You are on page 1of 8

Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée 61 (2011) 43–50

Original article

The effects of inattention on selective attention: How sadness and ruminations


alter attention functions evaluated with the Attention Network Test
Les effets de l’inattention sur l’attention sélective : comment la tristesse et les ruminations
altèrent les fonctions attentionnelles évaluées avec l’Attention Network Test
C. Pêcher a,∗ , C. Quaireau b , C. Lemercier a , J.-M. Cellier a
a
Cognition, langues, langage et ergonomie (CLLE), UTM, EPHE, CNRS, maison de la recherche, université de Toulouse-le-Mirail, 5, allée Antonio-Machado,
31058 Toulouse cedex 9, France
b
Laboratoire de psychologie expérimentale (LPE), CRP2C, UPRES EA 1285, université de Rennes 2 - Haute-Bretagne, place du Recteur-Henri-le-Moal,
35043 Rennes cedex, France

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In two in-laboratory studies, we investigated the influence of inattention when caused by sadness and
Received 17 October 2009 ruminations on attention functions. Being sad and ruminating over negative thoughts involves a long-
Received in revised form 22 October 2010 lasting attentional self-focus, altering the ability to process relevant information from the environment. It
Accepted 25 October 2010
subsequently leads to inattention, considered as a very specific attention deficit. In a preliminary method-
ological study, it was demonstrated that the combined imagery vignettes and music procedure was
Keywords: efficient to induce sadness and ruminations, propitious to inattention. Then, in a second experiment,
Inattention
sadness and ruminations were induced with this procedure in order to provoke inattention. The effects
Sadness
Ruminations
of inattention on selective attention processes were assessed using the Attention Network Test, which
Attention Network Test discriminates between three independent attention functions: alerting, orienting and conflict. Results on
Selective attention reaction times showed that only the orienting function was affected when inattentive, with a significant
alteration on spatial information processing. These results were discussed in the light of the literature on
emotion and attention.
© 2010 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.

r é s u m é

Mots clés : Dans ces deux études réalisées en laboratoire, nous avons examiné l’impact de l’inattention, causée par la
Inattention tristesse et les ruminations associées à cet état, sur les fonctions attentionnelles. Être triste et ruminer des
Tristesse pensées négatives implique un repli sur soi attentionnel durable, altérant notre capacité à traiter les infor-
Ruminations mations pertinentes de l’environnement. Ce phénomène génère ainsi de l’inattention, considéré comme
Attention Network Test
un défaut de l’attention très spécifique. Tout d’abord, une étude méthodologique préliminaire a démontré
Attention sélective
que la procédure combinant la présentation de vignettes et de musique permettait d’induire efficace-
ment de la tristesse et des ruminations, propices à l’inattention. Ensuite, dans une seconde expérience, la
tristesse et les ruminations ont été induites à l’aide de cette procédure, dans l’objectif de provoquer une
situation propice à l’inattention. Les effets de l’inattention ont ensuite été mesurés grâce à l’Attention
Network Test, qui distingue trois fonctions attentionnelles indépendantes : l’alerte, l’orientation et le
conflit. Les résultats sur les temps de réactions montrent que seule la fonction d’orientation est détéri-
orée lorsque le sujet est inattentif, montrant des effets délétères lors du traitement des informations
spatiales. Ces résultats seront discutés au regard de la littérature sur les émotions et sur l’attention.
© 2010 Elsevier Masson SAS. Tous droits réservés.

1. Introduction

Task efficiency in daily activities depends on the capability to


select relevant information and inhibit distractors. Thus, dysfunc-
∗ Corresponding author. tions of these two processes are supposed to generate maladjusted
E-mail address: cpecher@univ-tlse2.fr (C. Pêcher). attention behaviours such as inattention. According to Lemercier

1162-9088/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.erap.2010.10.003
44 C. Pêcher et al. / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée 61 (2011) 43–50

