You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/43519901

Psychological Essentialism, Implicit Theories, and Intergroup


Relations

Article  in  Group Processes & Intergroup Relations · January 2006


DOI: 10.1177/1368430206059861 · Source: OAI

CITATIONS READS

190 1,795

4 authors:

Nick Haslam Brock Bastian


University of Melbourne University of Melbourne
346 PUBLICATIONS   19,768 CITATIONS    166 PUBLICATIONS   7,781 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Paul Bain Yoshihisa Kashima


Queensland University of Technology University of Melbourne
39 PUBLICATIONS   2,899 CITATIONS    181 PUBLICATIONS   7,827 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Norm talk project View project

Taxometrics View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Paul Bain on 17 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Group Processes & Intergroup
Relations
http://gpi.sagepub.com/

Psychological Essentialism, Implicit Theories, and Intergroup Relations


Nick Haslam, Brock Bastian, Paul Bain and Yoshihisa Kashima
Group Processes Intergroup Relations 2006 9: 63
DOI: 10.1177/1368430206059861

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://gpi.sagepub.com/content/9/1/63

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

Additional services and information for Group Processes & Intergroup Relations can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://gpi.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://gpi.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://gpi.sagepub.com/content/9/1/63.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Jan 19, 2006

What is This?

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Group Processes &
G
Intergroup Relations P
2006 Vol 9(1) 63–76
I
R
Psychological
Essentialism, Implicit
Theories, and Intergroup
Relations
Nick Haslam, Brock Bastian, Paul Bain and Yoshihisa Kashima
University of Melbourne

Research on implicit person theories shows that beliefs about the malleability of human
attributes have important implications for social cognition, interpersonal behavior, and
intergroup relations. We argue that these implications can be understood within the framework
of psychological essentialism, which extends work on implicit theories in promising directions.
We review evidence that immutability beliefs covary with a broader set of essentialist beliefs, and
that these essentialist beliefs are associated with stereotyping and prejudice. We then present
recent studies indicating that associations between implicit person theories and stereotyping
may be explained in terms of essentialist beliefs, implying a significant role for these beliefs in
the psychology of group perception. Finally, we propose ways in which research and theory on
essentialist beliefs might clarify and advance research on implicit person theories.

KEYWORDS essentialism, lay theories, prejudice, stereotypes

R E S E A R C H on implicit person theories (IPTs; Recent work on IPTs has investigated their
Levy, Plaks, Hong, Chiu, & Dweck, 2001) has role in stereotyping and intergroup relations.
demonstrated that beliefs about the nature of Levy et al. (2001) argued that entity theorists
human attributes are critically important for hold a static view of human nature that deeply
cognition, motivation, and behavior. ‘Entity affects how information about social groups is
theorists’ and ‘incremental theorists’, who interpreted. Whereas entity theorists ascribe
believe that attributes are fixed and malleable group members’ behavior to static and
respectively, process information in distinctive decontextualized traits, incremental theorists
ways that have profound implications. A interpret behavior in terms of dynamic psycho-
vigorous program of research has established logical processes (e.g. goals, appraisals,
the role of IPTs about intelligence, morality
(Chiu, Dweck, Tong, & Fu, 1997), and person- Author’s note
ality (Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1997; Gervey, Address correspondence to Nick Haslam,
Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1999) in a wide variety Department of Psychology, University of
of cognitive and behavioral domains and with Melbourne, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia
both children and adults. [email: nhaslam@unimelb.edu.au]

Copyright © 2006 SAGE Publications


(London, Thousand Oaks, CA and New Delhi)
9:1; 63–76; DOI: 10.1177/1368430206059861

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 9(1)

motives) and the interpersonal and intergroup of a category, which is understood to determine
context. Consequently, entity theorists are their identity, explain their observable proper-
especially prone to social stereotyping. In an ties, render them fundamentally alike, and
influential study, Levy, Stroessner, and Dweck allow many inferences to be drawn about them.
(1998) found that entity theorists made more More formally, essentialism is a naive ontology
stereotypical trait judgments of ethnic and positing that categories have a deep and unob-
occupational groups, made more extreme and servable reality, that this reality or ‘essence’
rapid stereotypic judgments on the basis of gives rise to the surface features of category
limited information about novel groups, and members (i.e. ‘dispositionism’), that it is
attributed stereotyped traits more to innate unchanging and unchangeable by human inter-
group properties than did incremental theor- vention, and that it has a ‘natural’ basis. In the
ists. In addition, entity theories predicted domain of social categories this causal basis may
stereotype endorsement independently of often be understood in a biological fashion.
several stereotype-relevant individual difference However, non-biological essences (e.g. spirits,
variables (e.g. right-wing authoritarianism, souls) may also be imputed and research
need for closure). suggests that biological causal beliefs may not
Later research has offered further support underpin essentialist thinking in some cultures
for the role of IPTs in intergroup relations. (Kashima et al., 2005).
Plaks, Stroessner, Dweck, and Sherman (2001) Research on ‘psychological essentialism’
showed that entity theorists pay greater atten- (Medin & Ortony, 1989) began with early work
tion to stereotype-consistent information than on children’s contrasting understandings of
incremental theorists. Hong et al. (2004) living kinds and human artifacts (Gelman,
demonstrated that people who hold entity 2003), but in the past decade it has extended to
theories about moral character display greater adults’ and children’s understandings of social
negative bias and prejudice toward maligned categories and personality traits. Recent work
outgroups. Moreover, unlike incremental has documented essentialist thinking about a
theorists, entity theorists did not display a host of differences between people, including
reduction in bias towards these groups when an ethnicity (Gil-White, 2001), race (Hirschfeld,
inclusive self-categorization (i.e. common 1996), religion (Boyer, 1993), gender
ingroup identity) was held. In a similar vein, (Mahalingam, 2003), mental disorder (Haslam
IPTs have been found to moderate social & Ernst, 2002), and personality characteristics
identification effects on self-conception and (Giles, 2003; Haslam, Bastian & Bissett, 2004).
intergroup orientation (Hong et al., 2003). By This work has been notable for its theoretical
implication, entity theorists are less susceptible and methodological diversity, representing
to the influences of self-categorization and positions that span cognitive psychology to
social context, believing that outgroup critical theory, and methods that range from
members’ attributes are concrete and laboratory experiments to ethnography and dis-
immutable. Findings such as these demonstrate course analysis.
the many ways in which IPTs illuminate stereo- Although this diversity of approach has at
typing and prejudice, and the extent to which times left the meaning of essentialist beliefs
they are stimulating original research. somewhat obscure, there is substantial agree-
Interest in the perceived fixedness of human ment that immutability beliefs are funda-
attributes has not been the exclusive province mental. In a seminal contribution, Rothbart
of IPT research, however. Several theorists have and Taylor (1992) proposed that essentialist
argued that beliefs in the immutability of thinking about social categories amounted to a
human attributes and social categories reflect a misapprehension of socially constructed
broad assumption about the nature of human groupings as ‘natural kinds’, and that it had
difference. This ‘essentialist’ assumption two primary components. On the one hand,
ascribes a fixed, underlying nature to members essentializing a social category involves

