You are on page 1of 19

Revista de Psicodidáctica

ISSN: 1136-1034
revista-psicodidactica@ehu.es
Universidad del País Vasco/Euskal Herriko
Unibertsitatea
España

Ripoll, Juan C.; Aguado, Gerardo


La mejora de la comprensión lectora en español: Un meta-análisis
Revista de Psicodidáctica, vol. 19, núm. 1, enero-junio, 2014, pp. 27-44
Universidad del País Vasco/Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea
Vitoria-Gazteis, España

Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=17529569002

How to cite
Complete issue
Scientific Information System
More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal
Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative
Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44 ISSN: 1136-1034 eISSN: 2254-4372
www.ehu.es/revista-psicodidactica © UPV/EHU
DOI: 10.1387/RevPsicodidact.9001

Reading Comprehension Improvement


for Spanish Students: A Meta-Analysis
Juan C. Ripoll* and Gerardo Aguado**
*Colegio Santa María la Real de Sarriguren, **Universidad de Navarra

Abstract
A systematic review of interventions to improve reading comprehension was conducted in Spanish-
speaking students. Studies included had to have an experimental or quasi experimental design, the
equivalence of groups in reading comprehension before intervention had to be controlled, and the
participants had to be school-age. Thirty nine studies met the above criteria and were considered. A
meta-analysis of random effects was carried out obtaining a combined effect-size estimate of 0.71.
The interventions that proved to be more effective were those based on comprehension strategies like
locating the main ideas or making inferences, and those interventions combining teaching of strategies
with other methods such as motivation or improvement of decoding. Only two studies reported about
whether results remained over time after intervention, so maintenance of results is an aspect that should
be included in future research.
Keywords: Comprehension strategies, meta-analysis, reading comprehension.

Resumen
Se realizó una revisión sistemática de intervenciones para la mejora de la comprensión lectora en espa-
ñol. Se incluyeron estudios realizados con alumnado en edad escolar con diseños experimentales o di-
seños cuasi-experimentales, que habían controlado la equivalencia de los grupos en comprensión lec-
tora antes de la intervención. Se localizaron 39 estudios con los que se hizo un meta-análisis de efectos
aleatorios obteniendo una estimación combinada del tamaño del efecto de 0.71. Se muestra la eficacia
de las intervenciones basadas en estrategias de comprensión, como la identificación de ideas principales
o la construcción de inferencias, y de las que combinan la enseñanza de estrategias con otros métodos
como la motivación o la mejora de la descodificación. Solo dos estudios proporcionaron información
sobre cómo se mantenían los resultados tiempo después de finalizar la intervención por lo que se consi-
dera que ése tendría que ser uno de los puntos a tener en cuenta en futuras investigaciones.
Palabras clave: Comprensión lectora, estrategias de comprensión, meta-análisis.

Correspondence concerning this article should addressed to Juan C. Ripoll, Departamento de orien-
tación, Colegio Santa María la Real, Paseo de Champagnat, 2. 31621 Sarriguren (Navarra). E-mail:
juancruzripoll@maristaspamplona.es
28 JUAN C. RIPOLL AND GERARDO AGUADO

Introduction very important one is the report of


the United States National Read-
Several international studies ing Panel (NRP, 2000), which re-
show that, at least since 2000, the viewed 215 studies on methods to
reading comprehension of Span- improve reading comprehension,
ish-speaking students has not im- concluding that there were seven
proved (Mullis, Martin, Foy, & forms of intervention with a firm
Druker, 2012, OECD, 2010). scientific basis. Those are the fol-
There is a lack in efforts to lowing: self-monitoring of com-
improve the reading comprehen- prehension, cooperative learning,
sion of students, and it is due to graphic and semantic organizers,
the absence or lack of dissemina- story structure, question answer-
tion of strategies, programs and ing, question generating and sum-
intervention methods which have marizing.
proved their efficacy. Evidence Some later reviews focus on
of this deficit is that there is no a particular kind of student, for
agency to review the effectiveness example, elementary students
of the methods to improve reading (Slavin, Lake, Chambers, Cheung,
comprehension used for Spanish- & Davis, 2009), or students with
speaking students. Confirmation of learning disabilities (Berkeley,
this shortcoming is that there is no Scruggs, & Mastropieri, 2010; Ed-
synthesis of published research on monds et al., 2009; Solis et al.,
the effectiveness of those methods. 2012). Other reviews focus on dif-
This situation contrasts with ferent types of programs or strate-
English-speaking countries, es- gies such as reciprocal teaching of
pecially the United States, where comprehension strategies (Rosen-
evidence-based practice promotes shine & Meister, 1993), question
the use of research-based instruc- generation (Rosenshine, Meister, &
tional methods. Agencies and in- Chapman, 1996), repeated reading
stitutions such as What Works (Therrien, 2004), reading aloud in-
Clearinghouse, Best Evidence En- terventions (Swanson et al., 2011),
cyclopedia, or Promising Practices classroom discussions about texts
Network are available in those (Murphy, Wilkinson, Soter, Henne-
countries; these institutions review sey, & Alexander, 2009), self-mon-
and analyze research on educa- itoring strategies (Joseph & Eve-
tional programs to make recom- ligh, 2011), or writing activities to
mendations on methods which are improve reading comprehension
supported by rigorous studies and (Graham & Herbert, 2010). These
the most effective interventions. reviews show that students with
Moreover, various revisions and without learning disabilities
have reported on the effective- can improve their reading com-
ness of different interventions. A prehension through interventions