and Cellier (2008), inattention reflects a very specific deficit of 2008; Wang et al., 2005). Consequently, the ANT seems useful to
selective attention, which has to be drawn up Posner’s concept of measure the effects of inattention, due to sadness and ruminations
endogenous orienting of attention (Posner, 1980). Precisely, it is on attention functions.
the consequence of a long-lasting orienting of attention to a latent, We challenge then the relevance and validity of the ANT to test
non-salient, irrelevant and endogenous stimulus. In other words, the impact of inattention on the three independent attention func-
inattention is a maladaptive orientation of attention on personal tions. Plus, we examine the real impact of sadness and ruminations,
thoughts, i.e. attention self-focus, which occurs to the detriment of considered here as main causes of inattention on alerting, orienting
the processing of other relevant information. and conflict. Because inattention is precisely defined as a durable
Attentional self-focus is a main component in sadness (Frijda, orientation of attention on negative thoughts, we presume that
1986; Lazarus, 1991) and ruminations, i.e. repetitive negative inattention will alter principally the orienting function.
thoughts that capture attention for a long period of time and pre- Yet, before testing this hypothesis, we will propose a prelimi-
serve the negative emotional state (Joorman and Gotlib, 2008; nary methodological study. It was conducted in order to determine
McLaughlin et al., 2007; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991, 2000; Nolen- the efficiency of two induction procedures, i.e. the autobiographical
Hoeksema et al., 2008). Specifically, ruminations related to sadness recall and the imagery procedure, to provoke sadness and rumina-
reflect a processing of repetitive negative thoughts that bear on tions, propitious to inattention.
circumstances of one’s present sadness and which interfere with
problem resolution and imposed goal-oriented tasks (Conway 2. Study 1: methodological preliminaries
et al., 2000). Indeed, an abundant literature demonstrated an
interference of both sadness and ruminations on information pro- In this paper, we hypothesized that sadness and ruminations
cessing and particularly on attention processes (e.g., Huffziger give rise to attention self-focus, leading to inattention and sub-
and Kuehner, 2009; Watkins and Teadale, 2001). For instance, sequently affect attention functions, when performing the ANT.
Philippot and Brutoux (2008) used a traditional Stroop test to mea- Before experimenting, a requisite is that sadness and ruminations
sure the effects of dysphoria and ruminations on the inhibition have to be strong and durable to generate ruminations and then
and flexibility functions, in control and dysphoric groups. The two inattention. With regards to the abundant literature on induced
groups were induced in order to ruminate or to be distracted, sadness and depression (e.g., Clark, 1983; Scherrer and Dobson,
creating four experimental conditions: control/rumination, con- 2009; Velten, 1968), induction procedures appear as an interest-
trol/distraction, dysphoric/rumination and dysphoric/distraction. ing experimental alternative to natural emotions. There are a large
Results indicated a degradation of the ability to impede irrele- number of induction procedures including mood self-statements,
vant information when dysphoric and ruminating, compared to social feedback, hypnotic suggestion, film, music, etc. (for a detailed
the three other conditions. Additionally, the cognitive flexibility review, see Gerrards-Hesse et al., 1994, Juslin et al., 2008; Gilet,
was highly deteriorated for this group. Ruminations then alter 2008; Martin, 1990). According to Martin (1990), procedures which
the ability to inhibit and disengage attention to a different infor- require participants’ imagination are one of the most efficient.
mation source (Joorman et al., 2007; Leung et al., 2009). To our This category notably includes self-statements (Velten, 1968), auto-
point of view, those results also provide evidence that sadness and biographical recall (Brewer et al., 1980) and combined imagery
ruminations provoke a long-lasting attention self-focus, leading vignettes and music procedure (Mayer et al., 1995).
to inattention, and affect attention control as defined by Posner This first experiment was thus performed to compare the effi-
(1980). ciency of the autobiographical recall and the imagery induction
Indeed, Posner (1980) and later, Fan et al. (2002) characterized procedure, to provoke both sadness and ruminations in normal par-
attention control as a combination of three attention functions: ticipants. It is also pointed out that some questionnaires such as the
alerting, orienting and conflict (the executive control). Alerting Response Style Questionnaire (RSQ, Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991) or the
helps in the performance of continuous and vigilance tasks (acti- Beck Depression Inventory (BDI1A, Beck et al., 1996) help in dis-
vation of different levels of alertness), orienting involves directing criminating the presence, the strength and the type of ruminations.
attention focus to a located stimulus to select information. Execu- Nevertheless, those questionnaires are principally addressed to a
tive control, finally, requires the resolution of conflict among items. clinical depressed population. Because only normal people were
The three functions have been tested with the Attention Network recruited in our experiment, we used alternative techniques with
Test (ANT), which is a visual-attention task combining a flanker standard questionnaires and a post-experimental interview to be
task and a cued reaction time task (Fan et al., 2002). As it is showed sure of the presence of sadness and ruminations.
in Supplementary data, Fig. 1b, the target is a leftward or right-
ward arrowhead surrounded by flankers, which can be neutral (no 2.1. Method
direction), congruent (same direction), or incongruent (opposite
direction) with the target. Each target is preceded by a cue of four 2.1.1. Participants
types: no-cue, double, central and spatial (Supplementary data, Fig. Forty-five undergraduate students from the University of
1a). In their first study, Fan et al. (2002) tested the ANT efficiency Toulouse-2 voluntarily participated in the experiment. The sample
and reliability with 40 normal adults. Results showed that alerting, consisted of 20 males and 25 females, ranging from 21 to 35 years
orienting and conflict are not correlated, supporting the indepen- (mean = 26.33 years, S.D. = 3.45 years). All had normal or corrected-
dence of the three functions. Precisely, alerting was characterized to-normal vision. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the
by a benefit of 47 ms when double cues were presented compared three experimental conditions: neutral induction (control; n = 16),
to no cues presentation. Orienting was characterized by a benefit sad induction with the autobiographical recall (n = 15) and sad
of 51 ms when spatial cues were presented compared to central induction with the imagery procedure (n = 14).
cues presentation. Finally, conflict was characterized by a benefit
of 84 ms when flankers were congruent with the target, compared 2.1.2. Materials
to incongruent flankers. 2.1.2.1. Experimental inductions. Neutral induction required the
The ANT is a reliable and valid tool, which has already been participant to constantly focus on technical characteristics of eight
used to test the relationship between attention and working mem- complex classic 1-min music pieces (e.g., prelude in B minor, op.28,
ory capacity (Redick and Engle, 2006) or specific disorders such as No. 6 by Chopin). They had to determine the number and the type of
schizophrenia or ADHD (Adólfsdóttir et al., 2008; Odludas et al., all instruments for each excerpt. At the beginning of each excerpt,
C. Pêcher et al. / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée 61 (2011) 43–50 45

