You are on page 1of 12

666955

research-article2016
JADXXX10.1177/1087054716666955Journal of Attention DisordersHahn-Markowitz et al.

Article
Journal of Attention Disorders

Efficacy of Cognitive-Functional (Cog-Fun)


2020, Vol. 24(5) 655­–666
© The Author(s) 2016
Article reuse guidelines:
Occupational Therapy Intervention Among sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/1087054716666955
https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054716666955

Children With ADHD: An RCT journals.sagepub.com/home/jad

Jeri Hahn-Markowitz1, Itai Berger1,2, Iris Manor3,4, and Adina Maeir1

Abstract
Objective: To examine the efficacy of a Cognitive-Functional (Cog-Fun) intervention for children with ADHD. Method:
Random allocation of 107 children to study or control groups preceded 10 parent–child weekly Cog-Fun sessions
emphasizing executive strategy training in games and daily activities. Controls received treatment after crossover. Study
participants were followed up 3 months post-treatment. Outcomes included parent/teacher ratings of executive functions,
ADHD symptoms, and parent ratings of quality of life. Results: Eight children withdrew prior to treatment. All children
in both groups who began treatment completed it. Mixed effects ANOVA revealed significant Time × Group interaction
effects on all parent-reported outcomes. Treatment effects were moderate to large, replicated after crossover in the
control group and not moderated by medication. Parent-reported treatment gains in the study group were maintained
at follow-up. No significant Time × Group interaction effects were found on teacher outcomes. Conclusion: Cog-Fun
occupational therapy (OT) intervention shows positive context-specific effects on parent, but not teacher, ratings. (J. of
Att. Dis. 2020; 24(5) 655-666)

Keywords
child ADHD, executive functions (EF), quality of life (QoL), intervention

ADHD has been shown to profoundly disrupt functioning shown to be effective in providing temporary relief from
and creates significant barriers to successful participation in symptoms of ADHD; however, there is little evidence that
a host of daily activities, including play, learning, self-care, medication comprehensively addresses the EF and func-
and social interactions (Cordier, Bundy, Hocking, & tional challenges of these children (Biederman et al., 2011;
Einfeld, 2010; Dunn, Coster, Cohn, & Orsmond, 2009; Gol Smith et al., 2016). Children with ADHD may also be
& Jarus, 2005). The persistence of the disorder implies treated by psychosocial interventions, for example, behav-
long-term ramifications on interpersonal relationships, self- ioral programs, psychoeducation for parents, neurofeed-
efficacy, activities of daily living, and productivity, signifi- back, and expressive therapies (Pelham & Fabiano, 2008).
cantly compromising quality of life (QoL; Maeir, The focus of this study is on an intervention that addresses
Hahn-Markowitz, Fisher, & Traub Bar-Ilan, 2014; Stern, the EF cognitive deficit in ADHD; therefore, we limit our
Pollak, Bonne, Malik, & Maeir, 2017). The involvement of review to cognitive rehabilitation (CR) approaches.
impaired executive functions (EF) is recognized as one of CR approaches to treating children with ADHD include
the central neurocognitive impairments of ADHD (Barkley, remedial and compensatory approaches. The remedial
2012). Strong evidence supports the high prevalence of approach purports to improve specific EF by repeated train-
executive dysfunction in ADHD, which negatively affects ing, while the compensatory approach aims to provide tools
wide-ranging functional outcomes (Barkley, 2012; Stern to cope with impaired EF. These two approaches differ in
et al., 2017). Deficient EF are therefore major intervention
targets for individuals with ADHD. 1
To enable the child with ADHD to participate in daily Hadassah and the Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel
2
Neuro-Cognitive Center, Jerusalem, Israel
activities, treatment must address factors that hinder per- 3
Geha Medical Center, Petah Tikva, Israel
formance. A variety of pharmacological, psychosocial, 4
Tel Aviv University, Israel
educational, and computerized treatments to decrease
Corresponding Author:
symptoms of ADHD and to improve behavior and aca- Jeri Hahn-Markowitz, School of Occupational Therapy, Hadassah and the
demic performance exist (Pelham & Fabiano, 2008; Riccio Hebrew University, P.O. Box 24026, Mt. Scopus, Jerusalem 91240, Israel.
& Gomes, 2013). Pharmacological treatment has been Email: jerihahnmarkowitz@gmail.com
656 Journal of Attention Disorders 24(5)

