You are on page 1of 11

Computers in Human Behavior 124 (2021) 106932

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Partner phubbing: Why using your phone during interactions with your
partner can be detrimental for your relationship
Camiel J. Beukeboom a, *, Monique Pollmann b
a
Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, the Netherlands
b
Tilburg University, the Netherlands

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Previous research showed that phone use during co-present interactions with one’s partner (partner phubbing) is
Phubbing negatively related to relationship satisfaction. In two cross-sectional surveys (N = 507 and N = 386) we
Mobile phone confirmed this finding and also extended it by focusing on the mediating role of feelings of exclusion, perceived
Relationship satisfaction
partner responsiveness, perceived intimacy, conflict about phone use, and feelings of jealousy. Results of both
Exclusion
Partner responsiveness
studies demonstrate that the link between partner phubbing and relationship satisfaction was mediated by
Intimacy feelings of exclusion, less perceived partner responsiveness, and less intimacy. We observed no significant
mediation effects of conflict over phone use and jealousy when the three significant mediators were taken into
account. In contrast to previous work, this suggests that conflict and jealousy are not the primary mechanism
through which pphubbing results in reduced relationship satisfaction. Moreover, we demonstrated that shared
phone use moderates the adverse effects of pphubbing. This means that by involving and informing a partner
about one’s phone activities, it is possible to reduce feelings of exclusion, maintain more responsiveness and
intimacy in the conversation, and consequently reduce detrimental relationship effects.

Mobile phones have become ubiquitous in our daily lives, and however, is rather limited. Relying on existing experimental studies on
consequently they also influence the dynamics of intimate relationships. phubbing among strangers in laboratory settings (e.g., Przybylski &
On the one hand mobile phones can have positive effects as they allow Weinstein, 2013) and studies using video vignettes (e.g., Chotpitaya­
partners to stay in touch, and show interest when he or she is not around sunondh & Douglas, 2018), we propose and test a number of potential
(Murray & Cambell, 2015; Pollmann, Norman, & Crockett, 2021). On mediators that could explain the negative effects of partner-phubbing in
the other hand, when a mobile phone is used in the presence of one’s intimate relationships.
partner, it can distract from the present conversation and be a source of Moreover, not much research has focused on factors that could
annoyance and conflict (Miller-Ott & Kelly, 2015; Dwyer et al., 2018; reduce the partner-phubbing relationship satisfaction link. It is impor­
Przybylski & Weinstein, 2013). tant to learn more about such factors, as these could provide a potential
The use of a mobile phone during a conversation is called phubbing remedy against the detrimental effects of (nearly inevitable) phone use
(Ugur & Koc, 2015). The term phubbing (a portmanteau of ‘phone’ and in relationships. One factor relates to the way one uses the phone in co-
‘snubbing’) refers to the act of focusing on one’s mobile phone during a present interactions. We expect that when a partner involves and in­
conversation instead of paying attention to a conversation partner (Ugur forms the other about their phone use activities this could reduce its
& Koc, 2015). If this act occurs within relationships, it is called partner- negative effects.
phubbing (pphubbing; Roberts & David, 2016). We conducted two well-powered cross-sectional surveys among in­
Several studies have demonstrated that partner-phubbing is nega­ dividuals in a relationship, in order to test the mediating and moderating
tively related to relationship satisfaction (David & Roberts, 2021; Hal­ role of several variables in the hypothesized link between partner-
pern & Katz, 2017; Krasnova, Abramova, Notter, & Baumann, 2016; phubbing and relationship satisfaction.
McDaniel & Coyne, 2016; McDaniel & Drouin, 2019; McDaniel, Galo­
van, & Drouin, 2020; Roberts & David, 2016; Wang, Xie, Wang, Wang, &
Lei, 2017). Evidence for the underlying mechanisms of this link,

* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: c.j.beukeboom@vu.nl (C.J. Beukeboom).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106932
Received 30 March 2021; Received in revised form 22 June 2021; Accepted 23 June 2021
Available online 30 June 2021
0747-5632/© 2021 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
C.J. Beukeboom and M. Pollmann Computers in Human Behavior 124 (2021) 106932

1. Phubbing in relationships experiment by Chotpitayasunondh and Douglas (2018) participants


were exposed to a virtual conversation partner in a muted 3-min 3D
In partner-phubbing one can distinguish a phubber and a phubbee. animation, who either phubbed them extensively, partially, or not at all.
The phubber is the person who, during a co-present social interaction, Results showed that phubbing had negative effects on respondents’ four
focuses all or part of their attention on their cell phone. The phubbee is fundamental human needs associated with social exclusion: belonging,
the person who is snubbed by their mobile using partner (Chotpitaya­ self-esteem, meaningful existence, and control (Williams, 2001). Similar
sunondh & Douglas, 2016). Although it is likely that in relationships effects were observed by Gonzales and Wu (2016) and McDaniel &
phubbing behavior often occurs by both partners, in the present Wesselmann, 2021 in interactions with confederates, by Hales et al.
research, we focus on the side of the phubbee and their experienced (2018) using an autobiographical recall method, and David & Roberts,
relationship satisfaction. 2017 using a prime method.
Various scholars have noted the negative effects that partner- Research has shown that people show immediate negative responses
phubbing can have on conversation quality and relationship satisfac­ to signs of social exclusion, like a withdrawal of eye contact by one’s
tion (Vanden Abeele, 2020; Vanden Abeele et al., 2016; Vanden Abeele conversation partner (Wirth, Sacco, Hugenberg, & Williams, 2010).
et al., 2019). Using a cell phone during co-present interactions creates a However, when experiencing ostracism, people attempt to make attri­
situation in which one is physically, but not mentally present. Such butions about why one is being ostracized, and these attributions are
face-to-face interactions in which one is ‘absent present’ (Gergen, 2002) important for its effects (McDaniel & Wesselmann, 2021; Williams,
or ‘alone together’ (cf. Turkle, 2011) is argued to reduce the experienced 2009). Reduced eye contact that is caused by phone gazing (as opposed
quality of conversations, which in turn undermines relationship satis­ to newspaper gazing), can therefore have an additional detrimental ef­
faction (McDaniel & Coyne, 2014). fect, because a phone represents the “virtual other” (VanDen Abeele &
Although quite some research has been conducted on phubbing Postma-Nilsenova, 2018). Phubbees may attribute a phubbers phone
(Vanden Abeele, 2020 for a recent overview), the number of studies that gazing as an attempt to seek contact with others, and consequently as a
focused on phubbing in intimate relationships is limited. The survey sign of disinterest in the copresent conversation, and that they are not
studies that have been published, however, show quite consistent evi­ ‘worthy’ of the phubber’s full attention (Chotpitayasunondh & Douglas,
dence that partner-phubbing is negatively associated with relationship 2018; VanDen Abeele & Postma-Nilsenova, 2018). However, when the
satisfaction (David & Roberts, 2021; Halpern & Katz, 2017; Krasnova phubber or the situation provides clear attributional information for
et al., 2016; McDaniel & Coyne, 2016; McDaniel & Drouin, 2019; phone use during an interaction this may reduce feelings of exclusion
McDaniel et al., 2020; Roberts & David, 2016; Wang et al., 2017). and further negative effects (McDaniel & Wesselmann, 2021).
Although some studies have shown that the effect does not always Although the above studies did not focus on intimate relationships,
replicate; i.e., Cizmeci (2017) reports a small positive correlation be­ we expect that the extent in which people experience feelings of
tween pphubbing and relationship satisfaction, and Servies (2012) re­ exclusion when being phubbed by their partner explains a potential
ports that the correlation exists only in men but not in women, we still lower relationship satisfaction.
hypothesize that: (b) Partner responsiveness. A second potential explanatory variable for
the link between partner-phubbing and relationship satisfaction is
H1. The more partner-phubbing people perceive, the less satisfied they
partner responsiveness. Perceiving one’s partner as responsive is
are with their relationship.
strongly related to good relationship functioning (Reis, Clark, & Holmes,
2004). To build and maintain an intimate relationship, partner respon­
1.1. Explanatory factors for a partner-phubbing satisfaction link siveness in interactions plays a crucial role (Reis & Shaver, 1988). A
responsive partner is a partner who responds supportively and
Of the above-mentioned studies, only a few have tested potential empathically to the other, and makes them feel understood, validated,
mediators to explain the link between pphubbing and relationship and cared for. In an interaction that means that when one discloses
satisfaction. Conflict over phone use is the one variable that has, in personally revealing feelings or information to the partner, the partner
several studies, been demonstrated to significantly mediate this link should listen proactively and react with responses that show under­
(Halpern & Katz, 2017; McDaniel & Coyne, 2016; McDaniel & Drouin, standing (Bavelas, Coates & Johnson, 2002). When conversation part­
2019; McDaniel et al., 2020; Roberts & David, 2016). The idea is that the ners are distracted and interrupted by their phone use, however, this
negative relationship effects of partner-phubbing are caused by conflicts hinders in expressing those responsive behaviors that contribute to the
partners have about phone use. In the present contribution, we focus on development of affiliation and intimacy.
additional potential mediators that can explain a relationship between Laboratory studies focusing on interactions between strangers show
partner-phubbing and relationship satisfaction, and a moderator that that even the mere presence of a phone can have negative effects on
gives insight in ways to mitigate detrimental effects. closeness, perceived partner empathy, and conversation quality (Przy­
Relationship satisfaction is best described as the degree to which the bylski & Weinstein, 2013). Similar effects were observed in conversa­
partner satisfies the other’s wants and needs (Peleg, 2008). For rela­ tions among acquaintances in a naturalistic setting (pairs of visitors in a
tionship satisfaction, the quality of communication between partners is coffeeshop; Misra, Cheng, Genevie, & Yuan, 2016). If either participant
very important, and appears to matter more than the time partners spontaneously placed a cell phone on the table or held it in their hand
spend together (Guldner & Swensen, 1995). Based on other work, we during a 10-min conversation, the quality of the conversation and
expect that partner-phubbing trumps relationship satisfaction because it experienced empathetic concern was afterwards rated to be less ful­
affects feelings of exclusion, perceived partner responsiveness, filling compared with conversations held in the absence of cell phones.
perceived intimacy, conflict about phone use, and feelings of jealousy. These effects were more pronounced for participants who reported
(a) Feelings of exclusion. The first potential mediator, feelings of having a closer relationship, compared with those who were less
exclusion, has previously been related to phubbing (Chotpitayasunondh familiar with each other.
& Douglas, 2018; Gonzales & Wu, 2016; Hales, Dvir, Wesselmann, Based on findings that partner responsiveness is likely hampered by
Kruger, & Finkenauer, 2018; McDaniel & Wesselmann, 2021). Social phubbing during interactions, and it is regarded as an important pre­
exclusion, or ostracism, is defined as the perception of “being invisible dictor of relationship satisfaction (Lemay, Clark, & Feeney, 2007), we
and being excluded from the social interactions of those around you” hypothesize that partner responsiveness plays a mediating role between
(Williams, 2001, p. 2). Being phubbed by others has the crucial element partner-phubbing and relationship satisfaction.
of social exclusion in that one is ignored, and shut out of a social (c) Perceived intimacy. The third potential mediator is closely related
interaction, whilst remaining in the physical presence the other. In an to partner responsiveness. Intimacy is usually described as the result of a

