You are on page 1of 13

Early Conceptions of Positive Justice as Related to the Development of Logical Operations

Author(s): William Damon


Source: Child Development, Vol. 46, No. 2 (Jun., 1975), pp. 301-312
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1128122 .
Accessed: 29/09/2013 10:21

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Wiley and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to Child Development.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Early Conceptionsof PositiveJustice as Related
to the Developmentof Logical Operations

William Damon
Clark University

DAMON, WILLIAM. Early Conceptions of Positive Justice as Related to the Development of


Logical Operations. CHILD DEVELOPMENT,1975, 46, 301-312. In order to investigate the rela-
tion between the development of justice conceptions and the development of mathematical
and physical conceptions, 50 children (10 at each age from 4 through 8) were administered
a "positive justice" interview and 5 of Piaget's concrete-operationalmathematical and physical
tasks. Results indicate that (1) there is a strong association between the level of a child's
reasoning about positive justice and the level of his reasoning about mathematicaland physical
problems; (2) in the great majority of subjects, there was close synchrony (i.e., little segre-
gation) between parallel levels of reasoningin the 2 domains; and (3) there was little evidence
in support of the notion that 1 type of reasoning (e.g., mathematical-physical) provides a
necessary condition for the development of the other (e.g., justice). An analysis of logical
relations between parallel levels in the 2 domains is presented.

In a number of studies, Piaget and In- itself; it is the logic of values or of action
helder have proposed a stage model which among individuals, just as logic is a kind of
describes the development of children's reason- moral of thought. Honesty, a sense of justice,
ing about mathematical and physical concepts and reciprocity together form a rational system
(Inhelder & Piaget 1964; Piaget 1951; Piaget, of personal values. Without exaggeration this
Inhelder, & Szeminska 1960). The focus of this system can be compared to the 'groupings'
model is the child's acquisition and coordina- of relations or concepts that characterize
tion of logical "operations," reversible mental logic . . ." (Piaget 1968, p. 56). Beyond such
acts which enable the child to reason sys- general characterizations, however, Piaget has
tematically about both the stable and the not posited specific developmental relations
changing properties of reality. Operations are between the domains of logical and moral
first acquired and organized into "groupings" reasoning.
at approximately age 7, during the stage of
concrete operations. They are later reorganized Kohlberg and Gilligan (1971) have pre-
into "groups" at approximately age 11, during sented a model in which the attainment of
the stage of formal operations. certain Piagetian logical stages is a necessary
but not sufficient condition for the attainment
Piaget has suggested, but never tested the of certain stages in Kohlberg's moral judgment
hypothesis, that the development of moral rea-
system. Thus, in this model, fully consolidated
soning in children is a process paralleling the
development of logical operations. In recent concrete-operational thought is necessary for
the emergence of Kohlberg's stage 2 moral-
writings, he has noted that the coordination of
logical operations at the concrete level is sim- ity, and fully consolidated formal-operational
ilar to the structuration of moral' values that thought is necessary for stage 5 morality. This
occurs at approximately the same age: "The model does not, however, specify isomorphic
organization of moral values that characterizes structural elements in the two domains which
middle childhood is . . . comparable to logic might lead to such developmental relations.

An earlier version of this paper was presented at the meeting of the Western Psychological
Association, San Francisco, April 1974. I would like to thank Jonas Langer for his advice on
this research and Wendy Damon for her assistance in collecting data. I would also like to
thank N. Rankin, I. Uzgiris, L. Cirillo, and M. Wiener for their suggestions and comments on
the final versions of this manuscript. Author's address: Department of Psychology, Clark Uni-
versity, Worcester, Massachusetts01610.
[Child Development, 1975, 46, 301-312. @ 1975 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. All
rights reserved.]

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
302 Child Development
Recently there have been some empirical sions or between any aspect of moral reasoning
investigations into the developmental relations and concrete- or formal-operational thought.
between logical reasoning and moral judgment.
Kuhn, Langer, Kohlberg, and Haan (1975) re- By focusing exclusively on the develop-
ment of a specific moral concept-"positive
ported that all of their subjects who reasoned
predominantly at a "principled" moral level justice"-the present paper adopts a position
intermediate between Kohlberg's global model
(Kohlberg's stages 5 and 6) also were capable and Lee's response-oriented conceptualization.
of formal-operational thought-a finding which
lends support to the Kohlberg-Gilligan asser- Justice is viewed as a means of resolving con-
tion regarding the necessity of certain logical flicts between various interpersonal claims,
levels for certain moral levels. Tomlinson- such that the proposed resolution is acceptable
(or "fair") to all significant persons (Rawls
Keasey and Keasey (1974) reported similar
1971). Positive justice is that aspect of justice
findings with two samples of adolescent girls,
and likewise interpreted their results as sup- that is concerned with problems generated in
porting the notion that formal operations are a prosocial interaction: for example, problems of
how to distribute property fairly, of ownership
necessary condition for the development of
moral stages 5 and 6. Lee (1971) found that, and personal rights, of responsibility for an-
in children aged 5-15, an increase in certain other's welfare, and of what constitutes a good
moral responses (e.g., "peer" vs. "authority response to another's actions. Associated with
activities like sharing, taking turns, helping,
type" responses) was associated with the de-
etc., concerns of positive justice have been ob-
velopment of concrete-operational reasoning, served to be central to the social and moral
whereas an increase in certain other moral re-
behavior of children as young as 2 or 3 years
sponses (e.g., "societal order" considerations)
was associated with the development of formal- (see, e.g., Murphy 1937). Such concerns seem
to arise out of the day-to-day interpersonal
operational reasoning. Again, however, none of
the above-mentioned studies presented more contacts of a young child and evolve in the
than a cursory analysis of specific structural course of his ongoing establishment of friend-
relations between the developmental variables ship and affiliated types of social relations. De-
under investigation. velopmental aspects of a subject's conception
of positive justice then include: (1) the types
Part of the difficulty in precisely defining of interpersonal conflict recognized by S; (2)
relations between logical and moral thought the means that S constructs to resolve such
has arisen from the ways in which moral conflicts; (3) the collection of persons whom
thought has been conceptualized. Kohlberg's S considers significant in constructing "fair"
moral stages, for example, are intentionally resolutions to conflicts; and (4) the nature of
global, incorporating such seemingly distin- the justification that S invokes to support his
guishable issues as law, convention, honesty, resolutions.
responsibility, justice, religion, and the value In pilot work (Damon 1971, 1973), inter-
of life. Although Kohlberg has often asserted views with young children about problems in
the fundamental structural unity in these vari-
positive justice revealed that even those chil-
ous moral concerns (Kohlberg 1963, 1970), dren who were diagnosed at a nonmoral level
the basis of this assertion has come increasingly
under question (Peters 1971; Turiel 1974). (stage 0) by Kohlberg's measures nevertheless
showed consistent, patterned reasoning about
Since the relations between the various com-
positive justice. Thus, organized reasoning
ponents of Kohlberg's moral stages are as yet about this concept can be seen relatively early.
uncertain, it is difficult to speculate exactly In addition, the concept of positive justice was
what features of his system bear relations to seen to develop further through a series of six
the development of logical thought. On the distinct phases-hypothesized to be substages
other hand, attempts like Lee's to operation- -which were in turn shown to be closely age
alize moral development have encountered an related between the ages of 4 and 10. A brief
opposite set of problems. By viewing moral synopsis of these six positive-justice substages
development as a simultaneous increase in cer- follows. Though the numbering of these sub-
tain types of responses and decrease in others,
stages matches that of Kohlberg's model, no
Lee was able to quantify her subjects' progress necessary parallelism between this sequence
along various moral dimensions; but this quan- and Kohlberg's stage sequence is intended. Al-
titative approach did not enable her to define though there certainly may be some overlap
structural relations between the moral dimen- between Kohlberg's description of his early