the degree of contextual factors involved in treatment, In another study, Tamm, Nakonezny, and Hughes (2014)
namely, parents and/or teachers, as well as engagement in taught parents to administer a metacognitive EF training
daily activities. The compensatory approach usually pro- intervention to their preschool children with ADHD. The
vides a “real world,” ecological context in the intervention, objectives of the study were to establish feasibility of the
whereas the remedial approach assumes that treatment intervention and to determine whether it affected EF and
gains will transfer spontaneously to real-world tasks and ADHD symptoms. Children participated in small group
occupations. The authors of a recent meta-analysis on com- activities requiring aspects of EF while their parents
puterized cognitive training for children with ADHD received explanations and modeling of desired actions. The
(Cortese et al., 2015) found that there were no effects of program was found to be feasible and improvements on
working memory training when implemented on its own, on child performance on neuropsychological tasks corre-
ADHD symptoms. However, interventions targeting more sponded to parent ratings of working memory and atten-
than one neuropsychological domain had large effects on tional control on the Metacognitive Index and Global
ADHD symptoms in parent-rated outcomes. Six of the 15 Executive Composite (GEC) of the BRIEF and on ADHD
studies included parent ratings of EF according to the inattention symptoms. No significant improvements were
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF; found on teacher ratings. This study used an integrative
Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000), with results dem- approach, with parents employing strategies and specific
onstrating small-to-moderate effect sizes. These authors praise. Tamm and Nakonezny (2015) implemented their
suggest that multi-component training models may be more intervention on a different sample of the same age, finding
successful than computerized training for treating ADHD, significant improvement on BRIEF scales and on the GEC.
given the individualized neuropsychological profile of each The above studies add much merit to the development of
individual, the fact that children may be affected by more interventions to improve EF among children with ADHD.
than one deficit, and the complex, heterogeneous nature of The findings support the premise that parents can play an
the condition. active role in treatment for affecting child EF.
A recent randomized control trial (Smith et al., 2016) CR models in occupational therapy (OT) address the
evaluated the efficacy of an intervention combining com- impact of neurologically based cognitive deficits on daily
puterized cognitive remediation training, physical exer- functioning and QoL among individuals with a variety of
cises, and a behavior management strategy. The effects of acquired or developmental disorders. The performance of
the intervention on EF and components of attention mea- daily tasks that are meaningful to an individual is the
sured in ratings of ADHD symptoms and neurocognitive essence of OT practice for people with cognitive deficits.
tests were not statistically significant. The authors of this Toglia’s Dynamic Interaction Approach for CR in OT high-
study suggest that the treatment is insufficient in producing lights the dynamic interaction between the person, the task,
the desired effects without real-world application and rec- and the environment, when addressing problems in perfor-
ommend designing cognitive training to the individual mance (Toglia, 2011). The Cognitive Orientation to Daily
neurocognitive profiles of children and then tailoring it to Occupational Performance (CO-OP; Polatajko, Mandich, &
meet their needs in real-world settings or situations (Smith McEwen, 2011) is an important treatment approach that
et al., 2016). includes a major focus on metacognitive strategies within
A review of interventions for executive dysfunction in an occupational context. Moderate-level evidence in con-
children and adolescents recommended involving parents trolled studies and case series has shown that these models
in intervention, as the psychoeducation of parents is a criti- have been effective for populations with EF impairment
cal element in their children’s treatment (Riccio & Gomes, (Dawson et al., 2009; Goverover, Johnston, Toglia, &
2013). Halperin et al. (2013) conducted a study that aimed Deluce, 2007; Toglia, Johnston, Goverover, & Dain, 2010).
to examine whether cognitive enhancement administered However, they have not been applied to children with
to preschoolers through play would impact the severity of ADHD. These models provided the basis for the develop-
their ADHD symptoms. Children attended sessions target- ment of the Cognitive-Functional (Cog-Fun) OT integrative
ing EF, motor control, attention/tracking and visuospatial intervention for children with ADHD. Cog-Fun targets the
abilities. Parents attended separate psychoeducation and cognitive, emotional, and environmental barriers to partici-
group support sessions. Their role at home was to progres- pation as they interact in an occupational context. Cog-Fun
sively increase the cognitive and behavioral demands of employs learning and practicing executive strategies and
games as children mastered necessary skills. Results behavioral skills for the child and parent together, within
pointed to significant reduction of ADHD symptom sever- the context of playful activities and daily functioning.
ity after treatment, with 3-month maintenance, according A pilot study examining the feasibility of Cog-Fun on 14
to parent and teacher ratings. Although this intervention’s children diagnosed with ADHD found significant improve-
efficacy remains to be shown, the program was found to be ments with medium to large effects on measures of EF and
feasible and well-accepted by participating parents. occupational performance after intervention, with most
Hahn-Markowitz et al. 657

Table 1.  Description of Study and Control Groups on Mental Disorders (4th ed., text rev.; DSM-IV-TR; American
Demographics at Baseline. Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000) criteria, as assessed by
Study group Control group a certified pediatric neurologist/psychiatrist, including a
(n = 54) (n = 53) semi-structured interview with the child and parents, medi-
n (%) n (%) p cal/neurological/psychiatric examination, and completion
of a ADHD diagnostic questionnaire (DuPaul, Power,
Number male 37 (69) 39 (74) .952
Anastopoulos, & Reid, 1998). The primary inclusion crite-
Pharmacological 36 (67) 34 (64) .894
treatment for ADHD
ria for referral to the study included (a) a score above the
ADHD subtypes clinical cutoff point for ADHD symptoms on the ADHD
 Predominantly 23 (43) 29 (55) .444 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
inattentive (4th ed.; DSM-IV; APA, 1994) Scales (DuPaul et al., 1998;
 Combined 29 (54) 21 (40) Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder,
 Hyperactive- 2 (4) 3 (6) Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and
impulsive Management, 2011) and (b) approval by the pediatric neu-
Marital status of parents rologist/psychiatrist that the child fulfills the criteria as
 Single 2 (4) 1 (2) .766 required by the APA guidelines for ADHD diagnosis.
 Married 50 (93) 49 (92) Additional inclusion criteria included (a) age 7 to 10, in
 Divorced/separated 2 (4) 3 (6) second to fourth grade at first assessment; (b) enrollment in
regular school; (c) stability in medication/no medication
  M ± SD M ± SD  
status and other treatments for ADHD for the previous 2
Child age in years 8.4 ± 0.9 8.6 ± 0.8 .395 months; and (d) executive dysfunction in daily life as
Mother’s education in 16.6 ± 3.8 16.4 ± 2.9 .853 reflected by the score (T-score ≥ 65) of at least one scale on
years the parent version of the BRIEF questionnaire. Exclusion
criteria for all participants were based upon clinical diagno-
sis rendered by treating/referring physician (neurologist/
effects maintained at follow-up (Hahn-Markowitz, Manor, psychiatrist): intellectual disability, other chronic medical/
& Maeir, 2011). In another controlled study of 19 children neurological conditions, and primary psychiatric diagnosis
with a crossover design, significant differences between (e.g., depression, anxiety, psychosis). There was no signifi-
groups in change scores on outcome measures were found cant difference in demographic variables across groups at
before crossover. No significant differences were found in baseline (p > .05; Table 1).
treatment effects after crossover, and significant moderate All participating children gave verbal assent and their
to large treatment effects were found for measures of EF parents provided written informed consent to the study,
and occupational performance (Maeir, Fisher et al., 2014). approved by the Helsinki committees (institutional review
The purpose of this study was to further examine the effi- board [IRB]) of Hadassah-Hebrew University Medical
cacy of Cog-Fun OT intervention for children with ADHD Center, Jerusalem, Israel and Geha Medical Center, Petah
and its effect on EF, ADHD symptoms, and QoL in a ran- Tikva, Israel. The study was registered in a clinical trials
domized controlled crossover study. registry before recruitment (No. NCT01792921).

