You are on page 1of 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/286526146

Resilience and Positive Psychology

Chapter · March 2014


DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4614-9608-3_7

CITATIONS READS

72 18,934

3 authors, including:

Suniya S. Luthar Emily L. Lyman


Teachers College Columbia University
185 PUBLICATIONS   20,540 CITATIONS    3 PUBLICATIONS   110 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Resilience in the face of adversity View project

Youth in high-achieving schools View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Suniya S. Luthar on 16 December 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Resilience and Positive Psychology
7
Suniya S. Luthar, Emily L. Lyman,
and Elizabeth J. Crossman

Since its introduction to the scientific literature in ways in which progress in each of these areas
the mid-1990s, developmental science has seen might most usefully inform efforts in the other,
incremental refinements in research on resilience, collectively maximizing the promotion of well-
which is a process or phenomenon reflecting pos- being among individuals, families, and society.
itive child adjustment despite conditions of risk.
In this chapter, we describe accumulated evi-
dence on this construct in the field of develop- Historical Overview of Childhood
mental psychopathology and appraise critical Resilience Research
directions for future work. We begin by briefly
describing the history of work in this area through The roots of resilience research can be traced back
contemporary times, defining core constructs, to pioneering research with children of schizo-
and summarizing major findings on factors asso- phrenics during the 1960s and 1970s. Garmezy
ciated with resilience. In the second half of the (1974), along with Anthony (1974) and Rutter
chapter, we examine commonalities and differ- (1979), found that among these children at high
ences between the resilience framework and a risk for psychopathology was a subset of children
related, relatively new area of scientific inquiry: who had surprisingly healthy patterns. Their sci-
positive psychology. Our objective is to elucidate entific interest in the positive outcomes of these
children reflected a notable departure from the
symptom-based medical models of the time.
Expanding the research on resilience beyond
children of mentally ill parents, Murphy and
Moriarty (1976) examined vulnerability and cop-
ing patterns in children exposed to naturally
occurring stressors such as deaths or injuries in
S.S. Luthar, Ph.D. (*)
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, the family. Shortly after, Emmy Werner pub-
900S. McAllister Rd. Tempe, AZ 85287-1104, USA lished the first of many articles on the birth cohort
e-mail: Suniya.Luthar@asu.edu from 1954 from the Hawaiian island of Kauai
E.L. Lyman, M.A. (Werner & Smith, 1982, 1992, 2001). Werner
Department of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, observed a number of protective factors that
Teachers College, Columbia University,
distinguished well-functioning at-risk youth
New York, NY 10027, USA
from those faring more poorly, including strong,
E.J. Crossman, M.A.
supportive ties with the family, informal support
Department of Human Development,
Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, systems outside the home, and dispositional
NY 10027, USA attributes such as sociability.

M. Lewis and K.D. Rudolph (eds.), Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology, 125


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-9608-3_7, © Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014
126 S.S. Luthar et al.

The 1980s and early 1990s brought several academic resilience (Obradović et al., 2009),
changes in conceptual approaches to studying emotional resilience (Jain, Buka, Subramanian,
resilience, two of which were particularly salient. & Molnar, 2012), or external (behavioral) resil-
The first concerned perspectives on the locus of ience (Yates & Grey, 2012).
resilience. In early studies in this area, the effort
had been to identify personal qualities of resilient
children, such as autonomy or belief in oneself. Research on Resilience:
As work in the area evolved, however, research- Defining Critical Constructs
ers acknowledged that resilient adaptation often
may derive from factors external to the child. As noted earlier, resilience is defined as a phe-
Thus, three sets of factors came to be commonly nomenon or process reflecting relatively positive
cited as central to the development of resilience: adaptation despite experiences of significant
attributes of the children themselves, aspects of adversity or trauma. Because resilience is a
their families, and characteristics of their wider superordinate construct subsuming two distinct
social environments (Garmezy & Masten, 1986; dimensions—significant adversity and positive
Rutter, 1987; Werner & Smith, 1982). adaptation—it is never directly measured, but
The second change involved conceptions of rather is indirectly inferred based on evidence of
resilience as potentially fluctuating over time rather the two subsumed constructs.
than fixed. In some early writings, those who did
well despite multiple risks were labeled “invulner-
able” (Anthony, 1974). Recognizing that this term Adversity
implied that risk evasion was absolute and
unchanging, researchers gradually began to use the In developmental psychopathology research on
more qualified term “resilience” instead. Implicit resilience, risk or adversity is defined in terms of
in this change of terminology was the recognition statistical probabilities: A high-risk condition is
that positive adaptation despite adversity is never one that carries high odds for measured malad-
permanent; rather, it is a developmental progres- justment in critical domains (Luthar, 2006;
sion with new vulnerabilities and strengths emerg- Masten, 2001). Exposure to community violence
ing with changing life circumstances (Garmezy & or to maternal depression, for example, consti-
Masten, 1986; Werner & Smith, 1992). tutes high risk given that children experiencing
Another critical qualifier rested in the recogni- each of these factors reflect significantly greater
tion that resilience is never an across-the-board maladjustment than those who do not. Aside
phenomenon, but can be, and often is, domain from discrete risk dimensions such as community
specific. Much as children in general do not man- violence or parent psychopathology, researchers
ifest uniformly positive or negative adaptation have also examined composites of multiple risk
across different areas of adjustment, researchers indices, such as parents’ low income and educa-
cautioned that at-risk children too can display tion, histories of mental illness, and disorganiza-
remarkable strengths in some areas while show- tion in neighborhoods. Seminal research by
ing notable deficits in others (Luthar, Doernberger, Rutter (1979) demonstrated that when risks such
& Zigler, 1993). as these coexist (as they often do, in the real
Most importantly, children under stress could world), effects tend to be synergistic, with child
seem resilient in terms of their behaviors while outcomes being far poorer than when any of these
still struggling with inner distress in the form of risks exists in isolation.
problems, such as depression and anxiety (Farber
& Egeland, 1987; Luthar, 1991). Recognizing the
heterogeneity in adjustment levels across Positive Adaptation
domains, scientists now tend to use more
circumspect terms that specify domains in which The second component in the construct of resil-
resilience is manifest, referring, for example, to ience is positive adjustment: outcomes that are
7 Resilience and Positive Psychology 127

