You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/233300346

Three-generation attachment: How grandmothers and mothers contribute to


children's attachment security

Article  in  Family Science · April 2010


DOI: 10.1080/19424620.2010.531051

CITATIONS READS

4 5,979

2 authors:

Gina A. Cook Lori A. Roggman


California State University, Stanislaus Utah State University
32 PUBLICATIONS   1,061 CITATIONS    87 PUBLICATIONS   5,523 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Parental Communication and Athletic Performance View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Gina A. Cook on 06 November 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


This article was downloaded by: [Utah State University]
On: 25 January 2011
Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 918295143]
Publisher Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-
41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Family Science
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t908134890

Three-generation attachment: How grandmothers and mothers contribute


to children's attachment security
Gina A. Cooka; Lori A. Roggmanb
a
Early Intervention Research Institute, Utah State University, Logan, Utah, USA b Department of
Family, Consumer, and Human Development, Utah State University, Logan, Utah, USA

Online publication date: 16 December 2010

To cite this Article Cook, Gina A. and Roggman, Lori A.(2010) 'Three-generation attachment: How grandmothers and
mothers contribute to children's attachment security', Family Science, 1: 2, 112 — 122
To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/19424620.2010.531051
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19424620.2010.531051

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents
will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses
should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,
actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly
or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
Family Science
Vol. 1, No. 2, April 2010, 112–122

Three-generation attachment: How grandmothers and mothers contribute


RFSC

to children’s attachment security


Gina A. Cooka* and Lori A. Roggmanb
Family Science

a
Early Intervention Research Institute, Utah State University, Logan, Utah, USA; bDepartment of Family, Consumer, and Human
Development, Utah State University, Logan, Utah, USA

Infant attachment is influenced by the mother’s responsiveness, which is, in turn, thought to be based on her own history of
attachment. When a single mother lives with her own mother, the child’s grandmother may influence the child’s attachment
not only indirectly through the mother’s attachment history but also directly through the grandmother’s involvement in car-
egiving and through the grandmother-mother relationship. As part of an Early Head Start study, 57 single mothers and 21
coresident grandmothers reported their adult attachment style at program enrollment. Mothers’ parenting behaviors were
observed at 14 and 24 months, and children’s secure base behavior was reported by their mothers at 10, 14, and 18 months.
Grandmothers’ and mothers’ security scores were related to children’s security but in different directions. Because results
suggest the importance of context in understanding intergenerational processes, a follow-up qualitative study was conducted
Downloaded By: [Utah State University] At: 23:14 25 January 2011

when children were seven to eight years old in which three of the coresiding grandmother-mother dyads were interviewed
about their past and current relationships and living arrangements. The qualitative results enrich the quantitative results by
offering descriptions of how mothers and grandmothers see their relationships with each other and with the children in
terms of patterns of influence between grandmothers and mothers and between mothers and children.
Keywords: intergenerational; attachment; grandparenting; parenting behaviour; mother–grandmother relationship

The beginnings of social-emotional development can be situations (Bost, Vaughn, Washington, Cielinski, & Bradbard,
seen in the mother-infant relationship. It is usually the first 1998; Erickson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 1985; Howes, Matheson,
relationship a child develops, and the quality of this rela- & Hamilton, 1994; LaFreniere & Sroufe, 1985; Seifer &
tionship will influence future relationships and other Schiller, 1995). Securely attached children not only
aspects of later functioning. The mother’s sensitive and develop greater social competence, they also develop bet-
responsive caregiving is a key factor contributing to the ter problem-solving abilities (Colman & Thompson, 2002;
quality of her relationship with her child by its effects on Frankel & Bates, 1990; LaFreniere & Sroufe, 1985;
the development of attachment. Ainsworth, Blehar, Turner, 1991) and more expressive language skills (Belsky
Waters, and Wall (1978) described attachment as an affec- & Fearon, 2002). Thus, the quality of attachment with the
tional bond that an infant forms with a caregiver and that mother and other caregivers plays an early influential role in
endures across time and situations. Bowlby (1988) the development of children’s later social and academic
described the development of a secure-base figure that skills.
provides protection, experiences, and exploration from Infant attachment is related not only to a mother’s cur-
which the infant can organize a sense of self as well as rent parenting behavior and relationship with her child, but
form an emotional bond. Bowlby theorized that this primary also to the mother’s attitudes and feelings about attach-
attachment is formed around the child’s first birthday and ment from earlier experiences with her own mother’s
from then on has a major impact on the social-emotional responsiveness to her needs for comfort and care when she
development of the child. was a child (Ainsworth, 1989; Main, 1995; Main & Gold-
The quality of attachment in the early mother-infant wyn, 1998). The quality of the mother-infant attachment is
relationship has a broad influence on children’s developing thus influenced by the mother’s own thoughts and feelings
social competence. During the preschool and early school about attachment, her internal working model, developed
years, securely attached children, compared with their through her early attachment relationships (Bretherton,
insecurely attached peers, have the social advantages of 1996) in addition to being influenced by the mother’s abil-
fewer behavior problems, better self-concepts, more social ity to be a sensitive caregiver despite her early attachment
confidence, greater social participation, higher sociometric relationships. One of the first studies of attachment across
status indicated by more children naming them as friends, multiple generations, by Benoit and Parker (1994),
and overall simply a higher level of competence in social showed that secure grandmothers tend to have secure

*Corresponding author. Email: gina.cook@usu.edu

ISSN 1942-4620 print/ISSN 1942-4639 online


© 2010 Taylor & Francis
DOI: 10.1080/19424620.2010.531051
http://www.informaworld.com
Family Science 113

daughters and secure grandchildren. In the years since that tends to replicate itself across generations, whereas inse-
study, other researchers have also examined attachment in curity is not automatically transmitted” (p. 160). The inter-
three-generation models (e.g., Kretchmar & Jacobvitz, generational transmission of parenting styles, including
2002; Spieker & Bensley, 1994). attachment-related parenting behavior, has been found to
When a single mother lives with her own mother, the be less dependent on specific content in the past relation-
child’s grandmother may influence the child’s attachment ship and more related to the overall organization of the
not only indirectly through the mother’s attachment but current relationship. The consistency of the mother’s earlier
also more directly through the grandmother’s involvement attachment experiences does not affect her child’s attach-
in providing caregiving for the child. In 1997, almost 8% ment as much as the quality of her current relationship
of all children under the age of 18 lived in the United with the child’s grandmother does (Cassidy & Shaver,
States in homes with a grandparent (Bryson & Casper, 1999). Intergenerational continuity in parenting has been
1999). With nearly 5.8 million children living in the same found to be mediated by mothers’ present relationships
household as their grandparents, and approximately 75% with their children’s grandparents as well as by the social
of them living in grandparent-headed households (U.S. support received from them (Chen & Kaplan, 2001).
Bureau of the Census, 2001), the need to understand the Grandmothers’ influence may not be limited to indi-
context of attachment for these children is becoming rect transmission through the mother’s attachment, but
increasingly important. This study examined intergenera- may be more directly transmitted if the grandmother influ-
tional patterns of attachment by exploring the grand- ences the mother’s parenting. Although there is almost no
Downloaded By: [Utah State University] At: 23:14 25 January 2011