Method Measures
BRIEF.  The BRIEF (Gioia et al., 2000) is an 86-item eco-
Setting and Participants
logical rating scale completed by a parent or teacher
This study was conducted at two ADHD units at major med- designed to reflect the neuropsychological constructs of EF
ical centers in two cities in Israel. After receiving ethical in everyday situations among children aged 5 to 18 years. It
approval from both institutions, the medical directors of these consists of eight subscales (Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Con-
clinics referred children with ADHD to the study. All partici- trol, Initiation, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Organi-
pants were recruited among children referred from tertiary zation of Materials, and Monitor), two indices (behavioral
care university hospitals to the clinics by pediatricians, gen- regulation index and metacognitive index), and a GEC.
eral practitioners, teachers, psychologists, or parents and were Items are rated 1 (never), 2 (sometimes), or 3 (often). Raw
referred over the course of 18 months. The diagnosis of scores are converted to T-scores, with 65 or above consid-
ADHD was based on the required procedure (Subcommittee ered clinically impaired (SD 10). Internal consistency, test–
on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Steering retest reliability (r = .72-.84 for Parent version over 3
Committee on Quality Improvement and Management, weeks), and discriminant validity have been established for
2011), according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of children with ADHD, as well as convergent and concurrent
658 Journal of Attention Disorders 24(5)

validity (Gioia et al., 2000; McCandless & O’Laughlin, clinically important difference of 7 T points on the post-
2007). The GEC is the sum of the eight subscales. The GEC intervention score between the intervention and control
scores of parents and teachers are reported here. waitlist groups. A sample size of 34 participants per group
was needed. Taking into account an expected 30% dropout
Conners’ Rating Scales–Revised.  The Conners’ Parent Rating rate, 44 participants were needed in each group to detect
Scales–Revised (CPRS-R)/Conners’ Teacher Rating clinically significant effects.
Scales–Revised (CTRS-R; Conner, 2000) are parent and
teacher rating scales of ADHD-related behaviors. The Procedure and randomization. Attending physicians briefly
scales include seven subscales, four indices, and three described the study to parents of children they deemed good
DSM-IV subscales. The parent version has 80 items and the candidates, and provided the study coordinator (first author)
teacher version has 59 items, rated from 0 (not at all) to 3 with a list of parents who agreed to be contacted. Mothers
(very much). Raw scores are converted to T-scores, with 63 of children referred received a telephone explanation by the
or above considered clinically impaired. The scales are study coordinator regarding the conditions of the study and
valid and reliable, with test–retest reliability over 6 to 8 to determine whether they were interested in participation.
weeks ranging from .47 to .92 (teacher version) and .47 to If oral consent was given, they were asked to complete the
.85 (parent version; Angello et al., 2003). The Conners’ BRIEF questionnaire and invited to an interview, where
Global Total Index (CGTI) scores of parents and teachers they received a complete description of the study and pro-
are reported here. This index was chosen as it consists of vided written consent. If the scores on the questionnaire
items that are representative of ADHD symptoms and gen- were above cutoff, the family was invited to participate in
eral psychopathology, is considered to be a good indicator the study. Children were assigned randomly to study or con-
of global concerns about a child’s functioning, and is usu- trol waitlist group according to blocks of age-group and
ally sensitive to treatment effects (Sparrow, 2010). gender by the study coordinator, who had prepared the ran-
dom assignment schedule using computer-generated ran-
The Pediatric Quality of Life 4.0 Generic Core Scales (Ped- dom numbers. Parents were asked to give an envelope with
sQL).  The PedsQL is a 23-item measure of pediatric health, an explanatory letter about the study to the children’s teach-
and assesses perceptions of the child’s health-related qual- ers, together with the teacher version of the BRIEF and the
ity of life (HRQOL; Varni, Seid, & Kurtin, 2001). It is com- CTRS-R.
prised of four generic core scales (Physical, Emotional, Parents and teachers were blinded to group allocation
Social, and School Functioning), with parallel child self- while completing baseline forms, however, after random-
report and parent proxy-report formats. Internal consistency ization, each family was notified its allocation. Parents and
reliability for the Psychosocial Health Summary score (α = teachers completed questionnaires independently at all
.86 parent version) was acceptable for group comparisons. assessments. Participants in the study group commenced
Validity was demonstrated using the known-groups method, Cog-Fun treatment after baseline, while those in the control
correlations with indicators of morbidity and illness burden, group received no treatment. Three months later, partici-
and factor analysis. The PedsQL distinguishes between pants in both groups underwent Assessment 2, after which
healthy children and pediatric patients with acute or chronic crossover occurred. Assessment 3 (post-intervention for
health conditions. There are five items for each scale, controls and follow-up for 43 study participants) took place
excepting the Physical Function scale, which has eight at the end of an additional 3 months. All participants who
items. Item scores range from 0 (never a problem) to 4 began Cog-Fun treatment completed it and underwent post-
(always a problem). Items are reverse-scored and linearly intervention assessment. Figure 1 delineates recruitment,
transformed to a 0 to 100 scale so that higher scores indicate allocation, and participation in the study.
better HRQOL. The Physical Health Summary score is the
same as the Physical Function Subscale. The Psychosocial
Health Summary score is the sum of the 15 items (three
Intervention
other scales) divided by the number of items answered. The Cog-Fun is a manualized treatment approach (Maeir,
Studies have shown the reliability and validity of the Ped- Hahn-Markowitz, Fisher, & Traub Bar-Ilan, 2012) admin-
sQL as a measure of general physical and psychosocial istered by licensed OTs with Cog-Fun certification.
health in children (McCarthy et al., 2005). The parent Psy- Certification is awarded to OTs who successfully complete a
chosocial Health Summary scores are reported here. 6-month 60-hr theoretical and practical training course. Part
of the training includes 4 months of fieldwork, supervision,
Sample size.  An a priori sample size test of power was per- and the presentation of a case study. In the present study,
formed to identify the probability of detecting clinical each of the 12 OTs (three in one clinic and nine in the other)
effects in the primary outcome measure, the BRIEF, with who treated the child–parent dyads completed a Treatment
an alpha value of .05 and power of .80, using a minimal Log for each treatment session, which was examined by the
Hahn-Markowitz et al. 659

Referred to study
(n=124) Excluded (n=17)
Did not meet inclusion
criteria (n=3)
Declined (n= 14)
Randomized and
assessed (n=107)

Allocated to intervention (n=54) Allocated to wait list control (n=53)


Received intervention and Assessment Completed wait period and Assessment
Intervention-Wait Period

2 (Post-intervention) (n=50) 2 (Pre-intervention) (n=51)


Did not received allocated intervention Did not complete wait period (n=2)
(n=4) Unwilling to wait (n=1)
Withdrew from study (n=2) Found alternative intervention (n=1)
Lack of child compliance (n=1)
Suspected severe psychiatric
comorbidity (n=1)

Crossover: Wait period (n=50) Crossover: Intervention (n=49)


Completed wait period and Assessment Received intervention and Assessment
3 (Follow-up) (n=43) 3 (Post-intervention) (n=49)
Crossover

Lost to follow-up (n=7) Did not receive intervention (n=2)


Began medication (n=4) Withdrew from study (n=1)
Refused follow-up (n=3) Suspected severe psychiatric
comorbidity (n=1)

