You are on page 1of 5

The Relationship One visual-perceptual test, four l'isual-motor tests,

and a test of motor impairment were administered


to 22 cbildren witb learning disabilities and 22 cbil-
Between Visual- dren witbout learning disabilities, aged 5 to 8
years. Tbe cbildren with learning disabilities were
Perceptual Motor divided into two groups-"clumsy" and "non-
clumsy"-based on tbeir scores on tbe motor im-
Abilities and pairment test It was bypotbesized tbat tbe clumsy
cbildren witb learning disabilities would score sig-
Clllffisiness in Children nificantly lower on visual-perceptual and visual-
motor tests tban tbe non clumsy cbildren witb learn-
With and Without ing disabilities wbo, in turn, would score
Significantly lower tban tbe cbildren witbout learn-

Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/42/6/359/53515/359.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 30 September 2022
ing disabilities. It was furtber bypotbesized tbat
Learning Disabilities there would be a Significant correlation between
tbe degree of clumsiness and tbe degree of visual-
perceptual and visual-motor defiCit. Analysis of the
data indicated that, as expected, the clumsy chil-
Valerie O'Brien, Sharon A. Cermak, dren witb learning disabilities scored significantly
Elizabeth Murray lower tban the children without learning disabilities
(the control group). Tbere was no Significant differ-
ence between the clumsy and nonclumsy children
Key Words: dyspraxia • sensory with learning disabilities or between the nonclumsy
integration. visual perception children with learning disabilities and tbe control
group_ Degree of clumsiness Significantly correlated
with scores on four offive tests Results are discussed
in terms of subtypes of learning disabilities and
sample size.

any occupational therapists treat children

M with learning disabilities for visual-percep-


tual problems, visual-motor problems, and
motor incoordination_ Some researchers suggest that
Visual-perceptual deficits are characteristic of chil-
dren with learning disabilities (Bush & Waugh, 1982)
Others say that children with learning disabilities
have average to superior Visual-perceptual skills
(Geschwind & Galaburda, 1985) Because people
with learning disabilities form a heterogeneous
group, it is probable that there are subgroups of chil-
dren with learning disabilities who do have and who
do not have Visual-perceptual problems Rourke
Valerie O'Brien, MS, OTR, is an occupational therapist at (985) has emphasized that
the Second General Hospital, APO, New York
the confUSIOn that abounds 111 the literature dealing with the
Sharon A. Cermak, EdD, OTR, FAOTA, is Associate Profes- group of clinical problems knOWll as learning disabilities is,
sor of Occupational Therapy at Sargent CoJlege, Bosron in manywa\'s, a direct reflection of the failure of many scien-
University, One University Road, Bosron, Massachuserrs tists and praclitioners in this field to acknowledge and ad-
dress the heterogeneity and diversity extallt among the learn-
02115 ing disabled population. (p vii)
Elizabeth Murray, MEd, OTR, is Assistant Director of
He emphasizes the need to examine subtypes of
Training in Occupational Therapy, Eunice Kennedy
Shriver Center, Waltham, Massachusetts, and Adjunct As- learning disabilities
sistant Professor of Occupational Therapy at Sargent Col- In addition to visual-perceptual problems, non-
lege, Bosron University. specific awkwardness or clumsiness, though not spe-
cific to children with learning disabilities, appears in
Sharon A Cermak and Elizabeth Murray are faculty
greater numbers of these children than in children
members of Sensory Integration International, Torrance,
California without such disabilities Researchers have estimated
that between 5% and 18% of school-age children may

The Americanjournal of Occupational Therapy 359


have motor problems (Brenner, Gilman, Zangwill, & that visual-perceptual problems may be a cause of
Farrell, 1967; Gubbay, 1978; Henderson & Hall, clumsiness.
1982). These children form a heterogeneous group; Ayres (1985) also found that many dyspraxic
all show some degree of inability in their perfor- children had Visual-perceptual problems. However,
mance of skilled or complicated motor tasks. Prob- she theorized that visual perception is an end product
lems in the motor performance of children with learn- and not the basis for the clumsiness. Because factor
ing disabilities have been noted as far back as 1937 analytical studies showed that performance on tactile
when Orton (1937) described abnormal clumsiness tests significantly correlated with motor planning
in many of the children that he studied. Strauss and abilities (Ayres, 1966, 1969), Ayres (1969) suggested
Lehtinen (1947) discussed the incoordination that that tactile perceptual deficits may be a factor in the
frequently occurs in brain-injured children. Re- clumsiness.
searchers in the 1960s began to delineate this clumsi- The present study was designed to further exam-

Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/42/6/359/53515/359.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 30 September 2022
ness, or developmental dyspraxia, evident in many ine the relationship between visual-perceptual and vi-
children with learning disabilities (Gubbay, Ellis, sual-motor deficits and clumsiness in the child with
Walton, & Court, 1965; Kephart, 1960; Walton, Ellis, & learning disabilities. It was hypothesized that clumsy
Court, 1963). children with learning disabilities would score signif-
The terms developmental dyspraXia or clumsy icantly lower on Visual-perceptual and visual-motor
have been used to label children with a motor plan- tests than nonclumsy children with learning disabili-
ning problem. Gubbay (1985) described the clumsy ties, and that nonclumsy children with learning dis-
child as one "whose ability to perform skilled purpo- abilities would score significantly lower than children
sive movement is impaired, yet whose motor coordi- without learning disabilities. It was further hypothe-
nation is Virtually normal by the standards of routine, sized that there would be a significant correlation be-
conventional neurological assessment and who also tween the degree of clumsiness and the degree of
has normal bodily habits, intellect, physical strength Visual-perceptual and visual-motor deficit in the sub-
and sensory function" (p. 159). Ayres (1979) noted jects with learning disabilities.
that the clumsy child seems to have "less of a sense of
his body and what it can do" (p. 101).
Method
Clinical manifestations appear to be varied. Gen-
eral awkwardness, poor tactile perception abilities, Subjects
inadequate body scheme, delayed acqUisition of daily
liVing skills, poor gross and fine motor skills, and Forty-four children participated in the study. Twenty-
articulation deficits are a number of characteristics two had learning disabilities (age range 5 years to 8
frequently attributed to the dyspraxic child (Ayres, years 10 months) and the remaining 22, who had no
1979; Cermak, 1985; Knuckey, Apsimon, & Gubbay, learning disabilities, served as a control group (age
1983). range 5 years 6 months to 8 years 11 months). The
Visual-perceptual and visual-motor deficits have children were matched as closely as possible for age
been described as two of the many performance defi- and sex. Thirty-two of the subjects were male (16 with
cits associated with the clumsy child with learning disabilities, 16 without) and 12 were female (6 with
disabilities. These components are particularly rele- disabilities, 6 without).
vant because some researchers have suggested that The children participating in the study attended
visual-perceptual and visual-motor difficulties under- public and private schools within the Greater Boston
lie the clumsiness seen in some children with learn- area. Each child in the control group was in an age-
ing disabilities (Henderson & Hall, 1982; Hulme, appropriate grade and had no special education re-
Biggerstaff, Moran, & McKinley, 1982). Hulme, Big- quirements and no history of receiving remedial help.
gerstaff, Moran, and McKinley studied 16 clumsy and Each child in the experimental group had a diagnosed
16 normal children in a task of matching the length of learning disability and was receiving special services
lines between and within the modalities of vision and for his or her specific disability. The children with
kinesthesia. These researchers found that the visual learning disabilities (the LD group) were divided into
condition and motor performance correlated highly. two groups-"clumsy" and "nonclumsy"-based on
Although adequate kinesthetic sense was described to their scores on the Test of Motor Impairment (Stott,
be necessary to discriminate body position and am- Moyes, & Henderson, 1984). Of the LD group, 13
plitude of movement, kinesthetic and intersensory were classified as clumsy and 9 as nonclumsy. It
conditions did not correlate with motor ability. In a should be noted that 3 subjects in the control group
follow-up study (Hulme, Smart, & Moran, 1982) re- scored in the mild-clumsy range and the rest scored in
sults continued to reveal increased Visual-perceptual the nonclumsy range. (See Tables 1 and 2 for a de-
deficits in clumsy children. The authors suggested scription of subject groups.)

