You are on page 1of 6

Reading Acuity in Children: Evaluation and Reliability

Using MNREAD Charts


Gianni Virgili,1 Claudia Cordaro,2 Anna Bigoni,2 Sabrina Crovato,2 Paolo Cecchini,2 and
Ugo Menchini1

PURPOSE. To study reading acuity in children and assess the the subject is no longer able to read. Print size corresponding
reliability of its measurement using the Italian version of the to CPS is typically two times larger than RA, a difference of 0.3
MNREAD reading chart. in logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution (logMAR).
METHODS. One hundred sixteen children from grades 3 to 8 The effects of age on visual function testing have been
with normal near vision were tested at a pediatric eye care studied in adults.3–10 The Bailey-Lovie near word chart11 was
institution in northeastern Italy. Visual acuity was evaluated used in these studies to evaluate reading ability. Recently, the
with Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) reliability of visual acuity measurement with an Early Treat-
charts and reading acuity with MNREAD charts. Examination ment Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) chart in children has
with charts 1 and 2 by the same examiner in the same visit also been assessed,12 but little is known about the age-specific
were used as test–retest measurements. reading performance of children.
The MNREAD charts are continuous-text reading-acuity
RESULTS. Mean improvement of performance from grades 3 to charts suitable for measuring the RA and reading speed of
8 was approximately 1 line for ETDRS acuity and 1.5 sentences normal and low-vision patients, developed at the Minnesota
for reading acuity and critical print size. Mean maximum read- Laboratory for Low-Vision Research.13,14 Italian and Japanese
ing speed increased from 96 to 167 words/minute. Regression versions of the charts are currently commercially available.
analysis showed that a stable reading speed was obtained The purpose of this study was to evaluate the reliability of
throughout sentences of decreasing size up to the critical print measurements of RA obtained with the Italian MNREAD charts
size. Measurement error, expressed as 95% limits of agreement, in children in grades 3 through 8. A second purpose was to
was ⫾0.136 logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution characterize the expected improvement of reading ability
(logMAR) and ⫾0.142 logMAR for ETDRS acuity and reading across grades and compare it with an ETDRS chart, a world-
acuity, respectively. Critical print size had the lowest reliability wide standard for visual acuity measurement.
(⫾0.193 logMAR). Maximum reading speed limits of agree-
ment were ⫾0.077 logarithm of words per minute (logWPM),
or a 19% change in words per minute. There were no detect- METHODS
able effects of grade and refractive error on measurement
errors. Subjects were recruited at the pediatric ophthalmic practice of La
Nostra Famiglia, a scientific institution providing eye care to normal
CONCLUSIONS. In children, MNREAD charts provide measure-
and visually impaired children in northeastern Italy. Children are re-
ments of reading acuity as reliable as ETDRS chart measure-
ferred by pediatricians for screening of ophthalmic and refractive
ments of distance acuity on a logMAR scale. They also provide
abnormalities to the center, which is the only high-volume pediatric
highly reliable measurements of the maximum reading speed
eye care institution in an area of approximately 300,000 residents, one
across all grades, independent of its noticeable increase. (In-
third living in the town of Udine.
vest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004;45:3349 –3354) DOI:10.1167/
Consecutive children from grades 3 through 8 were included in the
iovs.03-1304
study. We planned to enroll 120 consecutive subjects who met inclu-
sion criteria, with a minimum of 10 children per grade. Children and