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


READING COMPREHENSION IMPROVEMENT FOR SPANISH STUDENTS:
A META-ANALYSIS 29

based on text activities such as the applied, the number of students


use of comprehension strategies or per instructor and the relationship
text analysis. between instructor and students.
Spanish-speaking countries All these factors have led to a dif-
may use the information provided ference in the effect size in the re-
by institutions and research syn- views cited above.
thesis of English-speaking coun-
tries, but there are two problems
in doing so. The first problem is Method
that there is evidence that read-
ing comprehension in languages Search strategy
with transparent orthographies,
such as Spanish, is less influenced A search was conducted using
by the decoding ability than Eng- the following methods: search in
lish reading comprehension (Florit databases, search for references in
& Cain, 2011; Share, 2008). The the studies which were already lo-
second inconvenience is that in- cated, manual search in the library
tervention strategies can be used of a university, and contact of rel-
with Spanish-speaking students, evant authors.
but normally, programs, and ma- The databases consulted were,
terials are written in English pre- in alphabetical order, the follow-
venting therefore their use with ing: CogPrints, Conycit, Ebsco,
Spanish-speaking students. Educ@ment, E-book, Dialnet,
To improve this situation, the Google, Proquest Dissertations
main purpose of this study is to of- and Theses, Isi web of Knowledge,
fer a synthesis of research made Mastesis, Periodicals Index On-
until 2012 on interventions to im- line, PsycINFO, publications of
prove Spanish reading compre- the Unesco Chair for Reading and
hension. Investigations with ex- Writing, Redined, Sage, SciELO,
perimental or quasi-experimental Theseus, and University of Nav-
designs, conducted with students arre library catalog.
from kindergarten, elementary, The key words used for the
middle and high schools are re- search were comprehension, read-
viewed. The moderators taken into ing comprehension, or their equiv-
account are: the way in which par- alents in the language of the da-
ticipants are allocated to groups, tabase. We limited the search to
the comprehension assessment Spanish or added the word Spanish
with standardized tests or with in English databases. Terms used
tests prepared by the researchers, in Google search were Spanish
the control of fidelity implement- equivalents for reading compre-
ing interventions, the quality of hension improvement intervention,
studies, the type of intervention and “reading comprehension” re-

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


30 JUAN C. RIPOLL AND GERARDO AGUADO

search “control group”. We also We excluded studies that did


made a search on Google Scholar, not meet the above criteria and
looking for studies that cited any studies that shared the sample, or
of the 56 that were found in the da- part of it. However, the major rea-
tabases. sons for exclusion were the lack of
A manual search was con- equivalence of the groups and not
ducted by consulting the indexes providing sufficient information to
of the following Spanish journals: calculate the effect size of the in-
Anales de Psicología, Bordón, tervention on reading comprehen-
Cognitiva, Estudios de Psicología, sion.
Infancia y Aprendizaje, Lectura y After this search we identified
Vida, Ocnos, Psicológica, Psico- 177 references. We could not get
thema, Revista de Investigación the full text of 50 of them, and we
Educativa, Revista Española de selected 29 publications from the
Pedagogía, Revista de Psicodidác- remaining references. Those publi-
tica, and Spanish Journal of Psy- cations described 39 separate stud-
chology. Finally, we requested in- ies with 3,520 participants. Eleven
formation from 43 people who of these studies had been published
had done research on Spanish- as dissertations, chapters, in jour-
language reading comprehension. nals without peer reviews, or sub-
We obtained response from 27 of mitted for publication, and the re-
them. maining ones had been published
in peer reviewed journals.
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
Coding procedure
We included studies that had
the following characteristics: a) an Two researchers, both hold a
intervention to improve reading PhD in education, developed and
comprehension was carried out; b) piloted a coding manual, and in-
participants were Spanish-speak- dependently recorded the informa-
ing students in non-university edu- tion contained in the studies with
cation; c) there was, at least, a con- it. The reliability of the evaluators
trol group; d) participants had been was checked comparing a third of
randomly assigned to the interven- the records randomly selected. In
tion or control group, if they had the qualitative variables average
not, it was established that groups Cohen’s kappa was .61, and the re-
were equivalent in reading com- sults were between 0 and .89. For
prehension before intervention, or quantitative variables, the average
differences were statistically con- intraclass correlation was .99, and
trolled; e) enough data was pro- the results were between .96 and
vided in order to calculate the ef- 1. Following this analysis, all var-
fect size of the intervention. iables whose Cohen’s kappa was

