

money, daily life arrangement, appearance, relationship between family members, and

individual privacy. For each kind of conflict, a detailed explanation was given to

respondents. Respondents were asked to rate the frequency of conflict with both father

and mother separately for each aspect of the adolescent-parents conflict in the

previous month. Each item was rated on a five-point Likert scale ranging from

1=never to 5=almost every day. Higher scores indicate higher frequency of conflict

with the father/mother. The adolescent-parents conflict questionnaire showed

acceptable reliability among Chinese adolescents [168, 169]. In our longitudinal study,

the Cronbach’s α were 0.84 for adolescent-father conflict and 0.85 for

adolescent-mother conflict at baseline, while they were 0.86 and 0.87 at follow-up

respectively.

3.5.3.4.4 Social non-confidence

The four-item social confidence subscale of the Social Skills Scale (SSS) was

used to measure level of the social non-confidence [170]. Psychometric properties of

70
the Chinese version of SSS have been previously evaluated among college

students [171]. A sample item on the social non-confidence subscale is “I am very

sensitive to criticism from others”, with a five-point Likert scale ranging from

1=definitely not like me to 5=exactly like me. Higher scores indicate higher social

non-confidence, meaning there is a great concern about what others think about

oneself as well as a strong likelihood of feeling nervous on social occasions. In the

longitudinal sample, the Cronbach’s α were 0.60 at baseline and 0.64 at follow-up.

3.5.3.3.5 Self-esteem

Self-esteem was measured using the 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale

(RSES) [172], which is a widely used self-reported instrument to measure global

self-worth by both positive and negative feelings about oneself. A sample item is “I

feel that I have a number of good qualities.” The RSES was unidimensional and all

items were answered using a four-point Likert scale ranging from 1=strongly agree to

4=strongly disagree. The Chinese version of RSES has been reported to have good

consistency reliability among adolescents [173]. In the longitudinal sample, the

Cronbach’s α were 0.79 at both baseline and follow-up, showing good consistency

reliability.

3.5.3.5 Depression

Depressive symptomatology was assessed using the Chinese version of the

20-item Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D). The CES-D

is one of the most commonly used self-report instruments for epidemiological studies

to screen depressive symptomatology. The CES-D was originally designed by Radloff

in 1977 to measure current level of depressive symptomatology in the general adult

71
population, with emphasis on the affective component, depressd mood [174]. In 1991,

Radloff applied the CES-D in high school and college students and supported its

acceptability and suitability in adolescents, including respondents as young as junior

high school [175]. The good psychometric properties (i.e. reliability and validity) of

CES-D among adolescents have also been reported in China (e.g. Hongkong, Taiwan

and Mainland) [176-179]. The CES-D comprises 16 negative affect and 4 positive affects

items, such as “I felt depressed,” and “I felt lonely”. Participants were asked about

rating the frequency of occurrence of the symptoms such as restless sleep and feeling

lonely during the previous week. All items were responded on a four-point Likert

scale ranging from 0=rarely or none of the time (less than 1 day) to 3=almost or all of

the time (5-7 days). The total score could range from 0 to 60, with a higher score

reflecting more depressive symptoms [174]. In the longitudinal sample, the Cronbach’s

α were 0.86 at baseline and 0.87 at follow-up, showing good internal reliability. The

respondents with scores equaling to or larger than 21 were defined as depressed [180].

3.5.4 Quality control

A research panel, consisting of one behavioral scientist, two psychologists, and

an epidemiologist, were formed for this study to conduct the study design, construct

the questionnaire, write up the Standard Operation Procedure (SOP) for data

collection, train the field workers, and to analyze the data. Moreover, a pilot study

among 46 grade seven students was conducted for testing the feasibility of the

questionnaire.

To ensure quality and integrity of the data collected in our study, quality control

was performed in all steps in the baseline and follow-up survey. All field workers

72
were recruited from senior university students and were well-trained so that they were

very familiar with the questionnaire and process of data collection.

3.5.5 Statistical strategies

The database was created with EpiData 3.0 software. All statistics are two-sided,

and the statistical significance was set as p≤0.05. The detailed statistical methods are

described in the subsequent chapters. SAS version 9.2 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC,

USA) and AMOS 20.0 were used for statistical analyses.

73
Table 3-1 Psychometric properties of the scales used in the longitudinal study
Number of Baseline Follow-up
items Cronbach’s α Cronbach’s α
Social networking variables
SFUI 10 0.88 0.89
EFUI 4 0.60 0.62
Emotional connection 6 0.88 0.89
‘SNA’ 8 0.86 0.89

Internet addiction (IA) 8 0.66 0.74

Psychosocial variables
Friendship quality 6 0.82 0.83
Perceived family support 4 0.90 0.91
Perceived friend support 8 0.93 0.95
Rosenberg self-esteem 10 0.79 0.79
Adolescent-father conflict 7 0.84 0.86
Adolescent-mother conflict 7 0.85 0.87
Social non-confidence 4 0.60 0.64

Mental health
CES-D 20 0.86 0.87
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity; SNA:
Social Networking Addiction scale; CES-D: Center for Epidemiological
Studies-Depression Scale.

74
Chapter 4 Description of the independent and dependent variables

in the longitudinal sample

4.1 Objectives

In this chapter, we describe the distributions of background characteristics of

participants obtained at baseline assessment and sample/subsamples involved in the

longitudinal analysis. Of particular importance are prevalence and incidence of

depression, ‘SNA’, and IA, and the remission rate from depression, ‘SNA’, and IA

during the follow-up period. Changes in the scale scores of independent variables

(including social networking related scales, psychosocial scales) from baseline to

follow-up in the longitudinal sample are also presented.

4.2 Statistical methods

Attrition analysis was performed by comparing the differences of background

characteristics at baseline between participants who remained in the follow-up and the

ones who were lost to follow-up by Chi-square test. The students who were social

networking users and were investigated both at baseline and follow-up constituted the

longitudinal sample (n=4237). The prevalence of depression, ‘SNA’, and IA were

calculated both at baseline and follow-up in the longitudinal sample. The differences

in the prevalence between genders were evaluated using the Chi-square test, while the

differences between baseline and follow-up were assessed by the McNemar test. The

differences of background variables between SNA and non-SNA cases (or IA vs.

non-IA cases) at baseline were estimated by univariate multilevel logistic regression

models, and ORu along with 95% confidence interval (95% CI) were derived. The

75
mean score differences of IA (or SNA) at baseline between four groups were assessed

by one-way ANOVA.

The age and gender-specific incidences were calculated by the number of events

divided by person-years of exposures; exposure for the event was estimated by half of

the duration between two assessments. Normal approximation for Poisson distribution

was used to calculate the 95% confidence interval (95% CI) of the point

incidence/remission estimates.

Continuous variables (including scores of social networking related scales, and

psychosocial scales) were described using the mean and standard deviation both at

baseline and follow-up in the longitudinal sample. The mean score differences of

social networking related scales and psychosocial scales between baseline and

follow-up were compared by paired t-test.

4.3 Results

4.3.1 Background characteristics of the entire sample at baseline assessment

A total of 5365 students were investigated at baseline assessment, 52.79% were

males, and 48.3% were from the seventh grade. Over 20% of the students reported

that their parents (either father or mother) achieved college education level or above.

The majority of students (87.83%) lived with both parents. About half of the students

(46.95%) perceived that their family financial situations were good/very good.

Additionally, more than one fifth of the students (21.47%) reported lower academic

performance, and 23.84% of students perceived heavy or even very heavy study

pressure (Table 4-1).

76
4.3.2 Attrition analysis for loss to follow-up

Around 9.2% of participants (494/5365) were lost to follow-up during the

nine-month longitudinal period. The results of the attrition analysis were presented in

Table 4-2. It showed that participants who were lost to follow-up were more likely to

be social networking non-users. In addition, students who were lost to follow-up were

more likely to be males, in their junior year of school, not living with both parents,

perceived poor/very poor family financial situation, and reported lower academic

performance (Table 4-2). Moreover, participants who were depressed at baseline were

more likely to be lost to follow-up compared with those who were non-depressed

(p=0.016, Table 4-2).

4.3.3 The longitudinal sample (n=4237)

The longitudinal sample was comprised of participants who were social

networking users (see chapter 3 for definition), and were surveyed both at baseline

and follow-up (n=4237). Six subsamples were also defined for subsequent data

analysis based on the status of depression, ‘SNA’, and IA, including subsamples Ia,

IIa, IIIa and Ib, IIb, IIIb (Figure 4-1).

77
Subsample Ia (n=3196), which consisted of
participants who were non-depressed at baseline

Subsample Ib (n=1041), which consisted of


participants who were depressed at baseline

Subsample IIa (n=3657), which consisted of


participants who were non-SNA at baseline
Longitudinal sample
(n=4237)
Subsample IIb (n=580), which consisted of
participants who were SNA at baseline

Subsample IIIa (n=3959), which consisted of


participants who were non-IA at baseline

Subsample IIIb (n=278), which consisted of


participants who were IA at baseline

Figure 4-1 Illustration of the subsamples

4.3.3.1 Background characteristics of the longitudinal sample and subsamples

The distributions of background characteristics in the longitudinal sample and

three subsamples (Ia, IIa, and IIIa) were presented in Table 4-3. In the entire

longitudinal sample (n=4237), about half of the students (49.68%) was males, 47.46%

of the students were in the seventh grade, and over 96% of students perceived medium

or above family financial situation. The majority of students (88.4%) lived with both

parents. In addition, over one fifth of students (20.11%) reported lower academic

performance, and about 23.44% of students perceived heavy or even very heavy study

pressure. The three subsamples showed similar distributions to the entire longitudinal

sample in terms of background characteristics (Table 4-3).

78
4.3.3.2 Comparison of background characteristics between those with ‘SNA’ and

those without ‘SNA’ at baseline

The table 4-4 presented the differences of background characteristices between

participants with SNA and without SNA at baseline. The unvariate analysis showed

that participants who were classified as ‘SNA’ were more likely to be those who

reported poor/very poor family financial situation (ORu=1.91, 95%CI: 1.21~3.01;

reference group: perceived good/very good family financial situation), those who

reported medium (ORu=1.81. 95%CI: 1.44~2.28) or lower academic performance

(ORu=3.39, 95%CI: 2.63~4.34; reference group: upper academic performance), and

those who perceived general (ORu=1.59, 95%CI: 1.20~2.11) or heavy/very heavy

study pressure (ORu=2.97, 95%CI: 2.21~3.99; reference group: perceived nil/light

study pressure). In addition, females were less likely to have ‘SNA’ as compared to

males (ORu=0.72, 95%CI: 0.60~0.86) (Table 4-4).

4.3.3.3 Comparison of background characteristics between those with IA and

those without IA at baseline

The results from univariate multilevel logistic regression analyses showed that

participants who were classified as IA cases at baseline were more likely to be those

who reported medium (ORu=1.60, 95%CI: 1.16~2.18) or lower academic

performance (ORu=2.70, 95%CI: 1.93~3.78; reference group: upper academic

performance), and those who perceived heavy/very heavy study pressure (ORu=2.92,

95%CI: 1.95~4.37; reference group: perceived nil/light study pressure) (Table 4-5).

Besides, females were less likely to have IA as compared to males (ORu=0.59, 95%CI:

0.46~0.76) (Table 4-5).

79
4.3.3.4 Discriminality between IA and ‘SNA’

The correlation between IA and SNA scores at baseline was less than 0.5 (α=0.49,

p<0.001), indicating a low positive correlation [181]. Moreover, the data showed that

only a small group of students (3.63%) were classified with both IA and ‘SNA’ at

baseline (Table 4-6).

The one-way ANOVA further revealed that students with both IA and “SNA”

have the highest mean IA score (F=1366.43, p<0.001; Table 4-6) and ‘SNA’ score

(F=1261.94, p<0.001; Table 4-6) at baseline. These findings indicated that IA and

‘SNA’were well discriminated by the YDQ and social networking addiction scale

respectively.

4.3.3.5 Depression, ‘SNA’, and IA prevalence in the longitudinal sample (n=4237)

4.3.3.5.1 Depression prevalence

The prevalence of depression was 24.57% at baseline, and it was significantly

higher in females than that in males (22.66% for males vs. 26.45% for females,

χ2=8.226, p=0.004). At follow-up, it was 26.55%, the gender difference became

non-significant (25.32% for males vs. 27.77% for females, χ2=3.251, p=0.071). The

prevalence of depression at follow-up was significantly higher than that at baseline

(paired χ2=7.459, p=0.006) (Table 4-7).

4.3.3.5.2 ‘SNA’ prevalence

The prevalence of ‘SNA’ was 13.69% at baseline, which was significantly higher

in males than that in females (15.77% for males vs. 11.63% for females, χ2=15.363,

p<0.001); while the prevalence at follow-up was 13.55%, the gender difference of

80
which was not statistically significant (14.25% for male vs. 12.85% for female,

χ2=1.773, p=0.183). Moreover, there was no significant difference for ‘SNA’

prevalence between baseline and follow-up (paired χ2=0.053, p=0.818) (Table 4-7).

4.3.3.5.3 IA prevalence

The prevalence of IA was 6.56% at baseline and 7.34% at follow-up, there was a

slight increase in IA prevalence during the follow-up period, but the difference was

non-significant (paired χ2=3.033, p=0.082). The prevalence was significantly higher in

males than that in females both at baseline and follow-up (8.17% vs. 4.97%,

χ2=17.682, p<0.001 at baseline, and 8.31% vs. 6.38%, χ2=5.828, p=0.016) (Table 4-7).

4.3.3.6 Depression, ‘SNA’, and IA incidence based on the subsamples Ia~IIIa

4.3.3.6.1 Depression incidence in the subsample Ia (n of subsample=3196)

In the longitudinal sample (n=4237), there were 3196 students who were

non-depressed at baseline (subsample Ia). Among these non-depressed cases at

baseline, there were 16.11% (515/3196) new incident depression cases during the

nine-month period, with 16.34% for males and 15.88% for females (χ2=0.125,

p=0.724).

The overall incidence of depression was 23.37 (95% CI: 21.43~25.48) per

100-person-years. The incidence among males was 23.72 (95% CI: 21.04~26.75) and

among females was 23.00 (95% CI: 20.32~26.04); However, gender difference was

non-significant. There was non-significant trend in age (Table 4-8).

4.3.3.6.2 ‘SNA’ incidence in the subsample IIa (n of subsample=3657)

Among participants who were non- ‘SNA’ at baseline (subsample IIa), around

81
9.16% (335/3657) of them transformed from non- ‘SNA’ to ‘SNA’ cases during the

follow-up period, with 9.42% for males and 8.92% for females (χ2=0.277, p=0.599).

The overall incidence of ‘SNA’ was 12.80 (95% CI: 11.50~14.25) per

100-person-years, with 13.18 (95% CI: 11.32~15.34) for males and 12.44 (95% CI:

10.70~14.48) for females. There were no significant differences in incidence in terms

of age and gender (Table 4-9).

4.3.3.6.3 IA incidence in the subsample IIIa (n of subsample=3959)

Among participants who were non-IA at baseline (subsample IIIa), about 4.95%

(196/3959) of them transformed from non-IA to IA cases during the follow-up period,

with 5.38% for males and 4.54% for females (χ2=1.481, p=0.224).

The overall incidence of IA was 6.77 (95% CI: 5.88~7.79) per 100-person-years,

with 7.37 (95% CI: 6.08~8.93) for males and 6.20 (95% CI: 5.05~7.60) for females.

The incidence of IA significantly decreased with age among females (from 8.23 in the

13 or younger age group to 4.08 in the 15 or older age group, χ2=5.15, p=0.023)

(Table 4-10). No gender difference was observed for incidence of IA.

4.3.3.7 Incidence of remission from depression, ‘SNA’, or IA based on the

subsamples Ib~IIIb

4.3.3.7.1 Remission from depression in subsample Ib (n of subsample=1041)

Among participants who were depressed at baseline (subsample Ib), around

41.40% of them were in remission from depression during the follow-up period. The

percentage of remitted cases in males and females were 44.03% and 39.18%

respectively (χ2=2.496, p=0.114).

82
The overall incidence of remission from depression was 69.61 (95% CI: 63.34,

76.51) per 100-person-years, with 75.27 (95% CI: 65.57, 86.17) per 100-person-years

in males and 64.98 (95% CI: 59.95, 74.13) per 100-person-years in females (Table

4-11). There were no significant differences for incidence of remission from

depression in terms of age and gender.

4.3.3.7.2 Remission from ‘SNA’ in subsample IIb (n of subsample=580)

Among participants who were ‘SNA’ at baseline (subsample IIb), around 58.79%

of ‘SNA’ cases spontaneously remitted during the follow-up period, with 59.94% for

males, and 57.26% for females (χ2=0.421, p=0.516).

The overall incidence of remission from ‘SNA’ was 111 (95% CI: 100, 123) per

100-person-years, with 114 (95% CI: 99,131) per 100-person-years in males and 107

(95% CI: 91,126) per 100-person-years in females (Table 4-12). There were no

significant differences for incidence of remission from ‘SNA’ in terms of age and

gender.

4.3.3.7.3 Remission from IA in subsample IIIb (n of subsample=278)

Among participants who were IA at baseline (subsample IIIb), about 58.63% of

IA cases remitted during the follow-up period, 58.72% in males and 58.49% in

females (χ2=0.001, p=0.970) (Table 4-13).

The overall incidence of remission from IA was 111(95% CI: 95, 129) per

100-person-years, with 111 (95% CI: 91, 135) per 100-person-years in males and 110

(95% CI: 86, 141) per 100-person-years in females (Table 4-13). There were no

significant differences for the incidence of remission from IA in terms of age and

83
gender.

4.3.3.8 Change in status of depression, ‘SNA’, and IA in subsample Ia~IIIa

4.3.3.8.1 Changes in status of ‘SNA’ and IA in subsample Ia (n=3196)

Among participants who were non-depressed at baseline (subsample Ia), the

frequency distribution of change in ‘SNA’ status was 84.29% for persistent non-

‘SNA’, 5.60% for remission from ‘SNA’, 2.97% for persistent ‘SNA’, and 7.13% for

incident ‘SNA’, while the frequency distribution of change in IA status was 94.87%

for persistent non-IA, 2.60% for remission from IA, 1.16% for persistent IA, and

3.38% for incident IA (Table 4-14).

4.3.3.8.2 Change in status of depression in subsample IIa (n=3657)

Among participants without ‘SNA’ at baseline (subsample IIa), the frequency

distribution of change in depression status over time was 67.27% for persistent

non-depression, 8.61% for remission from depression, 11.48% for persistent

depression, and 12.33% for incident depression (Table 4-14).

4.3.3.8.3 Change in status of depression in subsample IIIa (n=3959)

Among participants without IA at baseline (subsample IIIa), the frequency

distribution of change in depression status over time was 65.32% for persistent

non-depression, 9.47% for remission from depression, 12.83% for persistent

depression, and 12.38% for incident depression (Table 4-14).

84
4.3.3.9 Score changes of independent variables in the longitudinal sample

(n=4237)

4.3.3.9.1 Social networking-related scales

Except for the EC scale score, the scores of SFUI, EFUI, and ‘SNA’ had a slight

but significant decrease from baseline to follow-up (mean difference=-0.31 for SFUI,

paired t=-2.58, p=0.010; mean difference=-0.46 for EFUI, paired t=-8.86, p<0.001;

mean difference=-0.37 for ‘SNA’, paired t=-4.01, p<0.001) (Table 4-15).

Additionally, mean scores of SFUI and EC in females were significantly higher

than those in males (all p<0.001) (Table 4-12), whereas mean scores of EFUI were

larger in males than in females both at baseline and follow-up (p<0.001) (Table 4-15).

However, the gender differences for scores of ‘SNA’ were non-significant both at

baseline and follow-up.

4.3.3.9.2 Psychosocial scales

The descriptions of central tendency and variance for the scores of psychosocial

scales were presented in Table 4-16. The scores of friendship quality, perceived social

support, adolescent-father conflict, and social non-confidence slightly decreased from

baseline to follow-up (Table 4-16). However, there were no significant differences for

the scores of adolescent-mother conflict and self-esteem between baseline and

follow-up (Table 4-16).

4.4 Discussions

4.4.1 Internet addiction and online social networking among adolescents

IA in general has drawn increased attention in recent years. About 6.6% of IA

85
cases were identified at baseline in the present study. This finding is comparable to

previous studies in Mainland China (6.3%) [161] and Hong Kong (6.7%) [160] based on

the same assessment tool, but lower than those reported in south China (12.2%,

measured by 20-item Young-IAT) [182] and other countries (i.e. 17.6% by Young-IAT

in Qatar [183] and 21.2% by CIAS in the Philippines [184]) using different instruments.

A recent meta-analysis also estimated a similar global prevalence of IA (6.0%) [98]. As

such, in the absence of formal diagnostic criteria, diverse instruments, and cut-off

values used in the studies, prevalence estimates are highly tool-dependent and vary

widely worldwide. Caution should be taken when comparing the IA prevalence across

studies and different samples. Our results are consistent with several previous studies

that reported a male preponderance for IA [99, 161, 185]. It is likely that males are more

interested in games, pornography, and gambling activities that were associated with

IA[17] and have a lower level of self-control over Internet use [186].

It is worth noting that our longitudinal study observed a mild increase in IA

prevalence over a nine-month interval. It signifies that the IA condition is

continuously growing and is serious among this population, especially along with

increased availability of Internet access and portable devices (i.e. Wi-Fi access,

smartphone). A recent study reported that over 40% of Chinese adolescents had a

smartphone in 2014 [184]. It potentially increases the possibility of adolescents using

the Internet anywhere and anytime away from monitoring by their family or school.

Increased availability of Internet is associated with IA [17, 187]. Moreover, we observed

around 5.0% of new incident IA cases during the nine-month period, which was lower

than those reported from three one-year longitudinal studies conducted in Taiwan

(7.5%, 8.3%, and 15.8%, measured by CIAS) [38, 40, 41]. It appears that the potential

86
danger of IA among Chinese adolescents is increasingly serious. As a major public

health issue among adolescents, significant associations of IA with diverse psychiatric

and behavioral disorders (i.e. substance use, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder,

aggressive behaviors, social anxiety, and depression) have been documented in

previous studies [28, 188]. Therefore, more attention must be paid to this serious public

health issue, and effective IA prevention must be viewed as a necessity among

adolescents in China.

Online social networking as a new phenomenon has exponentially increased in

popularity among adolescents in the last decade. The majority of our sampled students

(92.3%) have their own social networking accounts. The extremely high popularity of

online social networking among adolescents has also been reported in the previous

studies, ranging from 85% to 94% [85, 189]. Adolescents are a subpopulation that are

disproportionately affected by online social networking [190]. Several studies have

indicated that social networking use is potentially associated with psychological

distress [50], lower self-esteem [36], loneliness [58, 111], and depression [35] among

adolescents. Online social networking has become the most popular form of online

activities globally. Health education should pay more attention to this increased

phenomenon.

Gender differences in social networking use should be emphasized. Our findings

revealed that females reported higher use intensity for social function of online social

networking, while males reported higher use intensity for entertainment function of

online social networking. These findings are consistent with previous studies,

reporting that males use the Internet mainly for entertainment and leisure purposes,

whereas females use it primarily for interpersonal communication [191, 192]. It suggests

87
that males and females use online social networking for different purposes and

reasons. These findings have a significant implication that strategies of health

education and intervention should be gender-specific.

‘Social networking addiction (SNA)’ in specific, similar to generalized IA, is

another public health concern that raises along with excessive engagement in such

Internet applications. We found a relatively lower prevalence of ‘SNA’ (13.7% at

baseline and 13.6% at follow-up) than previous data (24.0% and 34.3%) estimated by

modified Young’s Internet Addiction Test among Chinese college students [31, 32].

However, the prevalence is drastically higher than those reported in Nigerian (1.6%)

and Peruvian (8.6%) undergraduate students [29, 30]. One of the possible reasons is that

China has higher Internet penetration than these countries. Another possible reason

may be the lack of a universal instrument for ‘SNA’ assessment. Furthermore, around

9.2% of non-‘SNA’ participants converted into ‘SNA’ cases over the nine-month study

period. To our knowledge, this is the first longitudinal study documenting the

incidence of ‘SNA’ among early adolescents, specifically in Mainland China.

4.4.2 High prevalence and incidence of depression among adolescents

In the present longitudinal study, a high prevalence of depression was observed

at baseline (24.6%), which was considerably higher than that reported in the previous

study in Mainland China (14.8% measured by Children’s Depression Inventory) [193].

Moreover, it identified a significant increase in the prevalence of depression over a

nine-month interval, which has also been observed in a previous study [194]. This

signifies an increased threat of adolescents’ mental health problem.

Around 16.1% of non-depressed participants at baseline developed depression

88
during the follow-up period, which was almost double than that reported in a previous

nine-month longitudinal study conducted among high school students in Guangzhou

(8.4% new incident cases, measured by Zung Self-rating Depression Scale) [43], and

also higher than that in a seven-year longitudinal study in the United States (7.4% new

incident cases, measured by short version of CES-D). Depressive symptoms defined

by an epidemiological screening scale is a significant predictor of clinical depression

diagnosis [195] and suicidal ideation [196] among adolescents. Considering the high

prevalence, incidence, and serious consequences of depression among adolescents,

more attention should be paid to adolescents’ mental health (especially depression),

and effective depression intervention and prevention are highly warranted among this

population in Mainland China.

4.4.3 Natural remission from depression, ‘SNA’, and IA status

Another interesting finding in the present study is the large proportional

remission and recovery cases from depression, ‘SNA’, or IA status during the study

period without beforehand intervention, including 41.4% remission cases from

depression, 58.8% remission cases from ‘SNA’, and 58.6% remission cases from IA.

The remission rate from IA observed in this study are slightly higher than those

reported in previous longitudinal studies (one-year remission rate from IA: 49.5% and

51.5%, measured by CIAS) [40, 135]. As online social networking services are a very

recently developed phenomenon, the remission rate for ‘SNA’ has not yet been

reported. High recovery proportions from existing psychiatric and behavioral

disorders have several implications and concerns. Firstly, it suggests that depression,

‘SNA’, and IA status dynamically fluctuated over time rather than chance. The time

interval for follow-up should be cautiously defined in future longitudinal research, as

89
these conditions would be conversion between two assessment points. Prospective

studies involving a long-term period with little measurement waves may not reflect

the true status of depression, ‘SNA’, or IA. For example, although some participants

would be classified as non-IA both at baseline and follow-up, there is still a large

possibility that these participants may suffer from IA at some time in between baseline

and follow-up. Hence, in order to truly reflect the natural course of these negative

conditions, it is necessary to conduct more assessments within short time interval in

future longitudinal studies. Secondly, regression to the mean should be considered for

the observed remissions. Those with higher scores in depression, ‘SNA’, or IA scales

at baseline may have more likely tendency to move down in their scores at follow-up.

Special analysis strategies should be applied to adjust for regression to the mean [152].

Thirdly, it raises a concern whether these behavioral disorders are long-lasting or

short-term. Further investigation is warranted on the nature of the course of behavioral

disorders from a new perspective. Moreover, the status (i.e. depression, ‘SNA’, or IA)

observed in the initial assessment cannot be treated as an unchangeable condition over

time. It may be better to measure these predictors at each assessment instead of just

measuring at the initial assessment. The prospective studies and analysis strategies

should cautiously consider the change in such predictors during the course of the

study; for example, change in IA status: persistent non-IA, remission from IA,

persistent IA, and incident IA.

90
Table 4-1 Background characteristics of the participants of the entire sample at baseline (n=5365)
n %
Social-demographic variables
Gender
Male 2832 52.79
Female 2533 47.21
Grade
Seven 2592 48.31
Eight 2773 51.69
Father’s education level
Primary school or below 356 6.64
Junior secondary school 1816 33.85
Senior secondary school 1646 30.68
College or above 1317 24.55
Don't know 230 4.29
Mother’s education level
Primary school or below 588 10.96
Junior secondary school 1909 35.58
Senior secondary school 1497 27.90
College or above 1143 21.30
Don't know 228 4.25
Family financial situation
Very good/Good 2519 46.95
Average 2664 49.66
Poor /Very poor 182 3.39
Lives with both parents
Yes 4712 87.83
No 653 12.17

School-related variables
Academic performance
Upper 1817 33.87
Medium 2396 44.66
Lower 1152 21.47
Perceived study pressure
Nil/light 1034 19.27
General 3052 56.89
Heavy /Very heavy 1279 23.84

91
Table 4-2 Attrition analysis for loss to follow-up
Remained in study Lost to follow-up
p-value
(n=4871) (n=494)
Social-demographic variables
Gender
Male 51.71 63.36 <0.001
Female 48.29 36.64
Grade
Seven 47.85 52.83 0.035
Eight 52.15 47.17
Father’s education level
Primary school or below 6.69 6.07 0.975
Junior secondary school 33.75 34.82
Senior secondary school 30.69 30.57
College or above 24.55 24.49
Don't know 4.31 4.05
Mother’s education level
Primary school or below 10.92 11.34 0.950
Junior secondary school 35.72 34.21
Senior secondary school 27.92 27.73
College or above 21.19 22.47
Don't know 4.25 4.25
Family financial situation
Very good/Good 47.38 42.71 0.005
Average 49.46 51.62
Poor /Very poor 3.16 5.67
Lives with both parents
Yes 88.28 83.40 0.002
No 11.72 16.60

School-related variables
Academic performance
Upper 34.22 30.36 <0.001
Medium 45.12 40.08
Lower 20.65 29.55
Perceived study pressure
Nil/light 19.48 17.21 0.149
General 57.01 55.67
Heavy /Very heavy 23.51 27.13

Social networking users


Yes 92.69 88.26 <0.001
No 7.31 11.74

Depression at baseline
No 75.36 70.45 0.016
Yes 24.46 29.55
p values were obtained by Chi-square test.

92
Table 4-3 Background characteristics of participants among the longitudinal sample and the
subsamples
Longitudinal Subsample Ia~IIIa
sample Subsample Ia Subsample IIa Subsample IIIa
(n=4237) (n=3196) (n=3657) (n=3959)
n % n % n % n %
Social-demographic variables
Gender
Male 2105 49.68 1628 50.94 1773 48.48 1933 48.83
Female 2132 50.32 1568 49.06 1884 51.52 2026 51.17
Grade
Seven 2011 47.46 1549 48.47 1755 47.99 1887 47.66
Eight 2226 52.46 1647 51.53 1902 52.01 2072 52.34
Father’s education level
Primary school or below 273 6.44 186 5.82 226 6.18 250 6.31
Junior middle school 1425 33.63 1066 33.35 1217 33.28 1338 33.80
High middle school 1312 30.97 990 30.98 1141 31.20 1222 30.87
University or above 1053 24.85 817 25.56 922 25.21 985 24.88
Don't know 174 4.11 137 4.29 151 4.13 164 4.14
Mother’s education level
Primary school or below 445 10.50 302 9.45 370 10.12 413 10.43
Junior middle school 1507 35.57 1138 35.61 1304 35.66 1417 35.79
High middle school 1199 28.30 931 29.13 1040 28.44 1116 28.19
University or above 913 21.55 684 21.40 790 21.60 845 21.34
Don't know 123 4.08 141 4.41 153 4.18 168 4.24
Family financial situation
Very good/good 2047 48.31 1618 50.63 1795 49.08 1928 48.70
Medium 2072 48.60 1509 47.22 1771 48.43 1922 48.55
Poor/very poor 118 2.78 69 2.16 91 2.49 109 2.75
Lives with both parents
Yes 3747 88.44 2854 89.30 3238 88.54 3512 88.71
No 490 11.56 342 10.70 419 11.46 447 11.29

School-related variables
Academic performance
Upper 1465 34.58 1193 37.33 1347 36.83 1403 35.44
Medium 1920 45.32 1440 45.06 1653 45.20 1794 45.31
Lower 852 20.11 563 17.62 657 17.97 762 19.25
Perceived study pressure
Nil/light 811 19.14 698 21.84 745 20.37 778 19.65
Average 2433 57.42 1941 60.73 2128 58.19 2297 58.02
Heavy/very heavy 993 23.44 557 17.43 784 21.44 884 22.33
Subsample Ia was comprised of participants who were non-depressed at baseline.
Subsample IIa was comprised of participants who were non-SNA at baseline.
Subsample IIIa was comprised of participants who were non-IA at baseline.

93
Table 4-4 Comparision of background characteristics between those with SNA and those without
SNA at baseline (n=4237)
Non-SNA SNA
ORu (95% CI)
n % n %
Social-demographic variables
Gender
Male 1773 84.23 332 15.77 1
Female 1884 88.37 248 11.63 0.72(0.60, 0.86)***
Grade
Seven 1755 87.27 256 12.73 1
Eight 1902 85.44 324 14.56 1.16(0.97, 1.38)
Father’s education level
Primary school or below 226 82.78 47 17.22 1
Junior secondary school 1217 85.40 208 14.60 0.86(0.61, 1.23)
Senior secondary school 1141 86.97 171 13.03 0.76(0.53, 1.10)
College or above 922 87.56 131 12.44 0.78(0.53, 1.13)
Don't know 151 86.78 23 13.22 0.79(0.45, 1.36)
Mother’s education level
Primary school or below 370 83.15 75 16.85 1
Junior secondary school 1304 86.53 203 13.47 0.81(0.60, 1.09)
Senior secondary school 1040 86.74 159 13.26 0.81(0.59, 1.11)
College or above 790 86.53 123 13.47 0.87(0.63, 1.21)
Don't know 153 88.44 20 11.56 0.69(0.41, 1.19)
Family financial situation
Very good/Good 1795 87.69 252 12.31 1
Average 1771 85.47 301 14.53 1.12(0.93, 1.35)
Poor /Very poor 91 77.12 27 22.88 1.91(1.21, 3.01)**
Lives with both parents
Yes 3238 86.42 509 13.58 1
No 419 85.51 71 14.49 1.02(0.78, 1.34)

School-related variables
Academic performance
Upper 1347 91.95 118 8.05 1
Medium 1653 86.09 267 13.91 1.81(1.44, 2.28)***
Lower 657 77.11 195 22.89 3.39(2.63, 4.34)***
Perceived study pressure
Nil/light 745 91.86 66 8.14 1
General 2128 87.46 305 12.54 1.59(1.20, 2.11)**
Heavy /Very heavy 784 78.95 209 21.05 2.97(2.21, 3.99)***
SNA: Social Networking Addiction.
ORu: Univariate odds ratio, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression models.
* p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

94
Table 4-5 Comparision of background characteristics between those with IA and those without IA
at baseline (n=4237)
Non-IA IA
ORu (95% CI)
n % n %
Social-demographic variables
Gender
Male 1933 91.83 172 8.17 1
Female 2026 95.03 106 4.97 0.59(0.46, 0.76)***
Grade
Seven 1887 93.83 124 6.17 1
Eight 2072 93.08 154 6.92 1.13(0.88, 1.44)
Father’s education level
Primary school or below 250 91.58 23 8.42 1
Junior secondary school 1338 93.89 87 6.11 0.70(0.43, 1.13)
Senior secondary school 1222 93.14 90 6.86 0.77(0.47, 1.25)
College or above 985 93.54 68 6.46 0.73(0.44, 1.21)
Don't know 164 94.25 10 5.75 0.63(0.29, 1.36)
Mother’s education level
Primary school or below 413 92.81 32 7.19 1
Junior secondary school 1417 94.03 90 5.97 0.82(0.54, 1.24)
Senior secondary school 1116 93.08 83 6.92 0.93(0.60, 1.44)
College or above 845 92.55 68 7.45 1.02(0.65, 1.59)
Don't know 168 97.11 5 2.89 0.37(0.14, 0.96)*
Family financial situation
Very good/Good 1928 94.19 119 5.81 1
Average 1922 92.76 150 7.24 1.25(0.97, 1.60)
Poor /Very poor 109 92.37 9 7.63 2.19(0.64, 2.61)
Lives with both parents
Yes 3512 93.73 235 6.27 1
No 447 91.22 43 8.78 1.40(0.99, 1.97)†

School-related variables
Academic performance
Upper 1403 95.77 62 4.23 1
Medium 1794 93.44 126 6.56 1.60(1.16, 2.18)**
Lower 762 89.44 90 6.56 2.70(1.93, 3.78)***
Perceived study pressure
Nil/light 778 95.93 33 4.07 1
General 2297 94.41 136 5.59 1.41(0.95, 2.08)†
Heavy /Very heavy 884 89.02 109 10.98 2.92(1.95, 4.37)***
IA: Internet Addiction.
ORu: Univariate odds ratio, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression models.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

95
Table 4-6 Comparision for the mean differences of baseline IA and SNA scores between four groups (n=4237)
Groups IA SNA
n (%)
Baseline IA Baseline SNA Mean score F p Mean score F p
No No 3533(83.38) 1.16±1.17 1366.43 <0.001 15.27±4.34 1261.94 <0.001
No Yes 426(10.05) 2.11±1.34 26.04±2.96
Yes No 124(2.93) 5.46±0.75 18.23±4.40
Yes Yes 154(3.63) 6.03±1.14 28.96±4.39
IA: Internet Addiction; SNA: Social Networking Addiction.
p values were obtained by one-way ANOVA.

96
Table 4-7 Prevalence of depression, ‘SNA’, and IA at baseline and follow-up by gender in the
longitudinal sample (n=4237)
Total prevalence Gender
(%) Male Female p for gender
Depression
Baseline 24.57 22.66 26.45 0.004
Follow-up 26.55 25.32 27.77 0.071

‘SNA’
Baseline 13.69 15.77 11.63 <0.001
Follow-up 13.55 14.25 12.85 0.183

IA
Baseline 6.56 8.17 4.97 <0.001
Follow-up 7.34 8.31 6.38 0.016
SNA: Social Networking Addiction; IA: Internet Addiction.
p for gender were obtained by Chi-square test.

97
Table 4-8 Age- and gender-specific incidence of depression among participants who were non-depressed at baseline (n=3196)
% of Incident cases Incidence per 100-person-years (95% CI)
Subsample Ia
Age Male Female Total Male Female Total
(n=3196) $
(n=1628) (n=1568) (n=3196) (n=1628) (n=1568) (n=3196)
≤13 991 15.98 16.10 16.04 23.16(18.55, 28.92) 23.35(18.78, 29.03) 23.26(19.91, 27.17)
14 1582 16.98 15.88 16.43 24.74(20.90, 29.29) 23.00(19.31, 27.41) 23.88(21.14, 26.96)
≥15 623 15.36 15.47 15.41 22.19(16.95, 29.04) 22.35(16.58, 20.14) 22.26(18.22, 27.19)
Total 3196 16.34 15.88 16.11 23.72(21.04, 26.75) 23.00(20.31, 26.04) 23.37(21.43, 25.48)
$
Participants without depression at baseline.

98
Table 4-9 Age- and gender-specific incidence of ‘SNA’ among participants who were classified as non-‘SNA’ at baseline (n=3657)
% of Incident cases Incidence per 100-person-years (95% CI)
Subsample IIa
Age Male Female Total Male Female Total
(n=3657) $
(n=1773) (n=1884) (n=3657) (n=1773) (n=1884) (n=3657)
≤13 1129 8.16 10.51 9.39 11.35(8.44, 15.25) 14.79(11.53, 18.97) 13.14(10.86, 15.89)
14 1822 9.43 8.62 9.00 13.19(10.61, 16.41) 12.01(9.68, 14.89) 12.57(10.78, 14.65)
≥15 706 11.20 6.95 9.21 15.82(11.69, 21.41) 9.69(6.38, 14.44) 12.87(10.09, 16.41)
Total 3657 9.42 8.92 9.16 13.18(11.32, 15.34) 12.44(10.70, 14.48) 12.80(11.50, 14.25)
SNA: Social Networking Addiction.
$
Participants who were classified as non-SNA at baseline.

99
Table 4-10 Age- and gender-specific incidence of IA among participants who were classified as non-IA at baseline (n=3959)
% of Incident cases Incidence per 100-person-years (95% CI)
Subsample IIIa
Age Male Female Total Male Female Total
(n=3959) $
(n=1933) (n=2026) (n=3959) (n=1933) (n=2026) (n=3959)
≤13 1217 5.50 5.98 5.75 7.54(5.33, 10.66) 8.23(5.98, 11.30) 7.90(6.25, 9.98)
14 1970 5.08 4.19 4.62 6.96(5.24, 9.23) 5.71(4.23, 7.70) 6.30(5.13, 7.74)
≥15 772 5.90 3.01 4.53 8.10(5.43, 12.09) 4.08(2.26, 7.37) 6.19(4.44, 8.61)
Total 3959 5.38 4.54 4.95 7.37(6.08, 8.93) 6.20(5.05, 7.60) 6.77(5.88, 7.79)
IA: Internet Addiction.
$
Participants who were classified as non-IA at baseline.

100
Table 4-11 Age- and gender-specific remission from depression among participants who were depressed at baseline (n=1041)
% of remitted cases Remission per 100-person-years (95% CI)
Subsample Ib
Age Male Female Total Male Female Total
(n=1041) $
(n=477) (n=564) (n=1041) (n=477) (n=564) (n=1041)
≤13 307 45.58 38.13 41.69 78.71(61.95, 1.00) 62.81(48.87, 80.72) 70.23(89.06, 83.52)
14 516 45.98 41.44 43.41 79.61(65.63, 96.57) 69.69(58.32, 83.28) 73.93(64.85, 84.27)
≥15 218 37.74 24.82 36.24 62.02(45.49, 84.54) 56.22(41.07, 76.94) 59.01(47.33, 73.57)
Total 1041 44.03 39.18 41.40 75.27(65.57, 86.17) 64.98(56.95, 74.13) 69.61(63.34, 76.51)
$
Participants with depression at baseline.

101
Table 4-12 Age- and gender-specific remission from ‘SNA’ among participants who were classified as ‘SNA’ at baseline (n=580)
% of Incident cases Incidence per 100-person-years (95% CI)
Subsample IIb
Age Male Female Total Male Female Total
(n=580) $
(n=332) (n=248) (n=580) (n=332) (n=248) (n=580)
≤13 169 60.42 54.79 57.99 115(89, 149) 101(74, 137) 109(89, 133)
14 276 62.50 59.48 61.23 121(100, 147) 113(89, 143) 118(101, 137)
≥15 135 53.95 55.93 54.81 99(73, 134) 104(74, 146) 101(80, 126)
Total 580 59.94 57.26 58.79 114(99, 131) 107(91, 126) 111(100, 123)
SNA: Social Networking Addiction.
$
Participants who were classified as SNA at baseline.

102
Table 4-13 Age- and gender-specific remission from IA among participants who were classified as IA at baseline (n=248)
% of Incident cases Remission per 100-person-years (95% CI)
Subsample IIIb
Age Male Female Total Male Female Total
(n=278) $
(n=172) (n=106) (n=278) (n=172) (n=106) (n=278)
≤13 81 60.38 64.29 61.73 115(82, 163) 126(80, 200) 119(90, 157)
14 128 61.33 60.38 60.94 118(88, 157) 115(82, 163) 117(94, 146)
≥15 69 52.27 48.00 50.72 94(63, 142) 84(48, 148) 91(65, 126)
Total 278 58.72 58.49 58.63 111(91, 135) 110(86, 141) 111(95, 129)
IA: Internet Addiction.
$
Participants who were classified as IA at baseline.

103
Table 4-14 Frequency distribution of change in status of ‘SNA’, IA, and depression in subsample
Ia~IIIa
Subsample Ia Subsample IIa Subsample IIIa
(n=3196) (n=3657) (n=3959)
n (%) n (%) n (%)
Change in ‘SNA’ status
Persistent non-‘SNA’ 2694 (84.29) -- --
Remitted ‘SNA’ 179 (5.60)
Persistent ‘SNA’ 95(2.97)
Incident ‘SNA’ 228 (7.13)

Change in IA status
Persistent non-IA 2968(94.87) -- --
Remitted IA 83(2.60)
Persistent IA 37(1.16)
Incident IA 108(3.38)

Change in depression status


Persistent non-depression -- 2471(67.27) 2586(65.32)
Remitted depression 315(8.61) 375(9.47)
Persistent depression 420(11.48) 508(12.83)
Incident depression 451(12.33) 490(12.38)
SNA: Social Networking Addiction; IA: Internet Addiction.
Subsample Ia consisted of participants who were non-depressed at baseline.
Subsample IIa consisted of participants who were non-SNA at baseline.
Subsample IIIa consisted of participants who were non-IA at baseline.
--: Not applicable.