R eading is a complex process involving several visual and


nonvisual aspects. Print size is one of the factors that
mainly influence reading speed in normal and low-vision sub-
parents were informed about the purposes of the research and parents
signed an informed consent. The study adhered to the tenets of the
Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the institution’s institu-
jects.1,2 Typically, reading speed is stable across a wide range tional review board.
of print sizes for one individual (maximum reading speed Inclusion criteria were visual acuity at least 0.3 logMAR (20/40)
[MRS]). As print size decreases, there comes a point where with ETDRS charts, normal findings in an ocular examination, and
reading starts to slow down, referred to as the critical print size normal binocular motility and stereoscopic sense on the Lang II test
(CPS). The reading acuity (RA) limit has been reached when (complete test).15 The level of ETDRS acuity admitted in the study was
relatively low because compliance may not be optimal with children
using this chart. When tested with a single-letter presentation, all
From the 1Eye Clinic II, Department of Oto-Neuro-Ophthalmolog- children could see at least some letters at 20/20 level.
ical Surgical Sciences, University of Florence, Florence, Italy; and the Subjects were screened for dyslexia using the Memory Transfer
2 (MT) test (Organizzazioni Speciali, Florence, Italy) and were excluded
Neurophthalmology Unit, Scientific Institution E. Medea, La Nostra
Famiglia, Polo Friuli Venezia Giulia, Italy. if the reading speed score was less than 10 or if errors were more than
Submitted for publication December 2, 2003; revised March 2, 15, as recommended.
April 14, and April 30, 2004; accepted May 10, 2004. One hundred twenty children were evaluated. Four were excluded
Disclosure: G. Virgili, (P); C. Cordaro, None; A. Bigoni, None; S. because they tested positive for dyslexia with the MT test. Overall, 116
Crovato, None; P. Cecchini, None; U. Menchini, None children were finally included. Mean age was 10.5 ⫾ 1.5 years. Of the
The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by page
116 children, 68 (59%) were girls. The number of subjects from grades
charge payment. This article must therefore be marked “advertise-
ment” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. §1734 solely to indicate this fact. 3 through 8 were 26, 22, 22, 15, 20, and 11, respectively.
Corresponding author: Gianni Virgili, Eye Clinic, Department of Refraction was first obtained with an autorefractometer (Retino-
Oto-Neuro-Ophthalmological Surgical Sciences, University of Florence, max; Nikon, Tokyo, Japan) and then refined subjectively. Mean spher-
Viale Morgagni 85, 50134 Florence, Italy; gianni.virgili@unifi.it. ical equivalent was ⫺0.44 and ⫺0.56 D in the right and left eyes,

Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, September 2004, Vol. 45, No. 9
Copyright © Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 3349