a vignette “Excerpt no. 1”, “Excerpt no. 2” etc. was presented on a found (F(1,42) = 14.32, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.25) with an increase of sad-
computer-screen to guide participants. ness scores between the beginning and the end of the experiment
Sad induction with the autobiographical recall consisted of an (MPanas1 = 6.11, MPanas2 = 7.83). Finally, the interaction between the
adaptation of Brewer et al. (1980) procedure. As for the neutral condition and the stage of experiment (F(2,42) = 8.65, P < 0.001;
induction, the same eight complex classic 1-min music pieces were 2 = 0.29) was significant. Post-hoc analyses showed no differences
presented. For each excerpt, participants had to focus on the emo- on scores of sadness after the neutral induction (t < 1; sad1 = 5.68;
tional nature of music and on their emotions, and simultaneously sad2 = 5.56) and after the sad induction with the autobiographi-
they had to mentally recall a sad personal event. At the beginning cal recall (t(14) = −1.52; ns; sad1 = 6.93; sad2 = 7.86). Nevertheless,
of each excerpt, a vignette “Excerpt no. 1”, “Excerpt no. 2” etc. was after the sad induction with the imagery procedure, there was an
presented on a computer-screen to guide participants. important increase of the score of sadness (t(13) = −3.36; P < 0.001;
Finally, sad induction with the combined imagery and music sad1 = 5.71; sad2 = 10.07)
procedure consisted of the translated French version of the orig-
inal combined imagery vignettes and music procedure developed 2.3.1.2. Joviality. Analysis showed no significant effect of the con-
by Mayer et al. (1995). Each vignette was presented on a PC com- dition (F < 1; MControl = 23.40, MRecall = 23.66, MImagery = 22.96). A
puter for 1 min, consecutively in a fixed order. Each vignette was a main effect of the stage of the experiment was found F(1,42) = 27.91,
descriptive sentence of a sad situation (e.g, “You are told by a young P < 0.001; 2 = 0.39), indicating a decrease of the score of jovi-
relative that he has cancer and only six months to live”). Instructions ality after inductions (MPanas1 = 25.67, MPanas2 = 21.02). Finally,
for participants were to read and imagine themselves in each situ- there was an interaction between the condition and the stage of
ation and to feel the kind of emotions created by such a situation. experiment (F(2,42) = 5.70; P < 0.01; 2 = 0.21). Post-hoc analyses
In the background, the same eight complex classic 1-min music indicated no differences on scores of joviality after the neutral
pieces than for the neutral and sad inductions were presented to induction (t(15) = 1.07; ns; jov1 = 24.12; jov2 = 22.68). It appeared
immediately reinforce the emotional effects. a slight decrease of the score of joviality after the sad induction
with the autobiographical recall (t(14) = 2.61; P < 0.05; jov1 = 25.60;
2.1.2.2. Emotional self-rating scale. Changes in the emotional state jov2 = 21.70). Finally, after the sad induction with the imagery pro-
were assessed using the Positive Affect and Negative Affect Sched- cedure, there was an important decrease of the score of joviality
ule, Expanded form (PANAS-X, Watson and Clark, 1999). This scale (t(13) = 4.87; P < 0.001; jov1 = 27.28; jov2 = 18.64)
includes 60 items describing emotions, feelings, and states (e.g.,
“happy”, “sad”, “energetic” etc.). For each item, participants rated
2.3.1.3. Attention. Analysis revealed no significant effect of the con-
how they felt on a five point-Lickert scale from 1 “I do not feel
dition (F < 1; MControl = 12.37, MImagery = 12, MRecall = 12). There was a
at all” to 5 “I extremely feel”. Although the PANAS-X allows the
main effect of the stage of the experiment (F(1,42) = 20.20, P < 0.001;
measurement of general negative and positive affects as well as
2 = 0.32), which shows a higher score of attention before induc-
11 specific emotional states, we interested in items for only three
tion than after (MPanas1 = 12.89, MPanas2 = 11.35). Finally, there was
specific states: sadness, joviality and attention. The PANAS-X was
no interaction between the condition and the stage of experiment
presented two times: before and after the induction.
(F(2,20) = 1.78, ns)

2.1.2.3. Post-experimental interviews. In order to provide comple-


2.3.2. Post-experimental interviews
mentary qualitative information, interviews were performed after
After the experiment, all participants were interviewed con-
the induction for approximately 20 min. Participants were asked
cerning their emotions, their thoughts and perceived changes
about their difficulties and how they felt during the experiment. It
due to the induction. First, participants who received a neu-
led them to discuss not only changes in their emotional state but
tral induction (controls) all explained they did not feel changes
also changes in their way of thinking including the type of thoughts,
after the induction. Because they had to focus on technical char-
the duration and its perceived impact on their behaviours.
acteristics of complex music pieces, all of them argued they
processed information related to music and instruments (e.g.
2.2. Procedure
“Here, I hear a typical string instrument and it must be a violin”).
Finally, 68.75% of participants said that they sometimes dealt with
Experiments were run individually. At their arrival, participants
neutral factual thoughts (e.g. “There is a crow outside”) or with
were randomly assigned to one of the three induction conditions
prospective memory (e.g. “I must go to the bakery after the exper-
and then filled the PANAS-X for the first time. After it, they were
iment”).
induced, they filled the PANAS-X for the second time before being
Secondly, participants who received a sad induction with the
interviewed about their feelings, thoughts etc. Finally, they were
autobiographical recall all affirmed that they felt “sadder”, “in a
thanked for their participation.
bad mood” and “nervous” after the induction. Nevertheless, they
all voluntarily recalled only slightly sad events as they found the
2.3. Results autobiographical recall intrusive and difficult. In consequence, 66%
of participants preferred to think about future events such as a
2.3.1. Emotional manipulation checks planned abroad-journey or the technical characteristics of music
A repeated measure Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was con- excerpts.
ducted on scores of sadness, joviality and attention, using the Thirdly, participants who received a sad induction with
condition (control, sad induction with the autobiographical recall, the imagery procedure all observed an important negative
sad induction with the imagery procedure) as the between-subjects change after induction as they felt “sadder”, “overwhelmed”,
factor and the stage of the experiment (Panas 1 and Panas 2) as “angry and disgusted” and “depressed”. For 84.61% of par-
within-subjects factor. ticipants, sad music excerpts helped in imagining dramatic
situations and in easily linking situations with dramatic per-
2.3.1.1. Sadness. Analysis revealed a significant effect of the condi- sonal events by creating a “heavy”, “moody” and “sad atmo-
tion (F(2,42) = 3,77; P < 0.05; 2 = 0.15; MControl = 5.62, MRecall = 7.40, sphere”. As a consequence, 92.85% of participants regularly and
MImagery = 7.89). A main effect of the stage of the experiment was durably focused on the true meaning of sentences and on
46 C. Pêcher et al. / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée 61 (2011) 43–50