2
C.J. Beukeboom and M. Pollmann Computers in Human Behavior 124 (2021) 106932

dynamic, transactional, interactive process in which self-disclosure and relationship satisfaction link; shared phone use. With shared phone use
partner responsiveness are key components. Intimacy thus develops in we measured the extent in which one is informed by one’s partner about,
interactions in which one individual discloses personal information, and involved in, their phone use when they start to use it in their
thoughts, and feelings to a partner; receives a response from the partner, presence.1 We expect that when a phubbing partner shares what he or
and interprets that response as understanding, validating, and caring she is doing when using the phone (i.e., by explaining and/or showing
(Laurenceau, Barrett, & Pietromonaco, 1998; Reis & Shaver, 1988). his or her phone activities) this could mitigate its negative effects. A first
A two-wave survey by Halpern and Katz (2017) found evidence for reason for this expectation follows from the idea that phubbees use this
the mediating effect of intimacy between partner-phubbing and rela­ information in their attribution processes as discussed above. An
tionship quality. Their measures of both partner-phubbing and intimacy experiment by McDaniel and Wesselmann (2021) showed that feelings
were, however, unconventional. That is, pphubbing was measured with of exclusion were significantly weaker when a phubber (a confederate in
one item, and the items for intimacy were in themselves related to an actual interaction) provided an important reason (mother in hospital)
phubbing behaviors (e.g., ‘Text or chat one another despite being in the as compared to a trivial reason (making plans with friends). This sug­
same location’; ‘Feel social media and texting are negatively impacting gests that phubbees use such information and it can weaken negative
intimacy in your relationship’). In the present studies, we use the Social effects. In contrast, when a phubber does not share any information or
Intimacy Scale (Miller & Lefcourt, 1982) to measure experienced rela­ explanation for their phone use at all, there likely is more attributional
tionship intimacy independently, and test its mediating effect. ambiguity in a phubbee (McDaniel & Wesselmann, 2021). In such sit­
(d) Conflict about phone use. As noted above, conflict over phone use uations, the phubbee does not know if they are being excluded for
has previously been demonstrated to mediate between partner- something they did (e.g., “I’m boring”), for an external reason (e.g.,
phubbing and relationship satisfaction. McDaniel and Coyne (2016) distracted by a valid emergency) or because the phubber is seeking
McDaniel and Drouin (2019) and McDaniel et al. (2020) measured contact with a “virtual other”. This leaves more room for feelings of
conflicts over a broad range of technology (including TV, computer) and exclusion, potential jealousy that results from (mis)attributions (i.e.,
showed that disagreements among partners about such ‘technoference’ he/she is contacting others), and, arguably, potential conflicts.
was related to worse relationship quality. Roberts and David (2016), and A second reason for the moderating role of shared phone use is that
Halpern and Katz (2017) focused specifically on conflict related to when one is informed and involved by the other in one’s phone activities
phone phubbing and, each using a different conflict scale, showed evi­ the phubber takes an effort to maintain the current interaction, and
dence for mediation. thereby shows that the current interaction partner is still worthy of
(e) Feelings of jealousy. A final potential mediator, which we will attention. In fact, the phone acquires a function and, rather than hin­
focus on only in Study 2, is jealousy. Partner-phubbing may not only dering responsiveness, it becomes part of the interaction. This likely has
make the partner feel excluded or ignored, they may also feel threatened positive effects on perceived partner responsiveness, perceived in­
(Krasnova et al., 2016), particularly when they attribute their partner’s timacy, and affiliation. In line with this reasoning, recent research by
phubbing as an attempt to seek contact with a “virtual other”, as dis­ Cummings and Reimer (2020) provided support for a cellphone rele­
cussed above (Chotpitayasunondh & Douglas, 2018; VanDen Abeele & vance hypothesis. Two vignette studies in which participants imagined
Postma-Nilsenova, 2018). A sense that one loses exclusive attention of conversations, suggested that integrated cellphone use in the conver­
one’s romantic partner, who instead directs attention towards others (e. sation (i.e., to look up relevant information; to enrich the conversation)
g., on social media) has been suggested to be a primary driver of as opposed to incidental use (i.e, referring to an unrelated topic after
romantic jealousy (David & Roberts, 2021; Krasnova et al., 2016; Mill­ looking at the phone) yielded higher reports of anticipated interaction
er-Ott & Kelly, 2015). Research also demonstrated that a partner’s social involvement (i.e., immediacy and other-orientation) and conversational
media activities can induce jealousy (Muise, Christofides, & Dermarais, satisfaction.
2009), particularly among low (vs high) self-esteem individuals (Utz & In the present studies we will focus on phubbing in actual relation­
Beukeboom, 2011). Given that jealousy can, in turn, be expected to be ships and include a measure for the extent to which one’s partner tends
negatively associated with relational satisfaction (Elphinston & Noller, to share information about, and involve the other in, his or her phone
2011), we expect that it can partly explain the negative relationship use. We hypothesize:
effects of partner-phubbing.
H3. The hypothesized negative relationship between partner-
There are some previous studies that tried to find evidence of this
phubbing and relationship satisfaction (H1) is reduced to the extent
mediating effect of jealousy. Krasnova et al. (2016) showed mediation of
that there is more shared phone use.
jealousy in the relationship between partner-phubbing and relational
cohesion. However, their measure of relational cohesion is rather un­ H4. This reduction is due to reduced negative effects on feelings of
conventional and different from relationship satisfaction, and they also exclusion, intimacy, responsiveness, conflict, and jealousy. In other
relied on a very broad definition of jealousy (including sadness, worry, words, shared phone use will moderate the mediation effects.
anger as well as feelings of being excluded and offended), that shows
overlap with feelings of exclusion. Recent MTurk studies by David and 2. Study 1
Roberts (2021) used more established measures and, using different
methods, demonstrate that partner-phubbing has an indirect effect on 2.1. Method
relationship satisfaction via romantic jealously, but only among
anxiously attached individuals. 2.1.1. Participants
Based on the above we hypothesize: For this cross-sectional survey we recruited voluntary participants
with a minimum age of 18 years and a relationship of at least 4 months.
H2. The hypothesized link between partner-phubbing and relationship
They were recruited on online forums and social media, and through
satisfaction (H1) is mediated by: (a) higher feelings of exclusion, (b)
flyers spread on school campuses and door to door. Participants could
lower perceived partner responsiveness, (c) lower perceived intimacy,
win one of two gift vouchers of €25 in a lottery. After excluding
(d) more conflict about phone use (e) higher feelings of jealousy.