Downloaded From: https://iovs.arvojournals.org/ on 08/20/2018


3350 Virgili et al. IOVS, September 2004, Vol. 45, No. 9

TABLE 1. Crossing Scheme for Chart Presentation in the Study formula is used to compute RA in logMAR: RA ⫽ 1.4 ⫺ (sentences ⫻
0.1) ⫹ (errors ⫻ 0.01). There was no need to adjust the value
Presentation obtained with this formula by a factor accounting for viewing
Subject Eye Chart Order
distance, because a standard testing distance of 40 cm was adopted.
1 R 1 1 MRS is obtained by averaging the speed of the sentences with print
L 2 2 size corresponding to CPS or larger. MRS is measured in words per
2 R 2 1 minute (WPM), but the logarithm of words per minute (logWPM) is
L 1 2 used to make its progression linear, thus ensuring a proportional
3 R 1 2 percentage of change, which is recorded in 0.01 log units. CPS is
L 2 1 recorded at 0.1 logMAR intervals. CPS was identified as the print
4 R 2 2
size of the sentence at which both of the following conditions were
L 1 1
satisfied: (1) all the following (smaller) sentences were read at a
speed that was 1.96 times the standard deviation below the average
of the largest preceding sentences and (2) all the following (smaller)
respectively, ranging from ⫺7.88 to ⫹5.25 D overall. Approximately
30% of both right and left eyes needed a cylinder of 1 D or more; in 5%, sentences were read at a speed 5% slower (an approximate 25%
the cylinder was 2 to 3.5 D. Subjects wore refractive error correction change in WPM) than this average. In approximately one third of
during testing. the eyes (32%) the two criteria disagreed, suggesting two different
All psychophysical tests were conducted by the same examiner for sentences as potential CPS. In such cases, the sentence with the
each child. A second examiner helped record reading data obtained larger print size was chosen, to avoid averaging some slower sen-
with the MNREAD chart. tences in MRS computation, although this usually has little bearing
The MNREAD sentences provide samples of reading material de- on the estimate. An exception was made in five eyes in which the
signed to demand the visual processing capabilities and eye-movement smaller sentence seemed preferable after graphic examination of
control required for normal text reading. Each sentence contains 60 the test result. Of note, with criterion 2, the larger sentence was
characters (including a space between each word and at the end of preferred in 69 of 75 eyes presenting such a discrepancy.
each line) printed as three lines with even left and right margins. The A paired t-test was used to test for equality of means of measures
vocabulary used in the sentences is selected from high-frequency obtained with charts 1 and 2 of ETDRS and MNREAD. The intraclass
words that appear in second- and third-grade reading material. The correlation coefficient (ICC) was obtained as an index of reliability for
charts contain sentences with 19 different print sizes. From the rec- continuous variables.22 Error of measurement was calculated as the
ommended viewing distance of 40 cm (16 in.) the print size ranges square root of the error mean square from each ANOVA model.22
from ⫹1.3 to ⫺0.5 logMAR (Snellen equivalents 20/400 –20/6). This According to Arditi and Cagenello7 the standard deviation of the
range can be extended by using a shorter or longer viewing distance. distribution of the discrepancies between test and retest is equal to the
The MNREAD charts have already been used in clinical and low-vision SE of measurement times 公2. This value must then be multiplied by
research.14 –18 ⫾1.96 to obtain 95% confidence limits for the agreement between test
To evaluate the reliability of the measurement, MNREAD charts 1 and retest.
and 2 were tested on the same subject consecutively. The two charts Univariate regression was used to evaluate whether the absolute
have the same layout but contain different sentences. Each child was value of the difference between test and retest changed with grade or
asked to use the right eye to read one chart and the left eye to read the with refractive error. Both a linear and a quadratic association with
other. This design allowed us to evaluate whether laterality was asso- grade were assessed. Because subjects were the unit of analysis in
ciated with reading performance. To our knowledge, this question has
these models, spherical equivalent and cylinder power were recorded
not been addressed in psychophysical studies evaluating the associa-
as the average of the two eyes.
tion of lateral preferences with reading.19 –21 The order of chart pre-
To evaluate the effect of school grade on ETDRS visual acuity and
sentation was alternated in consecutive subjects, as shown in Table 1,
reading ability, a generalized least square (GLS) regression model was
to avoid an association between chart, eye, and order of presentation.
used to take into account the correlation between multiple measures in
Distance visual acuity was obtained with the ETDRS charts and
the same individual as a random effect. Multivariate models were then
recorded in logMAR for all subjects. Charts 1 and 2 were presented
using the same method as for the MNREAD charts. Because the ETDRS fitted in which other variables were evaluated, such as age, sex,
acuity score is based on the number of letters correctly identified, the refractive error (spherical equivalent and cylinder power of each eye),
true unit of measure is 0.02 logMAR, because the difference in visual chart (1 vs. 2), eye (right versus left), and order of presentation (first
angle between two adjacent lines is 0.1 logMAR and there are five versus second during individual test).
letters per line. GLS regression was also adopted to evaluate whether a significant
The difference in size between two MNREAD sentences is 0.1 slope existed in reading speed from larger to smallest sentences of the
logMAR. Each sentence comprises 60 characters, including spaces, MNREAD charts. Again, a random effect was attributed to intraindi-
corresponding to 10 average-length words each worth 0.01 logMAR. vidual correlation. Cronbach’s ␣ was used as an index of the measure
RA, or the smallest print size at which some words can still be of internal consistency of reading speed of sentences within each
recognized, can be approximated at 0.01 logMAR after subtracting chart.
the errors. These words are either missed or misread and are Statistical analyses were performed on computer (Stata, ver. 8.1;
typically clustered between CPS and acuity limit. The following Stata Corp., College Station, TX).