substantially better than what would be expected, family unit. In a similar vein, the label resilience
given exposure to a specific identified risk. In many can sometimes be most appropriate for communi-
studies of resilience across diverse risk circum- ties of well-functioning at-risk youth. Research
stances, this concept has been defined in terms of on neighborhoods, for example, has demon-
behaviorally manifested social competence or strated that some low-income urban neighbor-
success at meeting stage-salient developmental hoods reflect far higher levels of cohesiveness,
tasks (Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000; Masten organization, and social efficacy than others (Jain
& Tellegen, 2012). Among young children, for et al., 2012; Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000),
example, competence is often operationally with the potential, therefore, to serve as important
defined in terms of manifest secure attachment buffers against negative socializing influences.
with caregivers, and among older children, in As positive adaptation does not necessarily
terms of aspects of school-based functioning. occur as part of a continuous trajectory, an impor-
In addition to being developmentally appro- tant area of resilience research is concerned with
priate, indicators used to define “positive adapta- those who “bounce back” from earlier dysfunction
tion” must also be conceptually of high relevance (Luthar & Brown, 2007; Masten, 2001; Rutter,
to the risk examined in terms of both domains 2012). Long-term prospective studies have been
assessed and stringency of criteria used (Luthar, invaluable in identifying critical turning points not
2006; Vanderbilt-Adriance & Shaw, 2008). When only in childhood but also across the life span,
communities carry many risks for antisocial illuminating instances where apparently negative
problems, for example, it makes sense to assess adjustment trajectories were transformed into pos-
the degree to which children are able to maintain itive, healthy ones (Hauser, Allen, & Golden,
socially conforming behaviors (Jain et al., 2012), 2006; Sampson & Laub, 1993; Vaillant, 2012).
whereas among children of depressed parents, As we define terms, it is important to distinguish
the absence of depressive diagnoses would be of resilience from two related—and, in error, often
special significance (Beardslee, Gladstone, & conflated—constructs: competence and ego resil-
O’Connor, 2012). With regard to stringency of iency. Competence and resilience may be described
criteria, similarly, decisions must depend on the as closely related subconstructs as both represent
seriousness of the risks under consideration. In positive adaptation, but there are four major differ-
studying children facing major traumas, it is ences (Luthar, 2006; Yates & Masten, 2004). First,
entirely appropriate to define risk evasion simply resilience, but not competence, presupposes risk.
in terms of the absence of serious psychopathol- Second, resilience encompasses both negative and
ogy rather than superiority or excellence in every- positive adjustment indices (absence of disorder
day adaptation (Luthar et al., 2000; Rutter, 2012). and presence of health), and competence chiefly
Whereas approaches to measuring risk can reflects the latter. Third, resilient outcomes are
involve one negative circumstance, competence defined in terms of emotional and behavioral indi-
must necessarily be defined across multiple ces, whereas competence usually involves only
spheres, for overly narrow definitions can convey manifest, observable behaviors. Finally, resilience
a misleading picture of success in the face of is a superordinate construct that subsumes aspects
adversity [for a more in-depth discussion, see of competence (along with high levels of risk).
Luthar (2006)]. Furthermore, it should be noted A second overlapping construct—and one
that in some situations, competence is most with which resilience is frequently confused
appropriately operationalized in terms of better (Luthar et al., 2000)—is ego resiliency, a con-
than expected functioning of families or commu- struct developed by Block and Block (1980) that
nities, rather than the children themselves. To refers to a personal trait reflecting general
illustrate, toddlers are still too young to reliably resourcefulness, sturdiness of character, and flex-
be judged as manifesting resilience because their ibility in response to environmental circum-
functioning is largely regulated by others; thus, it stances. Commonalities with resilience are that
is more logical to operationalize positive both involve strengths. Differences are that
adjustment in terms of the mother–child dyad or (a) only resilience presupposes conditions of risk
128 S.S. Luthar et al.

