You are on page 1of 9

J. technol. behav. sci.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s41347-017-0034-2

Frequency of College Student Smartphone Use: Impact


on Classroom Homework Assignments
R. Terry Furst 1 & Douglas N. Evans 1 & Nicole M. Roderick 1

# Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2017

Abstract This mixed method study considers the effect of interviews to probe the association between smartphone use
high-frequency (HF) smartphone use on homework behavior. and the initiation and completion of homework assignments.
Analyzing 393 surveys and an additional 210 semi-structured We proffer that exploring behaviors associated with high-
interviews conducted with undergraduates, this study explores frequency use (HF) and low-frequency use (LF) of
the relationship between excessive smartphone use and home- smartphones may provide insight into how smartphone use
work at a college in New York City. Bivariate analyses indicate impedes initiating and completing homework assignments.
that students who check their phone more than 39 times a day Class discussions (first author) with students suggest that ex-
report that smartphone use interferes with homework compared cessive use of smartphones may hinder starting and complet-
to those who check their phones less than 39 times a day. Semi- ing classroom assignments. Research supports this observa-
structured interviews also indicate excessive smartphone use tion (Tossell et al. 2015; David et al. 2015). Our survey also
interferes with homework. Other factors such as employment, indicates many students (76%) believe smartphones interfere
number of courses taken, amount time spent at school, family with homework. Investigators use frequency of checking
obligations, and socializing all appear to affect if and when smartphones as a means to explore the Bstate of mind^ of
homework is undertaken. Nested within this web of obligations students, internet addiction, gaming disorder, excessive social
and activities is the frequency of smartphone use, which HF media engagement, and a variety of other mobile technologies
smartphone users claim interferes with homework. This re- (Ben-Yehuda et al. 2016). Repetitive smartphone use appears
search illuminates how often students use smartphones in rela- to be a good predictor of behaviors that generate adverse con-
tion to their daily life and their class assignments. The findings sequences for mental and physical health (Demirci et al. 2014;
provide teachers, administrators, counselors, students, and other Choi et al. 2012, Lepp et al. 2013; Wilmer et al. 2017). It has
preliminary indication of how smartphones influence the initi- also been related to addiction and directly connected to home-
ation and completion of class assignments. work via the expenditure and management of time. Frequency
of use provides a basis for the distinction between HF and LF
Keywords Smartphones . Homework . College students . smartphone use categories.
Distraction . High-frequency use A relationship with smartphones and social media starts
with students in their early teenage years. Reading and
responding to text messages, texting, surfing the internet, vis-
An exploratory study of undergraduate smartphone frequency
iting social media, and posting comments can consume a
use was undertaken at a college in New York City. Using
much as Bnine to eleven hours of your day^ (Sales 2016, p.
mixed methods, we surveyed and conducted semi-structured
10). BChatting, texting, friending, blogging, vlogging^ are a
central part of adolescent behavior, providing a digital conduit
* Douglas N. Evans for emotional expression that may be rooted in the social need
devans@jjay.cuny.edu to communicate and to gain social approval among peers
(Barth 2015, p. 201). Compulsive smartphone use is well de-
1
John Jay College of Criminal Justice, 524 W. 59th Street BMW Suite veloped by the time students enter college. The magnetic lure
605.10, New York, NY 10019, USA of smartphone use may be too much to resist. It can be likened
J. technol. behav. sci.