TABLE 2. Mean (SD) ETDRS Acuity and MNREAD Variables for the Entire Sample

ETDRS Visual Acuity RA CPS MRS


(logMAR) (logMAR) (logMAR) (logWPM)

Mean for chart 1 (SD) 0.074 (0.091) 0.026 (0.093) 0.244 (0.097) 2.101 (0.122)
Mean for chart 2 (SD) 0.054 (0.089) 0.023 (0.089) 0.234 (0.097) 2.105 (0.130)
Mean difference (95% CL) 0.020 (0.007; 0.033) 0.003 (⫺0.009; 0.015) 0.010 (⫺0.010; 0.028) ⫺0.004 (⫺0.011; 0.004)
Paired-t-test 0.004 0.640 0.259 0.322

Downloaded From: https://iovs.arvojournals.org/ on 08/20/2018


IOVS, September 2004, Vol. 45, No. 9 Evaluation of Reading Acuity with MNREAD Charts 3351

TABLE 3. Measurement Error for Visual Acuity and Reading Ability by School Grade

Grade

3 4 5 6 7 8 Overall
(n ⴝ 26) (n ⴝ 22) (n ⴝ 22) (n ⴝ 16) (n ⴝ 20) (n ⴝ 11) (n ⴝ 116)

ETDRS visual acuity logMAR ⫾0.130 ⫾0.103 ⫾0.180 ⫾0.170 ⫾0.138 ⫾0.161 ⫾0.146
RA (logMAR) ⫾0.163 ⫾0.124 ⫾0.134 ⫾0.126 ⫾0.104 ⫾0.109 ⫾0.132
CPS (logMAR) ⫾0.163 ⫾0.221 ⫾0.244 ⫾0.143 ⫾0.136 ⫾0.221 ⫾0.193
MRS (logWPM) ⫾0.069 ⫾0.094 ⫾0.065 ⫾0.069 ⫾0.083 ⫾0.075 ⫾0.077

Data are the 95% limits of agreement.

RESULTS smallest print size for sentences used to compute MRS. Only
the 10 largest sentences were included in this analysis (1.3– 0.4
Reliability of Measurement logMAR at the test distance of 40 cm) so as to avoid print size
Table 2 presents the mean and the standard deviation of the close to CPS, which obviously would have been read slowly.
measures obtained with ETDRS charts 1 and 2 for RA, CPS, and Although a significant linear decrease of ⫺0.002 logWPM (95%
MRS. As seen in the table, the test–retest means were signifi- CL ⫺0.001; ⫺0.003 logWPM) was predicted for one smaller
cantly different for ETDRS acuity, but not for measures ob- sentence (corresponding to 0.1-logMAR steps), it was clinically
tained with the MNREAD charts. In any case, mean differences unimportant, as it predicted a decline of only 4% in WPM from
were small and clinically unimportant, as indicated by their the largest sentence to the smallest one. Thus, the observation
95% confidence limits. The ICCs for ETDRS acuity (0.66, 95% that reading speed is stable across sentences of different size,
CL: 0.55; 0.73) and RA (0.73, 95% CL: 0.65; 0.82) were similar until the CPS is reached, is confirmed in children. The grade
and were consistent with good reliability. CPS yielded a lower variable was neither a confounder nor an effect modifier of this
ICC (0.49, 95% CL: 0.35; 0.63). On the other hand, reliability estimate, although it influenced absolute values of MRS. This
for MRS was very high (0.95, 95% CL: 0.93; 0.97). result reinforces the choice of averaging the reading speed of
The 95% confidence limits for agreement between test and several sentences to obtain MRS.
retest in one subject were ⫾0.136 logMAR for ETDRS acuity, To provide further support for the internal reliability of the
⫾0.142 logMAR for RA, and ⫾ 0.193 logMAR for CPS. MRS chart structure, the Cronbach ␣ was calculated for the same
was ⫾ 0.077 logWPM, which corresponds to ⫾19% change in sentences used in the regression model. Chart 1 yielded ␣ ⫽
WPM. 0.96 and chart 2 ␣ ⫽ 0.97, indicating that the internal consis-
Table 3 shows 95% limits of agreement in the overall sample tency of the chart was very high with respect to speed mea-
as well as by school-grade. Tables 4 and 5 present them for surement. When all chart sentences are considered, corre-
categories of spherical equivalent and cylinder power. Test– sponding ␣ values were still very high: 0.93 and 0.95,
retest differences were not associated with grade or refractive respectively.
error. Only a tendency of RA to have narrower limits with
increasing grade approached statistical significance (P ⫽
0.075). Influence of School Grade on Performance
Evaluating the relationship between grade and performance
Performance of Single Sentences within (Fig. 2) was one of the main objectives of the study and was
MNREAD Charts explored with univariate regression analysis. Because one
Figure 1 shows mean reading speed of all sentences in charts 1 grade means one additional year of education, it was consid-
and 2 plotted as the mean reading speed of each sentence by ered a discrete and quantitative variable, rather than a categor-
six groups of children in the same grade. For print size larger ical one.
than CPS, 0.4 logMAR or more, the mean reading speed of each Regression analysis predicted an improvement of ETDRS
sentences was substantially similar. It can be noted that reading acuity by ⫺0.016 logMAR (95% CL: ⫺0.025; ⫺0.007 logMAR,
speed increased with school grade, as described later. We P ⬍ 0.001) per grade, corresponding to a difference of approx-
evaluated whether reading speed declined from largest to imately 1 line from grades 3 through 8.