104
Table 4-15 Score changes of social networking related scales among longitudinal sample (n=4237)
Mean difference p for
Range Baseline Follow-up
(95% CI) time
SFUI
Male 0-40 16.91±8.52 16.82±8.80 -0.10(-0.47, 0.27) 0.608
Female 0-40 19.82±7.83 19.29±7.69 -0.53(-0.83, -0.23) <0.001
Total 0-40 18.38±8.31 18.06±8.35 -0.31(-0.55, -0.08) 0.010
p for gender <0.001 <0.001

EFUI
Male 0-16 8.53±3.37 8.05±3.46 -0.48(-0..64, -0.32) <0.001
Female 0-16 7.89±2.85 7.45±2.78 -0.45(-0.57, -0.32) <0.001
Total 0-16 8.21±3.14 7.75±3.15 -0.46(-0.56, -0.36) <0.001
p for gender <0.001 <0.001

EC
Male 6-30 14.77±5.55 14.83±5.67 0.06(-0.18, 0.31) 0.614
Female 6-30 15.74±5.25 15.94±5.15 0.20(-0.02, 0.41) 0.072
Total 6-30 15.26±5.42 15.39±5.44 0.13(-0.03, 0.29) 0.118
p for gender <0.001 <0.001

‘SNA’
Male 8-40 17.10±6.13 16.54±6.36 -0.56(-0.84, -0.28) <0.001
Female 8-40 16.78±5.49 16.60±5.83 -0.18(-0.42, 0.05) 0.124
Total 8-40 16.94±5.81 16.57±6.10 -0.37(-0.55, -0.19) <0.001
p for gender 0.073 0.765
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity; EC: Emotional
Connection to social networking; SNA: Social Networking Addiction.
Mean difference: Scores at follow-up minus corresponding scores at baseline.
95% CI: 95% confidence interval.
p for time was obtained by paired t test for mean difference between baseline and follow-up.
p for gender was obtained by two sample t-test.

105
Table 4-16 Score difference of interpersonal and psychosocial scales between baseline and
follow-up among longitudinal sample (n=4237)
Range Baseline Follow-up Mean difference (95%CI) p
Friendship quality
Male 6-24 18.91±3.51 18.79±3.62 -0.12(-0.27, 0.03) 0.105
Female 6-24 19.01±3.23 18.78±3.19 -0.22(-0.35, -0.10) <0.001
Total 6-24 18.96±3.37 18.79±3.41 -0.17(-0.27, -0.08) 0.001

Perceived family support


Male 4-28 20.28±5.51 19.72±5.48 -0.56(-0.80, -0.32) <0.001
Female 4-28 20.45±5.62 20.58±5.30 0.13(-0.08, 0.34) 0.230
Total 4-28 20.36±5.57 20.15±5.41 -0.21(-0.37, -0.05) 0.009

Perceived friend support


Male 8-56 40.76±10.24 39.58±10.71 -1.19(-1.65, -0.72) <0.001
Female 8-56 43.21±9.78 41.77±10.16 -1.44(-1.86, -1.02) <0.001
Total 8-56 42.00±10.08 40.68±10.49 -1.31(-1.63, -1.00) <0.001

Adolescent-father conflict
Male 7-35 13.58±6.13 13.17±6.15 -0.41(-0.73, -0.10) 0.010
Female 7-35 12.24±5.35 12.15±5.38 -0.09(-0.35, 0.17) 0.484
Total 7-35 12.91±5.79 12.66±5.80 -0.25(-0.45, -0.05) 0.016

Adolescent-mother conflict
Male 7-35 14.96±6.59 14.89±7.02 -0.07(-0.41, 0.28) 0.698
Female 7-35 13.94±5.92 14.30±6.35 0.36(0.06, 0.65) 0.018
Total 7-35 14.45±6.28 14.59±6.69 0.15(-0.08, 0.37) 0.207

Social non-confidence
Male 4-20 12.57±3.20 12.26±3.19 -0.31(-0.47, -0.15) <0.001
Female 4-20 13.04±2.97 12.90±2.92 -0.14(-0.27, -0.004) 0.044
Total 4-20 12.81±3.09 12.58±3.07 -0.22(-0.33, -0.12) <0.001

Rosenberg self-esteem
Male 10-40 27.83±4.38 27.77±4.43 -0.07(-0.25, 0.12) 0.482
Female 10-40 27.52±4.38 27.41±4.40 -0.10(-0.27, 0.06) 0.214
Total 10-40 27.67±4.38 27.59±4.42 -0.08(-0.21, 0.04) 0.175
Mean difference: scores at follow-up minus corresponding scores at baseline.
95% CI: 95% confidence interval.
p values were obtained by paired t test for mean difference between baseline and follow-up.

106
Chapter 5 Directionality for the causal relationships between

Internet/social networking addiction and depressive symptoms

5.1 Background

5.1.1 Relationship between IA and depression

Substantial cross-sectional studies have reported a positive association between

IA and depression among adolescents [197-201]. However, the results from these studies

are mixed [202, 203]. Due to limitations of the cross-sectional design, the causal

relationship between IA and depression could not be identified, although it is essential

to examine the causal relationship and magnitude effects for these two public health

issues. Our literature review found that only eight longitudinal studies investigated the

longitudinal relationship between IA and depression among adolescents. However, the

results are mixed. The causal direction between IA and depression remains inclusive.

There is a dearth of the research to test two causal directions in the same study.

Further studies on the direction(s) underneath the relationship between IA and

depression are therefore warranted. This chapter tackles such conceptual issues. It is

possible to hypothesize a bi-directional relationship existing for the relationship

between IA and depression.

5.1.2 Relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression

The aforementioned research questions on causality persist for the study of the

relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression, but are even less well studied as

compared to those between IA and depression. Online social networking could

provide adolescents with social convenience, social capital, selective self-disclosure,

107
and potential social support [51, 112]. Individuals who suffer from psychiatric disorders

(i.e. depressed, anxious) might view online social networking as safe and important

virtual communities where they can escape from emotional difficulty in the real

world [26, 97]. On the other hand, adolescents who have poorly adjusted to their

depression may suffer more detrimental effects from excessive social networking

use [204]. Therefore, a vicious circle between this behavioral addictive disorder and

psychopathology (i.e. depression) is also theoretically reasonable.

Our literature reviews did not find any longitudinal studies that explore the

causal relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression among adolescents and other

populations. However, due to limitations of the cross-sectional study design, it is

impossible to determine whether ‘SNA’ causes depressive psychopathology, or vice

versa. This chapter is also an attempt to fill up this knowledge gap.

5.1.3 Knowledge gaps

Conceptually, there are no longitudinal studies exploring the causal relationship

between ‘SNA’ and depression among adolescents. The existing longitudinal studies

reveal mixed results for the causal relationship between IA and depression.

Researchers continue to debate the causal directions of the relationships. Research

that theoretically tests bi-directional relationships between ‘SNA’/IA and depression

in a single longitudinal study is particularly scarce. Therefore, it is still unclear

whether ‘SNA’/IA are causes or outcomes of depression, and large-scale longitudinal

studies are warranted for clarifying such causal issues. Furthermore, most of the

studies that have been conducted on the psychological associations of IA fail to

differentiate between generalized IA and specific IA (i.e. ‘SNA’) simultaneously.

108
Moreover, methodologically, all of the current longitudinal studies involving

addictive behaviors and depression do not take into account the fluctuating change in

‘SNA’/IA status and/or depression status over time. Considering the dynamic changes

in ‘SNA’, IA, and depression status over time would provide more convincing insight

into the longitudinal nature of ‘SNA’, IA, and depression development. This study

therefore took into account change in ‘SNA’, IA or depression status over time (i.e.

remission) in the statistical analyses.

5.2 Objectives

This chapter aims to explore the causal directions between ‘SNA’/IA and

depression among adolescents. Specifically, for the causal relationship between ‘SNA’

and depression, two modeling approaches were used: Approach I: using baseline

‘SNA’ to predict incident depression among participants without depression at

baseline, and vice versa, using baseline depression to predict incident ‘SNA’ among

participants without ‘SNA’ at baseline; and Approach II: using change in SNA status

over time (persistent non-‘SNA’, remission from ‘SNA’, persistent ‘SNA’, and

incident ‘SNA’) to predict incident depression among participants without depression,

and vice versa, using change in depression status over time (persistent non-depression,

remission from depression, persistent depression, and incident depression) to predict

incident ‘SNA’ among participants without ‘SNA’ at baseline. These two types of

analyses were repeated for studying the causal relationship between IA and

depression.

The following hypotheses have been drawn:

• For the prediction of ‘SNA’/IA on incident depression

109
- Hypothesis 1: Baseline ‘SNA’ status would predict a higher probability of

incident depression during the follow-up period as compared to non-‘SNA’ cases

at baseline after adjusting for significant background variables and baseline

CES-D score among participants without depression at baseline.

- Hypothesis 2: Baseline IA status would predict a higher probability of incident

depression during the follow-up period as compared to non-IA cases at baseline

after adjusting for significant background variables and baseline CES-D score

among participants without depression at baseline.

- Hypothesis 3: Persistent ‘SNA’ and incident ‘SNA’ of change in ‘SNA’ status

from baseline to follow-up (defined as persistent non-‘SNA’, remission from

‘SNA’, persistent ‘SNA’, and incident ‘SNA’) would predict a higher probability

of incident depression as compared to persistent non-‘SNA’ cases after adjusting

for significant background variables and baseline CES-D score among

participants without depression at baseline.

- Hypothesis 4: Persistent IA and incident IA of change in IA status from baseline

to follow-up (defined as persistent non-IA, remission from IA, persistent IA, and

incident IA) would predict a higher probability of incident depression as

compared to persistent non-IA cases after adjusting for significant background

variables and baseline CES-D score among participants without depression at

baseline.

• For the prediction of depression on incident ‘SNA’/IA

- Hypothesis 5: Baseline depression status would predict a higher likelihood of

incident ‘SNA’ as compared to non-depressed cases at baseline after adjusting for

110
significant background variables and baseline ‘SNA’ score among participants

without ‘SNA’ at baseline.

- Hypothesis 6: Baseline depression status would predict a higher likelihood of

incident IA as compared to non-depressed cases at baseline after adjusting for

significant background variables and baseline IA score among participants

without IA at baseline.

- Hypothesis 7: Persistent depression and incident depression of change in

depression status over time (defined as persistent non-depression, remission from

depression, persistent depression, and incident depression) would predict incident

‘SNA’ as compared to persistent non-depressed cases after adjusting for

significant background variables and baseline ‘SNA’ score among participants

without ‘SNA’ at baseline.

- Hypothesis 8: Persistent depression and incident depression of change in

depression status over time (defined as persistent non-depression, remission from

depression, persistent depression, and incident depression) would predict incident

IA as compared to persistent non-depressed cases after adjusting for significant

background variables and baseline IA score among participants without IA at

baseline.

5.3 Statistical analysis

5.3.1 Subsamples

In this chapter, three subsamples were used for longitudinal data analyses

including: 1) subsample Ia (n of subsample=3196) which consisted of participants

111
who were non-depressed at baseline, 2) subsample IIa (n of subsample=3657) which

consisted of participants who were classified as non- ‘SNA’ at baseline, 3) subsample

IIIa (n of subsample=3959) which consisted of participants who were classified as

non-IA at baseline.

5.3.2 Modeling methods

Since the participants in the present study were selected using a stratified cluster

sampling method at the school level, two-level logistic regression models (level 1:

student, level 2: school) were fit (SAS GLIMMIX procedure) to investigate

longitudinal prediction effects by accounting for the clustering effect from the school

level, with the specification of random effects for the intercept in the regression

models. Ignoring the clustering effect would lead to an underestimation of standard

errors [205].

Background factors of incident depression, ‘SNA’ or IA (including gender, grade,

parental education level, family financial situation, living with both parents or not,

academic performance, and perceived study pressure) were selected by univariate

multilevel logistic regression analyses. Univariate odds ratios (ORu) with

corresponding 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) were derived. The significant

background factors hence found were adjusted for in the subsequent longitudinal

analyses.

5.3.2.1 Approach I: baseline status of measurement as a predictor

We firstly used baseline status of measurements to predict new incident

outcomes, including two models with incident depression as the dependent variable

and baseline ‘SNA’ or IA as predictors (see Figure 5-1a and Figure 5-2a), and two

112
models with incident ‘SNA’ or IA as dependent variables and baseline depression as

the predictor (see Figure 5-1b and Figure 5-2b). The model adjusted for significant

background variables (Model 1a), and the model adjusted for significant background

variables along with baseline scores of corresponding outcomes (Model 1b) were

constructed. A similar approach has been used in a previous study, which stated that

the difference in associations between models while controlling and not controlling

for baseline level of corresponding outcome measures enables identification of causal

pathways [206].

Univariate Model 1a Model 1b Univariate Model 1a Model 1b

Baseline Baseline Baseline Baseline Baseline Baseline


SNA SNA SNA depression depression depression
+ + + +
Background Background Background Background
variables variables variables variables
+ +
Baseline Baseline
CES-D score SNA score

(a) Baseline SNA to predict incident depression (b) Baseline depression to predict incident SNA

Figure 5-1 Causal direction between ‘SNA’ and depression using approach I

Univariate Model 1a Model 1b Univariate Model 1a Model 1b

Baseline Baseline Baseline


Baseline IA Baseline IA Baseline IA
depression depression depression
+ + + +
Background Background Background Background
variables variables variables variables
+ +
Baseline Baseline IA
CES-D score score

(a) Baseline IA to predict incident depression (b) Baseline depression to predict incident IA

Figure 5-2 Causal direction between IA and depression using approach I

113
5.3.2.2 Approach II: change in status from baseline to follow-up as a predictor

Changes in status of ‘SNA’/IA from baseline to follow-up were categorized as

persistent non-‘SNA’/IA (participants without ‘SNA’/IA both at baseline and

follow-up), remission from ‘SNA’/IA (participants with ‘SNA’/IA at baseline but

transitioning without ‘SNA’/IA at follow-up), persistent ‘SNA’/IA (participants with

‘SNA’/IA both at baseline and follow-up) and incident ‘SNA’/IA (participants

without ‘SNA’/IA at baseline but transitioning with ‘SNA’/IA at follow-up); Similarly,

change in depression status from baseline to follow-up also included four categories:

persistent non-depression, remission from depression, persistent depression and

incident depression, the definition is the same as those for ‘SNA’/IA.

Similarly, there were also two sets of prediction models: two models with change

in ‘SNA’ or IA status as the predictors and incident depression as the dependent

variable (Figure 5-3a and Figure 5-4a), and two models with change in depression

status as the predictor and incident ‘SNA’ or IA as the dependent variables (Figure

5-3b and Figure 5-4b). The two adjusted multilevel logistic regression models were fit:

one adjusted only for significant background variables (Model 2a) and the other

additionally adjusted for baseline scores of corresponding outcomes (Model 2b).

Univariate Model 2a Model 2b Univariate Model 2a Model 2b

Change in Change in Change in Change in Change in Change in


SNA status SNA status SNA status depression status depression status depression status
+ + + +
Background Background Background Background
variables variables variables variables
+ +
Baseline Baseline
CES-D score SNA score

(a) Change in SNA status to predict incident depression (b) Change in depression status to predict incident SNA

Figure 5-3 Causal direction between ‘SNA’ and depression using approach II

114
Univariate Model 2a Model 2b Univariate Model 2a Model 2b

Change in Change in Change in Change in Change in Change in


IA status IA status IA status depression status depression status depression status
+ + + +
Background Background Background Background
variables variables variables variables
+ +
Baseline Baseline IA
CES-D score score

(a) Change in IA status to predict incident depression (b) Change in depression status to predict incident IA

Figure 5-4 Causal direction between IA and depression using approach II

5.4 Results

5.4.1 Causal relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression

5.4.1.1 Background factors associated with incident depression for adjustment

The results from univariate multilevel logistic regression analyses showed that

three background variables significantly and positively predicted incident depression

during the follow-up period, including perceived average (ORu=1.32, 95% CI:

1.08~1.60, p=0.005) or poor/very poor family financial situation (ORu=1.98, 95% CI:

1.12~3.49, p=0.018; reference group: perceived good/very good family financial

situation), self-reported lower academic performance (ORu=1.66, 95% CI: 1.28~2.16,

p<0.001, reference group: upper academic performance), and perceived heavy/very

heavy study pressure (ORu=1.63, 95% CI: 1.20~2.20, p=0.002, reference group:

perceived nil/light study pressure) (Table 5-1).

5.4.1.2 Background factors associated with incident ‘SNA’ for adjustment

The results showed that participants who perceived poor/very poor family

115
financial situation (ORu=2.76, 95% CI: 1.60~4.76, p<0.001, reference group:

perceived good/very good family financial situation), those who did not live with both

parents (ORu=1.53, 95% CI: 1.12~2.09, p=0.005, reference group: living with both

parents), those who reported lower academic performance (ORu=1.61, 95% CI:

1.19~2.19, p=0.003, reference group: upper academic performance), and those who

perceived heavy/very heavy study pressure (ORu=1.65, 95% CI: 1.17~2.32, p=0.003,

reference group: nil/light study pressure) had a higher likelihood of suffering from

‘SNA’ during the follow-up period (Table 5-2).

5.4.1.3 Approach I: Using baseline ‘SNA’ to predict incident depression and using

baseline depression to predict incident ‘SNA’

Among participants who were non-depressed at baseline, results from univariate

analysis showed that baseline ‘SNA’ significantly predicted incident depression during

the follow-up period (ORu=1.65, 95% CI: 1.22~2.22, see Table 5-3). After adjusting

for significant background factors that included family financial situation, academic

performance and perceived study pressure, the predictive effect of baseline ‘SNA’ on

incident depression remained significant (AOR=1.48, 95% CI: 1.09~2.01, p=0.009;

see Model 1a in Table 5-3). However, when further adjusting for baseline CES-D

score on the basis of model 1a, this predictive effect become non-significant

(AOR=1.17, 95% CI: 0.86~1.60; see Model 1b in Table 5-3). Hypothesis 1 was hence

not supported by the results.

In reverse, in the univariate multilevel logistic analysis, participants with

depression at baseline were twice more as likely to develop ‘SNA’ during the

follow-up period compared to those without depression at baseline (ORu=2.02, 95%

CI: 1.58~2.58; see Table 5-4). After adjusting for significant background factors

116
(family financial situation, living with both parents or not, academic performance, and

perceived study pressure), the predictive effect of baseline depression on incident

‘SNA’ slightly attenuated but remained significant (AOR=1.78, 95% CI: 1.37~2.30,

p<0.001; see Model 1a in Table 5-4). This prediction was still statistically significant

even when further adjusting for baseline ‘SNA’ score in the model (AOR=1.47, 95%

CI: 1.13~1.92, p<0.001; see Model 1b in Table 5-4). Hypothesis 5 was therefore

supported by the current results.

5.4.1.4 Approach II: Using change in ‘SNA’ status over time to predict incident

depression and using change in depression status over time to predict incident

‘SNA’

A significant univariate prediction was observed between change in ‘SNA’ status

and incident depression during follow-up period (ORu=1.77, 95% CI: 1.21~2.58 for

remission from ‘SNA’; ORu=2.46, 95% CI: 1.54~3.93 for persistent ‘SNA’;

ORu=4.89, 95% CI: 3.67~6.52 for incident ‘SNA’; reference group: persistent

non-‘SNA’; see Table 5-5). After adjusting for significant background factors

(including family financial situation, academic performance, and perceived study

pressure), change in ‘SNA’ status still significantly predicted incident depression

(AOR=1.61, 95%CI: 1.10~2.36 for remission from ‘SNA’; AOR=2.23, 95%CI:

1.39~3.57 for persistent ‘SNA’; AOR=4.67, 95%CI: 3.49~6.24 for incident ‘SNA’,

reference group: persistent non- ‘SNA’; see Model 2a in Table 5-5). After additional

adjustment for baseline CES-D score on the basis of model 2a, this prediction

remained statistically significant (AOR=1.66, 95% CI: 1.02~2.70 for persistent ‘SNA’,

p=0.038; AOR=4.30, 95% CI: 3.17~5.82 for incident ‘SNA’, reference group:

persistent non- ‘SNA’; see Model 2b in Table 5-5). Therefore, hypothesis 3 was

117
supported by the adjusted results.

Reversely, univariate multilevel logistic regression showed that change in

depression over time significantly and positively predicted incident ‘SNA’ among

participants who were non- ‘SNA’ at baseline (ORu=4.62, 95% CI: 3.43~6.21 for

persistent depression; ORu=4.88, 95% CI: 3.67~6.50 for incident depression;

reference group: persistent non-depression; see Table 5-6). The predictive effect of

change in depression status over time on incident ‘SNA’ did not change after

adjustment for significant background factors and baseline ‘SNA’ score (AOR=3.40,

95% CI: 2.48~4.68 for persistent depression; AOR=4.47, 95% CI: 3.35~5.99 for

incident depression; reference group: persistent non-depression; see Model 2b in

Table 5-6), which supported hypothesis 7.

5.4.2 Causal relationship between IA and depression

5.4.2.1 Background factors associated with incident IA

Univariate multilevel logistic regression analyses found that only perceived

poor/very poor family financial situation (ORu=3.28, 95%CI: 1.82~5.91, p<0.001,

reference group: perceived good/very good family financial situation) was the

significant background risk factor of incident IA during the follow-up period (Table

5-7).

5.4.2.2 Approach I: Using baseline IA to predict incident depression and using

baseline depression to predict incident IA

Both univariate and adjusted multilevel logistic regression analyses (Model 1a

and Mode 1b in Table 5-8) showed that the prediction of baseline IA on incident

118
depression was not statistically significant. Hence, hypothesis 2 was not supported.

On the contrary, univariate multilevel logistic regression analysis showed that

baseline depression positively predicted incident IA during the follow-up period

(ORu=3.03, 95% CI: 2.26~4.06; see Table 5-9). The predictive effect of baseline

depression on incident IA remained significant after adjusting for significant

background variables (AOR=2.94, 95%CI: 2.19~3.96; see Model 1a in Table 5-9).

This predictive effect slightly attenuated, but was still significant when further

adjusting for baseline IA score (AOR=1.92, 95% CI: 1.41~2.62; see Model 1b in

Table 5-9). Hypothesis 6 was hence supported by the adjusted results.

5.4.2.3 Approach II: Using change in IA status over time to predict incident

depression and using change in depression status to predict incident IA

A significant univariate prediction was observed between change in IA status and

incident depression at follow-up (ORu=2.79, 95%CI: 1.39~5.60 for persistent IA;

ORu=4.62, 95%CI: 3.12~6.85 for incident IA; reference group: persistent non-IA; see

Table 5-10). This prediction remained significant after adjusting for significant

background variables, including family financial situation, academic performance and

perceived study pressure (AOR=2.61, 95%CI: 1.29~5.28 for persistent IA; AOR=4.59,

95%CI: 3.08~6.84 for incident IA, reference group: persistent non-IA; see Model 2a

in Table 5-10). However, when further adjusting for baseline CES-D score, only

incident IA significantly predicted incident depression (AOR=4.37, 95%CI: 2.87~6.67,

p<0.001, reference group: persistent non-IA; see Model 2b in Table 5-10). Hence,

hypothesis 4 was supported by the results.

Reversely, after adjusting for the significant background factor of family

119
financial situation, change in depression status significantly predicted incident IA

(AOR=2.53, 95% CI: 1.52~4.22 for remission depression; AOR=6.44, 95% CI:

4.47~9.28 for persistent depression; AOR=4.65, 95% CI: 3.14~6.89 for incident

depression; reference group: persistent non-depression; see Model 2a in Table 5-11).

After additional adjustment for baseline IA score on the basis of the model 2a, the

predictive effect of change in depression status on incident IA was still statistically

significant (AOR=3.91, 95% CI: 2.66~5.74 for persistent depression; AOR=4.10, 95%

CI: 2.73~6.17 for incident depression; reference group: persistent non-depression; see

Model 2b in Table 5-11). Therefore, hypothesis 8 was supported by the results.

5.5 Summary

5.5.1 Causal relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression

The results of the causal relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression by two

approach methods are summarized in Table 5-12. Based on the approach I, adjustment

for background variables and the baseline score of the corresponding outcome,

baseline ‘SNA’ did not predicted incident depression. Reversely baseline depression

significantly predicted incident ‘SNA’. The results indicated that the causal

relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression was unidirectional from earlier depression

to later ‘SNA’.

Results from approach II, adjusting for background variables and the baseline

score of the corresponding outcome, showed that change in ‘SNA’ status significantly

predicted incident depression, and reversely, change in depression status also

significantly predicted incident ‘SNA’ (Table 5-12). Specifically, for the prediction of

change in depression status on incident ‘SNA’, those who were persistently depressed

120
over time and those who became depressed during the follow-up period showed an

increased likelihood of developing ‘SNA’ compared with those who were free of

depression both at baseline and follow-up. On the other hand, for the prediction of

change in ‘SNA’ status on incident depression, persistent ‘SNA’ cases and new

incident ‘SNA’ cases showed an increased risk of developing depression compared

with persistent non- ‘SNA’ cases over time. Hence, a bi-directional relationship was

supported between change in ‘SNA’ and change in depression.

5.5.2 Causal relationship between IA and depression

The results for the causal relationship between IA and depression by two

approach methods are summarized in Table 5-13. After adjusting for background

variables and baseline score of the corresponding outcome, baseline depression

significantly predicted incident IA, but the predictive effect of baseline IA on incident

depression was not statistically significant. It is hence indicated a unidirectional

causal relationship from earlier depression to incident IA by modeling approach I.

When considering the change in status, the results from adjustment of

background variables and baseline score of the corresponding outcome showed that

change in IA status significantly predicted incident depression. Reversely, change in

depression status also significantly predicted incident IA (Table 5-13). Furthermore,

for the prediction of change in depression on incident IA, participants with persistent

depression and those with incident depression showed higher risk of developing IA

compared with those who were not depressed both at baseline and follow-up. In a

reverse trend, for the prediction of change in IA status on incident depression, incident

IA cases showed increased odds of developing depression when compared with those

without IA both at baseline and follow-up. Therefore, a bi-directional association was

121
supported between change in IA and change in depression.

5.6 Discussion

In this large-scale and school-based longitudinal study, we thoroughly examined

the directionality of the causal relationships between ‘SNA’/IA and depression using

two modeling approaches. In the first modeling approach, we used baseline ‘SNA’/IA

status to predict incident depression and vice versa, using baseline depression to

predict incident ‘SNA’/IA (approach I). In the second modeling approach, we used

change in ‘SNA’/IA status from baseline to follow-up (i.e. persistent non- ‘SNA’/IA,

remission from ‘SNA’/IA, persistent ‘SNA’/IA and incident ‘SNA’/IA) to predict

incident depression, and vice versa, using change in depression status (i.e. persistent

non-depression, remission from depression, persistent depression and incident

depression) to predict incident ‘SNA’/IA (approach II).

5.6.1 Depression as predictor of incident ‘SNA’/IA

Using the first approach, we found that baseline depression was a significant

predictor of incident ‘SNA’ (AOR=1.47) and incident IA (AOR=1.92) (Approach I).

These results are consistent with previous longitudinal studies among adolescents in

South Korea [44] and in Taiwan [39] that used the same statistical methods as our

approach I, which also reported that depressed participants are more likely of

developing IA. Consistently, the second approach showed that persistent depression

and incident depression over time significantly predicted incident ‘SNA’ (AOR=3.40

for persistent depression and AOR=4.47 for incident depression), and incident IA

(AOR=3.91 for persistent depression and AOR=4.10 for incident depression;

reference group: persistent non-depression). Therefore, change in depression status, as

122
compared to baseline depression as a predictor, was more predictive of incident ‘SNA’

and IA. There are no longitudinal studies that used this approach to predict new cases

of ‘SNA’/IA; thus no direct comparison can be made.

The two modeling approaches consistently revealed that depression is an

important determinant in the development of Internet addictive behaviors among

adolescents. These findings support the cognitive-behavioral theory [21], positing that

pre-existing psychopathology (i.e. depression, social anxiety, or substance dependence)

renders an individual vulnerable to Internet addictive behaviors. The

cognitive-behavioral theory proposes that maladaptive cognitions (i.e. rumination,

self-doubt, low self-efficacy, and negative self-appraisal) and dysfunctional behaviors

(i.e. using the Internet to escape from personal problems) are critical to the

development of Internet addictive behaviors. Depressed individuals always present

cognitive symptoms and usually possess positive expectancies for their Internet use

that the Internet could help them distracting from negative and personal problems (i.e.

depression, loneliness, etc.) [101, 207]. Online interaction (i.e. online social networking)

is attractive because of anonymity and absence of social cues (i.e. facial expression,

voice inflection, and eye contact), which make Internet-based interaction less

threatening in comparison to face-to-face communications [208, 209]. These features lead

depressed individuals to prefer for online social interactions as a more secure and less

threatening way and feel more comfortable in interaction online to escape from

emotional difficulty suffered in the real world, and the positive feelings and pleasures

from Internet-based interactions use cause excessive and addictive Internet/social

networking use.

123
5.6.2 ‘SNA’/IA as predictors of incident depression

Contrarily, the first approach did not support the prediction of baseline ‘SNA’

and IA onto incident depression, which is inconsistent with a previous longitudinal

study in China from Lam TL et al [43] that reported that participants with IA at

baseline were more likely to develop depression at follow-up. Such a difference may

be attributed to the fact that the results from Lam TL et al only controlled for the

background confounding variables, but the baseline level of depressive symptoms was

not controlled for even though the baseline level of depressive symptoms has a strong

association with future depression [210]. However, using the second modeling approach,

when change in ‘SNA’ and IA status over time (i.e. remission from ‘SNA’/IA at

baseline to non- ‘SNA’/IA at follow-up) were considered in the analysis, the results

showed a strongly positive prediction of persistent ‘SNA’/IA (AOR=1.66~1.88) and

incident ‘SNA’/IA (AOR=4.30~4.37) onto incident depression.

The difference in results obtained by approach I and II again suggests that the

latter approach is a potential methodological improvement, as it is more predictive in

both the case of depression predicting incident ‘SNA’/IA and the reverse.

Improvement may come from the fact that a high natural recovery rate from IA during

the follow-up period has been observed [40], and hence ignoring the remission effect

potentially underestimates the predictive effects of ‘SNA’/IA onto depression, such as

our non-significant findings for prediction of ‘SNA’ and IA at baseline on incident

depression. The modeling approach considering the dynamic change in ‘SNA’ and IA

status hence provides more convincing and robust estimation as such analysis

methodology rules out the potential effects from those recovery cases. One of the

explanations is that addictive Internet/social networking use would cause withdrawal

124
from interpersonal offline activities and displace the time that was originally spent

with family and peers in the real world, which then causes and increases mental health

problems (i.e. depression, loneliness) [61].

5.6.3 The bidirectional relationship

We hence have found unidirectional relationships from baseline depression to

incident ‘SNA’ and IA in approach I and bidirectional relationships between ‘SNA’/IA

and depression in approach II using change in status over time. The bi-directionality

suggests that a vicious circle between Internet addictive behaviors and depression

among adolescents may exist. It indicates that psychiatric individuals (i.e. depressed

individuals) are more likely to be prone to use the Internet and social networking

excessively to regulate their negative mood (i.e. alleviate negative emotions, anxiety

and personal problems) and prefer for online social interactions [45, 211-214]. These poor

cognitive and coping strategies accelerate the development and maintenance of

Internet addictive behaviors [207]. And as a feedback, increased addictive behaviors

further intensify and continue to produce negative outcomes (i.e. depressive mood),

producing diminished sense of self-worth and increased social withdrawal. As Internet

addictive behaviors worsen, they exacerbate the psychopathologies, creating a vicious

dysfunctional circle [17, 21]. Moreover, the strongest relationships were observed

between incident ‘SNA’/IA and incident depression in modeling approach II. This

suggests that the development of depression is highly likely to be accompanied by and

co-occurred with the development of Internet addictive behaviors.

5.6.4 Implications on prevention

The findings of this chapter entail several practical implications for designing

125
prevention and intervention programs. First, for the significant prediction of baseline

depression on incident Internet addictive behaviors, it is implied that adolescents with

depression are at high-risk group for increased probability of suffering from Internet

addictive behaviors later. Therefore, the prevention of Internet addictive behaviors

(‘SNA’ and IA) should be emphasized in students with depressive symptoms.

Intervention programs for Internet addictive behaviors should consider involving

specific skill training and components of reducing depressive mood (i.e. reducing the

maladaptive belief of positive outcome expectancies from the Internet, training social

skills, and planning leisure activities beyond the Internet use), which would

effectively prevent Internet addictive behaviors [215]. Second, epidemiological experts

and clinicians may suggest considering an assessment of depressive symptoms as a

marker of vulnerability of Internet addictive behaviors. Effective screening and

intervention for these psychiatric symptoms would be essential and effective for

preventing development of Internet addictive behaviors among adolescents. When

depression is screened, further interventions should be implemented to treat

depression. If depression is not well-treated, adolescents may spend more and more

time on the Internet to alleviate their depressive mood and then progress to addiction.

Therefore, interventions and preventions that target at high-risk group with identified

depressive symptoms would effectively prevent Internet addictive behaviors among

school adolescents.

For the strong prediction of change in ‘SNA’/IA status (i.e. persistent ‘SNA’/IA

and incident ‘SNA’/IA) on incident depression and the prediction of change in

depression status (i.e. persistent depression and incident depression) on incident

‘SNA’/IA, it is implied that Internet addictive behaviors are the concurrent predictors

126
of depression, and vice versa. Until now, no comprehensive therapeutic strategy for

Internet addictive behaviors has been established. Well-established therapy for

depression [216] could be considered as a prevention for Internet addictive behaviors

among adolescents. Accordingly, professionals working with adolescents who present

symptoms of Internet addiction also need to evaluate the potential presence of

depressive symptoms and prevent it simultaneously. Second, the vicious circle

relationship between depression and Internet addictive behaviors indicates a negative

reinforcement mechanism. Intervention campaigns should make efforts to block such

vicious circle.

5.6.5 Implications for future research

First, it should be cautioned that the status of Internet addictive behaviors and

depression might change during the longitudinal research period. As such, the time

interval should be carefully defined in the prospective study in order to truly reflect

the natural course of such pathological status. For example, although participants are

classified as IA both at baseline and follow-up, the IA status might be highly variable

between these two time points. Therefore, future longitudinal research is appropriate

to conduct the study with short-term interval assessments (e.g. three-month or

six-month interval). Additionally, statistical methodology should also consider such

status change in modeling specifications, such as using change in pathological status

over time rather than baseline status as predictors for mental health outcomes. Another

modeling method which could be alternatively considered in future similar research is

the cross-lagged panel model. However, this method has the limitation of controlling

diverse potential confounding factors in the model. Therefore, we did not apply this

analysis method in the present study as depression and internet addictive behaviors

127
are the consequences of multi-cause actions. Confounding factors are one of the

important issues that need to be carefully involved in the analysis. Second, ‘SNA’, one

subtypes of IA [17, 21, 217], showed a strong longitudinal relationship with depression in

the present study. This further highlights the necessity for investigating other specific

Internet addictive behaviors (e.g. gambling addiction) to explain the relationships

between Internet addictive behaviors and depression in future research. An increased

number of studies have emphasized the necessity for distinguishing specific Internet

addiction (e.g. social networking addiction) from generalized Internet addiction [22, 23].

Thirdly, the measurement and diagnostic criteria for ‘SNA’ is still in the way of its

development. It is necessary for future research to estimate an appropriate cut-off

value for the ‘SNA’ instrument with acceptable fitness (high sensitivity and specificity)

for community screening or diagnosis of ‘SNA’ among adolescents. Golden standard

criterion (e.g. systematic diagnostic interview by psychiatrists) is still needed for

cut-off value estimation of the ‘SNA’ instrument.

5.6.6 Strength and limitation

The main strength of the present study is the prospective study design with the

repeated measures of both ‘SNA’/IA and depression assessed by the same validated

survey instruments, as well as the large representative sample from the stratified

clustering sampling method that was used. Another major advantage is that two

directional relationships, including the longitudinal relationships from ‘SNA’/IA to

incident depression and the longitudinal relationships from depression to incident

‘SNA’/IA, were tested in the same study.

However, there are still some limitations that need to be emphasized in this

chapter specifically. Firstly, the two longitudinal directions between ‘SNA’/IA and

128
depression were tested separately using two sets of logistic regression models in the

present study. However, all models in the present study controlled for the baseline

level of corresponding outcomes and other potential confounding factors, the results

are therefore robust. Logistic regression using pathological status rather than

continuous scores could provide more meaningful explanations in epidemiological

research. Secondly, for the ‘social networking addiction’, we used the top 10% of the

scores to define social networking addictive status considering that there is no

available golden standard instrument and diagnostic criteria for ‘SNA’. The sensitivity

and specificity of such criterion for social networking addictive status in our study is

unclear and needs to be estimated in future research. However, the ‘SNA’ scale

showed acceptable psychometric properties in our study, and this strategy for

classification has been widely used in previous high-quality studies (e.g. using the top

10% of scores to create the group for psychological problems as no cut-off has been

established for South African child assessment schedule (SACAS) in Lancet

Psychiatry Journal). Thirdly, the generalizability of the study is limited to nonclinical

samples of school-based adolescents. Studies in diverse demographic populations (i.e.

psychiatric clinical populations) are warranted to further confirm such causal

relationships found in the present study. Other general limitations are mentioned in

Chapter 10.

In conclusion, the present study observed a vicious cycle between ‘SNA’/IA and

depression among adolescents, meaning that psychiatric disorder (i.e. depression)

significantly contributes to the course of social networking/Internet addictive

behaviors development, and in turn depressed individuals suffer more deleterious

effects from addictive Internet/social networking use. Future longitudinal studies with

129
three or more time points are warranted for further confirmation of these findings.

130
Table 5-1 Background factors as predictors of new incident depression among participants who
were non-depressed at baseline (n=3196)
Total Row %
ORu(95% CI)
(n=3196) (n=515)
Social-demographic variables
Gender
Male 50.9 16.3 1
Female 49.1 15.9 0.96(0.79,1.16)
Grade
Seven 48.5 16.1 1
Eight 51.5 16.1 1.00(0.83,1.21)
Father’s education level
Primary school or below 5.8 17.2 1
Secondary middle school 33.4 17.8 1.04(0.69,1.59)
High middle school 31.0 14.0 0.80(0.52,1.23)
University or above 25.5 15.8 0.92(0.60,1.42)
Don’t know 4.3 18.3 1.14(0.63,2.04)
Mother’s education level
Primary school or below 9.5 15.6 1
Secondary middle school 35.6 17.2 1.15(0.81,1.63)
High middle school 29.1 15.2 1.01(0.70,1.46)
University or above 21.4 15.4 1.03(0.70,1.52)
Don’t know 4.4 18.4 1.32(0.77,2.25)
Family financial situation
Very good/good 50.6 14.2 1
Average 47.2 17.8 1.32(1.08,1.60)**
Poor/very poor 2.2 24.6 1.98(1.12,3.49)*
Lives with both parents
Yes 89.3 15.8 1
No 10.7 18.7 1.25(0.93,1.67)

School-related variables
Academic performance
Upper 37.3 14.2 1
Medium 45.1 15.7 1.13(0.91,1.41)
Lower 17.6 21.3 1.66(1.28,2.16)***
Perceived study pressure
Nil/light 21.8 13.8 1
Average 60.7 15.7 1.16(0.90,1.48)
Heavy/very heavy 17.4 20.5 1.63(1.20,2.20)**
ORu: univariate odds ratios, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression models.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

131
Table 5-2 Background factors as predictors of new incident ‘SNA’ among participants who were
non-‘SNA’ at baseline (n=3657)
Total Row %
ORu (95% CI)
(n=3657) (n=335)
Social-demographic variables
Gender
Male 48.5 9.4 1
Female 51.5 8.9 0.94(0.75,1.17)
Grade
Seven 48.0 9.1 1
Eight 52.0 9.2 1.00(0.80,1.26)
Father’s education level
Primary school or below 6.2 11.5 1
Secondary middle school 33.3 9.5 0.81(0.52,1.28)
High middle school 31.2 8.2 0.67(0.42,1.07)
University or above 25.2 9.3 0.78(0.49,1.26)
Don’t know 4.1 9.3 0.79(0.40,1.59)
Mother’s education level
Primary school or below 10.1 8.4 1
Secondary middle school 35.7 9.1 1.11(0.73,1.69)
High middle school 28.4 10.5 1.28(0.84,1.96)
University or above 21.6 8.1 0.97(0.61,1.53)
Don’t know 4.2 8.5 1.03(0.52,2.03)
Family financial situation
Very good/good 49.1 8.1 1
Average 48.4 9.7 1.21(0.96,1.53)
Poor/very poor 2.5 19.8 2.76(1.60,4.76)***
Lives with both parents
Yes 88.5 8.7 1
No 11.5 12.9 1.53(1.12,2.09)**

School-related variables
Academic performance
Upper 36.8 8.1 1
Medium 45.2 8.8 1.10(0.85,1.42)
Lower 18.0 12.3 1.61(1.19,2.19)**
Perceived study pressure
Nil/light 20.4 7.9 1
Average 58.2 8.4 1.05(0.77,1.44)
Heavy/very heavy 21.4 12.5 1.65(1.17,2.32)**
SNA: Social Networking Addiction.
ORu: univariate odds ratios, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression models.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

132
Table 5-3 Predictive effect of baseline ‘SNA’ on incident depression among participants who were
non-depressed at baseline (n=3196)
Model 1a Model 1b
Row % ORu (95% CI)
AOR (95% CI) AOR (95% CI)
Baseline ‘SNA’
No 15.43 1 1 1
** *
Yes 23.36 1.65(1.22,2.22) 1.48(1.09,2.01) 1.17(0.86,1.60)
SNA: Social Networking Addiction.
ORu: univariate odds ratios, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression.
AOR: Adjusted odds ratios.
Model 1a was adjusted for family financial situation, academic performance, and perceived study
pressure.
Model 1b was additionally adjusted for baseline CES-D score on the basis of model 1a.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

Table 5-4 Predictive effect of baseline depression on incident ‘SNA’ among participants who were
non-‘SNA’ at baseline (n=3657)
Model 1a Model 1b
Row % ORu (95%CI)
AOR (95% CI) AOR (95% CI)
Baseline depression
No 7.80 1 1 1
*** ***
Yes 14.56 2.02(1.58,2.58) 1.78(1.37,2.30) 1.47(1.13,1.92)***
SNA: Social Networking Addiction.
ORu: univariate odds ratios, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression.
AOR: Adjusted odds ratios.
Model 1a was adjusted for family financial situation, living with both parents or not, academic
performance, and perceived study pressure.
Model 1b was additionally adjusted for baseline SNA score on the basis of model 1a.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

133
Table 5-5 Predictive effect of change in ‘SNA’ status over time on new incident depression among
participants who were non-depressed at baseline (n=3196)
Row Model 2a Model 2b
ORu (95% CI)
% AOR (95% CI) AOR (95% CI)
Change in ‘SNA’ status
Persistent non-‘SNA’ 13.14 1 1 1
** *
Remission from ‘SNA’ 21.23 1.77(1.21,2.58) 1.61(1.10,2.36) 1.30(0.87,1.92)
Persistent ‘SNA’ 27.37 2.46(1.54,3.93)*** 2.23(1.39,3.57)*** 1.66(1.02,2.70)*
Incident ‘SNA’ 42.54 4.89(3.67,6.52)*** 4.67(3.49,6.24)*** 4.30(3.17,5.82)***
SNA: Social Networking Addiction.
ORu: univariate odds ratios, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression.
AOR: Adjusted odds ratios.
Model 2a was adjusted for family financial situation, academic performance, and perceived study
pressure.
Model 2b was additionally adjusted for baseline CES-D score on the basis of model 2a.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

Table 5-6 Predictive effect of change in depression status over time on new incident ‘SNA’ among
participants who were non-SNA at baseline (n=3657)
Row Model 2a Model 2b
ORu (95%CI)
% AOR (95% CI) AOR (95% CI)
Change in depression status
Persistent non-depression 5.30 1 1 1
Remission from depression 6.67 1.28(0.80,2.07) 1.18(0.73, 1.92) 0.97(0.60,1.58)
Persistent depression 20.48 4.62(3.43,6.21)*** 4.12(3.02, 5.62)*** 3.40(2.48,4.68)***
Incident depression 21.51 4.88(3.67,6.50)*** 4.70(3.52, 6.27)*** 4.47(3.33,5.99)***
SNA: Social Networking Addiction.
ORu: univariate odds ratios, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression.
AOR: Adjusted odds ratios.
Model 2a was adjusted for family financial situation, living with both parents or not, academic
performance, and perceived study pressure.
Model 2b was additionally adjusted for baseline SNA score on the basis of model 2a.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