Analyzed after intervention (n=50) Analyzed after wait period (n=51)


Analysis

Analyzed at follow up (n=43) Analyzed after intervention (n=49)


Excluded from analysis (n=0) Excluded from analysis (n=0)

Figure 1.  Participant flow diagram for cognitive-functional.

study coordinator to verify treatment fidelity. The Log simulated in a social context identified as challenging to the
included a checklist to ensure that treatment was adminis- child.
tered according to Cog-Fun principles, as well as space allot- Parents learn to provide necessary supports (i.e., positive
ted for details such as description of activities, executive verbal mediation, daily planner, timer, checklists) and mod-
strategies learned, goals defined, and significant interper- ifications (i.e., preparing materials in advance, reducing
sonal interactions. Each therapist was initially blind to group clutter and distractions) for their child in the home environ-
allocation. ment to promote transfer of strategies and successful occu-
Cog-Fun is an integrative intervention in which the use pational performance at home. These are based on the
of effortful executive strategies is supplemented by envi- neurofunctional training (NFT) approach to CR (Guiles,
ronmental adaptations to facilitate success, which serves as 2011), which uses environmental resources to minimize
positive reinforcement, motivating the child to continue try- frustration and EF demands and enable successful perfor-
ing to attain personal goals by using strategies. Basic strate- mance during the learning process. The supports and adap-
gies (inhibition, effort, monitoring, and planning) are tations are designed to facilitate function and participation
learned in a playful setting, practiced according to individu- by modifying factors in the client’s surroundings and to
alized functional goals that are meaningful to the child and supplement executive strategy acquisition, the former often
parents. The strategies were designed to compensate for the aiding to boost motivation, the latter being a more effortful
neurocognitive barriers to participation rather than to reme- process. In Cog-Fun, these methods are exemplified by
diate them in a cognitive training model. For example, the structured treatment sessions, by adapting activities to the
“inhibit” strategy is acquired in games such as Simon Says, child’s abilities and by using cues. The therapist models
660 Journal of Attention Disorders 24(5)

Session Unit Content


1 1 Evaluation: Parent Cognitive-Functional interview on EF in daily life and parental concerns and goals
2 1 Evaluation: Introduction to Cog-Fun setting (session routine and treatment tools- games and activities) and establishing a
therapeutic alliance with the child
3-4 2 Strategy acquisition and practice: ‘Inhibit’ strategy in multiple activities (games requiring use of ‘inhibit’, e.g., Red Light
Green Light) and transfer to functional activities requiring ‘inhibit’ (e.g., waiting one’s turn)
5-6 3 Strategy acquisition and practice: ‘Effort recruitment’ strategy in multiple activities (games requiring use of ‘effort re-
cruitment’, Tug of War) and transfer to functional activities requiring ‘effort recruitment’ (e.g., remain engaged in game
with friend)
7 4 Home visit: Transfer of learning: practicing strategies in home-based activities
8 5 Strategy acquisition and practice: ‘Monitoring’ strategy in multiple activities (games requiring use of ‘monitor’, e.g.,
Rush Hour) and transfer to functional activities requiring ‘monitoring’ (e.g., preparing schoolbag according to list)
9 - 10 6 Strategy acquisition and practice: ‘Planning’ strategy in multiple activities (games requiring use of ‘plan’, e.g., Rush
Hour) and transfer to functional activities requiring ‘planning’ (e.g., planning birthday party)
11 7 Final session with parent-child dyad: summarizing strategy learning
12 7 Summary session with parents: consolidating parents’ tools for on-going implementation

Figure 2.  Cog-Fun OT intervention content.


Note. The progression between “inhibit,” “effort,” “monitoring,” and “planning” strategies is tailored to child’s progress. Not all strategies need to be
achieved. Cog-Fun = cognitive-functional; OT = occupational therapy; EF = executive functions.

these techniques for the parents, who are encouraged to use Hypothesis 3: There would be maintenance of the treat-
them to help their child at home and in the community. The ment effects at follow-up for the study group, with no
structure provides the necessary clarity and supports to significant changes.
enable effective learning for families with ADHD, while
flexibility provides for the unique individual executive and
Statistical Analyses
functional profile of each client. Treatment is administered
in weekly 1-hr sessions with child and parent over 12 All statistical analyses were conducted by using SPSS for
weeks. If a session is missed, it is made up so that the full Windows 19 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois). Descriptive sta-
program is received by all participants. tistics were generated for demographic and clinical vari-
In light of the fact that many parents of children with ables and are reported as means and SD values, as well as
ADHD report experiencing a sense of being burdened with those of the BRIEF GEC score, CGIT score, and the PedsQL
managing their child’s ADHD, there are no required “home- Psychosocial Health Summary score. Because the primary
work” assignments, nor are parents rated on what they do at research goal was the study of a new treatment, we chose to
home with their child. Rather, the therapist recommends use a non-intent-to-treat (non-ITT) approach to measure the
that parents encourage strategy implementation in daily effect of the treatment (Ten Have et al., 2008). To compare
activities at home, including during play with their child. between the study and control groups on various personal
Likewise, they are encouraged to praise their child’s attempt characteristics, t tests were performed (Table 1). Mixed
at using the strategies in the appropriate contexts. The pro- effects ANOVA with the within subject main effect of time
tocol comprises seven intervention units: an evaluation and the between subject main effect of group was performed
unit, four units of executive strategy acquisition, a home to examine the effect of treatment compared with waitlist
visit, and a summary unit (Figure 2). control on outcome measures. Significance was set at a
level of .05. Estimates of effect sizes and 95% confidence
intervals (CIs) were calculated. The effect sizes are repre-
Hypotheses sented by partial eta squared, while η2 < .04 is considered a
It was hypothesized that minimum effect size representing a clinically significant
effect, .04 < partial η2 < .25 is considered a moderate effect,
Hypothesis 1: There would be significant Time × Group and partial η2 > .25 is considered a strong effect size
(study and waitlist control) interaction effects on all out- (Ferguson, 2009).
comes measuring EF, ADHD symptomatology, and QoL
from baseline to Assessment 2.
Results
Hypothesis 2: There would be significant time effects
on all measures after crossover (replication of the treat- The data were collected from April 2013 through June
ment effect), with no Time × Group interaction. 2015. One hundred seven children were randomized and all
Hahn-Markowitz et al. 661

Table 2.  T-Scores at Baseline and at Assessment 2 on Outcome Measures (Group × Time Interaction Effect).