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


READING COMPREHENSION IMPROVEMENT FOR SPANISH STUDENTS:
A META-ANALYSIS 31

less than .60 were reformulated in we could not use these criteria, we
the coding manual, and after that, chose a group randomly.
all disagreements were resolved When the intervention results
by consensus, analyzing the stud- were assessed with different com-
ies again. prehension tests a standardized
The information collected from test was chosen to calculate the
each study was its identification: effect size. If not possible, the re-
description of the sample (school viewers decided which test as-
grade, intellectual capacity, de- sessed better the reading compre-
coding level, level of comprehen- hension, and in case of doubt or
sion, learning disabilities, socio- disagreement they selected a test
economic level and area where the randomly.
participants studied -urban/rural), The methods to improve read-
information on persons who car- ing comprehension were classi-
ried out the interventions, method fied into three groups. The first
(sample selection, group forma- one consists of decoding-based
tion, type of control group, equiva- interventions, including phono-
lency of groups, how reading com- logical ability, letter knowledge,
prehension was assessed, and the reading accuracy, and fluency im-
provement. The second group in-
way in which fidelity of treatment
cludes comprehension strategy in-
implementation was checked), in-
terventions such as activation of
tervention characteristics (type of
prior knowledge, making infer-
intervention, implementation, du- ences, sorting out of text ideas,
ration), and outcomes (effect size synthesizing (finding main ideas,
at the end of intervention and ef- summaries or outlines) and self-
fect size at follow-up). monitoring of comprehension. The
When several studies shared interventions in the third group
the same sample or part of it, only combine strategies from the sec-
one of them was selected, choosing ond group with other interven-
the study that had the largest sam- tions, such as those from the first
ple or gave more detailed infor- group, vocabulary enhancement,
mation. In studies with more than reasoning skills, answering ques-
two groups a group without treat- tions, or motivation.
ment was chosen as control group, The quality of each study was
if possible. If there were several assessed by giving one point for
groups receiving different treat- each of the following information:
ments it was chosen among them, socio-economic level of students,
in this order, the one which had area where they studied, instruc-
less attrition, the one with a better tor, sample selection, assignment
description of the intervention, the of participants to the groups, type
one with more participants, or, if of student grouping, classes that

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


32 JUAN C. RIPOLL AND GERARDO AGUADO

intervention replaced, number of The meta-regressions were cal-


students per teacher, number and culated using Wilson’s extension
length of sessions, and type of Metarreg for SPSS (http://mason.
texts used. Two additional points gmu.edu/ ~ dwilsonb / downloads /
were awarded if an assessment of spss_macros.zip).
implementation fidelity was made
and if a follow-up assessment was Control of publication bias
conducted.
Publication bias was control-
Effect-size calculation and meta- led by visual inspection of a fun-
analysis method nel plot, calculating the fail safe
N, and comparing the results of
People for which this research studies published in peer reviewed
method is unfamiliar can find gen- journals with those published in
eral information in Botella and other media.
Gambara (2002), or Sánchez-Meca
and Botella (2010), among others.
The effect size was calculated Results
as Hedges’ g because 56% of the
studies were conducted with sam- Participants
ples of fewer than 50 participants
and this measure removes possi- Participants in the reviewed
ble positive bias which Cohen’s studies were students from kinder-
d may show when sample sizes garten to 10th grade. In the stud-
are small. First we calculated Co- ies where the sample selection was
hen’s d employing Wilson’s cal- described, the sample is incidental
culator (http://gunston.gmu.edu/ (74%). In most groups there were
cebcp/EffectSizeCalculator/index. no restrictions applied due to in-
html), and then we converted it to tellectual capacity of the students
Hedges’ g. (79%), the existence of special
We had clear that we were education needs (82%), the abil-
comparing different types of inter- ity to decode (77%), or the level
ventions and hence a common ef- of reading comprehension (74%).
fect size was unlikely, so we em- There was no information about
ployed a random effects model the socio-cultural status of students
(Raudenbush, 2009) for meta-anal- in 64% of the studies, and in the
ysis. The formulas offered by Bo- remaining predominated middle
renstein, Hedges, Higgins, and class population. Thirty-eight per-
Rothstein (2009) were included in cent of the groups studied in ur-
a spreadsheet of Open Office. The ban areas, 3% in rural areas, and
weighting of the studies was done 13% were formed of students who
using the inverse of the variance. came from both areas. Such infor-

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


READING COMPREHENSION IMPROVEMENT FOR SPANISH STUDENTS:
A META-ANALYSIS 33

mation was not provided in the re- trol group received no intervention
maining groups. The sample sizes in 30 studies, carrying out ordinary
of the studies included in meta- class activities in most occasions.
analysis were of 10 to 825 partici- Alternative treatments were used
pants, with a mean of 90 and a me- in the other studies, such as vari-
dian of 41. ations of the treatment applied to
the experimental group or class ac-
Interventions for reading tivities designed to enhance under-
comprehension improvement standing.
The intervention activities that
No intervention was repeated were used the most were: iden-
in more than three studies, and tification of main ideas, topic or
when an intervention was carried thematic progression (22 studies),
out in two or more studies it was construction of inferences (21 stud-
normally because the same inter- ies), and abstracting (19 studies).
vention was conducted in differ- Self-monitoring of comprehension,
ent studies within the same inves- prior knowledge activation and ac-
tigation. Interventions were based tivation of schemas and knowledge
on comprehension strategies in about text structures were used in
23 studies, metacognitive strategies 11 studies each one. Graphic or-
were combined with other forms of ganizers and generating self-ques-
intervention in 12 studies, 3 studies tioning were used in 10 studies
focused on decoding and there was each one. The rest of the inter-
a study that used an intervention vention activities was used in 8 or
based on reasoning skills. The con- fewer studies.

Figure 1. Forest plot of the effect sizes of the studies.