mother-mother, mother-child, and grandmother-child research on grandparent-child attachment relationships, at


relationships in multigenerational families. least one study has found that grandmother support usually
facilitates positive caregiving by mothers (Spieker &
Bensley, 1994).
Intergenerational continuity of attachment security In many cases, a grandmother’s influence may not be
Some theorists and researchers have suggested that a limited only to transmission through the mother’s attach-
mother’s parenting behavior is based on her own history ment or parenting but may influence the child’s security of
of attachment developed in the context of her relationship attachment even more directly if the grandmother is
with her mother and other close relationships (Ainsworth, involved in the child’s caregiving, especially if the mother
1989). Mothers who remember feeling accepted by their and the child live with the grandmother. Attachment is not
mothers during childhood are generally more sensitive and exclusive to the mother and child; most infants form more
less intrusive with their infants compared with those who than one attachment (Bowlby, 1982). In fact, children
remember their own mothers as being either disengaged or often have several attachment figures in an “attachment
overprotective (Kretchmar & Jacobvitz, 2002). Bowlby’s hierarchy” in which the child develops a stronger prefer-
(1982) theory suggests that internal working models are ence for one caregiver, typically the mother but possibly
stable across development and even across generations. another person who is in the primary caregiving role, such
Mothers with internalized security are more likely to have as a father or a grandparent (Ainsworth, 1967; Bowlby,
been responded to by their own mothers sensitively, and in 1982).
turn, more likely to respond to their infants sensitively. Other risk factors, such as poverty or poor education,
Several studies have now shown that a mother’s recollection may also affect intergenerational transmission of attach-
of her own attachment experiences predicts her infant’s ment. When families are under stress, as many families in
attachment security (Benoit & Parker, 1994; Fonagy, poverty are, children are more likely to develop insecure/
Steele, Moran, Steele, & Higgitt, 1993; Fonagy, Steele, & anxious attachment relationships (Vaughn, Egeland,
Steele, 1991; Huth-Bocks, Levendosky, Bogat, & von Sroufe, & Waters, 1979). Living in poverty often has a
Eye, 2004; Slade & Cohen, 1996; Ward & Carlson, 1995). negative influence on the caregiver-child relationship,
While much emphasis has been placed on the trans- which influences a child’s ability to develop a secure
mission of parenting styles across generations and on the attachment. Families living in poverty, compared with
similarities between generations, few studies have exam- middle-income families, are more likely to have children
ined the characteristics of either the parent or the child that with insecure attachment (Diener, Nievar, & Wright,
might mediate the intergenerational transmission of 2003). Families living in poverty are also more likely to
attachment (Serbin & Karp, 2004). Specific protective fac- live in multigenerational homes, in which the relationship
tors have not been identified, yet research indicates that between the mother and grandmother can have more direct
while secure mothers are more likely to have secure influences on children’s caregiving environments. The
infants there is often intergenerational discontinuity among mother-grandmother relationship may influence the moth-
those with insecure relationships (George & Solomon, 1996). ers’ parenting behaviors in ways that may either protect
Recently, Poehlmann (2003) described this inconsistency children from or put them at risk for developing insecure
as “a positive finding because it indicates that security attachments (Lyons-Ruth, Easterbrooks, & Cibelli, 1997;
114 G.A. Cook and L.A. Roggman

Zeanah, Boris, & Larrieu, 1997). Among infants of teen- relationships? Are mothers’ and grandmothers’ responses
age mothers living in low-income multigenerational about close relationships related? Are these aspects of
households, social support was a better predictor of attach- attachment across generations related to mothers’ parent-
ment security than maternal maturity or self-esteem (Hess, ing behavior or to demographic or psychosocial factors?
Papas, & Black, 2002), suggesting a positive influence
from support by family members. Support from grand-
mothers in another study, however, was unrelated to car- Methods
egiving by the teenage mothers who resided with their Participants. In a study of applicants to a local Early Head
children’s grandmothers (Spieker & Bensley, 1994). To Start program, interview data were initially collected from
understand more about how attachment could be influ- 57 mothers who were single and/or living with their own
enced across generations in low-income families, we mothers and from 21 coresident grandmothers identified
explored aspects of attachment across three generations in by the mothers as secondary caregivers to the children par-
relation to maternal parenting behavior in a group of low- ticipating in the study. Among these 21 coresident three-
income families. generation families, 17 were single mothers who had
As part of a larger Early Head Start study, a local sam- never married, two were separated from the child’s father,
ple of low-income mothers with infants under a year old and two were married. The mothers’ ages averaged 18.4
were interviewed about attachment aspects of their close and ranged from 15 to 26. Education ranged from eighth
relationships and about their children’s secure base behav- grade to an associates degree, with over half the mothers
Downloaded By: [Utah State University] At: 23:14 25 January 2011