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
William Damon 303
moral stages and the sequence of positive- persons can have different, yet equally valid,
justice conceptions presented here, the exact justifications for their claims to justice. Choices
similarities and differences are difficult to attempt quantitative compromises between
specify because of the global nature of Kohl- competing claims (e.g., he should get the most
berg's stages. but she should get some too).

Stage 0 Substage 2-B.-The S coordinates consid-


erations of equality and reciprocity, such that
Substage 0-A.-Positive-justice choices de-
rive from S's wish that an act occur. Reasons S's positive-justice choices take into account the
claims of various persons and the demands of
simply assert the choices rather than attempt-
the specific situation in relation to one another.
ing to justify them (e.g., I should get it be-
cause I want to have it). Choices are firm and clear-cut, yet justifications
reflect the recognition that all persons should
Substage 0-B.-Choices still reflect S's de- be given their due (though, in many situations,
sires but are now justified on the basis of ex- this does not mean equal treatment).
ternal, observable realities such as size, sex,
or other physical characteristics of persons The present study was undertaken to in-
(e.g., we should get the most because we're vestigate the child's development through the
girls). Such justifications, however, are invoked above justice stages in relation to his develop-
in a fluctuating, a posteriori manner and are ment of concrete-operational reasoning. The
self-serving in the end. purpose of the study was to provide an analysis
of the structural and developmental relations
Stage 1 between this one aspect of moral reasoning and
Substage 1-A.-Positive-justice choices de- logical thought. Three areas of isomorphism
rive from notions of strict equality in actions between positive-justice conceptions and logi-
(e.g., everyone should get the same). Justifica- cal reasoning were postulated: (1) classifica-
tions are consistent with this principle but are tory acts, (2) compensatory acts, and (3)
unilateral and inflexible. perspective-taking acts. It was posited that
Substage 1-B.-Positive-justice choices de- classificatory skills would be manifested in
rive from a notion of reciprocity in actions: justice reasoning through S's grouping of per-
that persons should be paid back in kind for sons and their justice claims; that acts of com-
doing good or bad things. Notions of merit pensation would be seen in S's construction of
and deserving emerge. Justifications are uni- reciprocal relations between persons and acts,
lateral and inflexible. as in sharing, paying back, notions of desert,
etc.; and that perspective taking would influ-
Stage 2 ence S's mode of justifying his choices to an-
Substage 2-A.-A moral relativity devel- other, as well as the way in which S concep-
ops out of the understanding that different tualizes interpersonal conflict. Table 1 presents,

TABLE 1
HYPOTHESIZEDRELATIONSBETWEENSTAGESOF JUSTICE CONCEPTIONAND OPERATIONAL
REASONINGON
THREE ISOMORPHICDIMENSIONS

ISOMORPHICDIMENSIONS
JUSTICE OPERATIONAL
SUBSTAGE STAGE Classification Behavior Compensation Behavior Perspective Taking
0-B 1-intuitive Graphic collections; par- Quantitative correspon- No differentiation of
thought tial alignments; shifting dences (one-to-one or self's point of view from
classificatory criteria; size matched) that of other
transductive logic
1-B 2--preoperational, Nongraphic collections; Acts of reciprocal order- Awareness that other
transitional fixation on certain lim- ing along a single di- may have perspective
thought ited though stable cri- mension; no anticipation different from self's
teria of alternative possibilities
2-B 3--concrete Flexible construction of Coordination of recipro- Perspectives of others
operations hierarchical classes; use cal acts; anticipatory viewed in relation to that
of negation operations; compensations of self; ability to con-
advent of foresight, hind- struct all possible points
sight, and anticipation of view