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


34 JUAN C. RIPOLL AND GERARDO AGUADO

Average effect size dispersion. A meta-regression with


the method of moments confirmed
Figure 1 shows the forest plot this trend (β = –0.35; p = 0.009;
of the analyzed studies. The av- R2 = 0.12).
erage effect size of all studies The studies were distributed
was M = 0.71 (CI = 0.52 to 0.89, according to the grade the par-
p < .01). According to the result ticipants in, and we sought for
obtained in Q statistic (Q = 207.7, the resultant groups to allow the
p < .01) we must reject the hypoth- analysis of different ranges and
esis that there is an effect com- the conservation of the greater
mon to all studies, so these results number of studies because 8 stud-
are heterogeneous. The I2 statis- ies included students from differ-
tic (I2 = 81.7) indicates that almost ent grades. In all ranges of several
82% of the variance is attributable grades we observed a positive ef-
to heterogeneity. fect with no significant differences
due to the grade of the students.
Analysis of moderators The meta-regression showed that
there was no linear relationship
Since the hypothesis that the between the grade of participants
real effect is not the same in all and the effect-size of the inter-
studies was accepted, an analysis vention (β = –0.01; p = 0.95;
of moderators was conducted find- R2 = 0.00).
ing no significant differences in We started exploring the in-
any case. Table 1 shows the results fluence of the characteristics of
of the main analysis performed. the interventions analyzing the
Unlike initial expectations type of method used to enhance
the effect size of studies with ran- understanding. Studies based on
dom assignment of participants to teaching comprehension strategies
groups (M = 0.86) was higher than (M = 0.63) produced a moderate
that of studies with other forms of effect size, and studies that com-
assigning participants to different bined these strategies with other
treatments. As expected, the results forms of intervention, such as mo-
of groups evaluated with standard- tivation, questions, or vocabu-
ized tests (M = 0.62) were lower lary teaching produced a slightly
than those evaluated with other greater effect (M = .96). In con-
tests (M = 0.78). trast, studies based on decoding
A scatter plot distributed the ef- (M = –0.03) did not produce a sig-
fect sizes of the studies according nificant effect on reading compre-
to the quality score obtained show- hension (p = .94). The single study
ing a downward trend, so that the which conducted an intervention
higher quality studies the smaller based on reasoning skills obtained
effect sizes and a more reduced an effect size g = 1.28.

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


READING COMPREHENSION IMPROVEMENT FOR SPANISH STUDENTS:
A META-ANALYSIS 35

Table 1
Summary of Results

Analized group K M CI Q p(Q) I2

According to the way in which participants were assigned to the groups


Subject randomization 6 0.86 0.32 - 1.40 18.16 <.01 66.94
Group randomization 16 0.63 0.34 - 0.93 58.20 <.01 74.23
Paired groups 9 0.78 0.18 - 1.38 68.24 <.01 88.27
Non randomized assignment 8 0.69 0.35 - 1.03 49.87 <.01 85.97
According to the type of control group
No intervention 30 0.78 0.57 - 0.98 178.78 <.01 83.78
Alternative intervention 9 0.39 -0.15 - 0.94 26.64 <.01 69.75
According to the grade of participants
Kindergarten - 2nd grade 4 0.68 0.05 - 1.32 25.97 <.01 89.65
3rd - 4th grade 11 0.73 0.37 - 1.09 36.73 <.01 78.60
5th - 7th grade 13 0.69 0.36 - 1.01 68.00 <.01 82.35
8th -12th grade 6 1.15 0.67 - 1.62 35.45 <.01 87.64
According to the assessment of comprehension
Norm-referenced tests 18 0.62 0.35 - 0.90 101.30 <.01 93.09
Non standardized tests 21 0.78 0.53 - 1.04 95.49 <.01 79.06
According to intervention method
Comprehension strategies 23 0.63 0.43 - 0.83 61.09 <.01 63.99
Strategies + other 12 0.96 0.54 - 1.37 118.40 <.01 90.71
Decoding and phonological skill 3 –0.03 -0.73 - 0.65 0.11 0.95 0
According to instructor
Teachers 14 0.51 0.26 - 0.76 63.89 <.01 79.65
Other 11 0.55 0.30 - 0.80 15.96 0.10 37.33

Among the strategies, summa- tween 2 and 8 strategies (k = 16,


rizing (k = 4, M = 0.64, CI = 0.23- M = 0.52, CI = 0.25 to 0.79). The
1.05), self-questioning (k = 2, scatter plot distributing results ac-
M = 0.84, CI = 0.17-1.52) and ac- cording to the number of strate-
tivating schemes (a single study gies used showed a heterogene-
with g = 1.69) were the only ones ous distribution, so we did not
that were used in isolation. The analyze the relationship between
remaining studies combined be- the number of strategies taught

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


36 JUAN C. RIPOLL AND GERARDO AGUADO

and reading comprehension out- less than 150 subjects a remark-


comes. able asymmetry was evident pre-
Among the studies using com- dominating results greater than
prehension strategies, alone or 0.4. This configuration is typi-
in combination with other inter- cal of the fields in which there
ventions, those which followed is a publication bias due to the
a reciprocal teaching approach greater difficulty to publish stud-
(M = 0.51) had a lower and more ies with negative or low results
homogeneous result than those us- and small sample size preventing
ing different approaches such as their authors from obtaining re-
integration in the curriculum, com- sults that are significant, which is
puter-assisted instruction or other the main cause of publication bias
(M = .81). Nevertheless significant (Hopewell, Loudon, Clarke, Ox-
differences were not found. The man, & Dickersin, 2009).
groups that employed cooperative The fail-save N was calculated
learning, all of them included as re- by Rosenthal’s method (Rosenthal
ciprocal teaching, did not achieve a 1979). It was 3361, indicating that
significant improvement in reading the results of the meta-analysis are
comprehension (k = 5, M = 0.34, resistant to publication bias.
CI = –0.25-0.94). Moreover, this meta-analysis
We found scarce differences included 11 groups from no peer-
when the instructor was a teacher reviewed journals, dissertations,
(M = 0.51) and when he was a re- chapters and manuscripts submit-
searcher (M = .55). Finally, a meta- ted for publication. The effect size
regression analyzed two variables of these 11 groups (M = 0.51), was
related to the intensity with which lower than that of studies pub-
the interventions were carried out: lished in peer-reviewed journals
the ratio of students per instruc- (M = 0.75), although the difference
tor and duration. While the rela- was not significant (p = .16).
tionship between ratio and effect According to these analyses,
size was not significant (β = –0.02; the actual effect size may be less
p = 0.92; R2 = 0.00), a relationship than that obtained in the meta-anal-
was observed between duration ysis, although this does not seem to
of the intervention and effect size alter the main result: interventions
(β = 0.39; p = 0.02; R2 = 0.15). to improve reading comprehension
of Spanish produce a substantial
Control of publication bias improvement.