134
Table 5-7 Background factors as predictors of new incidence IA among participants who were
non-IA at baseline (n=3959)
Total Row %
ORu (95% CI)
(n=3959) (n=196)
Social-demographic variables
Gender
Male 48.83 5.38 1
Female 51.17 4.54 0.83(0.62,1.11)
Grade
Seven 47.66 5.30 1
Eight 52.34 4.63 0.87(0.65,1.16)
Father’s education level
Primary school or below 6.31 5.60 1
Secondary middle school 33.80 4.78 0.83(0.45,1.50)
High middle school 30.87 4.50 0.78(0.42,1.44)
University or above 24.88 5.69 0.98(0.53,1.82)
Don't know 4.14 4.27 0.74(0.29,1.89)
Mother’s education level
Primary school or below 10.43 5.81 1
Secondary middle school 35.79 4.45 0.75(0.47,1.23)
High middle school 28.19 4.66 0.81(0.49,1.34)
University or above 21.34 5.68 0.98(0.59,1.65)
Don't know 4.24 5.36 0.93(0.42,2.05)
Family financial situation
Very good/good 48.70 4.62 1
Average 48.55 4.79 1.03(0.76,1.39)
Poor/very poor 2.75 13.76 3.28(1.82,5.91)***
Lives with both parents
Yes 88.71 5.01 1
No 11.29 4.47 0.90(0.56,1.44)

School-related variables
Academic performance
Upper 35.44 4.21 1
Medium 45.31 5.24 1.29(0.92,1.81)
Lower 19.25 5.64 1.41(0.94,2.11)
Perceived study pressure
Nil/light 19.65 5.14 1
Average 58.02 4.22 0.81(0.56,1.19)
Heavy/very heavy 22.33 6.67 1.35(0.89,2.04)
IA: Internet Addiction.
ORu: univariate odds ratios, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression models.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

135
Table 5-8 predictive effect of baseline IA status on new incident depression among participants
who were non-depressed at baseline (n=3196)
Model 1a Model 1b
Row % ORu (95%CI)
AOR (95% CI) AOR (95% CI)
Baseline IA
No 15.93 1 1 1
Yes 20.83 1.42(0.90,2.24) 1.32(0.83,2.08) 1.05(0.65,1.68)
IA: Internet addiction.
ORu: univariate odds ratio, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression.
AOR: Adjusted odds ratio.
Model 1a was adjusted for family financial situation, academic performance and perceived study
pressure.
Model 1b was additionally adjusted for baseline CES-D score on the basis of model 1a.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

Table 5-9 predictive effect of baseline depression on incident IA among participants who were
non-IA at baseline (n=3959)
Model 1a Model 1b
Row % ORu (95% CI)
AOR (95% CI) AOR (95% CI)
Baseline depression
No 3.51 1 1 1
*** ***
Yes 9.97 3.03(2.26,4.06) 2.94(2.19,3.96) 1.92(1.41,2.62)***
IA: Internet Addiction.
ORu: Univariate odds ratio, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression.
AOR: Adjusted odds ratio.
Model 1a was adjusted for family financial situation.
Model 1b was additionally adjusted for baseline IA score on the basis of model 1a.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

136
Table 5-10 Predictive effect of change in IA status over time on incident depression among
participants who were non-depressed at baseline (n=3196)
Model 2a Model 2b
Row % ORu (95% CI)
AOR (95% CI) AOR (95% CI)
Changes in IA status
Persistent non-IA 14.86 1 1 1
Remission from IA 15.66 1.09(0.60,1.99) 1.00(0.55,1.83) 0.83(0.45,1.55)
Persistent IA 32.43 2.79(1.39,5.60)** 2.61(1.29,5.28)** 1.88(0.91,3.89)†
Incident IA 45.37 4.62(3.12,6.85)*** 4.59(3.08,6.84)*** 4.37(2.87,6.67)***
IA: Internet Addiction.
ORu: Univariate odds ratio, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression.
AOR: Adjusted odds ratio.
Model 2a was adjusted for family financial situation, academic performance, and perceived study
pressure.
Model 2b was additionally adjusted for baseline CES-D score on the basis of model 2a.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

Table 5-11 Predictive effect of change in depression over time on incident IA among participants
who were non-IA at baseline (n=3959)
Row Model 2a Model 2b
ORu (95% CI)
% AOR (95% CI) AOR (95% CI)
Changes in depression status
Persistent non-depression 2.28 1 1 1
*** ***
Remission from depression 5.60 2.53(1.52,4.22) 2.52(1.52,4.22) 1.66(0.98,2.81)†
Persistent depression 13.19 6.44(4.47,9.28)*** 6.26(4.32,9.06)*** 3.91(2.66,5.74)***
*** ***
Incident depression 10.00 4.65(3.14,6.89) 4.63(3.12,6.86) 4.10(2.73,6.17)***
IA: Internet Addiction.
ORu: univariate odds ratio, obtained by univariate multilevel logistic regression.
AOR: Adjusted odds ratio.
Model 2a was adjusted for family financial situation.
Model 2b was additionally adjusted for baseline IA score on the basis of model 2a.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

137
Table 5-12 Summary of the causal relationship between ‘SNA’ and depression
DV=Incident Depression DV=Incident ‘SNA’
Unadjusted for Adjusted for Unadjusted for Adjusted for
IV
baseline DV baseline DV baseline DV baseline DV
Approach I (Baseline)
‘SNA’ * NS -- --
Depression -- -- *** ***

Approach II (Change in status)


Change in ‘SNA’
Persistent non-‘SNA’ 1 1 -- --
Remitted ‘SNA’ * NS
Persistent ‘SNA’ *** *
Incident ‘SNA’ *** ***
Change in depression
Persistent non-depression -- -- 1 1
Remitted depression NS NS
Persistent depression *** ***
Incident depression *** ***
DV: Dependent variable; IV: Independent variable.
SNA: Social Networking Addiction.
NS: Non-significant.
--: Not applicable.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

138
Table 5-13 Summary of the causal direction between IA and depression
DV=Incident Depression DV=Incident IA
Unadjusted for Adjusted for Unadjusted for Adjusted for
IV
baseline DV baseline DV baseline DV baseline DV
Approach I (Baseline)
IA NS NS -- --
Depression -- -- *** ***

Approach II (change in status)


Change in IA
Persistent non-IA 1 1 -- --
Remitted IA NS NS
Persistent IA ** †
Incident IA *** ***
Change in depression
Persistent non-depression -- -- 1 1
Remitted depression *** †
Persistent depression *** ***
Incident depression *** ***
DV: Dependent variable; IV: Independent variable.
IA: Internet Addiction.
NS: Non-significant.
--: Not applicable.
† p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

139
Chapter 6 Validation of the Social Networking Activity Intensity

Scale

6.1 Background

In the Chapter 5, we found that ‘SNA’ and IA were predictive of depression

among adolescents. From the perspective of early prevention, it is important to

understand whether features of online social networking use, such as its use intensity

and online activities, are also predictive of depression. If it is affirmative,

interventions should efficiently target these features before problems turn into ‘SNA’

and/or IA. Some studies conducted in high school and university students have

reported that more time spent on social networking use was positively associated with

diverse negative outcomes such as depression [35] and internalizing problems [218]. In

contrast, some other studies found no significant associations between social

networking use and depression among college students [53, 108].

However, most of these studies measured online social networking use behavior

only by generic items (e.g. frequency of use per week, time spent on online social

networking in a typical day), these simple items often perform poorly and do not

adequately reflected full spectrum and complex activities related to online social

networking use. Moreover, these studies do not differentiate functions and purposes

that adolescents mainly conducted on these platforms. Some researchers have argued

that online communication use of the Internet could enhance individuals’ social

involvement and interaction with others, and then showed beneficial impacts on their

psychological well-being (i.e. reduced depression and loneliness, increased subjective

well-being), which is called the simulation hypothesis [219, 220]; while recreational use

140
of the Internet potentially influenced individuals’ psychological well-being negatively

(i.e. increased loneliness and depression) because of reduced social integration (i.e.

less time spent with friends, family and other in reality), which is consistent with the

displacement hypothesis [61]. Therefore, in order to gauge thorough impacts of online

social networking use on adolescents’ mental health outcomes, it is highly warranted

to have fully validated instruments to assess online social networking use intensity

(SNUI) by taking their functions and activities into consideration. It is a prerequisite

for studying health impact of online social networking use on mental health outcomes

among adolescents.

Several measures exist but they have serious limitations. Although some specific

scales assessed social networking use intensity [51, 81, 221] , they tend to focus only on

one single online social networking platform (especially Facebook). For instance, the

Facebook Intensity Scale focuses only on the attitudes towards Facebook while the

majority of adolescents use multiple types of social media [14]. Furthermore, the study

provided information on internal consistency without mentioning other psychometric

properties for this scale. Moreover, those measures failed to capture the intensity of

the full range of online social networking activities. For instance, a study from Yang et

al [84] only covered four specific types of Facebook activities (electronic interactions,

voyeurism, self-presentation, and gaming); questions on two of these four types of

activities only involved a single item. This is largely inadequate as adolescents usually

engage in multiple types of social networking activities [54, 84, 89, 113] (e.g. messaging

with friends, posting comments, status updating, etc.).

In summary, there is a lack of acceptable instruments to assess online social

networking use behavior. It is necessary to develop and validate a more

141
comprehensive instrument for sufficient assessment of online social networking use

intensity (SNUI), covering various types of online social networking activities and

functions without bounding into the specific platform. Our phase I study, as described

in this Chapter, thereby conducted the validation analyses for the self-developed

Social Networking Activity Intensity Scale (SNAIS) among adolescents in China.

6.2 Objectives

In the present study, SNUI was defined as self-reported frequency of using

multiple types of online social networking activities, and in multiple types of

platforms during the last month. It aimed to develop a new tool, the online Social

Networking Activity Intensity Scale (SNAIS), which can be used to assess online

social networking behavior intensity among Chinese secondary school students. We

examined its psychometric properties including internal consistency, test re-test

reliability and construct validity.

As a measure of concurrent validity, it was hypothesized that the SNAIS score

would be significantly correlated with external variables such as emotional connection

to social networking, social networking addiction, Internet addiction and general

characteristics related to social networking use (i.e. duration of social networking use,

number of days per week using social networking, time spent on social networking

per day and number of online social networking friends).

6.3 Sample and Statistical method

6.3.1 Sample used in this chapter

The sample for validation analysis in this chapter was a separate sample from the

142
longitudinal study. The sampling method and data collection procedures have been

described in Chapter 3.

6.3.2 Statistical method

Descriptive statistics (i.e. means, standard deviation, or percentage) were

presented when appropriate. Floor or ceiling effects were considered to be present if

more than 15% of respondents possessed the minimum or maximum score of the

SNAIS [222]. Internal consistency was assessed by Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, while

test-retest reliability was evaluated by Intra-class Correlation Coefficient (ICC).

Pearson correlation coefficients between the items and the overall scale, between the

items and their corresponding subscales, and between the items and the other

subscales of the SNAIS were calculated for the item analysis. Moreover, Pearson

correlation coefficients among SNAIS/subscales, emotional connection to social

networking, social networking addiction, and Internet addiction were derived.

Furthermore, relationships between the SNAIS/subscales and general characteristics

related to online social networking use were tested by both Spearman correlation

coefficients and one-way ANOVA to establish external validity.

To examine the factor structures of the SNAIS, the sample was randomly split

into two subsamples with equal sample size. In the first subsample, the factor

structure of the SNAIS was extracted by exploratory factor analysis (EFA), using

principal components extraction method and varimax rotation methods. In the second

subsample, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with maximum likelihood estimation

was conducted to cross-validate the factor structures which were suggested by the

EFA. The following goodness of fit statistics and cutoff criteria were used to evaluate

the factor structure model in the CFA [223-225]: χ2/df ratio<5.00, both Non-Normed Fit

143
Index (NNFI) and Comparative Fit Index (CFI) >0.95, and Root Mean Square Error

of Approximation (RMSEA) <0.08. The confirmatory factor analysis was conducted

by using Lisrel 8.70, while all other statistical analyses were performed by using SAS

version 9.2 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). The statistical significance level was

p<0.05.

6.4 Results

6.4.1 Social-demographic characteristics

Of 910 students, 44.4%, 40.2% and 15.4% were students of the seventh, eighth

and ninth grades respectively. Besides, around 59.2% were males and 64.1%

possessed a smartphone.

6.4.2 Factor structures

The EFA revealed two factors with eigenvalue larger than one. The first 10-item

factor explained 42.2% of the total variance, and was named the “Social Function Use

Intensity (SFUI) (via social networking platform)” subscale. The second 4-item factor

explained an additional 8.7% of the total variance, and was named the “Entertainment

Function Use Intensity (EFUI) (via social networking platform)” subscale. All factor

loadings exceeded 0.50 (Table 6-1), indicating that all items should be included in the

confirmatory factor analysis [226]. The results of the CFA confirmed the two-factor

solution, showing acceptable goodness of fit to the data: χ2/df=4.05 (Chi-square

=307.68, df=76, p<0.001), NNFI=0.96, CFI=0.96, RMSEA=0.082 (90%CI:

0.073~0.092). The standardized path estimates were above 0.48 (ranged from

0.48~0.76) (Table 6-1).

144
6.4.3 Ceiling and floor effects of SNAIS and its two subscales

The percentage of respondents reporting maximum scores of the SFUI, EFUI,

and SNAIS were 0.88%, 0.66%, and 0.55% respectively, while the percentage of

participants reporting minimum scores of the SFUI, EFUI, and SNAIS were 0.88%,

1.76% and 0.55% respectively. Therefore, no significant ceiling or floor effects were

observed for the SNAIS and its two subscales.

6.4.4 Item analysis

Except for Item 8 and Item 14, item means were close to the middle of the range.

The correlation coefficients between each item and the overall scale ranged from 0.43

to 0.72 (all p<0.001), while all correlation coefficients between the items and their

corresponding subscale ranged from 0.64 to 0.77 (all p<0.001). All correlation

coefficients between individual items and their respective subscale were higher than

those between the same items and the other subscale (Table 6-2).

6.4.5 Reliability

Internal reliability based on the entire sample was acceptable for the SNAIS

(Cronbach’s alpha=0.90, 0.60 and 0.89 for the SFUI, EFUI, and SNAIS respectively).

The test-retest intra-class correlation coefficients were 0.87, 0.67 and 0.85 for two

subscales and the overall scale respectively.

6.4.6 Correlation between SNAIS, EC, ‘SNA’, and IA

The scores of the SNAIS and its two subscales were positively correlated with

the scales of emotional connection to social networking and ‘SNA’. The SFUI showed

slightly stronger correlations with emotional connection (Pearson r=0.49, p<0.001)

145
and ‘SNA’ (Pearson r=0.32, p<0.001), as compared to those of the EFUI (Pearson

r=0.40 and 0.25, respectively, p<0.001). In contrast, the EFUI presented slightly

stronger correlations with IA (Pearson r=0.23, p<0.001) as compared to that of the

SFUI (Pearson r=0.18, p<0.001) (Table 6-3).

6.4.7 Relationship between SNAIS and participants’ characteristics of social

networking use

Duration of social networking use, number of days per week on average using

social networking, amount of time per day on average spent on social networking, and

number of social networking friends were all positively correlated with the SFUI

(Spearman r=0.26 to 0.38, all p<0.001), EFUI (Spearman r=0.27 to 0.30, all p<0.001),

and SNAIS (Spearman r= 0.30 to 0.40, all p<0.001).

The results of the one-way ANOVA analysis showed that those who reported

higher levels of duration of social networking use, number of days per week on

average using social networking, amount of time spent on social networking in a

typical day, and number of social networking friends scored significantly higher in the

SNAIS and its two subscales (all p<0.001, Table 6-4).

6.5 Discussion

In this chapter, we have developed and validated the Social Networking Activity

Intensity Scale (SNAIS) among secondary school students in China. To our

knowledge, this is the first study of developing a tool to assess online social

networking behavior intensity among adolescents based on diverse social networking

activities. The results showed that the SNAIS had satisfactory psychometric properties.

Considering the dramatic increasing prevalence and popularity of social networking

146
use among adolescents (90% prevalence in our sample), the development of the

SNAIS would facilitate the progression of related studies investigating the effects of

online social networking on psychosocial well-being among adolescents.

The developed SNAIS has some strength. It is a comprehensive inventory that

includes various social networking activities instead of basing on only one specific

platform (e.g. Facebook). We found high internal and test-retest reliability, good

content validity supported by the item analysis, and strong correlations with the scale

and criterion variables. Furthermore, there are no noticeable ceiling and floor effects.

Construct validity was also clearly established, as two subscales, namely the SFUI

and EFUI, were extracted by exploratory factor analysis and were cross-validated by

confirmatory factor analysis.

The two subscales that emerged from the SNAIS have practical implications.

Previous research has indicated that the effects of Internet use on psychosocial

well-being was partially dependent on the nature of Internet use [227]. A one-year

longitudinal study revealed that Internet use for communication purposes was

protective of depression, while the use for non-communicative purposes predicted

higher levels of depression and social anxiety [204]. The two constructs of the SNAIS

would hence facilitate future investigations of variations between effects of the two

main types of social networking use onto mental health outcomes among adolescents.

The EFUI subscale gave acceptable but relatively low internal (α=0.60) and

test-retested reliability (ICC=0.67), as compared to that of the SFUI subscale. Adding

additional items to the EFUI subscale may improve the scale’s reliability.

Furthermore, the specific entertainment functions included in the subscale (e.g.

information-seeking, games, shopping) did not necessarily correlate strongly with

147
each other. Further elaborations and refinements are warranted. We believe that the

EFUI subscale is still useful, considering its acceptable psychometric properties. We

suggest both subscales to be used together in the future research, as nowadays social

interactions and entertainments are not mutually exclusive. Furthermore, the results

showed that the EFUI had a slightly higher correlation with Internet addiction as

compared with that of the SFUI. Such an observation need to be confirmed by the

future research.

This study has a few limitations. First, participants were recruited from only two

secondary schools in Guangzhou, and the psychometric properties of SNAIS should

be further evaluated in a large representative sample in China, as Internet penetration

and pattern of use may vary across geographic regions in China. Besides, nature of

social networking use has been evolving rapidly; new activities and functions of

online social networking need to be considered in the future.

In summary, the present study developed and evaluated an instrument for

assessing extensive functions of social networking use intensity. It has satisfactory

psychometric properties and can easily to be self-administered by secondary school

students. Its application may catalyze the development of relevant research on the

impact of social networking use among adolescents in China and beyond.

148
Table 6-1 Factor loadings and path loading estimated by exploratory and confirmatory factor
analyses for the Social Networking Activity Intensity Scale (SNAIS)
How often had you performed the following online social Factor loading(EFA) Path
networking activities in the last month? SFUI EFUI estimate(CFA)
1 Sent messages to friends on message board 0.71 0.15 0.63
2 Chatted with friends via instant messaging function 0.54 0.38 0.67
3 Replied to comments made by social networking
0.70 0.21 0.74
friends
4 Commented on friends’ status, logs, and photos 0.80 0.08 0.76
5 Shared/Forwarded contents 0.72 0.12 0.73
6 Browsed others’ logs/photos/status/album 0.74 0.20 0.72
7 Updated self-status 0.66 0.40 0.70
8 Posted photos/videos on personal web profile 0.56 0.26 0.59
9 Wrote logs/weibo 0.66 0.29 0.65
Decorated personal web profile(changing
10 0.55 0.46 0.71
image/contact information/privacy setting)
11 Surfed entertainment/current news 0.20 0.61 0.49
12 Watched video/listened to music 0.34 0.53 0.63
13 Played games/applications -0.02 0.80 0.49
14 Bought/gave virtual Goods (e.g. birthday gifts) 0.24 0.57 0.48
Eigen value 5.91 1.22 --
Cumulative % of Variance explained 42.21 50.93 --
Note: SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity subscale; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity
subscale; EFA: Exploratory Factor Analysis; CFA: Confirmatory Factor Analysis.

149
Table 6-2 Item analysis of the SNAIS (n=910)
Cronbach's alpha if
Item-total Item-subscale Item -another
Item X ± SD item is deleted
correlation correlation subscale correlation
Subscale Total
1 1.34±1.02 0.89 0.88 0.66*** 0.69*** 0.34***
*** ***
2 2.24±1.11 0.89 0.88 0.68 0.68 0.45***
3 1.59±1.23 0.88 0.88 0.72*** 0.75*** 0.37***
*** ***
4 1.50±1.14 0.88 0.88 0.73 0.77 0.35***
5 1.76±1.25 0.89 0.88 0.70*** 0.74*** 0.36***
*** ***
6 1.87±1.15 0.88 0.88 0.72 0.75 0.40***
7 1.78±1.13 0.88 0.88 0.74*** 0.75*** 0.46***
*** ***
8 0.88±1.02 0.89 0.88 0.61 0.64 0.32***
9 1.29±1.19 0.89 0.88 0.70*** 0.71*** 0.40***
*** ***
10 1.61±1.09 0.89 0.88 0.72 0.72 0.46***
11 1.72±1.10 0.56 0.89 0.49*** 0.64*** 0.38***
*** ***
12 2.69±1.09 0.52 0.89 0.57 0.68 0.45***
13 2.22±1.25 0.49 0.89 0.43*** 0.74*** 0.26***
*** ***
14 0.87±1.07 0.56 0.89 0.51 0.64 0.39***
Note: Item-total correlation: Spearman correlation coefficient between each item and the Overall
scale. Item-subscale correlation: Spearman correlation coefficient between each item and its
corresponding subscale. Item-another subscale correlation: Spearman correlation coefficient
between each item and the other subscale.
***
p<0.001

150
Table 6-3 Correlations among SNAIS, emotional connection, social networking addiction and
Internet addiction (n=910)
SNAIS Emotional
‘SNA’ score
Overall SFUI EFUI connection
SNAIS --
SFUI 0.97*** --
***
EFUI 0.74 0.54*** --
Emotional connection 0.52*** 0.49*** 0.40*** --
*** *** ***
‘SNA’ score 0.34 0.32 0.25 0.56*** --
IA score 0.22*** 0.18*** 0.23*** 0.38*** 0.49***
SNAIS: Social Networking Activity Intensity Scale; SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity subscale;
EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity subscale; SNA: Social Networking Addiction; IA:
Internet Addiction.
***
p<0.001

151
Table 6-4 Mean score differences of SNAIS, SFUI, and EFUI by groups (n=910)
SNAIS SFUI EFUI
%
X ± SD p X ± SD p X ± SD p
Duration of social networking use
<3 months 19.1 17.8±9.6 11.6±7.7 6.2±2.9
<0.001 <0.001 <0.001
3-6 months 6.9 21.6±7.9 14.7±6.7 7.0±2.5
7-12 months 8.0 21.8±9.2 15.0±7.3 6.8±2.8
1-2 years 26.6 23.7±9.4 16.3±7.7 7.4±2.6
>2 years 39.3 26.4±10.3 18.0±8.4 8.4±3.3
Number of days/week
≤1 day 29.0 17.8±9.4 11.5±7.7 6.3±2.9
<0.001 <0.001
2-3 days 46.8 24.6±8.8 17.0±7.1 7.6±2.8 <0.001
4-5 days 10.7 25.0±9.2 17.2±7.7 7.8±2.8
≥6 days 13.5 29.6±11.4 22.2±9.3 9.4±3.3
Amount of time/day
<10 mins 20.0 17.3± 9.2 10.9±7.3 6.4±3.0
<0.001 <0.001 <0.001
11-30 mins 30.3 21.7± 8.2 14.7±6.8 7.0±2.6
31-60 mins 28.1 25.2± 9.2 17.5±7.4 7.7±2.9
>60 mins 21.6 28.9±11.3 20.0±9.0 9.0±3.3
Number of social networking friends
<50 45.9 19.5± 9.1 12.9±7.4 6.6±2.8
<0.001 <0.001 <0.001
51-100 25.3 23.9± 8.5 16.3±6.9 7.7±2.8
101-200 17.8 27.0± 9.6 18.7±7.9 8.3±2.8
201-400 7.3 31.3±10.0 21.8±8.1 9.5±3.3
>400 3.7 33.8±13.2 24.1±10.3 9.7±4.3
SNAIS: Social Networking Activity Intensity Scale; SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity;
EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity
X ± SD : Mean ± Standardized Deviation
p values were obtained by one-way ANOVA

152
Chapter 7 Intensity and emotional connection of social networking

use as predictors of incident depression

7.1 Background

We have validated the instrument to assess social networking behavior intensity,

and two behavioral patterns related to social networking activities (SFUI and EFUI)

are identified with acceptable psychometric properties as described in the Chapter 6,

which reflect two different but important functions involving online social networking

behaviors. Moreover, another dimension, such as emotional connection, is also

potentially important and should be considered. As mentioned before, the majority of

previous research operationalized online social networking use only through simple

behavioral items (i.e. frequency of use, number of friends, time spent on a typical day),

very few studies have involved psychological dimension related to online social

networking.

In the present study, we additionally involved the measure of emotional

connection as an index to reflect the psychological aspect related to online social

networking use. Emotional connection taps the degree to which a participant is

emotionally attached to online social networking and the extent to which online social

networking is integrated into his/her daily activities [36, 51, 111]. Previous surveys have

reported that adolescents mainly used social networking to establish and maintain

relationship. This dependency on online social networking as an alternative form for

communication and as an information-seeking channel may result in emotional

attachment to social networking use. Online social networking potentially changes the

traditional information sharing and relationship establishment environment, and may

153
transfer the traditional emotional ties between individuals to between individuals and

online social networking. From this perspective, it is important to assess the level of

emotional connection to social networking, which could provide additional insight for

clarifying the impacts of online social networking use on depressive symptoms among

adolescents. There is no study that has estimated the psychological impact of

emotional connection to online social networking. A knowledge gap hence exists.

Moreover, emotional connection has significant influence on social networking


[228]
use intensity . Participants who feel emotionally attached to social networking

would perceive social networking to be useful and possess positive feelings towards
[228]
the use of social networking . It implies that behavioral intensity and emotional

connection may interrelate with each other and ultimately affect mental health

outcomes (i.e. depression). Therefore, it is plausible to hypothesize that the

relationship between social networking behavior intensity and depression varies on

the level of an individual’s emotional connection to online social networking, such

that emotional connection might strengthen or weaken the impacts of social

networking use intensity on depression. The integrated effects of behavior intensity

and emotional connection to social networking on depression have not yet been

researched. We therefore objectively investigated the moderation effects of emotional

connection on the relationship between social networking use intensity and depression

in this chapter.

The current study is expected to fill these knowledge gaps. If the predictive

effects of social networking use on depression are affirmative in the longitudinal data,

further effective intervention programs targeting improvements in psychological

well-being should emphasize the controllability for social networking use intensity

154
among adolescents before such behaviors turn into addictive tendency.

7.2 Objectives

This chapter firstly aimed to explore the longitudinal predictive effects of

baseline SFUI, EFUI, and EC score on incident depression during the nine-month

period among those non-depressed participants at baseline.

It is hypothesized that:

1) Higher score in SFUI at baseline would predict higher likelihood of

developing depression during the follow-up period after adjusting for

significant background factors and baseline level of depressive symptoms.

2) Higher score in EFUI at baseline would predict higher likelihood of incident

depression during the follow-up period after adjusting for significant

background factors and baseline level of depressive symptoms.

3) Higher level of emotional connection towards social networking at baseline

would predict higher likelihood of incident depression during the follow-up

period after adjusting for significant background factors and baseline level of

depressive symptoms.

The second objective of this chapter was to test whether emotional connection

moderates the effects of social networking use intensity (SFUI and EFUI) at baseline

on incident depression. It is hypothesized that the predictive effects of SFUI and/or

EFUI on incident depression would be stronger among participants with higher level

of emotional connection to social networking compared to those with lower level of

emotional connection to social networking.

155
7.3 Statistical methods

The subsample Ia was used in this chapter, which consisted of participants who

were non-depressed at baseline (n of subsample=3196). The outcome variable was

incident depression during the follow-up period, while the independent variables were

SFUI, EFUI, and EC scores at baseline. The analysis methods were the same as those

used in the Chapter 5. The univariate predictive effects of baseline SFUI, EFUI, and

EC scores on incident depression were firstly assessed. And then, two sets of adjusted

models were constructed, one with adjusting for significant background factors and

one with additional adjustment for baseline level of depressive symptoms. The Odds

Ratios (ORs) and corresponding 95% CI were reported.

The moderation effects of EC on the prediction of SFUI and EFUI on incident

depression were tested by the models including the main effects (that a particular

independent variable and EC) and their interaction term (that EC times a particular

independent variable), and statistical significance of interaction terms were examined.

7.4 Results

7.4.1 Background factors associated with incident depression

As described in Table 5-1 in Chapter 5 that used the same subsample as used in

this Chapter, the results showed that participants with poor family financial situation,

those who reported lower academic performance, and those who perceived heavy

study pressure were more likely than others to develop new incidence of depression

during the nine-month follow-up period.

156
7.4.2 Prediction of baseline SFUI, EFUI, and EC score on incident depression

Univariate analyses showed that baseline SFUI, EFUI and EC scores all

significantly predicted increased odds of incident depression, with ORu=1.015 (95%

CI: 1.003~1.027) for SFUI, ORu= 1.050 (95% CI: 1.019~1.083) for EFUI and

ORu=1.021 (95% CI: 1.002~1.039) for EC respectively (Table 7-1).

When significant background factors (including family financial situation,

academic performance, and perceived study pressure) were adjusted for, the predictive

effect of baseline EC score on incident depression became non-significant

(AOR=1.018, 95%CI: 1.000~1.037, p=0.054; see Table 7-1), while the prediction of

baseline SFUI and EFUI scores on incident depression remained significant:

AOR=1.016 (95% CI: 1.004~1.028) for SFUI and AOR=1.047 (95% CI: 1.015~1.080)

for EFUI. The prediction of baseline SFUI and EFUI scores were still significant even

after further accounting for the effect of baseline level of depressive symptoms:

AOR=1.017 (95% CI: 1.004~1.029) for SFUI and AOR=1.046 (95% CI: 1.012~1.080)

for EFUI (Table 7-1).

7.4.3 Moderation effects of EC on the relationships between SFUI, EFUI and

incident depression

The interaction effects between baseline EC and baseline SFUI, and between

baseline EC and baseline EFUI on incident depression were shown in Table 7-2. None

of the interaction terms were significantly associated with incident depression (all

p>0.05 in Table 7-2). Therefore, the moderation hypothesis of baseline EC on the

relationship between SFUI, EFUI at baseline and incident depression was not

supported.

157
7.5 Discussion

The present study found that higher level of social networking use intensity (both

social function and entertainment function use) at baseline predicted increased odds of

incident depression among adolescents. However, the hypotheses about prediction of

emotional connection to social networking and its interaction with social networking

use intensity on depression are not supported by the results.

Social networking use can involve various functions; social function and

entertainment function use are two major ones. Regarding the prediction of

entertainment function use intensity on depression, similar findings were also reported

in a one-year longitudinal study among Dutch adolescents, showing that more time

per week spent on Internet surfing use significantly predicted a greater likelihood of

depression [204]. A similar association was also observed between weekly hours spent

on Internet use for non-communication (i.e. playing games, shopping) and depressive

symptoms among college students in a previous cross-sectional study [229]. The

finding that social function use intensity predicted increased odds of depression also

concurred with that of a previous two-wave longitudinal study conducted among

middle school students, which reported that high intensity of online communication

use (i.e. instant messaging use, measured by two five-point Likert items: how often

one uses such Internet function and the importance of such function of

communication for keeping in contact with friends and peers) positively predicted

depressive symptoms six months later [230].

This study is the first to involve the measure of emotional connection to social

networking as a predictor of depression. Contrary to our hypotheses, it found no

prediction of emotional connection to social networking and its interaction with social

158
networking use intensity on depression among adolescents. We only found one

cross-sectional study among college students that used the same measure as the one

used in our study and evaluated the association between emotional connection to

Facebook and well-being outcomes, which also found a non-significant correlation of

emotional connection with well-being outcomes (i.e. emotional adjustment and social

adjustment) [36]. Emotional connection to social networking measured in the present

study was modified from six attitudinal items in the Facebook Intensity scale [51]. This

emotional connection measure related to social networking use (e.g. Facebook) has

been used in previous studies [36, 231, 232], and a high Cronbach’s α is reported (e.g.

α=0.89). In our validation study, this measure revealed a unidimensional factor

solution which explained 60% of the total variance and showed acceptable validity

(Cronbach’s α=0.87, in Chapter 3). However, when further inspecting the items of this

measure, two items “Social networking is part of my everyday activity” and “Social

networking has become part of my daily routine” might mainly reflect the integration

of social networking use into one’s daily activities rather than the emotional

connection construct. One recent study deleted these two items from the emotional

connection to Facebook measure by exploratory factor analysis [233]. More studies are

needed to further investigate whether emotional connection to social networking truly

has no impact on depression or if the current non-significant findings may be

attributed to measurement features for emotional connection. Besides, refinement and

improvement for this measure might be necessary to precisely estimate the emotional

connection construct and its effect on mental health outcomes among adolescents.

Overall, the present study preliminarily explored the impacts of social

networking use on depression among adolescents. It is found that both types of social

159
networking use were predictive of depression among adolescents. The magnitude of

the prediction of EFUI on depression was comparable with that of SFUI on depression

(AOR=1.046 for EFUI score, AOR=1.017 for SFUI score). These findings implicate

that early intervention should be made to reduce both functions of social networking

use intensity as a means of preventing depression rather than waiting for social

networking addiction to occur, given that Internet addictive behaviors (IA and ‘SNA’)

strongly associated with incident depression as reported in Chapter 5.

However, there are still many research questions that need to be addressed for the

above prediction of social networking use intensity on depression in future research,

such as how and why higher social networking use intensity affects the level of

depression among adolescents. It is important to further understand the mechanisms

underlying such relationships (e.g. potential mediators), as this information would be

useful for improving intervention research. One of the plausible explanations is that

preoccupation in online social networking activities substitutes for the time that they

originally spent on offline social activities (e.g. with friends and family), and then

causes social withdrawal and decline in their social involvement and psychological

well-being (i.e. increased depression) [61]. Previous cross-sectional studies showed that

social networking use is associated with individuals’ psychosocial relationships (e.g.

friendship quality, perceived social support, self-esteem, and adolescent-parents

relationship) [58, 59, 234, 235]. Therefore, one set of potential underlying mechanisms

would be intervening/moderation effects related to aforementioned psychosocial

factors. A number of mediation/moderation hypotheses involving these psychosocial

factors (i.e. friendship quality, perceived social support, self-esteem, social

non-confidence and adolescent-parents conflict) are hence tested and the

160
corresponding results are presented in Chapter 8.

Another plausible explanation for the observed relationship between social

networking use intensity and depression might be that higher intensity of social

networking use would increase the addictive tendency to Internet/social networking

use, and such addictive use in turn causes adolescents’ mental health problems (e.g.

depression). The positive associations between social networking use intensity (e.g.

Facebook use for social purposes) and Internet addictive behaviors (IA or Facebook

addiction) have been reported in previous studies. It is therefore reasonable to explore

the potential mediation effects of Internet addictive behaviors (IA and ‘SNA’) on the

relationship between social networking use intensity and depression. The mediation

and magnitude of Internet/social networking addiction for the relationship are tested

in Chapter 9.

In conclusion, the present study is the first to investigate the effects of social

networking use intensity and emotional connection to social networking on depression

among adolescents. Social networking behavior intensity is a significant risk factor of

depression among adolescents. The potential underlying mechanisms for the

relationship between social networking use intensity and depression are investigated

in the next two chapters, including the mediation, suppression, and moderation effects

of psychosocial factors (Chapter 8), the mediation effects of IA and ‘SNA’, as well as

joint mediation effects of psychosocial factors and Internet addictive behaviors (‘SNA’

and IA) (Chapter 9).

161
Table 7-1 Predictive effects of baseline SFUI, EFUI, and EC scores on incident depression by
multilevel logistic regression (n=3196)
Univariate Adjusted a Adjusted b
OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)
* **
Baseline SFUI 1.015(1.003, 1.027) 1.016(1.004, 1.028) 1.017(1.004, 1.029)*
Baseline EFUI 1.050(1.019, 1.083)** 1.047(1.015, 1.080)** 1.046(1.012, 1.080)**
* †
Baseline EC 1.021(1.002, 1.039) 1.018(1.000, 1.037) 1.009(0.990, 1.029)
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity; EC: Emotional
Connection to social networking.
OR: Odds Ratios.
95% CI: 95% confidence interval.
a
models were adjusted by family financial situation, self-reported academic performance, and
perceived study pressure.
b
models were additionally adjusted by baseline CES-D score.
† p<0.10, * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001

162
Table 7-2 Testing the moderation effects of emotional connection on the relationships between SFUI, EFUI, and incident depression (n=3196)
Model 1a Model 1b Model 2a Model 2b
OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)
Social networking use measures
Baseline SFUI 1.017(1.003, 1.032)* 1.015(0.980, 1.052) -- --
Baseline EFUI -- -- 1.045(1.010, 1.082)* 1.041(0.949, 1.142)
Baseline EC 0.997(0.975, 1.018) 0.994(0.949, 1.042) 1.001(0.980, 1.021) 0.998(0.947, 1.052)

Interaction terms
Baseline SFUI × Baseline EC -- 1.000(0.998, 1.002) -- --
Baseline EFUI × Baseline EC -- -- -- 1.000(0.995, 1.006)
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity; EC: Emotional Connection to social networking.
All models were adjusted for family financial situation, academic performance, perceived study pressure, and baseline level of depressive symptoms.
OR: Odds Ratios.
95% CI: 95% confidence interval.
--: Not applicable
† p<0.10, * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001

163
Chapter 8 Mechanisms underlying the relationship between social

networking use intensity and depressive symptoms: the roles of

psychosocial factors

8.1 Background

In Chapter 7, we established that baseline social networking use intensity

(both SFUI and EFUI) are predictive of incident depression among adolescents. It is

important to further explore the potential underlying mechanisms whereby SFUI and

EFUI influence depression, as such information would be helpful for designing

intervention programs. Considering that adolescents use online social networking

mainly for social interaction and communication purpose [55, 236, 237], such as seeking

friends and social support, maintaining and establishing friendships, we contend that

SFUI and EFUI would significantly impact adolescents’ psychosocial status (e.g.

friendship quality, perceived social support, self-esteem, adolescent-parents

relationship, and social non-confidence), and these psychosocial factors would

partially explain (mediate or moderate) the effects of social networking use intensity

(both SFUI and EFUI) onto depression among adolescents.

There is a lack of longitudinal studies involving aforementioned psychosocial

factors to explain the effects of social networking use intensity on depressive

symptoms, and the roles of psychosocial factors on the relationship between social

networking use intensity and depressive symptoms are still under-researched and

unclear. This chapter attempts to fill these knowledge gaps.

164
8.2 Objectives

The first set of objectives in this chapter was to test the potential mediation and

suppression effects of psychosocial factors on the associations between change in

social networking use intensity (SFUI and EFUI) and change in depressive symptoms.

For this part, the mediation hypotheses and suppression hypotheses were tested (see

Table 8-1). To perform such mediation and suppression hypotheses testing, the

requirements of mediation and suppression effects were firstly investigated.

Specifically, we tested the significance of the associations (1) between changes in

SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D, (2) between changes in potential mediators (or

suppressors) and change in CES-D, and (3) between changes in SFUI/EFUI and

change in potential mediators (or suppressors). For the variables fulfilling all these

requirements, the multivariate linear regression models were further fit to test the

strength and significance of the corresponding mediation (or suppression) effects on

the associations between changes in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D.

The second set of objectives in this chapter was to test the moderation effects

of change in these psychosocial factors (i.e. change in friendship quality, change in

perceived social support, change in self-esteem, change in adolescent-parents conflict

and change in social non-confidence) on the relationships between changes in

SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D. For this part, the significance of interactive terms

was tested.

165
8.3 Part I: Testing mediation and suppression effects

8.3.1 Some examples of mediation and suppression effect for the relationships

between Internet use and mental health outcomes

There are some examples that investigated the mediating effects between Internet

use and psychological well-being, mainly from cross-sectional studies. For instance,

one study from Lee JS and Jeong B investigated a resilience model in the relationship

between worry, daily amount of Internet video game playing, daily sleep duration and

depression, and the results showed that the association between number of worries

and the severity of depression was partially mediated by daily amount of Internet

video game playing and daily sleep duration [238]. Nabi RL et al reported that the

number of Facebook friends was positively associated with perceived social support,

which in turn was associated with reduced stress and greater subjective well-being [60].

Another study in South Korean university students showed a full mediation effect of

social support for the association between self-disclosure on Facebook and life

satisfaction (self-disclosure means communication personal information, thoughts,

and feelings with others through social networking) [115]. A one-week experimental

research project revealed that the beneficial effect of induced increase in Facebook

status updating activity on reduced loneliness was fully mediated by the increased

feelings of social connectedness with friends [57].

In regards to the suppression effect, we found one cross-sectional study among

high school students in Belgium [116], and the results reported an opposite direct and

indirect effect, showing that active Facebook use was positively associated with

depressive mood. On the other hand, active Facebook use positively associated with

perceived online social support, and then increased perceived online social support

166
further negatively associated with depressive mood. Although the results were

consistent with the suppression hypothesis, the authors did not interpret the finding as

a suppression effect and suppression effect size was not reported.

Overall, there is a dearth of studies investigating such mediation and suppression

effects for the relationship between Internet use (especially social networking use) and

mental health outcomes among adolescents.

8.3.2 Mediation and suppression hypotheses for the associations between change

in social networking use intensity and change in depressive symptoms

In this section, we discussed the potential mediators and suppressors for the

association between social networking use intensity and depressive symptoms. In our

research context and based on Baron and Kenny’s statistical strategy, to be considered

a mediator, the following requirements must be fulfilled: (1) the independent variable

should be positively associated with the potential mediator, (2) the potential mediator

should be positively associated with the dependent variable; while as a suppressor, it

needs to fulfill the following requirements: (1) the independent variable should be

positively associated with the potential suppressor; (2) the potential suppressor should

be negatively associated with the dependent variable. We summarized literature on the

associations between the potential mediators/suppressors and depressive symptoms, as

well as the associations between social networking use and the potential

mediators/suppressors. Based on the literature search, a number of potential

suppressors (including friendship quality, perceived social support, and self-esteem)

and a number of potential mediators (including adolescent-parents conflict, and social

non-confidence) were considered, and corresponding mediation and suppression

hypotheses were hence tested in this study.

167
A summary for the literatures on requirements and the types of hypotheses to be

tested were presented in Table 8-1. Details were described in subsequent sections.

Table 8-1 Summary of the mediation/suppression hypotheses to be tested and the


rationales based on the literature
Requirements
Psychosocial Association between Association between Type of
factor social networking use potential hypothesis
to be tested and potential mediator/suppressor to be tested
mediator/suppressor and depression
Hs1 Friendship quality + - Suppression
Perceived family
Hs2 + - Suppression
support
Perceived friend
Hs3 + - Suppression
support

Adolescent-father
Hm1 + + Mediation
conflict
Adolescent-mother
Hm2 + + Mediation
conflict
Social
Hm3 + + Mediation
non-confidence
Hs4/ Suppression/
Self-esteem +/- -
Hm4 mediation
H: Hypothesis
“+” indicates a positive association between two variables
“-” indicates a negative association between two variables

8.3.2.1 Friendship quality as a potential suppressor for the relationship between

social networking use and depressive symptoms

(1) Association between friendship quality and depressive symptoms

Establishing and maintaining social relationships with friends and/or romantic

partners is one of the developmental tasks during adolescence. Strong relationships

with others can greatly reduce stress and improve mental health in general.

Substantive evidence has shown that greater friendship quality is protective of lower

depressive symptoms. For instance, it has been reported that depressed adolescents

had less optimal peer relationships and fewer friends compared to non-depressed

168
cases [239]. Another study among high school students in Australia also reported that

adolescents’ friendship significantly predicted depression [240].

(2) Association between social networking use and friendship quality

Adolescents mainly used online social networking to maintain and strengthen

existing offline connections. It has been reported that about half (49%) of participants’

top offline friends were also their top social networking friends in a small college

sample (n=131)[54], and around 43% of sampled high school students perceived that

online social networking use made them feel closer to their friends[59]. Generally,

social networking use appears to benefit adolescents’ sense of friendship. For instance,

self-disclosure on social networking platforms was positively related with friendship

quality [16], which is in line with the stimulation hypothesis [219], positing that online

social networking (i.e. online communication) facilitates social connectedness, and

then improve closer and higher-quality friendship among adolescents.