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
304 Child Development
with regard to these three types of acts, the mommy told you, "No, you can't let [Jimmy] play
developmental relations between children's with your toys anymore"?Why is it a good thing
conceptions of positive justice and their levels to share? Who gets the biggest piece of dessert in
of operational reasoning. The relations postu- your family? Is that fair? Should everyone get the
lated in table 1 are, in effect, the hypotheses same amount? Who should get the most dessert?
to be tested in this study. Why?
b) Here are some toys [poker chips]. Let's
Since the "B" justice substages are the
pretend your best friend [Jimmy] is sitting right
final, fully realized versions of each stage, the here. Now I'm going to give you all these toys to
relations posited in table 1 refer to justice sub- play with. Would you give Jimmy any of them?
stages 0-B, 1-B, and 2-B. The operational Which ones would you give him? Which ones
stages in this table refer to those proposed by would you keep yourself? What if he wanted all
Inhelder and Piaget (1964) and Piaget (1951) these-what would you say to him? Why don't
in describing the development of concrete you take them all for yourself?
operations.
Dilemma 1.-All of these boys and girls are
in the same class together. One day their teacher
Method let them spend the whole afternoon making paint-
ings and crayon drawings. The teacher thought
Subjects that these pictures were so good that the class
The Ss were 50 middle- and upper- could sell them at the fair. They sold the pictures
middle-class children, five boys and five girls to their parents, and together the class made a
at each of ages 4, 5, 6, 7, and 8. The Ss were whole lot of money. Now all the children gathered
drawn from a preschool and an elementary the next day and tried to decide how to split up
school in Berkeley, California. the money. What do you think they should do
with it? Why? Kathy says that the kids in the
Procedure class who made the most pictures should get most
of the money. Andy says the kids who made the
Each S was administered a positive-justice best ones should get the most. What do you think?
interview and a set of mathematical and physi- There were some lazy kids in the class who didn't
cal tasks designed to assess his level of op- draw very much in comparison to the others.
erational reasoning. As a control against possi- What about them? Jim says that the best-behaved
ble experimenter bias, the justice interview was kids should get more than the rest. Lisa says that
administered by one E and the operational the poor kids should get the money, because they
tasks were administered by another. For half don't have much. Someone says that the teacher
the Ss, the justice interview was administered should get the money, because it was her idea to
1 or 2 weeks prior to the operational tasks; sell the pictures. What do you think?
for the other Ss, the order was reversed.
The remaining three dilemmas probed
Instruments further for issues raised in the sharing ques-
The justice interview consisted of (1) an tions and dilemma 1 and were used to obtain
a further sampling of S's justice reasoning.
open-ended interview focusing on sharing and
a sharing problem adapted from Ugurel-Semin Briefly, the second dilemma pitted a smart but
(1952), and (2) four dilemmas of the type lazy girl against a dull but hard-working boy:
used by Kohlberg but newly designed espe- who should get the most stars (i.e., the better
cially for the presentation of positive-justice grade) on schoolwork? The third dilemma
conflicts to young children.1 The sharing task posed the problem of a boy who was too poor
and the first of the dilemmas are presented to get an allowance: should he receive a free
below. candy bar even when all the others had to pay
for theirs? In the fourth dilemma, four
children worked to earn bicycles: should
Sharing task.-(a) Who's your best friend?
they share their earnings with their father,
Do you let him play with your toys? Why? [Why
not?] What would happen if you said, "No among themselves, etc.? S's reasons for
his choices were always elicited by a series of
[Jimmy], you can't play with my toys?" What
happens if he won't let you play with his toys? probing questions and counterconditions. The
Does your mommy tell you to share your toys? Is probes were administered in a schedule that
that why you do it? What would you do if your was standard across Ss, so that each S had a
1 All
procedures and scoring materials for the justice tasks are available on request from
the author.

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
William Damon 305
chance to respond to each of the questions and then she wouldn't get as much as all of these kids.
counterconditions. But each interview was also Well, what about the poor kid here, who doesn't
administered with considerable flexibility, the have any money to begin with? Well, the poor kid
order and wording of every probe varying to should make some stuff, then he'd get more....
fit the pattern of the individual S's responses. What about the lazy kid? Well, he shouldn't get
as much if he didn't work as much, if he didn't do
In addition, S was always asked to repeat the his work. ....
story of the dilemma or otherwise show com-
prehension of the plot before the questions 2-A.-What if Kathy made more stuff? Should
were administered. she get more money? Oh, about seven more pen-
nies. It depends on what she made-if she made
A typed transcript of each S's justice in- something easy or hard. What if she made some-
terview was prepared. All protocols were thing hard? About ten cents more. What about
scored blind, with no information concerning Andy, who made the best stuff-should he get
more? Well, maybe he should. But since she made
age or identity of S. Each of the five justice
tasks-the more, she may have some good ones, so then maybe
sharing questions and the four he can get around five cents more. What about
dilemmas-was scored according to a scoring
Billy, who doesn't get any allowance? He should
guide which described characteristics of each only get about three cents more because-if he
of the six justice substages. Examples of re-
got a lot more, he might even have more than any-
sponses scored at each of the six substages body adding up their allowance. What about these
follow. others? No, because they don't have such a big
reason.
O-A.-Suppose you and Sammy are playing
2-B.-Should the kids who were most coop-
together and you have these [five] toys? Would erative get bigger shares? No! Because that really
you give him any? I would give him these two. doesn't make much sense. They are not in a con-
Why those two? Because I got to keep three. test about attitude and how you share with other
These are the ones I like. Suppose Sammy said,
"I want to have more"? If he took one then I people. They don't care about that. They just want
would take one back from him. Why is that? Be- to have people do good stuff, they don't care about
cause I want three... . What will Sammy do that. How about giving bigger shares to the poor
then? He will say that's O.K., because he likes kids? No. They [the class] don't care if they [the
these [toys]. poor kids] are poor or not. Well, we might feel a
little sorry for them. But they don't care about
that. They just want the ones who did the best to
O-B.-[Keeps four blue chips and gives Jenny
two white chips.] You would keep all these blues get the most money. And why is that? I just said,
for yourself? Yes, because I like blue. And then that way they'll all try to do better next time.
rd play with them. Let's pretend Jenny said, "I
like blue." Would you give her any blue? Never, Each of the positive-justice tasks was
because I have a blue dress at home. So you
given a stage score that reflected the reasoning
wouldn't give her any blue at all. Is it fair to do level predominantly used on that task by S.
it this way? Ah, ha, I've got it. I'd give her two Since all of these tasks probed the same funda-
of the white--'d give her those [two] because mental conceptual issue, an overall "major"
she's younger than me, and I get four because I'm
four. justice-stage score was determined for each S
on the basis of his scores on the majority of
1-A.-Do you think anyone should get any tasks; for example, if S scored 1-B on four of
more than anyone else? No, because it's not fair. the tasks and 2-A on one, his major justice-
Somebody has 350 and somebody has one penny. stage score was 1-B. Overall, an average of
That's not fair.... Andy said that he thought the
one who made the best stuff should get the most 74% of Ss' justice task scores were at the
money. The best person who makes te best stuff major substage level, 24 % were one substage
is not polite, because you should make them have higher or lower, and 2 % were two substages
the same alike-give everything the same. The higher or lower. It should also be noted that
same, because it's not polite when you give people there was not always a clear, scorable response
the most and they [the rest] don't have one. It's by S on all five justice tasks. In such cases, the
fair to the other children that they have to get it task(s) in question were not scored, and the
too.
major stage score was derived from S's per-
1-B.--But Kathy thought that she should get formance on the remainder of the tasks.
the most because she made the most things. Do
you think that's fair? Well, if she made more Five operational tasks (OTs), consisting
things, then she'd get more money. What if she of problems in mathematical and physical
didn't make--If she didn't want to make anything, logic, were administered to S. Each OT se-