ior and then later observed in parenting interactions with having at least a high school diploma. The family annual
their children. Data from mothers who were unmarried income mean was $10,015, ranging from $0 to $50,000;
and/or living with their own mothers, the grandmothers, averaging $2343 per person per year, ranging from $0 to
along with data from the coresident grandmothers who $10,000 per person per year. The U.S. poverty rate is an
were also interviewed about their attachment attitudes, annual income of less than $22,128 for a family of four
were used in a quantitative analysis of these aspects of (DeNavas-Walt, Proctor, & Smith, 2010). Over 92% of
attachment in relation to mothers’ parenting behaviors. In this sample fell below the poverty line. In the 36 other sin-
addition, three grandmother-mother-child triads were later gle-mother families, mothers reported that they were
identified to participate in qualitative interviews to explore unmarried and not living with a partner but did not iden-
their thoughts and feelings about their past and current liv- tify a grandmother as a secondary caregiver. The mothers’
ing arrangements and their intertwined relationships. This average age in this noncoresident group was 20.6, ranging
mixed-methods approach, using both quantitative and from 14 to 33. Education ranged from sixth grade to an
qualitative data from mothers, children, and grandmothers associates degree, with half of the mothers having at least
in three-generation households, is intended to add under- a high school diploma. The family annual income mean
standing beyond what might be found by a single was $4529, ranging from $0 to $32,000, averaging $1667
approach (Teddlie & Tashakkori, 2003). per person per year, ranging from $0 to $10,666 per
The purpose of this study was to identify a subset of person per year.
unmarried mothers of infants who lived either alone or Procedures. Families were recruited as research par-
with their own mothers and to examine quantitative and ticipants when they applied for the program. At that time,
qualitative data about aspects of attachment relationships all mothers and identified grandmothers were interviewed
from all three generations, collected by interviews, and by a trained data collector in a telephone interview or, if a
about parenting behaviors, coded from semi-structured telephone was not available, in person in their homes or
observations. As part of the larger study, all mothers were another location of their choosing. As part of the inter-
interviewed upon enrollment into the study and, in as view, mothers and grandmothers were asked about their
many cases as possible, were asked to identify a “second- attachment attitudes. Because enrollment could occur dur-
ary caregiver” who helped take care of the child. Not all ing pregnancy, questions about the child were asked in
single mothers identified a secondary caregiver, but many later interviews with mothers at a sequence of child age
identified the child’s coresident grandmother as a second- points. These interviews were conducted in families’
ary caregiver, and the grandmother was then also inter- homes as part of more extensive data collection that
viewed. Data from interviews with these mothers and involved both interview and observation methods. Moth-
grandmothers were used to examine associations among ers were interviewed about their children’s secure base
aspects of attachment from grandmothers, mothers, and behavior when the children were 10, 14, 18, and 36
children, to test these aspects of attachment in association months old. Mothers were also observed in semi-struc-
with observed maternal parenting behavior, and to explore tured play interactions with their children at ages 14, 24,
other related factors. The following questions were and 36 months.
addressed: Is children’s secure base behavior related to To obtain more contextual data, we used qualitative
their mothers’ or grandmothers’ responses about close interviews to explore relationships among grandmothers,
Family Science 115

mothers, and children in the three-generational households individually printed on cards, according to how well items
in our study. A separate qualitative interview was con- described their child (“Q-sort”). The AQS Q-sort criterion-
ducted with three of the families in the original research correlation score was used for analyses. The Q-sort crite-
sample. When the children from the original study were in rion-correlation was developed from the scores of 16
second grade and between seven and eight years old, nine experts, developmental psychologists who provided sorts
local grandmother-mother-child triads were identified for the ideal security-attached child. The reliability of
from the larger sample to participate in further qualitative these composite scores was .95 or higher (Waters &
study. Three triads were contacted and agreed to particip- Deane, 1985). The more similar the subject was to the
ate in the qualitative portion of this study. We purposely hypothetical “most secure child” the higher their correla-
selected a triad that would represent each attachment style, tion score, resulting in a score ranging from -1.0 to +1.0,
therefore one mother who scored high on ambivalence, with a +1.0 representing the most secure score possible.
one who scored high on avoidance, and one who scored Observer sorts such as the AQS were initially devel-
high on security were chosen. All three mothers inter- oped to be used by trained outside observers, and the use
viewed were teenagers when they became pregnant, and of such sorts by mothers has been controversial. Yet, in a
none were married at that time. The mothers ranged in age study conducted by Waters and Deane (1985) of 50 mid-
from 23 to 28 years old at the time of the interviews. Both dle-class families a correlation of .80 was reported
the mother and grandmother in the three families who par- between trained observers and mothers. The phone inter-
ticipated were interviewed at length, and transcripts of view prior to the in-home assessment provided the parents
Downloaded By: [Utah State University] At: 23:14 25 January 2011

their interviews were coded for content related to the with familiarity of the items as well as additional time to
research questions. think about their child’s behaviors and observe their child
to increase the reliability of these mother sorts.
Maternal behavior. A three-bag observation procedure
Measures was conducted and coded using seven-point coding scales
Adult security. As a measure of adult attachment security, of maternal behaviors of supportiveness, quality of assist-
questions from the Adult Attachment Style scale (AAS; ance, intrusiveness, detachment, and negativity. The
Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992) were used in inter- observation consisted of a videotaped 10-minute interac-
views with mothers and grandmothers at the time of tion of the mother and child in their home. The mother was
enrollment. The AAS includes avoidant and anxious/ given three bags with different items in each bag (e.g., a
ambivalent subscales from which scores for avoidance and book, pretend toys, manipulative toys) and told to play
ambivalence can be derived. Also, an overall security with their child dividing their time between the bags how
score can be derived by reverse coding the items and then they wished. A trained coding team established inter-rater
summing items from both subscales. The scale was reliability of 85% (exact or within one-point agreement)
developed based on work on adult romantic attachment and then conducted weekly inter-rater reliability checks on
relationships, so the wording on a few of the items was 15% of that week’s assignments. Percent agreement aver-
changed to refer to close relationships more generally so aged 94% for all 36-month coders, with a range of 86 to
that it would be more appropriate for single mothers not 100% (Berlin, Brady-Smith, & Brooks-Gunn, 2002).
currently in a romantic relationship. These item wording Qualitative interviews. Qualitative interviews were
changes were reviewed and approved by the second author conducted when the children were seven to eight years old
of the original measure. Items that refer to “partner” were and consisted of a series of open-ended questions about
revised to refer to “people close to me.” Items from this their relationships with each other, with the child, and with
scale include “I find it difficult to trust others completely” other family members and friends during the past seven
and “I’m comfortable having others depend on me.” This years. The interview also asked about living arrangements,
13-item scale has alphas ranging from .58 to .81. caregiving and discipline, and the caregiving roles of both
Child security. As a measure of child attachment the mother and the grandmother. Mothers and grandmoth-
security, mothers completed the Attachment Q-set (AQS; ers were visited in their homes and interviewed using
Waters, 1987) at ages 10, 14, 18, and 36 months. Ques- these open-ended questions (see Table 1). Joint interviews
tions from the 90 items of the AQS were used in inter- lasted approximately 90 minutes, an individual mother
views with the mothers two weeks before the in-home data interview was approximately 50 minutes long, and an indi-
collection session in which the mothers were helped to vidual grandmother interview lasted about 30 minutes.
complete the full card sort of the items. Mothers were The interviews were then transcribed and read for initial
encouraged to ask questions about the items and to codes. Two transcript coders read all transcripts and came
observe their children with the items in mind during the up with codes separately and then met to discuss similari-
two weeks between the interview and the in-home data ties and differences in codes. After initial coding was com-
collection session. During the in-home assessment, pleted the coders met on two more occasions to discuss
parents sorted the behavioral descriptions, which were themes and research questions. Preliminary content codes
116 G.A. Cook and L.A. Roggman

Table 1. Interview questions.