and (b) resilience is a phenomenon, not a person- Variable-based analyses such as multivariate
ality trait. Finally, just as competence is subsumed regressions allow researchers to look at continu-
within resilience, ego resiliency has been exam- ous scales of (a) adversity and (b) risk modifiers
ined as a potential predictor of resilient adapta- in relation to outcomes, examining how the latter
tion, that is, as a trait that could protect individuals are directly related (as main effects), and in inter-
against stressful experiences (Cicchetti & action effects with the former. One of the first
Rogosch, 1997; Eisenberg et al., 2010). efforts to use this variable-based approach was
In developmental psychopathology research, the groundbreaking paper by Garmezy, Masten,
it is critical that scientists proactively guard and Tellegen (1984), demonstrating that high IQ
against any suggestions that resilience is essen- was protective: Increases in life stress seemed to
tially a personal trait, as this can foster perspec- affect intelligent children far less than their low
tives that blame the victim (Luthar & Brown, IQ peers. Person-based analyses in resilience
2007; Yates & Masten, 2004). Toward this end, research, on the other hand, involve comparisons
several precautions have been noted for future between a group of children who are categorized
studies (Luthar et al., 2000; Rutter, 2012). Most according to their outcome and risk profiles. For
importantly, all reports should include clear defi- example, comparisons of two groups of at-risk
nitions of resilience, unequivocally stating that it youth, manifesting high and low competence
refers to a process or phenomenon and not a trait. respectively, can illuminate critical factors that
Additionally, it is best to avoid using the term confer protection against adversity.
resiliency, which carries the connotation of a per- In both variable- and person-based analyses, a
sonality characteristic even more so than does hallmark of the current generation of resilience
resilience. Furthermore, it is prudent to avoid research is attention to process: If studies are
using the term resilient as an adjective for indi- truly to be informative to interventions, they must
viduals and apply it instead to profiles or trajecto- move beyond simply identifying variables linked
ries because phrases such as “resilient adaptation” with competence toward understanding the spe-
carry no suggestion of who (the child or others) is cific underlying mechanisms (Luthar, 2006;
responsible for manifest risk evasion. Masten & Cicchetti, 2012). This need to unravel
underlying processes applies to risk, vulnerabil-
ity, and protective factors at multiple levels. With
Vulnerability and Protective regard to risk transmission, for example, maternal
Processes depression can affect children through various
environmental processes including negative fam-
The central objective of resilience researchers is ily interactions and routines, and child behavioral
to identify vulnerability and protective factors that and emotional problems (Valdez, Mills, Barrueco,
might modify the negative effects of adverse life Leis, & Riley, 2011). Similarly, protective factors
circumstances, and then to identify mechanisms such as high-quality caregiver–child relation-
or processes that might underlie associations ships could benefit a child through multiple path-
found. Vulnerability factors or markers encom- ways including feelings of being supported, a
pass those indices that exacerbate the ill effects of sense of being cherished as an individual, and a
the adverse condition (e.g., poverty) on child out- strong set of personal values (Werner, 2012).
comes, such as alienation from parents or a nega-
tive school climate. Promotive or protective factors
are those that modify the effects of risk in a positive What Promotes or Mitigates Resilient
direction. Examples include support from caregiv- Adaptation? Evidence on Salient Risk
ers and peers and strong social-emotional skills. Modifiers
In the resilience literature, there have been
two major approaches to identifying protective or The science of resilience is, fundamentally,
vulnerability factors (or risk modifiers): variable- applied in nature with the central goal of inform-
based and person-based statistical analyses. ing efficacious interventions (Garmezy &
7 Resilience and Positive Psychology 129