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
306 Child Development
lected for the study was assumed to elicit one was scored blind. Each S was given an op-
of the three dimensions, outlined above, which erational-stage score on each of the five OTs.
were posited to be isomorphic with both logi- Since the OTs reflected distinct (though re-
cal and justice reasoning. The first two OTs lated) operational abilities, no major OT score
were designed by Inhelder and Piaget (1964) was given.
to study the development of classification be-
havior. The OT 1 was the multiplicative classi- Reliability of scoring for both the justice
fication problem, and OT 2 was a class-inclu- and the operational measures was determined
sion problem. The OT 3, the "49 leaves" for a randomly chosen set of approximately
one-fourth (12) of the Ss. Research assistants
problem, was designed to study the child's use
of the logic of relations in constructing multiple were given copies of the scoring guides for the
seriations (Inhelder and Piaget 1964) and thus two sets of measures. For the justice scores,
represented a problem in logical compensation. reliability was determined by calculating the
The OT 4, Piaget's "mountains test," examines percentage of perfect agreement between two
the child's ability to take Euclidean spatial per- raters on the S's stage score, as well as the per-
spectives (Piaget 1951). A scoring guide based centage of disagreement by one substage, two
on Piaget's stage descriptions for these tasks substages, etc. For the justice scores, there was
was prepared. 83% perfect agreement between the two raters
and 17% disagreement by one substage. The
The fifth OT was a new task adapted justice ratings were never more than one sub-
from a series of experiments performed by stage apart. For the operational-stage scores,
Piaget et al. (1960) and Piaget, Grize, Szemin- there was 90% perfect agreement between the
ska, and Vinh Bang (1968), intended to diag- two raters and 10% disagreement by one
nose the child's ability to construct schemes of stage. Operational ratings were never more
proportional measurement (e.g., ratios). In so than one stage apart.
doing, the task taps the child's use and co-
ordination of both compensation and negation
Results
(see Piaget et al. 1960, p. 334). In this task,
S was presented with four cardboard pies, A, All Ss were ranked by age, justice-stage
B, C, and D. The pies were gradated in size, score, and stage score on each of the five OTs.
with A the largest and D the smallest. Pie A The Spearman rank order formula was used to
was divided into fourths, pie B into halves, calculate coefficients of correlation between all
pie C into eighths, and pie D into fourths. Cor- of the variables.
responding to the four pies were four paper
dolls, gradated in size proportionately to the In replication of the earlier finding men-
four pies. The S was told that the problem was tioned above, there was a significant level of
to be sure that each doll's stomach was always association between age and justice-stage score,
just as full of pie as were all the other dolls' r(48) = .85, p < .001. Justice substage 0-A
stomachs. The S was asked to figure out how was found predominantly at age 4; 0-B at ages
many pieces of their pies each doll should get 4 and 5; 1-A at ages 5 and 6; 1-B at ages 6
if doll A were given one piece (one-fourth) and 7; 2-A at age 8; and 2-B at age 8. In
of pie A, and then to figure out how many addition, there was a significant level of asso-
pieces each should get if doll A were given ciation, p < .001, between age and stage
two pieces (one-half) of pie A. Scoring criteria scores on each of the five OTs; for OT 1,
similar to those used by Piaget for similar tasks r(48) = .66; OT 2, r(48) = .59; OT 3, r(32)
were developed. = .65; OT 4, r(48) = .73; OT 5, r(48) =
.64. The association between age and justice
All 50 Ss were given all five OTs with the was significantly higher, p < .01, than was the
exception of OT 3, which, because of admin- association between age and any of the OTs.
istrative difficulties, was given to only 34 Ss.
Table 2 reports the intercorrelations be-
For each of the five OTs, there were three tween justice-stage scores and the five OT-
possible stage scores: 1 (intuitive), 2 (transi- stage scores. All of these relations were signifi-
tional), and 3 (concrete operational). How- cant at the p < .001 level. The correlation
ever, for OT 2, stages 1 and 2 were not easily between justice-stage score and OT 4 (the
distinguishable and were collapsed, for the spatial perspectives task) was significantly
present study, into one (stage 1-2). As with higher, p < .05, than was the correlation be-
the justice interviews, each S's typed protocol tween justice and any of the other OTs.