A. We have been studying (child’s name) for the past 7–8 years now. What is your relationship with your child like? Probes: When
do you spend time together? What do you do then? (Mother/Grandmother)
B. What have the living arrangements been like for you and your child/grandchild? (from birth until now) Probe: How have they
changed? (Mother/Grandmother)
If you don’t currently live together, how often do you see your grandchild? (Grandmother)
C. What has been your role with your child/grandchild? Who disciplines? Who does the child go to when they are hurt or need
something? Who makes decisions about the child? (Mother/Grandmother)
D. What can you tell me about your relationship with your mother/daughter? What is it like? How close [or distant] are you
emotionally? If you could change anything about your relationship with her, what would that be? Why? (Mother/Grandmother)
E. Tell me about other relationships in your life. Spouse/significant other? Parents? Other children? (Mother/Grandmother)
F. How would you describe your mother’s/daughter’s relationship with ________ [target child]? How would you describe your
mother’s/daughter’s relationship with her other grandchildren/children (if any)? What do you like about her relationship with
the child and what if anything bothers you? (Mother/Grandmother)
G. Tell me your favorite story about you and your son/daughter/grandchild. Tell me your favorite story about you and your mother/
daughter. (Mother/Grandmother)

included: single motherhood, teenage pregnancy, “being Grandmothers’ attachment attitudes also were related to
there,” divorce, parent-child relationship, parent-child children’s attachment security although not in the
Downloaded By: [Utah State University] At: 23:14 25 January 2011

friendship, lack of a father figure, independence, responsi- expected direction. Grandmothers’ secure attachment
bility, living arrangements, roles, critical toward men, and attitudes were negatively correlated with the children’s
parenting skills. For this group of grandmothers and moth- 10-month attachment security, r = -.56*. Intergenerational
ers there were many similarities across families and a few continuity was not reflected in this pattern of correlations
distinct differences. between mother, grandmother, and child security scores.

Results
Initially, demographic differences were examined in the Table 2. Mother attachment attitudes in relation to grand-
mother attachment attitudes for mothers living with their
resident and coresident groups. Group comparisons infants’ grandmother (N = 21).
showed that there were no significant differences in any of
the demographic variables except for household income Grandmother attachment attitudesat
baseline interview
(higher for coresident mothers) and for maternal age
(higher for noncoresident mothers). These differences Mother attachment
would be expected based on the reasons why mothers attitudes at baseline Summary
would be living with their children’s grandmothers. interview Avoidant Ambivalent score
Coresident grandmothers’ attachment attitudes were Avoidant .24 .25 .30
related to mothers’ attachment attitudes at baseline, r = .47*, Ambivalent .50* .34 .53*
but not at later times (see Table 2). Mothers’ attachment Summary score .43+ .34 .48*
attitudes were related to children’s attachment security for *Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
both coresident and noncoresident mothers (see Table 3). +Correlation is significant at the 0.10 level (2-tailed).

Table 3. Mother and grandmother attachment attitudes in relation to child attachment security for coresident mothers (n = 18–21) and
noncoresident mothers (n = 23–30).
Child attachment security
Adult attachment attitude 10 month (non) 14 month (non) 18 month (non)
Maternal avoidance at baseline interview −.06 (−.06) −.12 (−.52*) −.26 (−.24)
Maternal ambivalence at baseline interview −.16 (−.30) −.43+ (−.30) −.03 (−.50*)
Maternal attachment security at baseline interview .13 (.20) .31 (.55**) .18 (.44*)
Maternal avoidance at 10-month interview −.16 (−.06) −.39 (−.04) −.38 (−.31)
Maternal ambivalence at 10-month interview −.31 (.12) −.57* (−.20) −.33 (.17)
Maternal attachment security at 10-month interview .24 (.00) .53* (−.14) .44+ (.18)
Grandmother avoidance at baseline interview .50* .12 .17
Grandmother ambivalence at baseline interview .41+ −.18 −.01
Grandmother attachment security at baseline interview −.56* .02 −.12
*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
+Correlation is significant at the 0.10 level (2-tailed).
Family Science 117

Mothers’ parenting behaviors were related to children’s Table 6. Predictive model of 18-month attachment security.
attachment security in both coresident and noncoresident
Model: 18-month Standardized
mothers although in different directions and more strongly attachment security coefficients t Sig. R2
for those living independently. For mothers living with the
infants’ grandmothers several correlations were not in the Maternal supportiveness .12 .64
Coresident −.07 −.43
expected direction. For example, maternal intrusiveness in Supportiveness × coresident −.45 −2.48 .04 .26*
the coresident group was unexpectedly positively correlated
with 14-month infant security, while it was negatively *Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
correlated in the noncoresident group (see Table 4).
Although t-tests indicate no statistically significant group
differences for security scores, children of mothers who To understand more about the intergenerational
were not coresiding appear to have a higher average security process of attachment, contextual data about grandmother-
score than children of mothers who were coresiding with mother-child relationships need to be explored. The quali-
the children’s grandmothers (see Table 5). Demographic tative interviews provided contextual data from three
variables such as income, age, or education were not asso- grandmother-mother-child dyads. “Jamie,” a 28-year-old
ciated with security scores. married mother of two children, had lived with her mother
A regression model (see Table 6) was tested to determine when her children were infants. She was employed, and
if living with the grandmother moderates the influence of although she no longer lived with her mother, her mother
Downloaded By: [Utah State University] At: 23:14 25 January 2011