64

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Haslam et al. essentialism and implicit theories

attributing ‘inductive potential’ to it. A person’s Entity theories are elements of


membership in such a category is taken to be essentialist beliefs
richly informative about them, just as knowing
a creature’s biological species affords many As we have argued above, there are theoretical
inferences about its behavior, internal struc- grounds for taking entity theories to be closely
ture, ecological niche, and so on. On the other linked to psychological essentialism. As
hand, when a social category is essentialized it Rothbart and Taylor (1992) proposed, for
is also seen to be ‘inalterable’: membership in example, inalterability is one of two funda-
the category viewed as fixed and impermeable. mental components of essentialist thinking
The ascribed essence is changeless and there- about social categories, which represents them
fore a source of continuity through time. as natural kinds. However, the part-whole
Rothbart and Taylor’s two-component model of relationship between inalterability beliefs and
essentialist thinking has been influential among psychological essentialism is not simply a theor-
researchers in the field, who frequently invoke etical proposition, but has been demonstrated
their inalterability component as a core in several studies by members of our group and
element of the construct. others. We review this evidence below, which
If beliefs in the immutability of human attrib- provides a foundation for our later arguments
utes and social categories have been proposed that IPT research can be considered within the
as elements of an essentialist understanding of broader framework of psychological essential-
difference, they may illuminate research on ism.
IPTs. Entity theories can clearly be aligned with In the first study of the structure of essential-
one component of essentialist thinking (cf. ist beliefs about social categories, Haslam,
Brewer, Hong & Li, 2004), and drawing links Rothschild and Ernst (2000) had participants
between the two concepts and research tra- rate 40 categories on items assessing nine
ditions might be expected to pay theoretical elements of psychological essentialism. The
and empirical dividends. In this paper we focus of the investigation was participants’
explore these links, arguing that research on shared representations, and how these differen-
IPTs, especially as they relate to intergroup tiated among categories, so mean ratings were
phenomena such as stereotyping and preju- examined. Categories were found to vary along
dice, will benefit from making the connections two distinct dimensions of essentialist beliefs.
more clear. More controversially, we will argue Categories that scored high on the ‘natural
that IPT research can often be well framed in kind’ dimension—exemplified by gender,
the more encompassing terms of psychological racial, and ethnic groups—were believed to
essentialism, and that in some cases effects have immutable membership, to have sharp
attributed to IPTs might be better explained in category boundaries, to have necessary or
these terms. We begin by reviewing research on defining features, to have a natural basis, and to
the empirical association between entity be historically invariant. Categories scoring
theories and essentialist beliefs, and discuss high on the ‘entitativity’ dimension—exempli-
evidence that essentialist beliefs illuminate fied by several stigmatized groups such as
several intergroup phenomena. We then homosexuals, Jews, and AIDS patients—were
present recent studies by our group that believed to have relatively uniform members, to
account for links between entity theories, essen- be grounded in inhering or underlying similar-
tialist beliefs, and stereotyping, and conclude ities, to be identity-defining, and to be highly
by speculating on new ways in which essentialist informative about their members. Thus,
beliefs might contribute to the psychology of immutability beliefs were strongly associated
intergroup relations. (mean r = .65) with a set of interlinked essen-
tialist beliefs that represented some social
categories as being akin to biological species.
These associations, and the two-dimensional

65

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 9(1)