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
William Damon 307
TABLE 2
OF JUSTICE-STAGESCORESAND STAGESCORESON FIVE OPERATIONAL
INTERCORRELATIONS TASKS (OTs)

Justice OT 1 OT 2 OT 3 OT 4 OT 5
Justice ............................ .........
OT 1 (multiplicativeclassification)... .76 ... ...
OT 2 (class inclusion) .............. .76 .63
OT 3 (seriation) ................. .78a .77a .67a
OT 4 (Euclidianspatial perspectives) .88 .75 .73 .83a
OT 5 (proportionality)............. .77 .69 .65 .72a .76
NOTE.-All correlationsare significant at the p < .001 level. Unless otherwise specified, = 48.
a D = 32. d-

In addition to calculating the overall cor- intersection of (1) justice substage 2-B and
relations between the variables, the association (2) all other justice substages with (1) op-
between justice and each of the OTs was ex- erational stage 3 and (2) all other operational
amined at each of the five age levels studied. stages. Fisher Exact Tests were performed on
Table 3 reports the strength of these associa- each of these tables, examining the extent to
tions. In general there were few significant cor- which the following intersections represented
relations at the earliest age levels (4 and 5), deviations from their expected values: justice
whereas at ages 6 and above, justice-stage substage 0-B x operational stage 1; 1-B X 2;
scores were significantly correlated with OT and 2-B X 3. The results of these analyses are
scores on the great majority of tasks. But it reported in table 5.
should also be noted that there was relatively
little variability of scores on all measures at In sum, the following patterns are re-
the youngest ages. flected in tables 4 and 5. For justice substages
0-A and 0-B, OT scores are primarily at pre-
Table 4 shows the frequencies of justice- operational stage 1. At 1-A begins a distinct
stage scores across OT-stage scores on each of shift toward operational stage 2, and by justice
the five OTs. To test for the hypothesized re- substage 1-B there is a distinct clustering of
lations presented in table 1, three 2 X 2 fre- OT scores at operational stage 2. At justice
quency tables were constructed for each OT. substages 2-A and 2-B, OT scores are mostly
One of the three showed the intersection of at stage 3 (the level of concrete-operational
(1) justice substage 0-B and (2) all other jus- reasoning). No Ss at 0-A justice showed con-
tice substages with (1) operational stage 1 and crete-operational (stage 3) reasoning on any
(2) all other operational substages. The second OT; and only one 0-B S showed stage 3 reason-
showed the intersection of (1) justice substage
ing on one OT (OT 2). At the most advanced
1-B and (2) all other justice substages with levels, all Ss at 2-B justice performed at opera-
(1) operational stage 2 and (2) all other op- tional stage 3 on all five OTs (with the ex-
erational stages. The third table showed the
ception that one 2-B S was not given OT 3).
TABLE 3 Discussion
CORRELATIONSBETWEENJUSTICE-STAGESCOREAND
OT-STAGE SCORESFOR EACH OF FIVE AGE LEVELS The results of this study support the gen-
eral hypothesis of a strong association between
JUSTICE OPERATIONAL
TASKS the level of children's reasoning about positive
SCORE justice and the level of their reasoning about
BYAGE OT 1 OT 2 OT 3 OT4 OT 5
logicomathematical and logicophysical concep-
Justice score tions. With the exception of the children at the
at age: youngest age level, this association generally
4 ..... .10 .10 .08a -.02 -.22 held among children of the same age, an indi-
5 ..... .34 0 .87*b .59* .42
6 ..... .56* .84** .62C .58* .29 cation of a special relation between the vari-
7 ..... .55* .86** .62c .89** .75** ables beyond the manifest age relatedness of
8 ..... .82** .52 .87*b .70* .80** each. The failure to find this association among
NOTE.-Unless otherwise specified, df = 8. the 4-year-olds and the partial failure to find
a DI = 6.
b DI = 4. it among the 5-year-olds were likely due to the
Di
=D5.
* p
<.05. uniformly low response levels (and hence the
**
p<.01. almost negligible degree of stage variance on

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
308 Child Development
TABLE4
FREQUENCY DISTRIBUTION OF JUSTICE-STAGE SCORES ACROSS OPERATIONAL-STAGE SCORES
ON FIVE OPERATIONALTASKS

OPERATIONAL STAGE SCORES BY OT

OT1 OT 2 OT 3 OT 4 OT 5
JUSTICE STAGE SCORE 1 2 3 1-2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3
0-A ...... ........... .. 5 ... ... 5 ... 4 ... ... 5 ... ... 4 1
0-B .................. 7 3 ... 9 1 4 2 ... 8 2 ... 9 1
1-A .................. 2 8 1 10 1 2 6 ... 6 5 ... 9 2
1-B .................. 2 7 2 8 3 1 4 3 1 10 ... 4 5 2
2-A .................. ... 4 3 ... 7 ... 1 2 ... 2 5 2 2 3
2-B .................. ... ... 6 ... 6 ... ... 5 ... ... 6 ... ... 6
NOTE.-For OT 3, N = 34; for all others, N = 50.

all measures) found among the youngest sub- speculate on extrinsic factors leading to this
jects. stronger relation, for example, particular en-
vironmental influences such as schooling en-
The relationbetween justice stage and OT countered at various ages and having a differ-
score on the spatialperspectivestask was found
ential effect on reasoningin the two domains.
to be significantlyhigher than the relation be- But this study provides no evidence for or
tween justice stage and any other OT score.
Such a finding lends some support to certain against such speculations.
theoretical interpretations differing from the The hypotheses presented in table 1 pos-
model of structuralisomorphismbetween do- ited particular relations between justice and
mains asserted here. Two such alternative
operational reasoning at specified parallel
models are (1) Feffer's (1970) contentionthat levels. These hypothesized relationswere gen-
the development of both interpersonal and erally supported by the Fisher Exact analyses
mathematical-physicalconceptions reflects a of cell distributions(tables 4 and 5), although
processof decentering,which is uniquely allied the support was less than perfect and varied
to perspective-takingacts in each domain; and
widely in its strength from point to point. An
(2) Kohlberg's(1971) argumentthat all moral analysisof the distributionsreportedin table 4
judgmenthas a perspective-takingbasis, which is offered in an attempt to explain the mutual
leads to the conclusion that particularlystrong
relationsshould be expected between any facet growth of the two forms of reasoning.The dis-
cussionwill focus on justice substages0-B, 1-B,
of moral reasoning (e.g., justice conceptions) and 2-B in relation to their corresponding
and perspective taking as manifested in either levels of operationalreasoning. As in table 1,
social or spatial contexts. three reasoningdimensionselicited by the OTs
The strongerrelation between justice and will be invoked to analyze parallel levels of
age than between any OT and age could be operational thought: classification,compensa-
due to methodological factors. For example, tion, and perspectivetaking.
the justice score was comprised of a consider-
ably largersamplingof S's reasoningthan were Justice SubstageO-B
any of the OT scores, and thus may have rep- On most of the OTs, Ss at 0-B justice
resented a more stable score. One might also scored mostly at operationalstage 1, although

