You are on page 1of 7

’Original article

Cognitive assessment of patients with nonalcoholic


fatty liver disease
Asuman Celikbileka,*, Mehmet Celikbilekb,* and Gurbet Bozkurtc

Objective The aim of this study was to investigate cognitive performance for the first time in participants with nonalcoholic fatty
liver disease (NAFLD) using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA).
Participants and methods In total, 70 participants with NAFLD and 73 age-matched and sex-matched healthy participants
were enrolled in this prospective cross-sectional study. The diagnosis of NAFLD was made on the basis of abdominal
ultrasonography findings. Anthropometric indices were calculated, and routine laboratory analyses were carried out for each
participant. All participants provided sociodemographic data and completed the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Cognitive
functions were evaluated using the Turkish version of the MoCA, with a cut-off score for mild cognitive impairment of less than 21
points.
Results The MoCA scores were significantly lower in participants with NAFLD than in the healthy group (P < 0.05). In addition,
more NAFLD participants than healthy participants presented with deficits in the visuospatial (P < 0.05) and executive function
domains (P < 0.05). In the multivariate model, education level [2.79 (1.12–6.96); P < 0.05] and area of residence [5.68
(2.24–14.38); P < 0.001] were associated independently with cognitive dysfunction in both the NAFLD and the healthy groups.
The MoCA scores were correlated negatively with fibrosis 4 scores in NAFLD participants (r = − 0.359; P < 0.05). However,
hepatosteatosis grade and the presence of metabolic syndrome were not correlated with MoCA scores in the NAFLD group
(P > 0.05).
Conclusion Our results show that NAFLD patients may have early or subtle cognitive dysfunction, including in the visuospatial
and executive function domains, as indexed by scores on the MoCA test. Further targeted psychometric testing will be required
to confirm the presence of cognitive impairment in this population. Eur J Gastroenterol Hepatol 00:000–000
Copyright © 2018 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.

Introduction hyperglycemia and/or hypertension, may influence both


the structure and the function of small blood vessels,
Metabolic syndrome (MetS) refers to a cluster of condi-
potentially causing alterations in cerebral blood flow and,
tions, including obesity, hypertension, dyslipidemia, and
by extension, disruption of blood–brain barrier integrity
insulin resistance. There is general agreement that the
and function [9,10]. Certain cytokines, such as interleukin
prevalence of MetS is increasing, primarily because of
1 (IL-1), IL-6, and tumor necrosis factor-α (TNF-α), were
lifestyle factors and the well-documented increase in obe-
associated with cognitive decline in previous studies [11].
sity levels [1]. Cumulative evidence from different
In particular, TNF-α and IL-6 are known to be present in
population-based studies suggests that MetS and its com-
specific brain areas, including the hypothalamus and the
ponents have a negative effect on cognition [2–5]. In
hippocampus, and are both involved in neurogenesis,
numerous recent studies, obesity, diabetes, and/or hyper-
synaptic transmission, and plasticity [11]. Higher levels of
tension were associated with cognitive impairment, ran-
these cytokines might contribute toward the development
ging from mild cognitive changes to dementia [6–8].
of neuronal dysfunction and death, as well as memory
However, the underlying mechanism linking MetS and its
impairments [11,12]. Another factor in cognitive decline
components to cognitive impairment is not fully under-
may be the neurotrophin, brain-derived neurotrophic fac-
stood. One possibility is that microcirculatory changes,
tor, which is produced by hypothalamic neuronal cells and
mainly because of metabolic imbalances manifested in
regulates food intake and body weight in experimental
European Journal of Gastroenterology & Hepatology 2018, 00:000–000
animals and humans [13]. Brain-derived neurotrophic
factor, which is mainly present in the hippocampus and the
Keywords: cognitive dysfunction, executive functioning, Montreal Cognitive
Assessment, nonalcoholic fatty liver disease, visuospatial domain cortex (both of which are critically involved in cognition),
Departments of aNeurology, bGastroenterology and cPsychology, Bozok
plays a crucial role in the neuroplasticity involved in
University, School of Medicine, Yozgat, Turkey learning and memory, especially hippocampal-dependent
Correspondence to Asuman Celikbilek, MD, Department of Neurology, School of learning, as confirmed by animal studies [14,15].
Medicine, Bozok University, Yozgat 66200, Turkey MetS and its components are highly prevalent in par-
Tel: + 90 505 653 2615; fax: + 90 354 217 1072; ticipants with nonalcoholic fatty liver disease (NAFLD),
e-mail: asunebioglu@yahoo.com which is now considered a precursor to MetS [16].
*Asuman Celikbilek and Mehmet Celikbilek both contributed equally to the writing NAFLD is a chronic liver disease that affects approxi-
of this article. mately one-third of the world’s population and 30% of the
Received 23 February 2018 Accepted 10 March 2018 US population [17]. NAFLD is mainly induced by the

0954-691X Copyright © 2018 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. DOI: 10.1097/MEG.0000000000001131 1

Copyright r 2018 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
2 European Journal of Gastroenterology & Hepatology Month 2018 • Volume 00 • Number 00

consumption of a high-fat diet and can range from simple Participants and methods
steatosis (fatty liver) or steatohepatitis (fatty liver with cell
Study population
injury and inflammation) to hepatic fibrosis or cirrhosis
[18]. In recent years, liver dysfunction has been shown to A total of 70 consecutive participants with NAFLD,
affect cognitive abilities negatively [19]. Animal studies recruited from a Gastroenterology Outpatient Clinic, and
have shown that a high-fructose diet impairs hippocampal- 73 age-matched and sex-matched healthy participants
dependent memory in rats [20,21]. This finding is impor- from the outpatient clinics and hospital staff, ranging from
tant because the hippocampus is integral to many types of 18 to 70 years old, were enrolled in this prospective cross-
learning and memory [22], and a high-fructose diet, by sectional study. The study was carried out at Bozok
increases in triglycerides, impairs memory by causing University Medical Faculty Hospital in the Yozgat region
hippocampal insulin resistance [20,21,23]. In a recent (a rural area of Turkey) between September 2013 and
experimental study, Ghareeb et al. [24] reported that September 2014. Participants with malignancies, chronic
NAFLD induced disturbances in neurotransmitter activ- renal, hepatic, or cardiovascular disease, thyroid disease, a
ities, accompanied by oxidative stress and metabolic dis- history of dementia, psychiatric and/or central nervous
orders, thereby serving as a risk factor for brain system disorders (such as traumatic brain injury, multiple
dysfunction and brain tissue damage in NAFLD rats. More sclerosis, etc.), pregnancy, or morbid obesity, and those
recently, Kim et al. [25] showed that NAFLD-related who were current smokers or used drugs known to cause
chronic inflammation outside of the brain can induce a fatty liver, or who had vitamin B12 or folate deficiency
secondary increase in neuroinflammation and neurode- and/or memory problems in daily life, were excluded from
generation in mice, in turn leading to neuronal death in the study. Participants with a history of alcohol con-
cortical and hippocampal areas in the absence of any sumption, bile duct dilatation, hepatic mass, hepatitis,
genetic predisposition. Moreover, removal of mice from a viral or autoimmune liver disease, liver cirrhosis, or
high-fat diet regimen during acute NAFLD reversed liver hepatic surgery were also excluded.
pathology and decreased signs of neuroinflammatory and
neurodegenerative changes [25]. However, clinical data on Data collection
cognitive functioning in NAFLD are limited. A study by Comprehensive sociodemographic and clinical data were
Elliott et al. [26] examined the cognitive symptoms of collected, including demographic characteristics (age and
NAFLD by a cognitive failure questionnaire, which sex), comorbid conditions (diabetes mellitus, hypertension,
included items pertaining to memory, attention, con- and/or hyperlipidemia), marital status (single, married, or
centration, forgetfulness, word-finding ability, and confu- divorced), education level (illiterate, primary school, sec-
sion in 224 participants with biopsy-proven NAFLD. They ondary school, high school, or university), and area of
observed that increased cognitive difficulties were asso- residence (village, town, or city). All participants com-
ciated independently with functional difficulties in the pleted the Beck Depression Inventory-II; those with a score
NAFLD group compared with a healthy control group. In of 14 or higher were excluded from the study [33].
addition, a recent cross-sectional study by Seo et al. [27] Systolic blood pressure and diastolic blood pressure
examined the association between NAFLD and cognitive were obtained from two independent measurements. Waist
impairment using computer-based tests of attention and circumference was measured for each participant and BMI
reaction times. Their study included 4472 middle-aged was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in
adults drawn from the Third National Health and meters squared [34]. MetS was diagnosed according to the
Nutritional Examination Survey of Korea [27]. They National Cholesterol Education Program Adult Treatment
concluded that NAFLD was associated independently with Panel III criteria [35].
reduced cognitive performance, independent of cardio- Using standard laboratory methods, venous blood
vascular disease and its risk factors. In the present study, samples were obtained from the antecubital vein after an
we hypothesized that NAFLD, which is closely related to overnight fast for the measurement of fasting glucose,
MetS, may also show a relationship with cognitive creatinine, renal and liver function, complete blood count,
impairment. Although we did not study the candidate and lipid profile. The study protocol was approved by the
biomarkers of the biological mechanisms underlying this local research ethics committee of Bozok University.
association, we speculate that impaired vasculature Written informed consent was obtained from all
because of metabolic dysfunction, with a coincident participants.
proinflammatory state, may induce neuroinflammatory
cytokines, thereby promoting neuronal damage in brain Nonalcoholic fatty liver disease assessment
regions critical to cognition. To date, the Mini–Mental NAFLD was diagnosed by abdominal ultrasonography
State Examination (MMSE) has been used most commonly (US), which is a widely accessible imaging technique with
to index cognitive impairment in metabolic disorders [28]. high diagnostic accuracy and reliability with respect to
However, the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), fatty liver [36]. All US examinations were performed by
which is more sensitive and specific for mild cognitive the same experienced radiologist, who was blinded to the
impairment (MCI) [29], was used in some studies to research design, using a commercially available US scanner
demonstrate cognitive impairment in MetS, diabetes, and (Alpha 6; Japan Medical Systems, Tokyo, Japan) equipped
chronic atrial fibrillation [30–32]. However, no previous with a 2.0–5.0 MHz convex probe. The diagnosis of
study has applied the MoCA to an NAFLD cohort. NAFLD was made on the basis of increased echogenicity
Therefore, in this study, we investigated cognitive perfor- on US that was compatible with fatty infiltration of the
mance in participants with NAFLD using the MoCA. liver with or without elevated alanine transaminase (ALT)