TABLE 4. Measurement Error for Visual Acuity and Reading Ability by Quartile of Spherical Equivalent

Quartile

1 2 3 4 Overall
(n ⴝ 29) (n ⴝ 28) (n ⴝ 29) (n ⴝ 30) (n ⴝ 116)

Median of quartiles (D)


(minimum/maximum) ⫺3.3 (⫺7.9/⫺2.1) ⫺1.1 (⫺2/⫺0.1) ⫹0.5 (0/⫹0.8) ⫹1.8 (⫹0.9/⫹4.6)
ETDRS visual acuity
(logMAR) ⫾0.166 ⫾0.145 ⫾0.131 ⫾0.142 ⫾0.146
RA (logMAR) ⫾0.111 ⫾0.152 ⫾0.119 ⫾0.141 ⫾0.132
CPS (logMAR) ⫾0.189 ⫾0.166 ⫾0.241 ⫾0.164 ⫾0.193
MRS (logWPM) ⫾0.079 ⫾0.063 ⫾0.095 ⫾0.066 ⫾0.077

Data for RA, CPS, and MRS are 95% limits of agreement.
* Mean spherical equivalent of each individual.

Downloaded From: https://iovs.arvojournals.org/ on 08/20/2018


3352 Virgili et al. IOVS, September 2004, Vol. 45, No. 9

TABLE 5. Measurement Error for Visual Acuity and Reading Ability by Lovie-Kitchin and Brown4 provided data on age-related
Level of Cylinder Power changes of RA using the Bailey-Lovie near word chart in adults
aged 21 to 68. The mean value was 0.044 logMAR (SD: 0.057)
Cylinder Power
and it declined by 0.016 logMAR per decade. The standard
deviations of RA increased with age, but this was believed to be
<1 D >1 D Overall
(n ⴝ 87) (n ⴝ 29) (n ⴝ 116) the result of imperfect near correction, and overall test vari-
ability was not greatly influenced by age in their sample.
Median cylinder power (D) Overall, the 95% confidence limits for RA were ⫾0.114 log-
(minimum/maximum) 0.3 (0/0.9) 1.4 (1.0/3.4) MAR. Practice effect was also modest in this study—approxi-
ETDRS visual acuity mately ⫺0.013 logMAR improvement between the first and the
(logMAR) ⫾0.139 ⫾0.167 ⫾0.146 fifth of a series of measurements.
RA (logMAR) ⫾0.124 ⫾0.152 ⫾0.132 Camparini et al.23 conducted a study on measurement error
CPS (logMAR) ⫾0.190 ⫾0.199 ⫾0.193
MRS (logWPM) ⫾0.078 ⫾0.074 ⫾0.077 comparing full versus fast procedure for visual acuity measure-
ment using the ETDRS charts in Italian subjects. Confidence
Data are as described in Table 4. limits at the 95% level were approximately eight letters, or 1.6
* Mean cylinder power of each individual was used to group lines, for the full procedure. However, approximately one third
subjects. of the patients in the sample were affected by some form of eye
disease, and so these data cannot be assumed to be valid for
normal Italian adults.
A linear term (P ⫽ 0.003) and a quadratic term (P ⫽ 0.051)
were retained in the model evaluating the association of RA
with grade, accounting for a larger improvement of RA in early
grades and a tendency toward stabilization at higher grades.
The predicted difference in RA from grades 3 to 8 was ⫺0.156
logMAR (95% CL: ⫺0.224; ⫺0.088 logMAR)—approximately
1.5 sentences.
CPS increased linearly with grade by ⫺0.014 logMAR (95%
CL: ⫺0.023; ⫺0.005 logMAR, P ⫽ 0.003).
Grade was also a strong predictor of RS, accounting for 48%
of its variance. It increased by 0.052 logWPM (95% CL: 0.042;
0.062 logWPM, P ⬍ 0.001) per grade, corresponding to a 13%
increase in WPM and to an increase of the mean value from 96
to 167 WPM from grades 3 through 8.