Masten, 1986; Luthar, 2006); accordingly, in associated with greater severity and chronicity of
reviewing evidence on risk modifiers, it makes maltreatment, as well as early age of onset
sense to prioritize domains in terms of overall (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997; Kim, Cicchetti,
likelihood of yielding benefits in interventions Rogosch, & Manly, 2009). In terms of protective
(Luthar & Brown, 2007). In other words, it is processes, positive relationships with peers and
most useful to focus primarily on risk modifiers high school engagement can mitigate the delete-
that are (a) the most influential, with effects that rious effects of maltreatment (Afifi & MacMillan,
are relatively enduring or robust, and (b) rela- 2011; Williams & Nelson-Gardell, 2012). At the
tively modifiable (as are aspects of caregivers’ same time, research has suggested that even when
functioning, as opposed to intrinsic characteris- maltreated children function well at some critical
tics, such as IQ or genetic vulnerability). periods in time, this successful adaptation tends
With this prioritization in mind, we present, in to be unstable across development (Thompson &
sequence, findings on risk modifiers within the Tabone, 2010).
domains of the family—the most proximal and As maltreatment thwarts resilient adaptation,
the most enduring of children’s environments— conversely, positive, supportive family relation-
followed by the community, which can affect ships are vital in maintaining good adjustment in
children directly, as well as indirectly through the face of adversities. The critical importance of
their parents. Children’s own characteristics are family relationships is recurrently emphasized in
presented third, recognizing that many of these reviews of the literature (e.g., Luthar & Brown,
risk modifiers can and often do promote resilient 2007; Masten, 2001; Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000;
adaptation, but they are often, themselves, mal- Vanderbilt-Adriance & Shaw, 2008), resonant
leable to potent forces in the proximal and distal with early reports that the presence of a close
environments (cf. Luthar, 2006). relationship with at least one parent figure consti-
tutes a potent protective factor (Garmezy, 1974;
Rutter, 1979; Werner & Smith, 1982).
Family Processes Furthermore, the protective potential of positive
parenting is evident not only in early childhood
Of the many factors that affect the trajectories of but in later years as well, through adolescence
at-risk individuals, among the most powerful is and even emerging adulthood (Burt & Paysnick,
maltreatment by primary caregivers. Maltreatment 2012; Steinberg, 2001).
co-occurs with many high-risk circumstances Although maternal nurturance is widely dis-
including parent mental illnesses, parental con- cussed as critical for positive child development,
flict, community violence, and poverty (Mersky, high-quality relationships with other family
Berger, Reynolds, & Gromoske, 2009; Rogosch, members can also significantly modify the effects
Dackis, & Cicchetti, 2011), thus serving as a of adversity. For example, studies have estab-
widespread vulnerability factor. Maltreated chil- lished the protective potential of strong attach-
dren show deficits spanning multiple domains ment relationships with fathers and father figures
including interpersonal relationships, emotional (Coley, 2001; Martin, Ryan, & Brooks-Gunn,
regulation, cognitive processing, and even lin- 2010). Older siblings may often serve as critical
guistic development (Cicchetti, 2002). This role models, with younger siblings mirroring
degree of dysfunction is not surprising, given that their profiles of high behavioral competence (e.g.,
maltreatment connotes serious disturbances in Brody, Kim, Murry, & Brown, 2004) and, con-
the most proximal level of the child’s ecology, versely, emulating their negative behavior pat-
with the caregiving environment failing to pro- terns involving delinquency and substance use
vide typical experiences essential for normal (Stormshak, Comeau, & Shepard, 2004). Finally,
development (Cicchetti, 2002). support from extended kin can be important in
Despite the inimical effects of maltreat- protecting at-risk youth. Among children exposed
ment, profiles of adjustment are not homoge- to harsh maternal parenting, for example, high
neous. Pronounced deficits are most likely to be levels of grandmother involvement can reduce the
130 S.S. Luthar et al.