1.2. A potential moderator of the partner-phubbing relationship 1


Note that our variable “shared phone use” should be distinguished from the
satisfaction link variable “shared technology use” during leisure time, as measured by McDaniel
et al. (2020), which includes watching movies and TV together, playing games,
Finally, we focus on a factor that could reduce the partner-phubbing video chatting with friends together with the partner.

3
C.J. Beukeboom and M. Pollmann Computers in Human Behavior 124 (2021) 106932

participants who did not fulfill criteria or did not complete the ques­ rated statements on a 5-point scale (1 = never, 5 = always). Example
tionnaire, the final sample consisted of N = 507. With this sample size items include “I have already explicitly told my partner that his or her
we have a power of .80 to detect a small effect (f2 = 0.03) in our most mobile use irritates me”; “My partner and I argue about his or her mobile
elaborate analysis with five predictors (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buch­ use”. (Cronbach’s α = 0.92).
ner, 2007). The majority of the sample was female (n = 397, 78.3%), Shared phone use. To measure whether their partner involved par­
versus male (n = 110, 21.7%). The mean age of participants was M = ticipants in their phone use we developed a scale based upon Leggett and
31.9 (SD = 13.16, range 18–79). Rossouw (2014). Participants rated three statements on a 5-point scale
The average relationship length was M = 108.9 months (9 years), SD (1 = never, 5 = always). Example items “When we’re talking and my
= 119.1 months, with a wide range of 4–420 months (0.3–35 years)). partner picks up his or her cell phone, he or she lets me watch.“; “If my
Relationship types were living apart n = 190 (37.5%), living together n partner picks up his or her mobile while we are talking, he or she in­
= 194 (38.3%), married n = 123 (24.3%) of which Mmarriage length = volves me in his or her mobile use.” (Cronbach’s α = .79).
15.89 years, SDmarriage length = 12.4). A small number of participants (n The complete method, syntax and data, and supplement material can
= 25, 4.9%) was in a same-sex relationship (n = 8 both male, n = 17 both be found on the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/ea6mg/
female).
2.2. Results
2.1.2. Procedure
Participants were informed that the questionnaire focused on Because previous phubbing research reported gender and age dif­
communication within their romantic relationship. They first were ferences we first looked at potential correlations with these variables.
asked about demographics and specifics of their relationship. Next, they Table 1 shows that we do not find many differences for gender, although
answered questions about the following variables in this order: Partner- female participants reported to perceive somewhat higher shared phone
phubbing, shared phone use, conflict over phone use, perceived in­ use in their partner (Mfemale = 3.26; SD = 0.84 versus Mmale = 2.99, SD =
timacy, perceived partner responsiveness, feelings of exclusion, rela­ 0.91, t (505) = 2.85, p = .005) as well as higher perceived partner
tionship satisfaction. responsiveness (Mfemale = 7.31; SD = 1.32 versus Mmale = 6.98; SD =
1.57), t (505) = 2.23, p = .026). Participant age did show a number of
2.1.3. Measures (weak) correlations; participants with older age report lower percep­
Partner-phubbing. To measure the extent in which the partner is tions of partner phubbing as well as lower shared phone use by the
perceived to phub we used the Dutch translation by Boury (2016) of the partner. Relationship satisfaction, intimacy and responsiveness de­
9 item Partner Phubbing Scale (Roberts & David, 2016), with the creases with age, while feelings of exclusion increase with age. To test
addition of one item. Participants rated on a 5-point scale (1 = never, 5 whether these variations played a role we ran the models reported below
= always) the extent to which a number of behaviors occur, when they, with gender and age as control variables (see supplement material); this
in general, think about moments with their partner. Example items are showed that all reported effects remained virtually the same when
“My partner glances at his/her cell phone when talking to me.” “If there controlling for age and gender.
is a lull in our conversation, my partner will check his or her cell phone.” We also noticed quite high correlations between some predictors. To
(Cronbach’s α = 0.88). check for multicollinearity we first ran a regression analysis with all
Relationship satisfaction. To measure participants’ relationship satis­ predictors included. The multicollinearity statistics can be found in
faction, we used the 6-item Quality of Marriage Index (Norton, 1983). Table 2. As expected based on the correlations, perceived partner
Example items are “We have a good relationship”; “My relationship with responsiveness and intimacy show relatively higher VIF values, but the
my partner makes me happy” which were rated on 7-point (1 = not at all values are still acceptable (Menard, 1995).
true, 7 = completely true; Cronbach’s α = 0.93).
Feelings of exclusion. To measure feelings of exclusion we used the 20- 2.2.1. Hypotheses testing
item scale developed by Jamieson, Harkins, and Williams (2010) pre­ Our first hypothesis was that people who perceive more phubbing by
viously used and slightly adapted to the situation by Chotpitayasunondh their partner are less satisfied with their relationship. This simple rela­
and Douglas (2018). Participants rated on a 5-point scale (1 = never, 5 tionship was tested with a correlation analysis with bootstrapped con­
= always) how often they felt 20 feelings, during an average face-to-face fidence intervals. Confirming our first hypothesis, partner-phubbing was
conversation with their partner. Examples are “I feel excluded.“, “I feel negatively related to relationship satisfaction, r = − .29, p < .001, BCa
that I do not exist.” (Cronbach’s α = 0.93). The scale consists of four 95% CI [-0.40; − 0.20]. In Table 1, we present this correlation among all
subscales measuring the four needs related to exclusion: belonging, zero order correlations between our main variables. It also shows that
self-esteem, meaningful existence, and control (Williams, 2001). exclusion, partner responsiveness, intimacy and conflict about phone
Perceived intimacy. To measure participants’ perceived intimacy with use are possible mediators, as they are all related to both the predictor
their partner we adapted the Miller Social Intimacy Scale (Miller & variable partner-phubbing as well as to the outcome variable relation­
Lefcourt, 1982). Participants rated 15 statements on a 9-point scale (1 = ship satisfaction.
not correct; 9 = completely correct). Examples are “When we have lei­ To test hypothesis 2, that the link between partner-phubbing and
sure time, we often choose to spend this time together.”; “I often feel relationship satisfaction is explained by our mediators we conducted a
closeness between my partner and me.” (Cronbach’s α = 0.94). mediation model with PROCESS (Hayes, 2018) 2. In this model partner-
Perceived partner responsiveness. Here we used a 17-item perceived phubbing was the predictor, exclusion, partner responsiveness, in­
Partner Responsiveness Scale (Reis et al., 2017), previously used in timacy, and conflict about phone use were mediators, and relationship
Dutch for instance by Pollmann, Finkenauer, and Begeer (2010). Par­ satisfaction was the outcome variable (Model 4 in Process v3.5). The
ticipants rated the extent to which their partner understands them, overall model was significant, R2 = 0.69, F(5, 501) = 105.98, p < .001.
validates them and gives them the feeling to be cared for. Statements like The coefficients of the links between the variables can be found in Fig. 1,
“My partner understands me”; “My partner sees my real self” and “My in the text, we focus on the indirect effects. Confirming our hypothesis,
partner seems interested in what I am thinking and feeling” were there was a significant total indirect effect, b = − 0.39 [-0.52; − 0.26].
answered on a 9-point scale (1 = not correct; 9 = completely correct; More specifically, the indirect effects of exclusion (b = − 0.10 [-0.17;
Cronbach’s α = 0.96).
Conflict over phone use. To measure the extent in which participants
had conflict with their partner about their phone use we used a 6-item 2
In all of the PROCESS models we use the Huber-White correction method
scale previously developed and used by Boury (2016). Participants for a heteroscedasticity consistent inference.