mothers’ supportive play on their children’s 18-month did take care of her children every day for a couple of
attachment security. Due to the small sample size and the hours while she was at work. Jamie and her child both had
number of missing variables the regression model was high security scores while Jamie’s mother scored high on
tested using MPLUS with maximum likelihood estimates. ambivalence. “Carrie,” a 23-year-old single mother of one
As shown in Table 6, the interaction term was statistically child, was also employed, and while she also no longer
significant, with a coefficient of -.45, and the model lived with her mother, her mother did take care of her
accounted for 26% of the variance. The interaction effect daughter two to three afternoons a week and some week-
was explored by examining the relation of supportive play ends while she worked. Carrie’s child had a low security
to child attachment security within each group, the coresi- score, Carrie had a high ambivalence score and her mother
dent group and the noncoresident group. Only in the non- had a high avoidance score. “JoAnn,” a 23-year-old
coresident group did mothers’ supportive play predict mother of three children, had been recently divorced and
children’s attachment security. had only visitation rights with her children for the past
month. She had been living with her mother since the
divorce, and both were involved with the children only on
weekends. Before the divorce JoAnn lived with her chil-
Table 4. Correlation between maternal parenting behaviors
and infant security for coresident and noncoresident mothers. dren. JoAnn scored high on the avoidance scale, her child
scored low on security, and her mother scored high on
Child attachment security security. Each of the three triads lived together until the
Parenting behaviors 18 month (non) 36 month (non) child was anywhere from 11 months old to approximately
two years old.
Maternal play −.05 (.57*) −.33 (.51+)
supportiveness at 14 months
All three grandmothers indicated that it was important
Maternal intrusiveness .55+ (−.04) .57+ (.09) for them to “be there” for their daughter and their grand-
at 14 months child, admitted that there had been times during their
Maternal play −.21 (.38) −.19 (.10) daughters’ teenage years when things were difficult, but
supportiveness at 24 months concluded that over the years things had become better
Maternal intrusiveness at 24 −.16 (−.23) .38 (−.53*)
months
and they now considered themselves friends. Additionally,
all three stated that the mother was the primary caregiver,
+Correlation is significant at the 0.10 level (2-tailed). the one who made all of the major decisions for the child,
and the one who did the majority of the disciplining as
well. While all three mothers had moved out of the grand-
Table 5. Mean attachment security scores for coresident and
noncoresident children. mothers’ home by the time the child was two years old, all
three grandmothers were still very involved in their grand-
Attachment security Nonresident Coresident children’s lives and admitted to having a special relationship
mean score children children
with their grandchild because they spent time together
10 month .26 .16 during infancy. Two of the three grandmothers took care
14 month .28 .23 of their grandchildren several days a week while the
18 month .26 .20
mother worked.
118 G.A. Cook and L.A. Roggman

Four other distinct themes emerged from the analysis since she was born, and we’re just closer, she’s just a little
of the qualitative interview data. These themes included more special.”
independence, relationships, parenting styles, and security. Parenting styles. The biggest difference among the
The following patterns and connections became apparent. three triads was parenting style. Jamie considered herself a
Independence. All three triads seemed to be very strict disciplinarian, unlike her mother; while the other two
strong and independent and to share these values between mothers, Carrie and JoAnn, seemed to parent similarly to
mother and grandmother. A theme emerged of women as their mothers and were more like friends to their children
independent and able to be successful as single mothers. than disciplinarians. For Jamie, a major theme was that of
For example, Carrie’s mother said, “You have to wonder communication. She felt that communication had been
if that’s how I was raised, and my mother was very inde- poor between her and her parents when she was growing
pendent and so maybe I am, and I passed that on.” Inde- up, and she did not want her relationship with her daughter
pendence in the maternal role and the ability to care for to be like her relationships with her parents. She men-
her own child was also apparent; Jamie said, “She was just tioned the importance of communication 23 times in her
kind of grandma, not anything more. . . . . I always took interview. For Carrie, a concern was the lack of a father
care of her [the child] . . . I did everything except for when figure for the granddaughter. Both mother and grand-
I was at work.” JoAnn’s mother describes JoAnn as “very mother were concerned about future relationships and the
independent . . . she took care of him [the child] and she lack of any male figures in the child’s life. Another theme
did all that.” It was clear from many of these statements that seemed especially important to Carrie was her par-
Downloaded By: [Utah State University] At: 23:14 25 January 2011

that the mother was the primary caregiver in all three ents’ divorce and its impact on her mother and herself. She
triads and that the grandmother was there primarily to sup- thought that the divorce could have been partly to blame
port and assist financially and not as the primary caregiver for her inability to have good relationships with men, and
or disciplinarian. this led to a theme of three generations of single moms in
Relationships. The grandmother-mother relationships her family.
in these families seemed to be more like friendships than The mothers described their parenting styles as influ-
traditional mother-daughter relationships. All the mothers enced by the way they were parented. Two of the three
had become adults in their twenties, living on their own, mothers, Carrie and JoAnn, said that their parenting styles
and they seemed to consider their relationships with their were very similar to that of their mothers, but Jamie con-
mothers as friendships. Two of the three triads described tinually emphasized the differences between her and her
difficult times during their adolescence, but all three mother. When asked if she thought her discipline style was
agreed that they were more like friends with their mothers similar to her mother’s, she said, “No, I am really strict
once they became adults. Jamie commented, “Except for about things . . . I just look at a lot of stuff that I didn’t
when I was a teenager . . . . I’ve always had a good rela- have to do when I was younger, like bedtimes. . . . I don’t
tionship with her.” Carrie said, “We didn’t really get along ever remember being put to bed. . . . I am just a lot more
as well when I was a teenager.” All three agreed that their strict.” Carrie’s mother and JoAnn’s mother both thought
relationships are good now and have very little conflict. that their daughters were doing a good job parenting des-
Carrie’s mother describes their relationship as “close, but pite the struggles of being single mothers. Carrie’s mother
not so close that we can’t each have our own lives.” said; “She’s a very good mom, a very special mom,
Jamie’s mother believes that their relationship changed really.” Carrie thought that while her parenting style is
with the birth of the grandchild, she said “[we have] a similar to her mother’s she hopes to lecture her daughter
very, very good relationship, it didn’t begin until [the less than her mother lectures her. She commented; “It’s
grandchild was born], I think that’s what changed and just the lecturing, and I know she probably doesn’t think
turned things around, she was kind of a little rebellious she’s lecturing me [that] it’s just a suggestion, but some-
and different things like that, she . . . it’s just a really good times it’s a pain, sometimes it’s just like not really fun.”
relationship, I think we’re more like close friends, we like Security. Finally, there was a theme of security with
to do things together, go shopping together, we do a lot one another, but not necessarily in other relationships. As
now.” Very little conflict was discussed and in general noted earlier, all three triads feel very comfortable with
these relationships all seemed strong and positive. one another. Jamie’s mother said that they are “always
The grandmother-grandchild relationships were also there for each other” and that “if [Jamie] had any problems
strong. All three grandmothers admitted to having a closer you know that I’d always be there to help her and she’s
relationship with the grandchild that had lived with them always there for me as well.” For those interviewed
than with their other grandchildren. Carrie’s mother together they seemed at ease answering most of the ques-
described the relationship as different from that with her tions in front of one another. Interestingly, all three triads
other grandchildren. She said “in some ways [it is not dif- described their relationships with men as difficult and in
ferent] but in some ways just closer because I am around some cases described these men in a derogatory manner.
her more and I have been with her, tended her basically JoAnn discussed her relationship with her ex-husband
Family Science 119

