You are on page 1of 23

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/258180120

A Heuristic for Developing Transdiagnostic Models of Psychopathology


Explaining Multifinality and Divergent Trajectories

Article  in  Perspectives on Psychological Science · November 2011


DOI: 10.1177/1745691611419672

CITATIONS READS
589 5,699

2 authors, including:

Edward R Watkins
University of Exeter
214 PUBLICATIONS   17,124 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

PREVENT View project

Rumination View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Edward R Watkins on 06 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Perspectives on Psychological
Science http://pps.sagepub.com/

A Heuristic for Developing Transdiagnostic Models of Psychopathology : Explaining Multifinality and


Divergent Trajectories
Susan Nolen-Hoeksema and Edward R. Watkins
Perspectives on Psychological Science 2011 6: 589
DOI: 10.1177/1745691611419672

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://pps.sagepub.com/content/6/6/589

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Association For Psychological Science

Additional services and information for Perspectives on Psychological Science can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://pps.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://pps.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


Perspectives on Psychological Science

A Heuristic for Developing Transdiagnostic 6(6) 589­–609


© The Author(s) 2011
Reprints and permission:
Models of Psychopathology: Explaining sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1745691611419672

Multifinality and Divergent Trajectories http://pps.sagepub.com

Susan Nolen-Hoeksema1 and Edward R. Watkins2


1
Yale University and 2University of Exeter

Abstract
Transdiagnostic models of psychopathology are increasingly prominent because they focus on fundamental processes underlying
multiple disorders, help to explain comorbidity among disorders, and may lead to more effective assessment and treatment
of disorders. Current transdiagnostic models, however, have difficulty simultaneously explaining the mechanisms by which a
transdiagnostic risk factor leads to multiple disorders (i.e., multifinality) and why one individual with a particular transdiagnostic
risk factor develops one set of symptoms while another with the same transdiagnostic risk factor develops another set of
symptoms (i.e., divergent trajectories). In this article, we propose a heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models that can guide
theorists in explicating how a transdiagnostic risk factor results in both multifinality and divergent trajectories. We also (a)
describe different levels of transdiagnostic factors and their relative theoretical and clinical usefulness, (b) suggest the types
of mechanisms by which factors at 1 level may be related to factors at other levels, and (c) suggest the types of moderating
factors that may determine whether a transdiagnostic factor leads to certain specific disorders or symptoms and not others.
We illustrate this heuristic using research on rumination, a process for which there is evidence it is a transdiagnostic risk factor.

Keywords
transdiagnostic, multifinality, trajectories, comorbidity, psychopathology

Transdiagnostic models of psychopathology, which seek to presentations or disorders and particular symptom presentations
identify fundamental processes underlying multiple, usually or disorders in particular people. We also (a) specify different
comorbid, psychopathologies, are increasingly prominent levels of transdiagnostic factors and their relative theoretical
(Barlow, Allen, & Choate, 2004; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; and clinical usefulness, (b) suggest the types of mechanisms by
Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; Kring & Sloan, which factors at one level may be related to factors at other lev-
2010; Mansell, Harvey, Watkins, & Shafran, 2009). Transdi- els, and (c) suggest the types of moderating factors that may
agnostic models hold several theoretical and clinical advan- determine whether a transdiagnostic factor leads to certain spe-
tages over disorder-specific models (e.g., see Mansell et al., cific disorders or symptoms and not others. We then apply this
2009). Current transdiagnostic models, however, have diffi- heuristic to develop a transdiagnostic model of rumination, a
culty simultaneously explaining the mechanisms by which a cognitive emotion-regulation process that has been linked to
transdiagnostic risk factor leads to multiple disorders (i.e., multiple psychopathologies (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Sch-
multifinality) and why one individual with a particular transdi- weizer, 2010; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Watkins, 2011).
agnostic risk factor develops one set of symptoms while
another with the same transdiagnostic risk factor develops
another set of symptoms (i.e., divergent trajectories). For Existing Transdiagnostic Models: Advantages
example, current models of stress as a risk factor (e.g., Meyer, and Disadvantages
2003; Monroe, 2008) tend not to address how stress both is In the early and mid-20th century, psychology was dominated
related to many different disorders, including depression, anx- by broad models that described processes thought to underlie
iety, and alcohol abuse (i.e., multifinality), and leads to depres- most forms of psychopathology. Processes such as repression
sion in some people, anxiety in others, and alcohol abuse in
others (i.e., divergent trajectories).
Corresponding Author:
In this article, we propose a heuristic for developing transdi-
Susan Nolen-Hoeksema, Department of Psychology,Yale University, P.O. Box
agnostic models that can guide theorists in explicating how a 208205, New Haven, CT 06520
transdiagnostic risk factor results in both multiple symptom E-mail: susan.nolen-hoeksema@yale.edu

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


590 Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins

and defense mechanisms (Freud, 1937/1966), on the one hand, Third, if a transdiagnostic factor is causally related to
and operant and classical conditioning (Mowrer, 1939; Skin- two or more disorders, then assessment and training could
ner, 1957; Wolpe, 1969), on the other, were thought to account more parsimoniously focus on these factors than on the large
for disorders ranging from phobias and depression to number of discrete disorders currently in the DSM (Mansell
schizophrenia. et al., 2009). Moreover, treatments that remediate a transdi-
Then, particularly with the introduction in 1980 of the third agnostic factor could have effects on all the disorders it is
edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental related to.
Disorders (DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association, 1980), The concept of transdiagnostic processes has similarities
there was a shift toward distinguishing between disorders both to the concepts of intermediate phenotypes and endopheno-
in their characteristics and causes. The authors of the DSM-III types in psychiatric research (Cannon & Keller, 2006; Insel
rejected old classification systems based on the assumption & Cuthbert, 2009). Whereas mental disorders are heteroge-
that certain neuroses underpinned broadly defined disorders neous clusters of symptoms, intermediate phenotypes are
and carved psychopathology into a much larger number of conceived as neurocognitive and affective processes, such as
individual disorders, with attempts to define these disorders by learning, memory, attention, stress sensitivity, and emotional
specific, observable behaviors when possible. Many research- reactivity that are causally linked to the development of
ers then turned their attention to identifying risk factors and symptoms (Cannon & Keller, 2006). The recent Research
causes specific to a given disorder, and the specificity of a Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative is specifically focused on
putative risk or etiological factor became an important stan- intermediate phenotypes that can be linked to problems of
dard for its acceptance. neural circuitry and functioning, such as excessive fear, dis-
Now, in the early 21st century, the focus has shifted again ruptions in working memory, and poor impulse control (Insel
toward processes that may play a causal role in multiple disor- & Cuthbert, 2009; Sanislow et al., 2010). Endophenotypes
ders, often referred to as transdiagnostic processes (Barlow et are intermediate phenotypes that are heritable (Gottesman &
al., 2004; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Harvey et al., 2004; Kring & Gould, 2003).
Sloan, 2010; Mansell et al., 2009). Approaches focused on Psychologists have focused on a broader array of cognitive,
transdiagnostic processes have a number of theoretical and clin- behavioral, and emotional processes thought to underlie
ical advantages over disorder-specific approaches. First, there is comorbid disorders. Indeed, developmental psychopatholo-
growing agreement that the heterogeneous disorders in our cur- gists highlighted the phenomenon of multifinality decades ago
rent diagnostic system are each made up of dysfunctional ver- (Cicchetti, 1984; Egeland, Pianta, & Ogawa, 1996). Multifi-
sions of processes that vary along continua in the general nality refers to the process by which certain environments,
population (Cannon & Keller, 2006; Insel et al., 2010; Regier, experiences, or characteristics increase risk for a number of
Narrow, Kuhl, & Kupfer, 2009). Moreover, these dysfunctional different types of psychopathology in children or adults—for
processes are seen in a number of different disorders. For exam- example, stress is associated with multiple disorders (Cic-
ple, an attentional bias toward negative information is a com- chetti & Rogosch, 1996).
mon finding in healthy samples, and extreme attentional bias is The list of putative transdiagnostic processes or risk factors
seen in patients with a variety of psychological disorders (e.g., is quite long (e.g., see D. A. Clark & Taylor, 2009; Dudley,
Rozin & Royzman, 2001). A transdiagnostic approach focuses Kuyken, & Padesky, 2010; Mansell et al., 2009). There are a
on these fundamental dysfunctional processes and thus brings number of environments or experiences that have been reli-
us closer to understanding the true nature of psychopathology in ably linked to many different psychopathologies, including
a more parsimonious way. sexual and physical abuse, particularly in childhood (Mani-
Second, transdiagnostic approaches can help us understand glio, 2009); inconsistent, harsh, or neglectful parenting
the comorbidity between disorders. Over half of individuals (Dozois, Seeds, & Collins, 2009); and parental psychopathol-
with one diagnosis meet criteria for at least one other diagno- ogy (Avenevoli & Merikangas, 2006; Beidel & Turner, 1997;
sis (Kessler, Chiu, Demler, & Walters, 2005). In addition, the Johnson, Cohen, Kasen, & Brook, 2006). Then there are sev-
stability of diagnoses within individuals is low; instead, over eral intrapersonal factors that are related to a range of psycho-
time individuals may meet criteria for a variety of different pathologies. Harvey and colleagues (2004) identified a number
disorders (Forrester, Owens, & Johnstone, 2001). Certain dis- of cognitive processes reliably associated with multiple disor-
orders cluster together reliably, however, suggesting that there ders, including selective attention and memory, recurrent
are commonalities within groups of disorders (Krueger & memories, overgeneral memory, interpretation and expectancy
Markon, 2006; Watson, 2009). For example, major depression biases, emotional reasoning, recurrent negative thinking such
is highly comorbid with particular anxiety disorders, such as as worry and rumination, certain metacognitive beliefs,
generalized anxiety disorder and social anxiety (T. A. Brown thought suppression, attentional avoidance, and safety behav-
& Barlow, 2009). Transdiagnostic approaches hold promise in iors. Executive control deficits, particularly in working mem-
explaining comorbidities such as these by focusing on pro- ory, cognitive and behavioral inhibitory processes, and
cesses that are common to comorbid disorders and that caus- attentional control, have been associated with both emotional
ally contribute to symptoms. and behavioral disorders and some psychotic disorders (Barch,

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


Transdiagnostic Processes 591

2005; Gotlib & Joormann, 2010; Mathews & MacLeod, 2005). 2004; Mansell et al., 2009). However, this account leaves open
Two personality characteristics that have been associated with the question of where the current concerns come from.
many types of psychopathology are neuroticism and negative Similarly, transdiagnostic processes generally have not
affectivity (Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt, & Watson, 2010). Diffi- addressed phenotypic plasticity, wherein the pattern of symp-
culties in regulating emotion have also been identified as toms morphs across the life course, often unpredictably, espe-
transdiagnostic factors (Aldao et al., 2010; Kring & Sloan, cially during the transitions through childhood and adolescence
2010). into adulthood (e.g., childhood anxiety frequently being a pre-
Psychobiological factors may also serve as intermediate cursor for depression in adulthood; Pine, Cohen, Gurley,
transdiagnostic factors (Cannon & Keller, 2006; Sanislow Brook, & Ma, 1998). Thus, a key question is why the same
et al., 2010). A dysregulated stress response is evident in sev- transdiagnostic risk factor results in divergent trajectories of
eral emotional and behavioral disorders (Cicchetti & Toth, symptom expression across individuals or across time.
2005). Similarly, Harvey and colleagues (Harvey, Murray, In addition, more work is needed to specify the mecha-
Chandler, & Soehner, 2010) have shown that sleep distur- nisms linking some more distal transdiagnostic factors to the
bances are common across a wide range of disorders and pre- disorders they predict. For example, childhood sexual abuse is
dict onset and relapse of psychopathology. a global risk factor for mood disorders, anxiety disorders, eat-
Several transdiagnostic therapies have been proposed based ing disorders, sexual disorders, dissociative disorders, sub-
on the evidence just reviewed (see Mansell et al., 2009). Bar- stance use disorders, and most personality disorders (Maniglio,
low and colleagues (2004) designed a transdiagnostic cogni- 2009). In order for childhood sexual abuse to be theoretically
tive behavioral therapy to address faulty appraisals, avoidance, useful as a transdiagnostic factor, it is important to know how
and maladaptive action tendencies (e.g., withdrawal) associ- it could be linked to so many disorders. Some theorists have
ated with a wide range of emotional disorders. Similarly, suggested mediators that link childhood sexual abuse to spe-
Hayes et al. (2004) argued that experiential avoidance is a cific disorders, such as problems in attachment, poor emotion
common causal factor across many disorders and developed regulation, and hypervigilance for threat (e.g., Roche, Runtz,
treatments to overcome such avoidance. Fairburn, Cooper, and & Hunter, 1999; Sarin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010; Spasojevic
Shafran (2003) introduced a transdiagnostic therapy for vari- & Alloy, 2002). In general, little work has been done to deter-
ous eating disorders focused on concerns over weight and mine how distal transdiagnostic factors lead to intermediate
shape and related behavioral tendencies. Therapies to address transdiagnostic factors, which then lead to a group of disor-
deficits in emotion regulation are also emerging (Greenberg, ders, and, in turn, how other factors come into play to deter-
2002; Mennin & Fresco, 2010; Roemer, Erisman, & Orsillo, mine which specific cluster of disorders or symptoms
2009). Finally, given evidence that sleep disturbances occur individuals will manifest.
across many disorders, Harvey and colleagues (2010) have Thus, in summary, for transdiagnostic models to be of theo-
proposed cognitive and behavioral interventions for these retical and practical value and for them to have meaningful
disturbances. explanatory power, they need to be able to explain (a) the
Several limitations exist in current transdiagnostic models, mechanisms by which a transdiagnostic factor causes all the
however. First, transdiagnostic models generally do a better different disorders it is associated with (i.e., the mechanisms
job explaining multifinality than divergent trajectories of underpinning multifinality), and (b) why a given transdiagnos-
symptom expression across individuals or across time. For tic factor leads to different disorders in different people or to
example, why is it that one person with attentional biases different disorders within the same person over time (i.e.,
develops obsessive–compulsive disorder and another devel- divergent trajectories). Transdiagnostic models also need to
ops primary insomnia? Or why is it that an individual who be able to explain the mechanisms linking distal and more
carries a risk factor that leads him to experience both sub- proximal risk factors to each other and to psychopathologies.
stance abuse and depression is depressed but not abusing some We offer herein a heuristic for the development of transdi-
of the time and abusing but not depressed at other times? Har- agnostic models, with a particular focus on guidelines for
vey et al. (2004) suggested that cognitive behavioral processes identifying factors explaining multifinality and divergent tra-
could be common across disorders and still produce disorder- jectories. We (a) specify different levels of transdiagnostic
specific presentations because the exact clinical presentation factors and their relative theoretical and clinical usefulness,
would depend upon the interaction between processes and cur- (b) suggest the types of mechanisms by which factors at one
rent concerns (Klinger, 1996), defined as a nonconscious pro- level may be related to factors at other levels, and (c) suggest
cessing state initiated when a person commits to pursue a the types of moderating factors that may determine whether a
specific goal. Current concerns sensitize emotional responses transdiagnostic factor leads to certain specific disorders or
to, and cognitive processing of, cues associated with that goal. symptoms and not others. We then apply this heuristic to
Thus, a person with an attentional bias may develop obses- develop a transdiagnostic model of rumination, a risk factor
sive–compulsive disorder if her concerns focus on germs and for multiple disorders that has been relatively well researched
contamination, or she may develop primary insomnia if her (see Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, &
concerns focus on difficulties with sleeping (Harvey et al., Lyubomirsky, 2008).