other personal things such as the death of one of their rel- Russia under the Mongol Yoke”, Prokofiev, 1939). The negative induc-
atives, a past separation with their lover, a personal failure, tion was used for two groups: sad induction in phase 1 (sad
etc. induction and no music during the ANT) and induction in phases 1
and 2 (sad induction and sad music during the ANT).
2.4. Discussion
3.1.2.2. Music during the ANT session. In the control and sad induc-
As a basis for the measurement of the impact of inattention, tion in phase 1 conditions, the ANT session was performed without
due to sadness and ruminations, on attention functions, this pre- music.
liminary methodological study aimed to test the efficiency of On the contrary, in the sad induction in phase 2 only and induc-
the autobiographical recall and the combined imagery vignettes tion in phases 1 and 2 conditions, the ANT was realized with sad
and music procedure to induce sadness and ruminations. Results music in background (e.g., “Alexander Nevsky op.78, Russia under
showed that only the combined imagery and music procedure the Mongol Yoke”, Prokofiev, 1939). In accordance with Pêcher et al.
(Mayer et al., 1995) provoke an important increase of sadness (2009), music here was supposed to create a heavy and dramatic
and simultaneously a decrease of joviality. Additionally, post- background, propitious to ruminations.
interviews provided additional information on how participants
felt and on the nature of their thoughts. Whereas sad induction with 3.1.2.3. Emotional self-rating scale. Emotional changes were
the autobiographical recall led participants to plan events as well assessed using the Brief Mood Introspection Scale (BMIS, Mayer
as to reason on the task or music, sad induction with the imagery and Gaschke, 1988). It consists of 16 adjectives covering eight
procedure provoked a durable self-focus attention on negative per- emotions: happiness, loving, energetic, calmness, tiredness, fear,
sonal thoughts. sadness and anger. Participants were instructed to rate each
In sum, the imagery procedure appeared as the most efficient to adjective on a four-point Likert scale from 1 “definitively do not
induce sadness and to initiate ruminations propitious to inatten- feel” to 4 “definitively feel” on the basis of what they felt at the exact
tion. Thus, in our second experiment, this procedure was used again time they were completing the questionnaire.
to test the effects of inattention, due to sadness and ruminations, The pleasant–unpleasant scale was scored via subtraction by
on attention functions. Besides the sad induction, sad music was firstly summing up scores for all pleasant moods (happiness, loving,
presented while performing the ANT (Fan et al., 2002) to maintain energy and calmness), then summing up scores for all unpleas-
the previous induction effects. ant moods (tiredness, fear, sadness and anger). The total score was
obtained by subtracting the unpleasant total from the pleasant
3. Study 2: measurement of the impact of inattention on total. The Brief Mood Introspection Scale was completed three
attention functions times during the experiment: before the induction (BMIS 1), just
after the induction (BMIS 2) and after the ANT session (BMIS 3).
3.1. Method
3.1.2.4. The Attention Network Test. The Attention Network Test
3.1.1. Participants determines the efficiency of three independent attention functions:
Sixty-nine undergraduate students from the University of alerting, orienting and conflict (for a more detailed presentation of
Rennes 2 voluntarily participated in the experiment. The sam- the ANT, see Fan et al., 2002). Response times are dependent on
ple consisted of six males and 63 females, ranging from 19 to 24 alerting cues, orienting cues and flankers. Stimuli were presented
years of age (mean = 19.20 years, S.D. = 1.18 years). All had normal or via PROLAB, a commercial experiment application, on a Windows-
corrected-to-normal vision. Participants were randomly assigned PC, presenting to a 15-inch DELL monitor. Participants viewed the
to one of the four experimental conditions: control (n = 17), sad screen from a distance of 65 cm, and responses were collected via
induction in phase 1 (n = 19), sad induction in phase 2 (n = 19) and two input keys on the keyboard. Stimuli consisted of a row of five
both induction in phases 1 and 2 (n = 14). arrows pointing left or right, against a grey background. Partic-
ipants were instructed to determine the direction of the central
3.1.2. Materials arrow (the target) by pressing the “Q” key for the left direction and
3.1.2.1. Experimental inductions. Four experimental conditions the “M” key for the right direction. As shown in Supplementary
(control, sad induction in phase 1, sad induction in phase 2 and data, Fig. 1b, the target was flanked on either side by two arrows in
induction in phases 1 and 2) were created by crossing two main the same direction (congruent condition), in the opposite direction
factors: the induction type (neutral or negative) and the presence (incongruent condition), or by lines (neutral condition).
of sad music during the ANT session to maintain sadness and rumi- The Supplementary data, Fig. 1c presented a sequence of events.
nations (with or without). Both neutral and sad inductions were an Each trial began with a fixation period for random variable duration
adapted version of the procedure developed by Mayer et al. (1995). (400–1600 ms). Then, a warning cue was presented for 100 ms (for
Neutral induction required the participant to read and think cues, see Supplementary data, Fig. 1a). Alerting was due to the four
about eight vignettes, presented consecutively, in a fixed order in types of cue: a neutral cue, a central cue, a double cue and a spa-
Microsoft Power Point running on a PC computer. Each vignette tial cue. There was a fixation period for 400 ms after the warning
lasted 60 s and described a non-emotional situation (e.g., “Words fly, cue and then the target and its flankers appeared above or below
writings remain”). The neutral induction was used for two groups: the fixation point, until the participant responded (no longer than
control (neutral induction and no music during the ANT) and sad 1700 ms). The central arrow and its flankers disappeared imme-
induction in phase 2 (neutral induction and sad music during the diately after the participant’s response and a last fixation period
experimental phase, i.e. the ANT). began (variable duration based on the duration of the first fixa-
Negative induction was the translated French version of the tion and RT) (3500 ms minus duration of the first fixation period
original combined imagery vignette and music procedure devel- minus RT) before a new trial. The three effects were measured as
oped by Mayer et al. (1995). For a detailed description of vignettes following:
and instructions, see Section 2. In the background, similarly to Alerting = RTNo cue − RTDouble cue
Mayer et al. (1995), a sad music excerpt was presented to immedi- Orienting = RTCentral cue − RTSpatial cue
ately reinforce the emotional effects (e.g., “Alexander Nevsky op.78, Conflict = RTIncongruent − RTCongruent
C. Pêcher et al. / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée 61 (2011) 43–50 47