Baseline, M Assessment 2, Partial eta squared


± SD M ± SD M [95% CI] p time p interaction interaction
Parent measures
  Executive functions: .38 .00 .08
BRIEF GEC
   Study group (n = 50) 67.8 ± 9.3 61.9 ± 8.8 −5.9 [−8.5, −3.3]  
   Control group (n = 51) 66.5 ± 9.5 65.7 ± 7.3 −0.8 [−2.8, 1.7]  
  ADHD symptomatology: .02 .00 .14
CPRS-R GIT
   Study group (n = 45) 67.1 ± 10.5 61.6 ± 9.9 −5.5 [−8.3, −2.7]  
   Control group (n = 47) 67.2 ± 10.8 68.6 ± 11.1 1.4 [0.6, 3.4]  
  Quality of life: .01 .00 .08
PedsQL psychosocial health
summary score
   Study group (n = 45) 59.6 ± 11.8 68.1 ± 12.1 8.5 [3.7, 13.1]  
   Control group (n = 47) 60.1 ± 12.2 61.2 ± 12.6 1.1 [−3.9, 1.8]  
Teacher measures
  Executive functions: .63 .73 .03
BRIEF GEC
   Study group (n = 19) 58.9 ± 8.8 60.6 ±9.7 1.7 [−1.7, 5.1]  
   Control group (n = 17) 58.1 ± 12.7 57.8 ± 13.0 −0.3 [−4.9, 5.5]  
  ADHD symptomatology: .75 .65 .47
CTRS-R GIT
   Study group (n = 18) 55.9 ± 10.8 57.1 ± 7.2 1.2 [−2.8, 5.3]  
   Control group (n = 14) 56.0 ± 10.6 55.8 ± 12.4 −0.2 [−5.5, 5.1]  

Note. Lower scores signify improvement on BRIEF (65 ≥ indicates executive dysfunction) and CPRS-R (63 ≥ indicates dysfunction). Higher scores signify
improvement on PedsQL. CI = confidence interval; BRIEF = Behavioral Rating Inventory of executive functions; GEC = global executive composite;
CPRS-R = Conners’ Parent Rating Scales–Revised; GIT = global index total; PedsQL = Pediatric Quality of Life 4.0 Generic Core Scales; CTRS-R =
Conners’ Teacher Rating Scales–Revised.

completed the baseline assessment. Compliance was high, ADHD (art therapy, equestrian therapy, remedial educa-
with 99 children receiving intervention and eight leaving tion, psychotherapy, homeopathy, animal therapy, drama
the study prior to intervention. Five participants left the therapy).
study and one was excluded after baseline, one more left the
study and another was excluded after Assessment 2. All
Outcomes
children who commenced treatment completed it; however,
seven children were not followed up due to change in medi- Time × Group interaction effects on measures from baseline to
cation status (four) or lack of availability (three). There Assessment 2. There were significant interaction effects
were no significant differences between children who between the study and control groups from baseline to
received treatment and those that left the study before treat- Assessment 2 on all parent- reported measures. Mean scores
ment, nor between those followed up and those who were for both groups at baseline were worse (higher) than the
not, on all demographic and ADHD variables (age, gender, cutoff scores for significant impairment on the BRIEF (T ≥
mother’s education, ADHD subtype, medication status, 65) and the CPRS-R (T ≥ 63), and PedsQL Psychosocial
study location). Health Summary was one standard deviation below reported
All participating mothers had at least high school norms (Varni et al., 2001). At Assessment 2, mean scores
degrees, with the majority having higher education. The for the study group improved by 5.9 and 5.5 points, respec-
majority of parents (>94%) were married. The mean age tively (equivalent to >.5 SD) on the BRIEF and CPRS-R
of participating children was 8.5 years, including 72 chil- and by 8.5 points on the PedsQL (equivalent to >.5 SD),
dren aged 7 to 8 years and 35 children aged 9 to 10 years. whereas mean scores for the control group remained essen-
The majority of children (67%) received medication for tially unchanged (Table 2).
ADHD during their participation in the study. Eleven chil- The interaction effects between the study and control
dren in the study group and 10 children in the control groups from baseline to Assessment 2 on both teacher mea-
group were receiving ongoing additional treatments for sures were not significant. The low teacher response rate
662 Journal of Attention Disorders 24(5)

Table 3.  Outcome Comparisons After Crossover (Intervention Effect).

Pre-treatment, Post-treatment, Partial eta squared


M ± SD M ± SD M [95% CI] of time effect p time p interaction
Parent measures
  Executive functions:  
BRIEF GEC
   Study group (n = 50) 67.8 ± 9.3 61.9 ± 8.8 −5.9 [−8.5, −3.3] .39 .00 .38
   Control group (n = 49) 65.4 ± 9.5 58.0 ± 8.6 −7.4 [−9.4, −5.3]  
ADHD symptomatology:
CPRS-R GIT
   Study group (n = 45) 67.1 ± 10.5 61.6 ± 9.9 −5.5 [−8.3, −2.7] .27 .00 .61
   Control group (n = 46) 68.0 ± 10.9 61.4 ± 11.4 −6.6 [−9.5, −3.6]  
  Quality of life:
PedsQL psychosocial health summary score
   Study group (n = 45) 59.6 ± 11.8 68.1 ± 12.1 8.5 [3.7, 13.1] .17 .00 .14
   Control group (n = 46) 62.0 ± 12.1 66.2 ± 12.5 4.2 [0.7, 7.6]  
Teacher measures
  Executive functions: .002 .803 .245
BRIEF GEC
   Study group (n = 19) 58.9 ± 8.8 60.6 ± 9.7 1.7 [−1.7, 5.1]  
   Control group (n = 13) 57.7 ± 10.0 56.5 ± 12.5 −1.2 [−5.4, 2.9]  
  ADHD symptomatology .02 .962 .393
   Study group (n = 18) 55.9 ± 10.8 57.1 ± 7.3 1.2 [−2.8, 5.3]  
   Control group (n = 11) 57.5 ± 13.1 56.2 ± 11.4 −1.3 [−3.2, 6.0]  

Note. Lower scores signify improvement on BRIEF (65 ≥ indicates executive dysfunction) and CPRS-R (63 ≥ indicates dysfunction). Higher scores signify
improvement on PedsQL. CI = confidence interval; BRIEF = Behavioral Rating Inventory of executive functions; GEC = global executive composite;
CPRS-R = Conners’ Parent Rating Scales–Revised; GIT = global index total; PedsQL = Pediatric Quality of Life 4.0 Generic Core Scales; CTRS-R =
Conners’ Teacher Rating Scales–Revised.