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


592 Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins

A Heuristic for Developing Transdiagnostic transdiagnostic factors can be organized into those that are
Models of Psychopathology more distal to psychopathology (setting conditions with a
number of causal mechanisms intervening between the con-
Our heuristic for developing models of transdiagnostic pro- ditions and the psychopathology) and those that are more
cesses, illustrated in Figure 1, organizes variables that have proximal (processes with relatively few causal mechanisms
been proposed to be transdiagnostic and suggests what intervening between these process and the psychopathol-
needs to be specified about transdiagnostic processes to ogy). We suggest the types of mechanisms that may link the
illuminate how they work to create a general risk for psy- distal transdiagnostic risk factors with the more proximal
chopathology (multifinality) and a specific risk for certain transdiagnostic risk factors. We further suggest the types of
disorders (divergent trajectories). In brief, we argue that mechanisms that may link proximal transdiagnostic risk

Distal Risk Factors


- Environmental context
- Congenital biological
abnormalities

Possible mechanisms
- Shape responses to environment
- Shape negative beliefs/self-image
- Condition specific behaviors

Proximal Risk Factors


- Biological factors leading to emotional,
cognitive, or behavioral tendencies
- Basic cognitive factors
- Stable psychological individual difference
factors

Moderators
- Conditions that raise concerns
or themes
- Conditions that shape
responses
- Conditions that change the
reward value of stimuli

Disorder A Disorder B Disorder C

Fig. 1.  A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology.


Distal risk factors contribute to disorders only through mediating proximal risk factors. Proximal risk factors
directly influence symptoms relative to distal risk factors. Distal and proximal risk factors lead to multiple disorders
(i.e., multifinality), which are comorbid (indicated by dashed lines). Moderators interact with proximal risk factors to
determine which specific disorder individuals will experience (i.e., to determine divergent trajectories).

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


Transdiagnostic Processes 593

factors to psychopathology. We argue that identifying the Table 1.  Primary Characteristics of Components of the Heuristic
mechanisms linking distal risk factors to proximal risk fac-
tors and proximal risk factors to psychopathology is critical Component Characteristics
to a useful understanding of transdiagnostic processes. Finally, Distal risk Environmental context factors or congenital bio-
we suggest two types of moderators that lead individuals with factors logical characteristics that cause psychopathol-
transdiagnostic risk factors to develop specific types of ogy only via mediating proximal risk factors
symptoms. Tend to be distant in time from psychopatholo-
gies they predict but are not always temporally
distant
Distal and proximal transdiagnostic risk factors Tend to be difficult to control or modify
Proximal risk Within-person variables that mediate the
The variables that have been argued to be transdiagnostic risk
factors relationships between distal risk factors and
factors vary in their causal distance to the psychopathology symptoms
they predict. We can organize them roughly into two catego- Directly influence symptoms relative to distal
ries: (a) environmental and congenital biological variables risk factors
relatively distal to observable symptoms of disorders and (b) Follow distal risk factors in time; precede psy-
within-person variables that are more proximal to symptoms chopathology in time
of disorders. The critical distinction between distal and proxi- Tend to be more modifiable or controllable than
distal risk factors
mal risk factors is in how close the causal connection is
Moderators Environmental context factors or biological
between the risk factor and psychopathology; distal risk fac-
characteristics that
tors lead to psychopathology only via mediating proximal risk (a)  raise concerns or themes that the proximal
factors, whereas proximal risk factors more directly cause risk factors then act upon,
psychopathology. (b)  shape responses through conditioning, or
(c)  determine the reinforcement value of
Distal transdiagnostic factors. The distal transdiagnostic certain stimuli (i.e., both how rewarding
risk factors predict many disorders but it seems likely that a certain stimuli are and how aversive they
are and thus how much negative reinforce-
number of processes at various levels intervene between these
ment value occurs at their removal)
distal risk factors and the psychopathologies they predict. Closer in time and causal effect to outcomes
Thus, within this operationalization, a distal risk factor does than are distal risk factors; are concurrent
not directly cause symptoms but only influences symptoms with, or follow, proximal risk factors in time;
via mediating proximal factors (see Table 1). As such, a distal precede psychopathology in time
risk factor is distant from the expression of symptoms both in Interact with proximal risk factors to produce
probability and mechanism. It is important to note that expo- specific disorders or symptom expressions
sure to a distal risk factor does not necessarily mean that sub-
sequent psychopathology will follow.
We conceive of distal transdiagnostic risk factors as experi- experience of severe independent stressors, particularly during
ences or characteristics that are usually independent of childhood, is a clear risk factor for a large number of mental
any actions of the individual—basically they “happen” to an disorders (Monroe, 2008), but several processes must inter-
individual—and they set the stage for proximal risk factors (or vene to determine whether an individual who experienced
mediators) that more directly lead to psychopathology. This adversity as a child develops any type of psychopathology
characterization draws attention to the independent conditions and, if so, what specific symptoms he or she will develop.
that may create more proximal vulnerability factors; it also The second category of distal risk factors consists of con-
distinguishes between “setting conditions” that are less modi- genital biological abnormalities, principally genetic abnormal-
fiable and factors that may be more modifiable. However, the ities and congenital abnormalities in brain function or structure
nonmodifiability of a risk factor is not the critical characteris- (caused by genetic abnormalities or early brain injuries). These
tic making it a distal risk factor; instead the critical character- biological characteristics shape the individual’s development
istic of distal risk factors is that they influence symptoms only from birth, but multiple processes can intervene to determine
via mediating proximal factors. whether individuals born with these characteristics go on to
We suggest that it is useful to confine distal risk factors to develop psychopathology. As an example, abnormalities in
two categories. The first is environmental context factors, such certain genes, including G72/G30, the brain-derived neuro-
as parental psychopathology, a history of sexual or physical trophic factor (BDNF) gene, DISC1, COMT, neuregulin1, and
abuse, and other traumatic events. These are what Hammen dysbindin, seem to confer risk for both schizophrenia and
(1991, 2005) labels independent stressors because they are bipolar disorder (Hill, Harris, Herbener, Pavuluri, & Sweeney,
not caused by the individual experiencing them (e.g., the death 2008), although only a subset of individuals with these abnor-
of a spouse), and dependent stressors, which the individual may malities develop psychopathology. These genes do not have a
have played a role in creating (e.g., interpersonal conflict). The direct effect on symptoms of schizophrenia or bipolar disorder

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


594 Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins

but instead appear linked to deficits in neurocognitive factors such as attentional biases or working memory deficits, which
such as attention regulation, working memory, episodic mem- are consistently and strongly linked to several psychopatholo-
ory, and emotion processing (which we view as more proximal gies (for comprehensive reviews of this large literature, see
risk factors), that may lead to the development of psychotic Harvey et al., 2004; Mathews & MacLeod, 2005). Again, we
symptoms (Barch, 2005; Hill et al., 2008). place these in the proximal risk factors class because they have
Note that in most of the examples given here, the distal risk been linked through specific mechanisms to psychopathology
factors are not only causally distant from the psychopathology (e.g., working memory deficits contribute to symptoms of
they produce but temporally distant as well (e.g., childhood schizophrenia; Barch, 2005) but probably result from more
abuse contributing to adult psychopathology). Distal risk fac- distal factors such as genetic polymorphisms or early life
tors need not always be temporally distant from the symptoms experiences (e.g., MacLeod, 1999).
they produce. Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or acute A third category of proximal transdiagnostic risk factors is
stress disorder can result soon after the experience of a major stable psychological individual difference factors reflecting
trauma (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). In both the tendency to adopt particular styles of responding to situa-
cases, we would consider the trauma a distal risk factor tions, such as a pessimistic attributional style, emotion regula-
because it sets the conditions for the development of symp- tion tendencies like chronic suppression, or personality
toms, but not everyone who experiences the trauma will characteristics such as neuroticism or negative affectivity.
develop PTSD or acute stress disorder, and a number of proxi- Again, these individual differences have been directly linked
mal risk factors appear to intervene to determine who will to psychopathology—for example, greater negative emotional
develop symptoms (Keane, Marshall, & Taft, 2006). reactivity (neuroticism) is associated with increased negative
emotional states and depression (e.g., Boyce, Parker, Barnett,
Proximal transdiagnostic factors. We define proximal Cooney, & Smith, 1991; Kotov et al., 2010; Roberts & Kendler,
transdiagnostic risk factors as within-person variables that 1999). The mechanisms linking some individual difference fac-
have been linked more directly to disorders via specific mech- tors to psychopathology have been extensively studied; in sev-
anisms; these are akin to what many theorists have dubbed eral cases, these mechanisms involve other proximal risk
intermediate phenotypes (Cannon & Keller, 2006; Insel & factors. For example, the personality construct of neuroticism is
Cuthbert, 2009; Sanislow et al., 2010). Thus, proximal risk associated with a number of even more proximal risk factors,
factors are operationalized as those intrapersonal risk factors including increased attention toward threat (Rusting, 1998),
that (a) directly precede symptoms (relative to distal risk fac- increased accessibility of negative information (Martin, Ward,
tors), and/or (b) directly influence symptoms. Proximal risk & Clark, 1983), and increased amygdala reactivity to negative
factors are more often modifiable than distal risk factors, but stimuli (Hariri, Tessitore, Mattay, Fera, & Weinberger, 2002),
being modifiable is not a criterion for proximal risk factors. which in turn are associated with symptoms of depression and
We suggest that proximal risk factors fall into three catego- anxiety (Charbonneau, Mezulis, & Hyde, 2009).
ries, which can be interrelated. The first category is biological Although there is the possibility of causal relationships
factors leading to potentially maladaptive emotional, cogni- among proximal risk factors, as just described, we group them
tive, or behavioral tendencies. Some examples include hyper- together because they share the characteristics of relative
activity in the amygdala associated with excessive emotional closeness to psychopathology (compared with distal risk fac-
reactivity (Davidson, Pizzagalli, & Nitschke, 2009), dysfunc- tors) and causal mechanisms linking them to psychopathology.
tion in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis associated with Future models of the transdiagnostic factors leading to spe-
a dysregulated stress response (Belmaker & Agam, 2008), or cific disorders may delineate a larger number of levels of risk
hypoactivity in the prefrontal cortex that impairs impulse con- factors than our distal and proximal levels; we believe our
trol and emotion regulation (Davidson et al., 2009; Potenza et heuristic is a good starting point for the development of mod-
al., 2003). These biological factors are more proximal to psy- els with more levels.
chopathology than those we have categorized as distal factors
because they can lead more directly to symptoms (e.g., each of Mechanisms linking distal and proximal transdiagnostic
the conditions just listed have been linked to depressive and risk factors. Distal risk factors may lead to proximal risk fac-
anxiety reactions to events; see Davidson et al., 2009; Konar- tors via at least three mechanisms or processes. First, distal risk
ski et al., 2008; Southwick, Vythilingam, & Charney, 2005). factors may shape responses to the environment. For example,
Also, although some of these biological characteristics may be MacLeod (1999) has argued that prolonged exposure to an
due to congenital factors such as genetic abnormalities, they extended stressor, such as an abusive or neglectful environ-
may also result from experiences; for example, some individu- ment, may lead to attentional biases to negative stimuli, because
als with a childhood history of neglect or abuse develop a dys- under such circumstances it may be temporarily adaptive to
regulated stress response (Cicchetti & Toth, 2005; Heim & attend to threat. Attending to threat would be instrumentally
Nemeroff, 2001). reinforced, increasing the tendency to attend to threat in future
The second category of proximal transdiagnostic risk factors situations. Consistent with this analysis, experimental exposure
is basic cognitive deficits or biases in information processing, to contingencies that make it adaptive to attend toward