4
C.J. Beukeboom and M. Pollmann Computers in Human Behavior 124 (2021) 106932

Table 1
Pearson correlations among key variables for Study 1 (N = 507) above the diagonal, and Study 2 (N = 386) below the diagonal.
Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6. 7 8 9 10 11
a a a a a a c
1 Pphubbing .33 -.35 -.33 .57 -.29 -.18 -.10 .01
2 Exclusion .44a .75a -.75a .37a -.72a -.43a .19a .03
3 Intimacy -.32a -.51 a
.88a -.38a .79a .43a -.19a -.08
4 Responsiveness -.31a -.59a .79a -.32a .79a .43a -.19a -.10c
5 Conflict .61a .48a -.39a -.38a -.32a -.24a -.06 .05
6 Relationship satisfaction -.26a -.56a .60a .64a -.32a .38a -.12b -.03
7 Shared ph. use -.27a -.43a .43a .48a -.42a .39a -.29a -.13b
8 Own phubbing .47a .19a -.03 -.06 .13c -.06 -.02
9 Jealousy .11c .25a -.23a -.29a .17a -.23a -.30a .07
10 Age .07 .15b -.15b -.12b .08 -.06 -.14b -.09 -.18b .07
11 Gender .06 -.06 .03 .00 -.03 .09 -.03 -.00 -.03 .14b

Note: superscript a means p < .001, b means p < .01, c means p < .05 (2-tailed); Gender 0 = female, 1 = male.

with prior research, we also ran separate mediation models for all pro­
Table 2
posed mediators. These indirect effects can be found in Table 2. Here, we
Multicollinearity statistics of all variables involved in the analyses, and the in­
can see that conflict over phone use does have a significant indirect
dividual indirect effects of feelings of exclusion, perceived intimacy, perceived
partner responsiveness, conflict over phone use (Study 1 and 2), and feelings of effect when considered separately.
jealousy (only Study 2) between pphubbing and relationship satisfaction. Our third hypothesis was that the link between partner-phubbing
and relationship satisfaction would be moderated by shared phone
Study 1 Tolerance VIF Indirect effect CI
use. Specifically, we expected that particularly when one is not included
Pphubbing .655 1.53 in the partner’s phone use that phubbing has a negative effect on rela­
Exclusion .383 2.61 0.297 0.403–− 0.191
tionship satisfaction, but that this negative effect is weakened when one
− −
Intimacy .207 4.83 − 0.354 − 0.467–− 0.244
Responsiveness .205 4.88 − 0.354 − 0.467–− 0.244 is involved in the partner’s phone use. First, we tested this hypothesis
Conflict .622 1.61 − 0.162 − 0.260–− 0.072 with a PROCESS moderation model in which partner-phubbing was the
Shared use .779 1.28 predictor, shared phone use was the moderator, and relationship satis­
Study 2
faction was the outcome variable (Model 1 in Process v3.5). The overall
Pphubbing .608 1.64
Exclusion .553 1.81 − 0.186 − 0.242–− 0.132
model was significant, R2 = 0.23, F(3, 503) = 29.33, p < .001. There was
Intimacy .361 2.77 − 0.142 − 0.213–− 0.081 a significant interaction effect between partner-phubbing and shared
Responsiveness .317 3.16 − 0.147 − 0.213–− 0.093 phone use on relationship satisfaction, b = 0.22, t(503) = 3.39, p = .001,
Conflict .540 1.85 − 0.120 − 0.192–− 0.055 [0.09; 0.35]. The pattern was in line with our hypothesis. At low levels
Jealousy .850 1.18 0.017 0.037–− 0.001
of shared phone use there is a significant negative effect, b = − 0.42, t
− −
Shared use .656 1.52
(503) = 5.20, p < .001, [-0.58; − 0.26], but at high levels of shared
Note. Tolerance values below 0.10 and VIF values higher than 10 are commonly phone use there is not, b = − 0.05, t(503) = 0.77, p = .44, [-0.19; 0.08].
seen as indications of multicollinearity problems (e.g., Menard, 1995). Finally, for hypothesis 4, we tested whether shared phone use
moderated the above reported mediation effects (Model 7 in Process
v3.5). In this analysis separate models are tested in which shared phone
use is added as a moderator of the link between partner-phubbing and
respectively feelings of exclusion, perceived responsiveness, perceived
intimacy, and conflict over phone use. We found that shared phone use is
a significant moderator of the link between partner-phubbing and feel­
ings of exclusion, b = − 0.13, t(503) = 3.22, p = .001, [-0.21; − 0.05],
perceived partner responsiveness, b = 0.30, t(503) = 3.00, p = .003,
[0.10; 0.45], and perceived intimacy, b = 0.28, t(503) = 3.05, p = .002,
[0.10; 0.45], but not conflict over phone use, b = − 0.07, t(503) = 1.32,
p = .18, [-0.17; 0.03]. The three significant interaction effects all showed
the same pattern (see Fig. 3). At high levels of shared phone use, more
partner-phubbing is linked to a lesser extent to feelings of exclusion,
Fig. 1. Conceptual model of the proposed hypotheses with results from Study perceived partner responsiveness, and perceived intimacy, than at low
1. Hypothesis 1 is depicted in blue, Hypothesis 2 is depicted in red, Hypothesis levels of shared phone use.
3 is depicted in green, Hypothesis 4 is depicted in orange. Solid lines represent a
significant effect, dashed lines represent a non-significant effect. The numbers
are the coefficients from the mediation model. 2.3. Discussion

− 0.04]), partner responsiveness (b = − 0.13 [-0.23; − 0.06]), and in­ Study 1 confirmed most hypotheses. In line with previous work
timacy (b = − 0.15 [-0.24; − 0.08]) were significant, the indirect effect of (Halpern & Katz, 2017; Krasnova et al., 2016; McDaniel & Coyne, 2016;
conflict over phone use (b = 0.0002 [-0.07; 0.07]) was not. This means McDaniel & Drouin, 2019; Roberts & David, 2016; Wang et al., 2017)
that more partner-phubbing is linked to more feelings of exclusion, less our results provide convincing evidence for the negative relationship
perceived partner responsiveness, and less perceived intimacy by one’s between partner-phubbing and relationship satisfaction (H1). Second,
partner, which in turn is linked to lower relationship satisfaction. With we observed that this effect was explained by three significant media­
the mediators in the model the direct effect between partner-phubbing tors; more feelings of exclusion, less perceived partner responsiveness,
and relationship satisfaction is small and not significant (b = .01, t and less perceived intimacy (H2abc). In contrast to H2d, we did not find
(501) = 0.11, p = .91, [-0.09; 0.10]) indicating that the mediators evidence for mediation of conflict over phone use.
explain a large portion of the effect. Second, we tested a factor that we expected could reduce the partner-
In order to be able to compare the individual effects of the mediators phubbing relationship satisfaction link, namely the extent in which

5
C.J. Beukeboom and M. Pollmann Computers in Human Behavior 124 (2021) 106932

phone use is being shared and one is involved in the phone use activities (M = 2.73, SD = 0.66, Cronbach’s α = 0.84), Conflict over Phone use (M
of one’s partner. In line with H3 we observed that shared phone use was = 1.86, SD = 0.79, Cronbach’s α = 0.91), Feelings of exclusion (M = 1.77,
a significant moderator; at the higher levels of shared phone use the SD = 0.47, Cronbach’s α = 0.92), Perceived partner responsiveness (M =
negative relationship between partner-phubbing and relationship 7.65, SD = 1.11, Cronbach’s α = 0.95), and Perceived intimacy (M = 7.49,
satisfaction disappears. Moreover, in line with H4 we observed that SD = 0.91, Cronbach’s α = 0.84).
shared phone use moderates the above three significant mediation ef­ Relationship satisfaction. In study 2 we used the 7-item Relationship
fect, but again this was not the case for conflict over phone use. The Assessment Scale (RAS; Hendrick, 1988) to measure participants’ rela­
more one is involved in one’s partner’s phone use, the less one experi­ tionship satisfaction. Example items are “How satisfied are you in gen­
ences feelings of exclusion, and the less one experiences reduced partner eral with your relationship?“; “How much do you love your partner?”
responsiveness and intimacy, which in turn reduces negative effects on which were rated on different 5-point scales (e.g., 1 = unsatisfied, 5 =
relationship satisfaction. This suggests that involving and informing the very satisfied; 1 = a little, 5 = a lot; M = 4.46, SD = 0.52, Cronbach’s α
partner about one’s phone activities during interactions could provide a = 83).
potential remedy against the detrimental effects of (nearly inevitable) Shared phone use. In Study 2 we used a different, newly developed
phone use in relationships. scale to measure whether their partner involved participants in their
phone use. It referred to slightly more concrete behaviors than the scale
3. Study 2 used in Study 1. It included four statements following “When my partner
uses the mobile phone in my presence … then I know what he or she is
In order to test the robustness of these findings, we conducted a doing; … then I get to watch; … then my partner shares with me what he
second study to both replicate and extend the results of Study 1. In Study or she sees; … then I am involved in his or her telephone use. Items were
2 we used the same independent and mediator variables, but added rated on a 5-point scale (1 = never, 5 = always; M = 3.67, SD = 0.59,
feelings of jealousy as an additional mediator (David & Roberts, 2021; Cronbach’s α = 0.74).
Krasnova et al., 2016). We measured the dependent variable, relation­ Feelings of jealousy. We added a measure of feelings of jealousy as a
ship satisfaction, using a different scale (i.e., the Relationship Assess­ potential mediator. Just as David and Roberts (2021) we used a scale
ment Scale, Hendrick, 1988). By using a different measure of based on the cognitive jealousy subscale of the Multidimensional Jeal­
relationship satisfaction, we can test whether the effects generalize ousy Scale by Pfeiffer & Wong (1989), which was adapted to relate it to a
across measures. Moreover, we extended the moderator variable shared phubbing situation. Participants rated 6 statement following “When my
phone use and added participants’ own phubbing behavior as a potential partner is busy with his or her phone …” Example statements are “… I
control variable as it relates to the phone use norm in the relationship. suspect that my partner is intimate with someone behind my back”, “…
then I feel jealous”. We did not include Item 5 in the analysis because it
3.1. Method had too much overlap with exclusion (”.. then I feel rejected”). Items
were rated on a 5-point scale (1 = never, 5 = always; M = 1.55, SD =
3.1.1. Participants 0.70, Cronbach’s α = 0.87).
For this cross-sectional survey we recruited voluntary participants Own Pphubbing. We also measured to what extend the participant
with a minimum age of 18 years and a relationship of at least 3 months. him/herself showed partner-phubbing, to be able to include this as a
They were recruited mainly via social media using snowball sampling. control variable. We used the 9-item partner-phubbing scale, but
Participants could win one of two gift vouchers of €25,- in a lottery. After adapted the items such that they referred to the respondent. Example
excluding participants who did not fulfill criteria or did not complete the items are “I glance at my cell phone when talking to my partner”, “If
questionnaire, the final sample consisted of N = 386. With this sample there is a lull in our conversation, I will check my cell phone.” Items
size we have a power of .80 to detect a small to medium effect (f2 = 0.04) were rated on a 5-point scale (1 = never, 5 = always; M = 2.57, SD =
and in our most elaborate analysis with six predictors (Faul et al., 2007). 0.58, Cronbach’s α = 0.80).
The majority of the sample was female (n = 275, 71.2%), versus male (n See the OSF for the complete methodology, syntax and data, and
= 111, 28.8%). The mean age of participants was M = 27.9 (SD = 11.17, additional exploratory analyses: https://osf.io/ea6mg/
range 18–72).
The average relationship length was M = 82.1 months (7 years), SD 3.2. Results
= 106.7 months, with a wide range of 3–564 months (0.3–47 years).
Relationship types were living apart n = 189 (49.0%), living together n Again, we first looked at potential correlations with age and gender.
= 145 (37.6%), married n = 52 (13.5%). A small number of participants Table 1 shows that in Study 2 we do not find any differences for gender
(n = 10, 3.6%) was in a same-sex relationship (n = 7 both male, n = 3 on the key variables. In contrast to study 1, age was not related with
both female). In this study, we also asked how much time participants perceptions of partner phubbing nor with relationship satisfaction, but
and their partner spend on their mobile phone. This was answered on an did again show a weak negative correlation with shared phone use by
11-point scale ranging from 1 – less than 15 min per day to 11 – more the partner, and again intimacy and responsiveness decreases somewhat
than 8 h per day. The median response for both the participant and the with age, while feelings of exclusion increase. We again ran the models
partner was 5, which corresponds to 2–3 h per day. reported below with gender and age as control variables (see supple­
ment material), showing that all reported effects remained virtually the
3.1.2. Procedure same when controlling for age and gender.
Participants were informed that the questionnaire focused on phone Correlations among the predictors and relationship satisfaction are
use within romantic relationship. They first were asked about de­ comparable to Study 1 (see Table 2); again the tolerance and VIF values
mographics and specifics of their relationship. Next, they answered are acceptable and do not show indications of serious multicolinearity
questions about the following variables in this order: relationship (Menard, 1995).
satisfaction, phone use time (partner and own), partner-phubbing, own
pphubbing, shared phone use, conflict over phone use, feelings of 3.2.1. Hypotheses testing
exclusion, perceived partner responsiveness, perceived intimacy, feel­ We again tested hypothesis 1 (people who perceive more phubbing
ings of jealousy. by their partner are less satisfied with their relationship) with a corre­
lation analysis with bootstrapped confidence intervals. Even though we
3.1.3. Measures use a different scale to measure relationship satisfaction, our first hy­
We used the same scales as in Study 1 to measure Partner-phubbing pothesis was again supported: partner-phubbing was negatively related

6
C.J. Beukeboom and M. Pollmann Computers in Human Behavior 124 (2021) 106932

to relationship satisfaction, r = − 26, p < .001, BCa 95% CI [-0.37; = .001, 95% CI [-0.24; − 0.06] and shared phone use had a positive link
− 0.13]. This correlation as well as all other zero order correlations can with relationship satisfaction, b = 0.27, t(382) = 4.95, p < .001, 95% CI
again be found in Table 1. Exclusion, intimacy, partner responsiveness, [0.16; 0.37]. In contrast to H3 and Study 1, there was no interaction
and conflict over phone use are again possible mediators, as they are all effect between partner-phubbing and shared phone use on relationship
related to both the predictor variable partner-phubbing as well as to the satisfaction, b = 0.06, t(382) = 0.75, p = .46, [-0.11; 0.23]. This in­
outcome variable relationship satisfaction. This also holds for the new dicates that in Study 2 shared phone use does not directly buffer the
possible mediator; feelings of jealousy. effect of partner-phubbing on relationship satisfaction. However, it can
The extent to which the participant admitted to phubbing behavior still be the case that shared phone use buffers against changes in the
towards the partner (i.e., own pphubbing) was related to how much they mediators (H4). We therefore also tested for moderated mediation
perceive phubbing by the partner. It was, however, not related to rela­ effects.
tionship satisfaction and only weakly related to our mediators. We To test hypothesis 4, that shared phone use moderated the above
therefore present our analyses without this control variable here and reported mediation effects, we again ran moderated mediation models
provide the analyses with control variable in the supplement material. for each mediator in which shared phone use is entered as a moderator
To test the second hypothesis, we conducted a similar mediation of the link between partner-phubbing and respectively feelings of
model as in Study 1, with partner-phubbing as predictor, exclusion, exclusion, perceived partner responsiveness, perceived intimacy, con­
partner responsiveness, intimacy, conflict about phone use, and jealousy flict over phone use and jealousy. We find that shared phone use is a
as mediators, and relationship satisfaction as outcome variable. The significant moderator in the link between partner-phubbing and feelings
model was significant, R2 = 0.47, F(6, 378) = 27.50, p < .001. The co­ of exclusion, b = − 0.13, t(381) = 2.49, p = .01, [-0.24; − 0.03],
efficients of the links between the variables can be found in Fig. 2. In this perceived partner responsiveness, b = 0.41, t(381) = 2.58, p = .01,
analysis, we find the same total indirect effect as in Study 1 (b = − 0.22, [0.10; 0.73], perceived intimacy, b = 0.29, t(381) = 2.03, p = .04, [0.09;
95%CI [-0.31; − 0.13], so the mediators together explain the link be­ 0.58], and conflict over phone use, b = − 0.19, t(381) = 2.41, p = .02,
tween partner-phubbing and relationship satisfaction. The indirect ef­ [-0.34; − 0.04], but not for the link between partner-phubbing and
fects of exclusion (b = − .09, 95%CI [-0.15; − 0.02]), perceived partner jealousy b = 0.07, t(381) = 0.85, p = .40, [-0.09; 0.23]. The significant
responsiveness (b = − 0.07, 95%CI [-0.13; − 0.03]), and perceived in­ interaction effects all showed the same pattern, as displayed in Fig. 4. At
timacy (b = − 0.06, 95%CI [-0.11; − 0.02]) were significant; the indirect high levels of shared phone use, more partner-phubbing is linked to a
effects of conflict over phone use (b = 0.004, 95%CI [-0.05; 0.06]) and lesser extent to feelings of exclusion, perceived partner responsiveness,
jealousy (b = − 0.002, 95%CI [-0.01; 0.01]) were not. This means that perceived intimacy, and conflict over phone use than at low levels of
we again find, in line with H2abc, that more partner-phubbing is linked shared phone use. This again indicates that involving the partner in
to more feelings of exclusion between partners, less perceived partner one’s phone use acts as a buffer against detrimental relationship effects.
responsiveness, and less perceived intimacy, which in turn is linked to We ran also ran all models reported above with participants’ own
lower relationship satisfaction. Inconsistent with H2de we observe no phubbing behavior as control variable. The outcomes of these analyses
mediation of conflict over phone use and feelings of jealousy when all can be found in the online appendix. The pattern of results did not
mediators are included in the model. With the mediators in the model change when own phubbing was included, so the effects are robust
the direct effect between partner-phubbing and relationship satisfaction against the addition of this control variable.
is small and not significant (b = 0.02, t(378) = 0.39, p = .69, [-0.06; Finally, we explored the idea that a higher discrepancy between
0.10]) indicating that the mediators again explain a large portion of the partners in partner-phubbing behavior would increase negative effects
effect. of partner-phubbing. That is, particularly when one’s partner shows
Again, we also tested the individual indirect effects of all mediators high levels of partner-phubbing, while the respondent shows low levels
by running separate mediation models. As can be seen in Table 2, con­ of partner-phubbing, we expected that negative relationship effects are
flict over phone use and jealousy have significant indirect effects when higher. When partners show more similar phubbing behavior during
considered separately. interactions, we expected less negative effects, because there apparently
To test the third hypothesis, that the link between partner-phubbing is a shared norm about phone use during interactions. To test this, we
and relationship satisfaction is moderated by shared phone use, we ran a moderation analysis in which the respondents’ own partner-
again first ran a PROCESS moderation model in which partner-phubbing phubbing moderates the effect of the partner’s phubbing on relation­
was the predictor, shared phone use was the moderator, and relationship ship satisfaction. The predicted interaction effect was not significant, b
satisfaction was the outcome variable. The overall model was signifi­ = .04, t(382) = 0.49, p = .63, CI [-0.13, 0.21]. So, there is no indication
cant, R2 = 0.16, F(3, 382) = 18.89, p < .001. Partner-phubbing had a that the effect of being phubbed by one’s partner is dependent on how
negative link with relationship satisfaction, b = − 0.15, t(382) = 3.22, p much partner-phubbing one engages in oneself.

4. General discussion

In two cross-sectional surveys we aimed to find further evidence of


detrimental effects of partner-phubbing in intimate relationships. Con­
firming H1 and in line with previous studies (Halpern & Katz, 2017;
Krasnova et al., 2016; McDaniel & Coyne, 2016; McDaniel & Drouin,
2019; McDaniel et al., 2020; Roberts & David, 2016; Wang et al., 2017)
we observed that the extent in which a partner uses their phone during
co-present interactions is negatively related to relationship satisfaction.
The fact that we used different scales to measure relationship satisfac­
tion in the two studies, but the findings are very consistent speaks for
their robustness, and shows that they generalize across measures. It
Fig. 2. Conceptual model of the proposed hypotheses with results from Study should be noted that we cannot draw conclusions about the causal di­
2. Hypothesis 1 is depicted in blue, Hypothesis 2 is depicted in red, Hypothesis rection of the effect as this is based on correlation data;
3 is depicted in green, Hypothesis 4 is depicted in orange. Solid lines represent a partner-phubbing could cause reduced relationship satisfaction, but the
significant effect, dashed lines represent a non-significant effect. The numbers opposite effect is equally possible. We think it could best be seen as a
are the coefficients from the mediation model. reciprocal relationship, whereby partner-phubbing results in reduced

7
C.J. Beukeboom and M. Pollmann Computers in Human Behavior 124 (2021) 106932

Fig. 3. Visualization of the moderating effect of shared phone use on the link between partner phubbing and exclusion, perceived partner responsiveness, and
perceived intimacy from Study 1.

relationship satisfaction, which in turn could induce more pphubbing 1998).


during interactions. As such, pphubbing could play an adverse role in a Using mediation models with all five mediators included, we
downward spiral of communication quality and relationship observed in both studies that the link between partner-phubbing and
satisfaction. relationship satisfaction was fully mediated by feelings of exclusion
between partners, less perceived partner responsiveness and less
perceived intimacy, in line with H2abc. Interestingly, in contrast to
4.1. Explanatory factors for the link between partner-phubbing and H2de and previous studies, we observed no significant mediation effects
relationship satisfaction of conflict over phone use (Halpern & Katz, 2017; McDaniel & Coyne,
2016; McDaniel & Drouin, 2019; McDaniel et al., 2020; Roberts &
A second main goal of our studies was to find potential mediators David, 2016) or jealousy (David & Roberts, 2021; Krasnova et al., 2016)
that could explain the underlying mechanism of this link, as a better when the other three mediators were included in the model. The
understanding of these processes may inform interventions to address zero-order correlations (Table 1) did show that pphubbing was posi­
the detrimental effects of phubbing. Only a few previous studies focused tively related to both conflict over phone use and jealousy, which were
on explanatory variables in actual relationships, and these focused also both negatively related to relationship satisfaction. Moreover, the
merely on the mediating role of conflict about phone use (Halpern & separate mediation models (Table 2) showed that conflict over phone
Katz, 2017; McDaniel & Coyne, 2016; McDaniel & Drouin, 2019; use and jealousy have significant indirect effects if considered individ­
McDaniel et al., 2020; Roberts & David, 2016) or jealousy (David & ually. This is in line with previous work. Yet, our integrative mediation
Roberts, 2021; Krasnova et al., 2016). We took a broader and integrative findings show that when feelings of exclusion, perceived partner
approach by measuring five potential mediators, with three additional responsiveness, and perceived intimacy are taken into account, the ef­
variables that were derived from laboratory or vignette studies on fects of conflict and jealousy disappear. Thus our data suggests that
phubbing (feelings of exclusion, e.g., Chotpitayasunondh & Douglas, conflict and jealousy in itself are not the primary mechanism through
2018; Gonzales & Wu, 2016), and from the relationship literature which pphubbing results in reduced relationship satisfaction.
(perceived partner responsiveness, and intimacy; Laurenceau et al.,

8
C.J. Beukeboom and M. Pollmann Computers in Human Behavior 124 (2021) 106932

Fig. 4. Visualization of the moderating effect of shared phone use on the link between partner phubbing and exclusion, perceived partner responsiveness, perceived
intimacy, and conflict over phone use from Study 2.

Instead, detrimental relationship effects of partner-phubbing appear which we included these subscales (see online Appendix B) we were not
to be mainly explained by the extent to which one feels excluded or able to replicate this finding. In these analyses none of the subscales
ignored by one’s partner during social interactions, and the extent to emerged as the sole or main explaining factor. Based on this, we
which pphubbing results in reduced partner responsiveness and feelings conclude that, when testing for the effect of exclusion in intimate re­
of intimacy during interactions. These findings are in line with research lationships it is better to include all aspects of exclusion.
showing negative effects of phone use during interactions on conver­
sation quality and perceived partner empathy (Gonzales & Wu, 2016;
Misra et al., 2016; Przybylski & Weinstein, 2013). The present studies 4.2. Shared phone use as a moderator of the partner-phubbing satisfaction
demonstrate that these findings extent to intimate relationships in which link
one can frequently, in consecutive interactions, experience
partner-phubbing. Our third main goal was to study the moderating role of shared
It should be noted that recent longitudinal daily survey studies phone use, as we expected this could provide a potential remedy against
(McDaniel & Drouin, 2019; McDaniel et al., 2020) did show mediation the detrimental effects of nowadays nearly inevitable phone use in re­
effects of conflict between the broader concept of technoference and lationships. In line with H3 we observed a significant interaction be­
relationship quality. Our findings do not contradict the adverse role that tween pphubbing and shared phone use on relationship satisfaction in
conflict (about phone or technology use) may have in relationships. Yet, Study 1, but not in Study 2. However, in both studies moderated
our (albeit crossectional, between-person) data suggests that even when mediation tests showed that shared phone use moderates the effects of
couples do not have any conflicts about partner-phubbing, nor experi­ pphubbing on the three significant mediators discussed above. This
ence jealousy, their relationship satisfaction may be negatively affected shows that the more one is informed and involved by one’s partner in
as a result of feelings of exclusion, and reduced partner responsiveness their phone use (i.e., by showing and explaining their phone activities),
and intimacy in interactions in which their partner shows phubbing the less one feels excluded, and the less one experiences reduced partner
behavior. After all, not everyone will confront their partner and engage responsiveness and intimacy. This in turn mitigates the negative effects
in conflict about phone use, yet one may still experience its negative on relationship satisfaction.
consequences. Future work might be able to study the combined role of Shared phone use was, only in Study 2 also observed to reduce
the various mediators we measured in a longitudinal design. conflicts over phone use, but shared phone use did not reduce feelings of
With respect to social exclusion, Chotpitayasunondh and Douglas jealousy (only measured in Study 2). Still, the zero order correlations
(2018) observed that, of the four needs that are affected by social (Table 1) show that shared phone use is highly related to all mediators in
exclusion (Williams, 2001), only the sense of belonging mediated the both studies, and show strong negative correlations with conflict and
effect of phubbing on perceived communication quality and relationship jealousy, which suggests that shared phone use is related to both conflict
satisfaction with an animated conversation partner. Self-esteem, and feelings of jealousy. Previous work suggests that jealousy mainly
meaningful existence, and control did not. In additional analyses in plays a role among individuals with low self-esteem (Utz & Beukeboom,
2015) and with an anxious attachment style (David & Roberts, 2021). It