In the funnel groups with Fidelity of implementation


larger samples were distributed
around an effect size close to We considered that there was
g = 0.4, while in the groups with some control of treatment fidel-

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


READING COMPREHENSION IMPROVEMENT FOR SPANISH STUDENTS:
A META-ANALYSIS 37

ity in 44% of the studies. In 13% other forms of intervention, pro-


of the studies we assumed that duce significant improvements.
such monitoring existed because Nevertheless, the few interven-
the person who carried out the in- tions based on improving decod-
tervention had participated in its ing ability have not significantly
design. Intervention was imple- influenced reading comprehension.
mented by computer in 10% of the Follow-up evaluations were made
studies. In the remaining studies only in two studies at different mo-
(21%) intentional monitoring sys- ments, so it is unknown to what
tems were used, usually combining extent these results are maintained
different observation systems like over time. No specific method has
a diary of intervention or question- been studied independently by sev-
naires. Studies using intentional eral research teams. There are stud-
monitoring systems did not show a ies that have used the same strate-
significant improvement in reading gies, and the most used has been
comprehension (k = 8, M = 0.44, summarizing.
CI = –0.04-0.92). We observed no noticeable re-
lation between improvements and
Follow-up the grade in which the participants
where in. The results are concur-
Only in two studies a follow-up rent with other reviews of studies
of comprehension improvements that have applied methods of im-
was made after the posttest. Orel- proving reading comprehension in
lana and Bravo (2000) found that English: summarization and teach-
two years after the intervention, ing of mixed strategies had already
effect size of the intervention had been identified as methods with a
gone from g = 0.54 to g = –0.02. firm scientific basis (NRP, 2000).
Soriano, Chebaani, Soriano and The effect obtained with summa-
Descals (2011) found that the ef- rization is slightly higher, but not
fect after intervention (g = 0.56) significantly different from that
was maintained 4 months later obtained by Graham and Herbert
(g = 0.57). (2010), and the results obtained
with reciprocal teaching are mid-
way between those obtained by
Discussion Rosenshine and Meister (1993)
when standardized tests were em-
This review shows that, among ployed in the assessments and with
interventions to improve reading other sort of tests (M = 0.32 and
comprehension in Spanish, those M = 0.88).
based on teaching of comprehen- Some results do not agree with
sion strategies and those that com- those obtained in other reviews.
bine these teaching strategies with One is that there was almost no

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


38 JUAN C. RIPOLL AND GERARDO AGUADO

difference between the interven- ference that the first one did not
tions implemented by teachers and work cooperatively. These results
those carried out by others (usually question whether the effectiveness
researchers), unlike in Edmonds of methods based on cooperative
et al. (2009) where the effect was learning (NRP, 2000) is due to the
greater in interventions applied use of cooperative methods or to
by researchers. Also remarkable comprehension strategies taught
is the null effect size of interven- with them.
tions based on improving decoding The duration of interventions
and non-significant result of stud- is a variable that is described but
ies using cooperative methods for not discussed in several reviews
teaching strategies that had shown (Edmonds et al., 2009; Solis et al.,
positive results in the revision of 2012; Swanson et al., 2011). In this
the National Reading Panel (NRP, study we found a positive relation-
2000). ship between length of interven-
The effect of interventions tion and effect size, while Rosen-
based on decoding ability was shine and Meister (1993) found no
only calculated with three stud- relationship between the number of
ies. Other reviews (Berkeley et sessions and the significance of the
al., 2010; Edmonds et al., 2009; results, and Berkeley et al. (2010)
Slavin et al., 2009) obtained ef- found that interventions lasting be-
fects between 0.09 and 0.30, tween one week and one month
which are larger when students produced greater effects than those
with learning difficulties are pre- that lasted more or less than that
dominant in the sample. There- time. Our measure of the duration
fore, it should be noted that the of intervention was somewhat dif-
null result obtained here could be ferent (actual time spent on the in-
an effect of the low number of tervention) and we analyzed its re-
studies, or may reflect the greater lation to the understanding through
ease with which Spanish-speaking meta-regression, which makes the
students can reach a level of de- three analysis difficult to compare.
coding sufficient to succeed in the Finally, we cannot ignore that
comprehension tasks. a relationship between study qual-
Regarding cooperative meth- ity and effect size was found, ac-
ods of the 5 studies reporting its cording to which the higher quality
use three are taken from Car- studies tend to find discrete im-
dona (2002), and their results are provements in reading comprehen-
very low (between g = 0.19 and sion. The use of standardized tests
g = –0.70). These three studies are for assessing reading comprehen-
unusual because the control group sion and intentional systems of fi-
received the same training as the delity control relate to smaller ef-
experimental group, with the dif- fect sizes, although the differences