belief structure, were replicated with a different bounded, historically invariant, and defined by
sample of categories by Demoulin, Leyens, and necessary properties.
Yzerbyt (2003). Immutability beliefs have also been found to
Later work has extended these findings covary with other essentialist beliefs in several
regarding beliefs about social categories to more recent studies. Kashima et al. (2005)
beliefs about personality characteristics. found that beliefs in the inalterability of a range
Haslam et al. (2004) had participants rate 80 of social targets were associated with biological
trait terms on items assessing a subset of the causal beliefs about these targets. Haslam and
essentialist beliefs examined by Haslam et al. Levy (in press) addressed essentialist beliefs
(2000), again using aggregated ratings. In two about homosexuality, and found that indi-
studies, using exploratory and confirmatory viduals who believed sexual orientations to be
factor analysis respectively, characteristics were immutable also tended to believe them to be
differentiated along a single dimension of biologically based and fixed early in life. This
essentialist beliefs. Traits that were judged to be cohering set of essentialist beliefs was obtained
relatively fixed or immutable were also believed in three separate studies (Ns = 309, 487, & 215)
to be biologically based, discrete (i.e. defining using exploratory and confirmatory factor
personality ‘types’), consistent across situations, analytic procedures in student and community
and deeply rooted in and highly informative samples. Near identical findings have been
about the person who possessed them. reported by Hegarty (2002) and Hegarty and
Although the unidimensional structure Pratto (2001), who found consistent factor-
obtained in these studies differed from the analytic evidence for a dimension combining
structure obtained in research on social beliefs in the immutability, early fixing, and bio-
categories, immutability beliefs (i.e. entity logical underpinnings of sexual orientation.
theories) were again consistently associated Finally, Bastian and Haslam (in press) have
(mean r = .32) with a coherent set of essential- shown in two studies that an entity theory
ist beliefs. measure employed by Levy et al. (1998), which
Both of the studies reviewed above addressed assesses beliefs about ‘kinds of people’ in
the structure of essentialist beliefs about particu- general rather than specific social categories,
lar targets, namely social categories and person- covaries in a unifactorial manner with compar-
ality characteristics. Three further studies able scales assessing beliefs in the biological
suggest that similar structures capture individual basis, discreteness, and informativeness of
differences, with immutability beliefs covarying human attributes. These studies are presented
in a similar fashion with other essentialist beliefs in a later section of this paper, and add to a
between people. In the first of these studies, body of work that locates immutability beliefs
Haslam, Rothschild and Ernst (2002) examined within an encompassing set of essentialist
the individual difference structure of beliefs beliefs.
about three social categories (i.e. women, gay All of the research reviewed to this point
men, and African Americans), using ratings on involves correlational evidence, but one exper-
eight of the nine items developed by Haslam imental study indicates that immutability beliefs
et al. (2000). The same two-factor structure have a causal association with other essentialist
obtained in the earlier study was supported for beliefs. Haslam and Ernst (2002) investigated
each of the three categories, although this struc- essentialist beliefs about mental disorders,
ture was less well supported for beliefs about gay adapting a methodology developed by Chiu,
men and tendencies to essentialize different Hong, and Dweck (1997). When participants
categories were not strongly correlated. Partici- were supplied with mock scientific evidence
pants who believed membership in each that a mental disorder was difficult to cure (i.e.
category to be highly immutable, relative to immutable), they inferred that it was also his-
their peers, were particularly apt to believe that torically invariant, informative, grounded in
the category was biologically based, sharply necessary properties, and natural. Similar

66

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Haslam et al. essentialism and implicit theories

inferences were drawn when other essentialist conducted to date. However, it does at least
beliefs were manipulated (e.g. when discrete- raise the possibility that this research might
ness was manipulated disorders were inferred benefit from an expanded focus. If beliefs
to be natural, informative, deeply rooted, and about the fixedness of human attributes and
to have necessary properties and homogeneous groups have implications for intergroup
sufferers). Thus the links between among phenomena, then we believe it must be asked
essentialist beliefs, including immutability whether associated beliefs have similar, differ-
beliefs, appear to be cognitively interlinked ent, or additional implications. Further, it must
rather than just empirically covarying. be asked whether these implications should be
The research that we have reviewed in this ascribed to immutability beliefs in particular or
section of the paper establishes, we believe, that to essentialist beliefs in general. In the next
immutability beliefs, the cornerstone of section of this paper, we argue that essentialist
research on IPTs, should be considered as part beliefs as a set do have important bearings on
of a broader set of essentialist beliefs. intergroup phenomena, and in the following
Immutability beliefs cohere within this set section we argue that at least some effects
whether social categories or personality charac- attributed to entity theories might be explained
teristics are the relevant targets, whether differ- better in terms of essentialist beliefs.
ences between targets or differences between
people are concerned, whether targets are
Essentialist beliefs illuminate
specific or generalized, and whether correla-
tional or experimental methods are employed.
intergroup phenomena
We conceptualize essentialist beliefs as a From the outset, theorists have argued that
network whose components have overlapping essentialist beliefs about social categories are
but partially specific implications for group and not simply abstract ontological intuitions, but
person perception. Different beliefs may have that they have important implications for group
particular relevance to particular social-cogni- perception and evaluation. Half a century ago,
tive processes (e.g. discreteness beliefs for Allport (1954) proposed that a belief in a group
group perception, biological basis beliefs for essence was a fundamental attribute of the
explanation of individuals’ behavior), may have prejudiced personality, and Rothbart and
divergent implications for evaluation of certain Taylor (1992) maintained that viewing social
categories (e.g. Haslam & Levy in press, on categories as natural kinds exaggerates and
anti-gay attitudes), and may be differently deepens perceived differences between groups.
organized in relation to social categories (two- Yzerbyt and his colleagues have proposed a
factor model for social categories; Haslam et al., ‘subjective essentialist’ account of stereotyping
2000 vs. personal attributes (one-factor model (Yzerbyt & Rocher, 2002; Yzerbyt, Rocher &
for personality characteristics; Haslam et al., Schadron, 1997), according to which essences
2004)). Nevertheless, we propose that in the serve as theories that give explanatory coher-
typical case essentialist beliefs form a network ence to group stereotypes and foster disposi-
whose components are linked but not fully tional attributions. In addition, Yzerbyt and
redundant. colleagues argue that essentialist beliefs serve a
This proposal implies that the consistent function of rationalizing and legitimating
links between entity theories and other essen- existing social inequalities by portraying them
tialist beliefs reviewed above are not at all as natural and inevitable.
incompatible with immutability beliefs having a Empirical research on the implications of
specific role to play in social cognition and essentialist beliefs for group perception has
intergroup relations. The association between only recently begun to examine some of these
these beliefs and the broader network of theoretical claims, but it has yielded promising
essentialist beliefs therefore does not diminish results. Consistent with their position, Yzerbyt
the importance of IPT research as it has been and colleagues have shown that perceiving