to Ba mental itch constantly in need of being scratched^ (Wu what appeared to be her bag and a bunch of different
2016, p. 186). In this exploratory study of undergraduate stu- books open, textbooks, and notebooks. She was the spit-
dents at a 4-year college in New York City, it is not surprising ting image of a college student trying to do her home-
that high-frequency use of smartphones is extant and may be work but to my surprise, she had an iPhone gripped
associated with a failure to start and complete homework tightly as she was texting away. I had been rejected four
assignments. times that day while trying to make-up some attention
High-frequency smartphone use may be suggestive of ad- grabber, so I decided to try the least preferred route –
diction. It is associated with the completion and quality of straight up ask her for a few minutes of her time.
homework because homework requires time and focus and
smartphone use is one of many possible distractions (Reed We are confident that the in-class training adequately pre-
and Reay 2015). Multitasking—switching from one program pared student research assistants to collect survey data.
to another or texting while doing other tasks—is widespread Our methodological approach is to analytically integrate
among students and is detrimental to focusing on a single task closed-ended survey questions and semi-structured interview
(David et al. 2015). It is reasonable to assert that multitasking, questions to better understand the relationship between the
responding to notifications, texting, and seeking internet con- frequency of smartphone use and the initiation and completion
tent unrelated to homework will have a deleterious effect on of homework assignments. The collection of both qualitative
homework. To probe the association of smartphone use and and quantitative data describes different facets of smartphone
the initiation and completion of homework, considering stu- use and how it relates to starting and completing homework
dent frequency of smartphone use is a useful starting point. (Creswell 1994). We use quotes from our interviews to sup-
The answer to the following questions will help illuminate port our analysis, which subsumes a symbolic interactionist
the inquiry: Do HF smartphone users perceive that orientation. Coding was done by the first author, who sought
smartphones interfere with their work on class assignments? out themes in reference to questions raised, such as
Is HF and LF use linked to initiation or completion of home- smartphone use being responsible for ceasing homework as-
work assignments? Is working or not working associated with signments and pre-homework activities. Both methodological
HF or LF use? Does HF use relate to keeping smartphones on approaches add Bscope and breadth^ to a study (Creswell
while studying? 1994, p. 175).
The first phase of the research endeavors to capture college
student’s frequency of smartphone use and their specific uses
Methods of smartphones (social media, texting, internet surfing, etc.)
through survey responses. Close-ended survey questions tease
We used an exploratory mixed methods approach to broaden out the effects of frequency of smartphone use, number of
the analytical research lens and explore smartphone use and its hours studying, number of classes taken, amount of time de-
effect on homework (Al-Barashdi et al. 2015; Creswell 1994). voted to leisure time, time devoted to studying, concomitant
The study was approved by the college IRB and conducted by use of the phone while studying, and whether students work.
student research assistants in the first author’s class on drug These are some of the dimensions explored in the research
use and abuse, which had a field research project requirement. reported here.
The second and third authors were involved in data entry, An understanding of the initiation and completion of home-
analysis, and interpretation. Students were required to take a work assignments is a daunting research undertaking.
research with human subjects course and obtain a certificate of Comparing quantitative and qualitative data engenders a clear-
completion from the Colloborative Institutional Training er picture of factors affecting students doing homework. It
Initiative (CITI). They were trained in-class how to define a facilitates convergence of responses to both semi-structured
research problem, adopt a theoretical framework, design sur- and closed-ended questions, increasing the chance of explor-
vey questions, administer surveys, conduct interviews, and ing different facets of the phenomena (Creswell 1994).
analyze data. They were also required to write a research re- Comparisons of responses to semi-structured and closed-
port explaining their findings. Students were asked to describe ended questions enable us to expand our understanding of
in their research report the conditions under which they ob- respondents’ sentiments when queried about how they ap-
tained an interview. One student described how he obtained proach doing homework.
one of his five required interviews: Our qualitative research questions are as follows: What
kind of routines do students engage in before starting and
I saw a female sitting on the steps with an Apple com- completing homework? Are there conditions that prompt or
puter opened on her lap that was loaned from the college thwart students to start or complete homework? Do student’s
computer lab. I knew it was a loaner because it has a believe that professors inspire them to do homework? By
campus computer logo on it. On the step next to her was linking the answers to these questions and others through
J. technol. behav. sci.