Association of Other Variables with Performance


The association of performance with grade was strong in mul-
tivariate models, in which its coefficients were similar to those
from univariate analyses. RA worsened by approximately 0.029
logMAR for each additional diopter of astigmatism (95% CL:
0.017; 0.045 logMAR, P ⫽ 0.001). This value is almost identical
with that found for ETDRS visual acuity (0.032 logMAR, 95%
CL: 0.017; 0.048 logMAR, P ⬍ 0.001). Spherical equivalent
showed no association with any other independent variables
when grade was considered. CPS was found to be slightly
better in the left eye than the right eye (⫺0.021 logMAR, 95%
CL: ⫺0.003; ⫺0.038 logMAR; P ⫽ 0.021). No relationship
between reading speed and other covariates was found when
grade was considered in the model. Age was never as good a
predictor as grade. A small difference in visual acuity was
confirmed for ETDRS chart 1 versus chart 2, as seen in Table 2.

DISCUSSION
Several investigators have studied the reliability of visual acuity
measurement in adults.4 –9 In most studies, they found that
test–retest error is within 0.04 to 0.06 logMAR. Thus, a change
of 1 line or 0.1 logMAR is suggested to be clinically significant
in normal adults. A modest decrease in performance with older
age has been shown.4,10
A recent study12 has assessed the reliability of visual acuity
measurement with the ETDRS charts in children. Based on the
95% limits of agreement, the criterion for a statistically signif-
icant change in VA is no more than ⫾0.15 logMAR. These limits
were not found to be associated with age, but there was a small
difference between sexes, with boys approximately two letters FIGURE 1. Mean reading speed of all sentences in the MNREAD charts
more variable than girls. 1 (top) and 2 (bottom), across six groups of children in the same grade.

Downloaded From: https://iovs.arvojournals.org/ on 08/20/2018


IOVS, September 2004, Vol. 45, No. 9 Evaluation of Reading Acuity with MNREAD Charts 3353

FIGURE 2. Box plots of ETDRS acu-


ity (top left), RA (top right), CPS (bot-
tom left), and MRS (bottom right), by
grade.