TABLE 5
FISHER EXACT TEST LEVELSOF SIGNIFICANCEFOR CELLS SHOWING INTERACTIONOF JUSTICE SUBSTAGE
0-B, 1-B, AND 2-B WITH OPERATIONAL
STAGE1, 2, AND 3, RESPECTIVELY

OPERATIONAL STAGE BY OT

OT 1 OT 2 OT 3 OT 4 OT 5
JUSTICE SUBSTAGE 1 2 3 1-2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3
0-B ................. .01 .... ....07 .07 ... .. .005 ...... .05......
1-B .................. ... .18 ... .72 ... ... . 68.. ... ... .001 ... ... .05 ...
2-B .................. ... ... .001 ... .001 ... ... .001 ... ... .001 ... ... .001

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
William Damon 309
there was some beginning movement toward of compensation in the justice domain. These
operational stage 2, particularly on OT 1. quantitative correspondences serve to match
1. Fluctuatingclassificationcriteria.-The physical attributes with amounts of reward
(e.g., the boys should get more, the biggest
distinguishing characteristic of stage 1 classifi- should get the most, etc.). In such cases, the
cation behavior (OTs 1 and 2) is the forma-
tion of partial alignments and chain complexes. quantitative correspondence is an early form
of merit, that is, a preferential treatment of se-
The stage 1 S often begins grouping objects on lected personal attributes. But at 0-B there is
the basis of one common criterion (e.g., color) no pay back in these premerit notions-that is,
and then shifts to another criterion (e.g., there is no conception that reward can be a
shape) without noticing the inconsistency in compensation for being good, doing good, etc.
his behavior. The result of this "chaining" of -and therefore the 0-B form of compensation
classificatory criteria is a graphic collection is only precursory. Statements of direct reci-
rather than either a nongraphic collection or
a class. At 0-B in the justice domain, there is procity-the linking of prior events to present
action-are also precursory at 0-B. The 0-B S
a similar fluctuating of the criteria used in jus- can relate events by means of descriptive reci-
tifying certain judgments-specifically, judg- procity statements, describing action-reaction
ments about which persons in a situation are
sequences (e.g., tit for tat) but does not yet
worthy of regard. A 0-B S will often begin jus- prescribe such reactions. In this sense also 0-B
tifying a choice by citing one type of personal reciprocity is precursory, for without prescrip-
attribute as a basis for the choice (e.g., she
should get more because she's a girl) but will tivity a statement cannot function as a justice
then shift to another attribute in justifying the judgment.
same choice (e.g., give it to her because she's 3. Rudimentaryperspectivism.-At stage
the youngest). Often the main consistency in 1 on OT 4, S could reconstruct only his own
0-B justifications is that they serve to support a point of view. If the question is simple enough
choice that favors the self or those identified (e.g., when it involves a binary choice like
with the self. At such times, the 0-B justifica- "Can the doll see the river from his side of the
tions show many of the same transductive char- mountain?") the stage 1 S can sometimes dif-
acteristics as do the stage 1 partial alignments ferentiate his viewpoint from that of others.
and chain complexes of class logic. The 0-B But, when reconstructing a viewpoint that re-
child cannot yet group valued personal attri- quires the systematic transposition of spatial
butes into coherent collections that are defined relations like left-right or before-behind, the S
by stable intentional properties. Without this invariably falls back to reproducing his own
basic classification ability, the 0-B child cannot point of view. At 0-B in the justice domain, the
construct judgments that systematically sup- S begins to recognize certain conflicts in de-
port the rewarding of selected personal at- sire. For example, the child can see that he
tributes or deeds. The child is confined either might want more dessert than his sister and
to rewarding a fluctuating variety of physical that at the same time his sister might want
characteristics that he conceives as related to more than he. Also, unlike S at 0-A, S at 0-B
the self, or to constructing quantitative cor- sees the need to justify his choices to another
respondences. on a basis more objective than a statement of
his own desires. But the 0-B child constructs
2. Precursoryacts of compensationin the his justice judgments primarily from his own
form of quantitative correspondences.-With
point of view. Although 0-B justifications often
regard to compensation at operational stage 1, take external factors into account, 0-B choices
there were two manifestations. First, on OT 3,
the stage 1 child at best could arrange leaves usually serve the purpose of rewarding the
into "couplets" based on a figurative matching egocentric wishes of the self. Whatever a
of sizes or colors. He could not construct re- posteriori justifications the S might offer, the
0-B judgment attempts to assure pleasant con-
ciprocally seriated orderings of all the leaves
on the basis of either criterion. Second, on OT sequences for the self or for those associated
5, the stage 1 child employed a one-to-one cor- with the self (e.g., those of the same sex). The
respondence of pie pieces to dolls as a means only consistent exceptions to this type of judg-
of filling up each doll the same amount. This ment at 0-B are those justice choices based
one-to-one correspondence was the stage 1 on simple quantitative correspondences (see
precursor of proportional compensation for above). In any case, it is clear that, although
each doll's size. Similarly, quantitative corre- the 0-B S can recognize certain justice con-
spondences function as a 0-B precursory form flicts, he cannot conceptualize them from