qualitative and quantitative analysis, we can obtain a more 10 other students using a survey instrument. In the other clas-
detailed understanding of how smartphone use affects stu- ses, students conducted five to seven semi-structured inter-
dents’ approaches to doing homework and perhaps provide views. Students recorded and transcribed these interviews.
some insight into the compelling allure of smartphones. Students interviewed other students at different times and
locations on campus. Respondents had to own a smartphone
Survey Design to be eligible for participation. After obtaining consent, stu-
dent researchers administered either a closed-ended question-
Survey questions inquired about the number of times checking naire or a semi-structured interview. A pseudonym was
smartphones per day, which is the independent variable used assigned to each participant to maintain confidentiality.
to determine HF and LF use categories. To deepen the inquiry Respondents received verbal explanation on the nature and
into homework practices, the dimension of personal habits confidentiality of the survey or interview. The surveys took
was a point of inquiry. Other queries included number of clas- approximately 15 minutes to complete. Student researchers
ses enrolled in, amount of hours attending school, amount of entered paper responses into Survey Monkey. Semi-structured
time spent studying on weekends, number of hours studying interviews lasted approximately 15 minutes. Interviews were
on weekdays, amount of time spent on leisure including TV voice-recorded and transcribed.
watching, if students keep their smartphone on while study-
ing, whether they use smartphones while studying, if their
smartphone interferes with homework, amount of time spent Results
on social media, whether they work or not, and the number of
days they work per week. Some survey questions and re- Survey Results
sponses were informative: How many times do you check
your smartphone during the day? (mean of responses = The survey sample consisted of 867 students, including 417
39.46). Has the use of your smartphone ever interfered with males (48%) and 450 females (52%). The mean age of the
starting or completing your homework assignment? (n = 615 sample was 22 years with a range of 18 to 39 years.
or 71% responded yes). Do you have your smartphone on Demographically, the sample consisted of 217 white respon-
while studying? (n = 613 or 83% responded yes). dents (25%), 126 African-Americans (15%), 306 Hispanic
Semi-structured interviews conducted by student re- respondents (35%), 103 Asians, (12%), 75 mixed race respon-
searchers asked other students to describe their daily work dents (9%), and 40 who listed Bother^ (4%) (see Table 1).
and school routines, aiming to discern in detail students’ The survey sample was separated into four groups based on
habits and similarities in behavior before they initiate home- the number of times that respondents reported checking their
work and while working on homework. Interviewers also que- smartphones each day: high-frequency phone users (n = 176),
ried respondents about their daily activity routines before medium-high (n = 237), medium-low (n = 237), and low
starting homework, whether smartphone use interfered with (n = 217). Only HF and LF users are included in this report;
starting and completing homework, fears associated with thus, analyses only reflect these cases (n = 393). Bivariate
passing a class, whether professors inspired them to do home- analyses are based on comparisons between HF and LF users
work, and changes in their study habits since high school. Of with the following dependent variables: smartphone interfer-
equal interest are students’ descriptions of conditions under ence with homework, keeping phone on while studying, using
which they are impeded from either initiating or completing one’s phone while studying, weekday hours devoted to study,
homework. We conjecture that students engage in different amount of studying on weekends, working or not, amount of
habitual activities before starting homework (Duhigg 2012). time watching TV, amount of leisure time, and the amount of
time expended on social media. Frequencies are also included
Procedure in the results.
From the standpoint of HF use, 135 respondents (76%)
Over the course of one academic year, we conducted a two- report that smartphone use does interfere with homework
phase mixed method research study. The first author compared to 135 LF users (62%). The difference is signifi-
embarked on class research projects on smartphone use in cant, t(392) = 3.02, p = .003. There was a significant differ-
seven anthropology classes over this time. Some students en- ence between students who are HF users in that 153 (86%)
gaged in qualitative research while others students conducted report they keep their smart phones on while doing homework
quantitative research. The sequencing of the methods was not compared to 175 LF users (81%), t(392) = 3.32, p = .001. HF
intentional but rather depended on student interest. Student users (n = 132, 75%) also use their smartphones significantly
researchers selected a purposive sample of students at a col- more while doing homework compared LF users (n = 133,
lege in New York City for participation in these studies. In 61%), t(392) = 3.94, p = .001. LF users report studying sig-
some classes, students conducted face-to-face interviews with nificantly more during the week than HF users, t(392) = 2.16,
J. technol. behav. sci.

Table 1 Survey sample starting homework vary. Common activities undertaken be-
descriptives Total 394
fore starting homework, include, but are not limited to the
Age following: eating, doing chores, showering, video gaming,
Range 16–33 watching TV, texting, and cell phone talking with friends. In
Mean 21 addition, working, the number of classes taken, watching TV,
Gender presence on social media, notifications, and familial distrac-
Male 208 53% tions all play a part in student’s initiating and completing
Female 186 47% homework assignments. Semi-structured interviews suggest
Ethnicity some students develop strong study habits while others do
Caucasian 107 27% not. The following categories are derived from the semi-
Black 62 16% structured questions and from textual analyses of the tran-
Hispanic 132 34% scripts: interference of smartphone’s effect on starting and
Asian 45 11% finishing homework, daily habits preceding starting home-
Mixed 27 7% work, passing and failing classes, and changes in study habits
Other 21 5% since high school.
Number of classes enrolled in
For the qualitative portion of the study, 387 semi-structured
1–3 46 12%
interviews were conducted. However, only 210 of them had
4 152 39%
complete socio-demographic information (see Table 3). We
removed the 177 surveys with incomplete demographic data
5 160 41%
from analyses. For ease of qualitative analysis, a random num-
6–8 36 9%
ber generator is selected every fourth interview, resulting in 52
Employment status
completed interviews.
Employed 267 68%
Student acknowledgement among HF smartphone users
Unemployed 127 32%
that it interferes with homework was a pervasive theme in
Number of times per day checking
smartphone the semi-structured interviews. It was also the case in the
Low frequency (1–18) 217 55% survey where 71% (n = 615) of students reported that
High frequency 177 45%
smartphone use interferes with starting and completing home-
(55–150) work assignments. A 19-year-old white female described how
smartphone use affects her homework:

p = .032. However, there was no significant difference be- Oh it distracts me. Yeah, yeah, I always get
tween HF and LF users in terms of the amount of time they tempted to see what’s on Twitter and Instagram
study on the weekends, t(392) = 1.24, p = .218. For some HF and stuff. Well I usually just check what other
student respondents, perhaps this weekend study time is to people posted. Like what they’re doing and stuff.
compensate for weekday failure to undertake and complete I don’t post stuff often.
homework assignments. There was no significant difference
between LF and HF users in terms of the amount of TV they A 23-year-old male described how a smartphone affects his
watch per day, t(392) = 1.93, p = .055. HF and LF users report homework:
no significant differences in the amount of leisure time per
day, t(392) = −.025, p = .806 (see Table 2.). My friends calling me wanting me to come out with
From survey frequencies, when HF and LF collectively them, then I just waste a lot of time out with them and
check their smartphones they first tend to text (n = 383, when I get back I'm already tired and I don't feel like
42%), followed by social media use (n = 314, 36%), and then doing my homework. It is a big distraction, especially
email (n = 107, 12%). When time spent on social media is because if I get text messages from my friends or I start
calculated, there was no significant difference between LF and checking my social media, I get completely off track
HF users, t(392) = −1.87, p = .062 (see Table 2). It is far from from my homework.
conclusive that HF smartphone use has an impact on starting
homework, but HF use may influence completing homework. A 21-year-old African American female also pointed out
how her smartphone distracts her from homework:
Semi-Structured Interview Results
I be on Snapchat and Facebook the most. There’s always
Analyses of semi-structured interviews suggest that activities something interesting or entertaining going on and that
leading to the start of homework are highly variable. Cues for distracts me from doing my homework. Sometimes
J. technol. behav. sci.

Table 2 Comparison of low- and


high-frequency users Item Low-frequency High-frequency t tests

Users (% yes) Users (% yes) t df p

Phone interferes with homework 62% 76% 3.02 392 .003


Phone on while studying 81% 86% 3.32 392 .001
Use phone while studying 61% 75% 3.94 392 .001
Study on the weekends 57% 63% 1.24 392 .218
Do you work 66% 70% 0.66 392 .509
Mean hours Mean hours
Hours of study—weekday 2.64 2.25 2.16 392 .032
Hours of TV watching 2.03 1.73 1.93 392 .055
Hours of leisure time 3.70 3.76 −0.25 392 .806
Time spent on favorite social media 0.63 0.76 −0.25 392 .806

people call me to tell me gossip and I end up taking a A 20-year-old Hispanic female describes her failure to
longer break from my homework than I expected to. maintain her focus on completing her homework. She states:

An 18-year-old Hispanic female’s comments echo the [It is] distracting. I’m not going to lie but I would pick it
above remarks: up constantly while reading assignments, therefore mak-
ing me start from the beginning and trying to finish
I find myself using my phone as an escape when I’m without getting distracted. Something about just want-
doing my homework. When I am typing an essay and I ing to constantly look on social media or answer a text.
only have left a paragraph I use my phone. But it can be
a distraction too because I go on social media and start A 22-year-old white male expresses his frustration about
texting back my friends. not finishing his homework:

I get distracted easily. Like it’s really bad. I always end


up doing something like texting or browsing Facebook
when I’m doing homework and I never end up finishing.
Table 3 Interview
sample descriptives Total 210
In an effort to explore habitual activities that may or may not
Age affect initiation of starting homework, students were asked
Range 17–34 about their daily routines before starting homework.
Mean 21 Depending on work, home chores, and responsibilities, there
Gender are a variety of activities and times of the day that students
Male 82 39% start homework. Before starting homework, some students
Female 128 61% eat, play video games, shower, or check their smartphones.
Ethnicity Watching TV before starting homework is also a relatively
Caucasian 40 19% common activity. Going on social media and texting are also
Black 29 14% common before and after starting homework. Some students
Hispanic 93 44% establish a routine before commencing homework while
Asian 16 8% others are more arbitrary if and when they initiate homework.
Mixed 12 6% A 19-year-old Asian female described her daily routine
Other 20 9% before starting homework: BBefore starting my homework, I
Number of classes enrolled in wouldn't be able to do homework without setting up and get-
1–4 101 48% ting ready first. Does that make sense?^ Another student, a 21-
5–8 109 52% year-old Hispanic female, echoes the above sentiment about
Employment status starting homework: BBefore starting homework, I always like
Employed 151 72% to organize my room and eat before I start.^ The interviewer
Unemployed 59 28% then asked, BDo you do this every day before doing home-
work?^ The student replied:
J. technol. behav. sci.