risk of maladjustment in grandchildren (Barnett, Community Processes


Scaramella, Neppl, Ontai, & Conger, 2010).
Going beyond the general importance of As with maltreatment in the family, chronic
strong attachments with parent figures, there are exposure to violence in the community can have
also contextually salient vulnerability and protec- overwhelming deleterious effects that are diffi-
tive processes, or those that are important within cult for other positive forces to override. Exposure
particular family and cultural contexts. To illus- to violence substantially exacerbates risks for a
trate, upper-middle class American youth, in range of problems, encompassing internalizing
general, are at considerably elevated risk for sub- symptoms such as anxiety, depression, and post-
stance use, and perceived parental leniency on traumatic stress disorders (Herrenkohl, Sousa,
this front is a potent vulnerability factor for these Tajima, Herrenkohl, & Moylan, 2008; Walsh,
teens’ frequent use of alcohol, marijuana, and 2007), as well as externalizing problems such as
other substances (Luthar & Barkin, 2012). delinquent, antisocial behaviors (Aisenberg &
Among immigrant families, second-generation Herrenkohl, 2008) and attenuated academic com-
children’s revocation of traditional family values petence, social skills, and self-concept (Cedeno,
and mores can be linked with elevated adjustment Elias, Kelly, & Chu, 2010).
problems (García Coll & Marks, 2009). Among With regard to risk modifiers, support from
families affected by mental illnesses such as parents can serve protective functions but, unfor-
depression, unique protective processes include tunately, parents themselves are also highly vul-
the child’s understanding of the illness (including nerable to the stresses of chronic community
its potential causes), as well as the ability to violence (Jain et al., 2012), experiencing high
maintain healthy psychological boundaries from distress themselves and even, sometimes, dis-
the affected parent (Beardslee, 2002). playing elevated maltreatment of children
Recent years have seen an explosion of (Herrenkohl et al., 2008). Overall, the variability
research on family genetic factors in adjustment in children’s responses to community violence is
and in particular, on G × E interactions (Grigorenko likely to be least pronounced if exposure is spo-
& Cicchetti, 2012; Kim-Cohen & Turkewitz, radic rather than chronic, and if it does not involve
2012); while clearly invaluable for basic science, personally witnessing violent events or experi-
these findings are unlikely to inform psychologi- encing the loss of a friend or family member
cal interventions to foster resilience in the fore- (Gorman-Smith & Tolan, 2003).
seeable future [for a detailed discussion, see Whereas exposure to prolonged serious com-
Luthar and Brown (2007)]. Genetics research munity violence is rarely overcome by other pro-
might suggest, for some, the potential to guide tective processes, there certainly are exosystemic
treatment as an understanding of biological path- forces that can attenuate the ill effects of other
ways can inform pharmacotherapy. However, any types of adversities. In particular, studies have
such knowledge about “indicated pharmacothera- documented the benefits of early exposure to
pies” does not readily generalize to treating psy- high-quality childcare, where caregivers have
chological problems (Luthar & Brown, 2007). In positive personal characteristics and offer emo-
a recent review of relevant evidence, Dodge and tionally supportive caregiving (Maggi, Roberts,
Rutter (2011) concluded, explicitly, that the most MacLennan, & D’Angiulli, 2011). In later years
direct practical implication of the G × E revolu- as well, supportive relationships with teachers in
tion belongs to the field of personalized medicine. K-12 can be protective (Ebersöhn & Ferreira,
Furthermore, the authors reaffirmed that any such 2011). To illustrate, when teachers identify the
personalized medicine is unlikely to reduce indi- function of problem behaviors among at-risk
vidual psychopathologies as (a) G × E interac- youth and, in response, provide positive support
tions, even if replicated, tend to be very small, and strategies, there are significant benefits for adap-
(b) there is inevitably a plethora of other unmea- tive behaviors (Stoiber & Gettinger, 2011).
sured risks generated by both genes and environ- Aside from teachers, relationships with infor-
ments (Dodge & Rutter, 2011; Rutter, 2012). mal mentors also can promote resilient adaptation
7 Resilience and Positive Psychology 131