Fig. 1. Scores on the Brief Mood Introspection Scale at the three stages of the experiment (BMIS 1, BMIS 2 and BMIS 3) as a function of the four conditions (control, induction
in phase 1, induction in phase 2 and induction in phases 1 and 2).

3.2. Procedure 3.3.1.2. Induction in phase 1. Statistical differences between scores


on the BMIS 1 and the BMIS 2 were found (MBMIS 1 = 18.65,
The experiment was run with each individual. Upon arrival, MBMIS 2 = 9.65, t(18) = 4.25, P < 0.001). However, differences
the participant was randomly assigned to one of the four condi- between scores on the BMIS 2 and the BMIS 3 were not significant
tions: control, sad induction in phase 1, sad induction in phase 2 (MBMIS 3 = 10.7; t < 1). These results indicated that participants
and induction in phases 1 and 2. He rated the Brief Mood Intro- were sadder just after the sad induction and stayed into it during
spection Scale and received the neutral or sad induction (15 min the ANT session.
approximately). Just after it, he filled in the second BMIS before
being exposed to the ANT for 10 trials. Each participant was 3.3.1.3. Induction in phase 2. No significant differences between
instructed to focus on the central fixation point and to respond scores on the BMIS 1 and the BMIS 2 were found (MBMIS 1 = 22.05,
as quickly and accurately as possible to the target. When the MBMIS 2 = 20.36; t < 1). Nevertheless, differences between scores
training session ended, he began the experimental session for on the BMIS 2 and the BMIS 3 were significant (MBMIS 3 = 11.10;
96 trials. Sad music was added during the task for sad induc- t(18) = 5.19, P < 0.001). The emotional state was not affected by the
tion in phase 2 and sad induction in phases 1 and 2. After the neutral induction. However, sad music during the ANT led partici-
15-min ANT session, the participant filled in the third BMIS. A pants to be sadder.
debriefing took place and the participant was thanked for com-
ing. 3.3.1.4. Induction in phases 1 and 2. Statistical differences between
scores on the BMIS 1 and the BMIS 2 were found (MBMIS 1 = 20.62,
3.3. Results MBMIS 2 = 9.75; t(15) = 3.76, P < 0.001). Additionally, differences
between scores on the BMIS 2 and the BMIS 3 were significant
3.3.1. Emotional manipulation checks (MBMIS 3 = 7.25, BMIS 2–BMIS 3; t(15) = 2.20, P < 0.001). Here, par-
Fig. 1 showed changes of BMIS scores as a function of induction ticipants felt not only sadness after the negative induction but also
groups and stages of the experiment. A repeated measure ANOVA after the presentation of sad music during the ANT.
was conducted by using the condition (control, induction in phase Sad imagery and music inductions were efficient as participant’s
1, induction in phase 2 and inductions in phases 1 and 2) as the emotional state was modified. Additionally, music during the task
between-subjects factor and the stage of the experiment (BMIS 1, reinforced the induction effects.
BMIS 2 and BMIS 3) as within-subjects factor.
Analysis revealed no significant effect of the condition 3.3.2. Performances on the ANT
(MControl = 14.47, MInduction 1 = 12.98, MRumination 2 = 17.84 and Table 1 reports errors rates and mean reaction times (S.D.) for
MInduction 1 + 2 = 12.54; F < 1). A main effect of the stage of the the three attention functions (i.e., alerting, orienting and conflict) as
experiment was found (F(2,136) = 43.78, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.39). It a function of the induction conditions. Error rates were very low in
revealed a linear decrease in BMIS scores between the beginning all conditions. Because no group differences in error rates were evi-
and the end of the experiment (MBMIS 1 = 19.30, MBMIS 2 = 13.42 dent, only mean proportions of errors averaged over groups were
and MBMIS 3 = 10.64; linear contrast: F(1,68) = 67.64; P < 0.001; presented.
2 = 0.49), indicating that participants became sadder as the We carried out a 4 (condition: controls, induction in phase 1,
experiment progressed. Finally, the interaction between condition induction in phase 2 and induction in phases 1 and 2) × 4 (Cue type:
and stage of experiment (F(6,136) = 6.17, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.21) no cue, centre cue, double cue, and spatial cue) × 3 (Flanker type:
was significant. To be clear, we detailed for each condition its neutral, congruent and incongruent) repeated measure ANOVA.
interaction with the stage of experiment factor. Firstly, analysis showed no significant effect of the condition (F < 1).
A main effect of Cue type was showed (F(3,195) = 63.59, P < 0.001;
3.3.1.1. Control. No differences between scores on the BMIS 1 and 2 = 0.49), demonstrating significant alerting (MDouble = 527.53 ms,
the BMIS 2 were found (MBMIS 1 = 15.88, MBMIS 2 = 14; t < 1). Further- MNo cue = 558.83 ms, F(1,65) = 71.24, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.51), and
more, no differences between scores on the BMIS 2 and the BMIS 3 orienting effects (MCentral = 535.15 ms and MSpatial = 514.77 ms;
were observed (MBMIS 3 = 13.52; t < 1). The emotional state was not F(1,65) = 41.50, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.36). A main effect of Flanker type
affected by the neutral induction as it remained stable for the entire was also observed (MNeutral = 516.93 ms, MCongruent = 517.87 ms and
duration of the experiment. MIncongruent = 567.41 ms; F(2,130) = 179.59, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.73),
48 C. Pêcher et al. / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée 61 (2011) 43–50

Table 1
Mean RTs in milliseconds (S.D.) for the three attention functions and error rates in % in each induction condition (control, induction in phase 1, induction in phase 2, induction
in phases 1 and 2).