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


READING COMPREHENSION IMPROVEMENT FOR SPANISH STUDENTS:
A META-ANALYSIS 39

with studies that do not share these most of the studies located, the re-
features is not significant. sults of these analyses should only
be considered as guidelines.
Generalization of results
Implications for educative
The use of a random-effects practice and research
meta-analysis model allows us to
generalize the obtained results to It can be concluded from this
other studies from the same pop- review that interventions based on
ulation, which is that of studies teaching strategies and on these
about methods for improving read- strategies combined with other ac-
ing comprehension carried out tions such as increasing vocabu-
with school aged Spanish-speaking lary, motivation for reading or de-
students in which a control group coding, have shown significant
is employed to calculate the ef- effects on reading comprehension
fect of the intervention. However, of Spanish-speaking students. In
any generalization should be made contrast, in the studies analyzed,
with caution, taking into account methods based only on decoding
that the studies analyzed have con- activities have shown to be useful
siderable variability, as seen in for improving reading comprehen-
the width of confidence intervals sion.
and in measures of heterogeneity, There are signs showing that
which remained high even when teaching and practice of summa-
we analyzed groups of similar in- rizing can be a useful intervention
terventions according to the target system, but most successful stud-
processes or to the way in which ies have combined various strat-
comprehension skills were taught. egies, which include, apart from
Low heterogeneity was found only summarization, the activation of
in decoding based interventions. In prior knowledge, identification of
other cases it may happen that in- main ideas, self-questioning, con-
terventions did not produce a com- struction of inferences, paraphras-
mon effect, but different effects ing, schema activation, graphic or-
depending on variables that we do ganizers, and self-monitoring of
not have controlled. comprehension. Reciprocal teach-
Another limitation of this study ing seems to be a good method
is the small number of groups with for teaching these strategies, al-
which some analysis were per- though success was also achieved
formed, such as the effect of in- with other ways to teach and prac-
terventions based on decoding, tice. In general, these systems have
summarizing, self-questioning or proved to be a more effective com-
cooperative work. Taking into ac- prehension enhancement than or-
count the small sample size of dinary class activities which per-

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


40 JUAN C. RIPOLL AND GERARDO AGUADO

formed most of the comparison comparisons between different


groups. methods of intervention. A priority
We need more studies to com- for future studies should include
plete information in the areas in follow-up measures to determine
which only a few investigations whether the effects of interventions
have been found, and to make are sustained over time or fade.

References
(References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis)

*Anaya, D. (2005). Efectos del resumen mental. Madrid: Universidad Com-


de la mejora de la metacompren- plutense.
sión, de la comprensión lectora y *Carriedo, N. (1992). Enseñar a com-
del rendimiento académico. Revista prender: Diseño y valoración de
de Educación, 337, 281-294. oai: un programa de instrucción para
redined.mec.es:00820053000216 formar a los profesores en la ense-
Berkeley, S., Scruggs, T. E., & Mastrop- ñanza de estrategias de compren-
ieri, M. A. (2010). Reading compre- sión de las ideas principales en el
hension instruction for students with aula. Madrid: Universidad Autó-
learning disabilities, 1995-2006: A noma.
metha-analysis. Remedial and Spe- *Carriedo, N., & Alonso-Tapia, J.
cial Education, 31(6), 423-436. doi: (1995). Comprehension strategy
10.1177/0741932509355988 training in content areas. European
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Hig- Journal of Psychology of Educa-
gins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. tion, 10(4), 411-431. doi: 10.1007/
(2009). Introduction to meta-anal- BF03172930
ysis. Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power
doi: 10.1002/9780470743386 analysis for the behavioral sciences.
Botella, J., & Gambara, H. (2002). Nueva York: Academic Press.
¿Qué es el meta-análisis? Madrid: Edmonds, M. S., Vaughn, S., Wexler,
Biblioteca Nueva. J., Reutebuch, C., Cable, A., Tack-
*Cardona, M. C. (2002). Efectos del ett, K. K. & Schnakenberg J. W.
agrupamiento sobre la fluidez y (2009). A synthesis of reading in-
la comprensión lectora en alum- terventions and effects on reading
nos buenos lectores. Bordón, 54(2- comprehension outcomes for older
3), 359-374. oai: redined.mec. struggling readers. Review of Edu-
es:014200330086 cational Research, 79(1), 262-300.
*Carpio, C. (2000). Intervención me- doi: 10.3102/0034654308325998
tacognitiva sobre la comprensión Florit, E., & Cain K. (2011). The simple
lectora de personas con retraso view of reading: Is it valid for dif-

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


READING COMPREHENSION IMPROVEMENT FOR SPANISH STUDENTS:
A META-ANALYSIS 41

ferent types of alphabetic orthogra- Hopewell, S., Loudon, K., Clarke, M.