(Rhodes & Lowe, 2008). Examining the fre- In a similar vein, support gleaned from involve-
quently stressful transition from elementary to ment in religious communities can be beneficial
middle school, Van Ryzin (2010) found that 40 % (Pargament & Cummings, 2010), with the buffer-
of the children named their advisor as a secondary ing effects of religiosity on adolescent maladjust-
attachment figure. Furthermore, those who did so ment often operating by increasing social
reported greater engagement in middle school, resources and promoting prosocial behaviors
and manifested greater gains in achievement and (Sherman, Duarte, & Verdeli, 2011).
adjustment as compared to those who did not.
With regard to mediators and moderators, men-
toring effects tend to be mediated by improved Individual Attributes
family relations, while the duration and close-
ness of the relationship serve as significant mod- Intelligence is perhaps the most commonly men-
erators (DuBois, Portillo, Rhodes, Silverthorn, & tioned personal asset in promoting resilient adap-
Valentine, 2011). tation. Studies on diverse risk groups find that
Finally, positive relationships with peers can individuals with high IQs tend to fare better than
serve important ameliorative functions for at-risk others, with the underlying mechanisms poten-
children. Peer-assisted learning can result in sig- tially entailing superior problem-solving skills as
nificant increases in achievement (Neal, Neal, well as a history of successes (e.g., at school or
Atkins, Henry, & Frazier, 2011), and affiliation work) over time (Luthar, 2006; Masten, 2001).
with peers who model responsible behavior (e.g., At the same time, there is much evidence that con-
good students and good citizens) can mitigate, to tinuing adversities in the proximal environment
some degree, the effects of violence exposure can mitigate this personal asset. Young children
(Jain et al., 2012). At the same time, close friend- exposed to chronic adversities such as domestic
ships can confer vulnerability as well, particu- violence in the home or institutionalized care
larly when they entail deviant behaviors. Youth show significantly lower IQ scores than their
who affiliate with deviant peers can engage in counterparts who are not exposed to these risks
mutual “deviancy training” (Dishion, McCord, & (Koenen, Moffitt, Caspi, Taylor, & Purcell, 2003;
Poulin, 1999), resulting in poor outcomes across Rutter, 1998; Sameroff & Rosenblum, 2006).
multiple domains including conduct distur- One might argue that the protective potential
bances, substance use, and academic problems of high IQ would be more “fixed” later in devel-
(Tiet, Huizinga, & Byrnes, 2010; Véronneau & opment; although probably true, the evidence is
Dishion, 2010). not unequivocal, even at older ages. Among mul-
Moving from the relatively proximal extrafa- tiple samples of low-income adolescents (see
milial contexts of school, mentors, and peers to Luthar, 2006), intelligence was not found to be
those more distal, aspects of the neighborhood protective; on the contrary, there were sugges-
may also play an important role in buffering risk tions that bright youth may be more sensitive
for children. Particularly important are social than others to negative environmental forces.
organization processes in the neighborhood, Among adults, Fiedler (1995) reported that high-
which involve features such as high levels of IQ people showed leadership success under con-
cohesion, a sense of belonging to the community, ditions of low stress, but that when stress was
supervision of youth by community adults, and high, IQ was inversely correlated with leadership
high participation in local organizations (Rios, success. Findings such as these have been viewed
Aiken, & Zautra, 2012; Zimmerman & Brenner, as suggesting that the manifest “benefits” of
2010). Such social processes can help buffer the innate intelligence can vary substantially, depend-
impact of structural characteristics of the com- ing on the potency and chronicity of risks in the
munity such as poverty or violence (Jain et al., proximal environment.
2012), by providing, for example, opportunities The previously described evidence on intelli-
for structured and supervised extracurricular gence is paralleled by similar evidence on tem-
activities (Peck, Roeser, Zarrett, & Eccles, 2008). perament, also shown to confer protection against
132 S.S. Luthar et al.

stress, with benefits found in relation to diverse As resilience research began over 60 years ago
adjustment outcomes (e.g., Eisenberg et al., with a focus on strengths and not just disorder,
2010; Murry, Bynum, Brody, Willert, & Stephens, the field of positive psychology, christened in the
2001). Temperamental differences can be seen early 1990s, was established to address the nega-
as early as 4 months of age and they show tive bias and medicalization that suffused psy-
continuity over early childhood (e.g., Kagan, chological research since the end of the Second
Snidman, & Arcus, 1998). At the same time, the World War (Peterson & Park, 2003). As its name
manifestation of temperament can be modified suggests, positive psychology is the study of pos-
by environmental features. As Rutter (2000) has itive emotions (e.g., joy and hope), positive char-
underscored, scientists have long moved past the acter (e.g., creativity and kindness), and positive
point of assuming that “constitutional” factors institutions (e.g., family, communities, and the
are unalterable; whereas some children may tend workplace; Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000).
to be more impulsive or oppositional than others, In the decade since its inception, positive psy-
their interactions with the world contribute to chology has witnessed impressive refinements in
determining the behavioral conformity they dis- both theory and research, as exemplified most
play in everyday life. recently in a seminal edited volume designed to
Similar cautions apply to inferences about the “take stock, and move forward” (Sheldon,
positive personality traits. Shiner and Masten Kashdan, & Steger, 2011).
(2012) have demonstrated significant long-term
beneficial effects for childhood conscientious-
ness, agreeableness, and openness, as well as low Differences
neuroticism, even after controlling for childhood
adversity. Whereas these findings undoubtedly At this stage in the ontogenesis of the two fields,
indicate that personal strengths can help individ- there are some substantive differences between
uals overcome the effects of childhood life stress- positive psychology and resilience research,
ors, it is important to note also that even among among the most prominent of which is the con-
adults, positive personal attributes are typically sideration of life adversities. As noted before,
maximized only in the scaffolding of supportive studies of resilience presuppose exposure to
interpersonal contexts. Kashdan and Steger extreme adversity, whereas positive psychology
(2011) have presciently emphasized that across concerns all individuals, not just those who have
the life span, individuals can possess strengths experienced major risks [although there are now
without necessarily using them: Context is criti- increasing inroads into studies in the context of
cal in maximizing their use. We discuss this issue adversity, such as those of stress-related growth
in depth in the section that follows. (Park, 2010) and those showing that character
strengths can protect against major illness
(Peterson, Park, & Seligman, 2006)].
Resilience and Positive Psychology The second difference concerns the centrality
of developmental issues, which are at the very
In terms of central research questions and con- core of resilience research (Luthar, 2006; Masten,
structs, the scientific study of resilience has much 2001), not only during childhood and adoles-
in common with other disciplines including the cence, but also across adulthood (Collishaw,
long-standing fields of risk research and preven- Maughan, Goodman, & Pickles, 2004; Hauser
tion science [for a more in-depth discussion, see et al., 2006; Sampson & Laub, 1993; Staudinger,
Luthar (2006)]. In this chapter, we focus specifi- Freund, Linden, & Maas, 1999; Vaillant, 2012).
cally on differences and similarities with the rela- Positive psychology by contrast has been focused
tively new but burgeoning field of positive largely on adults, although there are now increas-
psychology, with an emphasis, specifically, on ing calls for attention to developmental varia-
useful directions for future work in both areas. tions, critically examining whether findings on
7 Resilience and Positive Psychology 133