Copyright r 2018 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Cognitive assessment of NAFLD Celikbilek et al. www.eurojgh.com 3

levels [37]. Hepatosteatosis was graded on US as mild, size calculations for the primary analysis are given as
moderate, or severe as described previously [38]. Of the Cohen’s d or φ coefficient. We used Pearson’s correlations
participants with NAFLD, 24 (34.3%) had mild, 41 to examine the relationships between MoCA scores and the
(58.6%) had moderate, and five (7.1%) had severe hepa- markers of NAFLD severity, such as FIB4 and hepatostea-
tosteatosis. The fibrosis 4 (FIB4) index (on the basis of age, tosis grade, and the presence of MetS in participants with
aspartate transaminase and ALT levels, and platelet count) NAFLD. Risk factors postulated to be linked to poorer
is used to evaluate hepatic fibrosis. The FIB4 score was cognition were included in a univariate analysis. Differences
calculated as follows: [age (years) × aspartate transaminase were considered statistically significant at a P value of less
(IU/l)]/[platelet count (109/l) × ALT (IU/l)]1/2 [39]. than 0.05 in univariate logistic regression analysis and
backward stepwise elimination was performed using the
Cognitive function assessment Wald statistic at the P value of less than 0.05 level to
determine independent predictors of cognitive performance.
One neuropsychologist, blinded to group identity, used the
Covariates of diabetes mellitus, hypertension, hyperlipi-
MoCA to evaluate cognitive functions. Six domains of
demia, and MetS were coded as dichotomous variables
cognition were assessed: visuospatial abilities, memory,
(yes/no). Marital status was divided into two categories:
executive functioning, attention, language, and orienta-
single or divorced (group 1) and married (group 2).
tion. In the MoCA, visuospatial abilities are evaluated by a
Education level was also divided into two categories: less
clock-drawing task (three points) and a three-dimensional
than 8 years of education (group 1) and more than
cube-copying task (one point). The memory assessment
8 years of education (group 2). Area of residence was
involves delayed recall (∼5 min) of five nouns subsequent
classified as village or town (group 1) and city (group 2).
to two learning trials (five points). Executive functioning is
For each factor, odds ratios were calculated with 95%
assessed using an alternation task adapted from the Trail
confidence intervals. Analyses were carried out using the
Making B task (one point), a phonemic fluency task (one
SPSS software (version 15.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, Illinois,
point), and a two-item verbal abstraction task (two
USA). Statistical significance was set at a P value of less
points). Attention is evaluated using a sustained attention
than 0.05.
task (target detection indicated by tapping; one point), a
serial subtraction task (three points), and digits forward
and backward tasks (one point each). Language is assessed Results
using a three-item confrontation naming task featuring
The demographic and laboratory data of the healthy and
low-familiarity animals (lion, camel, rhinoceros; three
NAFLD groups are summarized in Table 1. No significant
points), repetition of two syntactically complex sentences
differences were found between the two groups in age or
(two points), and the aforementioned fluency task.
sex (P > 0.05), and no significant associations were
Orientation to time and place is also evaluated (six points).
observed in BMI, waist circumference, systolic blood
Total scores range from 0 to 30, with a score less than 26
pressure, and diastolic blood pressure between these
indicating MCI in the original version of the MoCA [29].
groups (P > 0.05). However, fasting glucose (P < 0.05),
However, we used the Turkish version of the MoCA
liver enzyme (P < 0.001), and triglyceride levels (P < 0.05)
(MoCA-TR), which has a cut-off score for MCI of less
were significantly higher, whereas total bilirubin (P < 0.05)
than 21 points [40]. The MoCA-TR features certain cul-
and high-density lipoprotein cholesterol (P < 0.001) levels
tural and linguistic changes from the original version, and
were significantly lower in participants with NAFLD
shows high sensitivity and specificity for detecting MCI in
compared with those in the healthy group (Table 1).
patients who perform normally on the MMSE-TR. Total
Comorbidities, such as diabetes mellitus and hypertension,
scores on the MoCA-TR range from 0 to 30, with higher
were also significantly more prevalent in participants with
scores indicating better cognition and scores below 21
NAFLD than in the healthy group, but the incidence of
indicating cognitive dysfunction [40,41]. The highest
hyperlipidemia and the presence of MetS were similar
possible scores on the cognitive domains of visuospatial
between the groups (P > 0.05). The two groups were also
abilities, memory, executive functioning, attention, lan-
similar with respect to marital status, education level, and
guage, and orientation are 4, 5, 4, 6, 5, and 6, respectively.
area of residence (P > 0.05; Table 1). Table 2 shows that
Participants scoring below a certain threshold (< 4 points
the total MoCA scores were normally distributed and were
for visuospatial abilities, <5 points for memory, <4 points
significantly lower in the NAFLD group than in the heal-
for executive functioning, <6 points for attention, <5
thy group (P < 0.001; Fig. 1). We observed deficits in each
points for language, and <6 points for orientation) in any
cognitive domain for some participants. Furthermore,
of these domains are classified as having deficits, which
compared with the controls, a greater number of NAFLD
may indicate cognitive dysfunction.
participants presented with deficits in the visuospatial
(P < 0.05) and executive functioning (P < 0.05) domains
Statistical analysis
(Table 2).
A Shapiro–Wilks test, histograms, and q-q plots were used In the multivariate model, education level [2.79
to test the normality of the data, and Levene’s test was (1.12–6.96); P < 0.05] and area of residence [5.68
used to assess homogeneity of variance. Independent- (2.24–14.38); P < 0.001] were associated independently
sample t-tests and Mann–Whitney U tests were used to with cognitive dysfunction in both groups (odds ratios,
compare group differences in continuous variables, and 95% confidence intervals; Table 3). Dysfunction was more
χ2 tests were used to assess differences in categorical vari- likely in those reporting lower education levels (including
ables. Values are expressed as frequencies and percentages, primary or secondary school education) and in those living
mean and SD, or medians and interquartile ranges. Effect in a rural area (village or town).

Copyright r 2018 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
4 European Journal of Gastroenterology & Hepatology Month 2018 • Volume 00 • Number 00

Table 1. Demographic and laboratory data of the healthy and nonalcoholic fatty liver disease groups
Variables Healthy (n = 73) NAFLD (n = 70) P Effect size
Sociodemographic variables
Age (years) 43.9 ± 9.3 46.9 ± 10.1 0.067 − 0.30
Female sex 49 (67.1) 41 (58.6) 0.290 0.08
BMI (kg/m2) 34.93 (30.83–36.84) 35.02 (30.1–37.7) 0.682 − 0.07
WC (cm) 105 (96–112) 103 (95–110) 0.165 0.27
SBP (mmHg) 130 (120–140) 130 (120–140) 0.815 − 0.07
DBP (mmHg) 80 (80–90) 80 (80–90) 0.468 − 0.21
Marital status
Single 4 (5.5) 2 (2.8) 0.741 0.03
Married 69 (94.5) 67 (95.7)
Divorced 0 (0) 1 (1.4)
Education level
Illiterate 0 (0) 5 (7.1) 0.726 0.03
Primary school 38 (52.1) 38 (54.3)
Secondary school 13 (17.8) 4 (5.7)
High school 12 (16.4) 14 (20)
University 10 (13.7) 9 (12.9)
Area of residence
Village 6 (8.2) 6 (8.6) 0.219 0.10
Town 20 (27.4) 26 (37.1)
City 47 (64.4) 38 (54.3)
Laboratory markers
Fasting glucose (mg/dl) 92.1 ± 8.2 95.6 ± 12.7 0.046 − 0.34
Creatinine (mg/dl) 0.79 (0.71–0.87) 0.80 ± 0.70–0.89 0.629 −0
WBC (103/mm3) 7.82 (7.07–8.82) 7.81 (6.76–9.34) 0.787 − 0.17
Hemoglobin (mg/dl) 13.4 (12.6–15.1) 14.3 (12.9–15.3) 0.073 0.12
Platelet (103/mm3) 243 (212.5–275) 244.5 (200–295.7) 0.958 − 0.02
AST (IU/l) 17 (14–21.5) 22.5 (17–31.2) < 0.001 − 0.76
ALT (IU/l) 15 (12–23) 25.5 (17–39.5) < 0.001 − 0.91
GGT (IU/l) 17 (12–23.5) 32 (19.7–50) < 0.001 − 0.82
ALP (IU/l) 62.1 ± 17.5 73.4 ± 18.4 < 0.001 − 0.63
Total bilirubin (mg/dl) 0.90 (0.60–1) 0.70 (0.50–0.92) 0.030 0.32
Direct bilirubin (mg/dl) 0.30 (0.20–0.40) 0.30 (0.21–0.40) 0.215 − 0.06
TC (mg/dl) 189 (169.5–213.5) 191.5 (161–216.2) 0.480 0.14
TG (mg/dl) 108 (86.5–166.5) 158.5 (125–210.7) 0.001 − 0.42
HDL-C (mg/dl) 47 (38.5–53) 40 (35–45) < 0.001 0.39
LDL-C (mg/dl) 120.2 (104.9–135.3) 126.6 (94.2–141.4) 0.937 − 0.02
Comorbidities
DM 3 (4.1) 21 (30) < 0.001 0.35
HT 2 (2.7) 14 (20) 0.001 0.27
HL 1 (1.4) 5 (7.1) 0.085 0.14
MetS 25 (34.25) 34 (48.57) 0.082 0.14

Values are expressed as n (%), mean ± SD or median (25th–75th percentiles).


Effect size calculations were given as Cohen’s d or φ coefficient.
ALP, alkaline phosphatase; ALT, alanine aminotransferase; AST, aspartate aminotransferase; DBP, diastolic blood pressure; DM, diabetes mellitus; GGT, γ-glutamyl-
transferase; HDL-C, high-density lipoprotein cholesterol; HL, hyperlipidemia; HT, hypertension; LDL-C, low-density lipoprotein cholesterol; MetS, metabolic syndrome;
NAFLD, nonalcoholic fatty liver disease; SBP, systolic blood pressure; TC, total cholesterol; TG, triglyceride; WBC, white blood cells; WC, waist circumference.
Bold values indicate statistically significant (P < 0.05).

Table 2. Overall Montreal Cognitive Assessment scores and number of


the healthy participants and nonalcoholic fatty liver disease participants
showing point deficits within each cognitive domain
Variables Healthy (n = 73) NAFLD (n = 70) P
MoCA scores a
21.08 ± 3.93 18.17 ± 5.20 < 0.001
Frequency of cognitive 32 (43.8) 49 (70) 0.002
dysfunctionb
MoCA itemsc
Visuospatial 37 (50.7) 53 (75.7) 0.002
Memory 67 (91.8) 68 (97.1) 0.163
Executive 49 (67.1) 58 (82.9) 0.030
Attention 60 (82.2) 60 (85.7) 0.567
Language 62 (84.9) 60 (85.7) 0.895
Orientation 25 (34.2) 30 (42.9) 0.290

Values are expressed as n (%) or mean ± SD.


MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; NAFLD, nonalcoholic fatty liver disease.
a
The lower MoCA score represents the poorer cognitive performance (over 30
points).
b
Cognitive functions were evaluated using the Turkish version of the MoCA, with a
cut-off score for mild cognitive impairment of less than 21 points.
c
The numerical data indicate the number of the affected individuals showing point Fig. 1. Total Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) scores in healthy
deficits within each MoCA item. participants and nonalcoholic fatty liver disease (NAFLD) participants.
Bold values indicate statistically significant (P < 0.05).

Copyright r 2018 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Cognitive assessment of NAFLD Celikbilek et al. www.eurojgh.com 5

Table 3. Univariate and multiple logistic regression analyses to identify on computer-based cognitive tests. In line with these
the predictors of cognitive dysfunction in the healthy participants and results, we found that NAFLD participants had sig-
nonalcoholic fatty liver disease participants nificantly lower MoCA scores (in a range indicating cog-
Univariate Multiple nitive dysfunction) than the healthy group. Accordingly,
the incidence of cognitive dysfunction was higher in the
Variables OR (95%CI) P OR (95%CI) P
NAFLD group. We also found that the presence of
Age 1.06 (1.02–1.09) 0.004 – – NAFLD was a risk factor for cognitive dysfunction in
Sex 0.54 (0.27–1.08) 0.081 – –
Alkaline 1.02 (1.00–1.04) 0.024 – – univariate analysis. Despite the lack of a similar associa-
phosphatase tion in a multivariate model (possibly because of the small
Triglyceride 1.01 (1.00–1.01) 0.017 – – sample size), we speculate that the relationship between
DM 2.09 (0.81–5.40) 0.129 – –
HT 6.27 (1.37–28.72) 0.018 – –
NAFLD and cognitive dysfunction observed in the uni-
MetS 2.21 (1.10–4.42) 0.025 – – variate analysis was not coincidental. Nonetheless, we
Marital status 0.98 (0.21–4.54) 0.978 – – could not determine whether NAFLD and cognitive dys-
Education level 4.12 (1.95–8.73) < 0.001 2.79 (1.12–6.96) 0.028
Area of residence 4.71 (2.22–9.99) < 0.001 5.68 (2.24–14.38) < 0.001
function are conditions on the same spectrum. In contrast
Presence of 2.99 (1.50–5.96) 0.002 – – to previous studies, we evaluated cognitive functioning
NAFLD using the MoCA because it is more sensitive and specific to
MCI [29]. The participants in our study were not screened
Cognitive functions were evaluated using the Turkish version of the MoCA, with a
cut-off score for mild cognitive impairment of less than 21 points. a priori for cognitive deficits and thus would likely have
95%CI, 95% confidence intervals; DM, diabetes mellitus; HT, hypertension; MetS, ‘normal’ scores on the MMSE, which represents the
metabolic syndrome; NAFLD, nonalcoholic fatty liver disease; OR, odds ratio. instrument most widely used by frontline physicians. In
Bold values indicate statistically significant (P < 0.05).
fact, as a screening instrument, the MoCA is preferable
because it encompasses a broader range of cognitive
In the correlation analysis, the MoCA score was cor- domains, including abstract thought and executive func-
related negatively with age (r = − 0.450; P < 0.001) and the tioning, and enables a more rapid assessment [29]. The
FIB4 score in NAFLD participants (r = − 0.359; P < 0.05), MoCA can be administered in 10 min, fits on one page,
but no correlations were observed with either hepatos- and is therefore appropriate for use in the context of the
teatosis grade or the presence of MetS (P > 0.05). generally limited assessment window available in the
clinical setting.
Discussion We observed cognitive deficits mainly in the executive
functioning and visuospatial domains on the MoCA in our
Three main findings emerged from this study. First, MoCA NAFLD participants. Executive functioning, which is
scores were significantly lower, including in the visuospa- associated with the frontal lobe (especially the prefrontal
tial and executive functioning domains, in the participants area and the orbitofrontal cortex), encompasses planning,
with NAFLD compared with those in the healthy group. purposeful organized behavior, abstract thought, response
Second, education level and area of residence were asso- inhibition, mental flexibility, working memory, and
ciated independently with cognitive dysfunction in a mul- self-control [42]. The MoCA measures these executive
tivariable model. Third, MoCA scores were correlated functions in triplicate: it includes an alternation task
negatively with age and FIB4 scores in participants adapted from the Trail Making B task, a phonemic fluency
with NAFLD. task, and a two-item verbal abstraction task. Lower scores
NAFLD is characterized by an accumulation of lipids in indicate greater impairment in executive functioning.
the hepatocytes, and infiltration of inflammatory immune Intact executive functions have been identified as crucial
cells into the liver parenchyma (leading to the secretion of for the management of chronic conditions [32]. Several
proinflammatory cytokines that potentially cause liver studies have shown that executive functioning is impaired
damage) [18]. Recent data suggest that proinflammatory in chronic metabolic conditions, probably because of
cytokines, such as IL-1, IL-6, and TNF, also serve specific chronic vascular pathology in the white matter circuits
functions in synaptic plasticity, learning, and cognitive that connect the subcortical nuclei to frontal cortical
processing [11]. If so, NAFLD – a highly inflammatory areas [32,42,43]. NAFLD, which is another inflammatory
condition – is likely to also promote similar damage in metabolic condition, may cause a similar disturbance in
neurons involved specifically in cognition. Experimental these connections, possibly limiting an individual’s ability
studies, and a limited number of clinical investigations, to recognize their own symptoms and make decisions on
have pointed to a link between NAFLD and poor cognitive the targeted behavior. Spitznagel et al. [19] examined
functioning. However, it is unclear whether liver damage postoperative cognitive functions using the WebNeuro
directly impacts cognition or serves as a trigger for sub- computerized cognitive test battery, which measures
sequent deleterious inflammation or metabolic dysfunc- executive function, attention, and memory, in individuals
tion. According to animal models, chronic inflammation in with or without liver fibrosis at the time of bariatric
association with NAFLD can induce a secondary neu- surgery. They found poorer cognitive performance post-
roinflammation, which is followed by neurodegeneration operatively on executive function and memory tasks in the
and neuronal death in specific brain regions in NAFLD bariatric patients with liver fibrosis, which represents
rats [24,25]. In a clinical study, Elliott et al. [26] reported an advanced form of NAFLD. They concluded that the
cognitive difficulties on a cognitive symptom questionnaire accumulation of connective tissue in the liver occurs in the
among NAFLD participants. Similarly, Seo et al. [27] context of metabolic dysfunction and inflammatory pro-
showed that, among Korean adults, NAFLD was asso- cesses and in turn may affect cognition [19]. However, our
ciated independently with reduced cognitive performance NAFLD participants showed deficits in the visuospatial

Copyright r 2018 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
6 European Journal of Gastroenterology & Hepatology Month 2018 • Volume 00 • Number 00

domain more frequently than the healthy group. limit of our cut-off score for MCI of less than 21 points.
Visuospatial ability includes spatial memory, navigation, Finally, the MoCA scores in our study were correlated
and mental representation of three-dimensional space [44]. negatively with FIB4 scores. As the accumulation of con-
The MoCA measures this ability through a two-step test, nective tissue often coincides with increased levels of
composed of a clock-drawing task and a three-dimensional inflammatory cytokines [51], which have deleterious
cube-copying task. Lower scores indicate greater impair- effects on cognitive processing when present in large
ment in visuospatial ability. It is difficult to explain why quantities [11], it is not surprising that participants with
visuospatial abilities are impaired in metabolic disorders. NAFLD and considerable liver damage can show poorer
In previous reports, overweight adolescents showed poorer cognitive functioning than those with milder forms of
performance in visuospatial organization [45] and mental NAFLD. However, we failed to find a correlation between
rotation tasks [46] compared with healthy-weight con- cognitive dysfunction and hepatosteatosis grade, which
trols. In their recent functional magnetic resonance ima- might be attributable to an insufficient number of partici-
ging study, García-García et al. [47] reported decreased pants in the hepatosteatosis subgroups; larger cohorts may
functional connectivity of the middle frontal gyrus and the be needed to draw any firm conclusion.
lateral occipital cortex with the overall brain network in The present study had certain limitations. First, the
obese participants; these regions feature in several brain sample size was relatively small. Second, we used a cross-
circuits involved in the signaling of perceptual processes, sectional design and thus could not determine causality.
attention, and executive and motor functions. In a very Third, we cannot exclude an influence on cognition from
recent work, Tsai et al. [48] investigated behavioral and residual confounding factors, such as nutritional, sleeping,
cognitive electrophysiological functioning in 26 obese and physical exercise habits. Fourth, we did not confirm
children during the performance of a visuospatial attention fatty liver histologically because of the ethical concerns
task. The obese children showed poorer behavioral per- associated with using an asymptomatic population for
formance, with slower reaction times, lower accuracy research purposes. Fifth, there may have been a selection
rates, and smaller potentials compared with the healthy bias because we included illiterate participants, although
group. These two studies showed that obesity is associated we also carried out a supplementary analysis after
with impairment in the central processing of cognition excluding those participants. Our results did not change
during executive functioning, with concomitant disen- significantly in either MoCA scores or MoCA items
gagement of visuospatial attention [47,48]. This may (visuospatial and executive). Yancar Demir et al. [52]
provide a clue as to why impairments were observed in noted that the applicability of the MoCA did not differ
these domains on the MoCA in metabolic disorder according to education level among a Turkish population,
patients, but any such explanation for the impairments with the exception of the language domain.
observed in NAFLD is currently lacking. Future studies
should examine the multifaceted nature of these domains Conclusion
by using instruments that are more precise than the
MoCA. In conclusion, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first
In the present study, the independent risk factors for study to use the MoCA for the initial screening of cognitive
cognitive dysfunction were sociodemographic factors, such performance in participants with NAFLD. Our data show
as education level and area of residence. However, the that NAFLD patients may have early or subtle cognitive
relatively small sample size did not allow us to assess the dysfunction, including in the visuospatial and executive
effects of other independent parameters. Well-known fac- function domains, on the MoCA. We suggest that assess-
tors that promote positive neuroplasticity, thereby ments of NAFLD patients should also include a brief
increasing cognitive reserve, include education, physical cognitive screening test, such as the MoCA, to optimize
activity, social interaction, intellectual pursuits, and cog- management, which could be valuable from a preventive
nitive remediation [49]. Education may also have an perspective.
indirect effect on cognition by promoting healthful beha-
viors during aging. According to Borovok et al. [50], Acknowledgements
spatial memory depends on the hippocampus, which is
probably relatively vulnerable to aging. In support of this, Conflicts of interest
MoCA scores in our study were correlated negatively with There are no conflicts of interest.
age; poor performance on spatial tasks in particular was
noted with older age. Although our NAFLD participants
were slightly older than the healthy group, we did not References
consider this to be a potential confounder because the 1 Levin BE, Llabre MM, Dong C, Elkind MS, Stern Y, Rundek T, et al.
difference was not statistically significant. Factors that Modeling metabolic syndrome and its association with cognition: the
promote negative neuroplasticity, and consequently Northern Manhattan study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2014; 20:951–960.
2 Solfrizzi V, Scafato E, Capurso C, D’Introno A, Colacicco AM, Frisardi V,
decreased cognitive reserve, include poor health, nutrition,
et al. Metabolic syndrome, mild cognitive impairment, and progression
and sleep hygiene, as well as substance abuse, depression, to dementia. The Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging. Neurobiol Aging
and anxiety [49]. Individuals with a lower socioeconomic 2011; 32:1932–1941.
status and those who do not exercise frequently, receive 3 Vanhanen M, Koivisto K, Moilanen L, Helkala EL, Hanninen T,
medical care, and eat healthier diets containing fresh fruit Soininen H, et al. Association of metabolic syndrome with Alzheimer
disease: a population-based study. Neurology 2006; 67:843–847.
and vegetables typically live in rural areas as found in our 4 Raffaitin C, Gin H, Empana JP, Helmer C, Berr C, Tzourio C, et al.
study population, and this may explain why the average Metabolic syndrome and risk for incident Alzheimer’s disease or vas-
MoCA score of our healthy participants was at the lower cular dementia: the three-city study. Diabetes Care 2009; 32:169–174.

Copyright r 2018 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Cognitive assessment of NAFLD Celikbilek et al. www.eurojgh.com 7

5 Vieira JR, Elkind MS, Moon YP, Rundek T, Boden-Albala B, Paik MC, screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005;
et al. The metabolic syndrome and cognitive performance: the Northern 53:695–699.
Manhattan Study. Neuroepidemiology 2011; 37:153–159. 30 Chen B, Jin X, Guo R, Chen Z, Hou X, Gao F, et al. Metabolic syndrome
6 Kirton JW, Dotson VM. The interactive effects of age, education, and and cognitive performance among Chinese ≥50 years: a cross-
BMI on cognitive functioning. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging sectional study with 3988 participants. Metab Syndr Relat Disord
Neuropsychol Cogn 2016; 23:253–262. 2016; 14:222–227.
7 Huang L, Yang L, Shen X, Yan S. Relationship between glycated 31 Alagiakrishnan K, Zhao N, Mereu L, Senior P, Senthilselvan A. Montreal
hemoglobin A1c and cognitive function in nondemented elderly patients cognitive assessment is superior to standardized mini-mental status
with type 2 diabetes. Metab Brain Dis 2016; 31:347–353. exam in detecting mild cognitive impairment in the middle-aged and
8 Kovacs KR, Bajko Z, Szekeres CC, Csapo K, Olah L, Magyar MT, et al. elderly patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Biomed Res Int 2013;
Elevated LDL-C combined with hypertension worsens subclinical vas- 2013:186106.
cular impairment and cognitive function. J Am Soc Hypertens 2014; 32 Ball J, Carrington MJ, Stewart S. Mild cognitive impairment in high-risk
8:550–560. patients with chronic atrial fibrillation: a forgotten component of clinical
9 Mellendijk L, Wiesmann M, Kiliaan AJ. Impact of Nutrition on cerebral management? Heart 2013; 99:542–547.
circulation and cognition in the metabolic syndrome. Nutrients 2015; 33 Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri W. Comparison of Beck Depression
7:9416–9439. Inventories-IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess 1996;
10 Sala M, de Roos A, van den Berg A, Altmann-Schneider I, 67:588–597.
Slagboom PE, Westendorp RG, et al. Microstructural brain tissue 34 Guldiken B, Guldiken S, Taskiran B, Koc G, Turgut N, Kabayel L, et al.
damage in metabolic syndrome. Diabetes Care 2014; 37:493–500. Migraine in metabolic syndrome. Neurologist 2009; 15:55–58.
11 McAfoose J, Baune BT. Evidence for a cytokine model of cognitive 35 Grundy SM, Cleeman JI, Daniels SR, Donato KA, Eckel RH, Franklin BA,
function. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2009; 33:355–366. et al. Diagnosis and management of the metabolic syndrome: an
12 Ma XJ, Maimaitirexiati TX, Zhang R, Hu G, Luo X. Effects of glucose and American Heart Association/National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute
disorders in lipid metabolism on cytokine levels and cognitive impair- Scientific Statement. Circulation 2005; 112:2735–2752.
ment in the olanzapine-induced obesity rat model. Genet Mol Res 2015; 36 Hernaez R, Lazo M, Bonekamp S, Kamel I, Brancati FL, Guallar E, et al.
14:11396–11402. Diagnostic accuracy and reliability of ultrasonography for the detection
13 Molteni R, Barnard RJ, Ying Z, Roberts CK, Gomez-Pinilla F. A high-fat, of fatty liver: a meta-analysis. Hepatology 2011; 54:1082–1090.
refined sugar diet reduces hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic 37 Gao X, Fan JG. Diagnosis and management of non-alcoholic fatty liver
factor, neuronal plasticity, and learning. Neuroscience 2002; disease and related metabolic disorders: consensus statement from the
112:803–814. Study Group of Liver and Metabolism, Chinese Society of
14 Hall J, Thomas KL, Everitt BJ. Rapid and selective induction of BDNF Endocrinology. J Diabetes 2013; 5:406–415.
expression in the hippocampus during contextual learning. Nat Neurosci 38 Quinn SF, Gosink BB. Characteristic sonographic signs of hepatic fatty
2000; 3:533–535. infiltration. AJR Am J Roentgenol 1985; 145:753–755.
15 Liu IY, Lyons WE, Mamounas LA, Thompson RF. Brain-derived neu- 39 Sumida Y, Yoneda M, Hyogo H, Itoh Y, Ono M, Fujii H, et al. Validation
rotrophic factor plays a critical role in contextual fear conditioning. of the FIB4 index in a Japanese nonalcoholic fatty liver disease popu-
J Neurosci 2004; 24:7958–7963. lation. BMC Gastroenterol 2012; 12:2.
16 Lonardo A, Ballestri S, Marchesini G, Angulo P, Loria P. Nonalcoholic 40 Selekler K, Cangoz B, Uluc S. Power of discrimination of Montreal
fatty liver disease: a precursor of the metabolic syndrome. Dig Liver Dis Cognitive Assessment (MOCA) scale in Turkish patients with mild