phies? Educational Psychology Re- J., Osman, A.D., & Dikersin, K.
view, 23(4), 553-576. doi: 10.1007/ (2009). Publication bias in clini-
s10648-011-9175-6 cal trials due to statistical signifi-
*García-Madruga, J. A., Elosúa, M. R., cance or direction of trial results.
Gil, L., Gómez-Veiga, I., Vila, J. O., Cochrane Database of System-
Orjales, I., … Duque, G. (2012). atic Reviews 2009, N.º 1. doi:
Intervention on working memo- 10.1002/14651858.MR000006.pub3
ry’s executive processes to improve *Jiménez, J. E., Hernández-Valle, I.,
reading comprehension in primary Ramírez, G., Ortíz, M. d. R., Ro-
school children. Submitted for pub- drigo, M., Estévez, A., ... Trabaue,
lication. M. L. (2007). Computer speech-
*Gilabert, R. (1995). Enseñanza de es- based remediation for reading
trategias para la inferencia del sig- disabilities: The size of spelling-
nificado de las palabras. Infancia y to-sound unit in a transparent or-
Aprendizaje, 18(4), 139-152. doi: thography. The Spanish Journal of
10.1174/02103709560561203 Psychology, 10(1), 52-67.
*Gómez-Villalba, E., & Pérez, J. *Jiménez, J. E., Ortiz, M. d. R., Rod-
(2001). Animación a la lectura y rigo, M., Hernández-Valle, I., Ramí-
comprensión lectora en educación rez, G., Estevez, A., … Trabaue, M.
L. (2003). Do the effects of com-
primaria. Lenguaje y Textos, 17,
puter-assisted practice differ for
9-20.
children with reading disabilities
*González, A. (1991). Estrategias me-
with and without IQ-achievement
tacognitivas en la lectura. Madrid:
discrepancy? Journal of Learn-
Universidad Complutense.
ing Disabilities, 36(1), 34-47. doi:
*González, M. J., & Delgado, M. 10.1177/00222194030360010501
(2007). Rendimiento en lectura e in- Joseph, L. M., & Eveleigh, E. L.
tervención psicoeducativa en Edu- (2011). A review of the effects of
cación Infantil y Primaria. Revista self-monitoring on reading per-
de Educación, 344, 333-354. uri: formance of students with dis-
http://hdl.handle.net/11162/69036 abilities. The Journal of Spe-
Graham, S., & Herbert, M. (2010). cial Education, 45(1), 43-53. doi:
Writing to read. Evidence of how 10.1177/0022466909349145
writing can improve reading. Nueva *Mateos, M. d. M. (1991). Un pro-
York: Carnegie Corporation. Re- grama de instrucción en estrategias
trieved from http://carnegie.org/ de supervisión de la comprensión
fileadmin/Media/Publications/ lectora. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 56,
WritingToRead_01.pdf 61-76.
*Hernández, A. (2001). La enseñanza *Montanero, M., Blázquez, F., & León,
de estrategias de comprensión lec- J. A. (2002). Enfoques de interven-
tora y expresión escrita con los ción psicopedagógica para la mejora
alumnos de la E.S.O.: Diseño y de las capacidades de comprensión
desarrollo de tres programas de ins- en la educación secundaria. Infan-
trucción. Bordón, 53(1), 53-71. oai: cia y Aprendizaje, 25(1), 37-52. doi:
redined.mec.es:014200130668 10.1174/021037002753508511

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


42 JUAN C. RIPOLL AND GERARDO AGUADO

*Montanero, M., & González, L. *Pérez, M. (1990). Análisis experimen-


(2003). Estrategias para mejorar la tal y teórico sobre la explicación
comprensión de textos comparati- cognitiva de la comprensión de tex-
vos. Revista de Investigación Edu- tos. Psycothema, 2(2), 7-33.
cativa, 21(1), 215-230. *Ponce, H., López, M., & Mayer, R.
Mullis, I. V. S, Martin, M. O., Foy, P., E. (2012). Instructional effective-
& Drucker, K. T. (2012). PIRLS ness of a computer-supported pro-
2011 international results in read- gram for teaching reading strate-
ing. Amsterdam: International As- gies. Computers & Education,
sociation for the Evaluation of Ed- 59, 1170-1183. doi: 10.1016/j.
ucational Achievement. Retrieved compedu.2012.05.013
from http://timssandpirls.bc.edu/ *Ramos, J. A. (1998). Enseñanza de
pirls2011/downloads/P11_IR_Full- la comprensión lectora a perso-
Book.pdf nas con déficits cognitivos. Ma-
Murphy, P. K., Wilkinson, I. A. G., drid: Universidad Complutense. Re-
Soter, A. O., Hennesey, M. N., & trieved from http://eprints.ucm.es/
Alexander, J. F. (2009). Examining tesis/19972000/S/4/S4018201.pdf
the effects of classroom discussion Raudenbush, S. W. (2009). Analyzing
on students’ comprehension of text: effect sizes: random-effects models.
a metha-analysis. Journal of Educa- En H. Cooper, L. V. Hedges y J. C.
tional Psychology, 101(3), 740-764. Valentine (Eds.), The handbook of
doi: 10.1037/a0015576 research synthesis and meta-analy-
National Reading Panel (2000). Teach- sis (pp. 295-314). Nueva York: Rus-
ing children to read: an evidence- sell Sage.
based assessment of the scientific Rosenshine, B., & Meister, C. (1993).
research literature on reading and Reciprocal teaching: a review
its implications for reading instruc- 19 experimental studies. Tech-
tion. Washington DC: Author. nical Report N.º 574. Cham-
OECD (2010). PISA 2009 results: paing (Illinois): Center for the
Learning Trends: Changes in student Study of Reading. Retrieved from
performance since 2000 (vol. V). https://www.ideals.illinois.edu/
doi: 10.1787/9789264091480-en bitstream/handle/2142/17744/
*Orellana, E., & Bravo, L. (2000). In- ctrstreadtechrepv01993i00574_opt.
vestigación y experiencia de tra- pdf?sequence=1
bajo colaborativo con profe- Rosenshine, B., Meister, C., & Chap-
soras: Un seguimiento de los man, S. (1996). Teaching students to
alumnos. Estudios Pedagógicos, generate questions: A review of the
26, 79-89. doi: 10.4067/S0718- intervention studies. Review of Edu-
07052000000100006 cational Research, 66(2), 181–221.
*Pascual, G., & Goikoetxea, E. (2003). doi: 10.3102/00346543066002181
Resumen y formulación de pregun- Rosenthal, R. (1979). The “file drawer
tas: Efectos sobre la compresión problem” and tolerance for null
lectora en niños de primaria. Infan- results. Psychological Bulletin,
cia y Aprendizaje, 26(4), 439-450. 86(3), 638-641. doi: 10.1037/0033-
doi: 10.1174/021037003322553833 2909.86.3.638