negatively and indicated that he was very controlling. Car- her childhood home may try especially hard to get
rie’s mother described their discussions about men when involved in her child’s play, as a means of being engaged
she said, “Sometimes we bad mouth guys. . . we’re not and taking on an authority role with the child, such that her
man haters. . . we more like joke about it. Like, ‘Well, he’s behaviors appear intrusive to an objective observer but are
a real loser, isn’t he?’” In this interview the mother and actually indicators of engaged parenting in the context of a
grandmother were discussing their concerns over a lack of a coresident household. While these results suggest that
father figure for the child, and Carrie said, “She’s probably coresidence does moderate the associations between
not developing a good feeling of a relationship with men.” mother and child measures of security, there is not enough
Her mother replied, “Yeah, that’s something I wonder information to indicate the process by which living with a
about once in a while, because there’s not a father-figure grandmother may influence mother or child security.
or grandfather in her daily life.” Two of the triads also dis- While these results suggest that living with the grand-
cussed the multiple relationships that they have had over mother does affect the relationship between the mother
the years and their concerns about future relationships. and child, they also suggest that the pathways of influence
are complex. From the in-depth interviews with a small
subset of the mothers and grandmothers, qualitative
Summary of findings themes of communication, fathers/men, independence,
In general, the mothers and grandmothers in this study and security suggest both attachment-related and other
seem to have good relationships with each other. They do aspects of mother-grandmother relationships that add to
Downloaded By: [Utah State University] At: 23:14 25 January 2011

not remember having many disagreements or complica- the complexity.


tions over parenting, disciplining, or caregiving. Previous research has suggested that coresidence,
Although several themes were common across inter- compared to living independently, may cause tension
views, each pair seemed to have their own set of con- between grandmothers and mothers, may create a need for
cerns. Jamie thought that communication was very greater individuation for mothers to feel competent, and
important. It was something that she lacked with her par- may even decrease the influence of the grandmother-
ents when she was growing up, and she wanted to do mother relationship (Putallaz, Costanzo, Grimes, & Sherman,
things different with her daughter. Carrie and her mother 1998; Wakschlag, Chase-Lansdale, & Brooks-Gunn,
seemed most concerned with the lack of a father figure 1996). Although grandmothers and mothers in our study
for the granddaughter. Security in relationships seemed had similar attitudes about attachment and seemed to have
to be a strong theme in this interview; while both grand- good relationships with each other, their relationship atti-
mother and mother seemed to feel secure in their rela- tudes affected infants in opposite ways. In addition, the
tionship and in talking to each other and sharing their living arrangements of all participants did not support the
feelings, neither seemed to feel secure in their relation- grandmother as the primary caregiver, all three mothers
ships with men. They also discussed independence and had moved out of the grandmothers’ homes by the time
why this may be a reason they are both single. Finally, their children were two years old. All grandmothers
JoAnn and her mother discussed their independence and reported that their role in the care of the child was helping
joked about being critical of men. the mother to be successful in meeting the child’s needs
primarily in a financial and supporting role. This may
explain why grandmothers’ attachment security was nega-
Discussion tively correlated with the children’s attachment security; it
Unlike a simple repetition of similarity in aspects of may be due to the fact that in all three triads the mother
attachment across generations, as suggested by the term was the primary caregiver, thus fostering the primary
“intergenerational transmission,” the grandmothers and attachment.
mothers in our study reported attachment attitudes that Intergenerational influences can be complex when a
were related to the children’s secure base behavior in quite mother and child coreside with a grandmother because
different ways, with mother and child aspects of attach- coresidence, compared to living independently, may cause
ment associated more positively than grandmother with tension between grandmothers and mothers (Putallaz et al.,
either mother or child. Coresidence moderated the impact 1998; Wakschlag et al., 1996). Correlations for mothers liv-
of mothers’ parenting behavior on children’s attachment ing with the infants’ grandmothers were not always in the
security. In fact, coresidence seemed to reverse the typical expected direction and weaker at baseline compared with
effects of mothers’ positive parenting behaviors even the correlations for mothers in the noncoresiding group.
though mothers’ attachment attitudes were positively This suggests that intervening factors such as the child’s
related to children’s security. For a mother living with a relationship with the grandmother may influence how the
grandmother, the nature of behavior that appears intrusive mother and child interact with each other and, in turn, how
or supportive during play with the child may carry differ- their attachment relationship develops (Posada, Waters,
ent meanings. For example, a young mother still living in Crowell, & Lay, 1995). It is also possible that these differ-
120 G.A. Cook and L.A. Roggman

ences may be due to the relationship between the mother These qualitative results may explain why grandmoth-
and grandmother during the time of coresidence. While ers’ and mothers’ related attachment security affected the
the three dyads all report a good relationship with each children in different ways. In response to the question
other after a few years of living apart, the relationship may about who does the caretaking and disciplining of the
not have been as positive during the time of coresidence, child, all three triads reported that the mother was respons-
when the mothers were less mature and the challenges of ible for both the caretaking and disciplining of the child
parenting a young infant would have caused more stress more so than the grandmother was. This information sup-
than during the child’s school-age years. All three dyads ports the conclusion from the quantitative data that the
mentioned that their relationships had not been as positive child’s attachment security is more strongly associated
during the mother’s younger years. Mothers who continue with the mother’s own sense of security in close relation-
to coreside with their own mothers may have a different ships rather than with the grandmother’s sense of security.
relationship than those who choose to no longer coreside. This distinction between the mother and grandmother
The intergenerational transmission of attachment-related roles emerging from the qualitative interviews suggests
parenting behavior has been found to be less dependent on that most of the caregiving interactions were between
specific content in the past relationship and more related mother and child. The influence from the grandmother
to the overall organization of the current relationship with then, on the child’s developing sense of security and
the grandmother at the time the child is forming their early secure-base behavior, would be more likely to be indirect
attachment relationships (Cassidy & Shaver, 1999). For through the mother rather than through direct caregiving
Downloaded By: [Utah State University] At: 23:14 25 January 2011