I have never really thought about it, but now that I am never study unless I’m struggling but I never struggle.^ A 26-
thinking about it I realize that I always do that…. I don’t year-old African American was less confident about passing a
think I would be able to concentrate until I do whatever I course:
need to do beforehand. It would just stay on my mind
until I get it done. When my anxiety gets bad it’s really Yes I always do, like sometime these classes are not that
hard to concentrate on my homework. I get writer’s easy. It's very difficult to understand so I try my best to
block and it takes me hours to finish my homework. pass and to get better…. the fear of knowing that I'm
going to still pushes me to try harder so that I don't feel
A Hispanic male, age 20, explained his arbitrary approach [anxious].
to starting homework:
We thought that the influence of professors may prompt stu-
It changes almost every day. I have other obligations and dents to do homework. However, a majority of students do not
things to do. So I basically just do it whenever I get a believe their professors inspire them to do homework. A 19-
chance. I might do some before my classes or in the year-old Hispanic male student told why his professors do not
night or like when I have a break during the day. inspire him to do homework:

A Hispanic female, age 22, describes her approach to No, not really, only if they bore me. I hate when profes-
starting homework. She notes: sors got that monotone voice, shit puts me to sleep …
They don’t inspire me to do much of anything. I could
It’s extremely random, depends what I get caught up care less though… because I have a big ego honestly,
doing. No [it is] never the same. I work crazy hours size of a dinosaur. My ego doesn’t have anything to do
and especially during the fall holiday hours conflict a with homework. If I’m going to do it I’m gonna do it. If
lot with the availability and flexibility. I have to do I’m not gonna do it I’m not gonna do it.
things.
At the other end of the spectrum of responses, a 24-year-old
Interviews suggest that some students have daily routines for white student believes her professors inspire her to do home-
starting homework while others appear not to. Working a job, work. She states:
the number of courses taken, the amount of time spent at
school, family obligations, and socializing all appear to affect I guess some do. Some… mmm… like if it’s a
when and if homework is undertaken. Nested within this com- very… like if it’s a class where I feel like I have
plex web of activities is the frequency of smartphone use. to do the reading or I’m going to be left out be-
According to student responses, HF smartphone use appears hind you know I’m going to do the reading. Or if
to interfere with homework. there’s quizzes on the reading, I’m probably going
Responses to questions about passing classes elicited a to do them more often.
range of sentiments about student’s academic confidence in
passing classes and their ability to do homework. At one end Interviews suggest that a minority of students have not
of the spectrum, students express confidence and even over- changed their study habits from high school to college, while
confidence in passing classes. At the other end of the contin- a majority has changed their habits. A female Hispanic stu-
uum students express anxiety about their abilities to pass dent, age 22, described how her study habits have changed
courses. They were asked if their professors inspire them to since high school: BI did change my study habits… cause now
do homework. A majority said they did not. This is troubling I actually have to study and in high school I didn’t really have
but understandable given the abhorrence of homework assign- to study, it was easy.^ A 23-year-old female Hispanic echoed a
ments and competing domains for student’s attention, and similar sentiment:
their involvement in diverse activities, including but not lim-
ited to working, Bhanging out^ with friends, playing video Yes, maybe a little bit. I am definitely more focused now
games, watching TV, listening to music, household and famil- than I was in high school because I am in college at this
ial chores, and social media. Concomitant with this phenom- point. I have to take it more serious because I pay for
enon is that the majority of students said when they were in school.
high school, they thought that doing homework was not im-
portant. However, they generally believe that undergraduate An 18-year-old white female noted that her study habits
work demands more attention. A 21-year-old white male stu- have not changed: BIt stayed the same I think. Yeah, I follow
dent expressed his confidence in passing courses: BNo [fear]. the same routine mostly. Only difference is I would do home-
If I never fail then that means what I’m doing is working. I work in school mostly because I got home later.^
J. technol. behav. sci.