67

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 9(1)

groups to have an underlying reality accentu- beliefs substantially accounted for differences
ates perceived differences between them in prejudice as a function of ethnicity, religios-
(Yzerbyt & Buidin, 1998) and promotes the use ity, and gender. Ethnic and religious differences
of dispositional attributions for their members’ in anti-gay attitudes were substantially reduced
behavior (Yzerbyt, Rogier & Fiske, 1998). In a when corresponding differences in immutabil-
similar vein, Estrada, Yzerbyt and Seron (2004) ity and biological basis beliefs were statistically
demonstrated that people who scored high on controlled. Similarly, gender differences in anti-
an essentialist belief scale were more likely to gay attitudes, which were obtained only for
explain intergroup differences with reference attitudes to gay men, were reduced when dis-
to inherent biological factors. The role that creteness beliefs were controlled. This pattern
essentialist thinking plays in justifying social of findings, where predominantly heterosexual
inequalities and political claims has been men held more prejudiced attitudes than
demonstrated by Verkuyten (2003), in a study women only toward people of their gender, and
of discourse about ethnicity in the Netherlands. the difference was largely explained by their
Importantly, Verkuyten shows that essentialist greater belief that gay men are different in
positions are not straightforwardly regressive, kind, has clear implications for the function of
and may have progressive implications when essentialist beliefs. Such beliefs may, that is,
used by minority groups to advance their claims serve an ego defensive or boundary reinforce-
for cultural continuity. ment function, enabling prejudiced individuals
Other research has addressed individual to disavow and distance themselves from a
differences in essentialist beliefs as predictors of despised identity.
prejudice. Haslam et al. (2002) found that Research on the infra-humanization of out-
essentialist beliefs about the nature of women groups (e.g. Leyens et al., 2001, 2003) reveals
and African Americans did not correlate another way in which essentialist beliefs con-
consistently with explicit measures of sexism tribute to an understanding of intergroup per-
and racism, contrary to Allport (1954), but that ception and ‘emotional prejudice’. In a series
essence-related beliefs about male homosexu- of studies, Leyens and colleagues have demon-
ality were powerfully associated with anti-gay strated that people selectively attribute distinc-
attitudes. Interestingly, participants holding tively human or ‘secondary’ emotions to their
more essentialist beliefs were not invariably ingroups, and thereby ascribe a lesser degree of
more prejudiced, as some anti-essentialist humanity to outgroups. Leyens and colleagues
beliefs (e.g. in the mutability and lack of bio- argue that this ‘infra-humanization’ effect
logical basis of male homosexuality) were involves a denial of the ‘human essence’ to out-
associated with anti-gay attitudes. groups, and that it is distinct from the well-
Haslam and Levy (in press) followed up this established finding of ingroup favoritism. They
finding in studies of essentialist beliefs about further maintain that an essentialist perception
sexual orientation. Factor analyses yielded of groups is a precondition of infra-humaniza-
three dimensions of essentialist beliefs about tion. Consistent with this claim, Demoulin et al.
male homosexuality and lesbianism, which had (2002) found that the infra-humanization
conflicting associations with prejudice. Believ- effect was moderated by the extent to which
ing that homosexuality is biologically based and participants essentialized and identified with
immutable, and that it is universal across their ingroup, a finding that was substantially
cultures and throughout human history, was replicated by Paladino, Vaes, Castano,
associated with pro-gay attitudes, whereas Demoulin, and Leyens (in press). Thus it is only
believing that gay men and lesbians are categor- when people believe that their ingroup has a
ically distinct from heterosexuals was associated meaningful underlying basis and identify with it
with anti-gay attitudes. In addition to clarifying that they infra-humanize outgroups. Further
the linkages between essentialist beliefs and support for the role of essentialist beliefs in
attitudes, Haslam and Levy found that these infra-humanization was obtained by Haslam

68

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Haslam et al. essentialism and implicit theories

et al. (2004), who showed that the extent to To test this possibility, two of us (Bastian &
which personality characteristics are seen to be Haslam, in press) recently conducted studies
aspects of human nature is strongly correlated that substantially replicated Studies 1, 2, and 5
(r = .75) with the extent to which they are essen- from Levy et al. (1998). In a pilot study with 60
tialized. Thus human nature does appear to be undergraduate participants, Bastian and
understood in an essence-like fashion. Haslam developed new individual difference
Findings such as those reviewed in the measures of three essentialist beliefs. These
present section strongly suggest that essentialist scales were closely modeled in wording and
beliefs have a significant role to play in the response format on the eight-item entity theory
social psychology of intergroup relations. They measure employed by Levy et al. (1998), which
have been shown to bear on group perception, was also administered. The new scales assessed
stereotyping, social attribution, discourse about the extent to which human attributes are
group differences, and prejudice. Although believed to have a biological basis, to divide
immutability-related beliefs are specifically people into discrete categories, and to be richly
implicated in one or two findings, there is little informative about the individuals who possess
evidence that they have a privileged position them. In a separate part of the pilot study, par-
among other essentialist beliefs. In the next ticipants freely generated stereotypical attrib-
section, we present new empirical work that utes for nine social categories (based on
directly challenges the privileging of immutabil- gender, sexual orientation, ethnicity, and occu-
ity beliefs. pation) for use in the main study.
Results of the pilot study indicated that the
new essentialist beliefs scales had adequate
Essentialist beliefs and stereotype reliability, with the new scales somewhat less
endorsement reliable that the entity theory scale, and that
To this point we have argued that entity they correlated positively with one another and
theories—beliefs in the fixedness of human with the entity theory measure. Consistent with
attributes—belong to an encompassing set of the research reviewed above, that is, beliefs in
essentialist beliefs, and that these beliefs are the immutability of human attributes covaried
associated in a sometimes complex fashion with systematically with beliefs in their biological
the endorsement of prejudiced attitudes. basis, discreteness, and informativeness. As in
Research in the IPT tradition has addressed Levy et al. (1998), the essentialist belief and
related questions, and in particular the role of entity theory scales did not correlate with
entity theories in stereotype endorsement. stereotype knowledge, as indexed by the
Findings such as those of Levy et al. (1998) number of stereotypical attributes listed. These
demonstrate the powerful role that entity attributes were collated and the six most
theories may play in the perception of groups. commonly mentioned positive and negative
However, if these theories are simply elements attributes for each of the nine categories were
within a broader set of essentialist beliefs, then retained for the main study.
it is unclear whether the effects attributed to In the main study, modelled on Levy et al.’s
entity theories might not be better accounted (1998) Study 5, Bastian and Haslam (in press)
for by essentialist beliefs. Beliefs about the administered the entity theory and essentialist
immutability of human attributes might have a beliefs scales to 114 undergraduates. Partici-
privileged role among other covarying beliefs pants also completed five additional scales
in predicting stereotype endorsement, but it is assessing individual differences linked to stereo-
also possible that immutability beliefs have no typing: right-wing authoritarianism (RWA: Alte-
such role when other essentialist beliefs are meyer, 1988), need for cognitive closure
taken into account. Stereotype endorsement (Webster & Kruglanski, 1994), attributional
might be fostered by essentialist beliefs as a complexity (Fletcher, Danilovics, Fernandez,
whole, rather than entity theories in particular. Peterson, & Reeder, 1986), need to evaluate