(36/99 BRIEF and 32/99 CTRS-R questionnaires were medicated 66.9-59.9, not medicated 65.8-60.2, p = .381;
returned) must be noted. Mean scores for both groups at CGIT medicated 68.9-61.5, not medicated 65.0-61.2; p =
baseline were better than the cutoff scores for significant .123; PedsQL medicated 63.8-68.2, not medicated 58.2-
impairment on the BRIEF and on the CTRS-R. At 65.1, p = .314. Regarding teacher measures, a significant
Assessment 2, mean scores for the study group slightly Time × Medication status interaction was found on the
worsened and for the control group slightly improved. CGIT, F(1, 27) = 5.236 p = .03, whereby the scores of non-
medicated children (n = 9) improved after intervention
Time effect for both groups on outcomes after crossover. Rep- (63.7-59.4), whereas the scores of medicated children (n =
lication of the treatment effects was found on all parent 20) slightly worsened (53.3-55.6). A similar but not signifi-
measures after crossover, and interactions between groups cant trend was found for the teacher BRIEF GEC (medi-
were not significant (all p > .05; Table 3). Within subject cated 54.9-57.5, not medicated 62.0-60.2), F(1, 30) = 3.010,
comparisons between times revealed significant differ- p = .093.
ences before and after intervention on all measures.
Effect sizes were large for the BRIEF GEC and the CGIT Three-month follow-up for study group.  The treatment effects
and moderate for the PedsQL psychosocial health sum- on the study group were maintained at the 3-month follow-
mary score. The treatment effects on teacher measures up assessment on all measures (Table 4). On the BRIEF
were not significant in both groups, with very small GEC, there was an additional significant improvement with
effect sizes, and interactions between groups were not a moderate effect size (ES = .11). Post-treatment results
significant. The low teacher response rate (32/99 BRIEF were maintained on the CGIT and the PedsQL psychosocial
and 29/99 CTRS-R questionnaires were returned) must healthy summary score, with no significant change. Teacher
be noted. reports were not analyzed at follow-up due to the small
number of questionnaires returned (nine BRIEF, 16
Medication status and treatment effects.  No interaction was CTRS-R).
found between medication status and treatment effects (pre- No adverse events or side effects occurred among par-
post-intervention) on parent measures. Parent BRIEF GEC ticipants in either group.
Hahn-Markowitz et al. 663

Table 4.  Parent Outcome Comparisons for Study Group at Post-Intervention and Follow-Up.

Post-intervention, Follow-up, Partial eta


M ± SD M ± SD M [95% CI] squared p time
Executive functions: 60.8 ± 7.3 58.5 ± 7.8 −2.3 [−4.3, 0.3] .11 .02
BRIEF GEC (n = 43)
ADHD symptomatology: 60.7 ± 9.7 59.9 ± 14.1 −0.8 [−4.6, 2.9] .00 .63
CPRS-R GIT (n = 40)
Quality of life: 69.1 ± 12.3* 67.5 ± 14.1* −1.6 [−2.4, 5.7] .02 .41
PedsQL psychosocial health
summary score (n = 38)

Note. Lower scores signify improvement on BRIEF (65 ≥ indicates executive dysfunction) and CPRS-R (63 ≥ indicates dysfunction). Higher scores signify
improvement on PedsQL. CI = confidence interval; BRIEF = Behavioral Rating Inventory of executive functions; GEC = global executive composite;
CPRS-R = Conners’ Parent Rating Scales–Revised; GIT = global index total; PedsQL = Pediatric Quality of Life 4.0 Generic Core Scales.

Discussion element. To this end, the studies of Halperin et al. (2013),


Tamm et al. (2014), and Tamm and Nakonezny (2015) add
The findings suggest that Cog-Fun in OT shows positive
much merit to intervention development for children with
effects for improving EF, QoL, and decreasing symptoms
ADHD. Results of these studies are in line with those of the
among children with ADHD as perceived by their parents.
present study, supporting the role of parents for effective
Significant Time × Group interactions were found on all
impacting of positive outcomes.
parent outcomes, demonstrating that these effects cannot be
However, according to teachers’ reports, no significant
attributed to time, as the controls did not improve and, in
treatment effects were found in the classroom except for
some cases, worsened at Assessment 2. The positive treat-
non-medicated children, for whom teachers reported moder-
ment effects in the study group were replicated among the
ate improvement in ADHD symptoms after treatment. These
controls after crossover. Therefore, the effects cannot be
findings may indicate minimal transfer of treatment from the
attributed to one sample, to a random occurrence, to treat-
clinic to the school environment, indicating the context-spe-
ment site, or to a particular therapist, as findings were con-
cific nature of Cog-Fun. These results are similar to those of
sistent across both sites and for all therapists. Treatment
Malik, Rooney, Chronis-Tuscano, and Tariq (2017) from a
gains were maintained for the study group 3 months after
study examining the preliminary efficacy of a behavioral
completion of treatment. These results substantiate the find-
parent training program for children with ADHD in which
ings of previous studies (Hahn-Markowitz et al., 2011;
teacher ratings of symptoms and impairments showed no
Maeir, Fisher et al., 2014) and establish the impact of Cog-
improvement after treatment, while parent ratings did. Those
Fun on parent-reported outcomes.
authors noted that such findings are consistent with other
In Cog-Fun, parents are central “change agents,” helping intervention studies targeting the home environment and not
to transfer treatment principles so that the child is better the classroom in which ADHD behavioral treatment effects
able to monitor his or her behavior and performance outside do not usually generalize beyond the treatment setting
the clinic. Executive strategies, especially those designed to (Malik et al., 2017). In a different vein, it is important to note
address behavioral and emotional regulation, are taught and that prior to intervention teachers reported less EF deficits
practiced in treatment. The BRIEF GEC scores after treat- and less severe ADHD symptoms than parents reported, to
ment reflect the parents’ perceptions of their children’s the point where mean teacher scores were below (not
improved ability to self-regulate in the context of daily impaired) cutoff for both measures. This finding is in line
challenges in home and community settings. The higher with other studies (Goulardins et al., 2015; McCandless &
psychosocial health summary scores on the PedsQL provide O’Laughlin, 2007) in which overall agreement between par-
evidence that the treatment emphasis on changing psycho- ents and teachers on measures of problems related to ADHD
social components affected the children’s QoL positively. is low. McCandless and O’Laughlin’s findings suggest that
The improved scores on CGIT, which consists of items discrepancies in parent and teacher reports may be due to
reflecting symptoms and general psychopathology (i.e., their observing different types of behaviors in the home and
restlessness, impulsiveness, inattention, emotional out- school settings. Goulardins et al. (2015) suggested that the
bursts), lend further evidence to the efficacy of Cog-Fun. structured framework of the classroom may account for chil-
Riccio and Gomes (2013) conducted a review of inter- dren being more regulated there than they are at home.
ventions for executive dysfunction in children and ado- Parent–teacher discrepancies may also be due to the fact that
lescents and recommended involving parents in the some of the children were taking medication for ADHD dur-
intervention, as psychoeducation of parents is a critical ing school hours (Jarratt, Riccio, & Siekierski, 2005). Our
664 Journal of Attention Disorders 24(5)