Induction conditions

Control Induction in phase 1 Induction in phase 2 Induction in phases 1 and 2

Alerting
Double 510.49 (67.80) 518.92 (87.62) 524.24 (81.31) 556.31 (80.90)
No cue 542.45 (58.17) 541.40 (72.10) 558.38 (79.79) 593.08 (96.19)
Difference −31.96 −22.48 −34.14 −36.77

Orienting
Spatial 501.54 (67.04) 493.05 (92.54) 511.73 (73.72) 552.77 (93.58)
Central 524.60 (63.40) 517.71 (82.85) 541.01 (76.37) 557.26 (80.10)
Difference −23.06 −24.66 −29.28 −4.49

Conflict
Congruent 502.69 (66.74) 516.64 (67.06) 518.55 (68.39) 547.15 (86.44)
Incongruent 553.98 (63.80) 551.77 (85.77) 567.18 (71.96) 596.71 (82.01)
Difference −51.29 −35.13 −48.63 −49.56

Error rates in % (S.D.) 1.16 (0.32) 1.20 (0.31) 1.26 (0.31) 1.38 (0.34)

indicating a conflict effect (F(1,65) = 320.18, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.72). A music during the task did not affect any of the three attention
significant interaction between Cue type and Flanker type was also functions.
observed (F(6,390) = 5.49, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.17). RTs were longer in
the no-cue condition compared to others cued conditions. Addi- 3.3.2.4. Induction in phases 1 and 2. An alerting effect was found
tionally, RTs increased in all Cue type conditions for incongruent (F(13) = 9.36, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.42) with shorter reactions in dou-
flankers, compared to neutral and congruent flankers. There were ble cue conditions (556.31 ms) compared to no cue conditions
no significant interactions between condition and Cue type factors (593.08 ms). There was no more orienting effect, F < 1 with simi-
(F < 1), as well as between condition and Flanker type factors lar reaction times in spatial cue conditions (552.77 ms) compared
(F < 1). Finally, there was no triple interaction between condition, to central cue conditions (557.26 ms). Finally, a conflict effect was
Cue type and Flanker type factors (F < 1). Because we interested in observed (F(1,13) = 95.96, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.73) with shorter reac-
the effects of inattention on the three attention functions for each tions for congruent trials (547.15 ms) compared to incongruent
condition, complementary analyses were performed and results trials (596.71 ms). Interestingly, the combination of the sad induc-
are displayed for the four conditions independently. tion and the presentation of sad music during the task degraded
only the orienting function.
3.3.2.1. Control. An alerting effect was found (F(1,16) = 48.83, The three attention functions were tested using the ANT in the
P < 0.001; 2 = 0.75), with faster responses for double cue condi- four induction conditions. In the control condition, we replicated
tions (510.49 ms) compared to no cue conditions (542.45 ms). There results by Fan et al. (2002). In the three other induction conditions,
was also an orienting effect (F(1,16) = 11.89, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.42) analyses on mean reaction times indicated that alerting and conflict
with shorter reactions times for spatial cue conditions (501.54 ms) were not affected. Nevertheless, orienting was deteriorated only in
compared to central cue conditions (524.60 ms). Finally, a conflict the induction in phases 1- and 2-condition.
effect was found (F(1,16) = 125.80, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.88) with shorter
reactions times for congruent trials (502.69 ms) compared to incon-
4. General discussion
gruent trials (553.98 ms).
In this paper, we investigated the consequences of inattention,
3.3.2.2. Induction in phase 1. An alerting effect was observed due to sadness and ruminations, on attention functions. Inattention
(F(1,18) = 14.03, P < 0.001; 2 = 0.43) with shorter responses for was firstly defined as a dysfunction of selective attention, emerg-
double cue conditions (518.92 ms) compared to no cue condi- ing from an endogenous orientation of the attention control, and
tions (541.40 ms). There was an orienting effect (F(1,18) = 18.97, involving deteriorations on performances (Lemercier and Cellier,
P < 0.001; 2 = 0.51) with shorter reaction times for spatial 2008; Posner, 1980). With regards to the literature on emotions,
cue conditions (493.05 ms) compared to central cue conditions it was suggested that inattention would be encouraged in spe-
(517.71 ms) Finally, there was a conflict effect (F(1,18) = 127.23, cific emotional states such as sadness, associated with ruminations.
P < 0.001; 2 = 0.87) with shorter reaction times for congruent tri- Therefore, in a preliminary study, different emotional induction
als (516.64 ms) compared to incongruent trials (551.77 ms). In the procedures were used to create a propitious situation to inatten-
present study, the negative induction did not affect any of the three tion. Results have showed that the combined imagery and music
attention functions. procedure, compared to the autobiographical recall, was the most
efficient to induce sadness and to initiate ruminations, propitious to
3.3.2.3. Induction in phase 2. An alerting effect was found inattention. In a second study, the effects of inattention on atten-
(F(18) = 24.65; P < 0.001; 2 = 0.57) with shorter reactions tion functions, i.e alerting, orienting and conflict, were tested by
times for double cue conditions (524.24 ms) compared to no performing the Attention Network Test (Fan et al., 2002). Because
cue conditions (558.38 ms). Secondly, an orienting effect was inattention reflects a durable endogenous orientation of attention
found (F(1,18) = 28.46; P < 0.001; 2 = 0.61) with shorter reac- on thoughts, we thus hypothesized that it would degrade princi-
tion times in spatial cue conditions (511.73 ms) compared to pally the orienting function.
central cue conditions (541.01 ms). Finally, a conflict effect was First, considering methodological matters, analyses revealed
observed (F(1,18) = 95.96; P < 0.001; 2 = 0.84) with shorter that after the sad induction with the imagery procedure (phase
reaction times for congruent trials (518.55 ms) compared 1), scores to BMIS highly decreased, showing that participants
to incongruent trials (567.18 ms). Again, the presentation of were sadder. Also, participants’ reports attested that this effect was
C. Pêcher et al. / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée 61 (2011) 43–50 49

strengthened when sad music was presented all along the ANT ses- spatial information processing. The study also emphasized the need
sion (phase 2). Music not only evokes emotions in listeners (Juslin to distinguish inattention from other attention deficits. The real
et al., 2008; Krumhansl, 1997; Västfjäll, 2002; Pikler, 1961) but also challenge now is to provide a better understanding of the role of
creates a favourable environment for self-focus and ruminations inattention in driving performances. Therefore, further research
(Mayer et al., 1995; Sutherland et al., 1982). Thus, the combination is required to replicate findings with alternative methods and to
of the imagery procedure and music while performing the task are extend results to a simulated driving environment.
relevant and useful to create inattention.
Second, with regards to reaction times in the control condition Conflicts of interest statement
while performing the ANT, we replicated the effects on the alerting,
orienting and executive networks, as primarily found by Fan et al. There is no conflict of interest.
(2002). In the control condition (neutral induction, no music during
the ANT), an alerting effect was observed (31 ms), indicating that Acknowledgements
double cues activate the nervous system and help in detecting the
target, compared to the no cue situation. An orienting effect was The study was performed as part of the French National Project
also found (23 ms), giving evidence that the target is more rapidly PREDIT-DACOTA AR2 “Défauts d’attention en conduite automobile”
detected when the cue is spatial rather than central. Finally, a con- (2005–2008). The support of the Agence Nationale de la recherche
flict effect was observed (51 ms), pointing that participants are able (ANR) is then gratefully acknowledged. Additionally, the authors
to detect conflict between items and to resolve it more rapidly when would like to express their gratitude to contributors and members
flankers are congruent with the target rather than incongruent. Our of the European project Subvention FEDER for their support.
results support the idea that the ANT is reliable and valid to study
independently the three attention functions. It also highlighted that Appendix A. Supplementary data
the control group is a reference group to which we may compare
the three other induction conditions. Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in
The most important point of discussion concerns the deterio- the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.erap.2010.10.003.
ration of the orienting effect only for the induction in phases 1-
and 2-condition. Here, participants were negatively induced and References
sadness was maintained during the task with music to provoke a
ruminative process, propitious to inattention (Conway et al., 2000; Adólfsdóttir, S., Sørensen, L., Lundervold, A.J., 2008. The attention network test: a
characteristic pattern of deficits in children with ADHD. Behavioural and Brain
Joorman and Gotlib, 2008; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). Interestingly,
Functions 4, 9.
analysis revealed that their reaction times in central and spatial Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., Ball, R., Ranieri W, 1996. Comparison of Beck Depression Inven-
cue conditions were similar. In the ANT, spatial cues are supposed tories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatient. Journal of Personality Assessment 67,
to inform about the potential target location whereas central cues 588–597.
Brewer, D., Doughtie, E.B., Lubin, B., 1980. Induction of mood and mood shift. Journal
only warn about the target arrival. In our study, results showed that of Clinical Psychology 36, 215–226.
inattentive participants were ready to detect the target as the alert- Clark, D.M., 1983. On the induction of depressed mood in the laboratory: the
ing function was not affected. In addition, they were able to resolve evaluation and comparison of the Velten and musical procedure. Advances in
Behavioural Research and Therapy 5, 27–49.
conflict between items as the executive control was not deterio- Clore, G.L., Palmer, J., 2009. Affective guidance of intelligent agents: how emotion
rated. As a consequence, spatiality is a very specific dilemma while controls cognition. Cognitive Systems Research 10, 21–30.
inattentive: participants saw the spatial cue but did not expect Conway, M., Csank, P.A.R., Holm, S.L., Blake, C.K., 2000. On assessing individual dif-
ferences in rumination on sadness. Journal of Personality Assessment 75 (3),
anything about it with regards to the target location. 404–425.
It has been previously demonstrated that sadness affects cog- Fan, J., McCandliss, D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., Posner, M.I., 2002. Testing the efficiency
nition in many ways as it tends to promote a local and bottom-up and the independance of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuro-
sciences 19 (1–2), 340–347.
information processing (Clore and Palmer, 2009), where attention
Frijda, N.H., 1986. The Emotions. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
focus is on available immediate information (Power and Dalgleish, Gerrards-Hesse, A., Spies, K., Hesse, F., 1994. Experimental induction of emotional
2008; Silvia et al., 2005). Future event anticipation is consequently states and their effectiveness: a review. British Journal of Psychology 85, 55–78.
Gilet, A.-L., 2008. Procédures d’induction d’humeurs en laboratoire : une revue cri-
degraded. In our experiment, we hypothesize that spatial cues have
tique. [Mood induction procedures: a critical review]. L’Encéphale 34, 233–239.
actually been seen, similarly to other cues but the informative Huffziger, S., Kuehner, C., 2009. Rumination, distraction, and mindful self-focus in
nature on spatiality has not been used to anticipate the target loca- depressed patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy 47, 224–230.
tion. In sum, information was partially processed when inattentive. Joorman, J., Gotlib, I.H., 2008. Updating the contents of working memory in
depression: interference from irrelevant negative material. Journal of Abnormal
In a dynamic and complex situation requiring spatial attention such Psychology 117 (1), 182–192.
as driving, implications are severe. In case of inattention due to sad- Joorman, J., Yoon, K.R., Zetsche, U., 2007. Cognitive inhibition in depression. Applied
ness and ruminations, road signs using spatial information to warn and Preventive Psychology 12, 128–139.
Juslin, P.N., Liljeström, S., Västfjäll, D., Barradas, G., Silva, A., 2008. An experience
about dangers and future events (e.g., arrival at a new place, lane sampling study of emotional reactions to music: listener, music and situation.
changes, and motorway exits) could not be processed and have dra- Emotion 8 (5), 668–683.
matic consequences on traffic. In further research, a replication of Kessler, Y., Shencar, Y., Meiran, N., 2009. Choosing to switch: spontaneous task
switching despite associated behavioral costs. Acta Psychologica 131 (9),
this study in a simulated environment would help in verifying the 120–128.
influence of inattention on driving parameters. Krumhansl, C., 1997. An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiol-
An alternative explanation of this deficit of the orienting func- ogy. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 51 (4), 336–352.
Lazarus, R.S., 1991. Emotion and Adaptation. Oxford University Press, New York.
tion when inattentive may be expressed in terms of degradations
Lemercier, C., Cellier, J.M., 2008. Les défauts de l’attention en conduite automo-
to the ability to switch from one source to another source of infor- bile : inattention, distraction, et interférence [Attention deficits in car driving:
mation (Kessler et al., 2009; Logan, 2005; Ruthworth et al., 2005). inattention, distraction, and interference]. Le travail humain 71 (3), 271–296.
Leung, K.-K., Lee, T.M.C., Yip, P., Li, L.S.W., Wong, M.M.C., 2009. Selective attention
Because they ruminate, inattentive people focus their attention
biases of people with depression: positive and negative priming of depression-
on their thoughts and disengage it with difficulties to a different related information. Psychiatry Research 165, 241–251.
stimulus (Leung et al., 2009; Philippot and Brutoux, 2008). Logan, G.D., 2005. The time it takes to switch attention. Psychonomic Bulletin and
In summary, there were fairly consistent effects of inattention Review 12, 647–653.
Martin, M., 1990. On the induction of mood. Clinical Psychology Review 10, 669–697.
on the orienting effect. When people are sad and ruminate, they Mayer, J.D., Gaschke, Y.N., 1988. The experience and meta-experience of mood.
are likely to be inattentive. This state leads to a deterioration of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 55, 102–111.
50 C. Pêcher et al. / Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée 61 (2011) 43–50