A 19-year-old male Hispanic pointed out that his lack of image, or good news in the form of messages or Blikes.^
study habits has not changed: BI never studied in high school. I Alternatively, the lack of contact or new material is the ab-
was kinda just there, and now in college I haven’t changed.^ A sence of a reward (Stafford 2006, p. 2). From a similar per-
23-year-old African American male related how he has spective, Duhigg (Duhigg 2012, p. 19–20) argues persuasive-
changed his study habits: ly that individuals develop habits through a behavioral Bloop.^
The loop consists of a cue, routine, and reward (see Wood and
Um, time management for the most part, I usually find a Neal 2007). He points out that over time this behavioral pat-
specific time to do my homework, a specific place to do tern Bbecomes more and more automatic^ and notes that
it and just be a little more responsible than I was a few Bhabits are not destiny….Habits can be ignored, changed, or
years ago. Because I want to pass this class so I can take replaced.^
the class I need to take, and you know from there get a It is likely that HF checking of smartphones may impact the
degree, so I can get a job, get a lot of money and be amount of time and attention students devote to starting or
happy. finishing homework. Related to this phenomenon is multitask-
ing, or in this instance, using a smartphone while studying.
Frequency of smartphone use was not specifically queried in Multi-tasking, particularly texting, deflects the boredom of
the semi-structured interviews, but nevertheless it is plausible homework and can be emotionally and socially satisfying
that HF use is related to initiating and completing homework. (David et al. 2015). The conundrum of the apparent need to
be continually digitally connected while being responsible for
the completion of homework is an ongoing phenomenon
Discussion/Conclusion among students (Levine et al. 2007). Yet, research has indi-
cated that there is a negative relationship between multitask-
Given the increasing prevalence of smartphone use, many ing and academic performance. A study of this topic focused
students succumb to an immersion in the digital world, regard- on student’s Bability to learn from typical university lectures…
less of their awareness of negative consequences of not initi- and emailing, MSN messaging…Facebook use via computer
ating or completing homework (Schwartz 2015). A survey were all investigated as well.^ Findings indicate that students
across the US found that 52% of smartphone users check their who engaged in multitasking Bwith any of the technologies
phones Ba few times an hour^ (Newport 2015). Students in was associated with lower scores on following tests compared
this study report on average check their smartphones nearly 40 to those that did not multitask^ (Wood et al. 2012).
times per day, and when they access social media, they spend Workplace demands for greater productivity have grown
almost 50 minutes on social media app(s) and/or site(s). over the last several decades and they have affected the allo-
Earlier the question was raised: Do students have a need to cation of people’s time (Schor 1991). As students attempt to
continuously access their devices? It seems that they do ac- balance work, school, and their social life, they appear to
cording to the number of times each day that students check resort to compressing time needed for these activities by mul-
their smartphones as well as their propensity to spend consid- titasking. The idea that the ubiquity of multitasking is a
erable time on social media. A cognitive scientist argues that Bgenerational thing^ is not entirely accurate. In bygone days,
in the behavioral training of animals, if behavior is not it was not unusual for students to listen to radio or watch TV
rewarded it will gradually disappear. Operant conditioning while attempting to do homework. Today, numerous electron-
experiments with pigeons and mice indicate that if a behavior ic and digital capabilities of smartphones greatly facilitate
is required, the method to ensure consistency of that behavior navigating different social media websites and applications,
is to give the animal a reward only sometimes and at random such as Twitter, Facebook, and Instagram, while trying to
intervals, periodically withhold a reward, then re-introduce the complete classroom assignments. We found these distractions
reward. Thus, Bonce all rewards for the behavior are re- through our survey results, which indicated that 28% of stu-
moved… the animals work harder to obtain the reward and dents watch TV while doing homework and 83% keep their
take longer to give up the behavior^ (Stafford 2006, p. 2). The smartphones on while working on homework.
logic is that the animal should not get used to performing the Frequency of smartphone use matters. Students who are HF
behavior and receiving a reward; otherwise, it will stop users report that they keep their smartphones on more while
performing the behavior as soon as the reward ceases. doing homework compared to LF users, suggesting the possi-
Occasionally, the animal’s effort should produce a reward so bility that HF users are more likely to be distracted from com-
that it is unaware when the reward will appear. Intermittent pleting their homework assignments. Associations among these
rewards reinforce the behavior, establishing a repetitive be- variables are significant. Analyses of open-ended interviews
havioral process. In humans, there is a similar phenomenon: confirm this. Interestingly, HF users do not study less on week-
repetitive checking of smartphones and emails, knowing that ends and do not engage in more TV watching than LF users. For
sometimes we may be rewarded with an amusing web link, some students, perhaps this compensates for weekday failure to
J. technol. behav. sci.