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


READING COMPREHENSION IMPROVEMENT FOR SPANISH STUDENTS:
A META-ANALYSIS 43

Rothstein, H. R., Sutton, A, J., & Boren- *Soriano, M., Miranda, A., So-
stein, M. (2005). Publication bias in riano, E., Nievas, F., & Félix, V.
meta-analyisis: Prevention, assess- (2011). Examining the efficacy
ment and adjustments. Sussex: Wi- of an intervention to improve flu-
ley. doi: 10.1002/0470870168.ch1 ency and reading comprehen-
*Sánchez, E. (1989). Procedimientos sion in spanish children with read-
para instruir en la comprensión de ing disabilities. International
textos. Madrid: Ministerio de Edu- Journal of Disability, Development
cación. and Education, 58(1), 47-59. doi:
Sánchez-Meca, J., & Botella, J. (2010). 10.1080/1034912X.2011.547349
Revisiones sistemáticas y meta-aná- *Soriano, M., Vidal-Abarca, E., & Mi-
lisis: herramientas para la práctica randa, A. (1996). Comparación
profesional. Papeles del Psicólogo, de dos procedimientos de instruc-
31(1), 7-17. ción en comprensión y aprendi-
*Sanz de Acedo, M. L. (1991). Com- zaje de textos: Instrucción directa
prensión lectora y «programa enri- y enseñanza recíproca. Infancia
quecimiento instrumental». Comu- y Aprendizaje, 74(2), 57-65. doi:
nidad Educativa, 190, 15-21. 10.1174/021037096763000781
Share, D. (2008). On the anglocentrities Swanson, E., Vaughn, S., Wanzek,
of current reading research and prac- J., Petscher, Y., Heckert, J., Cav-
tice: the perils of overreliance on an anaugh, C., ... Tackett, K. (2011).
“outlier” orthography. Psychologi- A synthesis of read-aloud in-
cal Bulletin, 134(4), 584-615. doi: terventions on early reading out-
10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584 comes among preeschool through
Slavin, R., Lake, C., Chambers, B., third graders at risk for reading
Cheung, A., & Davis, S. (2009). Ef- difficulties. Journal of Learning
fective reading programs for the el- Disabilities, 44(3), 258-275. doi:
ementary grades: a best evidence 10.1177/0022219410378444
synthesis. Review of Educational Therrien, W. J. (2004). Fluency and
Research, 79(4), 1391-1466. doi: comprehension gains as a re-
10.3102/0034654309341374 sult of repeated reading. A meta-
Solis, M., Ciullo, S., Vaughn, S., analysis. Remedial and Special
Pyle, N., Hassaram, B., & Ler- Education. 25(4), 252-261. doi:
oux A. (2012). Reading compre- 10.1177/07419325040250040801
hension interventions for middle *Ugartetxea, J. X. (1996). La orienta-
school students with learning dis- ción metacognitiva. Un estudio so-
abilities: a synthesis of 30 years bre la capacidad transferencial de la
of research. Journal of Learning metacognición y su influencia en el
Disabilities, 45(4), 327-340. doi: rendimiento intelectual. Revista de
10.3102/0034654309341374 Psicodidáctica, 1, 27-53.
*Soriano, M., Chebaani, F., Soriano, E., *Vidal-Abarca, E., Gilabert, R., Ferrer,
& Descals, A. (2011). Enseñanza A., Ávila, V., Martínez, T., Mañá,
recíproca y autoobservación del A., … Serrano, M. A. (2012). Tuin-
uso de estrategias: Efectos sobre la LEC, un tutor inteligente para me-
comprensión de textos. Psicothema, jorar la competencia lectora. Sub-
23(1), 38-43. mitted for publication.

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44


44 JUAN C. RIPOLL AND GERARDO AGUADO

*Vieiro, P., Peralbo, M., & Risso, A.


(1998). Importancia del uso de es-
quemas y feedback correctivo en ta-
reas de comprensión lectora. Estu-
dios de Psicología, 60, 69-77. doi:
10.1174/02109399860341951

Juan Cruz Ripoll is a school counselor at the Santa María la Real school of Sar-
riguren, and a lecturer at the University of Navarre. His research work is about
reading comprehension enhancement and about improving academic perform-
ance of students with learning disabilities. He is author of several publications
about those subjects.

Gerardo Aguado is tenured professor at the University of Navarre and the director
of the Huarte de San Juan center. His research area is language development,
pathologies and comprehension. He is author of several publications about
those subjects.

Received date: 03-05-2013 Review date: 04-07-2013 Accepted date: 18-09-2013

Revista de Psicodidáctica, 2014, 19(1), 27-44

You might also like