Mayer, J.D., Allen, J.P., Beauregard, K., 1995. Mood induction for four specific moods: Redick, T.S., Engle, R.W., 2006. Working memory capacity and Attention Network
a procedure employing guided imagery vignettes with music. Journal of Mental Test performance. Applied Cognitive Psychology 20, 713–721.
Imagery 19 (1–2), 133–150. Ruthworth, M.F.S., Passingham, R.E., Nobre, A.C., 2005. Components of attentional
McLaughlin, K.A., Borkovec, T.D., Sibrava, N.J., 2007. The effects of worry and rumi- set-switching. Experimental Psychology 52 (2), 83–98.
nation on affect states and cognitive activity. Behavior Therapy 38, 23–38. Scherrer, M.C., Dobson, K.S., 2009. Predicting responsiveness to a depressive mood
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., 1991. Responses to depression and their effects on the duration induction procedure. Journal of Clinical Psychology 65, 20–35.
of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 100 (4), 569–582. Silvia, P.J., Eichstaedt, J., Phillips, A.G., 2005. Are rumination and reflection types
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., 2000. The role of rumination in depressive disorders and of self-focused attention? Personality and Individual Differences 38, 871–
mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 881.
504–511. Sutherland, G., Rachman, S., Newman, B., 1982. Experimental investigations of the
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Lyubomirsky, S., Wisco, B.E., 2008. Rethinking rumination. Per- relations between mood and intrusive, unwanted cognitions. British Journal of
spectives on Psychological Science 3, 400–424. Medical Psychology 55, 127–138.
Odludas, Y., Posner, M.I., Dishion, T.J., 2008. Functional MRI of attention and language Västfjäll, D., 2002. Emotion induction throughout music: a review of the musi-
in adolescent chronic marijuana abuse. International Journal of Psychophysiol- cal mood induction procedure. Musicae Scientiae 6 (Special Issue 2001/2002),
ogy 69 (3), 209–210. 171–203.
Pêcher, C., Lemercier, C., Cellier, J.M., 2009. Emotions drive attention: effects on Velten, E., 1968. A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Behavioral Research
driver’s attention. Safety Science 47 (9), 1254–1259. and Therapy 6, 473–482.
Philippot, P., Brutoux, F., 2008. Induced rumination dampens executive processes in Wang, K., Fan, J., Dong, Y., Wang, C.Q., Lee, T.M.C., Posner, M.I., 2005. Selective
dysphoric young adults. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychi- impairment of attentional networks of orienting and executive control in
atry 39 (3), 219–227. schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 78 (2–3), 235–241.
Pikler, A.G., 1961. Music as an aid in psychotherapy. Acta Psychologica 18, 317–333. Watkins, E., Teadale, E., 2001. Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression.
Posner, M.I., 1980. Orienting of attention. The Quaterly Journal of Experimental Journal of Affective Disorders 82, 1–8.
Psychology 32, 3–25. Watson, D., Clark, L.A., 1999. The PANAS X: Manual for the Positive Affect and Neg-
Power, M., Dalgleish, T., 2008. Cognition and Emotion: from Order to Disorder. Psy- ative Affect Schedule-Expanded Form. Unpublished Manuscript. University of
chology Press, Hove, UK, New York, USA. Iowa, Iowa City.

You might also like