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


Transdiagnostic Processes 595

threatening stimuli, or prolonged exposure to mild stressors, likely to develop depression than anxiety or another disorder
can induce attentional bias toward threat (Clarke, MacLeod, & because loss is associated with sadness and depression (G. W.
Shirazee, 2008; MacLeod, Rutherford, Campbell, Ebsworthy, Brown & Harris, 1986); again their emotional reactivity and
& Holker, 2002). attentional bias will maintain their sad emotions and aware-
Congenital biological abnormalities may also shape indi- ness of their losses, potentially leading to depression. Thus,
viduals’ responses to the environment, creating proximal risk the nature of the environmental circumstances may temporar-
factors. Neuroticism and negative affectivity demonstrate a ily make more accessible certain current concerns relative to
heritable component (e.g., Smoller & Tsuang, 1998) and have others (Klinger, 1996), thereby influencing the specific ways
been linked to various genetic polymorphisms, most fre- that proximal risk factors may manifest in symptoms.
quently on the 5-HT promoter gene (e.g., for positive evi- A second category of moderators is conditions that operate
dence, see Munafo, Brown, & Hariri, 2008; for negative on proximal transdiagnostic risk factors to shape them into
evidence, see Willis-Owen et al., 2005). These genetic abnor- disorder-specific responses through modeling, observational
malities may lead to neuroticism or negative affectivity in part learning, and reinforcement. For example, a teenager who car-
by creating chronic neural hyperreactivity to emotional and ries the proximal risk factor of a tendency toward stimulation
threat stimuli (Munafo et al., 2008). seeking will be more likely to develop aggressive or antisocial
Second, distal factors could shape individuals’ beliefs, behavior if he is exposed to environments that model and rein-
schemas, and self-images to create proximal factors. For force aggression (Lynam et al., 2000). Alternatively, if that
example, chronic uncontrollable stress could lead a child to sensation-seeking teenager is raised in an environment that
develop the belief that she is helpless or deficient or to view does not model or reinforce aggression or antisocial behaviors
attachment figures as untrustworthy (Cicchetti & Toth, 2005). but instead shapes that sensation seeking toward prosocial
These beliefs and schemas could then influence the coping behaviors such as sports, he may be unlikely to develop anti-
strategies adopted by individuals as well as the current con- social aggression (Dishion & Patterson, 1997). Thus, these
cerns driving their cognitive processes. modeling, shaping, and reinforcing processes will influence
Third, classical and operant conditioning, modeling, and the nature of responses and cognitive processes that individu-
observational learning are likely to play roles in linking distal als learn to use in response to particular stressors or motiva-
and proximal risk factors. For example, parents with psycho- tions. Moreover, these processes will also influence the type of
pathology may not model or teach adaptive emotion regula- current concerns that become more chronically important and
tion to their children (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Eggum, 2010). As accessible for an individual (Klinger, 1996) and thereby influ-
another example, behaviors that reduce distress, such as an ence the specific ways that proximal risk factors may manifest
avoidant information processing style or emotion regulation in symptoms.
strategy, are more likely to occur and will have greater nega- The third group of moderators is biological or environmen-
tive reinforcing value in the context of distal risk factors such tal characteristics that change the sensitivity to and reinforce-
as ongoing difficult environmental context (Hayes et al., ment value of internal and external stimuli. It is important to
2004). Thus, those individuals exposed to distal risk factors note that changes in reinforcement value can occur for both
are more at risk for conditioning processes strengthening the processes of positive reinforcement, which refers to
proximal risk factors. responses that are followed by the presentation of a stimulus
that increases the chance of that behavior occurring (typically
Moderators of the effects of proximal risk factors. The onset of a pleasant stimulus), and negative reinforcement,
final major component of our heuristic is the moderators that which refers to the removal of a stimulus that increases the
determine what particular symptoms proximal transdiagnostic chance of the behavior occurring (typically removal of some-
risk factors will lead to in a given individual. Moderators cre- thing aversive). Thus, biological characteristics may influence
ate symptoms by (a) raising concerns or themes that proximal the extent to which particular appetitive stimuli such as food
risk factors then act upon, (b) shaping responses through con- or alcohol are rewarding and increase the reinforcement value
ditioning, or (c) determining the reinforcement value of cer- of the occurrence of these stimuli and/or the extent to which
tain stimuli. potentially unpleasant stimuli such as negative mood states are
The first category of moderators is conditions that raise cer- experienced as aversive, thereby increasing the reinforcement
tain themes or concerns that proximal factors then act upon. value of removing these stimuli (Kalivas & Volkow, 2005).
For example, individuals prone to neuroticism may be more For example, individuals vary in how rewarding they find
likely to develop an anxiety disorder than another disorder if alcohol, and heightened reward sensitivity predicts increased
their environment is chronically threatening because threat use and abuse of alcohol (Jorm et al., 1999; Newlin & Thom-
tends to produce the emotions of fear and anxiety (LeDoux, son, 1990; Pardo, Aguilar, Molinuevo, & Torrubia, 2007). A
2000); their tendency to be emotionally overreactive and to heightened sensitivity to the rewarding effects of alcohol may
attend to threat (Rusting, 1998) will then exacerbate and main- shape several of the proximal risk factors, such as emotion
tain their anxiety. In contrast, individuals prone to neuroticism dysregulation, into alcohol abuse (cf. Armeli, Carney, Tennen,
who experience a series of important losses may be more Affleck, & O’Neil, 2000; Cooper, Russel, & George, 1988).

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


596 Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins

That is, people whose emotions are dysregulated who have We note that there are similarities between moderators and
high reward sensitivity for alcohol will be more likely to turn distal risk factors; that is, both involve environmental and bio-
to alcohol to cope than people who are emotionally dysregu- logical factors that are typically independent of the individual’s
lated but have low reward sensitivity for alcohol. As noted ear- actions. Indeed, we argue that the same types of environmental
lier, environmental circumstances may also change the or biological factors could act as either distal risk factors for
reinforcement value of stimuli; for example, prolonged stress psychopathology or moderators of a proximal risk factor. For
may increase the negative reinforcement occurring to the example, chronically threatening environments could act as a
reduction of distress; hunger will increase the positive rein- distal risk factor that creates an attentional bias to threat. In
forcement occurring in response to consummation of food. individuals with attentional bias to threat, exposure to a chroni-
Likewise, individual differences in the capacity to with- cally threatening environment could also act as a moderator of
stand and tolerate negative emotional states may contribute to their attentional bias, leading to anxiety reactions.
the extent to which the individual engages in escape behaviors Critical distinctions between distal risk factors and modera-
such as drinking or substance abuse in response to heightened tors are (a) their temporality relative to proximal risk factors
emotional reactivity (Leyro, Zvolensky, & Bernstein, 2010; and psychopathology and (b) how they exert their causal
Zvolensky, Vujanovic, Bernstein, & Leyro, 2010). Biological effects. Distal risk factors precede proximal risk factors and
(and psychological) characteristics that increase the aversive psychopathology in time; moreover, distal risk factors are con-
experience of particular emotional states would increase the ceived as causes of proximal risk factors; proximal risk fac-
reinforcement value of avoiding distress and thereby increase tors, in turn, cause psychopathology (i.e., are mediators
engagement in behaviors like substance abuse and binge eat- between distal risk factors and psychopathology). Moderators
ing. In particular, lower perceived distress tolerance for nega- have their effects by interacting with and acting on existing
tive affect is concurrently and prospectively related to proximal risk factors and thus follow them in time or are con-
increased risk for substance use disorders, coping-oriented current with them; however, moderators precede and have
drug use, bulimic symptoms, and posttraumatic stress symp- causal effects on psychopathology. For example, a history of
toms (Vujanovic, Bernstein, & Litz, in press). childhood sexual abuse can act as a distal risk factor for psy-
Some interactive models of psychopathology focus on the chopathology by creating the proximal risk factor of a dys-
interaction between two or more factors in explaining a par- regulated stress response; an incident of abuse during
ticular disorder or symptom expression. For example, several adulthood can act as a moderator of this dysregulated stress
theorists have argued that the interaction of maladaptive per- response, creating an emotional disorder that develops shortly
sonality characteristics, such as low self-esteem, and the expe- after the adult abuse incident.
rience of stressors increases risk for depression (G. W. Brown, Some moderators may produce symptoms on their own
Andrews, Bifulco, & Veiel, 1990; Kendler, Kuhn, & Prescott, without the additional vulnerability created by the proximal
2004). Indeed, it is likely true that having more than one proxi- risk factors. For example, having parents who are excessively
mal transdiagnostic risk factor or a combination of proximal concerned about weight and do not model healthy eating hab-
and distal risk factors increases one’s vulnerability to a given its may be enough to lead a child to develop an eating disorder
psychopathology more than having only one proximal risk (e.g., Stein et al., 2006). Or having parents who model inap-
factor. We argue, however, that we gain more explanatory propriate use of alcohol to regulate emotions may be enough
power by confining moderators to factors that do not simply to lead a teenager to develop alcohol abuse (Zucker, Kincaid,
increase overall risk but specifically shape the vulnerability Fitzgerald, & Bingham, 1995). These moderators alone, how-
created by proximal transdiagnostic factors into particular ever, do not explain the frequent comorbidity between eating
responses or symptoms. disorders or alcohol abuse and other psychopathologies. The
Moreover, we have confined moderators to environmental value of our heuristic is that it guides researchers to explain
and biological factors so as to distinguish them from proximal both specificity and comorbidity and both multifinality and
risk factors and avoid problems that sometimes emerge in divergent trajectories. Proximal risk factors explain comorbid-
interactive models involving multiple proximal risk factors. In ity and multifinality, that is, the mechanisms by which one
some of these models, the proximal risk factors said to interact process leads to multiple comorbid disorders. Moderators
with each other overlap considerably, and one could be said to explain how individuals with that proximal risk factor develop
be a proxy or cause of the other. For example, Vohs et al. specific disorders—that is, their divergent trajectories.
(2001) suggested that disordered eating is especially likely to
result when body dissatisfaction is combined with perfection-
ism and low-self esteem. Low self-esteem may be the result of Applying the Heuristic: Rumination as a
body dissatisfaction, however, or body dissatisfaction may be Transdiagnostic Factor
the result of perfectionism. We suggest that by confining mod- To illustrate the value of our heuristic, we turn to the literature
erators to environmental and biological factors as we do, we on rumination, the tendency to repetitively analyze one’s prob-
reduce the potential for circularity and increase the potential lems, concerns, and feelings of distress without taking action
explanatory power of a transdiagnostic model. to make positive changes (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991; Watkins,

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


Transdiagnostic Processes 597

Distal Risk Factors


Environmental Context: Sexual and
Emotional Abuse; Overcontrolling,
Neglectful Parenting
Congenital Biological Abnormalities:
BDNF Polymorphism

Possible Mechanisms
Shape Responses to Environment: Abuse and
Overcontrolling Parenting Lead to Vigilance,
Dysregulated Stress Response, Helplessness; Genetic
Factors Lead to Impaired Inhibitory Control and
Attention to Negative Stimuli In Environment
Shape Negative Beliefs: Abuse and Overcontrolling
Parenting Lead to Views of Self as not Able to Cope
Condition Specific Behaviors: Abusive or
Overcontrolling Parents do not Teach Efficacious
Coping Responses

Proximal Risk Factors


Rumination

Moderators Moderators Moderators Moderators


Conditions Conditions Conditions Conditions
Raising Concerns Raising Changing Changing Reward
or Themes: Loss, Concerns or Reward Value Value of Stimuli:
Rejection, Failure Themes: of Stimuli: High Reward
Events Chronic High Reward Sensitivity to Food
Uncontrollable Sensitivity to
Stress (PTSD); Substances Conditions
Social Stress Shaping Responses:
(Social Anxiety Conditions Exposure to &
Disorder); Shaping Reinforcement of
Disposition to Responses: Thin Ideal, Weight
Panic Attacks Exposure to & Concerns; Social
(PD); Basal Reinforcement Punishments for
Ganglia of Substance Substance Use
Dysfunction Use
(obsessions)

Depression Anxiety Substance Bulimia


Misuse Nervosa

Fig. 2.  Using the heuristic to develop a transdiagnostic model of rumination.