particular adult samples might generalize to chil- ditionally, emphasized overt behavioral success as
dren and to adults at different stages of the life judged by proximal others—adaptive behaviors as
span (see Oishi & Kurtz, 2011; Roberts, Brown, rated by teachers, friends, parents, or others. In
Johnson, & Reinke, 2002). positive psychology, by contrast, there do not seem
Third, studies of resilience, grounded firmly to be efforts to ascertain others’ opinions on
in the discipline of developmental psychopathol- whether the individual is doing well—as a good
ogy, adhere to a core, defining feature of this spouse or parent, for example, or as a colleague at
field: that studies of normal development aid our work. In fact, even when there are constructs
understanding of atypical processes and, con- tapping into interpersonal themes, these largely
versely, studies of the atypical inform our under- involve the individual’s own reports, with social
standing of normative development (Luthar, acceptance defined in terms of individuals having
2003; Yates & Masten, 2004). Thus far, in posi- positive attitudes toward others and social integra-
tive psychology, the tendency has been to “use tion as individuals’ feelings of being supported by
the normal as a base from which to understand their communities (Keyes & Lopez, 2002). Heavy
the abnormal, rather than also [emphasis added] reliance on self-reports can be a particularly salient
using the abnormal to illuminate the normal” source of bias in positive psychology, because
(Hames & Joiner, 2011, p. 314). many of the constructs studied are socially desir-
The fourth difference pertains to operational- able and people tend to want to portray themselves
ization of positive outcomes, and in this regard, favorably (Lambert, Fincham, Gwinn, & Ajayi,
there are two distinctions. First, resilience 2011). Thus, there is a pressing need for greater
researchers have considered both the presence of consideration of indicators not based in self-reports
competent, healthy adjustment, as well as the (Noftle, Schnitker, & Robins, 2011).
evasion of psychopathology (when individuals Conversely, there is an important lesson that
are exposed to severe or chronic stressors; cf. those of us seeking to maximize childhood resil-
Luthar & Brown, 2007; Rutter, 2012). In its early ience could learn from positive psychology, and
years, positive psychology was concerned only that is that we need to consider positive subjec-
with positive aspects of adjustment and health tive experiences. Developmental studies com-
promotion. Again, recent appraisals of the first monly include assessments of children’s feelings
decade of this science (Sheldon et al., 2011) have of depression, anxiety, or low self-worth, but we
led to exhortations to consider negative dimen- rarely ask youth about their own feelings of hap-
sions as well, because some of these aspects can piness or life satisfaction. In the future, it will be
be beneficial. Anger, for example, mobilizes us to important for childhood resilience researchers to
defend ourselves, and sadness is linked with criti- consider not only the degree to which young peo-
cal and detail-focused thinking, which is impor- ple conform to adults’ expectations and evade
tant for certain kinds of problem solving (Oishi & distress but also the degree to which they them-
Kurtz, 2011). More broadly, Ryff (1989) has selves subjectively experience feelings of happi-
noted that from a lifespan developmental per- ness, hope, and optimism.
spective, psychological health results from active
engagement of all that life has to offer—the posi-
tive, as well as the negative, just as Wong (2007) Similarities
has argued, if positive psychology is to address
the full potential of human beings, it must do so Despite these areas of difference, it should be
by addressing the challenges brought by life emphasized that resilience research has many
along with the successes. similarities to positive psychology. First, as both
The second difference in operationalizing posi- disciplines have matured, there have been ongo-
tive outcomes concerns the parameters used to ing critical appraisals of the scientific integrity of
define healthy or optimal development. When the corpus of work, examining issues of opera-
studying children, resilience researchers have, tra- tional definitions, methodological approaches,
134 S.S. Luthar et al.