coresiding young mothers, the conflicting roles between interactions with the child. Interestingly, the correlations
being a daughter and a mother may create patterns of suggest positive associations for security between grand-
behavior that are unique to the coresidence context. This mothers and mothers, and between mothers and children,
may explain the different findings for the coresident and but not between grandmothers and children. These results,
noncoresident groups. considered together with the qualitative data, suggest that
When the child is living with both mother and grand- the three-generation transmission of attachment is more
mother, the mix of caregiving influences may delay or dis- indirect than direct.
rupt the organization of attachment as reflected in There are several strengths and weaknesses that
children’s secure base behavior. The unexpected negative need to be addressed in this study. The major strength is
correlations of grandmother security with child security that there are so few studies that have examined the
with the mother at 10 months may indicate that the grand- topic of residential grandmothers and the cross-genera-
mother’s presence delayed the child’s organization of tional link with attachment security. The addition of
attachment behavior toward the mother. In other circum- qualitative interviews with three of the mothers is an
stances in which someone other than the mother provides additional strength while the timing of these interviews
much of the caregiving, such as in a kibbutz community, is considered a limitation. These interviews indicate a
attachment has been found to be more inconsistent across positive relationship between the mothers and grand-
generations and more likely to be characterized as inse- mothers that has been established five to six years after
cure (Sagi et al., 1997). Children living in a kibbutzim coresiding and may not be indicative of the relationship
with communal sleeping arrangements, in which unfamil- during the coresidential period. Two of the dyads also
iar adults often cared for children during the night, had a chose to have their interviews conducted together.
much lower incidence of secure attachment (55% secure) Although it was their choice, being interviewed in each
and were less likely to have an attachment style similar to others’ presence may have limited the openness of the
their parents, compared with children living in a kibbutzim interviews. These limitations could explain the lack of
community with family sleeping arrangements (80% continuity between the grandmother-mother security
secure) in which mothers could provide more caregiving scores for these three dyads.
for their infants. The lack of attachment style concordance Finally, sample size was a weakness of this study for
between parents and their children in the communal sleep- both the qualitative and quantitative measures. The ori-
ing kibbutz may reflect inconsistent caregiving patterns in ginal EHS study recruited 201 families, but only 57 of
the infant’s experience that interfered with a simple trans- those families reported the grandmother as a secondary
mission of the mother’s attachment style to the infant. As caregiver; additionally, a larger sample of qualitative
in the kibbutzim community, a child living with both the interviews would provide further understanding of this
mother and grandmother may also experience inconsistent relationship. The number of variables and the longitudinal
caregiving; the child may feel divided loyalties and may nature of the research questions made it difficult to com-
often feel torn between the multiple authority figures in plete some analyses due to the small sample size.
the home. Inconsistent parenting may occur, and added to American families are constantly changing and evolv-
that may be the insecurity of not knowing who to trust ing. Some of the biggest changes over the past 40 years
most (DeToledo & Brown, 1995). have involved the family structure and living arrange-
Family Science 121

ments. On the one hand, changes in the economy, divorce, Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical applications of
and marital instability, and the increased problems of pov- attachment theory. London: Tavistock.
Bretherton, I. (1996). Internal working models of attachment
erty and drug abuse have made it difficult for families to
relationships as related to resilient coping. In G.G. Noam &
provide the resources, support, and caregiving required by K.W. Fischer (Eds.), Development and vulnerability in close
family members (Bengtson, 2001). On the other hand, relationships (pp. 3–27). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bengtson argued, multigenerational families are becoming Bryson, K., & Casper, L.M. (1999). Co-resident grandparents
increasingly important, and that “multigenerational bonds and grandchildren. Washington, DC: U.S. Census Bureau,
Current Population Reports, Special Studies, U.S. Census
will not only enhance but in some cases replace nuclear
Bureau.
family functions.” (p. 2) Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. (1999). Handbook of attachment: Theory,
Future research needs to focus on the grandparent- research, and clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press.
child relationship and grandparents as attachment figures Chen, Z., & Kaplan, H.B. (2001). Intergenerational transmission
in general as well as on how grandparent-parent relation- of constructive parenting. Journal of Marriage and Family,
63, 17–31.
ships influence the intergenerational transmission of
Colman, R.A., & Thompson, R.A. (2002). Attachment security
attachment security. Research on alternative attachment and the problem-solving behaviors of mothers and children.
relationships such as with fathers, grandmothers, or Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 337–359.
childcare providers could clarify the multiple pathways DeToledo, S., & Brown, D. (1995). Grandparents as parents: A
of attachment formation. Qualitative studies such as this survival guide for raising a second family. New York:
Guilford Press.
one could also provide an enhanced picture of three-gen-
Diener, M.L., Nievar, M.A., & Wright, C. (2003). Attachment
Downloaded By: [Utah State University] At: 23:14 25 January 2011