data support this as medicated children were rated signifi- Funding


cantly better than non-medicated children at baseline by The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
teachers (53.3 vs. 63.7), as opposed to parent ratings, where for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This
they rated medicated children as worse than non-medicated study was supported by the Milton Rosenbaum Endowment Fund
children (68.9 vs. 65.0). Interestingly, teachers reported that for Research in Psychiatric Sciences, the Martin Levin Institute for
non-medicated children improved more than medicated chil- Child Development, and the Rama Shoval Foundation.
dren after intervention, suggesting that more impaired chil-
dren benefited from Cog-Fun. However, this finding must be References
viewed with caution due to the low teacher response rate American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statis-
and the fact that there were only nine children in the non-med- tical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC:
icated group. It is possible that low teacher compliance was due Author.
to lack of involvement in the treatment process. Further research American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and sta-
is required to examine the interaction of medication status on tistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.).
teachers’ reports as well as to examine the effect of Cog-Fun on Washington, DC: Author.
teacher ratings of the child in the classroom. Angello, L. M., Volpe, R. J., DiPerna, J. C., Gureasko-Moore, S.
P., Cureasko-Moore, D. P., Nebrig, M. R., & Ota, K. (2003).
Cog-Fun is an integrative cognitive behavioral approach,
Assessment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: An
harnessing executive strategies, learned and practiced in evaluation of six published rating scales. School Psychology
treatment, together with parental supports at home. Being Review, 32, 241-262.
equipped with tools (parental support, cognitive strategies) Barkley, R. A. (2012). Executive functions: What they are, how
acquired in treatment may aid children with ADHD to meet they work, and why they evolved. New York, NY: Guilford
daily challenges as they develop and mature. The results of Press.
this study add to the body of cognitive behavioral interven- Biederman, J., Mick, E., Fried, R., Wilner, N., Spencer, T. J., &
tions with a focus on executive cognitive outcomes in eco- Faraone, S. (2011). Are stimulants effective in the treatment
logical settings. Cog-Fun can impact on everyday functioning of executive function deficits? Results from a randomized
and QoL, and can be implemented by OTs in a relatively brief double blind study of OROS-methylphenidate in adults with
time span. ADHD. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 21, 508-515.
doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.11.005
The study has several limitations. The intervention was
Conner, K. C. (2000). Conners’ Rating Scales–Revised. North
compared with waitlist controls and not to another treat- Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.
ment. The sample comprised primarily educated, married Cordier, R., Bundy, A., Hocking, C., & Einfeld, S. (2010).
parents who were willing and able to commit to participa- Empathy in the play of children with attention deficit hyper-
tion in treatment with their child and to working with them activity disorder. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research,
at home, and therefore may not be representative of the gen- 30, 122-132. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1630.2009.00821.x
eral population of children with ADHD. There was no con- Cortese, S., Ferrin, M., Brandeis, D., Buitelaar, J., Daley, D.,
trol for children who received ongoing additional treatments. Dittmann, R. W., . . . Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2015). Cognitive
Teacher ratings were not used as inclusion criteria, and low training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Meta-
teacher return rates may have influenced the results of the analysis of clinical and neuropsychological outcomes from
study. Findings may also have been affected by potentially randomized controlled trials. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 164-174.
biased responding on the part of parents and teachers.
doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2014.12.010
Future studies that analyze Cog-Fun’s change agents, Dawson, D. R., Gaya, A., Hunt, A., Levine, B., Lemsky, C., &
such as the therapeutic interaction with parents and child, Polatajko, H. J. (2009). Using the cognitive orientation
the process of strategy acquisition, and increased aware- to occupational performance (CO-OP) with adults with
ness, are recommended. In addition, the interaction of Cog- executive dysfunction following traumatic brain injury.
Fun with medication status should be further examined. A Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 76, 115-127.
study with a larger sample in a more diverse population is doi:10.1177/000841740907600209
called for as well. Finally, the development of an adapted Dunn, L., Coster, W., Cohn, E. S., & Orsmond, G. I. (2009). Factors
Cog-Fun program in school settings is recommended in associated with participation of children with and without
which teachers can support children with ADHD as they ADHD in household tasks. Physical & Occupational Therapy
acquire and learn to implement executive strategies in the in Pediatrics, 29, 274-294. doi:10.1080/01942630903008327
DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., & Reid, R.
classroom.
(1998). ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Checklists, norms, and clini-
cal interpretation. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Ferguson, C. J. (2009). An effect size primer: A guide for clini-
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect cians and researchers. Professional Psychology: Research
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. and Practice, 40, 532-538. doi:10.1037/a0015808
Hahn-Markowitz et al. 665

Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). McCandless, S., & O’Laughlin, L. (2007). The clinical utility of
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF). the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF)
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. in the diagnosis of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders,
Gol, D., & Jarus, T. (2005). Effect of a social skills training group 10, 381-389. doi:10.1177/1087054706292115
on everyday activities of children with attention-deficit- McCarthy, M. L., MacKenzie, E. J., Durbin, D. R., Aitken, M. E.,
hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Jaffe, K. M., & Paidas, C. N., . . . the CHAT Study Group.
Neurology, 47, 539-545. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005. (2005). The Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory: An evaluation
tb0118.x of its reliability and validity for children with traumatic brain
Goulardins, J. B., Rigoli, D., Loh, P. R., Kane, R., Licari, M., Hands, injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 86,
B., . . . Piek, J. (2015). The relationship between motor skills, 1901-1909. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2005.03.026
social problems, and ADHD symptomatology: Does it vary Pelham, W. E., & Fabiano, G. A. (2008). Evidence-based psycho-
according to parent and teacher report? Journal of Attention social treatments for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Disorders, 23, 1-10. doi:10.1177/1087054715580394 Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37, 184-
Goverover, Y., Johnston, M. V., Toglia, J., & Deluce, J. (2007). 214. doi:10.1080/15374410701818681
Treatment to improve self-awareness in persons with Polatajko, H., Mandich, A., & McEwen, S. E. (2011). Cognitive
acquired brain injury. Brain Injury, 21, 913-923. doi:10.1080/ orientation to daily occupational performance (CO-OP): A
02699050701553205 cognitive-based intervention for children and adults. In N.
Guiles, G. M. (2011). A neurofunctional approach to rehabilitation Katz (Ed.), Cognition and occupation across the life span:
after brain injury. In N. Katz (Ed.), Cognition and occupation Models for intervention in occupational therapy (3rd ed.,
across the life span: Models for intervention in occupational pp. 299-321). Bethesda, MD: American Occupational
therapy (3rd ed., pp. 351-382). Bethesda, MD: American Therapy Association Press.
Occupational Therapy Association Press. Riccio, C. A., & Gomes, H. (2013). Interventions for EFDs in chil-
Hahn-Markowitz, J., Manor, I., & Maeir, A. (2011). Effectiveness dren and adolescents. Applied Neuropsychology in Children,
of cognitive-functional (Cog-Fun) intervention with children 2, 1-8. doi:10.1080/21622965.2013.748383
with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A pilot study. Smith, S. D., Vitulano, L. A., Katsovich, L., Li, S., Moore, C., Li,
American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 65, 384-392. F., . . . Leckman, J. F. (2016). A randomized controlled trial
doi:10.5014/ajot.2011.000901 of an integrated brain, body, and social intervention for chil-
Halperin, J. M., Marks, D. J., Bedard, A.-C. V., Chacko, A., dren with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 20, 1-15.
Curchack, J. T., Yoon, C. A., & Healey, D. M. (2013). Training doi:10.1177/1087054716647490
executive, attention, and motor skills: A proof-of-concept Sparrow, E. P. (2010). Essentials of conners behavior assess-
study in preschool children with ADHD. Journal of Attention ments. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
Disorders, 17, 711-721. doi:10.1177/1087054711435681 Stern, A., Pollak, Y., Bonne, O., Malik, E., & Maeir, A. (2017).
Jarratt, K. P., Riccio, C. A., & Siekierski, B. M. (2005). Assessment The relationship between executive functions and quality of
of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) using the life in adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders,
BASC and BRIEF. Applied Neuropsychology, 12, 83-93. 21(4), 323-330. doi:10.1177/1087054713504133
doi:10.1207/s15324826an1202_4 Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Steering
Maeir, A., Fisher, O., Traub Bar-Ilan, R., Boas, N., Berger, I., & Committee on Quality Improvement and Management. (2011).
Landau, Y. E. (2014). Effectiveness of cognitive-functional Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treat-
(Cog-Fun) occupational therapy intervention for young children ment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and ado-
with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A controlled study. lescents. Pediatrics, 128, 1007-1022. doi:10.1542/peds.2011-2654
American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 68, 260-267. Tamm, L., & Nakonezny, P. A. (2015). Metacognitive executive
doi:10.5014/ajot.2014.011700 function training for young children with ADHD: A proof-
Maeir, A., Hahn-Markowitz, J., Fisher, O., & Traub Bar-Ilan, of-concept study. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, 7,
R. (2012). Cognitive-functional (Cog-Fun) intervention in 183-190. doi:10.1007/s12402-014-0162-x
occupational therapy for children aged 5-10 with ADHD: Tamm, L., Nakonezny, P. A., & Hughes, C. W. (2014). An open
Treatment Manual. Jerusalem, Israel: Faculty of Medicine, trial of a metacognitive executive function training for young
School of Occupational Therapy, Hadassah and the Hebrew children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 18,
University. 551-559. doi:10.1177/1087054712445782
Maeir, A., Hahn-Markowitz, J., Fisher, O., & Traub Bar-Ilan, Ten Have, T. R., Normand, S. T., Marcus, S. M., Brown, C. H.,
R. (2014). Cognitive-functional (Cog-Fun) intervention for Lavori, P., & Duan, N. (2008). Intent-to-treat vs. non-intent-
children with ADHD: Theory and practice. In I. Berger & to-treat analyses under treatment non-adherence in mental
A. Maeir (Eds.), ADHD: A transparent impairment, clinical, health randomized trials. Psychiatric Annals, 38, 772-783.
daily-life and research aspects in diverse populations (pp. 81- doi:10.3928/00485713-20081201-10
98). New York, NY: Nova Science. Toglia, J. (2011). The dynamic interaction model of cognition
Malik, T. A., Rooney, M., Chronis-Tuscano, A., & Tariq, N. (2017). in cognitive rehabilitation. In N. Katz (Ed.), Cognition and
Preliminary efficacy of a behavioral parent training program occupation across the life span: Models for intervention in
for children with ADHD in Pakistan. Journal of Attention occupational therapy (3rd ed., pp. 161-202). Bethesda, MD:
Disorders, 21(5), 390-404. doi:10.1177/1087054714524158 American Occupational Therapy Association Press.
666 Journal of Attention Disorders 24(5)

Toglia, J., Johnston, M. V., Goverover, Y., & Dain, B. (2010). Jerusalem, Israel. She is also a member of the school’s Neurocognitive
A multicontext approach to promoting transfer of strat- Rehabilitation laboratory.
egy use and self-regulation after brain injury: An explor-
Itai Berger, MD, is director of the Neuro-Cognitive Center,
atory study. Brain Injury, 24, 664-677. doi:10.3109/
Pediatric Wing, Hadassah and Hebrew University Medical Center,
02699051003610474
Jerusalem, Israel.
Varni, J. W., Seid, M., & Kurtin, P. S. (2001). PedsQL 4.0:
Reliability and validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Iris Manor, MD, is director of the ADHD Unit, Geha Medical
Inventory Version 4.0 Generic Core Scales in healthy and Center, Petah Tikva, Israel and Sackler School of Medicine, Tel
patient populations. Medical Care, 37, 800-812. doi:10.1097/ Aviv University, Israel.
00005650-200108000-00006
Adina Maeir, PhD, is a professor, school chair, and director of
graduate studies, School of Occupational Therapy, Faculty of
Author Biographies Medicine of Hadassah and Hebrew University of Jerusalem,
Jeri Hahn-Markowitz, MSc, OTR, is a doctoral student at the Israel. She is also director of the school’s Neurocognitive
Hadassah-Hebrew University School of Occupational Therapy, Rehabilitation laboratory.

You might also like