Distal risk factors for rumination may include the environmental context variables of sexual and emotional abuse and overcontrolling
parenting and the congenital biological abnormality of certain polymorphisms in the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene.These may lead
to rumination by shaping vigilant, dysregulated, helpless, negatively focused responses to the environment; shaping negative beliefs about
one’s ability to cope; and failing to teach efficacious coping responses. Moderators may determine the divergent trajectories individuals
high on rumination take: conditions raising themes of loss lead to depression; conditions raising concerns about specific threats or about
panic symptoms or obsessions may lead to anxiety disorders; conditions leading to high reward sensitivity to substances or shaping use of
substances may lead to substance misuse; conditions leading to high reward sensitivity to food, shaping weight concerns, or preventing use
of substances may lead to eating disorders.

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


598 Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins

2008). A large literature (as reviewed in Ehring & Watkins, to new stressors and thus may fall into a ruminative pattern
2008) and a recent meta-analysis (Aldao et al., 2010) show (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2004b).
that rumination predicts a number of types of psychopathol- Spasojevic and Alloy (2002) also found that participants
ogy (i.e., it demonstrates multifinality). Briefly, rumination who reported that their parents were overcontrolling had
prospectively predicts both symptoms and diagnoses of major higher levels of rumination. Overcontrolling parenting could
depression (see meta-analyses by Aldao et al., 2010; Mor & foster rumination in children by undermining children’s sense
Winquist, 2002; Rood, Bogels, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schouten, of self-efficacy or mastery and instead sending messages to
2009; Watkins, 2008). Rumination also predicts symptoms of children that they are helpless and incompetent (Higgins &
anxiety, substance abuse, alcohol abuse, and eating disorders Silberman, 1998; Spasojevic & Alloy, 2002). In a study of
(see Aldao et al., 2010; Caselli et al., 2010; Nolen-Hoeksema, depressed and nondepressed mothers and their 5- to 7-year-old
Stice, Wade, & Bohon, 2007). Among the specific anxiety dis- children, Nolen-Hoeksema, Wolfson, Mumme, and Guskin
orders, rumination is associated with an increased risk for (1995) found that the children of mothers who were more
social phobia (Mellings & Alden, 2000), PTSD (Ehlers, intrusive during a puzzle task, often taking over the task from
Mayou, & Bryant, 1998; Mayou, Ehlers, & Bryant, 2002), and the child in a critical manner, were more helpless and passive
symptoms of generalized anxiety (Watkins, 2009a). and less likely to engage in problem solving both during the
We consider rumination a proximal risk factor. In this illus- puzzle task and, by teachers’ reports, in their classroom behav-
tration, we will link it back to possible distal risk factors and ior. Parents’ overcontrolling behaviors may also provide chil-
suggest some mechanisms by which the distal risk factors con- dren with little opportunity to learn persistence and proactive
tribute to rumination (see Figure 2). We will then briefly coping strategies, contributing to a tendency to ruminate (Spa-
review studies of the mechanisms linking rumination to dis- sojevic & Alloy, 2002).
tress and suggest moderators that may determine whether indi- A congenital biological abnormality that has been linked to
viduals who tend toward rumination experience depression, rumination is the Val66Met polymorphism in the BDNF gene.
anxiety, substance abuse, or eating disorder symptoms (i.e., An amino acid substitution (valine to methionine) at codon 66
moderators that may explain divergent trajectories). (Val66Met) of the BDNF gene results in two alleles: val and
met. Independent studies have found that adults who carried
the met allele (val/met or met/met) showed greater levels of
Distal risk factors and mechanisms linking rumination than did adults who did not (val/val) (Beevers,
them to rumination Wells, & McGeary, 2009; Hilt, Sander, Nolen-Hoeksema, &
Among the environmental context factors (i.e., independent Simen, 2007). In turn, rumination scores mediated the rela-
stressors) that have been associated with the tendency to rumi- tionship between the presence of the met allele and higher lev-
nate are childhood sexual and emotional abuse. Studies of col- els of depression in both studies.
lege students (Conway, Mendelson, Giannopoulos, Csank, & Carriers of the met allele may show more rumination
Holm, 2004), community participants (Sarin & Nolen-Hoek- because its presence is associated with impairments in cogni-
sema, 2010), and patients (Watkins, 2009b) have found that tive functioning that could make it more difficult for individu-
individuals with a history of sexual abuse score significantly als to control rumination. The met allele is associated with
higher on a self-report measure of rumination than do those decreased synaptic activity and plasticity in the hippocampus
who reported no history of sexual abuse. Similarly, Spasojevic and smaller hippocampal volumes (Frodl et al., 2007) as well
and Alloy (2002) found that a childhood history of sexual mal- as with poorer performance on tasks tapping prefrontal corti-
treatment (for females only) and emotional maltreatment (for cal functioning, including poorer episodic memory and execu-
both sexes) were significantly related to higher levels of rumi- tive functioning (Rybakowski et al., 2006). In turn, rumination
nation in college students. is associated with deficits in the ability to inhibit information,
Experiencing sexual or emotional abuse during childhood, particularly self-relevant or negative information, once it
particularly if the abuse is chronic, could contribute to rumina- enters working memory (Joormann, 2004, 2006; see also
tion by making children vigilant for signs of threat and making Johnson, Nolen-Hoeksema, Mitchell, & Levin, 2009). Thus,
them feel they are helpless to prevent severely negative events the met allele may confer risk for rumination because it results
(Spasojevic & Alloy, 2002). In turn, this could lead children to in deficits in brain areas involved in successful inhibition of
turn inward to cope, analyzing the problems they face passively, information in working memory. Making these potential con-
in a ruminative manner (Sarin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010). nections between genetic factors, neural functioning, basic
In addition, studies suggest that children with a history of cognitive processes, and rumination as an individual differ-
sexual or emotional abuse may develop dysregulated stress ence factor contributing to psychopathology is consistent with
responses, as measured by cortisol levels, adrenocorticotropic the goals of the RDoC initiative (Sanislow et al., 2010).
hormone levels, and cardiac measures (Heim, Plotsky, & In sum, distal risk factors for rumination appear to include
Nemeroff, 2004; Zahn-Waxler, 2000). Children who have the environmental context variables of sexual and emotional
more poorly regulated biological responses to stress will find abuse and overcontrolling parenting and the congenital bio-
it more difficult to engage in efficacious behavioral responses logical factor of a particular BDNF polymorphism. These may

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


Transdiagnostic Processes 599

lead to rumination by shaping vigilant, dysregulated, helpless, Miller, 2005; Rusting & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1998). Specifi-
negatively focused responses to the environment, shaping cally, experimentally induced rumination increases anxiety in
negative beliefs about one’s ability to cope, and conditioning already-anxious participants and participants whose thoughts
inefficacious coping responses. are directed toward anxious themes (Blagden & Craske, 1996;
McLaughlin, Borkovec, & Sibrava, 2007). The same mecha-
nisms of (a) amplifying current mood state and mood-congru-
Mechanisms linking rumination to depression ent cognition, (b) impairing problem solving, and (c)
The mechanisms linking rumination to depression have been interfering with instrumental behavior are also relevant to the
extensively studied and will be only briefly reviewed here (for development of anxiety disorders. Maladaptive cognition is a
comprehensive reviews, see Lyubomirsky & Tkach, 2004; characteristic and causal contributor to anxiety disorders (D.
Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008; Watkins, 2008, 2010). First, M. Clark, 1999), although it tends to be focused on future
rumination appears to amplify the reciprocal relationship threat rather than past loss as in depression (Watkins, Moulds,
between cognition and mood state, wherein negative mood & Mackintosh, 2005; see the next section). Similarly, poor
increases the accessibility of negative cognition, which in turn problem solving and passive, avoidant behavior when faced
fuels the negative mood (Ciesla & Roberts, 2007; Nolen-Hoek- with a stressful situation will lead to exacerbation of the
sema, 1991; Teasdale, 1988). Specifically, experimental studies stressor and increased anxiety.
indicate that rumination exacerbates existing dysphoric mood
(see Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008) and leads people in a
depressed mood to have more negative thoughts about the past, Moderators of the effects of rumination on
present, and future (Lavender & Watkins, 2004; Lyubomirsky, depression versus anxiety
Caldwell, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1998; Lyubomirsky & Nolen- For moderators interacting with rumination to produce one
Hoeksema, 1995; Rimes & Watkins, 2005), making it more type of symptom over another, our heuristic points to environ-
likely that people will use these negative thoughts to understand ments and biologically based individual differences that draw
their current circumstances. Furthermore, experience sampling individuals’ attention to certain concerns or themes over oth-
studies have found that real world rumination predicts subse- ers. A number of environmental factors may interact with
quent negative affect and mediates the effect of negative events rumination to produce major depression in some individuals
on subsequent affect (Moberly & Watkins, 2008a, 2008b). and anxiety disorders in others (or depression versus anxiety at
Explicitly training individuals to adopt the thinking style char- different times in an individual’s life). Interpersonal loss or
acteristic of rumination increases affective reactivity to a subse- rejection tends to create sad moods more than other types of
quent stressful event relative to training individuals to think in a moods (G. W. Brown & Harris, 1986; Eley & Stevenson,
way inconsistent with rumination (Moberly & Watkins, 2006; 2000; Pine, Cohen, Johnson, & Brook, 2002; Williamson, Bir-
Watkins, Moberly, & Moulds, 2008). maher, Dahl, & Ryan, 2005). This suggests that rumination
Second, rumination interferes with effective problem solv- among people who have recently had loss or rejection experi-
ing, in part by making thinking more pessimistic and fatalistic ences will be more likely to lead to depressive symptoms than
as well as more abstract and distanced from the specific details to other types of symptoms. A longitudinal study of adults who
of how to solve a difficulty (Donaldson & Lam, 2004; Lyu- lost a close loved one to cancer found that those who rumi-
bomirsky & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1995; Lyubomirsky, Tucker, nated experienced more depression symptoms and were more
Caldwell, & Berg, 1999; Watkins & Baracaia, 2002; Watkins likely to be diagnosed with depression than those who rumi-
& Moulds, 2005). Third, rumination also interferes with nated less (Nolen-Hoeksema & Larson, 1999). Further analy-
instrumental behavior (Lyubomirsky & Nolen-Hoeksema, ses of this study conducted for the present article showed that
1993). Thus, rumination prevents the resolution of problems the relationships between rumination and depressive symp-
and difficulties that may have led to depressed mood and may toms shortly before (r = .58, p < .001) and shortly after (r =
potentially produce further increases in stressful circum- .60, p < .001) the loved one’s death were significantly greater
stances. Indeed, recent prospective longitudinal studies sug- (at p < .05) than the relationships between rumination and
gest that rumination predicts increased chronic stress anxiety symptoms (before: r = .41, p < .001; after: r = .50, p <
(McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, in press; Pearson, Watkins, .001) or between rumination and anger (before: r = .33, p <
Mullan, & Moberly, 2010). .001; after: r = .39, p < .001). Thus, rumination in the context
of an important interpersonal loss was more strongly related to
depressive symptoms than to anxiety or anger, although rumi-
Mechanisms linking rumination to anxiety nation was associated with these moods as well.
Although most of the work on rumination has focused on its Still, some participants in this study were facing conditions
effects on individuals with an elevated depressed mood, rumi- that would likely induce considerable anxiety or anger, such as
nation appears to exacerbate whatever mood or theme is cur- a financial instability due to the loss of the loved one’s income
rently active in the individual (Blagden & Craske, 1996; or to medical bills, conflicts with relatives over the settlement
Bushman, 2002; Bushman, Bonacci, Pedersen, Vasquez, & of the loved one’s estate, or beliefs that doctors did not do all