In this study, a change of 1.5 lines was significant for ETDRS could be obtained using sentences of any size, provided that
acuity, which is slightly larger than the limit allowed for adults they are larger than the CPS.
in published reports on English-speaking people4 –9 and is In this study, one eye was used for test and the fellow eye
identical with that obtained by Manny et al.12 in English- for retest to assess whether reading with right versus left eye
speaking children aged 6 to 11 years. affects performance. Although this may cause the error esti-
The MNREAD charts allowed a precise measure of RA. In mate to include true interocular differences, these were likely
logMAR units, their reliability was at least as good as that of the to be small because subjects were best refracted and were
ETDRS charts for normal children, beginning at grade 3. The included only if no anomaly of the visual system was found.
reliability of MRS measurement was the highest, whereas that Moreover, even a small systematic bias would have been in the
of CPS measurement was the lowest, although still acceptable. direction of a larger error, rather than toward its underestima-
This could be because CPS is recorded in discrete steps of 0.1 tion. No difference of performance of right versus left eye was
logMAR—that is, with a larger approximation than other read- found for all measured variables, except for CPS which was
ing variables. The relatively low reliability coefficient for acuity better in the left eyes. This difference was clinically unimpor-
measures also results from a “range effect,” because vision was tant and could represent a chance finding. The confidence
restricted within a narrow normal range. limits of RA using the MNREAD charts were similar to those of
Both VA with the ETDRS charts and reading performance VA measured with the ETDRS charts, which are currently the
increased with grade. Whereas measures of acuity improved standard for distance acuity measure. They are also close to the
only slightly, MRS increased markedly from grades 3 to 8, in values found in English-speaking adults by Bailey and Lovie11
accordance with the data previously published by Taylor24 and using a Bailey-Lovie word chart. No effect of practice was
reviewed by Carver.25 Although a trend toward stabilization detected on the second test. Gender and age did not affect
was shown graphically for most measures, a quadratic term performance when grade was taken into account.
was retained in the model only for RA, indicating its tendency When results of reliability studies are considered, the char-
toward a plateau at grade 8. It is possible that the number of acteristics of the sample are relevant for their generalizability.
subjects in each grade was too small to detect a trend toward In this study, children were collected in an area in which
stabilization of performance for other variables. It should also industrial, commercial, and agricultural activities all contribute
be observed that a cross-sectional study is not the most appro- to provide one of the highest average incomes in Italy. The
priate design to obtain a precise estimate of the progression of institution in which the study was conducted is the only
reading ability with age, which would be best achieved with high-volume eye-care service for children in this area. Its ser-
longitudinal studies. vices are largely covered by the public healthcare system, so
Increasing astigmatism was found to be associated with that access is not limited by family income. Children are thus
lower RA. A similar association with VA suggests that there representative of a well-educated population with income lev-
could be some residual optical blur, despite accurate refrac- els close to those of other industrialized Western countries.
tion. No association was found between astigmatism and MRS, Of course, the possibility of generalizing results to other
as could be expected if mild blur is the only limiting factor. countries may be limited by the language. This may be partly
Both regression analysis and Cronbach’s ␣ indicate that overcome by the use of the ETDRS charts, a worldwide stan-
different sentences in the MNREAD charts provided very sim- dard tool, as a reference method for visual acuity testing. The
ilar reading speeds for one individual, despite the fact that each reliability of the ETDRS charts was similar to that found in a
sentence contained only 60 characters. This finding supports recent U.S. study.12 Limitation to near normal acuity restricts
the decision of averaging over reading speed of several sen- the generalization of the findings. Not only is true error ex-
tences to obtain MRS. It also suggests that estimates of MRS pected to be larger as acuity worsens, but with the MNREAD