and veridicality of conclusions (e.g., Lopez & Researchers in both fields have faced the
Snyder, 2009; Luthar et al., 2000; Rutter, 1987, complexities of defining “doing well,” given that
2000; Sheldon et al., 2011; Synder & Lopez, meaningful variations exist across domains of
2002; Vanderbilt-Adriance & Shaw, 2008). In adjustment. Just as childhood resilience has long
both cases, for example, there have been in-depth been recognized as being a non-unidimensional
discussions about whether and why the field war- construct (Luthar et al., 1993), increasingly,
rants a distinct identity as opposed to representing vicissitudes in adjustment are noted in the posi-
just a new term for other, long-established spheres tive psychology literature. To illustrate, McCrae
of inquiry, such as competence (Luthar et al., (2011) has argued that people have different per-
2000; Yates & Masten, 2004) or positive emo- sonal strengths, some of which can work against
tions (Oishi & Kurtz, 2011). Both fields have wit- each other, wherein high levels of conscientious-
nessed an emphasis on ensuring that research that ness, for example, can run counter to personal
is grounded in a set of strong organizing theory, growth. In broadly defining the life well lived,
with specific suggestions proffered in this regard similarly, Little (2011) has cautioned that an indi-
(Lambert et al., 2011; Luthar et al., 2000; Sheldon vidual’s exuberant pursuit of personally mean-
et al., 2011; Vanderbilt-Adriance & Shaw, 2008). ingful life goals can create problems for family
In terms of central goals of research, Michael members.
Rutter’s seminal 1987 paper spawned concerted In the field of resilience, we have long grap-
efforts among resilience researchers to under- pled with these complexities of varying profiles
stand the underlying processes or mechanisms of competence, compelled, eventually, to con-
via which a given promotive or vulnerability fac- front the fact that choices must be made in priori-
tor may operate, and we are now witnessing simi- tizing particular domains—and that such
lar exhortations in positive psychology. For prioritization must be made on strong theoretical
example, Oishi and Kurtz (2011) noted that ran- grounds (Luthar et al., 2000). As noted in the first
dom acts of kindness make people happier, but half of this chapter, our operationalizations of
we need to disentangle the major underlying doing well are always conceptually related, first,
mechanisms, illuminating whether these feelings to the nature and severity of the particular risk
occur because people see themselves in a positive experienced (e.g., emotional resilience among
light, or because they build a sense of trust and children of depressed parents, or behavioral resil-
social capital. As emphasized earlier, disentan- ience among youth at risk for conduct disorder).
gling these mechanisms is particularly critical Currently, there is a plethora of constructs sub-
when designing interventions. sumed in the field of positive psychology, rang-
Another parallel is that both disciplines entail ing from happiness [with various connotations;
concerted attention to interlinked, mutually ben- see Algoe, Fredrickson, and Chow (2011)] to
eficial salutary constructs. Rutter (1987, p. 57, meaning making, altruism, selflessness, grati-
316–331) described “chain” effects, wherein, for tude, and wisdom. As the field moves forward, an
example, the quality of family relationships important scientific task will be to derive, con-
affects children’s sense of self-worth and attach- sensually, some prioritization or hierarchy of
ment security, which, in turn, promotes openness dimensions that are deemed most central to oper-
to other potentially supportive relationships. ationally defining whether a life has, indeed,
Resonant with this premise is Fredrickson’s been lived well (see Sheldon et al., 2011), as
(1998, p. 300) “broaden and build” conceptual- opposed to other dimensions that are potentially
ization, where positive emotions—of joy, engage- informative, but not cardinal.
ment, meaning, and, perhaps most importantly, At a substantive level, both fields are funda-
love—“serve to broaden an individual’s momen- mentally applied in nature, seeking to make a dif-
tary thought-action repertoire, which in turn has ference. In both cases, an initial scientific interest
the effect of building that individual’s physical, in uncovering basic psychological processes has
intellectual, and social resources.” led to acknowledgements that the central goals
7 Resilience and Positive Psychology 135

are to benefit humanity (Csikszentmihalyi & there is explicit emphasis on context within posi-
Nakamura, 2011; Luthar & Brown, 2007; tive psychology interventions as well, as seen in
Sheldon et al., 2011; Yates & Masten, 2004). And Gillham, Brunwasser, and Freres (2008) school-
with this applied focus in mind, scientists in both based program to promote positive child attributes
fields explicitly highlight the charge of proac- (e.g., empathy and self-control), while developing
tively and responsibly disseminating our work. these skills within the teachers themselves. As the
Acknowledging early and often well-deserved fields of resilience and positive psychology con-
criticisms of research on resilience (and the tinue to delineate key principles for future inter-
inherent appeal of this notion to the lay public), ventions, we hope that there will be a steadfast
Luthar and Cicchetti (2000) underscored the attention, in both cases, to individuals’ contexts.
need for the highest possible standards of evi- