You are on page 1of 18

When a "Happy" Worker Is Really a "Productive" Worker

A Review and Further Refinement of the Happy-


Productive Worker Thesis
Russell Cropanzano Colorado State University
Thomas A. Wright University of Nevada, Reno

For decades, organizational scientists and prac- we enter the new millennium. For instance,
titioners alike have been fascinated by the happy- former American Psychological Association
productive worker thesis. According to this hy- President Martin Seligman (as cited in
pothesis, happy employees exhibit higher levels
Mattox, 1999) noted that an "epidemic" of
of job-related performance behaviors than do un-
clinical depression currently exists in the
happy employees. However, despite years of re-
search, supportfor the happy-productive worker United States. To be more specific, research
thesis remains equivocal. These ambiguous find- related to the happy-productive worker the-
ings resultfrom the variety of ways in which hap- sis assumes even greater relevance when we
piness has been operationalized. Researchers have consider Myers and Diener's (1997) claim
operationalized happiness as job satisfaction, as that only a minority of Americans can be
the presence of positive affect, as the absence of considered happy. Whether one agrees with
negative affect, as the lack of emotional exhaus- Myers and Diener's assessment, the fact is
tion, and as psychological well-being. Some of that roughly 17 million Americans are cur-
these measures exhibit appreciable associations rently taking Prozac, with millions more tak-
with job performance, others do not. The
ing other forms of antidepression medica-
circumplex framework is offered as a potentially
tions (Wright, 1999). Although there are
useful taxonomy for researchers interested in
better understanding andpromoting a happy and
productive workforce. Russell Cropanzano is a professor of indus-
trial/organizational psychology at Colorado State
Every one of these hundreds of millions of University. He is a Fellow in the Society for
human beings is in some form seeking hap- Industrial and Organizational Psychology
piness. (SIOP) and is coauthor of Organizational Jus-
—Hubert George Wells, The Outline of His- tice and Human Resources Management, which
tory, 1920 won the 1998 Book Award from the Interna-
tional Association of Conflict Management.
Similar to Diogenes the Cynic and his Thomas A. Wright is a professor of organiza-
quest to find an honest man, many re- tional behavior at the University of Nevada—
searchers and practitioners alike have to Reno. Included among his recent awards is the
come to consider the happy-productive American Management Association/Organiza-
worker thesis as a holy grail of the orga- tional Dynamics Best Paper Award, the Sage
nizational sciences (Landy, 1985). Yet de- McCune Best Paper Award, and the American
Psychological Association's Most Outstanding
cades of dialogue have not resolved the
Article Award for Division 13 (Consulting Psy-
question, and many organizational scien-
chology).
tists remain skeptical about the relation Correspondence concerning this article should
between happiness and job performance be addressed to Thomas A. Wright, Managerial
(Staw, 1986; Wright & Staw, 1999). Issues of Sciences Department, University of Nevada—
worker health and happiness have arguably Reno, Reno, Nevada 89557-0206. Electronic
never been more important than they are as mail may be sent to taw@unr.nevada.edu.

182 Copyright 2001 by the Educational Publishing Foundation and the Division of Consulting Psychology, 1061-4087/01/SS.00
DOI 10.1037//106W087.53.3.182
Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, Vol. 53, No. 3, 182-199
many causes of unhappiness, some are un- 1991; Sandvik, Diener, Seidlitz, 1993) and
doubtedly the result of work-related issues. responsive to therapeutic interventions
In this article, we reexamine the happy- (Seligman, 1994, 1995). Taken together,
productive worker thesis. We propose that it is generally accepted that happiness re-
the equivocal findings to date are largely fers to a subjective and global judgment
due to the inconsistent manner in which hap- that one is experiencing a good deal of
piness has been understood. To be more spe- positive emotion and relatively little nega-
cific, although there is reasonable consen- tive emotion.
sus in regard to the conceptual definition All things being equal, few people
of happiness (Diener, 1984), there is less would prefer to be unhappy when they
agreement as to the operational definition. could be happy. In fact, Sullivan (1989)
To illustrate this fact, in the pages that fol- argued that seeking pleasure and avoid-
low, we identify a variety of ways in which ing pain is fundamental to human motiva-
happiness has been operationalized in or- tion. Given the worth that most people
ganizational research. As we shall demon- place on happiness and the fact that only
strate, some of these approaches are more some people have enough of it, we can
robust than others in predicting perfor- begin to understand the relation between
mance. Despite this caveat, our general con- happiness and job performance. When one
clusion is an optimistic one for management looks across a group of people, happiness
scholars and practitioners alike: Happiness can be seen as a valuable, though some-
promotes higher job performance. times scarce, resource (Hobfoll, 1998;
Myers & Diener, 1997). This inherent
Why Happiness Should Be Related value placed on happiness, coupled with
its occasional scarcity, underscores the
to Job Performance
importance of conserving or maintaining
Diener (1984) noted that virtually all sci- happiness whenever possible (Hobfoll,
entific approaches to happiness converge 1988, 1989, 1998).
around three defining phenomenon. First, The necessity for resource maintenance
happiness is a subjective experience (Diener, places happy and unhappy people in dif-
1994; Diener, Sandvik, Seidlitz, & Diener, ferent psychological situations. When an
1993; Parducci, 1995). People are happy to unhappy person goes to work, he or she
the extent that they believe themselves to needs to protect and defend his or her rela-
be happy. For this reason, happiness involves tively limited reserve of happiness. One
some type of judgment as to the pluses and is less likely to take risks when something
minuses of one's life (Parducci, 1995). Sec- precious is in short supply. For this rea-
ond, happiness includes both the relative son, unhappy people (a) are sensitive to
presence of positive emotions and the rela- threats in their work environment, (b) are
tive absence of negative emotions (Argyle, defensive and cautious around their co-
1987; Diener & Larsen, 1993; Michalos, workers, and (c) are less optimistic and
1985; Warr, 1987,1990). Third, happiness is confident. Happy people, on the other
a global judgment. It refers to one's life as a hand, have a more plentiful stock of a valu-
whole. As the term is used here, happiness is able resource. When at work, they can
an overall evaluation that exhibits some mea- more afford to take risks that could po-
sure of stability over time (Diener, 1994; tentially increase their supply of happi-
Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999; Myers, ness. For this reason, happy people (a) are
1993; Veenhoven, 1988), though it is more sensitive to opportunities in their work
strongly influenced by environmental environments, (b) are more outgoing and
events (Pavot, Diener, Colvin, & Sandvik, helpful to their coworkers, and (c) are more

Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research 183


Summer 2001
optimistic and confident. According to this who are more cautious and protective in
resource maintenance model, the need to social situations, sometimes incline toward
conserve their limited resource supply can introversion and shyness (Argyle, 1987).
lead unhappy people to be poorer perform- Happy people tend to be more outgoing and
ers, whereas the flexibility afforded by their extroverted (Diener, Sandvik, Pavot, &
more bountiful resource reserve can im- Fujita, 1992; Headey & Wearing, 1988;
prove the performance of happy persons. Myers & Diener, 1995). At times, unhappy
These possibilities are discussed more thor- people can even become acrimonious. In
oughly in the paragraphs that follow. one longitudinal study, Bolger and Schilling
When something precious is also rare, (1991) found that people who were prone
individuals are more sensitive to cues that to negative emotion were apt to use conten-
signal a threat (Hobfoll, 1989; Lee & tious interpersonal tactics, thereby provok-
Ashforth, 1996). On the other hand, indi- ing the ire of coworkers. It is no surprise
viduals can afford less worry when a re- that unhappy people report feeling less co-
source is more abundant. As a result, happy worker and supervisory support than do
people are more sensitive to positive events, their more happy counterparts (Staw, Sutton,
whereas unhappy people are more sensitive & Pelled, 1994). All of these findings sug-
to negative events. For example, experimen- gest that, generally speaking, happy people
tal research by Seidlitz and Diener (1993) would perform better on the wide range of
and Seidlitz, Wyer, and Diener (1997) found jobs that require social interaction.
that, when compared with their happier coun- Accentuating negative events and
terparts, those who were low on well-being minimizing social contact eventually
were more likely to encode an ambiguous takes its toll. Relative to their happier co-
event as threatening. Likewise, another ex- workers, unhappy people are likely to have
periment by Larsen and Ketelaar (1989) lower self-esteem (Diener et al., 1999;
found that unfavorable feedback was more Myers, 1993). Likewise, unhappy people
hurtful to those who were prone to negative see themselves as having less control over
emotions and less hurtful to those who were events in their lives and are less optimis-
prone to positive emotions. On the other tic about the future (Dember & Brooks,
hand, favorable feedback yielded larger ben- 1989; Seligman, 1991). This sense of de-
efits for those who were predisposed to posi- moralization may make unhappy people
tive emotions while yielding smaller ben- less proactive (Argyle, 1987) and more
efits to those predisposed to negative prone to stress symptoms (Myers & Di-
emotions (for more evidence, see Derryberry ener, 1995).
& Read, 1994; Larsen & Ketelaar, 1991;
If happiness should conceptually be
Rusting & Larsen, 1997). These effects per-
related to job performance, then why are
sist over time because of the manner in
the connections viewed by some scholars
which happy and unhappy people recall
as tenuous? In our view, the problem has
events. Happy people tend to remember fa-
do to with the diverse manner in which
vorable events, whereas unhappy people
happiness has been operationalized. When
tend to remember unfavorable ones (Seidlitz
testing the happy-productive worker the-
& Diener, 1993; Seidlitz et al., 1997). This
sis, both scholars and practitioners have
tendency to emphasize the negative aspects
primarily treated happiness in four ways:
of work life is likely to have deleterious con-
as job satisfaction, as the profile of posi-
sequences for job performance.
tive and negative affectivity, as the lack
Interacting with other people is simulta- of emotional exhaustion, and as psycho-
neously fraught with emotional peril but logical well-being. The results of these
filled with opportunities. Unhappy people, four operationalizations are not fully con-

184 Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research


Summer 2001
sistent. In the remainder of this article, we However, other evidence has called these
examine tests of the happy-productive modest conclusions into question.
worker thesis to workplace happiness us- In a recent review, Judge et al. (1998)
ing each of these approaches. argued that the earlier meta-analytic re-
view by Iaffaldano and Muchinsky (1985)
Happiness as Job Satisfaction: may have produced overly conservative
More Than Meets the Eye? results. In fact, a meta-analysis by Petty,
McGee, and Cavender (1984) placed the
Historically, a common means of testing relation at +.31. In their meta-analysis,
the happy-productive worker thesis was by Judge and his colleagues estimated the
operationalizing happiness as job satisfac- association to be roughly +.30, though
tion. This approach was favored by early re- they observed some important moderators.
searchers, such as Herzberg, Mausner, and For instance, they found some job satis-
Snyderman (1959); Houser (1927); and faction measures were more closely re-
Roethlisberger and Dickson (1939). Al- lated to performance than others, ranging
though contemporary scholars distinguish from an aggregated correlation of +.06 for
between satisfaction with a particular job research measuring job satisfaction with
and happiness with life as a whole (e.g., the GM Faces Scale to an aggregated cor-
Diener et al., 1999; Judge, 1992; Judge & relation of +.51 for research using the
Locke, 1993), historical convention has Hoppock Job Satisfaction Blank to mea-
sometimes led the happy-productive sure job satisfaction. Judge et al. also re-
worker thesis to be operationalized as the ported considerable differences across
relation between job satisfaction and job occupations, with the job satisfaction-per-
performance. For example, Judge, Tho- formance correlation being more robust
resen, Bono, and Patton (1998, p. 3) opened in high-complexity jobs. Finally, their
their recent meta-analysis of the job satis- meta-analytic results demonstrated that
faction-performance relation with the ques- measures of overall job satisfaction dis-
tion: "Is a happy worker a productive one?" play higher correlations than facet mea-
sures of job satisfaction. Despite the vari-
With this in mind, several reviews have
ability in the job satisfaction-performance
provided excellent summaries of the sat-
relationship, many of the estimates are
isfaction—performance relation. For ex-
certainly large enough to have sizable fi-
ample, reviewing the literature to date,
nancial consequences for work organiza-
Brayfield and Crockett (1955, p. 421) de-
tions (Cascio, 1991). As a result, some or-
termined that "satisfaction ... need not
ganizational scholars have become more
imply strong motivation to outstanding
sanguine about the prospect of finding a
performance." Similar conclusions were
useful relation between job satisfaction
reached in a later review by Vroom (1964),
and performance (e.g., Brief, 1998; Judge,
who found that the median association
Hanisch, & Drankoski, 1995; Podsakoff &
between these two variables was a rather
Williams, 1986).
modest +.14. Finally, a meta-analytic re-
view by Iaffaldano and Muchinsky (1985) Of course, the positive associations
obtained a mean corrected correlation of obtained by Judge et al. (1998) and Petty
+.17. Together, these reviews generated a et al. (1984) do not address the issue of
good deal of skepticism regarding the re- causality. There are at least three causal
lation between job satisfaction and per- relations between job satisfaction and per-
formance (e.g., Cherrington, Reitz, & formance: (a) Job satisfaction may cause
Scott, 1971; Fisher, 1980; Locke, 1976; job performance, (b) job performance may
Podsakoff & Williams, 1986; Staw, 1986). cause job satisfaction, and (c) both perfor-

Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research 185


Summer 2001
mance and satisfaction may be caused by izes to both adults and children (Russell &
a variety of third variables. During the Ridgeway, 1983) as well as across a variety
1960s and 70s, several studies investi- of cultures (Russell, Lewicka, & Nitt, 1989)
gated these sundry causal paths, and at and different analytic methods (Meyer &
least nominal evidence was obtained for Shack, 1989). As a result, the circumplex
each (for reviews, see Lawler & Porter, structure is widely accepted (e.g., Larsen &
1967; Nord, 1976; Schwab & Cummings, Diener, 1992; Mayer & Gaschke, 1988),
1970). Unfortunately, as Judge et al. (1998) though the circumplex is not without prob-
observed, the general skepticism regard- lems of its own.
ing the job satisfaction-performance re- One concern with the circumplex is that
lation led to a decline in research. As a it is often impractical to apply. Because
consequence, the causal linkage between the model represents emotions in two-di-
these two variables, if any, remains a mensional space, it is not clear how one
worthwhile topic for future research. would use the entire model as a predictor
of organizational behaviors. For this rea-
Affective Dispositional Approaches to son, researchers simplify the circumplex
[Un]Happiness: [Un]Happiness as model by factor analyzing the emotion
Positive Affectivity, Negative Affectivity, terms. Given the circular structure, differ-
Emotional Exhaustion, and Weil-Being ent factor solutions could potentially pro-
vide an equally good fit to the model. For
An Overview and Organizing example, consider the solid vertical and
Taxonomy horizontal axes in Figure 1. These two fac-
tors result from an unrotated factor model.
Much of the confusion underlying the The vertical factor designates "hedonic
term happiness stems from the overall struc- tone" or "pleasantness-unpleasantness."
ture of dispositional affect. Because happi- It is anchored by such adjectives as "joy-
ness refers to emotions, a clear "map" of ous" on the high end and "sad" on the low.
emotion terms would enhance conceptual Thus, this dimension is often interpreted
precision. This need to dimensionalize emo- as psychological well-being (e.g., Myers,
tional well-being has long been recognized 1993). On the other hand, the horizontal
(Larsen & Diener, 1992). In the second cen- axis refers to the level of activation, or
tury, Galen devised a four-fold typology "affect intensity," of emotional experi-
(e.g., melancholic, choleric, sanguine, and ence (Larsen & Diener, 1992). In the or-
phlegmatic) to describe various differences ganizational sciences, this arousal dimen-
in emotions. Much later, Kant expanded on sion has sometimes been understood as
Galen's work in his treatise Anthropologie. emotional exhaustion. For example, Wright
Still later, Wundt transformed Galen and and Bonett (1997a, 1997b) maintained that
Kant's work into a pleasantness-unpleasant- high emotional exhaustion can be treated as
ness interpretation of emotion. More re- low emotional activation-intensity. This
cently, Russell and his colleagues (Russell, makes sense insofar as exhaustion is defined
1978, 1979, 1980; Russell, Weiss, & as a numb state of emotional detachment
Mendelsohn, 1989), using multidimen- (Maslach, 1982; Maslach & Leiter, 1997),
sional scaling techniques, found that emo- though we shall qualify this observation later.
tion terms can be organized into a roughly When the primary dimensional factors
circular structure called a circumplex. An are rotated 45 degrees, two new dimensions
idealized circumplex structure is displayed emerge: positive and negative affectivity.
in Figure 1. This circumplex structure has These are designated in Figure 1 by the two
been found to be quite robust, as it general- dashed lines. The positive affectivity (PA)

186 Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research


Summer 2001
Happy
Serene Joyous

Content Enthusiastic

Excited
Placid

Energetic
Relaxed

Calm Active

Quiet Intense

Quiescent Alarmed

Droopy Distressed

Bored Annoyed
Sad
Figure 1. An idealized representation of the circumplex model of dispositional happiness. The solid lines refer to
the unrotated factor solution, and the dashed lines refer to the rotated solution.

dimension begins in the upper right quad- These are the most widely accepted orga-
rant of Figure 1 and runs diagonally to the nizational perspectives on dispositional af-
lower left quadrant. High PAs tend to report fect. More important, each ofthese approaches
experiencing positive emotions, such as shares a common origin in the circumplex
"enthusiastic" and "excited." At the low end, model. In particular, they result from two dif-
PA is best characterized by the absence of ferent rotational solutions and, as such, are
positive emotions, such as "bored." In the different representations of the same underly-
lower right quadrant of Figure 1, and run- ing reality. We will now consider how each of
ning to the upper left quadrant, is the nega- these perspectives relates to job performance.
tive affectivity (NA) dimension. High NAs
report emotions that are negative, such as The Rotated Solution: Positive and
"alarmed" and "distressed." Low NAs tend Negative Affectivity
to endorse the absence of negative emotions.
For example, a low NA might indicate a ten- Happiness as positive and negative
dency to be "placid" or "relaxed." There is affectivity. According to Diener (1994; Di-
considerable evidence that the NA-PA so- ener et al., 1999), one way to conceptual-
lution is a useful one (e.g., Brief, Butcher, & ize happiness is as the presence of disposi-
Roberson, 1995; Brief & Roberson, 1989; tional positive affect (PA) and the absence
Burke, Brief, George, Roberson, & Webster, of dispositional negative affect (NA). This
1989; George, 1996; Watson, 1988; Watson approach is also evident in the work of
& Clark, 1984; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, Watson and Clark (1984) and Costa and
1988; Watson & Tellegen, 1985). Moreover, McCrae (1980). As discussed by George
NA and PA show up across cultures (Wat- (1992,1996) and Judge (1992), there is now
son, Clark, & Tellegen, 1984) and when both compelling evidence that PA and NA pre-
self- and peer ratings are examined. dict such work attitudes as job satisfaction

Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research 187


Summer 2001
and organizational commitment (e.g., general or trait measures of PA and NA do not
Cropanzano, James, & Konovsky, 1993; rule out the possibility that emotions experi-
Levin & Stokes, 1989; Watson & Slack, enced on the job will predict performance.
1993). NA also seems to be related to re- In an interesting field study, George
ports of job stress and strain (Brief, Burke, (1991) tested this hypothesis directly.
George, Robinson, & Webster, 1988; Burke, George measured PA experienced at work
Brief, & George, 1993; Watson & Penneba- during the past week using a subscale from
ker, 1989). In addition, NA has been posi- the Job Affect Scale (Burke et al., 1989).
tively related to voluntary employee turn- She also measured more general or trait
over (Wright & Cropanzano, 1998). levels of PA using the Positive Emotional-
Given the resource maintenance model we ity Scale from the Multidimensional Per-
presented earlier, one would expect PA to ex- sonality Questionnaire (Tellegen, 1982).
hibit a positive association to job perfor- Consistent with research reviewed above,
mance, whereas NA should exhibit a negative George found that a trait indicator of PA
association. However, the evidence is equivo- was unrelated to worker altruism and cus-
cal. Wright and Cropanzano (1998) were un- tomer service ratings. However, the PA one
able to find a relation between either NA or experienced at work over the past week was
PA and performance. Null relations were also correlated with both altruism and customer
reported by Wright and Staw (1999). However, service. Wright and Cropanzano (1999)
Cropanzano et al. (1993) did obtain some sig- found similar results in a later study. That
nificant interactions. In their Study 1, they is, although neither positive trait, negative
found a PA by tenure interaction, such that PA trait, or positive mood today were related
was related to performance for employees who to performance, negative mood today was
had high tenure but not for those with low so related. These findings suggest that
tenure. Furthermore, they obtained an NA by time- and work-specific measures of NA
tenure interaction in their Study 2. To be more and PA might prove better predictors of
specific, there was a relation between NA and job performance than more general or trait
performance for those who were low in tenure measures. For this reason, it is premature
but not for those who were high. Unfortu- to close the door to future inquiry, as there
nately, these interactions were obtained post are only a few studies that have examined
hoc and have yet to be replicated. the relation between NA-PA and job per-
Part of the reason for these disappoint- formance. We would call for future re-
ing results may have less to do with dis- search on this topic. In addition, there is
positional affectivity and more to do with another reason why inquiry into NA and
measurement. For example, Cropanzano PA should not be abandoned. There is in-
and his colleagues used the respected Posi- direct evidence that NA and PA might be
tive and Negative Affectivity Schedule related to performance if we conceptual-
(Watson et al., 1988) to query participants ize the predictors more broadly as either
regarding their general levels of positive extraversion or emotional stability. It is to
and negative emotions. Although general this evidence that we now turn.
levels of positive and negative emotion are Extraversion and emotional sta-
important in their own right, our resource bility. As discussed by George (1996),
maintenance model would posit that more NA is closely related to the broad per-
time-specific measures of emotion or sonality dimension of emotional stabil-
mood at work are the drivers of produc- ity (also called neuroticism), whereas PA
tive behaviors (for a related argument, see is related to the personality dimension
Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). For this rea- of extraversion (Watson, Clark, Mcln-
son, the meager findings in studies using tyre, & Hamaker, 1992). Given this fact,

188 Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research


Summer 2001
Costa and McCrae (1980) maintained Rothstein (1991), who found that extra-
that a "happy" person can be viewed as version was not an especially useful
one who is high on extraversion and low predictor of job performance (mean cor-
on neuroticism. In keeping with the rected validity = .16), though emotional
happy-productive worker thesis, it stability was. In a critique of this study,
therefore follows that extraversion and Ones, Mount, Barrick, and Hunter
emotional stability (or neuroticism) (1994) raised concerns about the data
could be related to job performance. In analytic techniques used by Tett and his
fact, meta-analytic evidence suggests colleagues. Fortunately, a more thor-
that this is probably the case (Hough, ough re-analysis reported by Tett, Jack-
Eaton, Dunnette, Kamp, & McCloy, son, Rothstein, and Reddon (1994)
1990). showed only small changes in the re-
Hough et al. (1990) reviewed valid- sults and no major changes in the over-
ity coefficients among several military all conclusions.
samples and found that emotional sta- Although the meta-analytic literature
bility predicted three dimensions of per- is not perfectly consistent, it strongly
formance (effort and leadership, per- suggests that NA (via emotional stabil-
sonal discipline, and physical fitness ity) and PA (via extraversion) could be
and military bearing) but not two oth- related to job performance. However,
ers (technical proficiency and general this evidence is at best indirect. Al-
soldiering). Unfortunately for our pur- though PA is correlated with extraver-
poses here, Hough and her colleagues sion and NA with emotional stability (or
did not examine a pure extraversion neuroticism), extraversion and emo-
measure that we can be certain included tional stability are substantially broader
PA. In another meta-analysis, Barrick constructs than PA and NA. Whereas
and Mount (1991) found that emotional emotional stability or neuroticism is
stability was not consistently related to conceptually close to the concept of NA
performance. However, Barrick and (for evidence, see Watson & Clark,
Mount cautioned that these findings 1984), differences remain (Costa &
might be due to range restriction—those McCrae, 1988; McCrae & Costa, 1987).
who are very unstable are likely to have Thus, we cannot be certain whether it is
opted out of the workforce or, at least, the emotional aspects of extraversion
out of particular jobs. Extraversion was and emotional stability that cause the
related to performance, though only for observed differences in job perfor-
certain jobs with a strong component mance. This meta-analytic research is
involving social interaction. Salgado promising, and we strongly recommend
(1997) conducted a meta-analysis of continued inquiry.
personality and performance among
studies involving the European commu- The Unrotated Solution: Happiness as
nity. He found that emotional stability
(the Lack of) Emotional Exhaustion
was a valid predictor of performance,
whereas extraversion was valid among and Psychological Weil-Being
occupations with a strong social com-
Happiness as emotional activation-in-
ponent. These findings lend limited sup- tensity: Emotional exhaustion. Somewhat
port to the happy—productive worker more supportive findings come when re-
thesis. searchers try to predict job performance
A more controversial meta-analysis from the other two dimensions: arousal and
was conducted by Tett, Jackson, and well-being. We shall begin with the arousal

Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research 189


Summer 2001
dimension that is represented by the hori- replicate this finding (Wright & Bonett,
zontal axis in Figure 1. As one can see, 1997b). Finally, additional supportive re-
this dimension designates the intensity or sults were obtained by both Jones and Best
vigor with which an emotion is felt. This (1995) and Wright and Cropanzano
is, of course, without regard to the hedonic (1998), who found that emotional exhaus-
tone of the emotion—one can feel fer- tion predicted supervisory ratings of job
vently good or fervently bad. Although performance. Wright and Cropanzano fur-
activation-intensity is an important topic ther observed that exhaustion scores were
in the personality literature (see Larsen & associated with employee voluntary turn-
Diener, 1992, for a review), it has received over as much as one year later. The net re-
minimal attention in the organizational sult is that emotional exhaustion holds
sciences. considerable promise as a predictor of job
One exception to this generalization can performance. Despite this promise, there
be found in research on emotional exhaus- are two obvious problems with using emo-
tion. Emotional exhaustion is widely con- tional exhaustion as an indicator of hap-
sidered to be a key component of burnout piness. One is specific to emotional ex-
(Cordes & Dougherty, 1993; Shirom, 1989; haustion, and the other is a general problem
Wright & Cropanzano, 1998). When indi- with activation-intensity measures.
viduals are placed under intensely stress- Let us begin with the problem specific to
ful conditions, they experience an emotional emotional exhaustion. By definition, emo-
numbness (Maslach, 1982; Maslach & tional exhaustion refers to the emotional
Leiter, 1997). As Wright and Bonett (1997b) numbing that accompanies burnout. Al-
have pointed out, high emotional exhaus- though this would seem to be indicative of
tion would seem to be a manifestation of low emotional intensity, an examination of
(very) low emotional arousal. Emotional ex- the survey items suggests that this might not
haustion has also proven to be a useful pre- be the case. The most widely used measure
dictor of various stress outcomes and work of emotional exhaustion is a subscale from
attitudes (Lee & Ashforth, 1996). However, the Maslach Burnout Inventory (Maslach &
until recently, the relation between emo- Jackson, 1986). In this scale, emotional ex-
tional exhaustion and job performance has haustion is indicated by such items as "Work-
seldom been investigated. ing with people all day is really a strain for
In one study, Quattrochi-Tubin, Jones, me," "I feel emotionally drained from my
and Breedlove (1982) found that self-re- work," "I feel used up at the end of the work-
ported emotional exhaustion was related day," "I feel frustrated with my job," and "I
to self-reported performance among so- feel like I'm at the end of my rope." These
cial service workers. However, this work items could be more indicative of the ab-
is limited in that it used self-report mea- sence of positive emotion or the presence of
sures of both independent and dependent negative emotion (or both), rather than the
variables: exhaustion and performance. absence of emotion in general (cf. Meyer &
Research by Garden (1991) suggested that Shack, 1989; Watson & Clark, 1984). If so,
burnout shows a higher relation to self- then this operationalization of emotional ex-
reported performance than to other per- haustion might suggest low PA, high NA, or
formance measures. In an extension of pre- low well-being, rather than low activation-
vious work, Wright and Bonett (1997a) intensity. Further validation of the emotional
found that emotional exhaustion was exhaustion scale, particularly noting its dis-
negatively correlated with supervisory criminate validity to other measures of af-
ratings of job performance. However, a fective dispositions, is necessary to rule out
second study by these authors failed to this possibility. Of course, if emotional ex-

190 Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research


Summer 2001
haustion proved to be an indicator of NA, sion makers, showed better interper-
PA, or well-being, then the findings we have sonal behaviors, and received higher
reviewed would still support the happy-pro- overall performance ratings. Staw and
ductive worker thesis, but they would not be Barsade's study is important for two rea-
supportive of the notion that activation-in- sons. First, it used objective, quantifi-
tensity is related to job performance. able indices of performance (e.g., an
Regardless of the outcomes of future re- "in-basket" measure). This argues
search, there is a more fundamental problem against the possibility that correlations
with using any measure of activation-inten- between well-being and job perfor-
sity as an indicator of happiness. With re- mance are simply misperceptions (Rob-
spect to emotion, by definition, an intensity bins & DeNisi, 1994). Second, Staw and
measure does not imply either positive or Barsade's experimental data suggest a
negative feeling states (Larsen & Diener, causal relation, such that performance
1992). It merely implies that one is experi- increases when well-being is high. This
encing some emotion with vigor; it does not is consistent with our resource mainte-
denote whether that emotion is positive or nance model, though it is not clear from
negative. By all accounts, a happy person is this evidence alone whether these find-
relatively free of negative emotion, while ings will generalize outside a laboratory
prone to experience positive emotion setting. These generalizability concerns
(Larsen & Diener, 1992). As a consequence, were addressed in a later field study by
activation—intensity can be ruled out as a Staw et al. (1994). In their longitudinal
type of happiness, though it will no doubt study, Staw and his colleagues first as-
remain important for other purposes. We now sessed well-being. To do so, the re-
examine happiness considered as psycho- searchers constructed a scale ad hoc out
logical well-being. of items indicating the presence of
negative emotion (e.g., depression) and
Happiness as psychological well-
the absence of positive emotion. Eigh-
being. The solid vertical line in Figure
teen months later they measured vari-
1 captures the construct of psychological
ous criterion variables. To be more spe-
or subjective well-being. Well-being cap-
cific, when compared with those who
tures both positive and negative emo-
were low in well-being, workers high in
tional states on a single axis. The high pole
well-being had superior performance
is anchored by such adjectives as "joyous."
evaluations and higher pay. Thus, well-
The low pole is anchored by "sad" and "an-
being predicted both a subjective and an
noyed." Thus, to be high on well-being is to
objective indicator of performance.
be simultaneously low on negative emotion
and high on positive emotion. For these rea- The work of Staw and his colleagues sug-
sons, various scholars have more or less gests that well-being causes increases in job
equated well-being with happiness (e.g., Di- performance. Also supporting this possibil-
ener, 1984; Myers, 1993). ity is a 2-year longitudinal study by Wright,
In support of the happy-productive Bonett, and Sweeney (1993). Using the 8-
worker thesis, a growing body of em- item Index of Psychological Weil-Being de-
pirical research has found significant veloped by Berkman (1971a, 1971b),
associations between various measures Wright and his colleagues found that aver-
of well-being and measures of job-re- age well-being (Time 1 and Time 2 together)
lated performance. For example, in an was positively related to supervisory per-
experimental study, Staw and Barsade formance ratings at Time 3. Similar results
(1993) found that students who were were found by Cropanzano and Wright
high on well-being were superior deci- (1999). Wright and Staw (1999) conducted

Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research 191


Summer 2001
additional research to address the possibility tively related to various measures of job
of a causal path from well-being to perfor- performance. This seems to be the case
mance. To that end, Wright and Staw's two lon- regardless of whether the criterion vari-
gitudinal studies—incorporating multiple ables are objective indices or subjective
measures of both performance and well-be- ratings (Staw & Barsade, 1993; Staw et
ing—demonstrated that psychological well- al., 1994). The effect also holds in ex-
being significantly predicted supervisory perimental, cross-sectional, and longi-
performance ratings beyond the variance ac- tudinal studies, even after controlling
counted for by prior supervisory perfor- for the effects of possible confounding
mance ratings. These findings support the variables (e.g., Wright & Cropanzano,
possibility that well-being is a cause of job 1997; Wright & Staw, 1999). Taken to-
performance. gether, these results are indicative of a
It is also useful to ascertain the effects plausible causal relation.
of other possible confounding variables.
This was done in a longitudinal study by Directions for Future Research:
Wright and Bonett (1997b). In this study, Management Versus Selection
the authors found that well-being was a Approaches to Increasing Happiness
positive predictor of job performance.
Moreover, this relation remained signifi- Through this review, we have alluded
cant even after controlling for the simul- to various possibilities for future re-
taneous effect of emotional exhaustion. A search inquiry. In this final section, how-
potentially pernicious third variable is job ever, we would like to raise what we see
satisfaction. Given that job satisfaction as a pivotal research question: What are
and well-being are correlated (e.g., Diener organizations to do with these findings?
et al., 1999; Judge & Locke, 1993), it could Assuming the findings reported in this
be that psychological well-being and job review are replicated and supported by
performance are only associated due to a future inquiry, they suggest two practi-
spurious relation with job satisfaction. On cal possibilities. Organizations could
the basis of the limited evidence to date, undertake various strategies to better
this seems not to be the case. In a cross- manage their employees to produce
sectional study, Wright and Cropanzano greater happiness, or they could select
(1997; Study 1) found that well-being was employees who are happier to begin with
positively related to job performance be- (Cropanzano & Wright, 1999). Little re-
yond job satisfaction. These findings were search exists comparing the efficacy of
replicated and extended in a longitudinal these two strategies. However, on the ba-
follow-up. In their Study 2, Wright and sis of the available literature as well as
Cropanzano found that well-being and job ethical considerations, we suggest that the
performance were positively associated management approach is likely to be the
even when the effects of job satisfaction, most beneficial.
NA, and PA were all taken into account.
Finally, well-being remained significantly
The Management Approach to
related to performance even when con-
trolling for employee age, gender, job ten- Promoting Happiness
ure, and educational level (Wright & Staw,
There is some measure of stability to
1999, Study 2).
self-reported happiness; people who are
The available data point to a common happy (or unhappy) at one time are
conclusion: When happiness is oper- likely to be happy (or unhappy) at an-
ationalized as well-being, it is posi- other (Diener et al., 1999; Cropanzano

192 Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research


Summer 2001
& Wright, 1999). Nevertheless, this chological measure as a screening tool
does not necessarily imply that the sta- (Cropanzano & Wright, 1999). This pro-
bility is entirely due to personal char- cess raises the mean level of workplace
acteristics. Happiness may be stable be- happiness by denying unhappy people
cause one's life circumstances are stable jobs. To our thinking, this approach raises
as well. For example, a person may re- serious ethical concerns. For example, the
main at the same job or at a very similar failure to select prospective employees on
job. For this reason, the fact of stability the basis of their psychological well-be-
cannot be used to argue against the pos- ing could depress these individuals fur-
sibility of meaningful interventions de- ther, which in turn could make these job
signed to promote worker happiness. As candidates even more unemployable in the
of yet, the evidence for this possibility future. As a consequence, careful consid-
is tentative, but it is highly suggestive. eration of these and other related issues is
In two separate longitudinal studies, of paramount importance for management
Kohn and Schooler (1982) and Wright personnel, their employing organizations,
and Bonett (1992) found that employ- and practicing consultants, using various
ees' mental health changed over time, measures of psychological well-being to
depending on their working conditions. select happy workers.
Those who were employed in favorable Even if one does not share our ethical
environments reported more positive reservations, there remain practical prob-
emotions than those who were in less fa- lems in need of future research. If well-
vorable environments. being measures were used for employee
A somewhat different perspective on selection, then it is likely that job appli-
this problem has been taken by Judge and cants would be motivated to fake their re-
Locke (1993) and Judge, Locke, and sponses (Cropanzano & Wright, 1999).
Durham (1997). These researchers have Some research suggests that those who
asked why a person might experience feel- dissimulate have a better chance of receiv-
ings of high or low well-being. Judge and ing offers than those who respond hon-
his colleagues argued that people make estly (Douglas, McDaniel, & Snell, 1996;
different core evaluations regarding their Rosse, Stecher, Miller, & Levin, 1998).
lives and their jobs. On the basis of these Obviously, such faking is unlikely to
core evaluations, individuals experience maximize the utility of a staffing system.
certain levels of job satisfaction and
well-being. For example, some workers Concluding Thoughts
overgeneralize failure or hold them-
selves to impossibly high standards. In this review, we proposed that the am-
These individuals are apt to experience biguous findings to date regarding the
low job satisfaction and poor well-be- happy-productive worker thesis can be con-
ing. This approach offers useful guide- sidered a result of the various ways in which
lines for implementing changes in core happiness has been operationalized. Al-
evaluations to promote enhanced well- though various mechanisms exist, we of-
being and subsequent performance. fered the circumplex framework as a poten-
tially useful taxonomy for helping to better
The Selection Approach to categorize and measure happiness. Re-
Promoting Happiness search is now needed to use systematically
this taxonomy to investigate further the role
Another way to raise the mean level of of happiness-unhappiness in the prediction
workforce happiness is to use some psy- of performance. For instance, research has

Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research 193


Summer 2001
demonstrated that performance is related to tive counterparts by a ratio of 17 to 1! Further-
various measures of depression (Staw et al., more, a number of prominent researchers in
1994). However, although various forms of the occupational health field propose that
depression (e.g., fatigued vs. agitated) are only a minority of Americans can be consid-
similar constructs because they possess ered truly happy (cf. Myers & Diener, 1997).
large measures of unpleasantness, they also Thus, it seems evident to us that promoting
differ because they both possess differing happiness-related issues is an intrinsic good
measures of activation (Larsen & Diener, for which all should work (Ruack, 1999). The
1992; Wright & Doherty, 1998). Awareness health model approach emphasizes the de-
of this distinction could be very relevant in velopment of what's right with people (Wright
better predicting job performance across & Cropanzano, 2000). If this health approach
different job situations. As an example, al- promotes better performance, and our review
though experienced levels of enthusiasm suggests that this is so, then so much the bet-
and excitement might be more influential ter. Regardless, the pursuit of employee hap-
in predicting performance or success in piness remains valuable for its own sake.
sales positions, experienced levels of fatigue
and exhaustion might be more predictive References
in various client contact intensive positions,
such as human services and social welfare. Argyle, M. (1987). The experience of happi-
We close by emphasizing an impor- ness. London: Methuen.
tant issue. That is, this need to consider Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The
Big Five personality dimensions and job
more carefully such taxonomies as the performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel
circumplex model has both theoretical Psychology, 44, 1-26.
and applied relevance in today's soci- Berkman, P. L. (1971a). Measurement of mental
ety. To be more specific, this review was health in a general population survey. Ameri-
primarily concerned with the relations can Journal of Epidemiology, 94, 105-111.
Berkman, P. L. (1971b). Life stress and psy-
among job satisfaction, dispositional chological well-being: A replication of
affect, emotional exhaustion, psycho- Langer's analysis in the midtown Manhat-
logical well-being, and performance. tan study. Journal of Health and Social Be-
However, it should in no way be con- havior, 12, 35—45.
strued to suggest that job-related per- Bolger, N., & Schilling, E. A. (1991). Personality
and the problems of everyday life: The role of
formance is the only, or even the most neuroticism in exposure and reactivity to daily
important, reason that happiness is im- stressors. Journal ofPersonality, 59,355-386.
portant. In fact, too often in applied re- Brayfield, A. H., & Crockett, W. H. (1955). Em-
search, the emphasis appears to have been ployee attitudes and employee performance.
on what one might call the disease model Psychological Bulletin, 52,396-424.
(Wright & Cropanzano, 2000)—that is, a Brief, A. P. (1998). Attitudes in and around or-
ganizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
focus primarily concerned with fixing Brief, A. P., Burke, M. X, George, J. M., Robinson,
what's wrong with someone as opposed to B., & Webster, J. (1988). Should negative af-
developing what's right. fectivity remain an unmeasured variable in the
study of job stress? Journal of Applied Psy-
This extreme emphasis on the negative, chology, 73, 193-198.
to the relative neglect of the positive, has Brief, A. P., Butcher, A. H., & Roberson, L. (1995).
apparently had a significant and detrimen- Cookies, disposition, andjob attitudes: The ef-
tal effect on how many people have come fects of positive mood-inducing events and
to perceive their world. For example, Myers negative affectivity on job satisfaction in a field
experiment. Organizational Behavior and Hu-
and Diener (1995) noted that psychologi- man Decision Processes, 62, 55—62.
cal publications focusing on the negative Brief, A. P., & Roberson, L. (1989). Job attitude
aspects of well-being outnumber their posi- organization: An exploratory study. Journal of

194 Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research


Summer 2001
Applied Social Psychology, 79,717-727. Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-be-
Burke, M. J., Brief, A. P., & George, J. M. (1993). ing: Progress and opportunities. Social Indi-
The role of negative affectivity in understand- cators Research, 31, 103-157.
ing relations between self-reports of stressors Diener, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1993). The experi-
and strains: A comment on the applied psy- ence of emotional well-being. In M. Lewis &
chology literature. Journal of Applied Psychol- J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions
ogy, 78, 402^12. (pp. 404-^15). New York: Guilford Press.
Burke, M. J., Brief, A. P., George, J. M., Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Pavot, W, & Fujita, F.
Roberson, L., & Webster, J. (1989). Measur- (1992). Extraversion and subjective well-be-
ing affect at work: Confirmatory analyses of ing in a U.S. national probability sample. Jour-
competing mood structures with conceptual nal of Research in Personality, 26, 205-215.
linkages to cortical regulatory systems. Jour- Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Seidlitz, L., & Diener,
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5 7, M. (1993). The relationship between income
1091-1102. and subjective well-being: Relative or abso-
Cascio, W. F. (1991). Costing human resources: lute? Social Indicators Research, 28,195-223.
Thefinancial impact of behavior in organiza- Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H.
tions (3rd ed.). Boston: PWS-Kent. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three de-
Cherrington, D. J., Reitz, H. J., & Scott, W. E. cades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125,
(1971). Effects of contingent and non-contin- 276-302.
gent reward on the relationship between satis- Douglas, E. F, McDaniel, M. A., & Snell, A. F.
faction and task performance. Journal of Ap- (1996, August). The validity of non-cognitive
plied Psychology, 55, 531-536. measures decays when applicantsfake. Paper
Cordes, C. L., & Dougherty, T. W. (1993). A re- presented at the 1996 meeting of the Academy
view and integration of research on job burn- of Management, Cincinnati, OH.
out. Academy ofManagement Review, 75,621— Fisher, C. D. (1980). On the dubious wisdom of
656. expecting job satisfaction to correlate with per-
Costa, P. X, & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence formance. Academy of Management Review,
of extroversion and neuroticism on subjective 5, 607-612.
well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Jour- Garden, A.-H.(1991). Relationship between burn-
nal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 33, out and performance. Psychological Reports,
668-678. 68, 963-977.
Costa, P. X, & McCrae, R. R. (1988). From cata- George, J. M. (1991). State or trait: Effects of
log to classification: Murray's needs and the positive mood on prosocial behaviors at work.
Five-Factor model. Journal of Personality and Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 299-307.
Social Psychology, 55, 258-265. George, J. M. (1992). The role of personality in
Cropanzano, R., James, K., & Konovsky, M. A. organizational life: Issues and evidence. Jour-
(1993). Dispositional affectivity as a predic- nal of Management, 18, 185-210.
tor of work attitudes and job performance. George, J. M. (1996). Trait and state affect. In K.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 14,595- M. Murphy (Ed.), Individual differences in
606. behavior in organizations (pp. 145-171). San
Cropanzano, R., & Wright, X A. (1999). A five- Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
year study of change in the relationship be- Headey, B., & Wearing, A. (1988). The sense of
tween well-being and job performance. Con- relative superiority—Central to well-being.
sulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Social Indicators Research, 20, 497-516.
Research, 51, 252-265. Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., & Snyderman, B.
Dember, W. N., & Brooks, J. (1989). A new in- (1959). The motivation to work (2nd ed.). New
strument for measuring optimism and pessi- York: Wiley.
mism: Test-retest reliability and relations with Hobfoll, S. E. (1988). The ecology ofstress. New
happiness and religious commitment. Bulletin York: Hemisphere.
of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 365-366. Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources:
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1994). Tempera- A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Ameri-
ment and attention: Orienting toward and away can Psychologist, 44, 513-525.
from positive and negative signals. Journal of Hobfoll, S. E. (1998). Stress, culture, and com-
Personality and Social Psychology, 68,1128— munity: The psychology and philosophy of
1139. stress. New York: Plenum.
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psy- Hough, L. M., Eaton, N. K., Dunnette, M. D.,
chological Bulletin, 95, 542-575. Kamp, J. D., & McCloy, R. A. (1990). Crite-

Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research 195


Summer 2001
rion-related validities of personality constructs cedures. Personality and Individual Differ-
and the effect of response distortion on those ences, 10, 1221-1228.
validities. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 75, Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality
581-595. and susceptibility to positive and negative
Houser, J. D. (1927). What the employee thinks. emotional states. Journal of Personality and
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Social Psychology, 61, 132-140.
Iaffaldano, M. T., & Muchinsky, P. M. (1985). Lawler, E. E., & Porter, L. W. (1967). The ef-
Job satisfaction and job performance: A fects of performance on job satisfaction. In-
meta analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 97, dustrial Relations, 7, 20-28.
366-373. Lee, R. T., & Ashforth, B. E. (1996). A meta-
Jones, R. G., & Best, R. G. (1995, August). analytic examination of the correlates of the
Further examination of the nature and im- three dimensions of job burnout. Journal of
pact of emotional work requirements. Paper Applied Psychology, 81, 123-133.
presented at the annual meeting of the Acad- Levin, I., & Stokes, J. P. (1989). Dispositional
emy of Management, Vancouver, British approach to job satisfaction: Role of nega-
Columbia, Canada. tive affectivity. Journal of Applied Psychol-
Judge, T. A. (1992). The dispositional perspec- ogy, 74, 752-758.
tive in human resources management. In G. Locke, L. A. (1976). The nature and causes of
R. Ferris & K. M. Rowland (Eds.), Research job satisfaction. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.),
in personnel and human resources manage- Handbook of industrial and organizational
ment (Vol. 10, pp. 31-72). Greenwich, CT: psychology (1st ed., pp. 1297-1349). Chi-
JAI Press. cago, IL: Rand McNally.
Judge, T. A., Hanisch, K. A., & Drankoski, R. D. Maslach, C. (1982). Burnout: The cost of car-
(1995). Human resources management and ing. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
employee attitudes. In G. R. Ferris, S. D. Rosen, Maslach, C , & Jackson, S. E. (1986). Maslach
& D. T. Barnum (Eds.), Handbook of human Burnout Inventory (2nd ed.). Palo Alto, CA:
resources management (pp. 574-596). Ox- Consulting Psychologists Press.
ford, England: Blackwell Publishers. Maslach, C , & Leiter, M. P. (1997). The truth
Judge, T. A., & Locke, E. A. (1993). Effects of about burnout: How organizations cause
dysfunctional thought processes on subjec- personnel stress and what to do about it. San
tive well-being and job satisfaction. Journal Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
of Applied Psychology, 78, 475-490. Mattox, W.R., Jr. (1999, April 8). The great
Judge, T. A., Locke, E. A., & Durham, C. C. (clinical) depression: Dow 10K shows
(1997). The dispositional causes of job sat- money can't buy happiness. San Francisco
isfaction: A core evaluations approach. In Chronicle, p. A23.
B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Re- Mayer, J. D., & Gaschke, Y. N. (1988). The ex-
search in Organizational Behavior (Vol. 17, perience and meta-experience of mood. Jour-
pp. 151-188). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55,
Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Bono, J. E., & Patton, 102-111.
G. K. (1998, August). Thejob satisfaction-job McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T, Jr. (1987). Valida-
performance relationship: 1939-1998. Paper tion of the five-factor model of personality
presented at the annual meeting of the Acad- across instruments and observers. Journal of
emy of Management, San Diego, CA. Personality and Social Psychology, 52,81-90.
Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1982). Job condi- Michalos, A. C. (1985). Multiple discrepancies
tions and personality: A longitudinal assess- theory (MDT). Social Indicators Research,
ment. American Journal ofSociology, 87,1257- 16, 347^13.
1286. Meyer, G. J., & Shack, J. R. (1989). Structural
Landy, F. W. (1985). The psychology of work be- convergence of mood and personality: Evidence
havior (3rd ed.). Homewood, IL: Dorsey for old and new directions. The Journal ofPer-
Press. sonality and Social Psychology, 57, 691-706.
Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1992). Promises and Myers, D. G. (1993). The pursuit of happiness.
problems with the circumplex model of emo- New York: Avon Books.
tion. Review of Personality and Social Psy- Myers, D. G., & Diener, E. (1995). Who is
chology, 13, 25-59. happy? Psychological Science, 6, 10-19.
Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1989). Extra- Myers, D. G., & Diener, E. (1997). The new
version, neuroticism, and susceptibility to pursuit of happiness. The Harvard Medical
positive and negative mood induction pro- Health Letter, 14, 4-7.

196 Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research


Summer 2001
Nord, W. R. (1976). Attitudes and performance. In Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of af-
W. R. Nord (Ed.), Concepts and controversies fect. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
in organizational behavior (pp. 518-525). chology, 39, 1161-1178.
Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. Russell, J. A., Lewicka, M., & Nitt, T. (1989). A
Ones, D. S., Mount, M. K., Barrick, M. R., & cross-cultural study of a circumplex model
Hunter, J. E. (1994). Personality and job per- of affect. Journal of Personality and Social
formance: A critique of Tett, Jackson, & Psychology, 57, 848-856.
Rothstein (1994) meta-analysis. Personnel Russell, J. A., & Ridgeway, D. (1983). Dimen-
Psychology, 47, 147-171. sions underlying children' emotion concepts.
Parducci, A. (1995). Happiness, pleasure, and Developmental Psychology, 19, 795-804.
judgment: The contextual theory and its ap- Russell, J. A., Weiss, A., & Mendelsohn, G. A.
plications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (1989). Affect Grid: A single-item scale of
Pavot, W., Diener, E., Colvin, C. R., & pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality
Sandvik, E. (1991). Further validation of and Social Psychology, 57, 493-502.
the Satisfaction With Life Scale: Evidence Rusting, C. G., & Larsen, R. J. (1997). Extra-
for cross-method convergence of well- version, neuroticism, and susceptibility to
being measures. Journal of Personality positive and negative affect: A test of two
Assessment, 57, 149-161. theoretical models. Personality and Indi-
Petty, M. M., McGee, G. W., & Cavender, J. W. vidual Differences, 22, 607-612.
(1984). A meta-analysis of the relationship Salgado, J. F. (1997). The five factor model of
between individual job satisfaction and in- personality and job performance in the Eu-
dividual performance. Academy of Manage- ropean Community. Journal of Applied Psy-
ment Review, 9, 712-721. chology, 82, 30-43.
Podsakoff, P. M., & Williams, L. J. (1986). Sandvik, E., Diener, E., & Seidlitz, L. (1993).
The relationship between job performance Subjective well-being: The convergence and sta-
and job satisfaction. In E. A. Locke (Ed.), bility of self-report measures. Journal ofPer-
Generalizing from laboratory to field stud- sonality, 61, 317-342.
ies (pp. 207-245). Lexington, MA: Lex- Schwab, D. P., & Cummings, L. L. (1970). Theo-
ington Books. ries of performance and satisfaction: A re-
Quattrochi-Tubin, S., Jones, J. W, & Breedlove, view. Industrial Relations, 9, 408^30.
V (1982). The burnout syndrome in geriat- Seidlitz, L., & Diener, E. (1993). Memory for
ric counselors and service workers. Activi- positive versus negative events: Theories for
ties, Adaptation, and Aging, 3, 65-76. the differences between happy and unhappy
Robbins, T. L., & DeNisi, A. S. (1994). A closer persons. Journal of Personality and Social
look at interpersonal affect as a distinct influ- Psychology, 64, 654-664.
ence on cognitive processing in performance Seidlitz, L., Wyer, R. S., & Diener, E. (1997).
evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, Cognitive correlates of subjective well-being:
341-453. The processing of valanced life events by happy
Roethlisberger, F. J., & Dickson, W. J. (1939). and unhappy persons. Journal of Research in
Management and the worker. Cambridge, Personality, 31,240-256.
MA: Harvard University Press. Seligman, M. E. P. (1991). Learned optimism.
Rosse, J. G., Stecher, M. D., Miller, J. L., & Levin, New York: Random House.
R. A. (1998). The impact of response distortion Seligman, M. E. P. (1994). What you can change
on preemployment personality testing and hir- and what you can't. New York: Knopf.
ing decisions. Journal of Applied Psychology, Seligman, M. E. P. (1995). The effectiveness of
83, 634-644. psychotherapy: The Consumer's Report study.
Ruack, J. K. (1999, February 12). Redefining American Psychologist, 50, 965-974.
the good life: A new focus in the social sci- Shirom, A. (1989). Burnout in work organizations.
ences. The Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. In C. L. Cooper & I. Robertson (Eds.), Inter-
A13-A15. national review of industrial and organiza-
Russell, J. A. (1978). Evidence of convergent va- tional psychology (pp. 25—48). New York:
lidity on the dimensions of affect. Jour- Wiley.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, Staw, B. M. (1986). Organizational psychology
36, 1152-1168. and the pursuit of the happy/productive
Russell, J. A. (1979). Affective space is bipolar. worker. California Management Review,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- XXVIII(4), 40-53.
ogy, 37, 345-356. Staw, B. M., & Barsade, S. G. (1993). Affect and

Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research 197


Summer 2001
managerial performance: A test of the sadder- Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W (1989). Health
but-wiser vs. happier-and-smarter hypotheses. complaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the
Administrative Science Quarterly, 38,304—331. central role of negative affectivity. Psycho-
Staw, B. M., Sutton, R. I., & Pelled, L. H. (1994). logical Review, 96, 234-254.
Employee positive emotion and favorable Watson, D., & Slack, A. K. (1993). General
outcomes at the workplace. Organization factors of affective temperament and their
Science, 5, 51-71. relation to job satisfaction over time. Orga-
Sullivan, J. J. (1989). Self theories and employee nizational Behavior and Human Decision
motivation. Journal of Management, 15, Processes, 54, 181-202.
345-363. Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward the
Tellegen, A. (1982). Brief manual for the Dif- structure of affect. Psychological Bulletin,
ferential Personality Questionnaire. Unpub- 98, 219-235.
lished manuscript, University of Minnesota. Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). An af-
Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., & Rothstein, M. fective events approach to job satisfaction.
(1991). Personality measures as predictors In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.),
of job performance: A meta-analytic review. Research in organizational behavior (Vol.
Personnel Psychology, 44, 703-742. 18, pp. 1-74). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Tett, R. P., Jackson, D. N., Rothstein, M., & Wright, T. A. (1999). An eight-step approach to
Reddon, J. R. (1994). Meta-analysis of per- enhance the application of organizational re-
sonality-job performance relations: A reply search. In L. Larwood & U. E. Gattiker (Eds.),
to Ones, Mount, Barrick, and Hunter (1994). Impact analysis—How research enters ap-
Personnel Psychology, 47, 157-172. plication and makes a difference (pp. 137—
Veenhoven, R. (1988). The utility of happiness. 143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Social Indicators Research, 20, 333-354. Wright, T. A., & Bonett, D. G. (1992). The effect of
Vroom, V H. (1964). Work and motivation. New turnover on work satisfaction and mental health:
York: Wiley. Support for a situational perspective. Journal
Warr, P. (1987). Work, employment, and mental of Organizational Behavior, 13, 603-615.
health. New York: Oxford University Press. Wright, T. A., & Bonett, D. G. (1997a). The contri-
Warr, P. (1990). The measurement of well- bution of burnout to work performance. Jour-
being and other aspects of mental health. nal of Organizational Behavior, 18,491^499.
Journal of Occupational Psychology, 63, Wright, T. A., & Bonett, D. G. (1997b). The
193-210. role of pleasantness and activation-based
Watson, D. (1988). Intraindividual and well-being in performance prediction.
interindividual analysis of positive and nega- Journal of Occupational Health Psychol-
tive affect: Their relation to health complaints, ogy, 2,212-219.
perceived stress, and daily activities. Jour- Wright, T. A., Bonett, D. G., & Sweeney, D. A.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, (1993). Mental health and work performance:
54, 1020-1030. Results of a longitudinal field study. Journal of
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative Occupational and Organizational Psychology,
affectivity: The disposition to experience 66, 277-284.
negative emotional states. Psychological Bulle- Wright, T. A., & Cropanzano, R. (1997). Well-
tin, 96,465^90. being, satisfaction and job performance:
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Mclntyre, C. W, & Another look at the happy/productive worker
Hamaker, S. (1992). Affect, personality, and relationship. In L. N. Dosier & J. B. Keys
social activity. Journal of Personality and So- (Eds.), Academy of management: Best pa-
cial Psychology, 63, 1011-1025. per proceedings (pp. 364-368). Statesboro,
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1984). GA: Office of Publications and Faculty Re-
Cross-cultural convergence in the structure search Services, College of Business Ad-
of mood: A Japanese replication and a com- ministration, Georgia Southern University.
parison with U.S. findings. Journal of Per- Wright, T. A., & Cropanzano, R. (1998). Emo-
sonality and Social Psychology, 47, 127- tional exhaustion as a predictor of job per-
144. formance and voluntary turnover. Journal
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). of Applied Psychology, 83, 486-493.
Development and validation of brief mea- Wright, T. A., & Cropanzano, R. (1999). When
sures of positive and negative affect: The a happy worker is a productive worker: A
PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and preliminary examination of three models.
Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070. Manuscript submitted for publication.

198 Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research


Summer 2001
Wright, T. A., & Cropanzano, R. (2000). The role emotional well-being. Journal of Organiza-
oforganizational behavior in occupational health tional Behavior, 79,481-485.
psychology: A view as we approach the mil- Wright, T. A., & Staw, B. M. (1999). Affect and
lennium. Journal of Occupational Health Psy- favorable work outcomes: Two longitudinal
chology, 5,5-10. tests of the happy-productive worker the-
Wright, T. A., & Doherty, E. M. (1998). Organi- sis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20,
zational behavior 'rediscovers' the role of 1-23.

Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research 199


Summer 2001

You might also like