undertake and complete assignments. HF smartphone users may It was thought that students may be inspired by their pro-
also be yielding to FOMO (fear of missing out) to their focus on fessors to undertake homework assignments. Semi-structured
maintaining social relationships through a continual presence on interview responses suggest that most do not believe profes-
social media (Sales 2016, p. 31–32). sors inspire them to do homework. The reasons for this are not
A grasp of how college students use smartphones and how clear but may be related to how confident students are at
use relates to homework has to take into consideration how passing their classes. Perhaps student overconfidence breeds
students organize their day and whether they work or not. In a belief that undertaking homework assignments is not essen-
both cases, managing time fundamentally relates to initiating tial to passing a course, nor is it vital to an overall understand-
and completing homework assignments. Most students ing of the elements of the course.
(n = 584, 67%) work an average of 25 h a week and at least Results of both survey and semi-structured interviews in-
4 days a week. This work commitment may also affect under- dicate that HF smartphone use detracts from the completion of
taking homework. homework. It is less clear if HF and LF smartphone use affects
The issues of student’s time management, while relevant to initiating homework. Initiating homework is affected by the
the frequency of smartphone use and the initiation and com- amount of hours students work, lifestyle activities, family re-
pletion homework, is also well beyond the scope of the pres- sponsibilities, homework habits, and other unidentified fac-
ent study. It deserves a separate treatment (see Brigitte and tors. Nevertheless, it appears that HF smartphone use during
Christel 2007 for a review of the literature). Similarly, we homework interferes with the task.
did not specifically explore the notion of procrastination. The quest for a fundamental explanation of the relationship
Notwithstanding, student comments suggest that procrastina- between students who do not initiate homework and high-
tion may be another culprit for not undertaking homework. By frequency smartphones use is beyond the grasp of the present
procrastination, we mean intentionally putting off doing study. In addition, the multitude of factors underlying phe-
homework with the understanding that there could be negative nomena such as self-regulation, time management, motiva-
consequences. It is a way of satisfying short-term pleasures for tion, and procrastination are also beyond the purview of the
the possibility of future hardship (Wang 2015). Semi- study. Nevertheless, HF smartphone use appears to affect the
structured interviews suggest that some students establish completion of homework. The reasons why students fail to
weekday habits that precede and trigger starting homework. initiate homework are challenges for other researchers to in-
Showering, eating, watching TV, doing chores, playing video vestigate and perhaps develop a comprehensive elucidation of
games, and engaging in smartphone use are commonly report- the homework enigma. Future researchers should more fully
ed activities. These activities may indicate habits that students investigate the ability of smartphones to diminish cognition
use to consistently initiate homework. and human intelligence (Carr 2017). Research should also
The question rose earlier: Is high-frequency smartphone consider addictive aspects of smartphone use and attempt to
use indicative of behavioral addiction to smartphones among define and deconstruct addiction to smartphones. In the mean-
students? The simple answer is yes. Yet, it is different than time, this research illuminates aspects of how students use
addiction to psychoactive substances, where there is an over- smartphones in relation to their daily life and their class as-
whelming physical and psychological need to relieve with- signments. The findings will provide teachers, administrators,
drawal symptoms (Lindesmith 1968). Both addictions compel counselors, students, and others preliminary indication of how
and induce the individual to repeat activities despite adverse smartphones influence the initiation and completion of class
consequences (Ben-Yehuda et al. 2016; Choliz 2010). It is assignments.
clear that addiction to psychoactive substances is problematic,
but is HF use of smartphones problematic for students?
Students acknowledge that it Binterferes^ with homework. Limitations
From this standpoint, HF smartphone use constitutes an ele-
ment of behavioral addiction: disruption of necessary daily A primary limitation of this study is that it is not based on a
activities. random sample. Therefore, it is not representative of the pop-
Semi-structured interviews indicate the majority of stu- ulation of college students. Initially, we utilized a mixed meth-
dents have changed their study habits from high school. od approach to conduct a survey that would deductively in-
Many students realize that college-level courses are more de- form the qualitative phase of the project (Creswell 1994).
manding than high school courses, but this does not necessar- Ideally, the construction of the qualitative questions was to
ily mean they are diligent about doing homework. Interviews be based on the results of the survey. This plan faltered due
also suggest that students who do their homework at approx- to a lack of resources. Unfortunately, the frequency tabulation
imately the same time each weekday appear to be less distract- and analysis of the survey was not completed in time to inform
ed from initiating homework by meeting with friends, keeping the construction of the qualitative questions. This resulted in a
their smartphone on, or texting during homework. missed opportunity to better analytically integrate and
J. technol. behav. sci.