69

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 9(1)

( Jarvis & Petty, 1996), and social dominance contribution was larger than all of them. We
orientation (SDO: Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth, replicated this finding in a multiple regression
& Malle, 1994). With the exception of SDO, all analysis in which the essentialism index sub-
of these scales were employed by Levy et al. stituted for the entity theory scale and served as
(1998). In an ostensibly separate part of the a predictor alongside the five individual differ-
study, participants rated their level of agree- ence measures. Essentialist beliefs ( = .31, p <
ment with 108 stereotypical attributes (12  9 .01) predicted stereotype endorsement more
categories), the average level constituting our strongly than RWA ( = .27, p < .05), and the
measure of stereotype endorsement. Partici- effects of need for cognitive closure, need to
pants then reported whether or not they were evaluate, attributional complexity, and SDO
aware that each attribute was commonly associ- were all weak and nonsignificant. A series of six
ated with each category (stereotype knowl- hierarchical multiple regressions was then con-
edge). Finally, they rated their agreement with ducted, in which all but one predictor was
three possible explanations for the existence included in the first step and the remaining
and perpetuation of a subset of these stereo- predictor was added in the second. Only essen-
types (i.e. three positive and three negative tialist beliefs and RWA made incremental pre-
attributes for Aboriginals and homosexuals). As dictive contributions, and the former was
in Levy et al.’s (1998) Study 2, two of these almost twice the magnitude of the latter (R 2 =
explanations referred to innate or inherent .082 vs. .042). Thus essentialist beliefs appear to
factors within category members, and one play a unique and relatively powerful role in the
referred to social or environmental factors. endorsement of group stereotypes.
As in the pilot study, the essentialist belief Given that the essentialism index incorpor-
and entity theory scales again intercorrelated ated Levy et al.’s (1998) entity theory scale, the
positively, and a principal components analysis immutability beliefs that it measures may be
indicated that they all loaded on a single responsible for the index’s association with
dimension. This is consistent with the hypothe- stereotype endorsement. To test this possibility
sis that immutability beliefs (entity theories) we conducted an additional analysis in which
belong to an encompassing set of essentialist stereotype endorsement was regressed on the
beliefs: people who understood human attrib- entity theory measure and the three other
utes to be fixed also tended to believe them to belief scales. The four scales collectively pre-
be biologically based, categorical, and induc- dicted stereotype endorsement (F(4,109) =
tively potent. In view of the coherence of the 3.45, p < .01). Biological basis ( = .19, p < .05)
four belief scales, we summed them to form an and informativeness ( = .14, p = .08) had sig-
‘essentialism index’. Replicating Levy et al. nificant or marginal individual effects, but
(1998), we found that the entity theory scale entity theory ( = .12, p = .11) and discreteness
was significantly associated with stereotype ( = .09, p = .18) did not.
endorsement. However, this association was not Although they only represent a single study,
specific to immutability beliefs, as the three new these findings have implications for the role of
essentialist belief scales also significantly pre- entity theories in stereotyping. Replicating Levy
dicted stereotype endorsement, as did the et al. (1998), we found that entity theorists do
essentialism index (r = .33, p < .01). Again, no indeed endorse stereotypes more than their
belief scales correlated with stereotype knowl- incremental theorist peers. However, this
edge. association appears to be substantially
One of the most striking findings obtained by explained by the part-whole relationship that
Levy et al. (1998) was that their entity theory exists between entity theories and essentialist
scale predicted stereotype endorsement inde- beliefs. Entity theories had no association with
pendently of a number of established individual stereotype endorsement independent of other
difference measures known to be associated essentialist beliefs, with which they covaried.
with stereotyping, and that its unique predictive Needless to say, immutability beliefs may have a

70

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Haslam et al. essentialism and implicit theories

privileged role among these beliefs in some implications for stereotyping and prejudice. In
domains—they did, for example, predict short, other essentialist beliefs might benefit
innate/inherent explanations for stereotypes from the sort of thorough assessment and
better than the other scales ( = .26, p < .01 vs. experimental investigation that until now has
 = .16, p < .05 for biological basis)—and our been lavished almost exclusively on immutabil-
findings in no way diminish the importance of ity beliefs.
entity theories. Nevertheless, they suggest that A second potential benefit for IPT research
effects attributed to entity theories might, in at from greater contact with work on psychologi-
least some cases, be attributed to essentialist cal essentialism is an appreciation of the poten-
beliefs as a set. Rather than ascribing IPT effects tial positive implications of essentialist beliefs.
specifically to a ‘fixed’ view of human character- Entity theories have usually been represented
istics, it may sometimes be more appropriate to in the literature in negative terms. Although
ascribe them to an ‘essentialist’ view. they enable swift inference of positive traits and
facilitate evaluation-based categorization in a
Advantages of conceptualizing implicit way that may promote efficient information
retrieval (Tong & Chiu, 2002), they also
theory research in terms of
promote stereotyping, bias, shallow processing
essentialist beliefs of social information, overly rapid interper-
We have reviewed research on the relationships sonal judgments, and problematic approaches
between essentialist beliefs, stereotype endorse- to learning. Although the evidence linking
ment, and prejudice, arguing that IPTs can be entity theories to these negative implications is
understood within a broader framework that persuasive, and a similarly negative impression
may in some cases make better sense of IPT runs through much research on essentialist
research findings. In this final section of the beliefs, recent evidence suggests that essential-
paper, we suggest a few ways in which IPT ist thinking is not always malignant. Some
research might benefit from a more explicit essentialist beliefs are associated with pro-gay
consideration of research and theory on attitudes (Haslam et al., 2002; Haslam & Levy,
psychological essentialism. in press), essentialist discourse can have pro-
The first potential benefit that we see coming gressive implications for minority groups facing
from a greater attention to essentialist beliefs pressure to assimilate (Verkuyten, 2003), and
among IPT researchers is an expansion of the essentialized ingroups are highly valued by
range of theories that they might study. Beliefs their members (Castano, 2004). In addition,
about the fixedness vs. malleability of human people attribute the ‘human essence’ (opera-
attributes are undoubtedly important in a host tionalized as uniquely human emotions) to
of ways, and they have stimulated a great deal their ingroups more than to outgroups (Leyens
of research, but very little comparable work has et al., 2001), and essentialized personality
addressed related essentialist beliefs. We know characteristics are seen as highly desirable
very little, for example, about how beliefs about (Haslam et al., 2004) and attributed more to
the biological basis of human attributes influ- self than to others (Haslam, Bain, Douge, Lee,
ence group perception and prejudice (Keller, & Bastian, in press). Although none of this
2005). Some evidence suggests that such beliefs evidence overrides the evident associations of
may be associated with stigmatization of people essentialist thinking with stereotyping and
with mental disorders (Read & Harré, 2001), prejudice, it suggests a more nuanced view that
but in our genomic age it is remarkable that IPT research might adopt. Are there contexts
they have not received greater attention. A where entity theories have personally or socially
similar case could be made for investigating advantageous implications?
beliefs in the discreteness of human attributes A third potential benefit for IPT research
and categories, to the degree that typological concerns the focus in some essentialism
thinking might be expected to have important research and theory on the social or personal