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
310 Child Development
points of view other than his own. The 0-B 2. Consolidationof compensatoryreason-
choice is still primarily an egocentric assertion ing along a single dimension.-At stage 2 on
of the S's own desires, unaffected by a possibly OT 3, S can order objects in a trial-and-error
conflicting justice claim of another. As at stage manner on the basis of one criterion. He can-
1 on OT 4, there seems to be no way at 0-B not coordinate two criteria (e.g., size and
to construct the other's position as opposed to color) to form a multiplicative arrangement.
that of the self. Thus, acts of reciprocal ordering at operational
On the basis of several important cogni- stage 2 are limited to a single series and do
not bear the possibility of coordination with
tive dimensions, 0-B justice reasoning is sim- other acts of reciprocal ordering. At justice
ilar to typical stage 1 logical reasoning. Of par-
ticular interest is the empirical finding that substage 1-B, S's compensatory justice reason-
no 0-B Ss scored higher than stage 2 on any ing develops in a number of ways. First, the
child begins to make prescriptive reciprocity
OT. Thus, there is reason to suspect, at this statements such as "I'll share with her if she
level, close informative relations between logi- shares with me." Second, S now infuses the
cal and justice reasoning, such that the segre-
concept of merit with a pay-back notion: S
gation of the two systems in a normal individual considers certain acts or personal qualities to
is a rare occurrence.
be meritorious and thus deserving of special
Justice Substage 1-B compensation. Justice judgments at 1-B are
At 1-B in the justice domain, table 4 thus primarily determined by compensatory
shows a distinct movement of Ss to operational reasoning in the form either of direct, pre-
stage 2 on OTs 4 and 5 and a consolidation of scriptive statements or of rewarding special
Ss at stage 2 on OTs 1 and 3. On OT 2, there claims to merit. But a reciprocity judgment at
is the small beginning of a movement toward 1-B is uncoordinated with equality judgments,
as well as with other coexisting forms of reci-
stage 3. These empirical relations support the
assertion that justice substage 1-B and opera- procity judgment. The S at 1-B is unable to
tional stage 2 are developmentally parallel. reconcile a plurality of conflicting justice
claims, each of which he may recognize as
1. Grouping of persons through use of worthy in itself. The typical 1-B solution to
constant, noncontradictorycriteria.-At stage such conflicts is to imbue with absolute moral
2 on OT 1, S was able to construct consistent, rightness one justice claim that is justifiable on
nongraphic collections through use of one reciprocal grounds. At 1-B, S does not weigh
or more of the available classificatory criteria. individual claims in light of one another or in
These collections were inflexible and non- light of the context from which they arise.
related: the stage 2 S could not switch criteria
On OT 5, the stage 2 S for the first time
once he had constructed his four arrangements,
and these arrangements could not themselves establishes an approximate proportional bal-
ance between size of doll and amount of pie.
be organized into sub- or supercollections. In The establishment of this kind of approximate
the justice domain, the 1-B S continues the amount of pie requires a compensation be-
process begun at 1-A of constructing justice tween the number of fractional pieces and the
judgments on the basis of consistently valued absolute size of each piece. But the stage 2
claims or attributes. These selected claims de-
termine the judgments rather than merely jus- proportionalities are only approximate, because
such reciprocal acts are uncoordinated with
tifying them as at the 0 substages. Whereas at acts of operational negation. This lack of co-
1-A the valued claims derive from equality cri-
ordination results in the S's inability to con-
teria, at 1-B they derive from considerations serve fractional relations and therefore in his
of merit. But the S's means of dealing with
these justice criteria is essentially the same at inability to construct exact ratios. And so again
1-B as it was at 1-A, even though the criteria we see at operational stage 2 the emergence of
uncoordinated acts of reciprocity paralleling
are somewhat different. At both substages, the the 1-B justice reasoning described above.
judgments are consistent but inflexible. Once
the S has decided whether a given personal 3. Partial but still-uncoordinated per-
attribute is worthy of reward, this judgment spective taking.-The S at stage 2 on OT 4
remains unaltered by changes in situation or attempts to distinguish between different view-
presentation of new, conflicting justice claims. points but cannot establish a system of co-
This absolutistic nature of judgments charac- ordination that will enable him to conserve the
terizes both justice substages 1-A and 1-B. relative position of objects across changes in

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
William Damon 311
viewpoint. When S does succeed at correctly 2-A. Such data support the initial assertion of
reconstructing part of another perspective, it developmental synchrony between justice 2-B
is usually with reference to perceptual cues and the stage of concrete operations.
rather than to a system of coordinated spatial
relations like left-right, before-behind. At 1-B The Development of OperationalGroupings
justice, S begins to recognize a wider range of and the Coordinationof Justice Elements
conflicts than do subjects at earlier stages. The At the stage of concrete operations S con-
1-B S recognizes justice claims arising from structs logical operations that are organized
merit consideration (see above); and, by rec- into groupings. Such groupings are necessary
for stage 3 solutions to each of the operational
ognizing these claims, becomes aware of situ-
ations in which they are placed in conflict with tasks. For one thing, these groupings give rise
to the anticipation that allows the stage 3 S
opposing justice considerations. But the 1-B S
cannot relate one claim to another in any sys- to construct flexible hierarchies on the multi-
tematic, coordinated manner. The different jus- plicative classification task (OT 1) and to sys-
tice claims are selected or rejected on their tematically construct all possible perspectives
own merits, independent of the opposing on the perspectives tasks (OT 4). Also, the
claims or of the specific situation in which the groupings establish an organization of opera-
claims have arisen. There is no attempt at 1-B tions that enables the S to coordinate acts of
to mediate between the various possible justice reciprocal ordering on the seriation task (OT
positions, nor to organize these positions into 3) and to perform successive acts of reciprocity
a flexible hierarchy capable of dealing with in- and negation on the proportionality task (OT
dividual situations. Such attempts are not pos- 5) and the inclusion task (OT 2).
sible at 1-B, because the S cannot yet systemat-
Now, something very similar to a group-
ically consider a plurality of justice claims. ing of operations occurs at substage 2-B in the
Each justice claim remains unrelated to any of
the others. The S at 1-B justice, like S at op- justice domain: the justice elements of equality
and reciprocity become coordinated for the
erational stage 2, can reconstruct certain posi- first time. The child at 2-B, like the S at 2-A,
tions, or isolated parts of these positions, but considers the equal worth and relative value of
in neither case has S established a system of
different justice claims. But the 2-B child can
relating the positions or the isolated parts. make an additional judgment as to which claim
Without such a system of reference, S cannot is most justifiable in the current situation. Such
represent (or, in justice reasoning, weigh to- a judgment requires the ability to hierarchize
gether) the various possible perspectives or the various justice claims in ways that are ap-
points of view.
propriate to varying contexts. The ability to
As should be expected from the preceding construct flexible hierarchies of disparate jus-
tice claims arises from the coordination of
discussion, table 4 shows a distinct clustering
of 1-B Ss at operational stage 2 on OTs diag- justice elements, which enables the child to
nosing S's reciprocity and perspective-taking successively weigh the demands of equality
abilities (OTs 3, 4, and 5), and a continuation and reciprocity. The coordination at 2-B im-
of trends begun at 1-A toward operational plies only a successive weighing of justice
demands. It is not until stage 3 that a simul-
stage 2 on those OTs which examine classifica-
tion abilities (OTs 1 and 2). But it is impor- taneous donsideration of equality and reciproc-
tant to note that, on each OT, there was at ity will emerge.
least one 1-B S who scored at operational stage The similarities between stage 3 opera-
1. Such data tend to disconfirm the hypothesis tional reasoning and substage 2-B justice are
that operational stage 2 provides a necessary clear. In each case there is an initial coordina-
condition for the emergence of justice substage tion of mental acts-logical operations in the
1-B.
operational domain, justice elements in the
justice domain. These coordinations unite vari-
Justice Substage 2-B ous forms of reciprocal and inverse reasoning
For no S at 2-B (N = 6) was operational into organized systems. Such systems enable
reasoning lower than stage 3 on any OT. At the S to construct flexible hierarchies that can
2-B we see distinct shifts from operational be rearranged at will. Finally, the initial co-
stage 2 to operational stage 3 on OTs 1, 3, 4, ordinations established at operational stage 3
and 5, whereas on OT 2 the 2-B Ss maintain and at justice 2-B enable S to successively per-
the shift to stage 3 that was begun at justice form mental acts based on reciprocity, nega-