360 June 1988, Volume 42, Number 6


Table 1 year-olds was used. This test was considered a mea-
Description of Subjects sure of visual perception.
Children With Learning 3. Block Design (Wechsler, 1974). This subtest
Disabilities of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Re-
Control Group
Clumsy NoncJumsy vised, requires reproduction of a presented two-di-
Age n Male Female Male Female Male Female mensional deSign by block manipulation. The test is
5 10 3 2 2 1 1 1 standardized on 6- to 16-year-olds. Scaled scores were
6 11 6 0 3 0 2 0 used, with scores extrapolated for the 5-year-olds.
7 12 3 2 3 1 1 2 4. Primary Visual Motor Test (Haworth, 1970).
8 11 4 2 2 1 2 0
This test consists of 16 designs, shown one at a time,
that the child is reqUired to copy. Suitable for 4- to
8-year-olds, it is used to assess visual-motor develop-

Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/42/6/359/53515/359.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 30 September 2022
Procedure ment and to evaluate deviation in visual motor func-
One visual-perceptual test, four visual-motor tests, tioning. Raw scores, the total number of errors, and
and the Test of Motor Impairment (TMI) were ad- category scores (impaired, or average to above aver-
ministered to each child by the same examiner in a age) were used in data analysis.
session lasting 1!4 to 1 Y2 hours. The children were 5. Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (Waber &
tested either at their homes or at the Boston Univer- Holmes, 1985). This test, recently described in a for-
sity Occupational Therapy Clinic. mat for children, requires the child to reproduce a
The series of tests, which was administered ac- complex design using five different colored pencils
cording to the directions in the test manual for each in a specified time limit. An organization score based
test, was given in the follOWing order for each child: on the correct features drawn in the design was re-
corded in standard deviation scores.
1. Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integra- 6. Test of Motor Impairment (TMI) (StOtt,
tion, Revised (Beery & Buktenica, 1980). This test Moyes, & Henderson, 1984) This screening test is
contains a series of 24 geometric forms that are to be designed to measure the degree of motOr impairment
copied with pencil in the protocol book. Normed on in children aged 5 years and up. Children are given a
2- to 15-year-olds, it is used to assess visual-motor series of 8 tasks at their age range that evaluate manual
integration. A visual-motor age-equivalent score is dexterity, static balance, dynamic balance, and ball
obtained based on the number of correct forms com- skills. Scores are interpreted as 0 to 3.5-no impair-
pleted up to three consecutive failures. In addition to ment, 4.0 to 55-mild to moderate impairment, and
the age-equivalent score, a visual-motor age differ- above 6.0-definite impairment. For the purposes of
ence score, reported in months, was computed for use this study, 0 to 3.5 was considered normal and scores
in this study by subtracting the child's chronological 4.0 and above were considered impaired, or clumsy.
age from his or her obtained visual-motor age-equiva-
lent score Results
2. Raven ProgreSSive Matrices (Raven, 1960). In order to test the hypothesis that clumsy children
This scale consists of 60 problems divided into five with learning disabilities will score significantly
sets of 12. The problems are meaningless figures that lower on the Visual-perceptual and visual-motor tests
require observation, derivation of relationships be- than nonclumsy children with learning disabilities,
tween the figures, and reasoning to complete each and that nonclumsy children with learning disabilities
figure. The test is suitable for ages 6 years to adult. A will score Significantly lower than children without
percentile score, obtained by extrapolation for the 5- learning disabilities, one·way between-group (clumsy
LD, nonclumsy LD, and control) analyses of variance
(ANOVAs) on each dependent measure were per-
Table 2
Age in Months and TMI Score for Children With and formed. The means and standard deviation for each
Without Learning Disabilities group on each test as well as the F values of the
Children With Learning ANOVAs are presented in Table 3. As noted in this
Disabilities table, there were between-group differences on each
Control
Group Clumsy Nonclumsy of the visual-perceptual and visual-motor tests.
11 22 13 9
Scheffe multiple comparisons were performed to see
Age Mean 8450 8392 85.00 where the differences were. A consistent pattern was
lin months) SD 1485 1228 1387 found, with significant differences on each measure
TMI score Mean 187 796 2.28 between the control group and the clumsy LD group
SD 149 2.23 1.12 but no other significant between-group differences.
Note. TM1 = Test of MotOr Impairment. In order to test the hypothesis that there would