71

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 9(1)

functions that essentialist beliefs may serve. A final advantage that we foresee coming
Most research on IPTs emphasizes the cognitive from a rapprochement between research on
processes associated with them rather than how IPTs and on essentialism is a concern with
(or if) they are motivated. When the functional patterns of change. IPT research proceeds from
basis of these theories is discussed, it is only the a basic opposition between stability and change,
cognitive or ‘epistemic’ functions that are con- and essentialist beliefs are also usually framed
sidered, specifically the role that IPTs play in in terms of stability. However, essences can be
the intuitive scientist’s sense making (Hong, understood not only as sources of observable
Levy, & Chiu, 2001). In contrast, theorists have stability, but also as explanations of underlying
entertained the possibility that essentialist continuity in the face of observable change. On
beliefs serve a variety of more social and affec- this view, an essence might be invoked to
tively charged functions, such as rationalizing explain why a larva and the butterfly it becomes
and legitimating social arrangements (Yzerbyt are one and the same organism. This sense of
et al., 1997), promoting social inferences about essence as continuity despite apparent trans-
outgroups and avoiding coordination failures formation underpins research on the develop-
with their members (Gil-White, 2001), or ment of children’s understandings of natural
defending against undesired social identities kinds (e.g. Gelman, 2003) and on how people
(Haslam & Levy, in press). Although empirical understand personal continuity in life narra-
work on the functions of essentialist beliefs has tives (Chandler, Lalonde, Sokol, & Halett,
lagged behind theory, an expanded focus on 2003). If researchers in the IPT tradition were
the functional implications of IPTs may repre- to take up this view of essentialism, they might
sent a fruitful direction for researchers. profitably examine theories about the sources
Social psychological research on essentialist or trajectories of change, rather than simply
beliefs has tended to address particular social about its presence or absence. Two people
categories, such as racial, ethnic, gender, or might equally believe change to be possible—
sexual orientation groups. IPT work, in and in this sense be incremental theorists—but
contrast, has tended to focus on relatively broad one might believe it occurs as a function of
psychological domains: intelligence, morality, environment and context while the other sees
and personality. We suggest that a fourth poten- it as the unfolding of an internal disposition
tial advantage to flow from greater contact (e.g. an individual’s maturational blueprint or a
between IPT and essentialism research is a new group’s destiny). In short, research informed by
focus on implicit theories about specific social work on psychological essentialism might begin
categories. It is certainly true that broad to examine people’s intuitions about how and
theories—in the fixedness of human personal- why groups have undergone historical change
ity, for example—have demonstrated associ- and may undergo further change in future. We
ations with intergroup phenomena such as are beginning a program of work on lay
stereotyping. However, we believe that there are theories about societal change that illustrates
likely to be benefits from addressing implicit this possibility.
theories about particular social groups in
addition to more global theories. There is some
evidence that people do not consistently essen-
Advantages of linking essentialism
tialize across social categories in a trait-like research to implicit theory research
fashion (e.g. Haslam et al., 2002) and also that We have emphasized ways in which research in
different social categories and personality the IPT tradition might benefit from research
attributes are essentialized to markedly differ- and theory on psychological essentialism, but
ent degrees (e.g. Demoulin et al., 2003; Haslam the benefits surely flow in both directions. IPT
et al., 2000, 2004). In view of this evidence, a research has much to offer students of essential-
more category- or attribute-specific approach to ism. We will briefly describe a few of these
IPTs might reap empirical benefits. benefits, as we see them.

72

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Haslam et al. essentialism and implicit theories