Based on the literature review, it is therefore hypothesized that:

Hypothesis s1 (Hs1): Change in friendship quality over time would suppress the

positive relationship between change in social networking use intensity (both change

in SFUI and EFUI) and change in depressive symptoms (Figure 8-1).

△Friendship △Friendship
quality quality
+ - + -

+ +
△SFUI △CES-D △EFUI △CES-D

(a) Suppression of △friendship quality (b) Suppression of △friendship quality


for the △SFUI-△CES-D link for the △EFUI-△CES-D link
Figure 8-1 Hypothesis illustration for suppression effect of change in friendship
quality on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D

169
8.3.2.2 Perceived social support as a potential suppressor for the relationship

between social networking use and depressive symptoms

(1) Association between perceived social support and depressive symptoms

It has been well-documented that perceived social support is protective factor of

lower depressive symptoms in previous studies. Adolescents with lower levels of

social support (family or peer) are at high risk compared to other adolescents for

experiencing increases in psychosomatic symptoms [241]. A longitudinal study among

adolescents in East London reported that lower level of social support were associated

with higher level of depression [242].

(2) Association between social networking use and perceived social support

Online social networking provides a new way for adolescents to obtain social

support [236]. Pew Internet and American Life Project reported that Facebook users had

higher level of perceived social support compared with non-Facebook users[243], and

additionally, the number of hours spent on Facebook and the number of Facebook

friends were also positively associated with greater perceived social support [60, 234].

However, some other studies found a non-significant association between social

networking use (i.e. Facebook intensity or Facebook interaction use) and perceived

social support [56, 244]. We did not find the study reporting a negative association

between social networking use and perceived social support.

In consideration of the literature discussed above, we hypothesized that:

Hypothesis s2 (Hs2): Change in perceived family support over time would

suppress the positive relationship between change in social networking use intensity

170
(both change in SFUI and EFUI) and change in depressive symptoms (Figure 8-2).

Hypothesis s3 (Hs3): Change in perceived friend support over time would

suppress the positive relationship between change in social networking use intensity

(both change in SFUI and EFUI) and change in depressive symptoms (Figure 8-3).

△Perceived △Perceived
family support family support
+ - + -

+ +
△SFUI △CES-D △EFUI △CES-D

(a) Suppression of △perceived family (b) Suppression of △perceived family


support for the △SFUI-△CES-D link support for the △EFUI-△CES-D link
Figure 8-2 Hypothesis illustration for the suppression of change in perceived
social support on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D

△Perceived △Perceived
friend support friend support
+ - + -

+
+ △EFUI △CES-D
△SFUI △CES-D

(a) Suppression of △perceived friend (b) Suppression of perceived friend


support for the △SFUI-△CES-D link support for the △EFUI-△CES-D link
Figure 8-3 Hypothesis illustration for the suppression of change in perceived social
support on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D

8.3.2.3 Adolescent-parents conflict as a potential mediator for the relationship

between social networking use and depressive symptoms

(1) Association between adolescent-parents conflict and depressive symptoms

Harmonious family relationships, especially adolescent-parents relationship, are

important for adolescents’ development by providing emotional and instrumental

171
support. On the other hand, higher frequent conflict with parents (father and/or mother)

could increase the probability of depressive symptoms among adolescents. Several

studies have documented that poorer relationships with parents were associated with

higher level of depression among adolescents [239, 245, 246].

(2) Association between social networking use and adolescent-parents conflict

Family relationship is an important concern related to Internet use. In line with

the displacement theory, adolescents’ preoccupations in online social networking

would interfere with their everyday family activities, which is linked with less time

spent with parents and greater adolescent-parents conflict [54, 61], and college students

have acknowledged that social networking use decreased their face-to-face

interaction [247]. Indeed, a previous online survey among college students found that

those students who used social networking to communicate with parents reported

higher levels of conflicts within the parental relationship [58]. Reich and colleagues

reported that about 9% of respondents’ parents (or their guardians) monitored their

social networking use [59], which may be another source that causes adolescents’

conflicts with parents. However, it is still worth noting that some parents might utilize

social networking to maintain closer contact with their teens. Thus, there are

significant implications for exploring the impact of social networking use on

adolescents’ relationships with parents, especially the potential conflict with parents

caused by social networking use.

Based on the literature, we hypothesized that:

Hypothesis m1 (Hm1): Change in adolescent-father conflict over time would

mediate the positive relationship between change in social networking use intensity

172
(both change in SFUI and EFUI) and change in depressive symptoms (Figure 8-4).

Hypothesis m2 (Hm2): Change in adolescent-mother conflict over time would

mediate the positive relationship between change in social networking use intensity

(both change in SFUI and EFUI) and change in depressive symptoms (Figure 8-5).

△Adolescent- △Adolescent-
father conflict father conflict
+ + + +

+ +
△SFUI △CES-D △EFUI △CES-D

(a) Mediation of △adolescent-father (b) Mediation of △adolescent-father


conflict for the △SFUI-△CES-D link conflict for the △EFUI-△CES-D link
Figure 8-4 Hypothesis illustration for the mediation of change in adolescent-father
conflict on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D

△Adolescent- △Adolescent-
mother conflict mother conflict
+ + + +

+
+ △EFUI △CES-D
△SFUI △CES-D

(a) Mediation of △adolescent-mother (b) Mediation of △adolescent-mother


conflict for the △SFUI-△CES-D conflict for the △EFUI-△CES-D
link link
Figure 8-5 Hypothesis illustration for the mediation of change in adolescent-mother
conflict on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D

8.3.2.4 Social non-confidence as a potential mediator for the relationship between

social networking use and depressive symptoms

(1) Association between social non-confidence and depressive symptoms

Adolescents with low social confidence and who experience social anxiety tend

to appear nervous during social interactions and are prone to be rejected by peers [248],

and consequently, those with low social confidence would foster social isolation and

173
other mental health problems. Social confidence as an important psychosocial factor

has been widely used in previous studies, e.g. the role of psychosocial factors on

neck-shoulder and low-back pain [249], among stroke patients [250], its relationships

with alcohol use and antisocial behaviors among middle school students [251], and its

association with school connectedness among children [122]. Socially non-confident

individuals demonstrate insufficient confidence and may feel highly anxious in social

occasions. One study from Zhuang MK et al reported that social non-confidence was

positively correlated with depressive symptoms among Chinese university

students [171].

(2) Association between online social networking use and social non-confidence

We did not find any studies that tested the association between online social

networking use and social non-confidence. Social networking use would potentially

reduce face-to-face interaction offline. Furthermore, online interaction is substantially

different from face-to-face interaction in terms of a lack of non-verbal cues (i.e. voice

tone, eye contact) and reduced degree of social presence [123]. Due to the reduced

probability of face-to-face interaction in real life, social confidence is likely to be

diminished. We therefore involved social non-confidence as an important

psychosocial factor in the present study and tested its mechanism for the association

between social networking use intensity and depressive symptoms.

Given the information presented above, we hypothesized that:

Hypothesis m3 (Hm3): Change in social non-confidence over time would

mediate the positive relationship between change in social networking use intensity

(both change in SFUI and EFUI) and change in depressive symptoms (Figure 8-6).

174
△Social non- △Social non-
confidence confidence
+ + + +

+ +
△SFUI △CES-D △EFUI △CES-D

(a) Mediation of △social non-confidence (b) Mediation of △social non-confidence


for the △SFUI-△CES-D link for the △EFUI-△CES-D link
Figure 8-6 Hypothesis illustration for the mediation of change in social
non-confidence on the association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in
CES-D

8.3.2.5 Self-esteem as a potential suppressor or a potential mediator for the

relationship between social networking use and depressive symptoms

(1) Association between self-esteem and depressive symptoms

Self-esteem is defined as a person’s overall self-evaluation of his/her worth, and

adolescence is an especially important period for self-esteem formation. Self-esteem

is an important factor in developing and maintaining mental health and overall quality

of life. Substantive studies reveal that lower self-esteem is prospectively associated

with depression. For instance, one recent systematic review summarized that

self-esteem is a protective factor for lower depressive symptoms [252]. In a study from

Brage and Meredith [253], the results showed that self-esteem had a strong and negative

effect on adolescent depression. Another study among Canadian adolescents also

reported that self-esteem was a key protective factor for depressive symptoms [254]. A

23-year longitudinal study showed that low self-esteem at adolescence and decreases

in self-esteem during the follow-up period were significantly associated with

depressive symptoms after two decades as adults [124].

175
(2) Association between social networking use and self-esteem

Some studies reported that time spent on social networking was negatively

associated with self-esteem [235]. Similarly, one study from Canadian university

Facebook users reported that greater Facebook activity was related with lower

self-esteem [64]. These results are consistent with the objective self-awareness (OSA)

theory. The OSA theory claims that individuals are prone to self-evaluations based on

the broader social standards and norms, and any stimulus (i.e. looking at self in a

mirror, hearing one’s own voice, or any other situation during which individual’

attention focuses on the his/her own self) causing the self to become the object

(instead subject) of consciousness would lead to a diminished impression of

oneself [255, 256]. Social networking profiles include information about oneself similar

to the information that would be used to prompt OSA; therefore, frequent visiting to

his/her own profile may cause a reduction of self-esteem according to the OSA

theory [63].

Moreover, the hyper-personal model is another relevant theoretical approach to

understanding the effect of social networking use on self-esteem, which stresses that

selective self-presentation on online social networking positively impacts

self-evaluation and then improves self-esteem [63]. Indeed, some authors have

presented that social networking use might potentially enhance self-esteem, such as

the study from Valkenburg et al among Dutch teenagers [62] and the study from

Gonzales et al [63], both of which reported a positive effect of social networking use on

self-esteem.

Give that the literature remains inconclusive regarding the potential influence of

social networking use on self-esteem, both mediation and suppression hypotheses

176
were tested in the present study.

We hypothesized that:

Hypothesis s4 (Hs4): Change in self-esteem over time would suppress the

positive relationship between social networking use intensity (both change in SFUI

and EFUI) and depressive symptoms (Figure 8-7).

Alternatively:

Hypothesis m4 (Hm4): Change in self-esteem over time would mediate the

positive relationship between change in social networking use intensity (both change

in SFUI and EFUI) and change in depressive symptoms (Figure 8-8).

△Self-esteem △Self-esteem
+ - + -

+ +
△SFUI △CES-D △EFUI △CES-D

(a) Suppression of △self-esteem for the (b) Suppression of △self-esteem for the
△SFUI-△CES-D link △EFUI-△CES-D link
Figure 8-7 Hypothesis illustration for the suppression of change in self-esteem on the
association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D

△Self-esteem △Self-esteem
- - - -

+ +
△SFUI △CES-D △EFUI △CES-D

(a) Mediation of △self-esteem for the (b) Mediation of △self-esteem for the
△SFUI-△CES-D link △EFUI-△CES-D link
Figure 8-8 Hypothesis illustration for the mediation of change in self-esteem on the
association between change in SFUI/EFUI and change in CES-D

177
8.3.3 Implications of testing the mediation and suppression hypotheses

The present study has several implications. First, the findings would determine

the impacts of social networking use intensity on adolescents’ psychosocial status.

Current studies on the impact of social networking use on psychosocial relationships

are under-researched and showed mixed results. Second, the present study is the first

to explore the underlying mechanisms of psychosocial factors using a longitudinal

sample. By investigating the roles of diverse psychosocial factors on the association

between social networking use intensity and depressive symptoms, the underlying

mechanisms of the detrimental effects of social networking use intensity on

depression could be explored. The findings could be suggestive for the development

of intervention campaigns to improve mental health outcomes among adolescents, i.e.

if the underlying mechanisms are confirmed, interventions should therefore focus on

improving their psychosocial conditions (e.g. improving self-esteem). Third, the

findings in the present study could provide robust evidence for the theoretical

understanding of social networking use and psychological well-being. Fourth, an

additional implication for future research is the use of mediation framework to

explicitly study the suppression effect. The objective of the suppression analysis is to

determine the extent to which one variable explains the variance in the outcome that is

mistakenly attributed to a second variable when that variable is uncontrolled for. This

could imply that without considering the suppressor in the regression, the observed

impact of social networking use intensity on depression would be underestimated.

178
8.3.4 Statistical analysis for mediation and suppression effects

8.3.4.1 Sample and types of variables used in this chapter

The sample used in this chapter was the entire longitudinal sample (n=4237). The

dependent variable was change in CES-D score during the follow-up period (indicated

as △CES-D), while the independent variables were change in SFUI and EFUI score

during the follow-up period (indicated as △SFUI and △EFUI). Potential suppressors

included △friendship quality, △perceived family support, △perceived friend support

and △self-esteem, while potential mediators included △adolescent-father conflict,

△adolescent-mother conflict and △social non-confidence. All change variables in

scale scores were calculated by subtracting scale scores at baseline from those at

follow-up. The mean score differences in SFUI, EFUI, psychosocial scales and

depressive symptoms were described in Table 4-12 and Table 4-13 in Chapter 4.

8.3.4.2 Mediation and suppression effects testing

Given that students were nested within the schools, the assumption of

independency between students in the same school required in traditional statistical

methods (i.e. ordinary least-squares regression) would be violated. Multilevel linear

regression models (level 1: students, level 2: schools) (SAS MIXED procedure) were

therefore applied in this chapter for the analyses so that the potential clustering effects

from schools could be removed by specification of the random intercept in the models.

Background factors that were associated with △CES-D were selected by univariate

linear regressions at the 0.05 level. The strategies from Baron and Kenny [139] were

applied to evaluate the potential mediation/suppression effects of change in

psychosocial factors on the associations between △SFUI/△EFUI and △CES-D

179
(Figure 8-9).

8.3.4.2.1 Testing requirements for mediation and suppression hypothesis

The linear regression models were fit to examine the significance of the

associations between △SFUI/△EFUI and △CES-D, associations between

△SFUI/△EFUI and potential mediators/suppressors, as well as associations between

potential mediators/suppressors and △CES-D after adjusting for significant

background factors.

8.3.4.2.2 Testing the strength and significance of the mediation and suppression

effects

For those variables fulfilling the above requirements, multivariate linear

regression models were fit to test the effects of mediator/suppressor on △CES-D when

controlling for the △SFUI/△EFUI.

The strength of indirect effects (mediation or suppression) were estimated by the

formula a*b according to the product of coefficients method, and the Sobel Z test was

used to test statistical significance of the indirect effect [143, 257]. As a rule of thumb

suggested by David A. Kenny, the indirect effect is defined as very small, small,

medium and large with cut-off values as 0.01, 0.09 and 0.25 respectively. For the

mediation effect, the proportion that the total effect was mediated by the specific

individual variable was calculated using the formula a*b/c to reflect the magnitude of

the mediation effect size [143, 146], where 𝑎 is the regression coefficient relating the

independent variable to the mediator in step 2, 𝑏 and c’ are the regression coefficients

relating the mediator to the dependent variable and independent variable to the

dependent variable in step 3, and c is the regression coefficient relating the

180
independent variable to dependent variable in step 1 (see Figure 8-9). For the

suppression effect, the total effect cannot be decomposed into indirect and direct

effect proportions because the suppression effect has an opposite sign of direct

effect [145, 258]. Therefore, the proportion of indirect effect on total effect is not

applicable and was not calculated for suppression effects. Only suppression effects

(a*b) were reported.

The mediators were firstly tested individually, and then tested as a whole to

explore the combined mediation/suppression effects. All models were adjusted by

significant background factors that were associated with △CES-D.

e (1)

Independent variable X Step 1: c Dependent variable Y


(i.e. △SFUI or △EFUI) (i.e. △CES-D)
Y = b 0(1) + cX + e (1)

e (2)
Intervening variable M/S
(i.e. Change in psychosocial
scale score)
Step 2: a Step 3: b

e (3)

Step 3: c’
Independent variable X Dependent variable Y
(i.e. △SFUI or △EFUI) (i.e. △CES-D)
M = b 0(2) + aX + e (2)
Y = b 0(3) + c ' X + bM + e (3)

Total effect=c
Direct effect=c’
Mediation/suppression effect=a*b

Figure 8-9 Illustrations for conceptual framework of change in psychosocial factors


for the associations between △SFUI/△EFUI and △CES-D

181
8.3.5 Results of mediation and suppression effects

8.3.5.1 Descriptive statistics

The description of changes in SFUI and EFUI scores over time is present in

Table 4-12 in Chapter 4. It shows that both SFUI score and EFUI score had a slight

but significant decrease from baseline to follow-up. Similarly, the results presented in

Table 4-13 in Chapter 4 show that scores in friendship quality, perceived family

support, perceived friend support, conflict with father, and social non-confidence

slightly decreased during the follow-up period. However, the mean score differences

in conflict with mother and self-esteem between baseline and follow-up were

non-significant.

8.3.5.2 Background factors associated with △CES-D

Univariate analysis showed that grade, academic performance, and perceived

study pressure were significant background factors associated with △CES-D at the 0.

05 level (Table 8-2). These significant background factors were hence adjusted for in

the following regression analyses.

8.3.5.3 Associations between △SFUI, △EFUI, and △CES-D

After adjusting for significant background factors, both △SFUI and △EFUI were

positively associated with △CES-D. The regression coefficients were 0.072 for

△SFUI and 0.139 for △EFUI (all p<0.001, see Table 8-3). These results were similar

with those reported in Chapter 7, showing that baseline SFUI and EFUI scores

significantly and positively predicted incident depression during the follow-up period.

182
8.3.5.4 Testing the mediation hypotheses

8.3.5.4.1 Mediation effects for the association between △SFUI and △CES-D

Given that △SFUI was not significantly associated with △adolescent-father

conflict and △self-esteem, hypothesis m1 and hypothesis m4 were not tested. After

adjusting for significant background factors, △SFUI was positively associated with

△adolescent-mother conflict and △social non-confidence (Table 8-4). Meanwhile,

△adolescent-mother conflict and △social non-confidence were also positively

associated with △CES-D (Table 8-5). Therefore, since hypothesis m2 and hypothesis

m3 met the requirements, mediation hypotheses were tested only for these two

potential mediators.

Hypothesis m2: When △adolescent-mother conflict was added into the model 1

in Table 8-7, the regression coefficient of △SFUI on △CES-D remained significant,

but diminished from 0.073 to 0.070 (Model 2a in Table 8-6 and Figure 8-10). It

indicated that the positive effect of △SFUI on △CES-D was partially mediated by

△adolescent-mother conflict. The mediation effect was 0.004 (95% CI: 0.001~0.007,

Sobel Z=2.434, p=0.015), which indicated a very small effect size (smaller than the

cut-off of 0.01). Furthermore, the proportion mediated by △adolescent-mother

conflict was 5.11%.

△Adolescent-
mother conflict
0.041*** 0.091***

Direct effect: 0.070***


△SFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0.073***
Figure 8-10 Mediation effect of change in adolescent-mother conflict on the
association between change in SFUI and change in CES-D

183
Hypothesis m3: When △social non-confidence was added into the model 1 in

Table 8-7, the regression coefficient of △SFUI on △CES-D remained significant but

diminished from 0.073 to 0.058 (Model 2b in Table 8-6 and Figure 8-11). It was hence

suggested that the positive effect of △SFUI on △CES-D was partially mediated by

△social non-confidence. The mediation effect from △social non-confidence was 0.015

(95% CI: 0.009~0.021, Sobel Z=5.137, p<0.001), which indicated a small effect size

(smaller than the cut-off of 0.09). Moreover, the proportion that total effect was

mediated by △social non-confidence was 20.97%.

△Social non-
confidence
0.04S*** 0.S56***

Direct effect: 0.058***


△SFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0.07S***

Figure 8-11 Mediation effect of change in social non-confidence on the association


between change in SFUI and change in CES-D

When these two significant mediators were tested in the same model, the

combined mediation effect via △adolescent-mother conflict and △social

non-confidence were approximately 25.33% (Model 3 in Table 8-6). These results

implied that increase in SFUI augmented probability of adolescents’ conflict with

mother and social non-confidence, which in turn further increased their level of

depressive symptoms. Moreover, the mediation effect was mainly from △social

non-confidence.

184
8.3.5.4.2 Mediation effects for the association between △EFUI and △CES-D

The adjusted results showed that △EFUI was positively associated with △social

non-confidence (Table 8-4), and △social non-confidence was also positively

associated with △CES-D (Table 8-5). Therefore, △social non-confidence (hypothesis

m3) fulfilled the requirements, and the mediation hypothesis was conducted only for

△social non-confidence. For hypothesis m1, hypothesis m2 and hypothesis m4, given

that △EFUI was not significantly associated with △adolescent-father conflict,

△adolescent-mother conflict, and △self-esteem, these three mediation hypotheses

were not tested.

Hypothesis m3: After adding the △social non-confidence into the model 1 in

Table 8-8, the regression coefficient of △EFUI on △CES-D remained significant but

diminished from 0.139 to 0.096 (Model 2 in Table 8-7 and Figure 8-12). It suggested

that △social non-confidence partially mediated the association between △EFUI and

△CES-D, and the mediation effect was 0.043 (95%CI: 0.029~0.056, Sobel Z=6.068,

p<0.001), which indicated a small effect size (smaller than the cut-off of 0.09). The

proportion of THE mediation effect to total effect was around 30.65%.

△Social non-
confidence
0.117*** 0.S58***

Direct effect: 0.096***


△EFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0.1S9***

Figure 8-12 Illustration for the mediation effect of △Social non-confidence on the
association between △EFUI and △CES-D

185
8.3.5.5 Testing the suppression hypotheses

8.3.5.5.1 Suppression effects for the association between △SFUI and △CES-D

The previous analyses showed that △SFUI was positively associated with

△friendship quality, △perceived family support, and △perceived friend support (Table

8-4). Additionally, △friendship quality, △perceived family support, and △perceived

friend support were all negatively associated with △CES-D (Table 8-5). Therefore, the

hypotheses s1, s2 and s3 were proceeded for the suppression testing. However, given

that △self-esteem was not significantly associated with △SFUI, hypothesis s4 was not

further tested.

Hypothesis s1: When adding the △friendship quality into the model 1 in Table

8-9, the regression coefficients of △SFUI on △CES-D increased from 0.073 to 0.084

(Model 2a in Table 8-8 and Figure 8-13). The suppression effect was -0.011 for

△friendship quality (Sobel Z=-2.946, p=0.003). Hence, hypothesis s1 was supported.

The results indicated that △friendship quality suppressed the positive association

between △SFUI and △CES-D. When statistically removing the effect of △friendship

quality, the association between △SFUI and △CES-D actually became larger.

However, the suppression effect from △friendship quality was small.

△Friendship
quality
0.018*** -0.621***

Direct effect: 0.084***


△SFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0.073***

Figure 8-13 Suppression effect of change in friendship quality on the association


between change in SFUI and change in CES-D

186
Hypothesis s2: When adding △perceived family support into the model 1 in

Table 8-9, the regression coefficients of △SFUI on △CES-D increased from 0.073 to

0.083 (Model 2b in Table8-9 and Figure 8-14). The suppression effect was -0.009 for

△perceived family support (Sobel Z=-3.008, p=0.003). Hypothesis s2 was supported.

It meant that △perceived family support suppressed the positive effect of △SFUI on

△CES-D. When controlling for △perceived family support in the model, the

predictive validity of △SFUI on △CES-D was enhanced. However, the suppression

effect size was very small as the absolute value was less than the suggested cut-off of

0.01.

△Perceived
family support
P.P31*** -P.298***

Direct effect: P.P83***


△SFUI △CES-D
Total effect: P.P73***

Figure 8-14 Suppression effect of change in perceived family support on the


association between change in SFUI and change in CES-D

Hypothesis s3: When adding △perceived friend support into the model 1 in table

8-9, the regression coefficients of △SFUI on △CES-D increased from 0.073 to 0.095

(Model 2c in Table 8-8 and Figure 8-15). The suppression effect was -0.022 for

△perceived friend support (Sobel Z=-6.468, p<0.001). Hypothesis s3 was therefore

supported by the results. It meant that △perceived friend support suppressed the

positive effect of △SFUI on △CES-D. However, the suppression effect size was small

as the absolute value was less than the suggested cut-off of 0.09.

187
△Perceived
friend support
P.176*** -P.124***

Direct effect: P.P95***


△SFUI △CES-D
Total effect: P.P73***

Figure 8-15 Suppression effect of change in perceived friend support on the


association between change in SFUI and change in CES-D

8.3.5.5.2 Suppression effects for the association between △EFUI and △CES-D

The adjusted analysis showed that △EFUI was positively associated with

△perceived family support and △perceived friend support (Table 8-4), and reversely,

△perceived family support and △perceived friend support were negatively associated

with △CES-D (Table 8-5). These two variables fulfilled the requirements of

suppression hypotheses s2 and s3. Therefore, the suppression hypotheses were tested

only for △perceived family support (hypothesis s2) and △perceived friend support

(hypothesis s3). For △friendship quality and △self-esteem, because of their

non-significant association with △EFUI, hypotheses s1 and s4 were not further tested.

Hypothesis s2: When adding △perceived family support into the model 1 in

table 8-10, the regression coefficients of △EFUI on △CES-D increased from 0.139 to

0.184 (Model 2a in Table 8-9 and Figure 8-16). Hypothesis s2 was hence supported.

The suppression effect was -0.036 (Sobel Z=-5.571, p<0.001). It suggested that

△perceived family support suppressed the positive association between △EFUI and

△CES-D. However, the effect size was small as the absolute value was less than the

suggested cut-off of 0.09.

188
△Perceived
family support
P.149*** -P.3P3***

Direct effect: P.184***


△EFUI △CES-D
Total effect: P.139***

Figure 8-16 Suppression effect of change in perceived family support on the


association between change in EFUI and change in CES-D

Hypothesis s3: When adding △perceived friend support into the model 1 in

Table 8-10, the regression coefficients of △EFUI on △CES-D increased from 0.139 to

0.178 (Model 2b in Table 8-9 and Figure 8-17). Hypothesis s3 was therefore

supported. The suppression effect was -0.039 (Sobel Z=-5.518, p<0.001, Table 8-10).

Hypothesis s3 was supported. It suggested that △perceived friend support acted as a

suppressor for the positive association between △EFUI and △CES-D. However, the

suppression effect was small.

△Perceived
friend support
P.322*** -P.121***

Direct effect: P.178***


△EFUI △CES-D
Total effect: P.139***

Figure 8-17 Suppression effect of change in perceived friend support on the


association between change in EFUI and change in CES-D

8.3.6 Summary for the mediation and suppression effects

8.3.6.1 Summary for the mediation effects

After adjusting for the significant background factors, change in

189
adolescent-mother conflict and change in social non-confidence partially mediated the

associations between change in SFUI and change in CES-D. The magnitudes of

mediation effect were 5.11% for change in adolescent-mother conflict and 20.97% for

change in social non-confidence respectively.

Moreover, change in social non-confidence also partially mediated the

association between change in EFUI and change in CES-D. The magnitude of

mediation effect via change in social non-confidence was 30.65%.

8.3.6.2 Summary for the suppression effects

The positive association between change in SFUI and change in CES-D was

suppressed by change in friendship quality, change in perceived family support, and

change in perceived friend support. The suppression effects were -0.011, -0.009 and

-0.022 respectively.

Change in perceived family support and change in perceived friend support

suppressed the positive association between change in EFUI and change in CES-D

after adjusting for significant background factors. The suppression effects were -0.036

for change in perceived family support and -0.039 for change in perceived friend

support.

8.4 Part II: Testing moderation effects

8.4.1 Moderation effects of psychosocial factors on the association between social

networking use intensity and depressive symptoms

Previous studies have suggested that social resources could moderate the

relationships between Internet use and psychological outcomes [219, 259]. Effect of

190
social networking use on mental health outcomes (i.e. depressive symptoms) might

vary in terms of levels of some psychosocial factors, stressing the necessity to

consider moderation effects. The psychosocial factors investigated in our study (e.g.

friendship quality, perceived family support, perceived friend support, self-esteem,

adolescent-father conflict, adolescent-mother conflict, and social non-confidence) can

be seen as participants’ social resources. In addition to being predictive factors on

depressive symptoms, they may also moderate the relationships between social

networking use intensity and depressive symptoms. Knowing such information could

clarify what people would be impacted greatly by social networking use, and could

then guide for future intervention campaigns.

The relevant and commonly mentioned hypotheses are the rich-get-richer and

poor-get-poorer effects, which forms the basis of the “Matthew effect” originated

initially by Merton RK in 1968 in sociological science [260]. Merton RK proposed the

Matthew effect originally to explain discrepancies in scientific career development,

such that the eminent scientists usually receive more credit for their contributions to

science, while relatively unknown scientists tend to get little credits for the

comparable achievement [260]. This states that a relatively favorable position acts as a

resource that produces further gains and creates inequality across any temporal

process [261]. In a general sense, the Matthew effect denotes the psychosocial process

that the “rich-get-richer and poor-get-poorer”, and its name quotes from the Bible:

“For unto every one that hath shall be given, and he shall have abundance: but from

him that hath not shall be taken away even that which he hath” (Matthew 25:29). The

Matthew effect has been introduced into other research fields, i.e. the educational field

to describe the development of individual differences in reading ability [262, 263] and in

191
economics field to explain the poverty and inequality [264]. The Matthew effect has

also been introduced into public health research. For instance, it is used to examine

whether patients with low socioeconomic status and patients with emotional problems

were more likely to become incapacitated for work after heart disease than those with

high socioeconomic status and those without emotional problems [265].

The rich-get-richer and poor-get-poorer effects had also been applied to explain

the effects of Internet use on mental health outcomes first by Kraut et al in 2002 [46].

The longitudinal results from Kraut et al showed that Internet use was associated with

better psychosocial outcomes (i.e. increased self-esteem) for extraverts and those with

more social support (rich-get-richer), while Internet use was associated with worse

psychosocial outcomes (i.e. increased loneliness) for introverts and those with less

social support (poor-get-poorer) [46]. A one-year longitudinal study among Dutch

adolescents tested the moderating effect of friendship quality on the association

between Internet use and depression and social anxiety by hierarchical regression

models with depression/social anxiety at time 2 as dependent variables and types of

Internet use and friendship quality at time 1 as independent variables. Meanwhile the

poor-get-poorer hypothesis was tested by the interaction terms of types of Internet use

and friendship quality, and the results found that the harmful effects of Internet surfing

on depression and social anxiety were significantly larger among adolescents with low

friendship quality than those with high friendship quality, which supported the

poor-get-poorer hypothesis [204]. However, a longitudinal study from Van den Eijnden

et al tested the poor-get-poorer effect based on the assumption that the positive

prediction of Internet use on depression would be larger for adolescents with high

level of loneliness than for those with low level of loneliness by multi-group

192
comparisons of SEM, but the poor-get-poorer hypothesis was not supported [230]. The

results are hence mixed.

Figure 8-21 illustrates the general concept of the poor-get-poorer hypothesis in

terms of the effect of social resources (e.g. friendship quality) on the positive

association between Internet use and depression. Based on the poor-get-poorer

hypothesis, the positive effect of Internet use on depression would be greater among

individuals with low levels of social resources as compared to those with high levels

of social resources.

Low social resource


Level of Depression

High social resource

Internet use

Figure 8-18 Illustration of the poor-get-poorer hypothesis

In this longitudinal study, score changes were used to reflect the within-subject

dynamic variations in the levels of individual’s social resources (i.e. change in

friendship quality, perceived family support, perceived friend support, self-esteem,

adolescent-father conflict, adolescent-mother conflict, and social non-confidence).

The poor-get-poorer model is thus modified to take into account longitudinal nature of

the data, which is poorer-get-poorer. We hypothesized that the positive association

between increase in social networking use intensity (SFUI and EFUI) and increase in

193
depressive symptoms would be stronger among those with ‘large deterioration’ in

social resources as compared to those with ‘small change’ in social resources during

the follow-up period (see Figure 8-19 below for the modified poorer-get-poorer

hypothesis of the ‘large deterioration’ in social resources as compared to ‘small

change’ in social resources).

Another hypothesis that is commonly applied for moderation effect testing

involving social resource variables is referred to a buffering effect model. The

buffering effect model has been elaborated in detail by Cohen sheldon [266] in 1985,

which posits that the pathogenic influence of risk events on mental health outcomes

(i.e. depression) are smaller under conditions of high perceived social support than

under conditions of low perceived social support. Studies on buffering effects (e.g.

social support) on the associations between risk behaviors and negative outcomes

have been reported in previous studies, especially those focusing on social support.

For instance, it is reported that social support could protect bullied adolescents from

poor academic achievement and depression [267]. A longitudinal study reported that

effortful control buffered the risk of adolescent family adversity on externalizing

problems (i.e. Aggressive Behavior and Rule-Breaking Behavior) [268]. However, there

are no studies that investigate the potential buffering effect of social resources on the

associations between social networking use intensity and depression among

adolescents. In the present study, for those whose personal social resources largely

improved during the follow-up period, it might be plausible that such ‘large

improvement’ in social resources would diminish the adverse effects of social

networking use on depression, suggesting that buffering effects could potentially exist.

Figure 8-19 illustrates the buffering effect hypothesis for ‘large improvement’ in

194
social resources as compared to ‘small change’ in social resource for the association

between change in social networking use intensity and change in depressive

symptoms.

'Large deterioration'
Poorer-get-poorer in social resource
Change in CES-D

Buffering effect 'Small change' in


social resource

'Large improvement'
in social resource

Change in social working use intensity

Figure 8-19 Illustration for modified poorer-get-poorer and buffering effect


hypothesis

Following the aforementioned logic and hypotheses, we developed a three-level

definition for the change scores of social resource variables (i.e. friendship quality,

perceived family support, perceived friend support, self-esteem, adolescent-father

conflict, adolescent-mother conflict, and social non-confidence) based on the strategy

of 0.5 standard deviation (SD) above and below the mean of change scores (i.e. mean

± 0.5SD). Definition for each level are as follows: ‘Large deterioration’ in social

resource (those scoring 0.5 SD below the mean of change scores), ‘Large

improvement’ in social resource (those scoring 0.5 SD above the mean of change

scores) and ‘Small change’ in social resource (those scoring between 0.5 SD below

the mean of change scores and above the mean of change scores).

195
Therefore, there are two set of hypotheses, one set for the poorer-get-poorer

hypotheses (i.e. ‘Large deterioration’ in social resources would increase the effects of

change in social networking use intensity on change in depressive symptoms as

compared to the ‘small change’ group in social resources), the other set for the

buffering effect hypotheses (i.e. ‘Large improvement’ in social resources would buffer

the effects of change in social networking use on change in depressive symptoms as

compared to the ‘small change’ group in social resources).

The specific poorer-get-poorer hypotheses for each social resource variable are

presented in detail below:

H4a: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be larger among participants with ‘large deterioration’ in friendship

quality as compared to those with ‘small change’ in friendship quality during the

follow-up period.

H4b: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be larger among participants with ‘large deterioration’ in perceived

family support as compared to those with ‘small change’ in perceived family support

during the follow-up period.

H4c: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be larger among participants with ‘large deterioration’ in perceived

friend support as compared to those with ‘small change’ in perceived friend support

during the follow-up period.

H4d: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be larger among participants with ‘large deterioration’ in self-esteem

196
as compared to those with ‘small change’ in self-esteem during the follow-up period.

H4e: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be larger among participants with ‘large deterioration’ in

adolescent-father conflict (meaning a large increase in conflicts with the father) as

compared to those with ‘small change’ in adolescent-father conflict during the

follow-up period.

H4f: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be larger among participants with ‘large deterioration’ in

adolescent-mother conflict (meaning a large increase in conflicts with the mother) as

compared to those with ‘small change’ in adolescent-mother conflict during the

follow-up period.

H4g: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be larger among participants with ‘large deterioration’ in social

non-confidence (meaning a large increase in social confidence) as compared to those

with ‘small change’ in social non-confidence during the follow-up period.

The specific buffering effect hypotheses for each social resource variable are

presented in detail below:

H5a: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be smaller among participants with ‘large improvement’ in

friendship quality as compared to those with ‘small change’ in friendship quality

during the follow-up period.

H5b: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

197
symptoms would be smaller among participants with ‘large improvement’ in

perceived family support as compared to those with ‘small change’ in perceived

family support during the follow-up period.

H5c: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be smaller among participants with ‘large improvement’ in

perceived friend support as compared to those with ‘small change’ in perceived friend

support during the follow-up period.

H5d: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be smaller among participants with ‘large improvement’ in

self-esteem as compared to those with ‘small change’ in self-esteem during the

follow-up period.

H5e: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be smaller among participants with ‘large improvement’ in

adolescent-father conflict (meaning large increases in conflicts with the father) as

compared to those with ‘small change’ in adolescent-father conflict during the

follow-up period.

H5f: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be smaller among participants with ‘large improvement’ in

adolescent-mother conflict (meaning large increases in conflicts with the mother) as

compared to those with ‘small change’ in adolescent-mother conflict during the

follow-up period.

H5g: The positive effects of change in SFUI/EFUI on change in depressive

symptoms would be smaller among participants with ‘large improvement’ in social

198
non-confidence (meaning large decreases in social non-confidence) as compared to

those with ‘small change’ in social non-confidence during the follow-up period.

8.4.2 Statistical methods for testing moderation effects

The moderating effects of △friendship quality, △perceived family support,

△perceived friend support, △self-esteem, △adolescent-father conflict,

△adolescent-mother conflict, and △social non-confidence on the relationship between

△SFUI/△EFUI and △CES-D were tested by fitting the models including one of the

independent variables (△SFUI or △EFUI), one of the psychosocial factors

(△friendship quality, △perceived family support, △perceived friend support,

△self-esteem, △adolescent-father conflict, △adolescent-mother conflict, and △social

non-confidence), and their interaction terms, after adjusting for the significant

background factors. The significance of interaction terms were evaluated by

comparing the main effect model and the model containing the main effect variables

and the interaction term by Likelihood Ratio Test [269], using the change in statistics

-2*Log Likelihood (-2LL), which followed a Chi-square distribution with degree of

freedom (df) equals to two.

When the significant interaction was shown by Likelihood Ratio Test, the

poorer-get-poorer and buffering effect hypotheses could be identified by inspecting

the sign of interaction terms. Specifically, if the sign of the interaction term between

△SFUI/EFUI and group in ‘large deterioration’ in social resources was significantly

positive, the poorer-get-poorer hypothesis would be supported (i.e. ‘large deterioration’

in social resources over time amplifies the positive influences of △SFUI/△EFUI onto

△CES-D as compared to the ‘small change’ group). For the interaction terms between

199
△SFUI/EFUI and the group in large improvements in psychosocial status, if the sign

of the interaction was negative, a buffering effect would be supported for

improvement in social resources, meaning that ‘large improvement’ in social

resources over time would weaken the positive impact of △SFUI/EFUI onto △CES-D

as compared to ‘small change’ group.

Model a Model b

Change in SFUI/EFUI Change in SFUI/EFUI

+ +
Change in psychosocial Change in psychosocial
variables variables

Change in SFUI/EFUI
×
Change in psychosocial
variables

Figure 8-20 Hierarchical regression models to test the interaction effects between
change in SFUI (or change in EFUI) and change in psychosocial variables on change
in depressive symptoms

8.4.3 Results

The moderation results are showed in Table 8-10 and Table 8-11. Models 1b~7b

were fit by adding an interaction term (e.g. △SFUI*△friendship quality, including two

dummy variables: △SFUI*large improvement and △SFUI*larger deterioration with

‘small change’ as reference group) to the corresponding main effect models (model

1a~7a). Significant interactions at the 0.05 level were detected between △SFUI and

△adolescent-father conflict (Change in -2LL between Model 5a and Model 5b =11.7,

p=0.003 in Table 8-10), and between △EFUI and △adolescent-father conflict (Change

in -2LL between Model 5a and Model 5b =7.9, p=0.019 in Table 8-11). The other

200
interactions were not statistically significant.

In particular, the interaction terms between △SFUI and the group in ‘large

deterioration’ in adolescent-conflict father as well as between △EFUI and the group in

‘large deterioration’ in adolescent-father conflict were marginally significant.

However, the direction did not support the poorer-get-poorer hypothesis as the

regression coefficients of these two interaction terms were negative, meaning that

those that deteriorated encountered smaller association between increase in social

networking use intensity and increase in depressive symptoms (see Figure 8-24).

Significant interactions between △SFUI and the group in ‘large improvement’ in

adolescent-father conflict, and between △EFUI and the group in ‘large improvement’

in adolescent-father conflict were found in the same two models. The negative sign of

the interaction terms indicated a buffering effect, meaning that the large improvement

in the relationship with the father over time (meaning a decrease in adolescent-father

conflict) buffered the positive influence of △SFUI and △EFUI onto △CES-D as

compared to the small change group. Figure 8-21 illustrated the significant

interactions found. It is seen that the positive effects of △SFUI and △EFUI on

△CES-D were smaller among those with ‘large improvement’ in adolescent-father

conflict as compared to the ‘small change’ group.

Besides, all main effect models (Model 1a~7a in Table 8-10 and Table 8-11)

showed that △SFUI and △EFUI were positively associated with △CES-D (bs ranging

from 0.063 to 0.091 for △SFUI; bs ranging from 0.108 to 0.168 for △SFUI), which

are consistent with those in part I.

201
8 Large deterioration
6 Large improvement
Small change
4

Change in CES-D
2

-2

-4

-6

-8
-40 -30 -20 -10 0 10 20 30 40
Change in SFUI

(a) Interaction between change in SFUI and change in adolescent-father conflict

8 Large deterioration
6 Large improvement
Small change
4
Change in CES-D

2
0
-2
-4
-6
-8
-20 -15 -10 -5 0 5 10 15 20
Change in EFUI
(b) Interaction between change in EFUI and change in adolescent-father conflict
Figure 8-21 Graphical illustration for the interactions between change in SFUI/EFUI
and change in conflict with father on change in depressive symptoms3

8.5 Discussion

8.5.1 Associations of social networking use intensity on psychosocial factors

In this chapter, we first described the potential influence of change in social

networking use intensity onto change in psychosocial factors over a nine-month

follow-up period, including friendship quality, perceived social support, self-esteem,

adolescent-parents conflict, and social non-confidence. First, it found that increased

202
social networking use intensity was positively associated with increased perceived

social support (both SFUI and EFUI), and friendship quality (SFUI only) during the

nine-month follow-up period. Similar results are reported in previous cross-sectional

studies among adolescents, reporting that more frequent Facebook use per day was

associated with higher perceived social support [243] and frequency of social

networking use was also associated with enhanced friendship quality [118]. The

findings imply that increase in intensity of social networking use increases

adolescents’ perceptions of increases in social support and sense of friendship quality.

Social networking use facilitates the interactions and social connections with each

other, and then fulfills adolescents’ needs for friendship maintenance during

adolescence. Engaging in diverse online social networking activities (e.g. wall posts,

instant messages, and comments) makes it easier for individuals both to broadcast

information about their activities/needs and to track the activities of their social

networking friends, which increases their feelings of closeness with friends and sense

of being supported. Another possible explanation is that social networking activities

(e.g. status updates, photo management, message posing, commenting on message

posted by their friends) encourage online self-disclosure (self-disclosure means

communicating personal information, thoughts, and feelings with others) [54, 270], and

the amount of online self-disclosure further enhances their sense of perceived social

support and friendship quality [16, 115].

Second, we also found that increase in SFUI was significantly associated with

increased adolescents’ conflict with the mother but not with conflict with the father

during the follow-up period. In general, using online social networking is a

time-demanding activity overall, and those who invest more time on social

203
networking use inevitably spend less time on shared activities with family

members [271]. Intensive social networking use and involvement in virtual

communities potentially isolates adolescents from their family, causing negative

impacts for adolescent-parents relationship. Adolescents might encounter more

conflict with their mothers as compared to that with their fathers. Previous studies

have revealed that social networking use among adolescents has been linked with

greater adolescent-parents conflict and less time spent with parents [58, 247]. We

speculate that this discrepancy is partially attributed to the differential roles of father

and mother in adolescents’ development. Mothers tend to be more likely involved in

parenting adolescents than fathers, and also interact more frequently with their

adolescents concerning personal issues (e.g. intensive social networking use, poor

time management) [272, 273], and this increases probability of conflicts with their

adolescents [274].