The American Journal oj Occupational Therapy 361


Table 3
Scores on the Visual-Perceptual Motor Tests for Children With and Without Learning Disabilities
Children With Learning
Disabilities
Control ANOVA
Test Group Clumsy Nonclumsy F(2, 41)
Visual Motor Integration
(age equivalent, months) x 97.32 7246 81.56 3.59"
SO 3078 21.96 24.70
Raven Matrices
(percentile score) X 9409 7661 88.11 4.88"
so 11.16 16.75 2376
WISC·R Block Design
(scale score) X 1377 9.00 11.67 7.15""

Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/42/6/359/53515/359.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 30 September 2022
SO 3.25 3.98 394
Primary Visual Motor
(raw error score) X 12.91 2854 1956 10.29" "
SO 839 11.61 10.54
Rey·Osterrieth Complex Figure
(SO score) X 021 -0.86 -0.56 6.57""
SO 1.11 0.34 0.76
"p< .05. "" P < .01.

be a significant correlation between the degree of perceptual and visual-motor tests than nonclumsy
clumsiness and the degree of visual· perceptual and children with learning disabilities and that nonclumsy
visual-motor deficit in children with learning disabili- children with learning disabilities will, in turn, score
ties, a Spearman rank order coefficient of correlation significantly lower than children without learning dis-
was used. Significant correlations (p < .05) were ob- abilities was only partially supported. For all visual-
tained between the TMI score and the Visual-percep- perceptual and visual-motor measures, the children in
tual test (Raven Progressive Matrices) as well as be- the clumsy LD group scored significantly lower than
tween the TMI and two of the three visual-motor tests the children in the control group, but the difference
(see Table 4). A point· biserial correlation was used to between the nonclumsy LD group and the control
examine the correlation between the TMI and the group and between the two LD groups-clumsy and
Primary Visual Motor Test because the scores on the nonclumsy-was not significant.
Primary Visual Motor Test result in placement in ei- One interpretation of the results is that there are
ther the impaired or the average·to·above·average indeed subgroups of learning disabilities, and one
category. A significant correlation, r = -0.44 way of categorizing these subgroups is based on
(t = -(-2.40), P < .05), was found, supporting the motor competence. Mean scores for all tests were best
hypothesis. in the control group and worst in the clumsy LD
group, indicating a trend in scores. It may be that
Discussion there was a difference between the nonclumsy and
the clumsy LD group, but, because of the small sam-
The hypothesis that clumsy children with learning ple size, the difference was not perceptible. The re-
disabilities will score significantly lower on visual- sults do suggest that visual-perceptual abilities are

Table 4
Correlations Between TMI, VMI, Raven, WISC-R Block, and Rey-Osterrieth Tests for Children With Learning Disabilities
(N = 22)
Raven WISe-R Rey·Osterrieth
TMI VMI Matrices Block Design Complex Figure
TMI: No. of errors -400" -,395" -4070 -.225
VMI age difference score" 438" .620"" 426"
Raven Matrices
percentile score 451" -.048
WISe-R Block Design
scale score .328
Rey·Osterrieth Complex
Figure SD score
Note. TMI = Test of Motor Impairment. VMI = Developmental Test of Visual Motor Integration. WISe-R = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children, Revised.
'VMI age difference score obtained by subtracting chronological age from VMI age·equivalent score.
" p < .05. ""p < .01.

362 June 1988, Volume 42, Number 6


clearly not superior in people with learning disabili- Bush, W. J-, & Waugh, K. W. (1982). Diagnosing learn-
ties, as was proposed by Geschwind and Galaburda ing problems. Columbus, OH: Charles Merrill.
Cermak, S. A. (1985). Developmental dyspraxia. In
(1985).
E. A. Roy (Ed.), Neuropsychological studies of apraxia and
Degree of clumsiness did appear, indeed, to be related disorders (pp. 225-243). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
significantly related to the degree of visual-perceptual Geschwind, N., & Galaburda, A. M. (1985). Cerebral
and visual-motor deficit in that the clumsier the child lateralization. Archives ofNeurology, 42, 428-459.
was the greater was the visual-perceptual and visual- Gubbay, S. S. (1978). The management of develop-
mental dyspraxia. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu-
motor problem. Whether the clumsiness is the result
rology, 20, 448-460.
of visual-perceptual and visual-motor deficits, as sug- Gubbay, S. S. (1985). Clumsiness. In P. J Vinken,
gested by some researchers (Henderson & Hall, 1982; G. W. Bruyn, & H. L. Klawans (Eds.), Handbook of clinical
Hulme, Biggerstaff, Moran, & McKinley, 1982; neurology (pp. 159-167). New York: Elsevier.
Hulme, Smart, & Moran, 1982), or whether the visual- Gubbay, S. S., Ellis, T., Walton, J N., & Court, S. D. M.