9
C.J. Beukeboom and M. Pollmann Computers in Human Behavior 124 (2021) 106932

could be that merely among these individuals pphubbing has detri­ Kelly, 2015), then negative effects on relationship satisfaction are likely
mental effects via jealousy. It also seems likely that among those who limited.
experience higher levels of jealousy, shared phone use can reduce such
feelings. That is, feelings of jealousy usually emerge when one experi­ 5. Conclusion
ences a loss of exclusive attention and one (erroneously) attributes a
partner’s phubbing as attempts to seek contact with others (David & To conclude, for many people it is nowadays hard to withstand the
Roberts, 2021; Krasnova et al., 2016; Miller-Ott & Kelly, 2015). A temptation of social media and other attention seeking apps on one’s
partner can probably take away such erroneous attributions by sharing smart phone (Du, Kerkhof, & van Koningsbruggen, 2019). A fear of
what one is doing with their phone instead. missing out (Rozgonjuk, Sindermann, Elhai, & Montag, 2020), an
In sum, our findings suggest that partners can prevent detrimental expectation of constant availability from friends (Miller-Ott & Kelly,
effects for their relationships by refraining from the use of one’s mobile 2017), or even the mere presence of a phone (Misra et al., 2016; Przy­
phone during co-present interaction. However, if one must use a phone, bylski & Weinstein, 2013) can draw attention away from an interaction
detrimental effects can be reduced by keeping the partner involved and one is having with a co-present partner. The present studies highlight
informed about what one is doing with it. If this is done well a phone that the use of one’s phone in the presence of a partner (pphubbing), has
actually has a function and becomes part of the interaction (e.g., look up an, often unintentional, detrimental relationship effect, as a partner feels
relevant information, enrich the conversation; Cummings & Reimer, excluded and it harms the experienced responsiveness and intimacy in
2020), rather than being a hindrance to responsiveness and conversation the interactions one is in. To prevent such detrimental effects it is
quality. important to become more aware of the consequences of phone use.
Particularly in contexts in which a co-present partner expects your un­
4.3. Strength and limitations divided attention, it is advisable to put your phone away. If one must use
a phone in co-present interactions, our results provide an easy remedy;
As all studies, our studies have their design-specific strength and shared phone use. By involving and informing a partner about one’s
limitations. One strength is that our sample size gave us good power to phone activities, and by making one’s phone use relevant and functional
detect medium to small effects. The majority of our participants were in the interaction, it is possible to reduce feelings of exclusion and
female, but we observed no remarkable correlations for gender as well as maintain more responsiveness and intimacy in the conversation.
age, and including these variables as covariate did not influence our
main results. A second strength is that we took a broad focus on re­ Credit author statement
lationships. That is, we measured relationship satisfaction, and the
various communication behaviors in the relationship, as more or less Beukeboom: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Valida­
stable trait-like perceptions of the current relationship, thus going tion, Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources, Data Curation, Writing -
beyond single interactions. Moreover, we did not focus on one possible Original Draft, Writing - Review & Editing, Supervision, Project
mediator, but painted a more complete picture by including several administration, Pollmann: Validation, Formal analysis, Resources, Data
different mediators. That way, we could test for unique effects and Curation, Visualization, Writing - Original Draft, Writing - Review &
determine which relationship process is most affected by partner Editing.
phubbing. We think this brings an important contribution to the litera­
ture, as such a broad approach (including multiple mediators) has not Acknowledgements
been done before.
A first limitation is that we only included one of the partners, We would like to thank our students Aaron Springer, Djoy Boer,
whereas an intimate relationship is a dyadic process and including both Roxana Joordens, Jasmijn Kruijt, Felicia Ramgrab and Jeanne Wiersma
partners can yield unique insights. A second limitation is that we only for their help in setting up the studies and collecting the data.
measured these processes at one time point. Future longitudinal studies
could include several measurements, and both partners, to gain under­ Appendix A. Supplementary data
standing on how these processes develop over time. In the above
mentioned study by McDaniel et al. (2020), for example, both partners Supplementary analyses to this article can be found online at htt
of 145 couples completed daily surveys measuring technology use and ps://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106932 and data, syntax and com­
relationship satisfaction in ten consecutive days. This design allowed for plete methodology are available on the Open Science Framework: htt
analyses of both between and within person variations in a multilevel ps://osf.io/ea6mg/.
model, and showed that daily fluctuations in respondents’ own- and
their partner’s technology use during leisure was predictive of variations References
in relationship satisfaction (McDaniel et al., 2020). A similar set-up
could be used to study the effects of variations in the variables we Boury, K. (2016). Euhm hallo, Ik zit recht voor je, die gsm is echt niet zo belangrijk: Een
kwantitatieve studie naar het verband tussen Partner Phubbing en relatietevredenheid
included over an extended period of time. (Masterthesis University Gent). Retrieved from https://lib.ugent.be/nl/catalog/
Finally, our method focused generically on partner phubbing, and rug01:002275408.
did not specify situations in which this occurs. However, effects of phone Chotpitayasunondh, V., & Douglas, K. M. (2016). How “phubbing” becomes the norm:
The antecedents and consequences of snubbing via smartphone. Computers in Human
use likely depend on context, as different levels of partner attentiveness Behavior, 63, 9–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.018
are expected across different contexts (Miller-Ott & Kelly, 2015). Chotpitayasunondh, V., & Douglas, K. M. (2018). The effects of “phubbing” on social
Following Expectancy Violations theory (Burgoon, 1993), it has been interaction. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 48(6), 304–316. https://doi.org/
10.1111/jasp.12506
argued that particularly in situations where one expects exclusive and Cizmeci Ümit, E. (2017). Disconnected, though satisfied: Pphubbing behavior and
undivided attention (e.g., a romantic date or with serious conversation relationship satisfaction. The Turkish Online Journal of Design, Art and Communication,
topics), phubbing is considered disrespectful and a hurtful violation of 7. https://doi.org/10.7456/10702100/018
Cummings, R., & Reimer, T. (2020). Cellphone relevance in face-to-face interactions: The
these expectations (Miller-Ott & Kelly, 2015, 2017; Vanden Abeele,
effects of cellphone use on conversational satisfaction: Mobile Media & Communication.
2020). It is likely that particularly when pphubbing frequently occurs in https://doi.org/10.1177/2050157920958437
such situations, it has most detrimental relationship effects. If a partner David, M., & Roberts, J. A. (2017). Phubbed and alone: Phone snubbing, social exclusion,
is sensitive to these situational expectations, however, by refraining and attachment to social media. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 2,
155–163. https://doi.org/10.1086/690940
from smartphone use when undivided attention is expected, and limits David, M. E., & Roberts, J. A. (2021). Investigating the impact of partner phubbing on
phone use to ‘informal’ contexts (e.g., “just hanging out”; Miller-Ott & romantic jealousy and relationship satisfaction: The moderating role of attachment