Third, this is the first study to estimate the influence of social networking use

intensity on the social non-confidence construct among adolescents. It is novel that

increased social networking use was significantly and positively associated with

increased social non-confidence during the follow-up period. It suggests that online

social networking activities decrease individuals’ confidence in social context. One

plausible explanation is that online social networking use reduces the opportunity of

face-to-face interaction in the real world. Adolescents with limited practice of

interactions with others in the social arena would appear to be nervous and may have

social anxiety during social interactions, manifesting as a lower level of social

confidence. Given the lack of previous data, this hypothesis needs to be tested in

future longitudinal studies among adolescents.

204
8.5.2 Mediation effects of change in social non-confidence and adolescent-mother

conflict

The mechanisms underlying the linkage between social networking use intensity

and depressive symptoms are not well understood. There is a dearth of studies to

explore the mediation effects of psychosocial factors on such associations. The

present study examined the mediation effects of change in adolescent-mother conflict

and social non-confidence on the relationship between change in social networking

use intensity and change in depressive symptoms among adolescents. The findings

revealed that the association between change in social networking use intensity and

change in depressive symptoms was partially mediated by change in social

non-confidence (both SFUI and EFUI) and change in adolescent-mother conflict

(EFUI only). Change in social non-confidence is a key mediator as compared to

change in adolescent-mother conflict. Proportions mediated by change in social

non-confidence were 20.97% for change in SFUI and 30.65% for change in EFUI

respectively.

The findings contend that the harmful impacts of increased social networking use

intensity on increased depressive symptoms are partially going through their

weakening effects on these two aspects of adolescents’ psychosocial status (i.e.

increased adolescent-mother conflict, increased social non-confidence), especially

through increased social non-confidence. It is important to strengthen social

confidence and improve adolescent-parents relationship (especially the

adolescent-mother relationship) to effectively prevent depressive symptoms among

adolescents with high intensity of social networking use.

205
8.5.3 Suppression effect of change in perceived social support and friendship

quality

It is interesting to find that change in perceived social support and friendship

quality negatively suppressed the influence of change in social networking use

intensity on change in depressive symptoms. This means that when controlling for

change in perceived social support and change in friendship quality, the magnitude of

the relationships between change in social networking use intensity and change in

depressive symptoms actually became larger. Harmful effects of increase in social

networking use intensity on depression are partially neutralized by simultaneous

improvement in perceived social support and friendship quality, as exhibited in the

first part of this chapter; such social improvements were protective against an increase

in depression. From the statistical perspective, it could be interpreted that inclusion of

change in perceived social support and friendship quality statistically removed the

outcome-irrelevant variance of change in social networking use intensity, and as a

result, the predictive validity of change in social networking use intensity on change

in depressive symptoms increased. Another interpretation is that increased perceived

social support and friendship quality partially compensate the effect of change in

social networking use intensity onto change in depressive symptoms. These

compensation effects cause researchers observing an underestimation of the harmful

effects of social networking use intensity on depressive symptoms than those that

actual existed, when not considering such opposite effects from improved perceived

social support and friendship quality. In addition, the findings implicate that harmful

effects of social networking use intensity on mental health outcomes (i.e. increased

depressive symptoms) could be suppressed by enhancing sense of perceived social

206
support and friendship quality. However, the suppression effects of change in

perceived social support and friendship quality are small. Future research is hence

needed to test such suppression effects further among adolescents.

8.5.4 Implications

The findings of this chapter have some practical implications for developing

intervention programs to improve mental health outcomes for adolescents with

intensive social networking use. Interventions that target at reducing the impact of

social networking use intensity on depressive symptoms should be effective by

improving psychosocial status (e.g. strengthening social confidence, improving

adolescent-parents relationships). Additionally, social skills training should be

involved in intervention programs for reducing depressive symptoms among

adolescents who use online social networking. Secondly, professionals and educators

should try to foster effective conflict solving strategies for adolescents to solve

conflicts with their mothers (e.g. conciliation, positive problem-copying

strategies) [275]. In addition, if possible, it would be helpful to involve parents, at least

mothers, in such training to reduce depressive symptoms of adolescents who use

social networking intensively. Thirdly, the suppression findings suggest the necessity

of measuring perceived social support and friendship quality in future research

estimating the relationship between social networking use and depression.

However, the mediation effects found in our study are relatively small. It

suggests that other potential mediators that are risk factors for depression may exist

but are not included in this Chapter. Future research is therefore warranted to explore

other potential important mediating variables that could further explain the

relationship between social networking use intensity and depressive symptoms among

207
adolescents. One set of such potentially mediating variables are Internet addictive

behaviors (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA) As a large public concern among adolescents, intensive

social networking use would increase the tendency of Internet addictive behaviors,

and which would in turn increase the level of depressive symptoms. These hypotheses

are tested and the results are presented in Chapter 9.

8.5.5 Moderation effects

We also explored the moderation effects of these psychosocial factors on the

relationship between change in social networking use intensity and change in

depressive symptoms. A significant moderation effect on change in depressive

symptoms was found between change in social networking use intensity (both SFUI

and EFUI) and change in adolescent-father conflict. The findings reveal that the

deteriorating effect of change in social networking use intensity on change in

depression was significantly smaller among participants whose relationship with their

father largely improved during the follow-up period (as indicated by large decreases

in adolescent-father conflict) as compared to the ‘small change’ group, supporting a

buffering effect for ‘large improvement’ in adolescents’ relationship with their father.

It suggests that improvement in adolescents’ relationship with their father could

weaken the harmful effects of social networking use on mental health outcomes.

8.5.6 Limitations

There are some limitations in the present study. First, the online and offline

psychosocial relationships were not differentiated by scales in our study (e.g. online

friendship quality vs. offline friendship quality, perceived online social support vs.

perceived offline social support). There is a study in Hong Kong that reported that

208
perceived online social support could not buffer stress for adolescents [276]. Therefore,

our study cannot answer whether social networking use has different effects on online

and offline psychosocial relationships (e.g. whether social networking use mainly

increases users’ online friendship quality rather than offline friendship, or increases

both). Second, we only investigated the perceived social support. Other forms of

social support (e.g. receiving and giving social support through social networking) are

important but not involved in the present study. Social support is not unidimensional,

but a process of social exchange encompassing receiving and giving social support.

Perceived social support is one of the most effective indicators of social support in

health-related contexts. Third, although the mediation observed in the present study

explained some possible mechanisms on how social networking use affects depressive

symptoms via its negative impacts on psychosocial factors, the causal mediational

sequence cannot be derived by the restriction of the two-wave study design, and at

least three separate time points are required, one for each of the variables.

209
Table 8-2 Background variables associated with change in depressive symptoms in longitudinal
sample (n=4237)
△CES-D Univariate linear regression
Mean SD b SE p
Social-demographic variables
Gender
Male 0.51 8.11 --
Female 0.45 9.00 -0.121 0.264 0.648
Grade
Seven 0.21 8.82 --
Eight 0.72 8.32 0.539 0.263 0.040
Father’s education level 0.880
Primary school or below -0.05 9.07 --
Junior middle school 0.54 8.66 0.529 0.569 0.352
High middle school 0.56 8.16 0.588 0.581 0.312
University or above 0.49 8.63 0.470 0.596 0.431
Don't know 0.14 9.49 0.273 0.837 0.744
Mother’s education level 0.053
Primary school or below -0.46 8.86 --
Junior middle school 0.81 8.47 1.237 0.465 0.008
High middle school 0.70 8.09 1.155 0.487 0.018
University or above 0.20 8.93 0.647 0.510 0.204
Don't know -0.05 9.56 0.514 0.773 0.506
Family financial situation 0.944
Very good/good 0.51 8.43 --
Average 0.44 8.49 0.023 0.270 0.933
Poor/very poor 0.67 11.66 0.274 0.810 0.734
Lives with both parents
Yes 0.57 8.46 --
No -0.19 9.29 -0.705 0.412 0.087

School-related variables
Academic performance 0.002
Upper 1.06 8.18 --
Medium 0.35 8.39 -0.681 0.297 0.022
Lower -0.22 9.49 -1.243 0.368 <0.001
Perceived study pressure <0.001
Nil/light 1.04 8.27 --
Average 0.68 8.15 -0.358 0.347 0.302
Heavy/very heavy -0.47 9.65 -1.464 0.404 <0.001
△CES-D: score in CES-D at follow-up minus its score at baseline.
SD: Standardized deviation.
b: Unstandardized regression coefficients from univariate multilevel linear regression model.
SE: Standard error.
--: Not applicable.

210
Table 8-3 Associations of change in SFUI and EFUI on change in depressive symptoms by
multilevel linear regression models (n=4237)
Univariate Adjusted a
b SE p β b SE p β
△SFUI 0.075 0.017 <0.001 0.069 0.073 0.017 <0.001 0.067
△EFUI 0.138 0.039 <0.001 0.055 0.139 0.039 <0.001 0.055
△SFUI: Change in Social function use intensity score; △EFUI: Change in Entertainment function
use intensity score.
a
Models were adjusted by grade, academic performance and perceived study pressure.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error.

211
Table 8-4 Associations between change in SFUI, EFUI and change in psychosocial variables by multilevel linear regression (n=4237)
IV=△SFUI IV=△EFUI
a
DV Univariate Adjusted Univariate Adjusted a
b SE β b SE β b SE β b SE β
△Friendship quality
** **
0.017 0.006 0.136 0.018 0.006 0.141 0.018 0.014 0.061 0.018 0.015 0.060
△Perceived family support 0.031** 0.010 0.241 0.031** 0.010 0.248 0.147*** 0.024 0.499 0.149*** 0.024 0.505
△Perceived friend support 0.178*** 0.020 1.404 0.176*** 0.020 1.394 0.323*** 0.047 1.094 0.322*** 0.047 1.093
△Rosenberg self-esteem 0.011 0.008 0.088 0.012 0.008 0.096 0.017 0.018 0.057 0.020 0.018 0.067
△Adolescent-father conflict 0.023† 0.013 0.178 0.020 0.013 0.161 0.018 0.031 0.061 0.011 0.031 0.038
△Adolescent-mother conflict 0.043** 0.015 0.335 0.041** 0.015 0.320 0.063† 0.034 0.215 0.056 0.034 0.189
△Social non-confidence 0.044 ***
0.007 0.346 0.043 ***
0.007 0.340 0.121 ***
0.015 0.411 0.119 ***
0.015 0.404
IV: Independent variable; DV: Dependent variable.
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity.
△ indicates the change in interpersonal and psychosocial scale scores from baseline to follow-up.
a
Models were adjusted by grade, academic performance and perceived study pressure.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error.

p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

212
Table 8-5 Association of change in psychosocial scale scores on change in depressive symptoms
by multilevel linear regression (n=4237)
Univariate Adjusted a
b SE β b SE β
△Friendship quality -0.621*** 0.040 -0.073 -0.612*** 0.040 -0.071
△Perceived family support -0.293*** 0.024 -0.034 -0.292*** 0.024 -0.034
△Perceived friend support -0.112*** 0.013 -0.013 -0.115*** 0.013 -0.013
△Rosenberg self-esteem -0.685*** 0.031 -0.080 -0.682*** 0.031 -0.080
△Adolescent-father conflict 0.097*** 0.019 0.011 0.093*** 0.019 0.011
△Adolescent-mother conflict 0.097*** 0.017 0.011 0.094*** 0.017 0.011
△Social non-confidence 0.377*** 0.038 0.044 0.369*** 0.038 0.043
△ indicates the change of psychosocial scale scores from baseline to follow-up.
a
Models were adjusted by grade, academic performance and perceived study pressure.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error.

p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

213
Table 8-6 Testing the mediation effects of change in psychosocial scale scores on the association between △SFUI and △CES-D by multilevel linear
regression (n=4237)
Model 1 Model 2a Model 2b Model 3
b SE β b SE β b SE β b SE β
△SFUI 0.073 ***
0.017 0.067 0.070 ***
0.017 0.064 0.058 ***
0.016 0.053 0.055 **
0.016 0.050

△Conflict with mother -- -- -- 0.091*** 0.017 0.011 -- -- -- 0.085*** 0.017 0.010


△Social non-confidence -- -- -- -- -- -- 0.356*** 0.038 0.042 0.349*** 0.038 0.041

Mediation effect size 5.11% 20.97% 25.33%


Sobel Z 2.434 5.137 --
p 0.015 <0.001 --
△ indicates the change scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
All models were adjusted by grade, academic performance and perceived study pressure.
b regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error.
Mediation effect size: ratio of indirect to total effect.

p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

214
Table 8-7 Summary for the mediation effects of change in psychosocial scale scores on the
association between △EFUI and △CES-D by multilevel linear regression (n=4237)
Model 1 Model 2
b SE β b SE β
△EFUI 0.139*** 0.039 0.055 0.096*** 0.039 0.038

△Social non-confidence -- -- -- 0.358*** 0.038 0.042

Mediation effect size 30.65%


Sobel Z 6.068
p <0.001
△ indicates the change scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
All models were adjusted by grade, academic performance and perceived study pressure.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error.
Mediation effect size: ratio of indirect to total effect.
--: Not applicable.

p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

215
Table 8-8 Testing the suppression effects of change in psychosocial variables on the association between △SFUI and △CES-D (n=4237)
Model 1 Model 2a Model 2b Model 2c
b SE β b SE β b SE β b SE β
△SFUI 0.073 ***
0.017 0.067 0.084 ***
0.016 0.078 0.083 ***
0.016 0.076 0.095 ***
0.017 0.088

△friendship quality -- -- -- -0.621*** 0.040 -0.072 -- -- -- -- -- --


△perceived family support -- -- -- -- -- -- -0.298*** 0.024 -0.035 -- -- --
△perceived friend support -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -0.124*** 0.013 -0.015

Suppression effect -0.011 -0.009 -0.022


Sobel Z -2.946 -3.008 -6.468
p 0.003 0.003 <0.001
△ indicates the change scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
All models were adjusted by grade, academic performance and perceived study pressure.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error.

p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

216
Table 8-9 Testing the suppression effects of change in psychosocial scale scores on the association between △EFUI and △CES-D by multilevel linear
regression (n=4237)
Model 1 Model 2a Model 2b
b SE β b SE β b SE β
△EFUI 0.139*** 0.039 -- 0.184*** 0.038 0.184 0.178*** 0.039 0.178

△perceived family support -- -- -- -0.303*** 0.024 -0.303 -- -- --


△perceived friend support -- -- -- -- -- -0.121*** 0.013 -0.121
Suppression effect -0.036 -0.039
Sobel Z -5.571 -5.518
p <0.001 <0.001
△ indicates the change scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
All models were adjusted by perceived family financial situation, academic performance, and perceived study pressure.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error.
--: Not applicable.

p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

217
Table 8-10 Summary of interaction effects of change in psychosocial variables on the association
between △SFUI and △CES-D (n=4237)
Models without interaction Models with interaction
b SE p b SE p
1a 1b
△SFUI 0.088 0.016 <0.001 0.090 0.026 <0.001
△Friendship quality
Small change ref ref
Improvement -1.944 0.316 <0.001 -1.953 0.316 <0.001
Deterioration 2.781 0.305 <0.001 2,754 0.306 <0.001
Interaction term
△SFUI × Small change -- ref
△SFUI × Improvement 0.024 0.040 0.548
△SFUI × Deterioration -0.029 0.039 0.452
△-2LL (df=2) 1.6
P for △-2LL 0.449

2a 2b
△SFUI 0.077 0.016 <0.001 0.055 0.026 0.035
△Perceived family support
Small change ref ref
Improvement -1.797 0.319 <0.001 -1.788 0.319 <0.001
Deterioration 1.954 0.307 <0.001 1.974 0.307 <0.001
Interaction term
△SFUI × Small change -- ref
△SFUI × Improvement 0.020 0.041 0.629
△SFUI × Deterioration 0.048 0.038 0.208
△-2LL (df=2) 1.5
P for △-2LL 0.472

3a 3b
△SFUI 0.091 0.017 <0.001 0.070 0.025 0.006
△Perceived friend support
Small change ref ref
Improvement -2.074 0.312 <0.001 -2.090 0.313 <0.001
Deterioration 1.180 0.318 <0.001 1.198 0.321 <0.001
Interaction term
△SFUI × Small change -- ref
△SFUI × Improvement 0.045 0.040 0.267
△SFUI × Deterioration 0.029 0.039 0.467
△-2LL (df=2) 1.3
P for △-2LL 0.522

4a 4b
△SFUI 0.082 0.016 <0.001 0.066 0.026 0.011
△Rosenberg self-esteem
Small change ref ref
Improvement -3.512 0.291 <0.001 -3.503 0.291 <0.001
Deterioration 2.962 0.318 <0.001 2.982 0.318 <0.001
Interaction term
△SFUI × Small change -- ref
△SFUI × Improvement 0.012 0.037 0.751
△SFUI × Deterioration 0.043 0.039 0.273
△-2LL (df=2) 1.3
P for △-2LL 0.522

218
(Cont’d) Table 8-10 Summary of interaction of change in psychosocial variables on the
association between △SFUI and △CES-D (n=4237)
Models without interaction Models with interaction
b SE p b SE p
5a 5b
△SFUI 0.070 0.017 <0.001 0.126 0.024 <0.001
△Adolescent-father conflict
Small change ref ref
Improvement -0.681 0.324 0.036 -0.756 0.325 0.020
Deterioration 1.213 0.330 <0.001 1.200 0.329 <0.001
Interaction term
△SFUI × Small change -- ref
△SFUI × Improvement -0.132 0.039 <0.001
△SFUI × Deterioration -0.075 0.041 0.069
△2LL (df=2) 11.7
P for △-2LL 0.003

6a 6b
△SFUI 0.070 0.017 <0.001 0.077 0.025 0.002
△Adolescent-mother conflict
Small change ref ref
Improvement -0.650 0.322 0.044 -0.680 0.323 0.035
Deterioration 1.352 0.318 <0.001 1.349 0.318 <0.001
Interaction term
△SFUI × Small change -- ref
△SFUI × Improvement -0.043 0.040 0.286
△SFUI × Deterioration 0.016 0.039 0.685
△-2LL (df=2) 1.9
P for △-2LL 0.387

7a 7b
△SFUI 0.063 0.016 <0.001 0.109 0.028 <0.001
△Social non-confidence
Small change ref ref
Improvement -1.561 0.309 <0.001 -1.574 0.310 <0.001
Deterioration 1.147 0.320 <0.001 1.159 0.320 <0.001
Interaction term
△SFUI × Small change -- ref
△SFUI × Improvement -0.053 0.039 0.182
△SFUI × Deterioration -0.090 0.041 0.030
△-2LL (df=2) 4.8
P for △-2LL 0.091
△ indicates the change scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity.
All models were adjusted by grade, academic performance, and perceived study pressure.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
SE: Standardized Error.
--: not applicable

219
Table 8-11 Summary of interaction effects of change in psychosocial variables on the association
between △EFUI and △CES-D (n=4237)
Models without interaction Model with interaction
b SE p b SE p
1a 1b
△EFUI 0.151 0.038 <0.001 0.119 0.058 0.041
△Friendship quality
Small change ref ref
Improvement -1.894 0.316 <0.001 -1.879 0.318 <0.001
Deterioration 2.741 0.306 <0.001 2.778 0.309 <0.001
Interaction term
△EFUI × Small change -- ref
△EFUI × Improvement 0.037 0.092 0.688
△EFUI × Deterioration 0.073 0.091 0.418
△-2LL(df=2) 0.6
P for △-2LL 0.741

2a 2b
△EFUI 0.164 0.038 <0.001 0.159 0.061 0.009
△Perceived family support
Small change ref ref
Improvement -1.819 0.319 <0.001 -1.826 0.321 <0.001
Deterioration 1.982 0.307 <0.001 2.015 0.311 <0.001
Interaction term
△EFUI × Small change -- ref
△EFUI × Improvement -0.046 0.095 0.628
△EFUI × Deterioration 0.051 0.090 0.568
△-2LL (df=2) 1.0
P for △-2LL 0.607

3a 3b
△EFUI 0.168 0.038 <0.001 0.167 0.060 0.006
△Perceived friend support
Small change ref ref
Improvement -2.026 0.312 <0.001 -2.028 0.313 <0.001
Deterioration 1.130 0.318 <0.001 1.159 0.325 <0.001
Interaction term
△EFUI × Small change -- ref
△EFUI × Improvement -0.029 0.091 0.748
△EFUI × Deterioration 0.034 0.94 0.715
△-2LL (df=2) 0.4
P for △-2LL 0.819

4a 4b
△EFUI 0.160 0.037 <0.001 0.178 0.059 0.003
△Rosenberg self-esteem
Small change ref ref
Improvement -3.492 0.291 <0.001 -3.500 0.294 <0.001
Deterioration 2.981 0.318 <0.001 2.952 0.322 <0.001
Interaction term
△EFUI × Small change -- ref
△EFUI × Improvement -0.010 0.087 0.912
△EFUI × Deterioration -0.054 0.093 0.556
△-2LL (df=2) 0.4
P for △-2LL 0.819

220
(Cont’d) Table 8-11 Summary of interaction effects of change in psychosocial variables on the
association between △EFUI and △CES-D (n=4237)
Models without interaction Model with interaction
b SE p b SE p
5a 5b
△EFUI 0.134 0.039 <0.001 0.243 0.055 <0.001
△Adolescent-father conflict
Small change ref ref
Improvement -0.671 0.324 0.039 -0.804 0.329 0.014
Deterioration 1.244 0.330 <0.001 1.176 0.332 <0.001
Interaction term
△EFUI × Small change -- ref
△EFUI × Improvement -0.241 0.092 0.009
△EFUI × Deterioration -0.178 0.095 0.063
△-2LL (df=2) 7.9
P for △-2LL 0.019

6a 6b
△EFUI 0.135 0.039 <0.001 0.217 0.057 <0.001
△Adolescent-mother conflict
Small change ref ref
Improvement -0.667 0.323 0.039 -0.737 0.326 0.024
Deterioration 1.365 0.318 <0.001 1.296 0.320 <0.001
Interaction term
△EFUI × Small change -- ref
△EFUI × Improvement -0.136 0.093 0.145
△EFUI × Deterioration -0.161 0.093 0.083
△-2LL (df=2) 3.7
P for △-2LL 0.157

7a 7b
△EFUI 0.108 0.039 0.005 0.073 0.065 0.259
△Social non-confidence
Small change ref ref
Improvement -1.577 0.309 <0.001 -1.478 0.315 <0.001
Deterioration 1.135 0.320 <0.001 1.149 0.321 <0.001
Interaction term
△EFUI × Small change -- ref
△EFUI × Improvement 0.132 0.091 0.148
△EFUI × Deterioration -0.047 0.97 0.627
△-2LL (df=2) 3.9
P for △-2LL 0.142
△ indicates the change scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity.
All models were adjusted by grade, academic performance and perceived study pressure.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
SE: Standardized Error.
--: not applicable

221
Chapter 9 Relationship between social networking use intensity,

addiction, psychosocial factors and depression

9.1 Background

Chapter 7 and Chapter 8 found that social networking use intensity at baseline

and its change in scores during the follow-up period are positively associated with

depressive symptoms among adolescents. However, it is still unclear the extent to

which the detrimental effect of social networking use intensity on depressive

symptoms is due to impulsive and addictive social networking/Internet use (i.e. the

magnitude of mediation effects of ‘SNA’ in specific and IA in general on the pathway

between social networking use intensity and depressive symptoms). Such mediations

are likely to be in operation because of the significant associations between high

social networking use intensity and social networking/Internet addiction.

Some recent studies have focused on addiction to specific online activities, such

as gambling, shopping, or social networking [23, 277]. It highlights the significance of

distinguishing addiction to specific Internet activities (i.e. ‘SNA’) from IA in general.

Indeed, some online activities and behaviors have particularly strong addictive

potential, and could foster the development of addictive behaviors. Social networking

addiction (‘SNA’), which is considered as a subtype of IA [94], is a very recent concern

related with excessive social networking use (e.g. Facebook). It argues that the

capacity for online socializing carries elements that could be addictive [18, 278], such as

the characteristics of online social networking (e.g. anonymous, selective

self-disclosure and convenient communication without the limitations by geographic

distance and time), which could facilitate social interaction online and then function

222
as an attractive factor for addictive Internet use [18].

Some studies have investigated the associations between social networking use

and Internet addictive behaviors (i.e. ‘SNA’ or IA). For example, one study among

college students reported that time spent online and frequency of Facebook use were

significant predictors of IA [127], and a one-year longitudinal study still showed that

higher frequency of social networking use significantly predicted compulsive Internet

use [65]. Similarly, one survey among Turkish college students revealed that students

who frequently used Facebook for social interaction reported higher level of Facebook

addiction [48]. Considering that online social networking is an important medium for

social interaction among adolescents, intensive social networking use might be more

susceptible to ‘SNA’ compared to IA in general, i.e. the effect of social networking

use intensity on ‘SNA’ might be stronger than that on IA. However, there are no

studies that have investigated and compared such differentiating impacts of social

networking use intensity on ‘SNA’ and IA.

The positive association between IA and depressive symptoms has been widely

documented in both cross-sectional [201] and longitudinal [43] studies among

adolescents. ‘SNA’ has also been linked with depressive symptoms among adolescents,

and a significant positive association between severe depression and Facebook

addiction has been reported among college students in a previous study [48]. Although

IA and ‘SNA’ are separately documented to be positively associated with depressive

symptoms in previous studies, there is still no study that investigates and compares

their impacts on depressive symptoms among adolescents in the same study.

Given the aforementioned associations between social networking use and

Internet addictive behaviors (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA), and the associations between Internet

223
addictive behaviors (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA) and depressive symptoms among adolescents,

it is definitely possible that ‘SNA’ and IA would mediate the positive association

between social networking use intensity and depressive symptoms. Our literature

review found no such studies that have estimated and compared these mediation

effects. The present study attempts to fill this knowledge gap.

In the present study, to test mediation effects of change in ‘SNA’ and IA scores

on the association between change in social networking use intensity and change in

depressive symptoms, the hierarchical linear regression models were conducted

following the four-step method of Baron and Kenny’s strategy. As a supplement, path

model analysis was further applied to estimate the indirect effects of change in social

networking use intensity onto change in depressive symptoms via change in ‘SNA’

and IA scores. Path analysis can allow for the examination of a set of relationships

between two or more independent variables and one or more dependent variables [279,
280]
. The advantages of the path analysis model over the correlational and multivariate

regression analyses are that 1) it permits more than one path to predict the dependent

variable (i.e. change in depressive symptoms), and therefore can estimate the indirect

impact via other variables (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA), and 2) the potential causal relationship

parameters can be estimated simultaneously in one single multistage regression

model [280, 281].

Moreover, some researchers argue that mediation processes may include multiple

mediators, and a multiple mediator model could likely provide a more accurate

assessment of mediation effects in the mediational research contexts [143]. Our Chapter

8 has provided the evidences that change in psychosocial factors (i.e. social

non-confidence, and adolescent-mother conflict) partially mediated the associations

224
between change in social networking use intensity and change in depressive

symptoms. Therefore, it is theoretically plausible that multiple mediational processes

might exist in the relationship between change in social networking use intensity and

change in depressive symptoms. Therefore, it is important to estimate the joint

mediation effects via multiple mediators (i.e. SNA, IA, and psychosocial factor) on

the associations between change in social networking use intensity and change in

depressive symptoms.

9.2 Objectives

The first objective is to investigate the associations of changes in SFUI and EFUI

with change in ‘SNA’ (or change in IA) by several linear regression models after the

adjustment of background variables. It is hypothesized that change in SFUI and EFUI

would be positively associated with change in SNA and with change in IA.

The second objective is to investigate the associations of changes in ‘SNA’ and

IA with change in CES-D by multivariate linear regression models. It is hypothesized

that change in ‘SNA’ and IA would be positively associated with change in CES-D,

and moreover, the relative magnitude of the association between change in ‘SNA’ and

change in CES-D would be relatively stronger than that between change in IA and

change in CES-D.

The third objective is to estimate the extent to which the association between

change in SFUI (or change in EFUI) and change in CES-D is mediated by changes in

‘SNA’ and IA based on Baron and Kenny’s statistical method of fitting a set of

hierarchical linear regression models after adjusting for the significant background

factors. Besides, it further compares the magnitude of two individual mediation

225
effects in order to determine whether the mediation effect via change in ‘SNA’ is

significantly larger than that via change in IA.

Fourthly, as previously mentioned, we additionally applied path analysis to

investigate the indirect effects of change in SFUI and EFUI on change in CES-D via

two types of addiction (i.e. change in ‘SNA’ and IA score). A path model in Figure 9-1

was hence synthesized and tested.

+
ΔSFUI ΔSNA +
+

+
+
ΔCES-D

+ +
ΔEFUI ΔIA

Figure 9-1 The hypothesized path model for indirect effects between △SFUI, △EFUI
and △CES-D via △SNA and △IA. △SFUI: Change in Social Function Use Intensity;
△EFUI: Change in Entertainment Function Use Intensity; △SNA: Change in Social
Networking Addiction; △IA: Change in Internet Addiction; △CES-D: Change in
depressive symptoms measured by Center for Epidemiological Study
Scale-Depression. “+” indicates a positive association between two variables

The fifth objective is to estimate the total mediation effects of change in

addiction (‘SNA’ and IA) scores and the significant psychosocial mediators identified

in Chapter 8 altogether for the association between change in SFUI (or change in

EFUI) and change in depressive symptoms.

226
9.3 Statistical methods

9.3.1 Linear regression analysis

The sample and statistical methods for linear regression analysis used in this

Chapter were the same as those mentioned in Chapter 8. The details have been

described in Chapter 8.

Contrasting two mediating effects in multiple mediator models

In order to quantify whether the mediation effect via △SNA was significantly

different from that via △IA for the association between ΔSFUI (or △EFUI) and

△CES-D in multiple-mediator models including ΔSNA and ΔIA as potential mediators.

The contrast was conducted to test such difference among pairs of specific mediation

effects (i.e. ΔSNA vs. ΔIA) based on the following formula [282, 283]:

a1b1 − a2b2
Z=
a1σ b21 + b1σ a21 + a2σ b22 + b2σ a22 − 2a1a2σ b1b2

This difference approximately follows a normal distribution, and the significance of

the difference with 95% CI was then established for hypotheses testing.

9.3.2 Path analysis for indirect effect model

The hypothesized path model in Figure 9-1 was fitted in our data. The

satisfactory “goodness-of-fit” for the hypothesized path model was indicated by the

Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) <0.08 and by the Comparative

Fit Index (CFI) >0.90 [284]. The path coefficients were estimated by the maximum

likelihood method by structure equation modeling method. The indirect effects and

227
95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) were estimated by the 1000 bootstrapping

method. Standardized path coefficients were presented in the path diagrams. The

standardized coefficients measure the relative strength and sign of the effect from an

independent variable to a dependent variable in the model, and can be used to

compare the relative magnitude of the effects of different independent variables in the

path model [285]. Path analyses were performed by AMOS 20.0 and a p<0.05 was

considered statistically significant.

9.4 Results

9.4.1 Associations between ΔSFUI/ΔEFUI and ΔIA/ΔSNA

After adjusting for the significant background factors, △SFUI (b=0.018,

SE=0.003, p<0.001) and △EFUI (b=0.054, SE=0.008, p<0.001) were all significantly

and positively associated with △IA at the 0.05 level (Adjusted models in Table 9-1).

Similar associations were also found between △SFUI and △SNA (b=0.156, SE=0.011,

p<0.001), and between △EFUI and △SNA (b=0.338, SE=0.338, p<0.001) (Adjusted

models in Table 9-1).

We also conducted multivariate linear regression models, which contained both

△SFUI and △EFUI as independent variables to predict △SNA (or △IA) after

adjustment of the significant background factors. It consistently showed that △SFUI

and △EFUI are independently and positively associated with △SNA and with △IA.

When inspecting their standardized regression coefficients within the same

multivariate model, the association between △EFUI and △IA (β=0.087, p<0.001) was

relatively stronger than that between △SFUI and △IA (β=0.044, p=0.011). Contrarily,

the association between △SFUI and △CES-D (β=0.147, p<0.001) was slightly

228
stronger than that between △EFUI and △CES-D (β=0.121, p<0.001) (Multivariate

models in Table 9-1).

9.4.2 Associations between ΔIA, ΔSNA and ΔCES-D

The adjusted models showed that △SNA was significantly associated with

△CES-D (b=0.284, SE=0.022, p<0.001, Table 9-2), and △IA was also significantly

associated with △CES-D (b=0.821, SE=0.077, p<0.001, Table 9-2).

The multivariate model, containing both △SNA and △IA as independent

variables, showed that △SNA and △IA are independently and positively associated

with △CES-D (Table 9-2). And the standardized regression coefficient between

△SNA and △CES-D (β=0.168, p<0.001) was slightly larger than that between △IA

and △CES-D (β=0.116, p<0.001).

9.4.3 Mediation effects via ΔIA and ΔSNA for the association between ΔSFUI and

ΔCES-D

The results in Chapter 8 showed that △SFUI was positively associated with

△CES-D (b=0.073, p<0.001) after adjustment for the significant background factors.

In addition, the above analyses in this chapter showed that △SFUI was also positively

associated with △IA (b=0.018, p<0.001) and with △SNA (b=0.156, p<0.001) (Table

9-3). The requirements for the mediation hypotheses of ΔIA and ΔSNA on the

association between △SFUI and △CES-D were hence fulfilled. The mediation

hypotheses were then tested by entering △IA and △SNA, both individually and

altogether, into the model that contained △SFUI as the independent variable.

When ΔIA was added into the model 1 in Table 9-3, the regression coefficient

229
between △SFUI and △CES-D was still significant, but diminished from 0.073 to

0.059 (Model 1a in Table 9-3). Given that △IA was also significantly associated with

△CES-D, △IA partially mediated the association between △SFUI and △CES-D

(Figure 9-2). The mediation effect via △IA was 0.014 (95% CI: 0.009~0.020, Sobel

Z=5.191, p<0.001). The proportion mediated via △IA was 19.65%.

△IA
0.018*** 0.797***

Direct effect: 0.059***


△SFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0.073***

Figure 9-2 Illustration for mediation effect of change in IA on the association between
change in SFUI and change in CES-D

Similarly, when adding △SNA into model 1 in Table 9-3, the association between

△SFUI and △CES-D became non-significant (Model 1b in Table 9-3). Given that

ΔSNA was still significantly associated with △CES-D, △SNA fully mediated the

association between △SFUI and △CES-D (Figure 9-3). The mediation effect was

0.043 (95%CI: 0.034~0.052, Sobel Z=9.397, p<0.001). The proportion mediated via

△SNA was notably high at 58.98%.

△SNA
0N156*** 0N276***

Direct effect: 0N0S0†


△SFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0N07S***

Figure 9-3 Illustration for mediation effect of change in ‘SNA’ on the association
between change in SFUI and change in CES-D

230
When adding △IA and △SNA altogether into model 1 that contained △SFUI as

the independent variable (Model 1c in Table 9-3), the association between △SFUI and

△CES-D became non-significant. Given that △IA and △SNA were all positively

associated with △CES-D, this indicated that △IA and △SNA altogether mediated the

association between △SFUI and △CES-D (see Figure 9-4). The total mediation effect

via △IA and △SNA was 0.047 (95% CI: 0.038~0.056), which accounted for 64.38%

of total effect. The specific mediation effect via △SNA was 0.036 (95% CI:

0.027~0.045) with proportion mediated equaling to 49.31%; while the specific

mediation effect via △IA was 0.011 (95% CI: 0.006~0.015) with proportion mediated

equaling to 15.07%. The magnitude of the specific mediation effect via △SNA was

significantly larger than the specific mediation effect via △IA (Z=4.923, p<0.001,

95% CI for difference: 0.015~0.036).

△IA
0.018*** 0.586***

Direct effect: 0.026


△SFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0.073***

0.232***
0.156***
△3.A

Figure 9-4 Illustration for mediation effect of change in ‘SNA’ and IA altogether on
the association between change in SFUI and change in CES-D

9.4.4 Mediation effects via ΔIA and ΔSNA for the association between ΔEFUI and

ΔCES-D

The adjusted results in Chapter 8 showed that △EFUI was positively associated

231
with △CES-D (b=0.139, p<0.001), and the results in this chapter showed that △EFUI

is still positively associated with △IA (b=0.054, p<0.001) and with △SNA (b=0.338,

p<0.001). Therefore, the requirements for mediation analyses of ΔIA and ΔSNA on

the association between △EFUI and △CES-D were fulfilled. The mediation

hypotheses were then tested by entering △IA and △SNA, both individually and

altogether, into the model containing △EFUI as the independent variable.

When adding △IA into the model 2 in Table 9-4, the regression coefficient

between △EFUI and △CES-D was still significant, but diminished from 0.139 to

0.096 (Model 2a in Table 9-4). Given that △IA was significantly associated with

△CES-D, △IA partially mediated the association between △EFUI and △CES-D

(Figure 9-5). The mediation effect via △IA was 0.043 (95% CI: 0.028~0.058, Sobel

Z=5.660, p<0.001). The proportion mediated via △IA was 30.96%.

△IA
0.054*** 0.800***

Direct effect: 0.096*


△EFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0.139***

Figure 9-5 Illustration for mediation effect of change in IA on the association between
change in EFUI and change in CES-D

Similarly, when adding △SNA into model 2 in Table 9-4, the association between

△EFUI and △CES-D became non-significant (Model 2b in Table 9-4). Given that

△SNA was still significantly associated with △CES-D, △SNA fully mediated the

association between △EFUI and △CES-D (Figure 9-6). The mediation effect was

0.094 (95% CI: 0.073~0.115; Sobel Z=8.909, p<0.001), and the proportion mediated

232
via △SNA was 67.84%.

△SNA
0NSS8*** 0N279***

Direct effect: 0N044


△EFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0N1S9***

Figure 9-6 Illustration for mediation effect of change in ‘SNA’ on the association
between change in EFUI and change in CES-D

When adding △SNA and △IA altogether into model 2 that contained △EFUI as

the independent variable (Model 2c in Table 9-4), the association between △EFUI and

△CES-D became non-significant. Given that △IA and △SNA were still significantly

associated with △CES-D, this indicated that △IA and △SNA together mediated the

association between △EFUI and △CES-D (Figure 9-7). The total mediation effect via

ΔIA and ΔSNA was 0.111 (95%CI: 0.090~0.133; Z=10.005, p<0.001), which

accounted for 79.86% of total effect. The specific mediation effect via ΔSNA was

0.080 (95% CI: 0.060~0.099), with proportion mediated of 57.55%; while the specific

mediation effect via △IA was 0.032 (95% CI: 0.019~0.044), with proportion mediated

of 23.02%. The magnitude of the specific mediation effect via △SNA was

significantly larger than the specific mediation effect via △IA (Z=3.819, p<0.001,

95% CI for difference: 0.023~0.073).

233
△IA
0.054*** 0.587***

Direct effect: 0.027


△EFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0.139***

0.338*** 0.236***
△3.A

Figure 9-7 Illustration for mediation effect of change in ‘SNA’ and IA altogether on
the association between change in EFUI and change in CES-D

9.4.5 Path analysis for indirect effect model via ΔIA and ΔSNA

The path model in Figure 9-8 showed that △SFUI and △EFUI were positively

associated with △SNA (β=0.148 for △SFUI, p<0.001; β=0.123 for △EFUI, p<0.001),

and △EFUI was positively associated with △IA (β=0.056, p<0.001). In turn, △SNA

was positively associated with △IA (β=0.268, p<0.001), while △SNA and △IA were

positively associated with ΔCES-D (β=0.172 for ΔSNA, p<0.001; β=0.120 for △IA,

p<0.001) (see Figure 9-8).

The path model suggested several indirect paths. The indirect effect of △SFUI

onto △CES-D followed two paths: (1) through △SNA to △CES-D, the indirect effect

was 0.026; and (2) through △SNA to △IA, and then to △CES-D, the indirect effect

was 0.005. The total indirect effect of △SFUI on △CES-D via two paths was 0.31

(95% CI: 0.021~0.042, p=0.002)

Regarding the indirect effect of △EFUI on △CES-D, there were three paths: (1)

through △SNA to △CES-D, the indirect effect of this path was 0.021; (2) through

△SNA to △IA, and then to △CES-D, the indirect effect of this path was 0.004; and (3)

234
through △IA to △CES-D, the indirect effect of this path was 0.007. The total indirect

effect of △EFUI on △CES-D via the three paths was 0.32 (95% CI: 0.22~0.43,

p=0.002).

ΔSFUI 0.148*** ΔSNA


0.123*** 0.172***
0.474*** 0.268*** ΔCES-D
***
0.006 0.120
ΔEFUI 0.056*** ΔIA

Figure 9-8 Path model for indirect effects between △SFUI, △EFUI and ΔCES-D via
△SNA and △IA. Paths shown in dashed lines represent non-significant pathway at the
0.05 level. Standardized path coefficients (β) were presented in the model. Model fit
indexes: χ2 (2) =2.595 with p=0.273, CFI=1.000, IFI=1.000 and RMSEA=0.008.

p<0.10, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001

9.4.6 Combined mediation effects of change in addiction and psychosocial factors

altogether on the associations between ΔSFUI (or ΔEFUI) and ΔCES-D

9.4.6.1 Summary of significant mediators in single mediator models

A summary for the results of all significant mediators from single mediator

models in Chapter 8 (i.e. psychosocial factors) and Chapter 9 (i.e. △SNA and △IA)

was presented in Table 9-5. Change in social non-confidence and change in

adolescent-mother conflict partially mediated the association between change in SFUI

and change in CES-D, with proportion mediated of 20.97% (change in social

non-confidence) and 5.11% (change in adolescent-mother conflict) respectively.

Besides, the association between change in SFUI and change in CES-D was also

mediated by change in SNA and IA, with proportion mediated of 58.98% (change in

235
SNA) and 19.65% (change in IA) respectively.

For the relationship between change in EFUI and change in CES-D, the results in

Chapter 8 showed that this association was partially mediated by change in social

non-confidence, and the proportion mediated was 30.65%. Meanwhile, change in

SNA and IA also mediated this association, with proportion mediated of 67.84%

(change in SNA) and 30.65% (change in IA) respectively.

Given the above significant mediators in single mediator models, the multiple

mediator models were further constructed to estimate the joint mediation effects by

involving two types of addictions (i.e. △SNA and △IA) and the significant

psychosocial factors as potential mediators. The statistical analyses were separately

conducted for △SFUI and △EFUI.

9.4.6.2 Combined mediation for the association between ΔSFUI and ΔCES-D

Given that △IA, △SNA, △social non-confidence, and △adolescent-mother

conflict were individual significant mediators for the association between △SFUI and

△CES-D (Table 9-5), the multiple mediator model was hence structured by involving

these four variables as potential mediators and △SFUI as the independent variable.

The results of the multiple mediator model were shown in Table 9-6 (Model 3a). The

sizes of the total, direct, and mediation effects were summarized in Table 9-7. Figure

9-9 illustrated the multiple mediator model. All four specific mediation effects were

statistically significant. The estimate of total mediation effect via these four mediators

was 0.057 (Z=11.177, p<0.001), and the proportion of the total mediation effect was

notably high at 78.77% (Table 9-7). The proportion mediated was highest for △SNA

(44.45% of total mediation effect), followed by △social non-confidence (17.02%),

236
ΔIA (13.59%), and △adolescent-mother conflict (3.71%).

△3.A

0.156*** 0.208***

0.018*** △IA 0.551***

Direct effect: 0.016


△SFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0.073***

0.043*** 0.289***

△3ocial non-
confidence
0.041*** 0.066***
△Adolescent-
mother conflict

Figure 9-9 Illustration of combined mediation effect via change in IA, SNA, social
non-confidence and adolescent-mother conflict on the association between change in
SFUI and change in CES-D

9.4.6.3 Combined mediation for the association between ΔEFUI and ΔCES-D

For the association between △EFUI and △CES-D, our findings showed that △IA,

△SNA and △social non-confidence were the individual significant mediators (Table

9-5). These three potential mediators were involved in the multiple mediator model.

The results of multiple mediator model are shown in Table 9-6 (Model 3b). The size

of the total, direct, and mediation effects are summarized in Table 9-7. Figure 9-10

illustrates the multiple mediator model via all three mediators for the association

between △EFUI and △CES-D. All three specific mediation effects were statistically

significant. The total mediation effect was 0.139 (Z=11.541, p<0.001, Table 9-7),

which completely accounted for the total effect (proportion mediated=100%). △SNA

237
again had the highest proportion mediated (52.52% of total mediation effect),

followed by △social non-confidence (25.18%), and △IA (22.30%).