converge deductive and inductive domains of the study. The addiction scale in a younger population. Bulletin of Clinical
Psychopharmacology, 24(3), 226–234.
focus, for the most part, has been on the analysis of frequency
Duhigg, C. (2012). The power of habit: Why we do what we do in life and
of smartphone use as an independent variable. This emphasis business. New York: Random House.
may have biased the analysis of semi-structured interviews. It Lepp, A., Barkley, J. E., Sanders, G. J., Rebold, M., & Gates, P. (2013). The
should be noted that frequency of use is not equivalent to relationship between cell phone use, physical and sedentary activity,
and cardiorespiratory fitness in a sample of US college students.
digital engagement, such as texting, telephonic conversations,
International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity,
accessing social media, internet browsing, and responding to 10(1), 79 Retrieved October 11, 2017 from https://ijbnpa.
notifications. Frequency, or the number of times that someone biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1479-5868-10-79.
accesses his or her phone, can simply mean momentary Levine, L. E., Waite, B. M., & Bowman, L. L. (2007). Electronic media
use, reading, and academic distractibility in college youth. Cyber
checking of one’s phone rather than a more extensive digital
Psychology & Behavior, 10(4), 560–566.
immersion. Thus, it can be a misleading metric unless it is Lindesmith, A. R. (1968). Addiction and opiates. Chicago: Aldine
supported by other explanatory variables. Publishing Company.
Newport, F. (2015). Most U.S. smartphone owners check phone at least
Acknowledgements We are deeply grateful and thank the student re- hourly. Retrieved May 1, 2017 from http://www.gallup.com/poll/
searchers and all their fellow student interviewees who generously 184046/smartphone-owners-check-phoneleast-hourly.aspx
granted interviews. Without their diligent efforts, this study would not Reed, P., & Reay, E. (2015). Relationship between levels of problematic
have been possible. internet usage and motivation to study in university students. Higher
Education, 70(4), 711–723.
Sales, N. J. (2016). American girls: Social media and the secret lives of
teenagers. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
References Schor, J. (1991). The overworked American: The unexpected decline of
leisure. New York: Basic Books.
Schwartz, T. (2015). Addicted to distraction: The story of how I lost, and
Al-Barashdi, H. S., Bouazza, A., & Jabur, N. H. (2015). Smartphone
found, my focus. New York Times. Retrieved October 18, 2017 from
addiction among university undergraduates: a literature review.
https://www.nytimes.com/2015/11/29/opinion/sunday/addicted-to-
Journal of Scientific Research & Reports, 4(3), 210–225.
distraction.html
Barth, F. D. (2015). Social media and adolescent development: hazards,
Stafford, T. (2006). Why email is addictive (and what to do about it).
pitfalls and opportunities for growth. Clinical Social Work Journal,
Neuroscience and Psychology News and Views. Retrieved May 1,
43(2), 201–208.
2017 from https://mindhacks.com/2006/09/19/why-email-is-
Ben-Yehuda, L., Greenberg, L., & Weinstein, A. (2016). Internet addic- addictive-and-what-to-do-about-it/
tion by using the smartphone-relationships between internet addic- Tossell, C. C., Kortum, P., Shepard, C., Rahmati, A., & Zhong, L. (2015).
tion, frequency of smartphone use and the state of mind of male and You can lead a horse to water but you cannot make him learn:
female students. Journal of Reward Deficiency Syndrome and smartphone use in higher education. British Journal of
Addiction Science, 2(1), 22–27. Educational Technology, 46(4), 713–724.
Brigitte, J., & Christel, G. (2007). Time management. Personnel Review, Wang, S. S. (2015). Never procrastinate again: new research sheds light
36(2), 255–276. on the emotions behind our daily tactics and how we can tackle
Carr, N. (2017). How smartphones hijack our minds. The Wall Street them. Wall Street Journal. Retrieved October 18, 2017 from
Journal. Retrieved October 18, 2017 from https://www.wsj.com/ https://www.wsj.com/articles/to-stop-procrastinating-start-by-
articles/how-smartphones-hijack-our-minds-1507307811 understanding-whats-really-going-on-1441043167
Choi, H. S., Lee, H. K., & Ha, J. C. (2012). The influence of smartphone Wilmer, H. H., Sherman, L.E.,& Chein, J.M. (2017). Smartphones and
addiction on mental health, campus life and personal relations: fo- cognition: A review of researchexploring the links between mobile
cusing on K university students. Journal of the Korean Data and technology habits and cognitive functioning. Frontiers in
Information Science Society, 23(5), 1005–1015. Psychology, 8(605), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.
Choliz, M. (2010). Mobile phone addiction: a point of issue. Addiction, 00605.
105(2), 373–374. Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2007). A new look at habits and the habit-goal
Creswell, J. W. (1994). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and interface. Psychological Review, 114(4), 843.
mixed method. London: Sage Publications. Wood, E., Zivcakova, L., Gentile, P., Archer, K., De Pasquale, D., &
David, P., Kim, J. H., Brickman, J. S., Ran, W., & Curtis, C. M. (2015). Nosko, A. (2012). Examining the impact of off-task multi-tasking
Mobile phone distraction while studying. New Media & Society, with technology on real-time classroom learning. Computers &
17(10), 1661–1679. Education, 58(1), 365–374.
Demirci, K., Orhan, H., Demirdas, A., Akpinar, A., & Sert, H. (2014). Wu, T. (2016). The attention merchants: The epic scramble to get inside
Validity and reliability of the Turkish version of the smartphone our heads. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

You might also like