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
312 Child Development
tion, and equality. The simultaneous perfor- Feffer, M. A developmental analysis of interper-
mance of such acts will not be possible until a sonal behavior. Psychological Review, 1970,
later stage in each domain-formal operations 77, 197-214.
in the logical domain (Inhelder & Piaget 1958) Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. The growth of logical
and stage 3 in the justice domain. But such thinking from childhood through adolescence.
later stages are beyond the scope of this study. New York: Basic, 1958.
Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. The early growth of logic
In table 4, all cells showing the intersec- in the child. New York: Harper & Row, 1964.
tion of 2-B justice and operational stages 1
Kohlberg, L. The development of children's orien-
and 2 are empty for each OT. At this one level tations toward a moral order, I: Sequence in
of reasoning, then, the data do not contradict the development of moral thought. Vita Hu-
the possibility that an operational level might mana, 1963, 6, 11-33.
provide a necessary condition for the existence Kohlberg, L. Stages of moral development as a
of a justice level. But such findings are incon- basis for moral education. In C. Beck & E.
clusive, not only because of the small N in- Sullivan (Eds.), Moral education. Toronto:
volved but also because 2-B justice and op-
University of Toronto Press, 1970.
erational stage 3 are the final points in the
Kohlberg, L., & Gilligan, C. The adolescent as a
developmental scales under investigation. It is philosopher. Daedalus, 1971, 28, 162-215.
safer at this point to conclude that, at the most Kuhn, D.; Langer, J.; Kohlberg, L.; & Haan, N.
advanced levels of reasoning investigated in The presence of formal operational thought
this study, there is once again evidence of structures in adolescence and adulthood. Ge-
synchrony between the two domains in most netic Psychology Monographs,1975 (in press).
Ss. There is relatively little segregation of the Lee, L. C. The concomitant development of cog-
two types of reasoning in most Ss studied: nitive and moral modes of thought: a test of
with only two exceptions, all Ss scoring at selected deductions from Piaget's theory.
stage 3 on any OT were at 1-B or higher in Genetic Psychology Monographs, 1971, 83,
their justice reasoning. Again, the data attest 93-146.
to the existence of close, informative relations
Murphy, L. Social behavior and child personality.
between the two domains. New York: Columbia University Press, 1937.
It appears that most of the data presented Peters, R. Moral development: a plea for plural-
here discount the notion that certain levels of ism. In T. Mischel (Ed.), Cognitive develop-
ment and epistemology. New York: Academic
logicomathematical thinking are strictly neces- Press, 1971.
sary for certain levels of moral thinking. Only
at the final points in each system did the data Piaget, J. The child's conception of space. London:
not contradict such a notion (although the data Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1951.
were also not strong enough to support it Piaget, J. The mental development of the child.
In D. Elkind (Ed.), Six psychological studies.
adequately). Taken as a whole, the develop- New York: Random House, 1968.
mental patterns presented here suggest that
Piaget, J.; Grize, J.; Szeminska, A.; & Vinh Bang.
logical and moral reasoning inform and support
each other in the course of ontogenesis as a Epistdmologie et psychologie de la fonction.
result of isomorphic structural features in the Paris: Presses Universitairesde France, 1968.
two domains. The priority of logical to moral Piaget, J.; Inhelder, B.; & Szeminska, A. The
reasoning does not appear necessary in devel- child's conception of geometry. New York:
Basic, 1960.
opment: even among normal Ss, the pattern
Rawls, J. A theory of justice. Cambridge, Mass.:
may be quite the reverse. Harvard University Press, 1971.
Tomlinson-Keasey,C., & Keasey, C. B. The medi-
References ating role of cognitive development in moral
judgment. Child Development, 1974, 45,
Damon, W. The positive justice concept from 291-298.
childhood to adolescence: a developmental Turiel, E. Conventional and moral reasoning.
analysis. Unpublished master's thesis, Uni- Paper presented at the meeting of the West-
versity of California, Berkeley, 1971. ern Psychological Association, San Francisco,
Damon, W. The development of the child's con- April 1974.
ception of justice. Paper presented at the Ugurel-Semin, R. Moral behavior and moral judg-
meeting of the Society for Research in Child ment of children. Journal of Abnormal and
Development, Philadelphia, March 1973. Social Psychology, 1952, 47, 463-474.

This content downloaded from 130.15.241.167 on Sun, 29 Sep 2013 10:21:27 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like