△3.A

0.338*** 0.216***

0.054*** △IA 0.571***

Direct effect: -0.004


△EFUI △CES-D
Total effect: 0.139***

0.119*** 0.296***
△3ocial non-
confidence

Figure 9-10 Illustration of joint mediation effect via change in IA, SNA and social
non-confidence on the association between change in SFUI and change in CES-D

9.4.7 Summary for mediation effects of all mediation models

Table 9-8 summarized the proportion of mediation effects (% mediated) of all

mediation models for the relationships between change in SFUI (or change in EFUI)

and change in depressive symptoms in Chapter 8 and Chapter 9. Change in SNA,

change in IA, change in social non-confidence, and change in adolescent-mother

conflict were individually significant mediators for the association between change in

SFUI and change in depressive symptoms. The proportions mediated were 58.98%,

19.65%, 20.97% and 5.11% respectively. In the multiple mediator model, these four

mediators altogether mediated 78.77% of the relationship between change in SFUI

and change in depressive symptoms (Table 9-8).

Change in SNA, change in IA and change in social non-confidence were

238
individually significant mediators for the association between change in EFUI and

change in depressive symptoms, and the proportions mediated were 67.84%, 30.96%

and 30.65% respectively. These three mediators altogether completely mediated the

association between change in EFUI and change in depressive symptoms (Table 9-8).

9.5 Discussion

9.5.1 Associations between change in social networking use intensity and change

in Internet addictive behaviors

In the present study, it was found that increased social networking use intensity

(both SFUI and EFUI) are significant risk factors of increased ‘SNA’ and IA during

the follow-up period. The positive association between change in social networking

use intensity and change IA score found in the present study is consistent with a

previous two-wave longitudinal study among adolescents from Van Rooij AJ and

Schoenmakers TM [65], which reported that change in frequency of online social

networking use was positively associated with change in IA score. However, that

study measured social networking use intensity only by one item (i.e. frequency per

week), while our study assessed such use by a multi-item validated scale (Chapter 6).

From this perspective, the results in our study are more informative than that from

Van Rooij AJ and Schoenmakers TM [65]. The findings in the present study are also

similar with the results of previous cross-sectional studies that reported that frequency

and hours per week spent on social networking use were positively associated with

SNA [48] and IA [127, 277].

Moreover, results from multivariate regression model showed that change in

SFUI (β=0.147) was slightly more predictive of ‘SNA’ than change in EFUI (β=0.121).

239
This is understandable as adolescents mainly use social networking services for social

purpose and interactions (e.g. communicating with friends, maintaining relationships,

self-presentation). The reverse magnitude was observed for prediction of IA by

change in SFUI and change in EFUI, showing that change in EFUI (β=0.087) is more

predictive of IA than change in SFUI (β=0.044). This result is similar with reports of a

previous study [65], finding that online entertainment (i.e. online gaming) has strongest

association with IA as compared to other Internet applications (i.e. chatting, blogging,

MSN use). It indicates that entertainment function use plays a significant role in the

development of IA. One possible explanation is that IA in general as a compulsive

behavior usually requires more time spent online. Entertainment function use

generally requires a large amount of commitment and time investments in order to

achieve imminent goals which may then contribute to the development of maladaptive

behaviors and addiction [277]. Besides, excessive of online gaming, information

seeking and video watching/downloading have great impact on IA in general[24].

These activities (e.g. browsing information, playing games watching video) can be

conducted on social networking platforms, and also the key elements of entertainment

function use intensity subscale that was used in the present study.

These findings highlight the importance and necessity of early intervention for

reducing social networking use intensity in order to prevent the condition progressing

to addictive status, as intensive social networking use renders adolescents vulnerable

for addiction (both ‘SNA’ and IA). Both social function and entertainment function

use could cause Internet addictive behaviors, and therefore need to be properly

controlled simultaneously for adolescents in future intervention programs.

240
9.5.2 Associations between change in ‘SNA’, change in IA and change in

depressive symptoms

The hypothesis that change in ‘SNA’ and change in IA would be positively

associated with change in depressive symptoms was supported by the multivariate

regression model. These positive findings are in accordance with those findings in the

Chapter 5 with reporting that change in ‘SNA’/IA status (categorical variables,

persistent ‘SNA’/IA and incident ‘SNA’/IA) significantly predicted incidence of

depression by modeling approach II, and are also similar with those reports of

previous studies [43, 105, 197, 286].

The results further revealed that the association between change in ‘SNA’ and

change in depressive symptoms (β=0.168) is slightly stronger than that between

change in IA and change in depressive symptoms (β=0.116) in the same model. These

findings indicate that ‘SNA’ in specific and IA in general are both significant risk

factors of depressive symptoms among adolescents. However, the ‘SNA’ in specific is

more predictive as compared to IA in general for depressive symptoms. It might be

that excessive involvement in specific online activities (i.e. social networking

activities) is more devastating for adolescents’ mental health rather than IA in general.

Therefore, it is more important to tailor and prevent ‘SNA’. It is also suggestive for

future research that measure specific Internet addiction (e.g. ‘SNA’) might be more

appropriate and informative as risk factors of depression when comparing to IA in

general. Intervention programs aiming to reduce the level of depressive symptoms

should make effort to prevent both types of Internet addiction (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA), but

more attention should be paid to ‘SNA’ in specific.

241
9.5.3 Mediation effects for the association between change in social networking

use intensity and change in depressive symptoms via change in ‘SNA’ and change

in IA

The present chapter further estimated the mediation effects of change in

addictive scores (both ‘SNA’ and IA) on the associations between change in social

networking use intensity and change in depressive symptoms. Change in ‘SNA’ fully

mediated the association between change in SFUI and change in depressive symptoms

(proportion mediated=58.98%), and it also fully mediated the association between

change in EFUI and change in depressive symptoms (proportion mediated=67.84%).

Whereas change in IA only partially mediated these two associations (proportion

mediated=19.65% for change in SFUI and 30.96% for change in EFUI). Moreover,

multiple mediator models revealed that change in ‘SNA’ and IA altogether mediated

64.38% of the effect of change in SFUI on change in depressive symptoms, while

they altogether mediated 79.86% of the effect of change in EFUI on change in

depressive symptoms.

These findings provide evidences that increased social networking use might

make adolescents at high-risk of suffering from ‘SNA’ and IA, which might in turn be

detrimental for adolescents’ depressive symptoms. However, ‘SNA’ in specific is a

more important mediator given that the higher proportion of mediation effect it

carried as compared to IA in general. This discrepancy may be accounted for by the

fact that measurement for social networking use intensity in the present study more

exclusively related to aspects of ‘SNA’ in specific as compared to IA in general. The

mediation findings of change in ‘SNA’ and IA on the associations between change in

social networking use intensity and change in depressive symptoms indicate that

242
interventions for preventing/reducing SNA and IA may alleviate the impacts of social

networking use intensity on depressive symptoms. Prevention on ‘SNA’ and IA are

hence warranted in future research.

9.5.4 Path model for indirect effects via ‘SNA’ and IA

The hypothesized path model is well supported by our data. It gives new insights

into relationships of social networking use intensity and associated addictive

behaviors on depressive symptoms, and presents more informative pathways for such

associations. First, the indirect effect of change in SFUI on change in depressive

symptoms was through two paths: via change in SNA, and via change in SNA to

change in IA. The indirect effect of change in EFUI on change depressive symptoms

was through three paths. The first two paths were the same as those between ΔSFUI

and ΔCES-D. The third path was the indirect effect justly via change in IA, which is

specified for the association between ΔEFUI and ΔCES-D.

There are some implications for these findings. First, ‘SNA’ in specific plays an

important role on the potential causal chain between social networking use intensity

and IA in general, as four of the five paths initially through ‘SNA’ (either via or not

via IA to depression), but only one path directly through IA to depression. Second,

‘SNA’ in specific might temporally precede IA in general. One explanation is that

adolescents might be attracted to specific online activities firstly (i.e. social

networking), and the potential gratification received from such use reinforces the

attachment to social networking and progresses to ‘SNA’. Addictive social networking

use then amplifies the severity of IA. Third, ‘SNA’ and IA as intervening variables

between social networking use intensity and depressive symptoms have significant

devastating effects on depressive symptoms. Future intervention programs should

243
make more efforts to block the potential causal chain from social networking use to

depression among adolescents by reducing the addictive tendency. However, more

effort should be given to ‘SNA’ prevention.

9.5.5 Combined mediation effects via Internet addictive behaviors and

psychosocial factors altogether

Results of the multiple mediator models showed that the combination of change

in addiction (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA) and significant psychosocial mediators altogether

explained the mediating mechanisms very well for the relationship between change in

social networking use intensity and change in depressive symptoms. More than 78%

of the effect from change in SFUI to change in depressive symptoms is attributable to

the joint effects on change in addiction (i.e. ‘SNA’ and IA) and change in psychosocial

factors (i.e. increased social non-confidence and adolescent-mother conflict); while

the impact of change in EFUI on change in depressive symptoms were completely

through its joint impacts on change in addiction (‘SNA’ and IA) and change in social

non-confidence.

These findings provide strong evidence for understanding the mechanisms on

how and why social networking use intensity could increase adolescents’ level of

depressive symptoms, and gives robust informative evidences for future intervention

design. First, the results firstly evidence-based confirm that there are multiple indirect

pathways that carry the detrimental effect of social networking use onto depressive

symptoms. On one hand, higher levels of social networking use could increase

adolescents’ susceptibility to addiction (i.e. both ‘SNA’ and IA, but especially for

‘SNA’ in specific); on the other hand, higher levels of social networking use could

devastate adolescents’ psychosocial status (i.e. weakening social confidence in social

244
contexts, disharmonious adolescent-parents relationship). The pathological social

networking/Internet use and deteriorative psychosocial relationships further amplify

the level of depressive symptoms among adolescents.

Second, future intervention programs should more specifically emphasize these

intervening components in order to effectively reduce the detrimental effect of social

networking use on depressive symptoms among adolescents. For instance, on the one

hand, intervention campaigns should improve psychosocial status of adolescents who

are social networking users (i.e. strength social confidence, improve family

relationship), and on the other hand, it is also important to simultaneously encourage

adolescents lessened involvement in online social networking to prevent addictive

tendency. These intervention strategies exclusively targeting significant intervening

variables could theoretically and effectively block the negative influence of social

networking use on adolescents’ mental health.

9.5.6 Limitations

Some limitations in this Chapter need to be considered with caution. First, only

one type of specific IA (i.e. ‘SNA’) was involved. Internet addictive behaviors,

however, cover a broad-spectrum of maladaptive behaviors (e.g. cybersex, chatting,

games and gambling). These online activities also have the potential for excessive use

and may lead to symptoms similar to addiction. Future studies are warranted to

include other specific forms of Internet addiction (e.g. online gaming addiction, online

gambling addiction or other social media addiction). Second, our multiple mediator

models specified that social networking use intensity is related to multiple potential

mediators that are altogether related to the outcome, but their interrelationships

between potential mediators are not considered due to complexity and the limitations

245
by time-points observed in our study. There may have been other potential multiple

mediator models, such as social networking use increases ‘SNA’/IA, increased

‘SNA’/IA further decreases social confidence, and ultimately increases levels of

depressive symptoms, or contrastingly, social networking use decreases social

confidence, and socially non-confident adolescents further prefer interactions through

social networking/Internet rather than offline interactions, which ultimately increases

levels of depressive symptoms, etc. Future longitudinal studies with three or more

observations are highly warranted to clarify these complex interrelationships of

potential mediators on the relationship between social networking use intensity and

depressive symptoms among adolescents.

9.5.7 Conclusion

In summary, the findings of this chapter reveal that change in social function use

intensity and entertainment function use intensity were significantly associated with

change in IA and with ‘SNA’ in the multivariate analyses. change in ‘SNA’ and IA

were significant mediators for the associations between change in social networking

use intensity and change in depressive symptoms, and change in ‘SNA’ in specific

was a more important mediator as compared to change in IA in general. Moreover, the

underlying mechanisms for the relationship between change in social networking use

intensity and change in depressive symptoms were well explored by the combination

of change in addiction (‘SNA’ and IA) and significant psychosocial factors (i.e. social

non-confidence, adolescent-mother conflict), which explained the large proportion of

the effects of change in social networking use intensity on change in depressive

symptoms. These findings provide insights for developing interventions for reducing

levels of depressive symptoms. Future interventions should effort to make early

246
interventions for reducing social networking use intensity, and simultaneously

improve adolescents’ psychosocial status in order to prevent their detrimental effects

on mental health among adolescents.

247
Table 9-1 Associations of ΔSFUI and ΔEFUI on ΔIA/ΔSNA among longitudinal sample (n=4237)
DV=ΔIA DV=ΔSNA
b SE β b SE β
Univariate models
ΔSFUI 0.018*** 0.003 0.086 0.156*** 0.011 0.205
ΔEFUI 0.054 ***
0.008 0.108 0.338 ***
0.027 0.191

Adjusted models
ΔSFUI 0.018*** 0.003 0.085 0.156*** 0.011 0.204
ΔEFUI 0.054*** 0.008 0.108 0.338*** 0.027 0.191

Multivariate models
ΔSFUI 0.009* 0.004 0.044 0.112*** 0.012 0.147
ΔEFUI 0.043 ***
0.009 0.087 0.214*** 0.030 0.121
△ indicates change scale scores by subtracting baseline score from corresponding follow-up score.
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity; SNA: Social
Networking Addiction; IA: Internet Addiction.
Adjusted models: adjusted by grade, academic performance, and perceived study pressure.
Multivariate model: ΔSFUI and ΔEFUI were entered together in the model after adjustment of
background variables.
b: regression coefficients.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error of the regression coefficients.
*
p<0.05; *** p<0.001

248
Table 9-2 Associations between change in ‘SNA’, change in IA and change in CES-D among
longitudinal sample (n=4237)
Univariate Adjusted Multivariate model
b SE β b SE β b SE β
ΔSNA 0.290*** 0.022 0.203 0.284*** 0.022 0.199 0.239*** 0.022 0.168
ΔIA 0.841*** 0.077 0.166 0.821*** 0.077 0.162 0.590*** 0.079 0.116
△ indicates the change scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
SNA: Social Networking Addiction; IA: Internet Addiction; CES-D: Center for Epidemiological
Studies-Depression Scale.
Adjusted models: adjusted by grade, academic performance, and perceived study pressure.
Multivariate model: ΔSNA and ΔIA were both entered in the model after adjustment of
background variables.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error of the regression coefficient.

p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

249
Table 9-3 Hierarchical regression models for testing mediation effects of ΔIA and ΔSNA on the association between ΔSFUI and ΔCES-D among longitudinal sample
(n=4237)
Model 1 Model 1a Model 1b Model 1c
b SE β b SE β b SE β b SE β
ΔSFUI 0.073 ***
0.017 0.067 0.059 ***
0.016 0.054 0.030 †
0.017 0.028 0.026 0.017 0.024
ΔIA -- -- -- 0.797*** 0.077 0.157 -- -- 0.586*** 0.079 0.115
ΔSNA -- -- -- -- -- -- 0.276 ***
0.022 0.194 0.232 ***
0.023 0.163
Sobel Z -- 5.191 9.397 9.997
p -- <0.001 <0.001 <0.001
△ indicates the change in scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity, IA: Internet Addiction, SNA: Social Networking Addiction; CES-D: Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale.
All models were adjusted by grade, academic performance, and perceived study pressure.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error.
--: Not applicable

p<0.10; * p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

250
Table 9-4 Hierarchical regression models for testing mediation effects of ΔIA and ΔSNA on the association between ΔEFUI and ΔCES-D among longitudinal sample
(n=4237)
Model 2 Model 2a Model 2b Model 2c
b SE β b SE β b SE β b SE β
ΔEFUI 0.139 ***
0.039 0.055 0.096 *
0.038 0.038 0.044 0.039 0.018 0.027 0.039 0.011
ΔIA -- -- -- 0.800*** 0.077 0.157 -- -- -- 0.587*** 0.079 0.115
ΔSNA -- -- -- -- -- -- 0.279 ***
0.022 0.196 0.236 ***
0.023 0.166
Sobel Z -- 5.660 8.909 10.005
p -- <0.001 <0.001 <0.001
△ indicates the change in scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity, IA: Internet Addiction, SNA: Social Networking Addiction; CES-D: Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression
Scale.
All models were adjusted by grade, academic performance, and perceived study pressure.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error.
--: Not applicable
*
p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

251
Table 9-5 Summary for all significant mediators in single mediator models for the association
between change in SFUI (or change in EFUI) and change in CES-D
Total Mediation 95%CI for
Z % Med.$
effect effect mediation effect
ΔSFUI → ΔCES-D 0.073***
Addiction variables
Via △IA 0.014*** 0.009, 0.020 5.191 19.65%
Via △SNA 0.043*** 0.034, 0.052 9.397 58.98%
Psychosocial factors
Via △social non-confidence 0.015*** 0.009, 0.021 5.137 20.97%
Via △adolescent-mother conflict 0.004* 0.001, 0.007 2.434 5.11%

ΔEFUI → ΔCES-D 0.139***


Addiction variables
Via △IA 0.043*** 0.028, 0.058 5.660 30.96%
Via △SNA 0.094*** 0.073, 0.115 8.909 67.84%
Psychosocial factor
Via △social non-confidence 0.043*** 0.029, 0.056 6.068 30.65%
△ indicates the change in scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity; IA: Internet
Addiction, SNA: Social Networking Addiction; CES-D: Center for Epidemiological
Studies-Depression Scale
$
: Proportion mediated, calculated by dividing absolute value of mediation effect by absolute value
of total effect.
*
p<0.05, *** p<0.001

252
Table 9-6 Multivariate linear regression models for testing the joint mediation effects of addiction
and psychosocial factors on the associations between ΔSFUI/ΔEFUI and ΔCES-D among
longitudinal sample (n=4237)
Model 3a Model 3b
b SE β b SE β
Social networking use
△SFUI 0.016 0.016 0.014 -- -- --
△EFUI -- -- -- -0.004 0.038 -0.0002

Addictive variables
△IA 0.551*** 0.078 0.108 0.571*** 0.079 0.112
△SNA 0.208*** 0.023 0.146 0.216*** 0.023 0.152

Psychosocial factors
△Social non-confidence 0.289*** 0.037 0.034 0.296*** 0.038 0.035
△Adolescent-mother conflict 0.066 ***
0.017 0.008 -- -- --
△ indicates the change in scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity, IA: Internet Addiction, SNA: Social Networking
Addiction; CES-D: Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale.
All models were adjusted by grade, academic performance, and perceived study pressure.
b: regression coefficients obtained by multilevel linear regression models.
β: Standardized regression coefficients.
SE: Standardized Error.
--: Not applicable
***
p<0.001

253
Table 9-7 Summary of total, direct, and combined mediation effects of multiple mediator models
on change in depressive symptoms (n=4237)
95%CI for %
Estimates Z
estimates Med.$
ΔSFUI → ΔCES-D
Total effect 0.073*** 0.041, 0.106 -- --

Direct effect 0.016 -0.017, 0.048 -- --

Total mediated effect 0.057*** 0.047, 0.068 11.177 78.77


△IA 0.010*** 0.006, 0.014 4.573 13.59
△SNA 0.032*** 0.024, 0.041 7.625 44.45
△Social non-confidence 0.012*** 0.007, 0.017 4.829 17.02
△Adolescent-mother conflict 0.003* 0.0003, 0.005 2.235 3.71

ΔSFUI → ΔCES-D
Total effect 0.139*** 0.063, 0.215 3.590 --

Direct effect -0.004 -0.076, 0.075 -0.01 --

Total mediated effect 0.139*** 0.115, 0.163 11.541 100.00


△IA 0.031*** 0.019, 0.043 4.933 22.30
△SNA
***
0.073 0.054, 0.092 7.512 52.52
△Social non-confidence 0.035*** 0.023, 0.047 5.786 25.18
△ indicates the change in scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity; IA: Internet
Addiction, SNA: Social Networking Addiction; CES-D: Center for Epidemiological
Studies-Depression Scale.
$
: Proportion mediated, calculated by dividing absolute values of mediation effect (specific or
total) by absolute value of total effect.
--: Not applicable
*
p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001

254
Table 9-8 Summary for mediation effects (% mediated) of all mediation models in Chapter 8 and
Chapter 9
Mediation models % Mediated $
ΔSFUI → ΔCES-D
Addiction variables
Via △IA 19.65
Via △SNA 58.98
Via △IA + △SNA 64.38

Psychosocial factors
Via △social non-confidence 20.97
Via △adolescent-mother conflict 5.11
Via △social non-confidence + △adolescent-mother conflict 25.33

Combined addiction and psychosocial variables


Via △IA + △SNA + △social non-confidence + △adolescent-mother conflict 78.77

ΔEFUI → ΔCES-D
Addiction variables
Via △IA 30.96
Via △SNA 67.84
Via △IA + △SNA 79.86

Psychosocial factor
Via △social non-confidence 30.65

Combined addiction and psychosocial variables


Via △IA + △SNA + △social non-confidence 100.00
△ indicates the change in scale scores using follow-up minus baseline.
SFUI: Social Function Use Intensity; EFUI: Entertainment Function Use Intensity; IA: Internet
Addiction; SNA: Social Networking Addiction; CES-D: Center for Epidemiological
Studies-Depression Scale.
$
: Proportion mediated, calculated by dividing absolute value of mediation effect by absolute value
of total effect.

255
Chapter 10 Conclusions

10.1 Summary and interpretation of key findings

10.1.1 Depression and Internet addictive behaviors epidemic among adolescents

The high depression prevalence and incidence were observed among adolescents

in our study. The depression prevalence was respectively 24.6% at baseline and 26.7%

at follow-up, showing a significant increase over the nine-month interval. Moreover,

the depression incidence was highly 23.4 per 100-person years. These figures signify

that depression epidemic is serious and likely to worsen over time. Effective

prevention and interventions aiming to reduce depression is highly warranted among

adolescents in China. Mental healthcare prevention services may be needed to

promote adolescents’ healthy development through routine school health education.

Internet addiction (IA) in general is a real threat for adolescents’ healthy

development that has drawn extensive concern among public health researchers in the

last two decades. In this study, around 6.6% of participants were classified with IA at

baseline, and the IA prevalence mildly increased to 7.3% at follow-up. During the

nine-month period, the IA incidence was approximately 6.77 per 100-person-years. In

particular, online social networking use as a new Internet-related phenomenon has

exponentially increased among adolescents in recent years. Adolescents have become

important subpopulations that are disproportionately influenced by online social

networking use. Along with excessive social networking use, a high prevalence of

‘SNA’ was found both at baseline (13.7%) and follow-up (13.6%). The incidence of

‘SNA’ was 12.80 per 100-person-years during the nine-month interval. The findings

256
determined that Internet addictive conditions are continuously worsening and are

serious problems among this population. Similar to IA in general, ‘SNA’ as another

newly raised potential public health issue should be given significant attention in the

future research.

10.1.2 Causal directions between Internet addictive behaviors and depression

This study suggests that the longitudinal relationships between Internet addictive

behaviors (both ‘SNA’ and IA) and depression are likely to be bi-directional and

reciprocal. That is, adolescents with depression have higher risk of developing

Internet addictive behaviors, and in turn, adolescents who suffer from symptoms of

Internet addictive behaviors further aggravate their level of depressive symptoms.

These findings suggest a vicious dysfunctional circle for the relationships between

Internet addictive behaviors and depression among adolescents. In such a condition,

interventions focusing on Internet addictive behaviors without paying attention to

psychiatric disorders (i.e. depression), and vice versa, might result in inefficient or

even failure of interventions. Future intervention programs should therefore make

strong efforts to reduce both depressive disorder and Internet addictive behaviors

together. Co-treatment/prevention strategy may be appropriate for optimal effects. For

adolescents suffering from depression, alleviating emotional difficulty may be a more

important mechanism underlying Internet addictive behaviors development. And

contrastingly, for adolescents with Internet addictive behaviors, being isolated from

real-world activities and interactions caused by excessive Internet/social networking

involvement may be boosting depression development. Furthermore, adolescents

might be comorbidity of depression and Internet addictive behaviors. These

mechanisms need to be considered in future intervention studies aiming to prevent

257
depression and Internet addictive behaviors. In this sense, it could be useful to work

with different types of cognitive distortions and dysfunctional behaviors that could be

maintaining and exacerbating these problems.

10.1.3 A validated scale for assessing social networking use intensity

Social networking use is a relatively new phenomenon, but speedily attracts high

popularity among adolescents. There is, however, a scarcity of formally validated

instruments to measure social networking use intensity, which partly hinders the

research to explore the health impacts of social networking use for adolescents. The

present study is the first to formally develop and validate a comprehensive instrument

to assess social networking use intensity by considering diverse social networking

activities and functions without bonding to a specific platform. The 14-item online

Social Networking Activity Intensity Scale (SNAIS) developed in this study showed

acceptable psychometric properties. The two subscales, SFUI and EFUI, have been

definitively identified and cross-validated with internal and external validity. Such

instrument that follows the standard scale development procedure could facilitate and

serve future relevant research aiming to explore the impact of social networking use

intensity on adolescents’ mental health outcomes both in China and worldwide.

10.1.4 The impact of social networking use intensity on depression

By applying SFUI and EFUI subscales as the index of social networking use

intensity, higher levels of social networking use intensity at baseline (both SFUI and

EFUI) was a significant risk predictor of incident depression, even after accounting

for baseline level of depressive symptoms. The results suggest that the causal effect of

social networking use intensity on depression does not vary in terms of the two broad

258
types of functions that adolescents mainly conduct on such platforms. As a significant

risk factor, social networking use should be paid more attention in depression

prevention and intervention programs targeting adolescents. Early interventions

should be considered to reduce social networking use intensity as a means of

preventing depression.

10.1.5 Impact of social networking use intensity on psychosocial status

This is the first longitudinal study to investigate the effects of social networking

use intensity onto diverse psychosocial factors among adolescents. This study found

that increased social networking use intensity is positively associated with increased

sense of perceived social support and friendship quality during the follow-up period.

In addition, increased social networking use intensity is positively associated with

increased adolescent-mother conflict and social non-confidence during the follow-up

period. These results confirm the direct impacts of social networking use on

adolescents’ psychosocial statuses. These important psychosocial effects of social

networking use intensity highlight the necessity and attention for future intervention

studies.

10.1.6 Underlying mechanisms for the relationship between social networking

use intensity and depression

It is important to understand the underlying mechanisms on how and why social

networking use increases the level of depressive symptoms among adolescents. The

present study firstly demonstrates some potential mechanisms underlying the

relationship between social networking use intensity and depressive symptoms among

adolescents. The results of multiple-mediator models reveal the first evidence that

259
change in addiction (‘SNA’ and IA) and significant psychosocial factors (i.e. social

non-confidence and adolescent-mother conflict) altogether mediated a large

proportion of the effect of change in SFUI on change in depressive symptoms

(proportion mediated=78%). Meanwhile, the effect of change in EFUI on change in

depressive symptoms was completely mediated by change in addiction (i.e. ‘SNA’ and

IA) and change in social non-confidence. The mediational findings indicate that

higher intensity of social networking use could increase users’ addictive tendency to

Internet and social networking, and could also decrease their psychosocial status (i.e.

social confidence and family relationships), leading to increased level of depressive

symptoms.

Additionally, a significant moderation effect of increased adolescent-father

conflict was observed for the relationship between change in social networking use

intensity and change in depressive symptoms. These results showed that improvement

in adolescent-father relationship could significantly buffer the detrimental effect of

increased social networking use intensity on increased depressive symptoms.

Furthermore, it was interesting to find that change in friendship quality and

change in perceived social support slightly but significantly suppressed the

association between change in social networking use intensity and change in

depressive symptoms. The findings reveal that increased perceived social support and

friendship quality partially compensate the harmful effect of increased social

networking use intensity onto increased depressive symptoms. However, the

suppression effects observed in the present study are relatively small. Future research

is needed to investigate and confirm such negative suppression effects among this

population.

260
These results contribute to our understanding of the underlying mechanisms for

the relationship between social networking use intensity and depressive symptoms.

This information is important and helpful for developing effective intervention

programs in future research. These psychosocial factors and addictive behaviors

should be taken into consideration when formulating intervention programs targeting

to improve adolescents’ mental health.

10.2 Implications of the study

10.2.1 Intervention programs for reducing depressive symptoms

The positive prediction of social networking use intensity on depression and the

identified underlying mechanisms suggest some new directions for implementing

effective intervention programs to prevent or reduce depressive symptoms among

adolescents who are social networking users. This direction is particularly important

under the condition of high popularity of social networking use among adolescents.

First, intervention programs should include components to strengthen

adolescents’ confidence in social situations and to improve their relationships with

parents, such as through skills training programs that encourage positive coping

strategies. Coping effectiveness training should emphasize positive appraisal. The

optimal use of positive coping methods is effective in improving confidence in social

situations and adolescent-parents relationships.

Second, early intervention is important and necessary to reduce intensive and

excessive social networking use, as intensive social networking use renders

adolescents vulnerable for addiction. Therefore, adolescents’ social networking use in

future intervention programs should be properly restrained, rather than waiting for

261
Internet/social networking addiction to occur.

Intervention strategies in future research exclusively targeting these significant

mediation components (i.e. social non-confidence, adolescent-parents relationships,

and Internet addictive behaviors) would effectively block the negative impacts of

social networking use on adolescents’ mental health.

10.2.2 Intervention programs for preventing Internet addictive behaviors

Internet addictive behaviors are associated with wide range of adverse mental

and physical problems, and have become a noticeable public health issue. Policy

makers and health workers who work with adolescents must aid in the elimination of

these harmful behaviors. Unlike other addictive behaviors (e.g. alcohol use, gambling),

Internet and social networking use is now an integral aspect of modern life. It is not

suitable or sustainable to require students to abstain completely from Internet and

social networking use. As recommended by Young [287], the primary objective of

Internet addictive behaviors intervention should be abstinence from problematic

online applications. Moderated and controlled use of Internet/social networking is the

most appropriate to prevent Internet addictive behaviors. Moreover, school and public

health educators should encourage students to have more interactions and activities

with peers, family members and others offline. These strategies could improve social

adjustment and reduce the dependency on Internet/social networking by increasing

offline social activities.

In addition, health care providers who work with adolescents may find it useful

to evaluate social networking or other social media exposures during routine health

education sections. When large amounts of social networking use are reported, a

262
further detailed assessment of Internet addictive behaviors should be considered for

early intervention and treatment.

10.2.3 Collaboration and integration of Internet addictive behaviors and

depression intervention

The bi-directional and highly co-occurred relationships between Internet

addictive behaviors and depression highlight the necessity for integration and

collaboration of strategies to prevent these two public health challenges. Collaboration

and integration of these intervention strategies can improve efficiency,

cost-effectiveness, and health outcomes. For those students who seek help regarding

addictive Internet use, health care workers and professionals should consider whether

they have depressive psychopathology simultaneously, which may pinpoint the root of

the problem and can help to choose appropriate intervention strategies. Screening for

depression is hence recommended for these students. If depression is not found,

effective interventions should specifically focus on reducing addictive behaviors more

effectively. Meanwhile, efforts should be made to prevent the presence of depression

attributed to Internet/social networking addiction, as adolescents who use the Internet

or social networking pathologically are at high-risk group of developing depression in

the absence of effective interventions for Internet addictive behaviors. Such early

interventions and prevention by targeting at-risk groups of adolescents is effective in

reducing the burden of depression [288]. Alternatively, if comorbidities of Internet

addictive behaviors and depression are present, it may indicate that these students

treat Internet/social networking as a more comfortable and secure way to alleviate

their depressive mood as compared to seeking offline support and help. Interventions

should target at both issues to maximize the treatment outcomes. Effective

263
interventions including specific components to reduce depressed mood, such as

improving social self-efficacy, social skills, and coping strategies training, may help

these students to involve social interactions in the real-life settings and reduce the

dependency on the Internet and social networking. It is important to evaluate and treat

these psychiatric symptoms in adolescents with Internet addictive behaviors. To treat

these disorders concurrently could provide benefits to interventions for Internet

addictive behaviors among adolescents.

On the other hand, for those students with depressive symptoms, brief screenings

of Internet addictive behaviors may be appropriate as a standard battery of health

questions at care settings. Explicitly screening would aid in the early identification of

individuals at risk for potential Internet addictive behaviors, to whom timely and

appropriate counseling and interventions could be provided to reduce harms and

related complications. Furthermore, since depressive symptoms might predispose

Internet addictive behaviors, appropriate treatment for depressive symptoms might

prevent the emergence of Internet addictive behaviors. A strategy of integrated

therapy for both problems in dual diagnosis could be provide more consistent

treatment effects [289].

10.2.4 Assessment for social networking use intensity

This study provides a useful and easy-administered instrument with acceptable

psychometric properties, Social Networking Activity Intensity Scale (SNAIS), for

assessing social networking use intensity. The SNAIS takes diverse social networking

activities and functions into consideration, and generalizes the features that are

common to standard social networking use. The two subscales of SNAIS reflect the

main functions of social networking use that adolescents conduct on social

264
networking platforms. Its application could catalyze the progression of relevant

studies on the impacts of social networking use intensity on mental health outcomes

among adolescents in China and worldwide.

10.2.5 Methodological considerations in longitudinal causal-effect models

The statistical methodologies that are applied to explore causal-effects in

longitudinal observational studies are diverse and not in consensus. Our results

suggest that statistical modeling by taking change in pathological status over time into

consideration is a potential methodological improvement in exploring the

causal-effects in longitudinal studies, especially for those pathological conditions,

such as depression and Internet addictive behaviors, with significant fluctuations

during the study period. Therefore, statistical analysis should consider such

pathological status change into modeling specifications in future longitudinal research.

Moreover, it may be better for robust causal directional inference to combine results

from multiple statistical modeling methods.

In addition, some other alternative statistical modeling methods, such as

cross-lagged SEM, could be considered in future research. However, caution should

be given that this method is disadvantageous in controlling many potential

confounding factors and in handling categorical variables, which are the main reasons

why we did not apply the SEM statistical method in the present study. Future

longitudinal studies aiming to explore causal relationships between Internet addictive

behaviors and depression might properly consider such method as an alternative to

supply direct comparison with our findings to give a more convincing picture for

these research questions.

265
10.3 Limitations of the study

10.3.1 Study design

The nine-month observational time is a relatively short longitudinal period,

which is no long enough to verify the long-term effects of online social networking

use intensity on psychosocial and mental health among adolescents. Two studies by

Kraut et al have revealed that the short-term detrimental effects may dissipate over a

longer period time [46, 61]. Longitudinal studies with longer follow-up time (e.g. one or

two years) would be preferable to convincingly assess the long-term effects of online

social networking use intensity on adolescents’ psychological and mental health (e.g.

depressive symptoms) in China.

The present study explored some possible mediational mechanisms on how

social networking use intensity affects depression among adolescents. However, due

to the restriction of two-wave study design, it could not derive causal mediational

sequence. Thus, experimental studies with three or more assessments would be

needed to convincingly establish the temporal mediational evidence among

adolescents.

Since the present study focused on a very specific demographic population, i.e.

non-clinical school-based secondary students in Guangzhou of South China, the

generalizability of the results to other population and regions should be cautious.

Similar studies in other demographic populations (e.g. other age group, or psychatric

clinical population) are warranted in the future.

266
10.3.2 Sampling

The districts and schools in the present study were conveniently rather than

randomly selected in each stratification since randomized sampling is difficult and not

feasible. There may be selection bias as the sampling frame did not include all junior

middle school students in Guangzhou. Therefore, it could not fully ensure that the

reported prevalence and incidence rates (e.g. depression) represent the true situation

of adolescents in Guangzhou. Although the stratified cluster sampling method could

partially guarantee the representativeness of the sample for the targeted population,

attentions should also be made to the representativeness of the sample when

interperting the prevalence and incidence rates reported in this study.

It was a limitation that students were not screened about whether they previously

have depression (or currently receive treatment) and other mental illness by

physicians at baseline. However, to our understanding, the proportion of such students

is relately small since those with such psychiatric conditions would be refered to

receive treatment. Moreover, such students are more likely to be unwilling to

participate the survey, and then excluded from the study.

10.3.3 Causal inference for the relationships between ’SNA’/IA and depression

The present study provided the evidences of temporal sequence and strength of

the associations for causal inference between ’SNA’/IA and depression among

adolescents. However, it is still far away to establish causal relationships for these

public health issues because of the lack of evidences of other critetia for causal

inference [290], such as biological gradient, consistency, specificity etc. Moreover,

there may be other potential confounders (e.g. prolonged time of watching TV) that

267
were not included and adjusted in our analysis. From this persepctive, the findings in

this study could only imply possible causal relationships between ’SNA’/IA and

depression. More evidences from experimental or randomized controlled studies, if

possible, are highly warranted in order to establish convinced causal relationships

between these public health problems.

10.3.4 Measures and reporting bias

Depression measured by CES-D, a self-reported epidemiological screening, only

represents probable depression, which is rather than clinical depression. It should be

remembered that the CES-D was a measure for epidemiologic research and was not

intented to be used for clinical diagnosis. However, significant depressive

symptomatology is a strongly predictive of clinical depression, and CES-D has been

widely used among adolescents in China and worldwide. The depression defined by

cut-off value of 21 indicates a moderate level of depression in epidemiologic research.

Thus, we cannot completely exclude misclassification as a source of measurement

error that explains current observed associations. Future studies using clinical

diagnosis critetia for depression (or among clinical psychiatric populations) are hence

warranted to further confirm the findings in this study.

Some online social networking-related measures such as “emotional connection

to social networking” and “social networking addiction (SNA)” were firstly modified

from Facebook-related scales of previous studies. These scales were not previously

validated in Chinese cultural context. However, we strictly translated and validated

these measures, and all these measures in this study have satisfactory psychometric

properties (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.8). The internal consistency of the modified scales

should not be a problem.

268
Since there is a lack of evidence-based diagnostic criteria and cut-off value for

‘SNA’ classification, ‘SNA’ was experimentally defined by the top 10% of scores

(around mean + 1.2 Standard deviation). Thus, caution must be given when

interpreting the related results, i.e. causal direction, prevalence and incidence.

Additional refinement and improvement for these measurements are needed.

Similarly, there is currently no acceptable consensus in the diagnostic criteria for

IA in general. There are several commonly used scales for generalized Internet

addiction assessment among adolescents, e.g. 20-item Chen Internet addiction

scale [99], Young’s 20-itme Internet addiction test [185, 202], and Young’s 8-item

diagnostic questonnaire [160, 291]. The IA prevalence and incidence in this study is

assessed based on Young’s 8-item Diagnostic Questionnaire (YDQ). The YDQ is a

more stringent criterion than Chen’s Internet Addiction Scale, and therefore is less

likely to overestimate the situation. Comparison of the prevalence and incidence data

with other studies should be done carefully.

Only online social networking activities intensity were measured in the present

study. It is a limitation that the conventional offline social network activities were not

measured, which may be a major confounder in this study. Offline social network is

important and fundamental to adolescents’ living and development. For example,

adolescents with poor offline social networks (e.g. poor social skills to make friends)

may have increased use of online social networking and then increased the risk of

developing depression. It is therefore recommended to involve offline social

network-related measures in future studies.

Self-reported data collecting method was applied in this study, and reporting bias

may consequentially exist (i.e. social desirable bias, recall bias). However, the great

269
efforts were made to minimize this bias, including the anonymous and private nature

of the study, the absence of teachers during the investigation, using well-trained field

workers, and guaranteeing participants that their personal data was highly confidential

and inaccessible to third parties. The students were also told that their participations in

the study was voluntary and that they have the right to terminate their participation at

any time.

The questionnaire was relatively long and participants may have lost patience

when completing it. However, the pilot test was conducted to assess the feasibility of

the questionnaire before we put it into use in the longitudinal study, and the feedback

showed that the length was acceptable among participants. During the follow-up

period, only 9.2% of participants were lost to follow-up in the nine-month interval.

10.4 Recommendations for future research

10.4.1 Longitudinal study with three or more observations

Due to practical constraints and resource limitations, our longitudinal study only

included two time points with nine-month interval. Therefore, in order to further

confirm these causal relationships and the longitudinal mediational effects, more

longitudinal studies with at least three measurement waves are highly warranted in

different populations (e.g. other age group, or psychiatirc population) and under

different circumstances (i.e. social-cultural). Especially when testing causal

mediational effects, three or more measurement waves are essentially needed in order

to provide a clear temporal sequence for robust causal mediational relationships.

Moreover, considering the highly dynamic status changes in IA, ‘SNA’, and

depression over time, the time interval should be carefully defined in the prospective

270
study. In order to truly reflect the natural course of such pathological statuses, future

longitudinal research should appropriately conduct the follow-up within a short-term

interval (e.g. three-month or six-month interval), which could provide more pertinent

information. Additionally, these pathological conditions should be assessed at each

time point rather than just measured in the initial assessment.

10.4.2 Mining online social networking content

Most of the published studies have focused on behavioral characteristics (such as

frequency and duration of social networking use); however, the actual contents

created and exchanged through social networking by adolescents have been largely

ignored, which could be an invaluable asset to the study of adolescents. However, the

potential of social networking to provide rich and valid data about the nature of

adolescents’ computer-mediated communications remains largely under-researched.

Online social networking has been used to identify depression among college

students who display reference of depressive symptoms on Facebook [292]. Transcripts

from social networking content are potential fruitful avenues to pursue and explore in

future research. Better documentation and understanding of social networking

activities and content could be useful to identify adolescents who are at an increased

risk for a variety of psychosocial challenges, including social isolation, deviant

behavior, and psychopathology. Therefore, mining social networking content that

adolescents conduct online would be considered as a future research direction, as such

studies could facilitate the early identification of deteriorating conditions, improve

both public and professional awareness about early warning of risk behaviors

(cyber-bullying, online harassment), and enhance our understanding of the

characteristics of social networking use among adolescents [49, 62, 86, 293, 294].

271
10.4.3 Research on benefits of proper online social networking use

Our study mainly investigated the harmful effects of online social networking

use intensity on adolescents’ psychological and mental health outcomes. Some

previous studies have reported that online social networking use could increase users’

satisfaction with life [114]. It is recommended to explore and elaborate the potential

beneficial effects of proper online social networking use on psychosocial well-being

among adolescents, such as satisfaction with life, social capital, and reduced

loneliness etc.

10.4.4 Considerations for other measures in future direction

It is unknown whether (or extent to which) online social networking use

intensity has different impacts on adolescents’ online and offline psychosocial

relationships, e.g. whether online social networking use only increases users’

perceived online social support, but have no effect or even harmful impacts on

perceived offline social support. One previous study reported that Facebook use

helped college students to obtain online social support, but online social support

appears to have little direct effects on well-being [295]. Future studies are therefore

recommended to distinguish online and offline social relationships, and investigate

their potential mediating/moderating effects seperately. Such knowledge will extend

the scope and depth for understanding of underlying mechanisms.

Secure relationship with one or a few good friends is important for adolescents’

development. Apart from the measure for online social networking use intensity, it is

recommended to measure the intensity that adolescents use online social networking

to interact with particular good friends in future studies.

272
It is also recommended that other asepcts of psychosocial relationships should

further be investigated and measured apart from those measured in the present study.

For instance, quantity of friendship such as time spent with particilar good friends,

degree of social connectedness with friends and others, and other types of social

support (e.g instrumental support, or emotional social support) might be important

measures and covariates that may influence the current findings. Future research

should consider to measure these aspects of adolescents’ psychosocial relationships.

10.4.5 Moderation effects of gender and personality for the effects of online social

networking use intensity on psychosocial and mental health among adolescents

Personality and gender might interact with online social networking use intensity

to affect adolescents’ psychosocial and mental health. For instance, whether

extraverted individuals are more likely to use online social networking intensively

when compared to introverted individuals, or whether detrimental effects of online

social networking use intensity on psychosocial and mental well-being are stronger or

weaker among males than that in females, or even only existed among males/females?

Such research questions were not explored in this study, but they would contribute to

a better understanding on who would have more tendency to use online social

networking intensively and would be more influenced by online social networking

use.

10.4.6 Intervention studies using social networking as an innovative avenue

Mental health interventions through online social networking platforms can

easily reach targeted populations and could potentially be cost-effective. There is clear

evidence that online interventions using the cognitive-behavioral theory are promising

273
in reducing depression in young people. Social networking as a tool to enhance

awareness of adolescents at risk and to intervene online is an important new direction.

Further study is warranted to identify the effectiveness of online interventions by

delivering cognitive behavioral subcomponents to prevent depression among

adolescents, such as problem solving therapy or other approaches. Given the high

popularity of social networking among adolescents and adults, social networking

would be an innovative means of identifying young individuals at high risk for

depression, and can also provide a cost-effective intervention, such as disseminating

intervention-related information, or healthy-related information.

10.5 Overall remarks

This study is one of the first to thoroughly explore the causal directions between

Internet addictive behaviors (both ‘SNA’ in specific and IA in general) and depression

by a large-scale school-based sample among adolescents in China. Bidirectional

relationships between Internet addictive behaviors and depression were supported.

This study provides a useful instrument for assessing social networking use

intensity, which is a feasible, acceptable and easy-administered among adolescents. It

is firstly identified that social networking use intensity is a significant risk factor of

depression among adolescents even after controlling for the baseline level of

depressive symptoms.

Additionally, this study is the first longitudinal study to provide robust evidence

on how and why social networking use intensity increases depressive symptoms

among adolescents. It has showed that the impact of social networking use intensity

on depressive symptoms are mainly indirect through its effects on increased addictive

274
behaviors and decreased psychosocial conditions (i.e. social non-confidence and

adolescent-mother conflict).

The results of this study not only enrich our understanding of the impact of social

networking use on adolescents’ psychosocial health in China, but also provide

important and useful information in designing effective intervention in preventing

depression and Internet addictive behaviors. Furthermore, the results of this study also

provide some implications for future research. More longitudinal studies with three or

more waves of observations are highly warranted in China and worldwide.

275
Reference

1. Christie D, Viner R. Adolescent development. BMJ 2005,330:301-304.


2. Merikangas KR, He JP, Burstein M, Swanson SA, Avenevoli S, Cui L, et al.
Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in US adolescents: results from the
National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A).
Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry
2010,49:980-989.
3. Knopf D, Park MJ, Mulye TP. The mental health of adolescents: A national
profile, 2008. San Francisco, CA: National Adolescent Health Information
Center 2008.
4. Thapar A, Collishaw S, Potter R, Thapar AK. Managing and preventing
depression in adolescents. BMJ 2010,340:C209.
5. Lewinsohn PM, Clarke GN, Seeley JR, Rohde P. Major depression in
community adolescents: age at onset, episode duration, and time to
recurrence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry 1994,33:809-818.
6. Birmaher B, Arbelaez C, Brent D. Course and outcome of child and adolescent
major depressive disorder. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North
America 2002,11:619-637.
7. Rushton JL, Forcier M, Schectman RM. Epidemiology of depressive symptoms
in the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 2002,41:199-205.
8. Feng ZZ, Zhang DJ. An epidemiological study on depressive symptoms in 2634
middle school students. Chinese Journal of Behavioral Medical Science
2005,14:7-9.
9. ITU ICT Facts and Figures-The world in 2015.
In: http://www.itu.int/en/ITU-D/Statistics/Documents/facts/ICTFactsFigures2
015.pdf; 2015.
10. China Internet Network Information Center (CNNIC). [The 27th statistical
report of Internet network develpoment in China].
In: http://www.cnnic.net.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/201101/P020120709345289031
187.pdf; 2011.
11. China Internet Network Information Center (CNNIC). [The 36th statistical
report of internet network develpoment in China].
In: http://www.cnnic.net.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/hlwtjbg/201507/P02015072354
9500667087.pdf; 2015.
12. China Internet Network Information Center (CNNIC). [Report on Internet
behavior among adloescents in China in 2014].
In: http://www.cnnic.net.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/qsnbg/201506/P020150603434
893070975.pdf; 2015.
13. Boyd DM, Ellison NB. Social Network Sites: Definition, History, and Scholarship.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 2007,13:210-230.
14. Lenhart A, Madden M, Smith A, Purcell K, Zickuhr K, Rainie L. Teens, Kindness
and Cruelty on Social Network Sites: How American Teens Navigate the New

276
World of" Digital Citizenship". Pew Internet & American Life Project 2011.
15. Livingstone S, Haddon L. EU Kids Online. Journal of Psychology
2009,217:236-239.
16. Wang JL, Jackson LA, Gaskin J, Wang HZ. The effects of Social Networking Site
(SNS) use on college students’ friendship and well-being. Computers in
Human Behavior 2014,37:229-236.
17. Shaw M, Black DW. Internet addiction. CNS drugs 2008,22:353-365.
18. Byun S, Ruffini C, Mills JE, Douglas AC, Niang M, Stepchenkova S, et al.
Internet addiction: metasynthesis of 1996-2006 quantitative research.
CyberPsychology & Behavior 2009,12:203-207.
19. Young KS. Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder.
CyberPsychology & Behavior 1998,1:237-244.
20. Dalbudak E, Evren C, Aldemir S, Coskun KS, Ugurlu H, Yildirim FG. Relationship
of internet addiction severity with depression, anxiety, and alexithymia,
temperament and character in university students. Cyberpsychology, Behavior,
and Social Networking 2013,16:272-278.
21. Davis RA. A cognitive-behavioral model of pathological Internet use.
Computers in Human Behavior 2001,17:187-195.
22. Montag C, Bey K, Sha P, Li M, Chen YF, Liu WY, et al. Is it meaningful to
distinguish between generalized and specific Internet addiction? Evidence
from a cross‐cultural study from Germany, Sweden, Taiwan and China.
Asia‐Pacific Psychiatry 2014,7:20-26.
23. Laconi S, Tricard N, Chabrol H. Differences between specific and generalized
problematic Internet uses according to gender, age, time spent online and
psychopathological symptoms. Computers in Human Behavior
2015,48:236-244.
24. Pontes HM, Szabo A, Griffiths MD. The impact of Internet-based specific
activities on the perceptions of Internet addiction, quality of life, and
excessive usage: A cross-sectional study. Addictive Behaviors Reports
2015,1:19-25.
25. Griffiths M. Social networking addiction: emerging themes and issues. Journal
of Addiction Research and Therapy 2013,4:e118.
26. Andreassen CS. Online Social Network Site Addiction: A Comprehensive
Review. Current Addiction Reports 2015,2:175-184.
27. Kuss D, Griffiths M, Karila L, Billieux J. Internet addiction: a systematic review
of epidemiological research for the last decade. Current Pharmaceutical
Design 2014,20:4026-4052.
28. Ko CH, Yen JY, Yen CF, Chen CS, Chen CC. The association between Internet
addiction and psychiatric disorder: a review of the literature. European
Psychiatry 2012,27:1-8.
29. Alabi OF. A survey of Facebook addiction level among selected Nigerian
University undergraduates. New Media and Mass Communication
2013,10:70-80.
30. Wolniczak I, Cáceres-DelAguila JA, Palma-Ardiles G, Arroyo KJ, Solís-Visscher R,
Paredes-Yauri S, et al. Association between Facebook dependence and poor
sleep quality: a study in a sample of undergraduate students in Peru. PLoS

277
ONE 2013,8:e59087.
31. Zhou SX. Gratifications, loneliness, leisure boredom and self-esteem as
predictors of SNS-game addiction and usage pattern among Chinese college
students. Hong Kong: The Chinese University of Hong Kong; 2010.
32. Wan C. Gratifications & loneliness as predictors of campus-SNS websites
addiction & usage pattern among Chinese college students. Hong Kong: The
Chinese University of Hong Kong; 2009:35.
33. Ko CH, Yen JY, Chen CS, Chen CC, Yen CF. Psychiatric comorbidity of internet
addiction in college students: an interview study. CNS Spectrums
2008,13:147-153.
34. Ni X, Yan H, Chen S, Liu Z. Factors influencing internet addiction in a sample of
freshmen university students in China. Cyberpsychology & Behavior
2009,12:327-330.
35. Pantic I, Damjanovic A, Todorovic J, Topalovic D, Bojovic-Jovic D, Ristic S, et al.
Association between online social networking and depression in high school
students: behavioral physiology viewpoint. Psychiatria Danubina
2012,24:90-93.
36. Kalpidou M, Costin D, Morris J. The relationship between Facebook and the
well-being of undergraduate college students. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and
Social Networking 2011,14:183-189.
37. Labrague LJ. Facebook use and adolescents’ emotional states of depression,
anxiety, and stress. Health Science Journal 2014,8:80-89.
38. Chang FC, Chiu CH, Lee CM, Chen PH, Miao NF. Predictors of the initiation and
persistence of Internet addiction among adolescents in Taiwan. Addictive
Behaviors 2014,39:1434-1440.
39. Ko CH, Yen JY, Chen CS, Yeh YC, Yen CF. Predictive values of psychiatric
symptoms for internet addiction in adolescents: a 2-year prospective study.
Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 2009,163:937-943.
40. Ko CH, Yen JY, Yen CF, Lin HC, Yang MJ. Factors predictive for incidence and
remission of internet addiction in young adolescents: a prospective study.
CyberPsychology & Behavior 2007,10:545-551.
41. Ko CH, Liu TL, Wang PW, Chen CS, Yen CF, Yen JY. The exacerbation of
depression, hostility, and social anxiety in the course of Internet addiction
among adolescents: A prospective study. Comprehensive Psychiatry
2014,55:1377-1384.
42. Dong G, Lu Q, Zhou H, Zhao X. Precursor or sequela: pathological disorders in
people with Internet addiction disorder. PloS ONE 2011,6:e14703.
43. Lam LT, Peng ZW. Effect of pathological use of the internet on adolescent
mental health: a prospective study. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent
Medicine 2010,164:901-906.
44. Cho SM, Sung MJ, Shin KM, Lim KY, Shin YM. Does psychopathology in
childhood predict internet addiction in male adolescents? Child Psychiatry &
Human Development 2013,44:549-555.
45. Gámez-Guadix M. Depressive Symptoms and Problematic Internet Use
Among Adolescents: Analysis of the Longitudinal Relationships from the
Cognitive–Behavioral Model. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social

278
Networking 2014,17:714-719.
46. Kraut R, Kiesler S, Boneva B, Cummings J, Helgeson V, Crawford A. Internet
paradox revisited. Journal of Social Issues 2002,58:49-74.
47. Hong FY, Huang DH, Lin HY, Chiu SL. Analysis of the psychological traits,
Facebook usage, and Facebook addiction model of Taiwanese university
students. Telematics and Informatics 2014,31:597-606.
48. Koc M, Gulyagci S. Facebook Addiction Among Turkish College Students: The
Role of Psychological Health, Demographic, and Usage Characteristics.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 2013,16:279-284.
49. Lee G, Lee J, Kwon S. Use of social-networking sites and subjective well-being:
a study in South Korea. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking
2011,14:151-155.
50. Chen WH, Lee KH. Sharing, liking, commenting, and distressed? The pathway
between Facebook interaction and psychological distress. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking 2013,16:728-734.
51. Ellison NB, Steinfield C, Lampe C. The benefits of Facebook “friends:” Social
capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of
Computer‐Mediated Communication 2007,12:1143-1168.
52. Simoncic TE, Kuhlman KR, Vargas I, Houchins S, Lopez-Duran NL. Facebook use
and depressive symptomatology: Investigating the role of neuroticism and
extraversion in youth. Computers in Human Behavior 2014,40:1-5.
53. Jelenchick LA, Eickhoff JC, Moreno MA. “Facebook depression?” Social
networking site use and depression in older adolescents. Journal of
Adolescent Health 2013,52:128-130.
54. Subrahmanyam K, Reich SM, Waechter N, Espinoza G. Online and offline
social networks: Use of social networking sites by emerging adults. Journal of
Applied Developmental Psychology 2008,29:420-433.
55. Pempek TA, Yermolayeva YA, Calvert SL. College students' social networking
experiences on Facebook. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
2009,30:227-238.
56. Li XQ, Chen WH, Popiel PW. What happens on Facebook stays on Facebook?
The implications of Facebook interaction for perceived, receiving, and giving
social support. Computers in Human Behavior 2015,51:106-113.
57. Deters FG, Mehl MR. Does Posting Facebook Status Updates Increase or
Decrease Loneliness? An Online Social Networking Experiment. Social
Psychological & Personality Science 2013,4:1-8.
58. Gentzler AL, Oberhauser AM, Westerman D, Nadorff DK. College students' use
of electronic communication with parents: links to loneliness, attachment,
and relationship quality. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking
2011,14:71-74.
59. Reich SM, Subrahmanyam K, Espinoza G. Friending, IMing, and hanging out
face-to-face: overlap in adolescents' online and offline social networks.
Developmental Psychology 2012,48:356-368.
60. Nabi RL, Prestin A, So J. Facebook friends with (health) benefits? Exploring
social network site use and perceptions of social support, stress, and
well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking

279
2013,16:721-727.
61. Kraut R, Patterson M, Lundmark V, Kiesler S, Mukopadhyay T, Scherlis W.
Internet paradox. A social technology that reduces social involvement and
psychological well-being? American Psychologist 1998,53:1017-1031.
62. Valkenburg PM, Peter J, Schouten AP. Friend networking sites and their
relationship to adolescents' well-being and social self-esteem.
CyberPsychology & Behavior 2006,9:584-590.
63. Gonzales AL, Hancock JT. Mirror, mirror on my Facebook wall: Effects of
exposure to Facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social
Networking 2011,14:79-83.
64. Mehdizadeh S. Self-presentation 2.0: Narcissism and self-esteem on Facebook.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 2010,13:357-364.
65. Van Rooij AJ, Schoenmakers TM, Van de Eijnden RJ, Van de Mheen D.
Compulsive internet use: the role of online gaming and other internet
applications. Journal of Adolescent Health 2010,47:51-57.
66. WHO. Depression: A Global Crisis. World Federation for Mental Health.
2012:1-32.
67. Whiteford HA, Degenhardt L, Rehm J, Baxter AJ, Ferrari AJ, Erskine HE, et al.
Global burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use disorders:
findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. The Lancet
2013,382:1575-1586.
68. WHO. Health for the World’s Adolescents A second chance in the second
decade. 2014.
69. Toolan JM. Depression in children and adolescents. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry 1962,32:404-415.
70. Andrade L, Caraveo‐anduaga JJ, Berglund P, Bijl RV, Graaf RD, Vollebergh W,
et al. The epidemiology of major depressive episodes: results from the
International Consortium of Psychiatric Epidemiology (ICPE) Surveys.
International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 2003,12:3-21.
71. Wesselhoeft R, Sørensen MJ, Heiervang ER, Bilenberg N. Subthreshold
depression in children and adolescents–a systematic review. Journal of
Affective Disorders 2013,151:7-22.
72. Sajjadi H, Kamal SHM, Rafiey H, Vameghi M, Forouzan AS, Rezaei M. A
systematic review of the prevalence and risk factors of depression among
Iranian adolescents. Global Journal of Health Science 2013,5:16-27.
73. Stapinski LA, Montgomery AA, Heron J, Jerrim J, Vignoles A, Araya R.
Depression symptom trajectories and associated risk factors among
adolescents in Chile. PloS ONE 2013,8:e78323.
74. Xin Z, Niu J, Chi L. Birth cohort changes in Chinese adolescents' mental health.
International Journal of Psychology 2012,47:287-295.
75. Liu XC, Ma DD, Kurita H, Tang MQ. Self-reported depressive symptoms among
Chinese adolescents. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology
1999,34:44-47.
76. Yang J, Yao S, Zhu X, Zhang C, Ling Y, Abela JR, et al. The impact of stress on
depressive symptoms is moderated by social support in Chinese adolescents
with subthreshold depression: A multi-wave longitudinal study. Journal of

280
Affective Disorders 2010,127:113-121.
77. Yang WH, Zhou T, Peng F, Liu HH. Prevalence of Depression in Adolescents in
Yueyang County of Hunan Province. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology
2013,6:937-941.
78. Internet Live Stats. In: http://www.internetlivestats.com/internet-users/;
2014.
79. Pantic I. Online Social Networking and Mental Health. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking 2014,17:652-657.
80. Schneider F, Feldmann A, Krishnamurthy B, Willinger W. Understanding online
social network usage from a network perspective. In: Proceedings of the 9th
ACM SIGCOMM conference on Internet measurement conference: ACM; 2009.
pp. 35-48.
81. Alhabash S, Park H, Kononova A, Chiang YH, Wise K. Exploring the motivations
of Facebook use in Taiwan. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking
2012,15:304-311.
82. Spiliotopoulos T, Oakley I. Understanding motivations for Facebook use:
Usage metrics, network structure, and privacy. In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI
Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems: ACM; 2013. pp.
3287-3296.
83. Joinson AN. Looking at, looking up or keeping up with people?: motives and
use of facebook. In: Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on Human Factors
in Computing Systems: ACM; 2008. pp. 1027-1036.
84. Yang CC, Brown BB. Motives for using Facebook, patterns of Facebook
activities, and late adolescents' social adjustment to college. Journal of Youth
and Adolescence 2013,42:403-416.
85. Ross C, Orr ES, Sisic M, Arseneault JM, Simmering MG, Orr RR. Personality and
motivations associated with Facebook use. Computers in Human Behavior
2009,25:578-586.
86. Moreno MA, Kolb J. Social networking sites and adolescent health. Pediatric
Clinics of North America 2012,59:601-612.
87. Bull SS, Levine DK, Black SR, Schmiege SJ, Santelli J. Social media-delivered
sexual health intervention: a cluster randomized controlled trial. American
Journal of Preventive Medicine 2012,43:467-474.
88. Media CS. Is Social Networking Changing Childhood? In:
https://www.commonsensemedia.org/about-us/news/press-releases/is-social
-networking-changing-childhood. SanFrancisco,CA; 2009.
89. China Internet Network Information Center (CNNIC). [Annual report of social
networking sites use appplication among Chinese netizen in 2012].
In: http://www.cnnic.net.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/sqbg/201302/P0201302196116
51054576.pdf; 2013.
90. China Internet Network Information Center (CNNIC). [The 32th statistical
report of internet network develpoment in China].
In: http://www.cnnic.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/hlwtjbg/201307/P02013071750534
3100851.pdf; 2013.
91. Shi JQ, Niu QK. SNSs Usage among Chinese Internet Users: An Empirical Study.
In: Annual Review of Cybertherapy and Telemedicine 2010: Advanced

281
Technologies in Behavioral, Social and Neurosciences. Edited by Wiederhold
BK, Riva G, Kim SI. Washington, DC: ISO Press; 2010. pp. 150-154.
92. Tsitsika A, Janikian M, Schoenmakers TM, Tzavela EC, Ólafsson K, Wójcik S, et
al. Internet Addictive Behavior in Adolescence: A Cross-Sectional Study in
Seven European Countries. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking
2014,17:528-535.
93. Kuss D, Griffiths M. Excessive online social networking: Can adolescents
become addicted to Facebook. Education and Health 2011,29:68-71.
94. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Internet gaming addiction: A systematic review of
empirical research. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction
2012,10:278-296.
95. Griffiths M. A'components' model of addiction within a biopsychosocial
framework. Journal of Substance Use 2005,10:191-197.
96. Echeburua E, De Corral P. Addiction to new technologies and to online social
networking in young people: A new challenge. Adicciones 2010,22:91-95.
97. Griths MD, Kuss DJ, Demetrovics Z. Social Networking Addiction: An Overview
of Preliminary Findings. In: Behavioral Addictions: Criteria, Evidence and
Treatment. Edited by Rosenberg KP, Feder LC. London: Elsevier; 2014. pp.
119-141.
98. Cheng C, Li AYL. Internet addiction prevalence and quality of (real) life: a
meta-analysis of 31 nations across seven world regions. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking 2014,17:755-760.
99. Lin MP, Ko HC, Wu JYW. Prevalence and psychosocial risk factors associated
with Internet addiction in a nationally representative sample of college
students in Taiwan. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking
2011,14:741-746.
100. Machold C, Judge G, Mavrinac A, Elliott J, Murphy A, Roche E. Social
networking patterns/hazards among teenagers. Irish Medical Journal
2012,105:151-152.
101. Wu AM, Cheung VI, Ku L, Hung EP. Psychological risk factors of addiction to
social networking sites among Chinese smartphone users. Journal of
Behavioral Addictions 2013,2:160-166.
102. Moreno MA, Jelenchick LA, Breland DJ. Exploring depression and problematic
internet use among college females: A multisite study. Computers in Human
Behavior 2015,49:601-607.
103. Yoo YS, Cho OH, Cha KS. Associations between overuse of the internet and
mental health in adolescents. Nursing & Health Sciences 2014,16:193-200.
104. Ryan T, Chester A, Reece J, Xenos S. The uses and abuses of Facebook: A
review of Facebook addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions
2014,3:133-148.
105. Błachnio A, Przepiórka A, Pantic I. Internet use, Facebook intrusion, and
depression: Results of a cross-sectional study. European Psychiatry
2015,30:681-684.
106. De Cock R, Vangeel J, Klein A, Minotte P, Rosas O, Meerkerk G-J. Compulsive
Use of Social Networking Sites in Belgium: Prevalence, Profile, and the Role of
Attitude Toward Work and School. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social

282
Networking 2014,17:166-171.
107. Trepte S, Reinecke L. The reciprocal effects of social network site use and the
disposition for self-disclosure: A longitudinal study. Computers in Human
Behavior 2013,29:1102-1112.
108. Datu J, Valdez J, Datu N. Does Facebooking make us sad? Hunting relationship
between Facebook use and depression among Filipino adolescents.
International Journal of Research Studies in Educational Technology (IJRSET)
2012,1:83-91.
109. Jenkins-Guarnieri MA, Wright SL, Johnson B. Development and validation of a
social media use integration scale. Psychology of Popular Media Culture
2013,2:38-50.
110. Grieve R, Indian M, Witteveen K, Anne Tolan G, Marrington J. Face-to-face or
Facebook: Can social connectedness be derived online? Computers in Human
Behavior 2013,29:604-609.
111. Lou LL, Yan Z, Nickerson A, McMorris R. An Examination of the Reciprocal
Relationship of Loneliness and Facebook Use among First-Year College
Students. Journal of Educational Computing Research 2012,46:105-117.
112. Steinfield C, Ellison NB, Lampe C. Social capital, self-esteem, and use of online
social network sites: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology 2008,29:434-445.
113. Burke M, Marlow C, Lento T. Social network activity and social well-being. In:
Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing
Systems: ACM; 2010. pp. 1909-1912.
114. Guo Y, Li Y, Ito N. Exploring the Predicted Effect of Social Networking Site Use
on Perceived Social Capital and Psychological Well-Being of Chinese
International Students in Japan. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social
Networking 2014,17:52-58.
115. Lee KT, Noh MJ, Koo DM. Lonely people are no longer lonely on social
networking sites: The mediating role of self-disclosure and social support.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 2013,16:413-418.
116. Frison E, Eggermont S. Exploring the relationships between different types of
Facebook use, perceived online social support, and adolescents’ depressed
mood. Social Science Computer Review 2015:1-19.
117. O'Keeffe GS, Clarke-Pearson K. The impact of social media on children,
adolescents, and families. Pediatrics 2011,127:800-804.
118. Quinn S, Oldmeadow JA. Is the igeneration a ‘we’generation? Social
networking use among 9‐to 13‐year‐olds and belonging. British Journal
of Developmental Psychology 2013,31:136-142.
119. Barrera Jr M. Distinctions between social support concepts, measures, and
models. American Journal of Community Psychology 1986,14:413-445.
120. Lee SJ. Online Communication and Adolescent Social Ties: Who benefits more
from Internet use? Journal of Computer‐Mediated Communication
2009,14:509-531.
121. Kanter M, Afifi T, Robbins S. The impact of parents “friending” their young
adult child on Facebook on perceptions of parental privacy invasions and
parent–child relationship quality. Journal of Communication 2012,62:900-917.

283
122. Rice M, Kang DH, Weaver M, Howell CC. Relationship of Anger, Stress, and
Coping With School Connectedness in Fourth‐Grade Children. Journal of
School Health 2008,78:149-156.
123. Chan DKS, Cheng GHL. A comparison of offline and online friendship qualities
at different stages of relationship development. Journal of Social and Personal
Relationships 2004,21:305-320.
124. Steiger AE, Allemand M, Robins RW, Fend HA. Low and decreasing self-esteem
during adolescence predict adult depression two decades later. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 2014,106:325-338.
125. Orth U, Robins RW, Roberts BW. Low self-esteem prospectively predicts
depression in adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology 2008,95:695.
126. Cheak APC, Goh GGG, Chin TS. Online social networking addiction: exploring
its relationship with social networking dependency and mood modification
among undergraduates in Malaysia. In: Proceedings Int Conference
Management, Economics and Finance; 2012. pp. 247-262.
127. Kittinger R, Correia CJ, Irons JG. Relationship between Facebook use and
problematic Internet use among college students. Cyberpsychology, Behavior,
and Social Networking 2012,15:324-327.
128. Toseeb U, Brage S, Corder K, Dunn VJ, Jones PB, Owens M, et al. Exercise and
Depressive Symptoms in Adolescents: A Longitudinal Cohort Study. JAMA
Pediatrics 2014,168:1093-1100.
129. Van Gool CH, Kempen GI, Penninx BW, Deeg DJ, Beekman AT, Van Eijk JT.
Relationship between changes in depressive symptoms and unhealthy
lifestyles in late middle aged and older persons: results from the Longitudinal
Aging Study Amsterdam. Age and Ageing 2003,32:81-87.
130. Da Silva MA, Singh-Manoux A, Brunner EJ, Kaffashian S, Shipley MJ, Kivimäki
M, et al. Bidirectional association between physical activity and symptoms of
anxiety and depression: the Whitehall II study. European Journal of
Epidemiology 2012,27:537-546.
131. Colver A, Rapp M, Eisemann N, Ehlinger V, Thyen U, Dickinson HO, et al.
Self-reported quality of life of adolescents with cerebral palsy: a
cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis. The Lancet 2015,385:705-716.
132. Strawbridge WJ, Camacho TC, Cohen RD, Kaplan GA. Gender differences in
factors associated with change in physical functioning in old age: a 6-year
longitudinal study. The Gerontologist 1993,33:603-609.
133. Schnall PL, Schwartz JE, Landsbergis PA, Warren K, Pickering TG. A longitudinal
study of job strain and ambulatory blood pressure: results from a three-year
follow-up. Psychosomatic Medicine 1998,60:697-706.
134. Lindwall M, Larsman P, Hagger MS. The reciprocal relationship between
physical activity and depression in older European adults: a prospective
cross-lagged panel design using SHARE data. Health Psychology
2011,30:453-462.
135. Ko CH, Wang PW, Liu TL, Yen CF, Chen CS, Yen JY. Bidirectional associations
between family factors and Internet addiction among adolescents in a
prospective investigation. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

284
2015,69:192-200.
136. Chin WW. Commentary: Issues and opinion on structural equation modeling.
In: JSTOR; 1998.
137. Kupek E. Beyond logistic regression: structural equations modelling for binary
variables and its application to investigating unobserved confounders. BMC
Medical Research Methodology 2006,6:13.
138. VanderWeele TJ. Invited commentary: structural equation models and
epidemiologic analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology 2012:1-5.
139. Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social
psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1986,51:1173-1182.
140. Conger AJ. A revised definition for suppressor variables: A guide to their
identification and interpretation. Educational and Psychological Measurement
1974,34:35-46.
141. Fioravanti G, Dèttore D, Casale S. Adolescent Internet addiction: testing the
association between self-esteem, the perception of Internet attributes, and
preference for online social interactions. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and
Social Networking 2012,15:318-323.
142. Cheung GW, Lau RS. Testing mediation and suppression effects of latent
variables: Bootstrapping with structural equation models. Organizational
Research Methods 2007.
143. MacKinnon DP, Fairchild AJ, Firtz MS. Mediation analysis. Annual Review of
Psychology 2008,58:593-641.
144. Paulhus DL, Robins RW, Trzesniewski KH, Tracy JL. Two replicable suppressor
situations in personality research. Multivariate Behavioral Research
2004,39:303-328.
145. Rose RA, Parish SL, Yoo J, Grady MD, Powell SE, Hicks-Sangster TK.
Suppression of racial disparities for children with special health care needs
among families receiving Medicaid. Social Science & Medicine
2010,70:1263-1270.
146. Bentley JP. An examination of statistical methods for longitudinal mediation
modeling: The University of Alabama at Birmingham; 2011.
147. MacKinnon DP. Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. New York,
London: Routledge; 2008.
148. Roth DL, MacKinnon DP. Mediation analysis with longitudinal data. In:
Longitudinal data analysis: A practical guide for researchers in aging, health,
and social sciences. Edited by Newsom JT, Jones RN, Hofer SM. London:
Routledge; 2012. pp. 181-216.
149. Altman M, Holland JC, Lundeen D, Kolko RP, Stein RI, Saelens BE, et al.
Reduction in Food Away from Home Is Associated with Improved Child
Relative Weight and Body Composition Outcomes and This Relation Is
Mediated by Changes in Diet Quality. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and
Dietetics 2015,115:1400-1407.
150. Harte CB, Meston CM. Association between cigarette smoking and erectile
tumescence: the mediating role of heart rate variability. International Journal
of Impotence Research 2013,25:155-159.

285
151. Lydersen S. Statistical review: frequently given comments. Annals of the
Rheumatic Diseases 2015,74:323-325.
152. Twisk JW. Applied longitudinal data analysis for epidemiology: a practical
guide: Cambridge University Press; 2013.
153. Bennett JA. Mediator and moderator variables in nursing research:
Conceptual and statistical differences. Research in Nursing & Health
2000,23:415-420.
154. Rogol AD, Clark PA, Roemmich JN. Growth and pubertal development in
children and adolescents: effects of diet and physical activity. The American
journal of clinical nutrition 2000,72:521s-528s.
155. Sanders R. Adolescent psychosocial, social, and cognitive development.
Pediatrics in Review/American Academy of Pediatrics 2013,34:354-358; quiz
358-359.
156. Kessler RC, Avenevoli S, Merikangas KR. Mood disorders in children and
adolescents: an epidemiologic perspective. Biological psychiatry
2001,49:1002-1014.
157. Sampasa-Kanyinga H, Chaput J-P, Hamilton HA. Associations between the use
of social networking sites and unhealthy eating behaviours and excess body
weight in adolescents. British Journal of Nutrition 2015,114:1941-1947.
158. Sampasa-Kanyinga H, Lewis RF. Frequent use of social networking sites is
associated with poor psychological functioning among children and
adolescents. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking
2015,18:380-385.
159. Aboujaoude E. Problematic Internet use: an overview. World Psychiatry
2010,9:85-90.
160. Fu KW, Chan WS, Wong PW, Yip PS. Internet addiction: prevalence,
discriminant validity and correlates among adolescents in Hong Kong. British
Journal of Psychiatry 2010,196:486-492.
161. Li Y, Zhang X, Lu F, Zhang Q, Wang Y. Internet addiction among elementary
and middle school students in china: a nationally representative sample study.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 2014,17:111-116.
162. Pereira SMP, Geoffroy MC, Power C. Depressive symptoms and physical
activity during 3 decades in adult life: bidirectional associations in a
prospective cohort study. JAMA Psychiatry 2014,71:1373-1380.
163. Verkuijl NE, Richter L, Norris SA, Stein A, Avan B, Ramchandani PG. Postnatal
depressive symptoms and child psychological development at 10 years: a
prospective study of longitudinal data from the South African Birth to Twenty
cohort. The Lancet Psychiatry 2014,1:454-460.
164. Wu HR, Liu P, Meng H. Norm, Reliability and validity if childern and
Adolescents' QOL scale. Chinese Journal of School Health 2006,27:18-21.
165. Chen JY, Fang YH, Jiang Y. Investigation of children and adolescent QoL in
Guangzhou. Chinese Journal of School Health 2009,30:647-648.
166. Zimet GD, Dahlem NW, Zimet SG, Farley GK. The multidimensional scale of
perceived social support. Journal of Personality Assessment 1988,52:30-41.
167. Chou KL. Assessing Chinese adolescents’ social support: the multidimensional
scale of perceived social support. Personality and Individual Differences

286
2000,28:299-307.
168. Fang XY, Dong Q. Parent-child conflicts during early adolescence. Psychology
Research 1998,21:122-125.
169. Fang XY, Zhang JT, Sun L, Liu Z. Parent-adolescent conflict and adolescents'
social adjustment. Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology 2003,9:14-21.
170. Baron RA, Markman GD. Beyond social capital: The role of entrepreneurs'
social competence in their financial success. Journal of Business Venturing
2003,18:41-60.
171. Zhuang MK, Gan YQ, Liu HH. The Revision and Preliminary Application of
Social Skill Scale. Chinese Mental Health Journal 2004,18:755-759.
172. Rosenberg M, Schooler C, Schoenbach C, Rosenberg F. Global self-esteem and
specific self-esteem: Different concepts, different outcomes. American
Sociological Review 1995,60:141-156.
173. Kong F, Zhao J, You X. Emotional intelligence and life satisfaction in Chinese
university students: The mediating role of self-esteem and social support.
Personality and Individual Differences 2012,53:1039-1043.
174. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale a self-report depression scale for research in the
general population. Applied Psychological Measurement 1977,1:385-401.
175. Radloff LS. The use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale
in adolescents and young adults. Journal of youth and adolescence
1991,20:149-166.
176. Wang M, Armour C, Wu Y, Ren F, Zhu X, Yao S. Factor Structure of the CES‐D
and Measurement Invariance Across Gender in Mainland Chinese Adolescents.
Journal of Clinical Psychology 2013,69:966-979.
177. Lee SW, Stewart SM, Byrne BM, Wong JP, Ho S, Lee PW, et al. Factor structure
of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression scale in Hong Kong
adolescents. Journal of Personality Assessment 2008,90:175-184.
178. Chen ZY, Yang XD, Li XY. Psychometric feartures of CES-D in chinese
adolescents. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology 2009,17:443-448.
179. Cheng CP, Yen CF, Ko CH, Yen JY. Factor structure of the center for
epidemiologic studies depression scale in Taiwanese adolescents.
Comprehensive Psychiatry 2012,53:299-307.
180. Stockings E, Degenhardt L, Lee YY, Mihalopoulos C, Liu A, Hobbs M, et al.
Symptom screening scales for detecting major depressive disorder in children
and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis of reliability, validity
and diagnostic utility. Journal of Affective Disorders 2015,174:447-463.
181. Mukaka M. A guide to appropriate use of Correlation coefficient in medical
research. Malawi Medical Journal 2012,24:69-71.
182. Wang H, Zhou X, Lu C, Wu J, Deng X, Hong L. Problematic Internet Use in high
school students in Guangdong Province, China. PLoS ONE 2011,6:e19660.
183. Bener A, Bhugra D. Lifestyle and depressive risk factors associated with
problematic internet use in adolescents in an Arabian Gulf culture. Journal of
Addiction Medicine 2013,7:236-242.
184. Mak KK, Lai CM, Watanabe H, Kim D-I, Bahar N, Ramos M, et al. Epidemiology
of Internet behaviors and addiction among adolescents in six Asian countries.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 2014,17:720-728.

287
185. Cao H, Sun Y, Wan Y, Hao J, Tao F. Problematic Internet use in Chinese
adolescents and its relation to psychosomatic symptoms and life satisfaction.
BMC Public Health 2011,11:802-810.
186. Wang HJ, Lu JM. The Compilation of the Middle School Students' Self-control
Ability Questionnaire. Psychological Science 2004,27:1477-1482.
187. Mann RE. Availability as a law of addiction. Addiction 2005,100:924-925.
188. Carli V, Durkee T, Wasserman D, Hadlaczky G, Despalins R, Kramarz E, et al.
The association between pathological internet use and comorbid
psychopathology: a systematic review. Psychopathology 2013,46:1-13.
189. Thompson SH, Lougheed E. Frazzled by Facebook? An exploratory study of
gender differences in social network communication among undergraduate
men and women. College Student Journal 2012:88-98.
190. Mikami AY, Szwedo DE. Social Networking in Online and Offline Contexts. In:
Encyclopedia of Adolescence. Edited by Levesque RJR. New York: Springer;
2014. pp. 2801-2808.
191. Weiser EB. Gender differences in Internet use patterns and Internet
application preferences: A two-sample comparison. CyberPsychology and
Behavior 2000,3:167-178.
192. Joiner R, Gavin J, Duffield J, Brosnan M, Crook C, Durndell A, et al. Gender,
Internet identification, and Internet anxiety: Correlates of Internet use.
CyberPsychology & Behavior 2005,8:371-378.
193. Wang X, Sun Y, An J, Hao J, Tao F. Gender difference on depressive symptoms
among Chinese children and adolescents. Zhong Hua Liu Xing Bing Xue Za Zhi
2013,34:893-896.
194. Fleming TM, Clark T, Denny S, Bullen P, Crengle S, Peiris-John R, et al. Stability
and change in the mental health of New Zealand secondary school students
2007–2012: Results from the national adolescent health surveys. Australian
and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 2014,48:472-480.
195. Pietsch K, Allgaier AK, Frühe B, Hoyler A, Rohde S, Schulte‐Körne G.
Screening for adolescent depression in paediatric care: validity of a new brief
version of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale. Child and
Adolescent Mental Health 2013,18:76-81.
196. Kaltiala-Heino R, Rimpelä M, Marttunen M, Rimpelä A, Rantanen P. Bullying,
depression, and suicidal ideation in Finnish adolescents: school survey. BMJ
1999,319:348-351.
197. Ha JH, Kim SY, Bae SC, Bae S, Kim H, Sim M, et al. Depression and Internet
addiction in adolescents. Psychopathology 2006,40:424-430.
198. Liu TC, Desai RA, Krishnan-Sarin S, Cavallo DA, Potenza MN. Problematic
Internet use and health in adolescents: data from a high school survey in
Connecticut. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 2011,72:836-845.
199. Yen CF, Ko CH, Yen JY, Chang YP, Cheng CP. Multi‐dimensional discriminative
factors for Internet addiction among adolescents regarding gender and age.
Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 2009,63:357-364.
200. Cheung LM, Wong WS. The effects of insomnia and internet addiction on
depression in Hong Kong Chinese adolescents: an exploratory cross‐
sectional analysis. Journal of Sleep Research 2011,20:311-317.

288
201. Şaşmaz T, Öner S, Kurt AÖ, Yapıcı G, Buğdaycı R, Şiş M. Prevalence and risk
factors of Internet addiction in high school students. The European Journal of
Public Health 2013:1-6.
202. Lam LT, Peng ZW, Mai JC, Jing J. Factors associated with Internet addiction
among adolescents. CyberPsychology & Behavior 2009,12:551-555.
203. Casale S, Fioravanti G. Psychosocial correlates of internet use among Italian
students. International Journal of Psychology 2011,46:288-298.
204. Selfhout MH, Branje SJ, Delsing M, Ter Bogt TF, Meeus WH. Different types of
Internet use, depression, and social anxiety: the role of perceived friendship
quality. Journal of Adolescence 2009,32:819-833.
205. Maes L, Lievens J. Can the school make a difference? A multilevel analysis of
adolescent risk and health behaviour. Social Science & Medicine
2003,56:517-529.
206. Hinkley T, Verbestel V, Ahrens W, Lissner L, Molnár D, Moreno LA, et al. Early
childhood electronic media use as a predictor of poorer well-being: a
prospective cohort study. JAMA Pediatrics 2014,168:485-492.
207. Brand M, Laier C, Young KS. Internet addiction: coping styles, expectancies,
and treatment implications. Frontiers in Psychology 2014,5:1256.
208. Caplan SE. Preference for online social interaction a theory of problematic
Internet use and psychosocial well-being. Communication Research
2003,30:625-648.
209. Young KS, Rogers RC. The relationship between depression and Internet
addiction. CyberPsychology & Behavior 1998,1:25-28.
210. Primack BA, Swanier B, Georgiopoulos AM, Land SR, Fine MJ. Association
between media use in adolescence and depression in young adulthood: a
longitudinal study. Archives of General Psychiatry 2009,66:181-188.
211. Gámez-Guadix M, Villa-George FI, Calvete E. Measurement and analysis of the
cognitive-behavioral model of generalized problematic Internet use among
Mexican adolescents. Journal of Adolescence 2012,35:1581-1591.
212. Yu LW. An investigation into the problematic use of Facebook: Hong Kong
Baptist University Hong Kong; 2011.
213. Caplan SE, High AC. Online Social Interaction, Psychosocial Well‐Being, and
Problematic Internet Use. In: Internet addiction: A handbook and guide to
evaluation and treatment. Edited by Young KS, De Abreu CN. Hoboken, NJ,
USA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2011. pp. 35-53.
214. Muñoz-Rivas MJ, Fernández L, Gámez-Guadix M. Analysis of the indicators of
pathological Internet use in Spanish university students. The Spanish Journal
of Psychology 2010,13:697-707.
215. Chou WP, Ko CH, Kaufman EA, Crowell SE, Hsiao RC, Wang PW, et al.
Association of stress coping strategies with Internet addiction in college
students: The moderating effect of depression. Comprehensive Psychiatry
2015,62:27-33.
216. Gelenberg AJ, Freeman MP, Markowitz JC, Rosenbaum JF, Thase ME, Trivedi
MH, et al. Practice guideline for the treatment of patients with major
depressive disorder third edition. The American Journal of Psychiatry
2010,167:1-3,9-11,13-118.