eration households and provide the contextual back- security among mothers and their young children living in
ground information needed to better understand the roles poverty: Associations with maternal, child, and contextual
of coresiding mothers and grandmothers in a child’s characteristics. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 154–182.
development. A better understanding of attachment rela- Erickson, M.F., Sroufe, L.A., & Egeland, B. (1985). The rela-
tionship between quality of attachment and behavior prob-
tionships and intergenerational transmission of caregiv-
lems in a high-risk sample. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters
ing is especially important in our changing society when (Eds.), Growing points in attachment theory and research.
caregiving is not always exclusively the role of the Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
mother. ment, 50 (1–2, Serial No. 209).
Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Moran, G., Steele, H., & Higgitt, A.
(1993). Measuring the ghost in the nursery: An empirical
study of the relation between parents’ mental representations
References of childhood experiences and their infants security of attach-
Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and ment. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association,
the growth of attachment. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Univer- 41, 957–989.
sity Press. Fonagy, P., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1991). Maternal representa-
Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American tions of attachment during pregnancy predict the organiza-
Psychologist, 44, 709–716. tion of infant-mother attachment at one year of age. Child
Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Development, 62, 891–905.
Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange Frankel, K.A., & Bates, J.E. (1990). Mother-toddler problem
situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. solving: Antecedents in attachment, home behavior, and
Belsky, J., Fearon, R.M.P. (2002). Early attachment security, temperament. Child Development, 61, 810–819.
subsequent material sensitivity, and later child develop- George, C., & Solomon, J. (1996). Representational models of
ment: Does continuity in development depend on continu- relationships: Links between caregiving and attachment.
ity or caregiving? Attachment & Human Development, 4, Infant Mental Health Journal, 17, 198–216.
361–387. Hess, C.R., Papas, M.A., & Black, M.M. (2002). Resilience
Bengtson, V.L. (2001). Beyond the nuclear family: The increasing among African American adolescent mothers: Predictors of
importance of multigenerational bonds. Journal of Marriage positive parenting in early infancy. Journal of Pediatric Psy-
and Family, 63, 1–16. chology, 27, 619–629.
Benoit, D., & Parker, K. (1994). Stability and transmission of Howes, C., Matheson, C.C., & Hamilton, C.E. (1994). Maternal,
attachment across three generations. Child Development, 65, teacher, and child care history correlates of children’s rela-
1444–1456. tionships with peers. Child Development, 65, 264–273.
Berlin, L.J., Brady-Smith, C., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). Links Huth-Bocks, A.C., Levendosky, A.A., Bogat, G.A., & von Eye,
between childbearing age and observed maternal behaviors A. (2004). The impact of maternal characteristics and con-
with 14-month-olds in the Early Head Start Research and textual variables on infant-mother attachment. Child Devel-
Evaluation Project. Infant Mental Health Journal. Special opment, 75, 480–496.
Issue: Early Head Start, 23, 104–129. Kretchmar, M.D., & Jacobvitz, D.B. (2002). Observing mother-
Bost, K.K., Vaughn, B.E., Washington, W.N., Cielinski, K.L., & child relationships across generations: Boundary patterns,
Bradbard, M.R. (1998). Social competence, social support, attachment, and the transmission of caregiving. Family Proc-
and attachment demarcation of construct domains, measure- ess, 41, 351–374.
ment, and paths of influence for preschool children attending LaFreniere, P.J., & Sroufe, L.A. (1985). Profiles of peer competence
Head Start. Child Development, 69, 192–218. in the preschool: Interrelations among measures, influence of
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. social ecology, and relation to attachment history. Develop-
New York: Basic. mental Psychology, 21, 56–69.
122 G.A. Cook and L.A. Roggman

Lyons-Ruth, K., Easterbrooks, A., & Cibelli, C. (1997). Infant situation: The role of attachment styles. Journal of Personality
attachment strategies, infant mental lag, and maternal and Social Psychology, 62, 434–446.
depressive symptoms: Predictors of internalizing and Slade, A., & Cohen, L.J. (1996). The process of parenting and the
externalizing problems at age 7. Developmental Psychology, remembrance of things past. Infant Mental Health Journal,
33, 681–692. 17, 217–238.
Main, M. (1995). Attachment: Overview, with implications for Spieker, S.J., & Bensley, L. (1994). Roles of living arrangements
clinical work. In S. Goldberg, R. Muir, & J. Kerr (Eds.), and grandmother social support in adolescent mothering and
Attachment theory: Social, developmental, and clinical per- infant attachment. Developmental Psychology, 30, 102–111.
spectives (pp. 407–474). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic. Teddlie, C., & Tashakkori, A. (2003). Major issues and contro-
Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1998). Adult attachment scoring and versies in the use of mixed methods in the social and behav-
classification system. Unpublished manuscript, University of ioral sciences. In A. Tashakkori & C. Teddlie (Eds.),
California at Berkeley. Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral
Poehlmann, J. (2003). An attachment perspective on grandparents research (pp. 3–50). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
raising their very young grandchildren: Implications for interven- Turner, P.J. (1991). Relations between attachment, gender, and
tion and research. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24, 149–173. behavior with peers in preschool. Child Development, 62,
Posada, G., Waters, E., Crowell, J.A., & Lay, K. (1995). Is it 1475–1488.
easier to use a secure mother as a secure base? Attach- U.S. Bureau of the Census. (2001). Current populations reports:
ment q-sort correlates of the adult attachment interview. Census 2000 summary file 3. (Series P-20, No. 450). Wash-
In E. Waters, B.E. Vaughn, G. Posada, & K. Kondo-Ikemura ington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
(Eds.), Caregiving, cultural, and cognitive perspectives on Vaughn, B., Egeland, B., Sroufe, L.A., & Waters, E. (1979).
secure-base behavior and working models: New growing Individual differences in infant-mother attachment at twelve
points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the and eighteen months: Stability and change in families under
Downloaded By: [Utah State University] At: 23:14 25 January 2011

Society for Research in Child Development, 60 (2–3, Serial stress. Child Development, 50, 971–975.
No. 244). Wakschlag, L.S., Chase-Lansdale, P.L., & Brooks-Gunn, J.
Putallaz, M., Costanzo, P.R., Grimes, C.L., & Sherman, D.M. (1998). (1996). Not just “ghosts in the nursery”: Contemporaneous
Intergenerational continuities and their influences on children’s intergenerational relationships and parenting in young African-
social development. Social Development, 7, 389–421. American families. Child Development, 67, 2131–2147.
Sagi, A., van IJzendoorn, M.H., Scharf, M., Joels, T., Koren- Ward, M.J., & Carlson, E.A. (1995). Associations among adult
Karie, N., Mayseless, O., et al. (1997). Ecological constraint attachment representations, maternal sensitivity, and infant-
for intergenerational transmission of attachment. Interna- mother attachment in a sample of adolescent mothers. Child
tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 287–299 Development, 66, 69–79.
Seifer, R., & Schiller, M. (1995). The role of parenting sensitivity, Waters, E. (1987). Attachment Behavior Q-Set (Revision 3.0).
infant temperament, and dyadic interaction in attachment the- Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, State
ory and assessment. In E. Waters, B.E. Vaughn, G. Posada, & University of New York at Stony Brook.
K. Kondo-Ikemura (Eds.), Caregiving, cultural, and cognitive Waters, E. & Deane, K.E. (1985). Defining and assessing indi-
perspectives on secure-base behavior and working models: vidual differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodol-
New growing points of attachment theory and research. ogy and the organization of behavior in infancy and early
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, childhood. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing
60 (2–3, Serial No. 244, pp. 146–174). points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of
Serbin, L.A., & Karp, J. (2004). The intergenerational transfer of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50, 41–65.
psychosocial risk: Mediators of vulnerability and resilience. Zeanah, C.H., Boris, N.W., & Larrieu, J.A. (1997). Infant devel-
Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 333–363. opment and developmental risk: A review of the past 10
Simpson, J., Rholes, W., & Nelligan, J. (1992). Support seeking years. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adoles-
and support giving within couples in an anxiety-provoking cent Psychiatry, 36, 165–178.

View publication stats

You might also like