10
C.J. Beukeboom and M. Pollmann Computers in Human Behavior 124 (2021) 106932

anxiety. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. , Article 026540752199645. Misra, S., Cheng, L., Genevie, J., & Yuan, M. (2016). The iPhone effect: The quality of in-
https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407521996454 person social interactions in the presence of mobile devices. Environment and
Du, J., Kerkhof, P., & van Koningsbruggen, G. M. (2019). Predictors of social media self- Behavior, 48(2), 275–298. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916514539755
control failure: Immediate gratifications, habitual checking, ubiquity, and Muise, A., Christofides, E., & Dermarais, S. (2009). More information than you ever
notifications. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 22(7), 477–485. wanted: Does facebook bring out the green-eyed monster of jealousy?
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2018.0730 CyberPsychology and Behavior, 12, 441–444. https://doi: 10.1089=cpb.2008.0263.
Dwyer, R. J., Kushlev, K., & Dunn, E. W. (2018). Smartphone use undermines enjoyment Pollmann, M. M. H., Finkenauer, C., & Begeer, S. (2010). Mediators of the link between
of face-to-face social interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 78, autistic traits and relationship satisfaction in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Autism
233–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.10.007 and Developmental Disorders, 40(4), 470–478. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-
Elphinston, R., & Noller, P. (2011). Time to face it! Facebook intrusion and the 0888-z
implications for romantic jealousy and relationship satisfaction. Cyberpsychology, Pollmann, M. M., Norman, T. J., & Crockett, E. E. (2021). A daily-diary study on the
Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 631–635. https://doi.org/10.1089/ effects of face-to-face communication, texting, and their interplay on understanding
cyber.2010.0318 and relationship satisfaction. Computers in Human Behavior Reports, 3, 100088.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chbr.2021.100088
power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Przybylski, A. K., & Weinstein, N. (2013). Can you connect with me now? How the
Research Methods, 39, 175–191. presence of mobile communication technology influences face-to-face conversation
Gergen, K. J. (2002). The challenge of absent presence. In J. E. Katz, & M. Aakhus (Eds.), quality. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 30(3), 237–246. https://doi.org/
Perpetual contact: Mobile communication, private talk, public performance (pp. 10.1177/0265407512453827
227–241). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/ Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., & Holmes, J. G. (2004). Perceived partner responsiveness as an
CBO9780511489471.018. organizing construct in the study of intimacy and closeness. In D. J. Mashek, &
Gonzales, A. L., & Wu, Y. (2016). Public cellphone use does not activate negative A. P. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 201–225). Lawrence
responses in Others…Unless they hate cellphones. Journal of Computer-Mediated Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Communication, 21(5), 384–398. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12174 Reis, H. T., Crasta, D., Rogge, R. D., Maniaci, M. R., & Carmichael, C. L. (2017). Perceived
Guldner, G. T., & Swensen, C. H. (1995). Time spent together and relationship quality: partner responsiveness scale (PPRS). The sourcebook of listening research (pp.
Long-distance relationships as a test case. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 516–521). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119102991.ch57
12(2), 313–320. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407595122010 Reis, H. T., & Shaver, P. (1988). Intimacy as an interpersonal process. In S. Duck,
Hales, A. H., Dvir, M., Wesselmann, E. D., Kruger, D. J., & Finkenauer, C. (2018). Cell D. F. Hay, S. E. Hobfoll, W. Ickes, & B. M. Montgomery (Eds.), Handbook of personal
phone-induced ostracism threatens fundamental needs. The Journal of Social relationships: Theory, research and interventions (pp. 367–389). John Wiley & Sons.
Psychology, 158(4), 460–473. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2018.1439877 Roberts, J. A., & David, M. E. (2016). My life has become a major distraction from my cell
Halpern, D., & Katz, J. E. (2017). Texting’s consequences for romantic relationships: A phone: Partner phubbing and relationship satisfaction among romantic partners.
cross-lagged analysis highlights its risks. Computers in Human Behavior, 9. Computers in Human Behavior, 54, 134–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Hendrick, S. S. (1988). A generic measure of relationship satisfaction. Journal of Marriage chb.2015.07.058
and Family, 50, 93–98. Rozgonjuk, D., Sindermann, C., Elhai, J. D., & Montag, C. (2020). Fear of missing out
Jamieson, J. P., Harkins, S. G., & Williams, K. D. (2010). Need threat can motivate (FoMO) and social media’s impact on daily-life and productivity at work: Do
performance after ostracism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(5), WhatsApp, facebook, instagram, and snapchat use disorders mediate that
690–702. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167209358882 association? Addictive Behaviors, 110, 106487. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Krasnova, H., Abramova, O., Notter, I., & Baumann, A. (2016). Why phubbing is toxic for addbeh.2020.106487
your relationship: Understanding the role of smartphone jealousy among ‘Generation Servies, A. (2012). Cell phones and couple communication: The impact of mobile device
Y’ users [Conference session]. June 12–15. In Twenty fourth European conference on distractions during a dyadic interaction. USA: Psychological Science Undergraduate
information systems (ECIS), istanbul, Turkey. Honors Theses, University of Arkansas. http://scholarworks.uark.edu/psycuht/1.
Laurenceau, J.-P., Barrett, L., & Pietromonaco, P. (1998). Intimacy as an interpersonal Turkle, S. (2011). Alone together: Why we expect more from technology and less from
process: The importance of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and perceived partner ourselves. New York: Basic Books.
responsiveness in interpersonal exchanges. Journal of Personality and Social Vanden Abeele, M. M. P. (2020). The social consequences of phubbing: A framework and
Psychology, 74, 1238–1251. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.74.5.1238 a research agenda. In R. Ling, L. Fortunati, G. Goggin, S. S. Lim, & Y. Li (Eds.), The
Leggett, C., & Rossouw, P. J. (2014). The impact of technology use on couple oxford handbook of mobile communication and society (pp. 157–174). Oxford
relationships: A neuropsychological perspective. International Journal of University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190864385.013.11.
Neuropsychotherapy, 2, 44–99. https://doi.org/10.127441/ijnpt.2014.0044-0099 Vanden Abeele, M. M. P., Antheunis, M. L., & Schouten, A. P. (2016). The effect of mobile
Lemay, E. P., Jr., Clark, M. S., & Feeney, B. C. (2007). Projection of responsiveness to messaging during a conversation on impression formation and interaction quality.
needs and the construction of satisfying communal relationships. Journal of Computers in Human Behavior, 62, 562–569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Personality and Social Psychology, 92(5), 834–853. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022- chb.2016.04.005
3514.92.5.834 Vanden Abeele, M. M. P., Hendrickson, A. T., Pollmann, M. M. H., & Ling, R. (2019).
McDaniel, B. T., & Coyne, S. M. (2016). “Technoference”: The interference of technology Phubbing behavior in conversations and its relation to perceived conversation
in couple relationships and implications for women’s personal and relational well- intimacy and distraction: An exploratory observation study. Computers in Human
being. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 5(1), 85–98. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Behavior, 100, 35–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.06.004
ppm0000065 VanDen Abeele, M. M. P., & Postma-Nilsenova, M. (2018). More than just gaze: An
McDaniel, B. T., & Drouin, M. (2019). Daily technology interruptions and emotional and experimental vignette study examining how phone-gazing and newspaper-gazing
relational well-being. Computers in Human Behavior, 99, 1–8. https://doi.org/ and phubbing-while-speaking and phubbing-while-listening compare in their effect
10.1016/j.chb.2019.04.027 on affiliation. Communication Research Reports, 35(4), 303–313. https://doi.org/
McDaniel, B. T., Galovan, A. M., & Drouin, M. (2020). Daily technoference, technology 10.1080/08824096.2018.1492911
use during couple leisure time, and relationship quality. Media Psychology. https:// Wang, X., Xie, X., Wang, Y., Wang, P., & Lei, L. (2017). Partner phubbing and depression
doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2020.1783561 among married Chinese adults: The roles of relationship satisfaction and relationship
McDaniel, B. T., & Wesselmann, E. (2021). “You phubbed me for that?” Reason given for length. Personality and Individual Differences, 110, 12–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
phubbing and perceptions of interactional quality and exclusion. Human Behavior paid.2017.01.014
and Emerging Technologies, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbe2.255 Williams, K. D. (2001). Ostracism: The power of silence. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Menard, S. (1995). Applied logistic regression analysis: Sage university series on quantitative Williams, K. D. (2009). Ostracism: Effects of being excluded and ignored. In M. P. Zanna
applications in the social sciences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 41, pp. 275–314). New York,
Miller-Ott, A., & Kelly, L. (2015). The presence of cell phones in romantic partner face-to- NY: Academic Press.
face interactions: An expectancy violation theory approach. Southern Communication Wirth, J., Sacco, D., Hugenberg, K., & Williams, K. (2010). Eye gaze as relational
Journal, 80(4), 253–270. https://doi.org/10.1080/1041794X.2015.1055371 evaluation: Averted eye gaze leads to feelings of ostracism and relational
Miller-Ott, A. E., & Kelly, L. (2017). A politeness theory analysis of cell-phone usage in devaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 869–882. https://doi.org/
the presence of friends. Communication Studies, 68(2), 190–207. https://doi.org/ 10.1177/0146167210370032
10.1080/10510974.2017.1299024

11

You might also like