289
217. Medenica S, Račić M, Joksimović V. Internet and computer addiction:“new
age” disease of the 21st century. Биомедицинска истраживања 2015,6.
218. Tsitsika AK, Tzavela EC, Janikian M, Ólafsson K, Iordache A, Schoenmakers TM,
et al. Online Social Networking in Adolescence: Patterns of Use in Six
European Countries and Links With Psychosocial Functioning. Journal of
Adolescent Health 2014,55:141-147.
219. Valkenburg PM, Peter J. Preadolescents' and adolescents' online
communication and their closeness to friends. Developmental Psychology
2007c,43:267-277.
220. Valkenburg PM, Peter J. Online Communication and Adolescent Well‐Being:
Testing the Stimulation Versus the Displacement Hypothesis. Journal of
Computer‐Mediated Communication 2007b,12:1169-1182.
221. Chan M, Wu X, Hao Y, Xi R, Jin T. Microblogging, Online Expression, and
Political Efficacy Among Young Chinese Citizens: The Moderating Role of
Information and Entertainment Needs in the Use of Weibo. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking 2012,15:345-349.
222. Terwee CB, Bot SD, de Boer MR, van der Windt DA, Knol DL, Dekker J, et al.
Quality criteria were proposed for measurement properties of health status
questionnaires. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2007,60:34-42.
223. Gil-Monte PR, Carlotto MS, Câmara SG. Validation of the Brazilian version of
the" Spanish Burnout Inventory" in teachers. Revista de Saúde Pública
2010,44:140-147.
224. Beauducel A, Wittmann WW. Simulation study on fit indexes in CFA based on
data with slightly distorted simple structure. Structural Equation Modeling
2005,12:41-75.
225. Anderson JC, Gerbing DW. The effect of sampling error on convergence,
improper solutions, and goodness-of-fit indices for maximum likelihood
confirmatory factor analysis. Psychometrika 1984,49:155-173.
226. Floyd FJ, Widaman KF. Factor analysis in the development and refinement of
clinical assessment instruments. Psychological Assessment 1995,7:286-299.
227. Vergeer M, Pelzer B. Consequences of media and Internet use for offline and
online network capital and well‐being. A causal model approach. Journal of
Computer‐Mediated Communication 2009,15:189-210.
228. Teo T. Modelling Facebook usage among university students in Thailand: the
role of emotional attachment in an extended technology acceptance model.
Interactive Learning Environments 2014:1-13.
229. Morgan C, Cotten SR. The relationship between Internet activities and
depressive symptoms in a sample of college freshmen. CyberPsychology &
Behavior 2003,6:133-142.
230. Van den Eijnden R, Meerkerk G, Vermulst AA, Spijkerman R, Engels R. Online
communication, compulsive Internet use, and psychosocial well-being among
adolescents: a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology
2008,44:655-665.
231. Seto E. Associations between Self-Reported Narcissism, Self-Esteem, and
Social-Emotional Functions of Facebook: Baylor University; 2012.
232. Clayton R. Loneliness, Anxiousness, Alcohol, and Marijuana Use: Examining

290
the Predictors of Facebook Connections and Emotional Connectedness to
Facebook Amongst College Freshmen: Texas State University-San Marcos;
2012.
233. Wisniewski P, Xu H, Lipford H, Bello‐Ogunu E. Facebook apps and tagging:
The trade‐off between personal privacy and engaging with friends. Journal
of the Association for Information Science and Technology
2015,66:1883-1896.
234. Olson DA, Liu J, Shultz KS. The influence of Facebook usage on perceptions of
social support, personal efficacy, and life satisfaction. Journal of
Organizational Psychology 2012,12:133-144.
235. O'Dea B, Campbell A. Online social networking amongst teens: friend or foe?
Studies in Health Technology and Informatics 2011,167:133-138.
236. Kim Y, Sohn D, Choi SM. Cultural difference in motivations for using social
network sites: A comparative study of American and Korean college students.
Computers in Human Behavior 2011,27:365-372.
237. Tosun LP. Motives for Facebook use and expressing “true self” on the Internet.
Computers in Human Behavior 2012,28:1510-1517.
238. Lee JS, Jeong B. Having mentors and campus social networks moderates the
impact of worries and video gaming on depressive symptoms: a moderated
mediation analysis. BMC Public Health 2014,14:426.
239. Field T, Diego M, Sanders C. Adolescent depression and risk factors.
Adolescence 2001,36:491-498.
240. Wilkinson RB. Best friend attachment versus peer attachment in the
prediction of adolescent psychological adjustment. Journal of Adolescence
2010,33:709-717.
241. He F, Zhou Q, Li J, Cao R, Guan H. Effect of social support on depression of
internet addicts and the mediating role of loneliness. International Journal of
Mental Health Systems 2014,8:34.
242. Smith NR, Clark C, Smuk M, Cummins S, Stansfeld SA. The influence of social
support on ethnic differences in well-being and depression in adolescents:
findings from the prospective Olympic Regeneration in East London (ORiEL)
study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 2015,50:1701-1711.
243. Hampton K, Goulet LS, Rainie L, Purcell K. Social networking sites and our lives
(Pew Internet & American Life Project). PewResearchCenter Internet, Science
& Tech 2011:1-85.
244. Vitak J, Ellison NB, Steinfield C. The ties that bond: Re-examining the
relationship between Facebook use and bonding social capital. In: System
Sciences (HICSS), 2011 44th Hawaii International Conference on: IEEE; 2011.
pp. 1-10.
245. O'Shea G, Spence SH, Donovan CL. Interpersonal factors associated with
depression in adolescents: are these consistent with theories underpinning
interpersonal psychotherapy? Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy
2014,21:548-558.
246. Hu J, Ai H. Self-esteem mediates the effect of the parent–adolescent
relationship on depression. Journal of Health Psychology 2014:1-8.
247. Briones RL, Janoske M. How American students perceive social networking

291
sites: An application of uses and gratifications theory. In: Social Networking
and Education. Edited by Issa T, Isaias P, Kommers P: Springer; 2016. pp.
239-253.
248. Betts LR, Stiller J. Centrality in children's best friend networks: The role of
social behaviour. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 2014,32:34-49.
249. Viikari-Juntura E, Vuori J, Silverstein B, Kalimo R, Kuosma E, Videman T. A
life-long prospective study on the role of psychosocial factors in
neck-shoulder and low-back pain. Spine 1991,16:1056-1061.
250. Horne J, Lincoln NB, Preston J, Logan P. What does confidence mean to people
who have had a stroke?–A qualitative interview study. Clinical Rehabilitation
2014,28:1125-1135.
251. Griffin KW, Nichols TR, Birnbaum AS, Botvin GJ. Social competence among
urban minority youth entering middle school: Relationships with alcohol use
and antisocial behaviors. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and
Health 2006,18:97-106.
252. Scott SM, Wallander JL, Cameron L. Protective Mechanisms for Depression
among Racial/Ethnic Minority Youth: Empirical Findings, Issues, and
Recommendations. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review
2015,18:346-369.
253. Brage D, Meredith W. A causal model of adolescent depression. The Journal of
Psychology 1994,128:455-468.
254. Ames ME, Rawana JS, Gentile P, Morgan AS. The protective role of optimism
and self-esteem on depressive symptom pathways among Canadian
Aboriginal youth. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 2013,44:142-154.
255. Duval S, Wicklund RA. A theory of objective self awareness. Oxford, England:
Academic Press; 1972.
256. Lckes WJ, Wicklund RA, Ferris CB. Objective self awareness and self esteem.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 1973,9:202-219.
257. Teng MS, Hsu LA, Wu S, Sun YC, Juan SH, Ko YL. Association of CDH13
Genotypes/Haplotypes with Circulating Adiponectin Levels, Metabolic
Syndrome, and Related Metabolic Phenotypes: The Role of the Suppression
Effect. PloS ONE 2015,10.
258. Wen Z, Fan X. Monotonicity of effect sizes: Questioning kappa-squared as
mediation effect size measure. Psychological Methods 2015,20:193-203.
259. Bessiere K, Kiesler S, Kraut R, Boneva BS. Effects of Internet use and social
resources on changes in depression. Information, Community & Society
2008,11:47-70.
260. Merton RK. The Matthew effect in science. Science 1968,159:56-63.
261. DiPrete TA, Eirich GM. Cumulative advantage as a mechanism for inequality: A
review of theoretical and empirical developments. Annual Review of
Sociology 2006:271-297.
262. Bast J, Reitsma P. Analyzing the development of individual differences in
terms of Matthew effects in reading: Results from a Dutch longitudinal study.
Developmental Psychology 1998,34:1373-1399.
263. McNamara JK, Scissons M, Gutknecth N. A longitudinal study of kindergarten
children at risk for reading disabilities: The poor really are getting poorer.

292
Journal of Learning Disabilities 2011:1-10.
264. Wade RH. The causes of increasing world poverty and inequality; or, why the
Matthew effect prevails. International Journal of Health Services
2005,35:631-653.
265. Meland E, Grønhaug S, Øystese K, Mildestvedt T. Examining the Matthew
effect on the motivation and ability to stay at work after heart disease.
Scandinavian Journal of Public Health 2011:1-8.
266. Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis.
Psychological Bulletin 1985,98:310-357.
267. Rothon C, Head J, Klineberg E, Stansfeld S. Can social support protect bullied
adolescents from adverse outcomes? A prospective study on the effects of
bullying on the educational achievement and mental health of adolescents at
secondary schools in East London. Journal of Adolescence 2011,34:579-588.
268. Bakker MP, Ormel J, Verhulst FC, Oldehinkel AJ. Adolescent family adversity
and mental health problems: the role of adaptive self-regulation capacities.
The TRAILS study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2011,39:341-350.
269. Ene M, Smiley W, Bell BA. MIXED_FIT: A SAS® macro to assess model fit and
adequacy for two-level linear models. In: SAS Glob Forum Pap: Citeseer; 2013.
pp. 255-2013.
270. Tidwell LC, Walther JB. Computer‐mediated communication effects on
disclosure, impressions, and interpersonal evaluations: Getting to know one
another a bit at a time. Human Communication Research 2002,28:317-348.
271. Mesch GS. The Family and the Internet: The Israeli Case*. Social Science
Quarterly 2003,84:1038-1050.
272. Collins WA, Russell G. Mother-child and father-child relationships in middle
childhood and adolescence: A developmental analysis. Developmental Review
1991,11:99-136.
273. Wierson M, Armistead L, Forehand R, Thomas AM, Fauber R.
Parent-adolescent conflict and stress as a parent: Are there differences
between being a mother or a father? Journal of Family Violence
1990,5:187-197.
274. Krishnan UD. Parent-adolescent conflict and adolescent functioning in a
collectivist, ethnically heterogenous culture: Malaysia. The United States: The
Ohio State University; 2004:123.
275. Zhao HY, Xu Y, Wang F, Jiang J, Zhang XH, Wang XR. Chinese adolescents'
coping tactics in a parent-adolescent conflict and their relationships with life
satisfaction: the differences between coping with mother and father. Frontiers
in Psychology 2015,6:1572.
276. Ho TY. Functional similarities and differences between online and offline
social support on psychological well-being. Pokfulam, Hong Kong: The
University of Hong Kong; 2014.
277. Kuss DJ, van Rooij AJ, Shorter GW, Griffiths MD, van de Mheen D. Internet
addiction in adolescents: Prevalence and risk factors. Computers in Human
Behavior 2013,29:1987-1996.
278. Kormas G, Critselis E, Janikian M, Kafetzis D, Tsitsika A. Risk factors and
psychosocial characteristics of potential problematic and problematic internet

293
use among adolescents: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health
2011,11:595-602.
279. Yakubu A. Path coefficient and path analysis of body weight and biometric
traits in Yankasa lambs. Slovak Journal of Animal Science 2010,43:17-25.
280. Grapentine T. Path analysis vs. structural equation modeling. Marketing
Research 2000,12:12-19.
281. Chen W, Srinivasan SR, Berenson GS. Path analysis of metabolic syndrome
components in black versus white children, adolescents, and adults: the
Bogalusa Heart Study. Annals of Epidemiology 2008,18:85-91.
282. MacKinnon DP. Contrasts in multiple mediator models. In: Multivariate
applications in substance use research: New methods for new questions.
Edited by Rose JS, Chassin L, Presson CC, Sherman SJ. New York London:
Psychology Press; 2000. pp. 141-160.
283. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and
comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research
Methods 2008,40:879-891.
284. Tabri N, Elliott CM. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling.
Canadian Graduate Journal of Sociology and Criminology 2012,1:59-60.
285. Grover G, Sabharwal A, Mittal J. Estimation of Survival Function and
Probability of Onset of Diabetic Nephropathy using Path Analysis and Analysis
of Covariance. International Refereed Journal of Engineering and Science
(IRJES)(India) 2014,3:50-58.
286. Kim K, Ryu E, Chon MY, Yeun EJ, Choi SY, Seo JS, et al. Internet addiction in
Korean adolescents and its relation to depression and suicidal ideation: a
questionnaire survey. International Journal of Nursing Studies
2006,43:185-192.
287. Young K. Internet addiction: diagnosis and treatment considerations. Journal
of Contemporary Psychotherapy 2009,39:241-246.
288. Bramesfeld A, Platt L, Schwartz FW. Possibilities for intervention in
adolescents’ and young adults’ depression from a public health perspective.
Health Policy 2006,79:121-131.
289. Drake RE. Dual diagnosis. Psychiatry 2007,6:381-384.
290. Rothman KJ, Greenland S. Causation and causal inference in epidemiology.
American journal of public health 2005,95:S144-S150.
291. Wang L, Luo J, Bai Y, Kong J, Luo J, Gao W, et al. Internet addiction of
adolescents in China: Prevalence, predictors, and association with well-being.
Addiction Research & Theory 2013,21:62-69.
292. Moreno MA, Jelenchick LA, Egan KG, Cox E, Young H, Gannon KE, et al. Feeling
bad on Facebook: depression disclosures by college students on a social
networking site. Depress Anxiety 2011,28:447-455.
293. Kim J, Lee JER. The Facebook paths to happiness: Effects of the number of
Facebook friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 2011,14:359-364.
294. Sampasa-Kanyinga H, Hamilton HA. Use of social networking sites and risk of
cyberbullying victimization: a population-level study of adolescents.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 2015,18:704-710.

294
295. Liu C-Y, Yu C-P. Can Facebook use induce well-being? Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking 2013,16:674-678.

295
Appendix I
Questionnaire for phase I validation study (Chinese version)

初中生社交网络使用强度调查问卷

同学们,你们好。这是一份调查问卷,目的是了解初中生社交网络的使用情况、及其对
心理健康的影响。研究结果将有助于促进我国青少年身心的健康发展。现诚挚邀请你参加本
研究,并如实填写此调查问卷。本调查采取匿名方式填写,你无需将自己的姓名写在问卷上,
同时你们提供的宝贵信息将仅用于科学研究,个人资料绝对保密,不会告知老师、家长、同
学等人,保证不会外泄。
请注意:问卷中涉及的问题,答案并无对错之分,你只需根据个人的看法或感受在相应
选项中打“√”。在回答问卷的过程中,如有不清楚的地方,请举手询问我们的研究人员,
他们会尽力给予解释。你们的参与对我们的研究十分重要,再次感谢你们的参与!

请首先填写下列信息,以下信息将用于问卷匹配,请务必准确填写:
1.家庭住宅电话最后四位数字:X X X
2.你的身份证号最后四位数字:X X X
3.你出生于:_____年_____月_____日
4.你姓名拼音的首字母:___________ (如“张三”,请填: ZS)

第一部分、基本资料

1.1 你的性别: □1男 □2女


1.2 你现在的年级: □ 1 初一 □ 2 初二 □ 3 初三
1.3 父亲的文化程度:
□ 1 小学及以下 □ 2 初中 □ 3 高中/中专 □ 4 本科(或大专)及以上
1.4 母亲的文化程度:
□ 1 小学及以下 □ 2 初中 □ 3 高中/中专 □ 4 本科(或大专)及以上
1.5 你是否拥有自己的智能手机? □ 1 是, 用了多久:_____年____月 □2否

第二部分、网络成瘾

2. 请仔细阅读下面每一句话,根据自己的实际情况进行选择 是 否
1). 你是否沉溺于互联网(总想着以前上网的经历或是期待着下一次的上网) 1 2
2). 你是否需要通过增加上网时间以获得满足感 1 2
3). 你是否经常不能抵制上网的诱惑或很难下网 1 2
4). 停止使用互联网时你是否会产生消极的情绪体验和不良的生理反应 1 2
5). 每次上网实际所花的时间是否都比原定时间要长 1 2
6). 上网是否已经对你的人际关系、学习造成负面影响 1 2
7). 你是否对家人、朋友或心理咨询人员隐瞒了上网的真实时间和费用 1 2
8). 你是否将上网作为逃避问题和排遣消极情绪的一种方式 1 2

296
第三部分、社交网络使用情况

【社交网络】是指QQ空间、人人网、朋友网、微博、开心网等可以建立自己社交页面
的网络服务,能够为人们提供交流互动的平台,其中不包括QQ等即时通讯工具。请根据你
使用社交网络的实际情况回答。

3.1 你是否拥有自己的社交网络账户? □ 1 是 □ 2 否(选“否”,请跳至“第四部分”)


a) 你主要通过什么设备登陆这些社交网络 (选最主要的一项) ?
□ 1 电脑(台式/笔记本) □ 2 平板电脑(如iPad等)
□ 3 智能手机 □ 4 其他,_________
b) 过去 1 年,你使用频率较高的社交网络有(最多选三项)
□ 1 QQ空间 □ 2 人人网(原校内网) □ 3 朋友网 □ 4 开心网
□ 5 微信 □ 6 微博(如新浪/腾讯等) □ 7 其他, 请说明___________

请根据正在使用的所有社交网络情况(包括各种社交网络的使用情况总和)回答下述问题。
3.2 你使用这些社交网络的时间有多久?(按使用时间最长的回答)
□ 1 <3 月 □ 2 3-6 月 □ 3 6-12 月 □ 4 1-2 年 □ 5 2-4 年 □ 6 >4 年
3.3 过去 1 个月,你平均每周有多少天用过社交网络?
□ 1 几乎没有用过 □2 1 天 □32 天 □43 天
□54 天 □6 5 天 □7 6 天 □87 天
3.4 过去 1 个月有使用社交网络的日子里,你平均每天花多少时间在社交网络上?
□ 1 <10 分钟 □ 2 10-30 分钟 □ 3 31-60 分钟
□ 4 61-90 分钟 □ 5 >90 分钟
3.5 在这些正在使用的社交网络上,你大约总共有多少个不同的朋友?
□ 1 ≤10 □ 2 11-50 □ 3 51-100 □ 4 101-150
□ 5 151-200 □ 6 201-250 □ 7 251-300 □ 8 301-400 □ 9 ≥400

3.6 回想你【最近 1 个月】使用社交网络(如QQ空间,人人网,朋友网等)的情况,选择


合适的选项。
最近 1 月的使用频率
【最近 1 个月】
从不 较少 有时 经常 总是
1). 在留言板上给好友留言 1 2 3 4 5
2). 通过即时通讯功能与朋友聊天 1 2 3 4 5
3). 回复朋友对自己社交网络内容的评论(如头
1 2 3 4 5
像、照片、状态等)
4). 评论朋友的动态、日志、照片等 1 2 3 4 5
5). 分享、转帖 1 2 3 4 5
6). 浏览其他人的日志、照片、动态、相册等 1 2 3 4 5
7). 浏览信息(如娱乐/时事新闻等) 1 2 3 4 5
8). 更新自己的状态 1 2 3 4 5
9). 发照片到自己的社交页面 1 2 3 4 5
10). 写日志、发微博 1 2 3 4 5
11). 编辑自己的社交页面(如更换头像、修改 1 2 3 4 5

297
联系方式、隐私设置等)
12). 看视频或听音乐等 1 2 3 4 5
13). 玩游戏或应用等 1 2 3 4 5
14). 购买/赠送虚拟物品 1 2 3 4 5
15). 其他活动,请说明:_____________ 1 2 3 4 5

3.7 请仔细阅读下面每句话,根据自己使用社交网络的经历(或感受)
,选择最能够代表你
自己想法的选项。
完全不 同 完全
不同意 一般
同意 意 同意
1). 社交网络是我每天活动的一部分 1 2 3 4 5
2). 我很骄傲的告诉他人我在使用社交网络 1 2 3 4 5
3). 社交网络已经成为我每天例行项目的一部分 1 2 3 4 5
4). 当一段时间不登录社交网络,我就会感觉与外
1 2 3 4 5
界失去了联系
5). 我感觉我是社交网络群体的一部分 1 2 3 4 5
6). 如果关闭社交网络,我会感到难过 1 2 3 4 5

3.8 请选择一个合适的选项,来代表你【最近 1 个月】使用社交网络的情况。


极不 不符 一 符 非常
【最近 1 个月】
符合 合 般 合 符合
1). 因为使用社交网络,我很难集中精力在我的学业上 1 2 3 4 5
2). 每天起床后我脑海里的第一件事就是登陆社交网络 1 2 3 4 5
3). 我花了太多的时间在社交网络上而出现失眠 1 2 3 4 5
4). 使用社交网络妨碍了我的社交活动 1 2 3 4 5
5). 当我情绪低落的时候,登陆社交网络会让我感觉好点 1 2 3 4 5
6). 我的家人/朋友认为我在社交网络上花的时间太多 1 2 3 4 5
7). 如果我不能访问社交网络,我会感到很焦虑 1 2 3 4 5
8). 我曾经尝试少花些时间在社交网络上,但是没有成功 1 2 3 4 5

298
Appendix I
Questionnaire for phase I validation study (English version)

Questionnaire for social networking use intensity among junior middle school students

Please fill the following information accurately, which is only used for questionnaire matching.
1. Last 4 digital of home-telephone number: X X X
2. Last 4 digital of your Identity Card number: X X X
3. Your birthday: ________year______month_____day
4. Initial letter of your spellname: _______(e.g. If your names is “Zhang San”, please fill: ZS)

Part I: Social-demographic background

1.1. Your gender: □ 1 Male □ 2 Female


1.2. Your current grade: □ 1 Seven □ 2 Eight □ 3 Ninth
1.3. Father education level:
□ 1 Primary school or below □ 2 Junior high school
□ 3 Senior high school (or equal academic qualification) □ 4 University or above
1.4. Mother education level:
□ 1 Primary school or below □ 2 Junior high school
□ 3 Senior high school (or equal academic qualification) □ 4 University or above
1.5. Do you possess a smartphone?
□ 1 Yes, Duration: _____year____month □ 2 No

Part II: Internet addiction


2. Do you agree with the following statement for yourself? Yes No
1) Do you feel preoccupied with the Internet (think about previous on-line
1 2
activity or anticipate next on-line session)?
2) Do you feel the need to use the Internet with increasing amounts of time in
1 2
order to achieve satisfaction?
3) Have you repeatedly made unsuccessful efforts to control, cut back, or stop
1 2
Internet use?
4) Do you feel restless, moody, depressed, or irritable when attempting to cut
1 2
down or stop Internet use
5) Do you stay on-line longer than originally intended? 1 2
6) Have you jeopardized or risked the loss of significant relationship,
1 2
educational opportunity because of the Internet?
7) Have you lied to family member, a therapist, or others to conceal the extent of
1 2
involvement with the Internet?
8) Do you use the Internet as a way of escaping from problem or of relieving a
1 2
dysphonic mood (e.g. feelings of helplessness, guilt, anxiety, depression)?

299
Part III: Social networking use
3.1. Do you have your own social networking accounts? □ 1 Yes □ 2 No (Please jump to Part
IV)
a) Device that you most frequently used for accessing social networking platforms
□ 1 Computer (Desk/Laptop) □ 2 Tablet (e.g. Ipad)
□ 3 Smartphone □ 4 Other, _________
b) During the past year, the first three mostly frequently used social networking services
□ 1 Qzone □ 2 RenRen/Xiaonei □ 3 Pengyou □ 4 Kaixin
□ 5 WeChat □ 6 Weibo (e.g. Sina/Tencent) □ 7 Other, _______

Please answer the following questions according to the conditions that you use online social
networking (total of all online social networking use conditions)
3.2. How long have you used these online social networking (Based on the longest one)?
□ 1 <3 months □ 2 3~6 months □ 3 6~12 months
□ 4 1~2 years □ 5 2~4 years □ 6 more than 4 years
3.3. During the past one month, on average, approximately how many days per week do you
have used online social networking?
□ 1 Almost not □ 2 1 day □ 3 2 days □ 4 3 days
□ 5 4 days □ 6 5 days □ 7 6 days □ 8 7 days
3.4. During the past one month, on average, approximately how many minutes per day have
you spent on using online social networking?
□ 1 <10 mins □ 2 10~30 mins □ 3 31~60 mins □ 4 61~90 mins □ 5 >90 mins
3.5. About how many total social networking friends do you have?
□ 1 ≤10 □ 2 11-50 □ 3 51-100 □ 4 101-150
□ 5 151-200 □ 6 201-250 □ 7 251-300 □ 8 301-400 □ 9 ≥400

3.6. During the past one month, how frequent do you do the following activities on online social
networking (e.g. Qzone, Renren)?
Frequency during the past one month
During the past one month
Never Few Sometimes Often Always
1). Sent messages to friends on message board 1 2 3 4 5
2). Chatted with friends via instant messaging
1 2 3 4 5
function
3). Replied to comments made by social networking
1 2 3 4 5
friends
4). Commented on friends’ status, logs, and photos 1 2 3 4 5
5). Shared/Forwarded contents 1 2 3 4 5
6). Browsed others’ logs/photos/status/album 1 2 3 4 5
7). Updated self-status 1 2 3 4 5
8). Posted photos/videos on personal web profile 1 2 3 4 5
9). Wrote logs/Weibo 1 2 3 4 5
10). Decorated personal web profile(changing
1 2 3 4 5
image/contact information/privacy setting)
11). Surfed entertainment/current news 1 2 3 4 5

300
12). Watched video/listened to music 1 2 3 4 5
13). Played games/applications 1 2 3 4 5
14). Bought/gave virtual Goods (e.g. birthday gifts) 1 2 3 4 5
15). Others, __________ 1 2 3 4 5

3.7. Please clarify the extent to which the following statement is appropriate to you?
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Neutral Agree
disagree agree
1). Social networking is part of my
1 2 3 4 5
everyday activity
2). I am proud to tell people I’m on social
1 2 3 4 5
networking
3). Social networking has become part of
1 2 3 4 5
my daily routine
4). I feel out of touch when I haven’t
logged onto social networking for a 1 2 3 4 5
while
5). I feel I am part of the social networking
1 2 3 4 5
community
6). I would be sorry if social networking
1 2 3 4 5
shut down

3.8. Please clarify the extent to which the following statement is appropriated to you during the
past one month?
Strongly Strongly
During the past one month Disagree Neutral Agree
disagree agree
1). I have difficulties for focusing on my
academic work due to mu social 1 2 3 4 5
networking use
2). The first thing on my mind when I get up
1 2 3 4 5
is to log into social networking
3). I lose sleep over spending more time on
1 2 3 4 5
social networking
4). My social networking use interferes with
1 2 3 4 5
doing social activities
5). I log into social networking to make
1 2 3 4 5
myself feel better when I am down
6). My family or friends think that I spend
1 2 3 4 5
too much time on social networking
7). I feel anxious if I cannot access to social
1 2 3 4 5
networking
8). I have attempted to spend less time on
1 2 3 4 5
social networking but have not succeeded

301
Appendix II
Questionnaire for phase II longitudinal study (Chinese version)

初中生社交网络使用及其对社会心理健康的影响调查问卷

同学们,你们好。这是一份调查问卷,目的是了解初中生社交网络的使用情况、及其对
社会心理健康的影响。研究结果将有助于促进我国青少年身心的健康发展。现诚挚邀请你填
写一份调查问卷,并于本次调查后约 10 个月再次填写一份相似的问卷。本调查以自愿参加
为原则,采取匿名方式填写,你无需将自己的姓名写在问卷上,同时你们提供的宝贵信息将
仅用于科学研究,个人资料绝对保密,不会告知老师、家长、同学等人,保证不会外泄。
请仔细阅读问卷内容,根据条目的要求认真作答,不要遗漏。问卷中涉及的问题,选项
并无对错之分。在回答问卷的过程中,如有不清楚的地方,请举手询问我们的研究人员,他
们会尽力给予解释。如果你填写了此问卷,即视为知情同意。你的参与对我们的研究十分重
要,再次感谢你的参与!

 填写方法:在相应选项的“□”处打“√”,或在“_____”处填写

请首先填写下面的信息,以下信息仅用于问卷匹配,请务必准确填写:
1.家庭住宅电话最后四位数字:X X X
2.父亲手机号码最后四位数字:X X X
3.母亲手机号码最后四位数字:X X X
4.你的身份证号最后四位数字:X X X
5.你的生日: 年 月 日
6.你、父亲及母亲姓名最后一个字的汉语拼音的最后一个字母:
自己:_______; 父亲: _______; 母亲: _______
(例如你叫“张小天(TIAN)”,父亲叫“张海肖(XIAO)”,母亲叫“欧晓辉(HUI)”,请依次填:N O I)

第一部分、基本情况

1.1. 你的性别: □男 □女
1.2. 你现在的年级: □初一 □初二
1.3. 父亲的文化程度:
□小学及以下 □初中 □高中/中专 □本科(或大专)及以上
1.4. 母亲的文化程度:
□小学及以下 □初中 □高中/中专 □本科(或大专)及以上
1.5. 你家里的经济状况:
□非常好 □较好 □一般 □较差 □非常差
1.6. 你在班级中的学习成绩:
□中上(班中前 1/3) □中等 □中下(班中后 1/3)
1.7. 你认为自己的学习压力如何?
□没有 □较轻 □一般 □较重 □很重

302
1.8. 你是否和父母住一起?
□与父母同住 □只与父亲同住 □只与母亲同住 □父母都不与我同住

第二部分、网络成瘾

2.请仔细阅读下面每一句话,根据自己的实际情况进行选择 是 否
a). 你是否沉溺于互联网(总想着以前上网的经历或是期待着下一次的上网) 1 2
b). 你是否需要通过增加上网时间以获得满足感 1 2
c). 你是否经常不能抵制上网的诱惑或很难下网 1 2
d). 停止使用互联网时你是否会产生消极的情绪体验和不良的生理反应 1 2
e). 每次上网实际所花的时间是否都比原定时间要长 1 2
f). 上网是否已经对你的人际关系、学习造成负面影响 1 2
g). 你是否对家人、朋友或心理咨询人员隐瞒了上网的真实时间和费用 1 2
h). 你是否将上网作为逃避问题和排遣消极情绪的一种方式 1 2

三、社交网络使用情况

【社交网络】是指QQ空间、人人网、朋友网、微博、开心网等可以建立自己社交页面
的网络服务,能够为人们提供交流互动的平台,其中不包括QQ等即时通讯工具。请根据你
使用社交网络的实际情况回答。

3.1. 你有自己的社交网络吗?
□有 □没有(如选“没有”,请跳至“四、社会心理等健康状况”)

3.2. 回想你【过去 1 个月】使用社交网络(如QQ空间,人人网,朋友网等)的情况,你都


在社交网络上做什么?请选择合适的选项。
最近 1 个月,你在社交网络上 从不 较少 有时 经常 总是
a). 在留言板上给好友留言 1 2 3 4 5
b). 通过即时通讯功能与朋友聊天 1 2 3 4 5
c). 回复朋友对自己社交网络内容的评论(如头像、
1 2 3 4 5
照片、状态等)
d). 评论朋友的动态、日志、照片等 1 2 3 4 5
e). 分享、转帖 1 2 3 4 5
f). 浏览其他人的日志、照片、动态、相册等 1 2 3 4 5
g). 更新自己的状态 1 2 3 4 5
h). 发照片到自己的社交页面 1 2 3 4 5
i). 写日志、发微博 1 2 3 4 5
j). 编辑自己的社交页面(如更换头像、修改联系方
1 2 3 4 5
式、隐私设置等)
k). 浏览信息(如娱乐/时事新闻等) 1 2 3 4 5
l). 看视频或听音乐等 1 2 3 4 5
m). 玩游戏或应用等 1 2 3 4 5
n). 购买/赠送虚拟物品 1 2 3 4 5

303
3.3. 请仔细阅读下面每句话,根据自己使用社交网络的经历(或感受)
,选择最能够代表你
想法的选项。
完全不 不同 完全
一般 同意
同意 意 同意
a). 社交网络是我每天活动的一部分 1 2 3 4 5
b). 我很骄傲的告诉他人我在使用社交网络 1 2 3 4 5
c). 社交网络已经成为我每天例行项目的一部分 1 2 3 4 5
d). 当一段时间不登录社交网络,我就会感觉与外
1 2 3 4 5
界失去了联系
e). 我感觉我是社交网络群体的一部分 1 2 3 4 5
f). 如果关闭社交网络,我会感到难过 1 2 3 4 5

3.4. 请选择一个合适的选项,来代表你【最近 1 个月】使用社交网络的情况。


极不 不符 一 符 非常
【最近 1 个月】
符合 合 般 合 符合
a). 因为使用社交网络,我很难集中精力在我的学业上 1 2 3 4 5
b). 每天起床后我脑海里的第一件事就是登陆社交网络 1 2 3 4 5
c). 我花了太多的时间在社交网络上而出现失眠 1 2 3 4 5
d). 使用社交网络妨碍了我的社交活动 1 2 3 4 5
e). 当我情绪低落的时候,登陆社交网络会让我感觉好点 1 2 3 4 5
f). 我的家人/朋友认为我在社交网络上花的时间太多 1 2 3 4 5
g). 如果我不能访问社交网络,我会感到很焦虑 1 2 3 4 5
h). 我曾经尝试少花些时间在社交网络上,但是没有成功 1 2 3 4 5

四、社会心理等健康状况

4.1. 请阅读下面每句话,并根据你自己的实际情况,选择一个最能够代表自己的选项。
极不 很不 稍不 中 稍同 很同 极同
同意 同意 同意 立 意 意 意
a). 当我有需要的时候,总有一个好朋友在我身边 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
b). 我有一个好朋友,无论开心或者不开心,我都可
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
以同他/她分享
c). 我的家人真的十分愿意帮助我 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
d). 我的家人可以给我心理上的支持 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
e). 我有一个真的可以安慰我的朋友 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
f). 我的朋友真的愿意帮助我 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
g). 如果有什么事发生,我可以依靠我的朋友 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
h). 我可以和家人诉说我自己的问题 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
i). 我有一些朋友,无论开心或不开心,我都可以同
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
他们分享
j). 我生命中有个好朋友,他/她会关心我的感受 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

304
k). 我的家人愿意和我一起做决定 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
l). 我可以和朋友倾诉自己的问题 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

4.2. 以下是一些用来描述个人感受的句子,请选择最能够代表你的感受的选项。
很不 非常
不符合 符合
符合 符合
a). 我感到我是一个有价值的人,至少与其他人在同一水 1 2 3 4
平上
b). 我感到我有许多好的品质 1 2 3 4
c). 归根结底,我倾向于觉得自己是一个失败者 1 2 3 4
d). 我能像大多数人一样把事情做好 1 2 3 4
e). 我感到自己值得自豪的地方不多 1 2 3 4
f). 我对自己持肯定态度 1 2 3 4
g). 总的来说,我对自己是满意的 1 2 3 4
h). 我希望我能为自己能得更多尊重 1 2 3 4
i). 我确实时常感到毫无用处 1 2 3 4
j). 我时常认为自己一无是处 1 2 3 4

4.3. 下面是对你可能存在的或最近有过的感受的描述,请按照你【最近 1 周】出现这种感


受的实际情况,选择最适合你的答案。
沒有或偶 时常或一 多数时间
有时
最近 1 周 尔(少于 1 半时间 或持续
(1-2 天)
天) (3-4 天) (5-7 天)
1). 一些通常并不困扰我的事使我心烦 1 2 3 4
2). 我不想吃东西;我胃口不好 1 2 3 4
3). 我觉得即便有家人或朋友帮助也无
1 2 3 4
法摆脱这种苦闷
4). 我感觉同别人一样好 1 2 3 4
5). 我很难集中精力做事 1 2 3 4
6). 我感到压抑 1 2 3 4
7). 我感到做什么事都很吃力 1 2 3 4
8). 我觉得未来有希望 1 2 3 4
9). 我认为我的生活一无是处 1 2 3 4
10). 我感到恐惧 1 2 3 4
11). 我睡觉不解乏 1 2 3 4
12). 我很幸福 1 2 3 4
13). 我比平时话少了 1 2 3 4
14). 我感到孤独 1 2 3 4
15). 人们对我不友好 1 2 3 4
16). 我生活快乐 1 2 3 4
17). 我曾经放声痛哭 1 2 3 4
18). 我感到忧愁 1 2 3 4

305
19). 我觉得别人厌恶我 1 2 3 4
20). 我走路很慢 1 2 3 4

4.4. 下面几个问题是想了解你与朋友的关系,请选择与你的实际情况最相符合的选项。
1). 你觉得班上的同学对你友好吗?
□不友好 □有一点友好 □比较友好 □非常友好
2). 你的朋友关心你吗?
□不关心 □有一点关心 □比较关心 □非常关心
3). 你的朋友多吗?
□有一两个 □有三四个 □有五六个 □有很多
4). 你对自己的朋友满意吗?
□非常不满意 □有一点不满意 □比较满意 □非常满意
5). 需要帮助时,你能找到可信赖的朋友吗?
□从不这样 □很少这样 □经常这样 □总是这样
6). 你能从集体活动中得到快乐吗?
□从不这样 □很少这样 □经常这样 □总是这样

4.5. 请认真阅读下面每一句话,看自己是否这样,然后选择一个合适的选项。
完全不 比较不 比较 完全
一般
符合 符合 符合 符合
a). 我经常会考虑别人对我的看法 1 2 3 4 5
b). 我对于别人的批评非常敏感 1 2 3 4 5
c). 在一大群人面前演讲,我感到非常困难 1 2 3 4 5
d). 一本正经地讲笑话对我来说是件很困难
1 2 3 4 5
的事情

4.6. 过去 1 个月,你与父母在下面这些方面是否经常发生冲突(如情绪、语言或身体方面)?
请根据实际情况,选择最接近实际情况的选项,并在表格内填上相应的数字。

1=从未发生 2=每月一两次 3=约一周一次 4=一周几次 5=几乎每天都发生

父亲 母亲
a). 你的学业方面(如家庭作业、成绩等)
b). 家务方面(如打扫自己的房间、帮助家人干活)
c). 花钱方面(如怎样花钱、买什么等)
d). 日常生活安排方面(如睡觉时间、饮食习惯、看电视等)
e). 外表方面(如衣着、发式等)
f). 家庭成员关系方面(如尊重、礼貌、兄弟姐妹的关系)
g). 你的隐私方面(如私拆信件、偷听电话等)

306
Appendix II
Questionnaire for phase II longitudinal study (English version)

The impact of social networking use intensity on psychosocial well-being among


junior middle school students

Please fill the following information accurately, which is only used for questionnaire matching.
1. Last 4 digital of home-telephone number: X X X
2. Last 4 digital of father mobile phone number: X X X
3. Last 4 digital of mother mobile phone number: X X X
4. Last 4 digital of your Identity Card number: X X X
5.Your Birthady: year month day
6. Last letter of the last word of your, your father’s and mother’ spellname:
Self:_______; Father: _______; Mother: _______
(e.g. Your name is ”Zhang Xiaotian (TIAN)”, your father name is ”Zhang Haixiao(XIAO)”, and your mother
name is ”Ou Xiaohui (HUI)”, please successively fill in “N O I”.

Part I: Background characteristics


1.1 Your gender: □Male □Female
1.2 Your current grade: □Seven □Eight
1.3 Father education level:
□Primary school or below □Junior high school
□Senior high school (or equal academic qualification) □University or above
1.4 Mother education level:
□Primary school or below □Junior high school
□Senior high school (or equal academic qualification) □University or above
1.5 How do you think of your family financial situation?
□very good □Good □General □Poor □very poor
1.6 Your academic performance in the class:
□Above the average (The first of one third) □Average
□Below the average (The end of one third)
1.7 How do you perceive your current study pressure?
□Nil □Light □General □Heavy □Very heavy

Part II: Internet addiction


2. Do you agree with the following statement for yourself? Yes No
a). Do you feel preoccupied with the Internet (think about previous on-line activity
1 2
or anticipate next on-line session)?
b). Do you feel the need to use the Internet with increasing amounts of time in
1 2
order to achieve satisfaction?
c). Have you repeatedly made unsuccessful efforts to control, cut back, or stop
1 2
Internet use?
d). Do you feel restless, moody, depressed, or irritable when attempting to cut 1 2

307
down or stop Internet use
e). Do you stay on-line longer than originally intended? 1 2
f). Have you jeopardized or risked the loss of significant relationship, educational
1 2
opportunity because of the Internet?
g). Have you lied to family member, a therapist, or others to conceal the extent of
1 2
involvement with the Internet?
h). Do you use the Internet as a way of escaping from problem or of relieving a
1 2
dysphonic mood (e.g. feelings of helplessness, guilt, anxiety, depression)?

Part III: Social networking use


3.1 Do you have your own social networking accounts?
□Yes □No (Please jump to Part IV “psychosocial conditions”)
3.2 During the past one month, how frequent do you do the following activities on social
networking (e.g. Qzone, Renren)?
During the past one month Never Few Sometimes Often Always
a). Sent messages to friends on message board 1 2 3 4 5
b). Chatted with friends via instant messaging function 1 2 3 4 5
c). Replied to comments made by social networking
1 2 3 4 5
friends
d). Commented on friends’ status, logs, and photos 1 2 3 4 5
e). Shared/Forwarded contents 1 2 3 4 5
f). Browsed others’ logs/photos/status/album 1 2 3 4 5
g). Updated self-status 1 2 3 4 5
h). Posted photos/videos on personal web profile 1 2 3 4 5
i). Wrote logs/weibo 1 2 3 4 5
j). Decorated personal web profile(changing
1 2 3 4 5
image/contact information/privacy setting)
k). Surfed entertainment/current news 1 2 3 4 5
l). Watched video/listened to music 1 2 3 4 5
m). Played games/applications 1 2 3 4 5
n). Bought/gave virtual Goods (e.g. birthday gifts) 1 2 3 4 5

3.3 Please clarify the extent to which the following statement is appropriate to you?
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Neutral Agree
disagree agree
a). Social networking is part of my
1 2 3 4 5
everyday activity
b). I am proud to tell people I’m on social
1 2 3 4 5
networking
c). Social networking has become part of
1 2 3 4 5
my daily routine
d). I feel out of touch when I haven’t
logged onto social networking for a 1 2 3 4 5
while
e). I feel I am part of the social networking
1 2 3 4 5
community

308
f). I would be sorry if social networking
1 2 3 4 5
shut down

3.4 During the past one month, please clarify the extent to which the following statement is
appropriate to you?
Strongly Strongly
During the past one month Disagree Neutral Agree
disagree agree
a). I have difficulties for focusing on my
academic work due to mu social 1 2 3 4 5
networking use
b). The first thing on my mind when I get up
1 2 3 4 5
is to log into social networking
c). I lose sleep over spending more time on
1 2 3 4 5
social networking
d). My social networking use interferes with
1 2 3 4 5
doing social activities
e). I log into social networking to make
1 2 3 4 5
myself feel better when I am down
f). My family or friends think that I spend
1 2 3 4 5
too much time on social networking
g). I feel anxious if I cannot access to social
1 2 3 4 5
networking
h). I have attempted to spend less time on
1 2 3 4 5
social networking but have not succeeded

Part IV Psychosocial conditions


4.1 We are interested in how you feel about the following statements. Read each statement
carefully. Indicate how you feel about each statement.
Very Very
Strongly Mildly Mildly Strongly
strongly Neutral strongly
disagree disagree agree agree
disagree agree
a). There is a special person who is
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
around when I am in need
b). There is a special person with
whom I can share my joys and 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
sorrows
c). My family really tries to help me 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
d). I got the emotional help and
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
support I need from my family
e). I have a special person who is a
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
real source of comfort to me
f). My friends really try to help me 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
g). I can count on my friends when
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
things go wrong
h). I can told about my problems with
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
my family
i). I have friends with whom I can 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

309
share my joys and sorrows
j). There is a special person in my
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
life who cares about my feelings
k). My family is willing to help me
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
make decisions
l). I can talk about my problems with
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
my friends

4.2 Below is a list of statements dealing with your general feelings about yourself. Please
indicate how strongly you agree or disagree with each statement
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree
disagree agree
a). I feel that I am a person of worth, at least on
1 2 3 4
an equal plane with others
b). I feel that I have a number of good qualities 1 2 3 4
c). All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a
1 2 3 4
failure
d). I am able to do things as well as most other
1 2 3 4
people
e). I feel I do not have much to be proud of 1 2 3 4
f). I take a positive attitude toward myself 1 2 3 4
g). On the whole, I am satisfied with myself 1 2 3 4
h). I wish I could have more respect for myself 1 2 3 4
i). I certainly feel useless at times 1 2 3 4
j). At times I think I am no good at all 1 2 3 4

4.3 Below is a list of some of the ways you may have felt or behaved. Please indicate how often
you’ve felt this way during the past week. Respond to all items
Rarely or Some or a Occasionally
All of the
none of the little of the or a moderate
During the past one week times (5-7
time (less time (1~2 amount of time
days)
than 1 day) days (3~4 days)
1). I am bothered by things that
1 2 3 4
usually don’t bother me
2). I did not feel like eating; my
1 2 3 4
appetite was poor
3). I felt that I could not shake off
the blues even with help from 1 2 3 4
my family
4). I felt that I was just as good as
1 2 3 4
other people
5). I have trouble keeping my mind
1 2 3 4
on what I was doing
6). I felt depressed 1 2 3 4
7). I felt that everything I did was
1 2 3 4
an effort
8). I felt hopeful about the future 1 2 3 4
9). I thought my life had been a 1 2 3 4

310
failure
10). I felt fearful 1 2 3 4
11). My sleep was restless 1 2 3 4
12). I was happy 1 2 3 4
13). I talked less than usual 1 2 3 4
14). I felt lonely 1 2 3 4
15). People were unfriendly 1 2 3 4
16). I enjoyed life 1 2 3 4
17). I had crying spells 1 2 3 4
18). I felt sad 1 2 3 4
19). I felt that people disliked me 1 2 3 4
20). I could not “get going” 1 2 3 4

4.4 The following questions is related to your relationship quality with your friends, please
indicate how much the statement is appropriate with your condition.
1). How do you think that your classmates are friendly with you?
□Not friendly □A little friendly □Friendly □Very friendly
2). Do your friends care about you?
□Not care □A little care □Care □Very care
3). How many do you have friends?
□One or two □Three or four □Five or six □So many
4). Do you satisfy with your friends?
□Very dissatisfied □Dissatisfied □Satisfied □Very satisfied
5). When you need help, can you find a trusted friend?
□Never □Rarely □Often □Always
6). Can you obtain happiness from group activities?
□Never □Rarely □Often □Always

4.5 Please read the following statement carefully, and then according to your personal condition,
select an appropriate number for each item.
Definitely Relatively
Relatively Exactly
not like not like Neutral
like me like me
me me
a). I often care what others think of
1 2 3 4 5
me
b). I am very sensitive to others’
1 2 3 4 5
criticism to me
c). I often feel uncomfortable when
1 2 3 4 5
talking before a crowd of people
d). It is a very difficult or me to tell
1 2 3 4 5
jokes seriously

4.6 During the past one month, how often do you have conflict with your father/mother in
terms of the following aspects (e.g. emotional, spoken, or physical)? Please fit the appropriate
number for each item, father and mother separately, according to your personal condition.
1=Never 2=1~2 times/month 3=about 1 time/week 4=several times/week 5=Almost every day

311
Father Mother
a). Your academic aspects (e.g. homework, academic record)
b). Housework (e.g. cleaning your room, Helping family members to do
housework )
c). Spending money (e.g. how do you spend money, what do you buy spending
money?)
d). Daily living arrangements(e.g. sleeping time, Eating habits, watching TV)
e). Body image (e.g. clothes, Hair styles)
f). Relationships with family members (e.g. respects, Courtesy, Relationship
between siblings)
g). Your privacy (watching you letter without your permission, Eavesdropping
your phone talking)

312

You might also like