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


600 Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins

that was possible to save their loved one (Nolen-Hoeksema & strategies to deal with the trauma will view the trauma as more
Larson, 1999). Among people facing such circumstances, high controllable and predictable, and they will therefore be less
levels of rumination would likely lead to anxiety and/or anger likely to develop PTSD. For example, torture victims who
as well as depressive symptoms over the loss of their loved were political activists and understood their risk for torture,
one. Thus, the environmental context can influence the diver- what the torture might entail, and how to remain stoic during
gent trajectories of relative anxiety versus depression symp- the experience of torture were less likely to develop PTSD
toms in response to rumination by influencing the initial than torture victims who were ordinary, nonpolitical citizens,
negative mood state (sadness, anger, anxiety, or some combi- even when the degree of torture experienced was greater
nation thereof) that subsequent rumination amplifies and among the political activists (Basoglu et al., 1997). For those
prolongs. who are unprepared for a trauma, rumination may be espe-
Environmental circumstances, including a prior history of cially likely to amplify their sense of uncontrollability and
exposure to particular circumstances, that raise concerns over unpredictability because rumination focuses on abstract ques-
specific types of threats or that condition anxious reactions to tions about an experience rather than on concrete features of
specific threats may produce anxiety disorders rather than the experience that can make it seem more controllable and
depression among people prone to rumination (Ehring & Wat- predictable (Watkins, 2008). For example, studies of patients
kins, 2008; Mineka & Zinbarg, 2006). Further, different types with PTSD have found that their ruminations tend to be
of threat concerns may lead to different types of anxiety focused on analyzing long-term consequences of the traumatic
disorders. event, what life would be like if the event had not happened,
People with social phobia frequently report having experi- what else might have happened, how unfair it was, and the
enced traumatic social embarrassment prior to developing patient’s relationship to other people (Ehlers & Clark, 2000;
their phobias. For example, McCabe, Antony, Summerfeldt, Michael, Halligan, Clark, & Ehlers, 2007).
Liss, and Swinson (2003) reported that 92% of adults with Biological factors that raise certain concerns or symptoms
social phobia reported a history of severe teasing in childhood, may also interact with rumination to produce specific anxiety
compared with only 50% and 35% of people with panic disor- syndromes. For example, rumination in conjunction with a
der or obsessive–compulsive disorder, respectively. Likewise, susceptibility to panic attacks (due to dysfunction in systems
Hackmann, Clark, and McManus (2000) found that the major- regulating the fight-or-flight response; Roy-Byrne, Craske, &
ity of individuals with social phobia had intrusive images of Stein, 2006) could contribute to panic disorder as the individ-
memories of previously embarrassing events, with these mem- ual ruminates over past episodes of panic and the possibility of
ories dated to the time of onset of the disorder. Among people future episodes. Dysfunction in the brain regions implicated in
who have had socially traumatizing experiences, those prone obsessive thoughts and compulsive behaviors (the frontal cor-
to ruminate will rehearse memories of the experiences, analyz- tex, basal ganglia, and thalamus; see Rauch et al., 2007; Sax-
ing what they did right and wrong and how other people ena & Rauch, 2000), in combination with a tendency to
reacted to them. Indeed, studies of people with social phobia ruminate over the meaning of such thoughts and behaviors
find that they do “postmortem” analyses of social encounters, (Salkovskis, 1999), could contribute to obsessive–compulsive
focusing on how they performed and what others think of disorder.
them (Abbott & Rapee, 2004; Kashdan & Roberts, 2007; Thus, environmental conditions (loss events versus specific
Kocovski, Endler, Rector, & Flett, 2005; Mellings & Alden, threats) or biological factors (predispositions to panic attacks
2000). Wong and Moulds (2009) found that following a social- or obsessive thoughts) that raise certain concerns may interact
evaluative task, rumination maintained anxiety in both high with rumination to determine whether individuals experience
and low socially anxious controls relative to distraction. Fur- depressive symptoms or anxiety symptoms. These suggestions
thermore, Vassilopoulos and Watkins (2009) found that in are speculative and require empirical testing. They illustrate,
individuals high in fear of negative evaluation, a concrete however, how our heuristic can be used to generate predictions
rumination condition (thinking about the concrete details of a about moderators of transdiagnostic risk factors leading to the
situation and how it occurred) decreased ratings of the self as divergent trajectories of depression or anxiety.
worthless and incompetent, pre- to postmanipulation, whereas
an abstract rumination condition (thinking about the meanings
and implications of a situation) maintained such negative self- Mechanisms and moderators of the
judgments, implicating abstract rumination in the maintenance relationship between rumination and alcohol
of negative cognition. Thus, such postevent rumination seems and substance abuse
likely to increase risk for the development of social phobia.
Mineka and Zinbarg (2006) suggest that PTSD develops Other types of environmental or biological moderators may
specifically in response to events that are both uncontrollable lead people prone to rumination to diverge onto trajectories of
and unpredictable, particularly if these events are chronic. alcohol or substance abuse. Heatherton and colleagues
They further suggest that individuals who are more prepared (Abramson, Bardone-Cone, Vohs, Joiner, & Heatherton, 2006;
for trauma by knowing what might happen and having Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991) argue that some people who

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


Transdiagnostic Processes 601

are highly aware of aversive aspects of themselves and their who are sensitive to rewards (specifically the rewarding effects
experiences, as are people who ruminate, turn to escapist of alcohol) and who have poorly regulated emotions because
behaviors such as binge drinking and substance abuse to tem- of a tendency to ruminate are more likely to consume alcohol
porarily quell their self-directed thoughts and related distress. and develop alcohol-related problems.
Accordingly, researchers have suggested that psychoactive The social environment also influences how much reward
substances such as alcohol may be sought by people prone to or punishment individuals receive for using alcohol. Individu-
rumination as a way of “escaping” from, or drowning out, als, particularly youth, with high levels of exposure to alcohol
their ruminations (Caselli et al., 2010; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., use and reinforcement for alcohol use are more likely to see
2007; Nolen-Hoeksema & Harrell, 2002). What determines alcohol use as normative and as an appropriate way to respond
why some people turn to substances to escape their rumina- to distress, including the distress caused by rumination. Ado-
tions (i.e., why they diverge onto this path)? We suggest that lescents’ alcohol use over time is predicted by peers’ perceived
people prone to ruminate will be more likely to turn to sub- drinking, offers of alcohol, and perceived norms for drinking
stances as a preferred means of escape if they experience (e.g., Wills & Cleary, 1999; Wood, Read, Mitchell, & Brand,
greater reinforcement from substances due to certain biologi- 2004). Children raised in homes where parents drink exces-
cal characteristics and/or learning histories. Thus, both bio- sively and regard drinking as an appropriate response to stress
logical and environmental moderators likely play a role in are more likely to have positive expectancies about the effects
determining which ruminators diverge down the path to sub- of alcohol, to begin to drink earlier, and to use alcohol exces-
stances and which do not. sively in adolescence and young adulthood (Fitzgerald &
Substances of abuse activate the mesocorticolimbic region Zucker, 2006; Zucker et al., 1995). Individuals prone to rumi-
of the brain, known as the reward center (Kalivas & Volkow, nation who have been exposed to environments that reinforce
2005), but as noted earlier, people differ in how rewarding alcohol use may be more likely to turn to alcohol to “drown
they find these substances (Newlin & Thomson, 1990; Sher, out” their ruminations. Conversely, ruminative people whose
Grekin, & Williams, 2005). Higher self-reported reward sensi- environments discourage or prohibit the use of alcohol may be
tivity is correlated with earlier age of onset of drinking alcohol less likely to turn to alcohol (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2004a).
in young adults (Pardo et al., 2007), with alcohol use and Thus, biological conditions leading to high reward sensitiv-
abuse in nonclinical samples (Loxton & Dawe, 2001), and ity to substances and environmental conditions enhancing
with craving and positive affect in response to alcohol cues in reinforcement and modeling of substance use may interact
young adult hazardous drinkers (Zisserson & Palfai, 2007). with rumination to send some people down the pathway to
Emotional distress, which is chronically higher in people who alcohol or other substance use. Again, these predictions require
ruminate, appears to potentiate reward systems in the brain future testing but illustrate how our heuristic can be used to
(Brady & Sinha, 2005), and this potentiation may be even generate predictions about divergent trajectories.
greater in individuals high in reward sensitivity, increasing the
chances they will turn to substances. Thus, rumination may
interact with individuals’ sensitivity to the rewarding aspects Mechanisms and moderators of the
of substances like alcohol, determining whether they will relationship between rumination and eating
develop alcohol abuse (Aldao et al., 2010; Carver, Johnson, & disorders
Joormann, 2008).
We know of no direct tests of this hypothesis, but the litera- What will determine which people high on rumination diverge
ture on alcohol expectancies provides some evidence that the onto a trajectory toward symptoms of eating disorders? As
combination of high reward sensitivity and emotional dysreg- with alcohol use, reward sensitivity and conditioning by the
ulation as seen in rumination is a strong predictor of alcohol social environment may influence which individuals prone to
use. Individuals who have positive expectancies for the effects rumination turn to binge eating to quell their self-aversive
of alcohol (including the reduction of negative mood) are thoughts (Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991) and develop symp-
more prone to drink when distressed and to develop alcohol toms of eating disorders. Like alcohol consumption, food acti-
abuse and dependence (Armeli et al., 2000; Cooper, Frone, vates the reward centers of the brain (Avena, Rada, & Hoebel,
Russell, & Mudar, 1995; Cooper, Russell, Skinner, Frone, & 2008; Di Chiara, Acquas, & Tanda, 1996). Indeed, both animal
Mudar, 1992). Positive expectancies for the effects of alcohol and human research suggest that food and alcohol may serve
are not the same as reward sensitivity but may reflect self- as substitutes for each other as rewards (see Gearhardt &
knowledge that substances have rewarding consequences Corbin, in press). Animal studies show that rats that consumed
(Brunelle, Barrett, & Pihl, 2007). Moreover, because many of large amounts of sugar as adolescents later drank more alcohol
these studies looked specifically at the interaction between as adults (Pian, Criado, Walker, & Ehlers, 2009) and when
positive expectancies and the experience of distress (e.g., sugar-addicted rats are deprived of sugar they consume more
Armeli et al., 2000) or between positive expectancies and mal- alcohol (Avena, Carillo, Needham, Leibowitz, & Hoebel,
adaptive emotion regulation styles (e.g., Cooper et al., 1988), 2004). Humans recovering from alcohol dependence prefer
their results are in line with our arguments that individuals more intense levels of sweetness than those never dependent

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


602 Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins

on alcohol (Kampov-Polevoy, Garbutt, & Janowsky, 1997), with this hypothesis comes from Holm-Denoma and Hankin
and individuals with a family history of alcoholism prefer (2010), who found that perceived physical appearance medi-
sweets at higher rates than those without a family history of ated the prospective relationship between rumination and the
alcoholism (Kampov-Polevoy, Garbutt, & Khalitov, 2003). development of bulimic symptoms. Thus, adolescent girls for
Further, individual differences in the activity of brain whom rumination exacerbated concerns about their physical
reward centers in response to food cues are associated with appearance were more likely to develop bulimic symptoms.
food intake: neuroimaging studies found greater reactivity in This finding suggests that current concerns focused on physi-
mesocorticolimbic areas in response to food cues in obese than cal appearance influence whether rumination increases bulimic
in nonobese participants (Rothemund et al., 2007; Stice, behavior.
Spoor, Bohon, Veldhuizen, & Small, 2008; Stoeckel et al., Among ruminators with high sensitivity to reward, the
2008), and greater reactivity in this brain area predicted future choice of food over alcohol or other substances may also be
weight gain (Stice, Yokum, Bohon, Marti, & Smolen, 2010). influenced by social punishments for substance use. People in
Among people prone to ruminate, greater sensitivity to the cultures that prohibit or frown upon alcohol use are less likely
rewarding aspects of food may determine whether they turn to to use alcohol and develop alcohol-related problems (Sher
binge eating to shut out ruminations and the distress that rumi- et al., 2005). Similarly, one factor associated with women’s
nations create. lower levels of alcohol use compared with men is social disap-
Again, we know of no direct test of this hypothesis, but proval of excessive use in women and adoption of the female
studies on the use of food to regulate emotions are consistent. gender role (Huselid & Cooper, 1992; White & Huselid,
People who believe that binge eating will improve their mood 1997). An interesting hypothesis for future research is that
and who binge eat in an attempt to reduce negative affect are rumination will be more strongly associated with binge eating
at increased risk to develop eating disorders (McCarthy, 1990; in cultures and groups that prohibit alcohol use than in cultures
Polivy & Herman, 2002). For example, Stice, Presnell, and that do not prohibit alcohol use.
Spangler (2002) followed a group of adolescent girls over Because alcohol and food may serve as reward substitutes
2 years and found that the girls who engaged in emotional for each other (Gearhardt & Corbin, in press), some rumina-
eating—eating when they felt distressed in an attempt to feel tors may turn to food at some times in their lives and alcohol
better—were significantly more likely to develop chronic at other times; thus, a tendency to ruminate may partially
binge eating over the 2 years. Thus, individuals who ruminate explain lifetime comorbidities of substance use and excess
and who find that binge eating quells their distressing rumina- food intake in some people (Franko et al., 2005; Herzog et al.,
tions and is inherently rewarding may develop eating disorder 2006). In conjunction with the literatures on alcohol and food,
symptoms. our heuristic therefore can generate predictions both about the
Why would some people with a high biologically deter- comorbidity of alcohol and food-related disorders (i.e., multi-
mined reward sensitivity turn to food and some to alcohol or finality) and about why some ruminative people develop alco-
other substances? Environmental shaping likely plays a role in hol-related disorders and some develop food-related disorders
leading some individuals to focus on food-related concerns (i.e., divergent trajectories).
and develop disordered eating behaviors rather than substance-
related behaviors (Gearhardt & Corbin, in press). Many theo-
rists have argued that concerns about shape and weight that Conclusions and Future Directions
characterize and predict eating disorder symptoms (Fairburn We have put forth a heuristic for developing models of trans-
& Harrison, 2003; Stice, 2002; Thompson & Stice, 2001) are diagnostic processes that (a) specifies the characteristics of
influenced by social factors (Garner & Garfinkel, 1980; distal transdiagnostic risk factors and proximal trandiagnostic
McCarthy, 1990; Sobal & Stunkard, 1989). Cultural pressures risk factors, (b) suggests the types of mechanisms by which
to be thin are often indicted as causes of females’ preoccupa- distal transdiagnostic risk factors can lead to proximal transdi-
tions with shape and weight (Garner & Garfinkel, 1980; agnostic risk factors, and (c) suggests the types of moderators
McCarthy, 1990). For example, in experimental studies, Stice that may determine whether a given proximal transdiagnostic
and colleagues (Stice, Maxfield, & Wells, 2003; Stice & Shaw, risk factor leads to one type of symptoms over another. We
1994; Stice, Spangler, & Agras, 2001) showed that even brief constrain distal risk factors to independent environmental con-
exposures to fashion magazines that feature ultrathin models texts and congenital biological abnormalities to avoid circular
or to peers talking about weight and shape lead to increases in arguments about which risk factors precede others; we also
body dissatisfaction in young women. These effects are likely specify the types of mechanisms that can link distal risk fac-
exacerbated in young women who ruminate, because they are tors to proximal risk factors. We argue that moderators create
more likely to rehearse and analyze their self-dissatisfaction symptoms by (a) raising concerns or themes that proximal risk
and the distress they experience over it. Body dissatisfaction, factors then act upon, (b) shaping responses through condi-
in turn, is a reliable predictor of eating disorder symptoms tioning, or (c) determining the reinforcement value of certain
(Thompson & Stice, 2001). Preliminary evidence consistent stimuli. Thus, this heuristic can guide a deeper and