Downloaded From: https://iovs.arvojournals.org/ on 08/20/2018


3354 Virgili et al. IOVS, September 2004, Vol. 45, No. 9

charts, there are also fewer data above CPS on which to 9. Elliott DB, Sheridan M. The use of accurate visual acuity measure-
estimate maximum reading rate. This would increase measure- ments in clinical anti-cataract formulation trials. Ophthalmic
ment error when estimating MRS and CPS. Physiol Opt. 1988;8:397– 401.
Present results encourage more research on the validity of 10. Elliott DB, Yang KC, Whitaker D. Visual acuity changes throughout
the MNREAD charts for assessing reading performance. In adulthood in normal, healthy eyes: seeing beyond 6/6. Optom Vis
particular, reliability of measures from different examiners and Sci. 1995;72:186 –191.
among low-vision patients should to be assessed. Ideally, stud- 11. Bailey IL, Lovie JE. The design and use of a near-vision chart. Am J
Optom Physiol Opt. 1980;57:378 –387.
ies to evaluate the reliability in patients with similar diseases
12. Manny RE, Hussein M, Gwiazda J, Marsh-Tootle W. Repeatability of
and visual loss should be conducted in different countries,
ETDRS visual acuity in children. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003;
because MNREAD charts are already available in various lan- 44:3294 –3300.
guages and more are being developed. The ultimate goal of 13. Ahn JS, Legge GE, Luebker A. Printed cards for measuring low-
such studies would be to test whether the MNREAD charts can vision reading speed. Vision Res. 1995;35:1939 –1944.
be a valid translinguistic tool. This would help multicenter 14. Ahn SJ, Legge GE. Psychophysics of reading. XIII: Clinical predic-
low-vision research and facilitate the adoption of reading per- tors of magnifier-aided reading speed in low vision. Vision Res.
formance as an outcome measure in clinical research, given the 1995;35:1931–1938.
fact that reading is a primary activity of daily life. 15. Ohlsson J, Villarreal G, Sjostrom A, Abrahamsson M, Sjostrand J.
Screening for amblyopia and strabismus with the Lang II stereo
Acknowledgments card. Acta Ophthalmol Scand. 2002;80:163–166.
The authors thank Greta Codutti, Simonetta Geotti, and Marta Makuc 16. Ho AC, Maguire MG, Yoken J, et al., the Choroidal Neovascular-
(Neurophthalmology Unit, IRCCS, E. Medea, La Nostra Famiglia, Polo ization Prevention Trial Research Group. Laser-induced drusen
reduction improves visual function at one year. Ophthalmology.
Friuli Venezia Giulia, Italy) for examining subjects and for managing
1999;106:1367–1374.
study data; and Rodolfo Saracci (Director), and Franco Merletti, Euge-
17. Fujikado T, Asonuma S, Ohji M, et al. Reading ability after macular
nio Paci, Emanuele Crocetti and Annibale Biggeri, (teachers) in the
translocation surgery with 360-degree retinotomy. Am J Ophthal-
Masters Program in Epidemiology (University of Turin, Italy) for com- mol. 2002;134:849 – 856.
ments on the manuscript. 18. Tejeria L, Harper RA, Artes PH, Dickinson CM. Face recognition in
age related macular degeneration: perceived disability, measured
References disability, and performance with a bioptic device. Br J Ophthal-
1. Legge GE, Pelli DG, Rubin GS, Schleske MM. Psychophysics of mol. 2002;86:1019 –1026.
reading. I. Normal vision. Vision Res. 1985;25:239 –252. 19. Longoni AM, Scalisi TG, Grilli M. Lateral preference and verbal
2. Legge GE, Rubin GS, Pelli DG, Schleske MM. Psychophysics of skills: a survey in school settings. Int J Neurosci. 1989;44:41–52.
reading. II. Low vision. Vision Res. 1985;25:253–265. 20. Steenhuis RE, Bryden MP, Schroeder DH. Gender, laterality, learn-
3. Raasch TW, Bailey IL, Bullimore MA. Repeatability of visual acuity ing difficulties and health problems. Neuropsychologia. 1993;31:
measurement. Optom Vis Sci. 1998;75:342–348. 1243–1254.
4. Lovie-Kitchin JE, Brown B. Repeatability and intercorrelations of 21. Biersner RJ, Larocco JM. Determinants of reading performance and
standard vision tests as a function of age. Optom Vis Sci. 2000;77: achievement. Percept Mot Skills. 1980;50:715–721.
412– 420. 22. Armstrong BK, White E, Saracci R. Validity and reliability studies.
5. Bailey IL, Lovie JE. New design principles for visual acuity letter In: Armstrong BK, White E, Saracci R, eds. Principles of Exposure
charts. Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1976;53:740 –745. Measurement in Epidemiology. New York: Oxford University
6. Bailey IL, Bullimore MA, Raasch TW, Taylor HR. Clinical grading Press; 1992;78 –114.
and the effect of scaling. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1991;32:422– 23. Camparini M, Cassinari P, Ferrigno L, Macaluso C. ETDRS-Fast:
432. implementing psychophysical adaptive methods to standardized
7. Arditi A, Cagniello R. On the statistical reliability of letter-chart visual acuity measurement with ETDRS charts. Invest Ophthalmol
visual acuity measurement. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993;34: Vis Sci. 2001;42:1226 –1231.
120 –129. 24. Taylor SE. Eye movements in reading: facts and fallacies. Am Educ
8. Vanden Bosch ME, Wall M. Visual acuity scored by the letter-by- Res J. 1965;2:187–202.
letter or probit methods has lower retest variability than the line 25. Carver RP. Reading Rate: A Review of Research and Theory. San
assignment method. Eye. 1997;11:411– 417. Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1990.

Downloaded From: https://iovs.arvojournals.org/ on 08/20/2018

You might also like