First, research on IPTs provides a good preliminary support for a categorical view of
example of methodological rigor, a focus on essentialist beliefs (Haslam, 2002), and research
social-cognitive processes that is often lacking in the physical domain suggests that if objects
in social psychological research on essentialist are not conceptualized as (essentialized)
beliefs. Much social psychological research on natural kinds they are understood either as con-
essentialism employs descriptive and correla- vention-based nominal kinds or function-based
tional methodologies, which certainly have artifacts (Keil, 1989). Essentialism researchers
their place, but IPT researchers have taken might benefit by following the lead of IPT
more steps to link theories to information pro- researchers and paying more equal attention to
cessing strategies, using social cognition the nature of non-essentialist thinking. Are
methodologies (e.g. Plaks et al., 2001; Tong & non-essentialized social categories understood
Chiu, 2002) in ways that essentialism researchers primarily in terms of social conventions or par-
might emulate. ticular social functions, and does this distinc-
Second, IPT researchers have a record of tion have important consequences for group
addressing lay theories of several psychological perception?
phenomena that have been inexplicably
neglected by essentialism researchers. Implicit
Conclusions
theories of intelligence have been a major focus
of attention, but researchers have yet to investi- The IPT approach to the study of intergroup
gate the extent to which people essentialize phenomena is a promising one that rests on a
intelligence more generally, despite the topical- solid foundation of achievement. Research on
ity of this issue during the ongoing debates sur- essentialist beliefs about social categories is of
rounding IQ. The same could be said for more recent vintage. However, we believe that it
implicit theories of character and morality has a great deal to contribute to the empirical
(Chiu et al., 1997), which have yet to attract the and theoretical understanding of people’s basic
attention of essentialism researchers. Implicit understandings of the nature of human groups
theories have also been investigated in domains and attributes, a topic that it shares with the IPT
that do not refer to person attributes (e.g. tradition. We have argued that implicit theory
relationships; Knee, Patrick, Vietor, & Neigh- approach emphasizes one among several com-
bors, 2004), and these might also be investi- ponents of essentialist thinking, and that it can be
gated from the standpoint of psychological usefully understood within that more encompass-
essentialism, although they do not invariably ing framework. Drawing connections between
involve immutability beliefs and the relevance the IPT and essentialist beliefs approaches, and
of essentialism may therefore be reduced. learning reciprocal lessons, should have benefi-
Third, IPT researchers have argued that cial consequences for both traditions. We believe
implicit theories are organized as discrete, that the fruits of these connections will become
coherent belief systems (Levy et al., 2001) increasingly obvious in the coming years.
rather than as varying by degree along a con-
tinuum like ordinary traits (Haslam & Kim, References
2002). Entity theories differ qualitatively from
incremental theories, and both are worthy of Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice.
attention in their own right. Accordingly, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom. San
people are usually classified into distinct groups
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
in IPT studies. In contrast, essentialism Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (in press). Psychological
researchers have generally been silent on the essentialism and stereotype endorsement. Journal
issue of whether essentialist beliefs apply cate- of Experimental Social Psychology.
gorically or dimensionally, and if the former is Boyer, P. (1993). Pseudo-natural kinds. In P. Boyer,
the case, what a non-essentialist belief might be. Cognitive aspects of religious symbolism (pp. 121–141).
One study of beliefs about depression finds Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

73

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 9(1)

Brewer, M. B., Hong, Y., & Li, Q. (2004). Dynamic Haslam, N. (2002). Essentialist thinking about
entitativity: Perceiving groups as actors. In depression: Evidence for polarized beliefs.
V. Yzerbyt, C. M. Judd, & O. Corneille (Eds.), The Psychological Reports, 91, 1253–1254.
psychology of group perception: Perceived variability, Haslam, N., Bain, P., Douge, L., Lee, M., & Bastian,
entitativity, and essentialism (pp. 25–38). New York: B. (in press). More human than you: Attributing
Psychology Press. humanness to self and others. Journal of Personality
Castano, E. (2004). On the advantages of reifying and Social Psychology.
the ingroup. In V. Yzerbyt, C. M. Judd, & O. Haslam, N., Bastian, B., & Bissett, M. (2004).
Corneille (Eds.), The psychology of group perception: Essentialist beliefs about personality and their
Perceived variability, entitativity, and essentialism implications. Personality and Social Psychology
(pp. 381–400). New York: Psychology Press. Bulletin, 30, 1661–1673.
Chandler, M. J., Lalonde, C. E., Sokol, B. W., & Haslam, N., & Ernst, D. (2002). Essentialist beliefs
Halett, D. (2003). Personal persistence, identity about mental disorders. Journal of Social and
development, and suicide: A study of Native and Clinical Psychology, 21, 628–644.
non-Native North American adolescents. Haslam, N., & Kim, H. (2002). Categories and
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child continua: A review of taxometric research. Genetic,
Development, 68, vii–130. Social and General Psychology Monographs, 128,
Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Tong, Y. Y., & Fu, H. Y. 271–320.
(1997). Implicit theories and conceptions of Haslam, N., & Levy, S. R. (in press). Essentialist
morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, beliefs about homosexuality: Structure and
73, 923–940. implications for prejudice. Personality and Social
Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1997). Lay Psychology Bulletin.
dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2000).
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 19–30. Essentialist beliefs about social categories. British
Demoulin, S., Cortes, B., Viki, T., Rodriguez, A., Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 113–127.
Rodriguez, R., Paladino, M.-P. et al. (2002). The Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2002). Are
role of in-group identification in infra-humanization. essentialist beliefs associated with prejudice? British
Unpublished manuscript. Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 87–100.
Demoulin, S., Leyens, J-P., & Yzerbyt, V. (2003). Hegarty, P. (2002). ‘It’s not a choice, it’s the way we’re
Subjective essentialism: Two kinds of underlying reality. built’: Symbolic beliefs about sexual orientation in
Unpublished manuscript, Catholic University of the US and Britain. Journal of Community and Applied
Louvain. Social Psychology, 12, 153–166.
Estrada, C., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Seron, E. (2004). The Hegarty, P., & Pratto, F. (2001). Sexual orientation
effect of subjective essentialism on lay theories of beliefs: Their relationship to anti-gay attitudes and
group differences. Psicothema, 16, 181–186. biological determinist arguments. Journal of
Fletcher, G. J. O., Danilovics, P., Fernandez, G., Homosexuality, 41, 121–135.
Peterson, D., & Reeder, G. D. (1986). Hirschfeld, L. A. (1996). Race in the making:
Attributional complexity: An individual Cognition, culture, and the child’s construction of
differences measure. Journal of Personality and Social human kinds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Psychology, 51, 875–884. Hong, Y., Chan, G., Chiu, C., Wong, R. Y. M.,
Gelman, S. A. (2003). The essential child: Origins of Hansen, I. G., Lee, S. et al. (2003). How are social
essentialism in everyday thought. Oxford, UK: Oxford identities linked to self-conception and intergroup
University Press. orientation? The moderating effect of implicit
Gervey, B. M., Chiu, C. Y., Hong, Y. Y., & Dweck, C. S. theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
(1999). Differential use of person information in 85, 1147–1160.
decisions about guilt versus innocence: The role Hong, Y., Coleman, J., Chan, G., Wong, R. Y. M.,
of implicit theories. Personality and Social Psychology Chiu, C., Hansen, I. G. et al. (2004). Predicting
Bulletin, 25, 17–27. intergroup bias: The interactive effects of implicit
Giles, J. W. (2003). Children’s essentialist beliefs about theory and social identity. Personality and Social
aggression. Developmental Review, 23, 413–443. Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1035–1047.
Gil-White, F. (2001). Are ethnic groups biological Hong, Y., Levy, S. R., & Chiu, C. (2001). The
species to the human brain? Essentialism in contribution of the lay theories approach to the
human cognition of some social groups. Current study of groups. Personality and Social Psychology
Anthropology, 42, 515–554. Review, 5, 98–106.