Downloaded from http://research.aota.org/ajot/article-pdf/42/6/359/53515/359.pdf by Integracion Sensorial Acis, Adriana Ramirez on 30 September 2022
(1965). Clumsy children: A study of apraxia and agnosic
perceptual and visual-motor difficulties and clumsi- deficits in 21 children. Brain, 88,295-312.
ness all are the result of tactile and kinesthetic pro- Haworth, M. (1970) Primary Visual Motor Test New
cessing dysfunction, as noted by Ayres (1966, 1969, York: Grune & Stratton.
1979,1985), remains unclear and will be explored in Henderson, A. E., & Hall, D. (1982). Concomitants of
a future study. Further examination of the relationship clumsiness in young schoolchildren. Developmental Medi-
cine and Child Neurology, 24,461-467.
between these factors is needed because this rela- Hulme, c., Biggerstaff, A., Moran, G., & McKinley, I.
tionship would appear to have implications for the (1982). Visual, kinesthetic and cross-modal judgements of
treatment of the clumsy child with learning disabili- length by normal and clumsy children. Developmental
ties. For example, if the underlying problem involves Medicine and Child Neurology, 24,461-471.
an impairment in tactile and kinesthetic processing, Hulme, c., Smart, S., & Moran, G. (1982) Visual per-
ceptual deficits in clumsy children Neuropsychologia, 4,
then treatment would be aimed at improving sensory 475-481
processing, and procedures such as sensory integra- Kephart, N. (1960). The slow learner in the classroom.
tion would be emphasized. On the other hand, if the Columbus, OH: Charles Merrill.
problem underlying the child's clumsiness is visual- Knuckey, N. W., Apsimon, T. T., & Gubbay, S. S.
perceptual in nature, then remediation might focus (1983) Computerized axial tomography in clumsy children
with developmental apraxia and agnosia. Brain and Devel-
on visual-spatial analysis and/or activities involving opment, 5, 14-20.
the integration of visual and motor components of Orton, S. T. (1937). Reading, writing and speech
performance. As yet, the causative relationship is problems in children. New York: W. W. Norton.
speculative. Raven, J c. (1960). Guide to the Standard Progressive
Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis.
Rourke, B. P. (1985). Neuropsychology of learning dis-
References
abilities: Essentials ofsub-type analysis. New York: Guilford
Ayres, A. ]. (1966) Interrelationships among percep- Press
tual-motor functions in children. American journal of Oc- Stott, D., Moyes, F. A., & Henderson, S. E. (1984). Man-
cupational Therapy, 20, 68-71. ual of Test of Motor Impairment-Henderson Revision.
Ayres, A. J (1969). Deficits in sensory integration in Ontario: Brook Educational.
educationally handicapped children. Journal of Learning Strauss, A. A., & Lehtinen, L. E. (1947). Psychopathol-
Disabilities,2,160-168. ogy and education of the brain-injured child. New York:
Ayres, A.]. (1979). Sensory integration and the child. Grune & Stratton.
Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Waber, D P., & Holmes,]. M. (1985) Assessing chil-
Ayres, A. ]. (1985). Developmental dyspraXia and dren's copy productions of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Fig-
adult-onset apraxia. Torrance, CA: Sensory Integration In· ure. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychol-
ternational. ogy, 7, 264-280
Beery, K., & Buktenica, M. (1980). Developmental Test Walton, H. M., Ellis, E, & Court, S. D M. (1963).
of Visual Motor Integration, Revised. Chicago: Follett. Clumsy children: Developmental apraxia and agnosia.
Brenner, M. W., Gilman, S., Zangwill, O. L., & Farrell, Brain, 85, 603-612.
M. (1967). Visuomotor disability in schoolchildren. British Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Medicaljournal, 4, 259-262. Children-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 363

You might also like