2015; 47:181–190. cognitive impairement and Alzheimer’s disease. Turk J Geriatri Dergisi
17 Than NN, Newsome PN. A concise review of non-alcoholic fatty liver 2010; 13:166–171.
disease. Atherosclerosis 2015; 239:192–202. 41 Ozdilek B, Kenangil G. Validation of the Turkish Version of the Montreal
18 Tarantino G, Finelli C. What about non-alcoholic fatty liver disease as a Cognitive Assessment Scale (MoCA-TR) in patients with Parkinson’s
new criterion to define metabolic syndrome? World J Gastroenterol disease. Clin Neuropsychol 2014; 28:333–343.
2013; 19:3375–3384. 42 Fitzpatrick S, Gilbert S, Serpell L. Systematic review: are overweight and
19 Spitznagel MB, Gunstad J, Manderino L, Heinberg L. Liver fibrosis obese individuals impaired on behavioural tasks of executive function-
predicts cognitive function following bariatric surgery: a preliminary ing? Neuropsychol Rev 2013; 23:138–156.
investigation. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2015; 23:1957–1959. 43 Van den Berg E, Dekker JM, Nijpels G, Kessels RP, Kappelle LJ, de
20 Ross AP, Bartness TJ, Mielke JG, Parent MB. A high fructose diet Haan EH, et al. Cognitive functioning in elderly persons with type 2
impairs spatial memory in male rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2009; diabetes and metabolic syndrome: the Hoorn study. Dement Geriatr
92:410–416. Cogn Disord 2008; 26:261–269.
21 Ross AP, Bruggeman EC, Kasumu AW, Mielke JG, Parent MB. Non- 44 Jeffery KJ, Wilson JJ, Casali G, Hayman RM. Neural encoding of large-
alcoholic fatty liver disease impairs hippocampal-dependent memory in scale three-dimensional space-properties and constraints. Front
male rats. Physiol Behav 2012; 106:133–141. Psychol 2015; 6:927.
22 Ergorul C, Eichenbaum H. The hippocampus and memory for ‘what,’ 45 Li Y, Dai Q, Jackson JC, Zhang J. Overweight is associated with
‘where,’ and ‘when’. Learn Mem 2004; 11:397–405. decreased cognitive functioning among school-age children and ado-
23 Farr SA, Yamada KA, Butterfield DA, Abdul HM, Xu L, Miller NE, et al. lescents. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2008; 16:1809–1815.
Obesity and hypertriglyceridemia produce cognitive impairment. 46 Jansen P, Schmelter A, Kasten L, Heil M. Impaired mental rotation
Endocrinology 2008; 149:2628–2636. performance in overweight children. Appetite 2011; 56:766–769.
24 Ghareeb DA, Hafez HS, Hussien HM, Kabapy NF. Non-alcoholic fatty liver 47 Garcia-Garcia I, Jurado MA, Garolera M, Marques-Iturria I,
induces insulin resistance and metabolic disorders with development of Horstmann A, Segura B, et al. Functional network centrality in obesity: a
brain damage and dysfunction. Metab Brain Dis 2011; 26:253–267. resting-state and task fMRI study. Psychiatry Res 2015; 233:331–338.
25 Kim DG, Krenz A, Toussaint LE, Maurer KJ, Robinson SA, Yan A, et al. 48 Tsai CL, Chen FC, Pan CY, Tseng YT. The neurocognitive performance
Non-alcoholic fatty liver disease induces signs of Alzheimer’s disease of visuospatial attention in children with obesity. Front Psychol 2016;
(AD) in wild-type mice and accelerates pathological signs of AD in an 7:1033.
AD model. J Neuroinflammation 2016; 13:1. 49 Vance DE, Roberson AJ, McGuinness TM, Fazeli PL. How neuroplas-
26 Elliott C, Frith J, Day CP, Jones DE, Newton JL. Functional impairment ticity and cognitive reserve protect cognitive functioning. J Psychosoc
in alcoholic liver disease and non-alcoholic fatty liver disease is sig- Nurs Ment Health Serv 2010; 48:23–30.
nificant and persists over 3 years of follow-up. Dig Dis Sci 2013; 50 Borovok N, Nesher E, Levin Y, Reichenstein M, Pinhasov A,
58:2383–2391. Michaelevski I. Dynamics of hippocampal protein expression during
27 Seo SW, Gottesman RF, Clark JM, Hernaez R, Chang Y, Kim C, et al. long-term spatial memory formation. Mol Cell Proteomics 2016;
Nonalcoholic fatty liver disease is associated with cognitive function 15:523–541.
in adults. Neurology 2016; 86:1136–1142. 51 Lemoine M, Ratziu V, Kim M, Maachi M, Wendum D, Paye F, et al.
28 Viscogliosi G, Andreozzi P, Chiriac IM, Cipriani E, Servello A, Ettorre E, Serum adipokine levels predictive of liver injury in non-alcoholic fatty liver
et al. Screening cognition in the elderly with metabolic syndrome. Metab disease. Liver Int 2009; 29:1431–1438.
Syndr Relat Disord 2012; 10:358–362. 52 Yancar Demir E, Ozcan T. Evaluating the relationship between educa-
29 Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, tion level and cognitive impairment with the Montreal Cognitive
Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief Assessment Test. Psychogeriatrics 2015; 15:186–190.

Copyright r 2018 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like