74

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Haslam et al. essentialism and implicit theories

Jarvis, G., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The need to attention allocation: Preferences for stereotypic
evaluate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, versus counterstereotypic information. Journal of
70, 172–196. Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 876–893.
Kashima, Y., Kashima, E., Chiu, C., Farsides, T., Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B.
Gelfand, M., Hong, Y. et al. (2005). Culture, F. (1994). Social dominance orientation: A
essentialism, and agency: Are individuals universally personality variable predicting social and political
believed to be more real entities than groups? attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 147–169. 67, 741–763.
Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and cognitive Read, J., & Harré, N. (2001). The role of biological
development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. and genetic causal beliefs in the stigmatization of
Keller, J. (2005). In genes we trust: The biological ‘mental patients’. Journal of Mental Health, 10,
component of psychological essentialism and its 223–235.
relationship to mechanisms of motivated social Rothbart, M., & Taylor, M. (1992). Category labels
cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, and social reality: Do we view social categories as
88, 686–702. natural kinds? In G. R. Semin & K. Fiedler (Eds.),
Knee, C. R., Patrick, H., Vietor, N. A., & Neighbors, Language and social cognition (pp. 11–36). London:
C. (2004). Implicit theories of relationships: Sage.
Moderators of the link between conflict and Tong, Y., & Chiu, C. (2002). Implicit theories and
commitment. Personality and Social Psychology evaluation-based organization of impressions: An
Bulletin, 30, 617–628. application of the memory search paradigm.
Levy, S. R., Plaks, J. E., Hong, Y. Y., Chiu, C. Y., & Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
Dweck, C. S. (2001). Static versus dynamic 1518–1527.
theories and the perception of groups: Different Verkuyten, M. (2003). Discourses about ethnic group
routes to different destinations. Personality and (de-)essentialism: Oppressive and progressive
Social Psychology Review, 5, 156–168. aspects. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42,
Levy, S. R., Stroessner, S. J., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). 371–391.
Stereotype formation and endorsement: The role Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994).
of implicit theories. Journal of Personality and Social Individual differences in need for cognitive
Psychology, 74, 1421–1436. closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Leyens, J. Ph., Cortes, B. P., Demoulin, S., Dovidio, J. 67, 1049–1062.
F., Fiske, S. T., Gaunt, R. et al. (2003). Emotional Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Buidin, G. (1998). The impact of naive
prejudice, essentialism, and nationalism. European theories about the nature of groups on the accentuation
Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 704–717. effect. Unpublished manuscript, Catholic University
Leyens, J. Ph., Rodriguez, A. P., Rodriguez, R. T., of Louvain.
Gaunt, R., Paladino, P. M., Vaes, J. et al. (2001). Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Rocher, S. (2002). Subjective
Psychological essentialism and the differential essentialism and the emergence of stereotypes. In
attribution of uniquely human emotions to C. McGarty, V. Y. Yzerbyt, & R. Spears (Eds.),
ingroups and outgroups. European Journal of Social Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of
Psychology, 31, 395–411. meaningful beliefs about social groups (pp. 38–66).
Mahalingam, R. (2003). Essentialism, culture, and Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
beliefs about gender among the Aravanis of Tamil Yzerbyt, V. Y., Rocher, S., & Schadron, G. (1997).
Nadu, India. Sex Roles, 49, 489–496. Stereotypes as explanations: A subjective
Medin, D. L., & Ortony, A. (1989). Psychological essentialistic view of group perception. In
essentialism. In S. Vosniadou & A. Ortony (Eds.), R. Spears, P. J. Oakes, N. Ellemers, &
Similarity and anlogical reasoning (pp. 179–195). S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social psychology of
New York: Cambridge University Press. stereotyping and group life (pp. 20–50). Oxford:
Paladino, M-P., Vaes, J., Castano, E., Demoulin, S., & Blackwell.
Leyens, J-P. (in press). Emotional infra- Yzerbyt, V. Y., Rogier, A., & Fiske, S. T. (1998). Group
humanization in intergroup relations: The role of entitativity and social attribution: On translating
national identification in the attribution of situational constraints into stereotypes. Personality
primary and secondary emotions to Italians and and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1089–1103.
Germans. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive.
Plaks, J. E., Stroessner, S. J., Dweck, C. S., & Paper received 22 July 2004; revised version accepted 10
Sherman, J. W. (2001). Person theories and May 2005.

75

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 9(1)

Biographical notes is lecturer in psychology at Murdoch


PA U L B A I N
University. He recently completed his PhD at the
NICK HASLAM is associate professor of psychology at University of Melbourne, and his research
the University of Melbourne. His research interests include the psychology of values and
interests include psychological essentialism and conceptions of human nature.
interpersonal cognition.
YOSHIHISA KASHIMA is associate professor of
BROCK BASTIAN is a graduate student in psychology psychology at the University of Melbourne. He has
at the University of Melbourne. His dissertation research interests in stereotyping, communication
research examines essentialism in relation to processes, essentialism, cross-cultural psychology,
stereotyping, social identity, and social cognition. and connectionist modeling.

76

Downloaded from gpi.sagepub.com at UQ Library on October 12, 2011


View publication stats

You might also like