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


Transdiagnostic Processes 603

more systematic exploration of the mechanisms by which Acknowledgments


transdiagnostic risk factors have their effects and create
comorbidity among disorders (i.e., multifinality); the heuristic The authors wish to thank the members of the Yale Depression and
can also guide the search for moderators that determine what Cognition Lab for helpful comments on drafts of this article.
specific symptoms individuals who carry transdiagnostic risk
factors will develop (i.e., divergent trajectories). Declaration of Conflicting Interests
A major limitation of this heuristic is that it is new and has The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
not been directly tested. Our application of the heuristic to the respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
rumination literature suggests it is promising, but this applica-
References
tion, by necessity, pieced together studies with different sam-
ples and methodologies, some of which were only indirectly Abbott, M.J., & Rapee, R.M. (2004). Post-event rumination and neg-
relevant to the component of the model being tested. It is ative self-appraisal in social phobia before and after treatment.
unlikely that any one study will be able to test all levels of the Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 136–144.
model (distal factors, the mechanisms leading from distal to Abramson, L.Y., Bardone-Cone, A.M., Vohs, K.D., Joiner, T.E., Jr.,
proximal factors, the mechanisms leading from proximal fac- & Heatherton, T.F. (2006). Cognitive vulnerability to bulimia. In
tors to multiple outcomes, and the moderators determining L.B. Alloy & J.H. Riskind (Eds.), Cognitive vulnerability to emo-
which outcomes the proximal factors lead to). Given that the tional disorders (pp. 329–364). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
literature already has a large number of distal and proximal Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion
risk factors that have been linked to multiple outcomes (as regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic
reviewed in the first section of this article), we suggest that review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237.
priorities for future studies will be determining (a) the proxi- American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical
mal risk factors mediating between established distal risk fac- manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
tors (e.g., stress) and outcomes, (b) the mechanisms linking American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical
those distal and proximal risk factors, and (c) the moderators manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC:
interacting with proximal risk factors to determine divergent Author.
trajectories to various outcomes. These three areas of research Armeli, S., Carney, M.A., Tennen, H., Affleck, G., & O’Neil, T.
seem to us to be underdeveloped but critical for a deeper (2000). Stress and alcohol use: A daily process examination of the
understanding of both multifinality and divergent trajectories. stressor-vulnerability model. Journal of Personality and Social
A second limitation of our heuristic is that it is likely to be Psychology, 78, 979–994.
too simplistic. In our effort to be parsimonious, we reduced Avena, N.M., Carillo, C.A., Needham, L., Leibowitz, S.F., & Hoe-
risk factors to two levels, distal and proximal, but it is highly bel, B.G. (2004). Sugar-dependent rats show enhanced intake of
likely there are more levels. In our effort to avoid circularity, unsweetened ethanol. Alcohol, 34, 203–209.
we set certain conditions for designating a factor a distal risk Avena, N.M., Rada, P., & Hoebel, B.G. (2008). Evidence for sugar
factor, a proximal risk factor, or a moderator, but it is likely addiction: Behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent,
that some important factors will not fit neatly into our excessive sugar intake. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,
operationalizations. 32, 20–39.
Third, the heuristic has not specified the potential role of Avenevoli, S., & Merikangas, K.R. (2006). Implications of high-risk
developmental factors in the effects of risk factors and mod- family studies for prevention of depression. American Journal of
erators, although we believe that developmental processes are Preventative Medicine, 31(S61), S126–S135.
important. For example, there may be critical periods during Barch, D.M. (2005). The cognitive neuroscience of schizophrenia.
which some moderators have particularly strong effects in Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 321–353.
determining the trajectory of symptoms in individuals with a Barlow, D.H., Allen, L.B., & Choate, M.L. (2004). Toward a uni-
given transdiagnostic risk factor. We look forward to further fied treatment for emotional disorders. Behavior Therapy, 35,
refinement and expansion of our heuristic as new research 205–230.
indicates is necessary. Basoglu, M., Mineka, S., Parker, M., Aker, T., Livanou, M., & Gok,
Understanding transdiagnostic processes in psychopathol- S. (1997). Psychological preparedness for trauma as a protec-
ogy is critically important, both theoretically and clinically. tive factor in survivors of torture. Psychological Medicine, 27,
Greater understanding of both how individual risk factors lead 1421–1433.
to multiple disorders (i.e., multifinality) and how specific dis- Beevers, C.G., Wells, T.T., & McGeary, J.E. (2009). The BDNF Val-
orders emerge from that risk factor (i.e., divergent trajectories) 66Met polymorphism is associated with rumination in healthy
will help us to identify who is at most risk for what disorders, adults. Emotion, 9, 579–584.
how that risk develops and changes over the lifespan, and how Beidel, D.C., & Turner, S.M. (1997). At risk for anxiety: Psychopa-
best to prevent and treat single and comorbid symptom thology in the offspring of anxious parents. Journal of the Ameri-
presentations. can Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 918–924.

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


604 Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins

Belmaker, R.H., & Agam, G. (2008). Major depressive disorder. New Ciesla, J.A., & Roberts, J.E. (2007). Rumination, negative cogni-
England Journal of Medicine, 358, 55–68. tion, and their interactive effects on depressed mood. Emotion,
Blagden, J.C., & Craske, M.G. (1996). Effects of active and pas- 7, 555–565.
sive rumination and distraction: A pilot replication with anxious Clark, D.A., & Taylor, S. (2009). The transdiagnostic perspective on
mood. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 10, 243–252. cognitive-behavioral therapy for anxiety and depression: New
Boyce, P., Parker, G., Barnett, B., Cooney, M., & Smith, F. (1991). wine for old wineskins? Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23,
Personality as a vulnerability factor to depression. British Journal 60–66.
of Clinical Psychology, 159, 106–114. Clark, D.M. (1999). Anxiety disorders: Why they persist and how
Brady, K.T., & Sinha, R. (2005). Co-occurring mental and substance to treat them. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(Suppl. 1),
us disorders: The neurobiological effects of chronic stress. American s5–s27.
Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1483–1493. Clarke, P., MacLeod, C., & Shirazee, N. (2008). Prepared for the
Brown, G.W., Andrews, B., Bifulco, A., & Veiel, H. (1990). Self-esteem worst: Readiness to acquire threat bias and susceptibility to ele-
and depression: 1. Measurement issues and prediction of onset. vate trait anxiety. Emotion, 8, 47–57.
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 25, 200–209. Conway, M., Mendelson, M., Giannopoulos, C., Csank, P.A.R., &
Brown, G.W., & Harris, T.O. (1986). Establishing causal links: The Holm, S.L. (2004). Childhood and adult sexual abuse, rumination
Bedford College studies of depression. In H. Katsching (Ed.), on sadness, and dysphoria. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 393–410.
Life events and psychiatric disorders (pp. 107–187). Cambridge, Cooper, M.L., Frone, M.R., Russell, M., & Mudar, P. (1995). Drink-
UK: Cambridge University Press. ing to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational
Brown, T.A., & Barlow, D.H. (2009). A proposal for a dimensional model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
classification system based on the shared features of the DSM- ogy, 69, 990–1005.
IV anxiety and mood disorders. Psychological Assessment, 21, Cooper, M.L., Russel, M., & George, W.H. (1988). Coping, expec-
256–271. tancies, and alcohol abuse: A test of social learning formulations.
Brunelle, C., Barrett, S.P., & Pihl, R.O. (2007). Relationship between Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 218–230.
the cardiac response to acute intoxication and alcohol-induced Cooper, M.L., Russell, M., Skinner, J.B., Frone, M.R., & Mudar, P.
subjective effects throughout the blood alcohol concentration (1992). Stress and alcohol use: Moderating effects of gender,
curve. Human Psychopharmacology, 22, 437–443. coping, and alcohol expectancies. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
Bushman, B.J. (2002). Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame? ogy, 101, 139–152.
Catharsis, rumination, distraction, anger, and aggressive respond- Davidson, R.J., Pizzagalli, D.A., & Nitschke, J.B. (2009). Repre-
ing. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 724–731. sentation and regulation of emotion in depression: Perspectives
Bushman, B.J., Bonacci, A.M., Pedersen, W.C., Vasquez, E.A., & from affective neuroscience. In I.H. Gotlib & C. Hammen (Eds.),
Miller, N. (2005). Chewing on it can chew you up: Effects of Handbook of depression (Vol. 2, pp. 218–248). New York, NY:
rumination on triggered displaced aggression. Journal of Person- Guilford Press.
ality and Social Psychology, 88, 969–983. Di Chiara, G., Acquas, E., & Tanda, G. (1996). Ethanol as a neuro-
Cannon, T.D., & Keller, M.C. (2006). Endophenotypes in the genetic chemical surrogate of conventional reinforcers: The dopamine-
analyses of mental Disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psy- opioid link. Alcohol, 13, 13–17.
chology, 2, 267–290. Dishion, T.J., & Patterson, G.R. (1997). The timing and severity
Carver, C.S., Johnson, S.L., & Joormann, J. (2008). Serotonergic of antisocial behavior: Three hypotheses within an ecologi-
function, two-mode models of self-regulation, and vulnerability cal framework. In D.M. Stoff, J. Breiling, & J.D. Maser (Eds.),
to depression: What depression has in common with impulsive Handbook of antisocial personality disorder (pp. 205–217). New
aggression. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 912–943. York, NY: Wiley.
Caselli, G., Ferretti, C., Leoni, M., Rebecchi, D., Rovetto, F., & Spada, Donaldson, C., & Lam, D. (2004). Rumination, mood and social
M.M. (2010). Rumination as a predictor of drinking behaviour in problem-solving in major depression. Psychological Medicine,
alcohol abusers: A prospective study. Addiction, 105, 1041–1048. 34, 1309–1318.
Charbonneau, A.M., Mezulis, A.H., & Hyde, J.S. (2009). Stress and Dozois, D.J.A., Seeds, P.M., & Collins, K.A. (2009). Transdiagnostic
emotional reactivity as explanations for gender differences in approaches to the prevention of depression and anxiety. Journal
adolescents’ depressive symptoms. Journal of Youth and Adoles- of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 44–59.
cence, 9, 1050–1058. Dudley, R., Kuyken, W., & Padesky, C.A. (2010). Disorder specific
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathol- and trans-diagnostic case conceptualisation. Clinical Psychology
ogy. Child Development, 55, 1–7. Review, 31, 213–224. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.07.005
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F.A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality Egeland, B., Pianta, R., & Ogawa, J. (1996). Early behavior prob-
in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopa- lems: Pathways to mental disorders in adolescence. Development
thology, 8, 597–600. and Psychopathology, 8, 735–749.
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S.L. (2005). Child maltreatment. Annual Ehlers, A., & Clark, D.M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic
Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 409–438. stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 319–345.

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


Transdiagnostic Processes 605

Ehlers, A., Mayou, R.A., & Bryant, B. (1998). Psychological predic- Hammen, C. (1991). Generation of stress in the course of unipolar
tors of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 555–561.
accidents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 508–519. Hammen, C. (2005). Stress and depression. Annual Review of Clini-
Ehring, T., & Watkins, E.R. (2008). Repetitive negative thinking as cal Psychology, 1, 293–319.
a transdiagnostic process. International Journal of Cognitive Hariri, A.R., Tessitore, A., Mattay, V.S., Fera, F., & Weinberger, D.R.
Therapy, 1, 192–205. (2002). The amygdala response to emotional stimuli: A compari-
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T.L., & Eggum, N.D. (2010). Emotion- son of faces and scenes. NeuroImage, 17, 317–323.
related regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Harvey, A.G., Murray, G., Chandler, R.A., & Soehner, A. (2010).
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 495–525. Sleep disturbance as transdiagnostic: Consideration of neurobio-
Eley, T.C., & Stevenson, J. (2000). Specific life events and chronic logical mechanisms. Clinical Psychology Review,31, 225–235.
experiences differentially associated with depression and anxiety in doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.003
young twins. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 383–394. Harvey, A.G., Watkins, E.R., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004).
Fairburn, C.G., Cooper, Z., & Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive behav- Cognitive behavioural processes across psychological disorders:
iour therapy for eating disorders: A “transdiagnostic” theory and A transdiagnostic approach to research and treatment. Oxford,
treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 509–528. UK: Oxford University Press.
Fairburn, C.G., & Harrison, P. (2003). Eating disorders. The Lancet, Hayes, S.C., Strosahl, K.D., Wilson, K.G., Bissett, R.T., Pistorello, J.,
361, 407–416. Toarmino, D., . . . McCurry, S.M. (2004). Measuring experiential
Fitzgerald, H.E., & Zucker, R.A. (2006). Pathways of risk aggrega- avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model. The Psycho-
tion for alcohol use disorders. In K. Freeark & I.W.S. Davidson logical Record, 54, 553–578.
(Eds.), The crisis in youth mental health: Critical issues and Heatherton, T.F., & Baumeister, R.F. (1991). Binge eating as escape
effective programs: Issues for families, schools, and communities from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 86–108.
(Vol. 3, pp. 249–271). Westport, CT: Praeger. Heim, C., & Nemeroff, C.B. (2001). The role of childhood trauma
Forrester, A., Owens, D.G.C., & Johnstone, E.C. (2001). Diagnostic in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorder: Preclinical
stability in subjects with multiple admissions for psychotic ill- and clinical studies. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 1023–1039.
ness. Psychological Medicine, 31, 151–158. Heim, C., Plotsky, P.M., & Nemeroff, C.B. (2004). Importance of
Franko, D.L., Dorer, D.J., Keel, P.K., Jackson, S., Manzo, M.P., & studying the contributions of early adverse experience to neuro-
Herzog, D.B. (2005). How do eating disorders and alcohol use biological findings in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology,
disorder influence each other? International Journal of Eating 29, 641–648.
Disorders, 38, 200–207. Herzog, D.B., Franko, D.L., Dorer, D.J., Keel, P.K., Jackson, S., &
Freud, A. (1966). The ego and the mechanisms of defense. New York, Manzo, M.P. (2006). Drug abuse in women with eating disorders.
NY: International Universities Press. (Original work published International Journal of Eating Disorders, 39, 364–368.
1937) Higgins, E.T., & Silberman, I. (1998). Development of regulatory
Frodl, T., Schule, C., Schmitt, G., Born, C., Baghai, T., Zill, P., . . . focus: Promotion and prevention as ways of living. In J. Heck-
Meisenzahl, E.M. (2007). Association of the brain-derived neuro- hausen & C.S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self regulation
trophic factor Val66Met polymorphism with reduced hippocam- across the life-span (pp. 78–113). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
pal volumes in major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, University Press.
64, 410–416. Hill, S.K., Harris, M.S.H., Herbener, E.S., Pavuluri, M., & Sweeney,
Garner, D.M., & Garfinkel, P.E. (1980). Socio-cultural factors in the J.A. (2008). Neurocognitive allied phenotypes for schizophrenia
development of anorexia nervosa. Psychological Medicine, 10, and bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34, 743–759.
647–656. Hilt, L.M., Sander, L.C., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Simen, A.A.
Gearhardt, A.N., & Corbin, W.R. (in press). Interactions between (2007). The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism predicts rumination
alcohol consumption, eating, and weight. Handbook of Food and and depression differently in young adolescent girls and their
Addiction. mothers. Neuroscience Letters, 429, 12–16.
Gotlib, I.H., & Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: Cur- Holm-Denoma, J.M., & Hankin, B.L. (2010). Perceived physical
rent status and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psy- appearance mediates the rumination and bulimic symptom link in
chology, 6, 285–312. adolescent girls. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychol-
Gottesman, I.I., & Gould, T.D. (2003). The endophenotype concept ogy, 39, 537–544.
in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. The American Huselid, R.F., & Cooper, M.L. (1992). Gender roles as mediators of
Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 636–645. sex differences in adolescent alcohol use and abuse. Journal of
Greenberg, L.S. (2002). Emotion-focused therapy, coaching clients Health and Social Behavior, 33, 348–362.).
to work through their feelings. Washington, DC: APA. Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D.S., Quinn,
Hackmann, A., Clark, D.M., & McManus, F. (2000). Recurrent K., . . . Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC):
images and early memories in social phobia. Behaviour Research Toward a new classification framework for research on mental
and Therapy, 38, 601–610. disorders. American Journal of Psychology, 167, 748–751.

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


606 Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins

Insel, T.R., & Cuthbert, B.N. (2009). Endophenotypes: Bridging investigations in mood disorders: Bipolar disorder versus major
genomic complexity and disorder heterogeneity. Biological Psy- depressive disorder. Bipolar Disorder, 10, 1–37.
chiatry, 66, 988–989. Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F., & Watson, D. (2010). Linking
Johnson, J.G., Cohen, P., Kasen, S., & Brook, J.S. (2006). A multi- “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use
wave multi-informant study of the specificity of the association disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 768–821.
between parental and offspring psychiatric disorders. Compre- Kring, A.M., & Sloan, D.M. (Eds.). (2010). Emotion regulation and
hensive Psychiatry, 47, 169–177. psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and
Johnson, M.K., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Mitchell, K., & Levin, Y. treatment. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
(2009). Medial cortex activity, self-reflection and depression. Krueger, R.F., & Markon, K.E. (2006). Reinterpreting comorbidity:
Social, Cognitive Affective Neuroscience, 4, 313–327. A model-based approach to understanding and classifying
Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibi- psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2,
tory processes. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 125–147. 111–133.
Joormann, J. (2006). Differential effects of rumination and dysphoria Lavender, A., & Watkins, E.R. (2004). Rumination and future think-
on the inhibition of irrelevant emotional material: Evidence from ing in depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43,
a negative priming task. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 129–142.
149–160. LeDoux, J.E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of
Jorm, A.F., Christensen, H., Henderson, A.S., Jacomb, P.A., Korten, Neuroscience, 23, 155–184.
A.E., & Rodgers, B. (1999). Using the BIS/BAS scales to mea- Leyro, T.M., Zvolensky, M.J., & Bernstein, A. (2010). Distress toler-
sure behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation: Factor ance and psychopathological symptoms and disorders: A review
structure, validity, and norms in a large community sample. Per- of the empirical literature among adults. Psychological Bulletin,
sonality and Individual Differences, 26, 49–58. 136, 576–600.
Kalivas, P.W., & Volkow, N.D. (2005). The neural basis of addiction: Loxton, N.J., & Dawe, S. (2001). Alcohol abuse and dysfunctional
A pathology of motivation and choice. American Journal of Psy- eating in adolescent girls: The influence of individual difference
chiatry, 162, 1403–1413. in sensitivity to reward. International Journal of Eating Disor-
Kampov-Polevoy, A.B., Garbutt, J.C., & Janowsky, D.J. (1997). Evi- ders, 29, 455–462.
dence of preference for a high-concentration sucrose solution in Lynam, D.R., Caspi, A., Moffit, T.E., Wikstrom, P.H., Loeber, R.,
alcoholic men. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 269–270. & Novak, S. (2000). The interaction between impulsivity and
Kampov-Polevoy, A.B., Garbutt, J.C., & Khalitov, E. (2003). Family neighborhood context on offending: The effects of impulsivity
history of alcoholism and response to sweets. Alcoholism: Clini- are stronger in poorer neighborhoods. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
cal & Experimental Research, 27, 1743–1749. chology, 109, 563–574.
Kashdan, T.B., & Roberts, J.E. (2007). Social anxiety, depressive Lyubomirsky, S., Caldwell, N.D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998).
symptoms, and postevent rumination: Affective consequences Effects of ruminative and distracting responses to depressed
and social contextual influences. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, mood on retrieval of autobiographical memories. Journal of Per-
21, 284–301. sonality and Social Psychology, 75, 166–177.
Keane, T.M., Marshall, A.D., & Taft, C.T. (2006). Posttraumatic Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Self-perpetuating
stress disorder: Etiology, epidemiology, and treatment outcome. properties of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2, 161–197. Social Psychology, 65, 339–349.
Kendler, K.S., Kuhn, J.W., & Prescott, C.A. (2004). The interrela- Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-
tionship of neuroticism, sex, and stressful life events in the pre- focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal prob-
diction of episodes of major depression. American Journal of lem-solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,
Psychiatry, 16, 631–636. 176–190.
Kessler, R.C., Chiu, W.T., Demler, O., & Walters, E. (2005). Preva- Lyubomirsky, S., & Tkach, C. (2004). The consequences of dysphoric
lence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders rumination. In C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive
in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of rumination: Nature, theory, and treatment (pp. 21–42). New
General Psychiatry, 62, 617–627. York, NY: Wiley.
Klinger, E. (1996). Emotional influences on cognitive processing, with Lyubomirsky, S., Tucker, K.L., Caldwell, N.D., & Berg, K. (1999).
implications for theories of both. In P.M. Gollwitzer & J.A. Bargh Why ruminators are poor problem solvers: Clues from the phe-
(Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation nomenology of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and
to behaviour (pp. 168–189). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Social Psychology, 77, 1041–1060.
Kocovski, N.L., Endler, N.S., Rector, N.A., & Flett, G.L. (2005). MacLeod, C. (1999). Anxiety and anxiety disorders. In T. Dalgleish
Ruminative coping and post-event processing in social anxiety. & M.J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 971–984. 447–477). New York, NY: Wiley.
Konarski, J.Z., McIntyre, R.S., Kennedy, S.H., Rafi-Tari, S., Soc- MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., &
zynska, J.K., & Ketter, T.A. (2008). Volumetric neuroimaging Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability:

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


Transdiagnostic Processes 607

Assessing the causal basis of their association through the exper- Moberly, N.J., & Watkins, E.R. (2008b). Ruminative thinking and
imental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal negative affect: An experience sampling study. Journal of Abnor-
Psychology, 111, 107–123. mal Psychology, 117, 314–323.
Maniglio, R. (2009). The impact of child sexual abuse on health: A Monroe, S.M. (2008). Modern approaches to conceptualizing and
systematic review of reviews. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, measuring human life stress. Annual Review of Clinical Psychol-
647–657. ogy, 4, 33–52.
Mansell, W., Harvey, A., Watkins, E., & Shafran, R. (2009). Concep- Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative
tual foundations of the transdiagnostic approach to CBT. Journal affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 638–662.
of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 6–19. Mowrer, O.H. (1939). Stimulus response theory of anxiety. Psycho-
Martin, M., Ward, J.C., & Clark, D.M. (1983). Neuroticism and the logical Review, 46, 553–565.
recall of positive and negative personality information. Behav- Munafo, M.R., Brown, S.M., & Hariri, A.R. (2008). Serotonin trans-
iour Research and Therapy, 21, 495–503. porter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation: A meta-
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 852–857.
emotional disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, Newlin, D.B., & Thomson, J.B. (1990). Alcohol challenge with sons
167–195. of alcoholics: A critical review and analysis. Psychological Bul-
Mayou, R.A., Ehlers, A., & Bryant, B. (2002). Posttraumatic stress letin, 108, 383–402.
disorder after motor vehicle accidents: 3-Year follow-up of a pro- Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their
spective longitudinal study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnor-
40, 665–675. mal Psychology, 100, 569–582.
McCabe, R.E., Antony, M.M., Summerfeldt, L.J., Liss, A., & Swin- Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2004a). Gender differences in risk factors and
son, R.P. (2003). Preliminary examination of the relationship consequences for alcohol use and problems. Clinical Psychology
between anxiety disorders in adults and self-reported history of Review, 24, 981–1010.
teasing or bullying. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 32, 187–193. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2004b). The response styles theory. In C. Papa-
McCarthy, M. (1990). The thin ideal, depression, and eating disorders georgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination: Nature, the-
in women. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 205–215. ory, and treatment (pp. 107–124). New York, NY: Wiley.
McLaughlin, K.A., Borkovec, T.D., & Sibrava, N.J. (2007). The Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Harrell, Z.A. (2002). Rumination, depres-
effects of worry and rumination on affect states and cognitive sion, and alcohol use: Tests of gender differences. Journal of
activity. Behavior Therapy, 38, 23–28. Cognitive Psychotherapy, 16, 391–403.
McLaughlin, K.A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (in press). Interpersonal Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Larson, J. (1999). Coping with loss. Mah-
stress generation as a mechanism linking rumination to internal- wah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
izing symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psy- Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Stice, E., Wade, E., & Bohon, C. (2007).
chology. Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance
Mellings, T.M.B., & Alden, L.E. (2000). Cognitive processes in social abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. Journal
anxiety: The effects of self-focus, rumination, and anticipatory of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 198–207.
processing. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 243–257. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B.E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethink-
Mennin, D.S., & Fresco, D.M. (2010). Emotion regulation as an inte- ing rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400–424.
grative framework for understanding and treating psychopathol- Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wolfson, A., Mumme, D., & Guskin, K. (1995).
ogy. In A.M. Kring & D.M. Sloan (Eds.), Emotion regulation and Helplessness in children of depressed and nondepressed mothers:
psychopathology (pp. 356–379). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Special section: Parental depression and distress: Implications for
Meyer, I.H. (2003). Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in development in infancy, childhood, and adolescence. Develop-
lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: Conceptual issues and mental Psychology, 31, 377–387.
research evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 674–697. Pardo, Y., Aguilar, R., Molinuevo, B., & Torrubia, R. (2007). Alcohol
Michael, T., Halligan, S.L., Clark, D.M., & Ehlers, A. (2007). Rumi- use as a behavioural sign of disinhibition: Evidence from J.A.
nation in posttraumatic stress disorder. Depression and Anxiety, Gray’s model of personality. Addictive Behaviors, 32, 2398–2403.
24, 307–317. Pearson, K.A., Watkins, E.R., Mullan, E.G., & Moberly, N.J. (2010).
Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learning theory Psychosocial correlates of depressive rumination. Behaviour
perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: It’s not what you Research and Therapy, 48, 784–791.
thought it was. American Psychologist, 61, 10–26. Pian, J.P., Criado, J.R., Walker, B.M., & Ehlers, C.L. (2009). Milk con-
Moberly, N.J., & Watkins, E. (2006). Processing mode influences the sumption during adolescence decreases alcohol drinking in adult-
relationship between trait rumination and emotional vulnerabil- hood. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 94, 179–185.
ity. Behavior Therapy, 37, 281–291. Pine, D.S., Cohen, P., Gurley, D., Brook, J., & Ma, Y. (1998). The risk
Moberly, N.J., & Watkins, E.R. (2008a). Ruminative self-focus, neg- for early adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adoles-
ative life events, and negative affect. Behaviour Research and cents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Archives of General
Therapy, 46, 1034–1037. Psychiatry, 55, 56–64.

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


608 Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins

Pine, D.S., Cohen, P., Johnson, J.G., & Brook, J.S. (2002). Ado- Salkovskis, P.M. (1999). Understanding and treating obsessive-com-
lescent life events as predictors of adult depression. Journal of pulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, S29–S52.
Affective Disorders, 68, 49–57. Sanislow, C.A., Pine, D.S., Quinn, K.J., Kozak, M.J., Garvey, M.A.,
Polivy, J., & Herman, C.P. (2002). Causes of eating disorders. Annual Heinssen, R.K., . . . Cuthbert, B.N. (2010). Developing constructs
Review of Clinical Psychology, 53, 187–214. for psychopathology research: Research Domain Criteria. Jour-
Potenza, M.N., Steinberg, M.A., Skudlarski, P., Fulbright, R.K., nal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 631–639.
Lacadie, C.M., Wilber, M.K., . . . Wexler, B.E. (2003). Gambling Sarin, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). The dangers of dwelling:
urges in pathological gambling: A functional magnetic resonance An examination of the relationship between rumination and
imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 828–836. consumptive coping in survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Cog-
Rauch, S.L., Wedig, M.M., Wright, C.I., Martis, B., McMullin, K.G., nition & Emotion, 24, 71–85.
Shin, L.M., . . . Wilhelm, S. (2007). Functional magnetic reso- Saxena, S., & Rauch, S.L. (2000). Functional neuroimaging and the
nance imaging study of regional brain activation during implicit neuroanatomy of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatric
sequence learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Clinics of North America, 23, 563–596.
Psychiatry, 61, 330–336. Sher, K.J., Grekin, E.R., & Williams, N.A. (2005). The development
Regier, D.A., Narrow, W.E., Kuhl, E.A., & Kupfer, D.J. (2009). The of alcohol use disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology,
conceptual development of DSM-V. American Journal of Psy- 1, 493–523.
chiatry, 166, 1–7. Skinner, B.F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: Copley Publish-
Rimes, K.A., & Watkins, E.R. (2005). The effects of self-focused ing Group.
rumination on global negative self-judgments in depression. Smoller, J.W., & Tsuang, M.T. (1998). Panic and phobic anxiety:
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1673–1681. Defining phenotypes for genetic studies. American Journal of
Roberts, S.B., & Kendler, K.S. (1999). Neuroticism and self-esteem Psychiatry, 155, 1152–1162.
as indices of the vulnerability to major depression in women. Sobal, J., & Stunkard, A.J. (1989). Socioeconomic status and obe-
Psychological Medicine, 29, 1101–1109. sity: A review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 105,
Roche, D.M., Runtz, M.G., & Hunter, M.A. (1999). Adult attachment: 260–275.
A mediator between child sexual abuse and later psychological Southwick, S.M., Vythilingam, M., & Charney, D.S. (2005). The psy-
adjustment. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 184–207. chobiology of depression and resilience to stress: Implications for
Roemer, L., Erisman, S.M., & Orsillo, S.M. (2009). Mindful- prevention and treatment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology,
ness and acceptance-based treatments for anxiety disorders. In 1, 255–292.
M.M. Antony & M.B. Stein (Eds.), Oxford handbook of anxiety Spasojevic, J., & Alloy, L.B. (2002). Who becomes a depressive
and related disorders (pp. 476–487). New York, NY: Oxford ruminator? Developmental antecedents of ruminative response
University Press. style. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 16, 405–419.
Rood, L.R., Bogels, S.J., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schouten, E. Stein, A., Woolley, H., Cooper, S., Winterbottom, J., Fairburn, C.G.,
(2009). The influence of emotion-focused rumination and dis- & Cortina-Borja, M. (2006). Eating habits and attitudes among
traction on depressive symptoms in non-clinical youth: A meta- 10-year-old children of mothers with eating disorders. British
analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 607–616. Journal of Psychiatry, 189, 324–329.
Rothemund, Y., Preuschhof, C., Bohner, G., Bauknecht, H., Klinge- Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology:
biel, R., Flor, H., & Klapp, B.F. (2007). Differential activation of A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 825–848.
the dorsal striatum by high-calorie visual food stimuli in obese Stice, E., Maxfield, J., & Wells, T. (2003). Adverse effects of social
individuals. NeuroImage, 37, 410–421. pressure to be thin on young women: An experimental investiga-
Roy-Byrne, P.P., Craske, M.G., & Stein, M.B. (2006). Panic disorder. tion of the effects of “fat talk”. International Journal of Eating
Lancet, 368, 1023–1032. Disorders, 34, 108–117.
Rozin, P., & Royzman, E.B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity domi- Stice, E., Presnell, K., & Spangler, D. (2002). Risk factors for binge
nance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, eating onset in adolescent girls: A 2-year prospective investiga-
5, 296–320. tion. Health Psychology, 21, 131–138.
Rusting, C. L., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Regulating responses Stice, E., & Shaw, H. (1994). Adverse effects of the media portrayed
to anger: Effects of rumination and distraction on angry mood. thin-ideal on women and linkages to bulimic symptomatology.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 790–803. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13, 288–308.
Rusting, C. L. (1998). Personality, mood, and cognitive processing Stice, E., Spangler, D., & Agras, W.S. (2001). Exposure to media-
of emotional information: Three conceptual frameworks. Psycho- portrayed thin-ideal images adversely affects vulnerable girls: A
logical Bulletin, 124, 165–198. longitudinal experiment: Media exposure and disordered eating.
Rybakowski, J., Borkowska, A., Skibinska, M., Szczepankiewicz, A., Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 270–288.
Kapelski, P., Leszczynska-Rodziewicz, A., . . . Hauser, J. (2006). Stice, E., Spoor, S., Bohon, C., Veldhuizen, M., & Small, D. (2008).
Prefrontal cognition in schizophrenia and bipolar illness in rela- Relation of reward from food intake and anticipated intake to
tion to Val66Met polymorphism of the brain-derived neurotrophic obesity: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal
factor gene. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 60, 70–76. of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 924–935.

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011


Transdiagnostic Processes 609

Stice, E., Yokum, S., Bohon, C., Marti, N., & Smolen, A. (2010). Watkins, E.R., Moulds, M., & Mackintosh, B. (2005). Comparisons
Reward circuitry responsivity to food predicts future increases in between rumination and worry in a non-clinical population.
body mass: Moderating effects of DRD2 and DRD4. NeuroIm- Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1577–1585.
age, 50, 1618–1625. Watson, D. (2009). Differentiating the mood and anxiety disorders:
Stoeckel, L.E., Weller, R.E., Cook, E.W., Twieg, D.B., Knowlton, A quadripartite model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5,
R.C., & Cox, J.F. (2008). Widespread reward-system activation 221–247.
in obese women in response to pictures of high calorie foods. White, H.R., & Huselid, R.F. (1997). Gender differences in alco-
NeuroImage, 41, 636–647. hol use during adolescence. In R.M. Wilsnack & S.C. Wilsnack
Teasdale, J.D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depres- (Eds.), Gender and alcohol (pp. 176–198). New Brunswick, NJ:
sion. Cognition & Emotion, 2, 247–274. Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies.
Thompson, J.B., & Stice, E. (2001). Thin ideal internalization: Williamson, D.E., Birmaher, B., Dahl, R.E., & Ryan, N.D. (2005).
Mounting evidence for a new risk factor for body image distur- Stressful life events in anxious and depressed children. Journal
bance and eating pathology. Current Directions in Psychological of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 15, 571–580.
Science, 10, 181–183. Willis-Owen, S.A.G., Turri, M.G., Munafo, M.R., Surtees, P.G.,
Vassilopoulos, S., & Watkins, E.R. (2009). Adaptive and maladaptive Wainwright, N.W.J., Brixey, R.D., & Flint, J. (2005). The sero-
self-focus: A pilot extension study with individuals high and low tonin transporter length polymorphism, neuroticism, and depres-
in fear of negative evaluation. Behavior Therapy, 40, 181–189. sion: A comprehensive assessment of association. Biological
Vohs, K.D., Voelz, Z.R., Pettit, J.W., Bardone, A.M., Katz, J., Psychiatry, 58, 451–456.
Abramson, L.Y., . . . Joiner, T.E., Jr. (2001). Perfectionism, body Wills, T.A., & Cleary, S.D. (1999). Peer and adolescent substance
dissatisfaction, and self-esteem: An interactive model of bulimic use among 6th-9th graders: Latent growth analyses of influence
symptom development. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychol- versus selection mechanisms. Health Psychology, 18, 453–463.
ogy, 20, 476–497. Wolpe, J. (1969). The practice of behavior therapy. New York, NY:
Vujanovic, A.A., Bernstein, A., & Litz, B.T. (in press). Distress tol- Penguin.
erance and traumatic stress. In M.J. Zvolensky, A. Bernstein, Wong, Q.J.J., & Moulds, M.L. (2009). Impact of rumination versus
& A.A. Vujanovic (Eds.), Distress tolerance. New York, NY: distraction on anxiety and maladaptive self-beliefs in socially
Guilford. anxious individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47,
Watkins, E.R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive 861–867.
thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163–206. Wood, M.D., Read, J.P., Mitchell, R.E., & Brand, N.H. (2004).
Watkins, E.R. (2009a). Depressive rumination and co-morbidity: Do parents still matter? Parent and peer influences on alcohol
Evidence for brooding as a transdiagnostic process. Journal of involvement among recent high school graduates. Psychology of
Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 27, 160–175. Addictive Behaviors, 18, 19–30.
Watkins, E.R. (2009b). Depressive rumination: Investigating mecha- Zahn-Waxler, C. (2000). The development of empathy, guilt, and
nisms to improve cognitive-behavioral treatments. Cognitive internalization of distress: Implications for gender differences in
Behaviour Therapy, 38, 8–14. internalizing and externalizing problems. In R. Davidson (Ed.),
Watkins, E.R. (2011). Dysregulation in level of goal and action iden- Anxiety, depression, and emotion: Wisconsin symposium on emo-
tification across psychological disorders. Clinical Psychology tion (Vol. 1., pp. 222–265). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Review, 31, 260–278. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.05.004 Zisserson, R.N., & Palfai, T.P. (2007). Behavioral activation system
Watkins, E.R., & Baracaia, S. (2002). Rumination and social problem- (BAS) sensitivity and reactivity to alcohol cues among hazardous
solving in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 32, 2178–2186.
1179–1189. Zucker, R.A., Kincaid, S.B., Fitzgerald, H.E., & Bingham, C.R.
Watkins, E.R., Moberly, N.J., & Moulds, M. (2008). Processing (1995). Alcohol schema acquisition in preschoolers: Differences
mode causally influences emotional reactivity: Distinct effects of between children of alcoholics and children of nonalcoholics.
abstract versus concrete construal on emotional response. Emo- Alcoholism, 19, 1011–1017.
tion, 8, 364–378. Zvolensky, M.J., Vujanovic, A.A., Bernstein, A., & Leyro, T.M.
Watkins, E.R., & Moulds, M. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative (2010). Distress tolerance: Theory, measurement, and relations to
self-focus: Impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on psychopathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
problem solving in depression. Emotion, 5, 319–328. 19, 406–410.

Downloaded from pps.sagepub.com by Susan Nolen-Hoeksema on October 18, 2011

View publication stats

You might also like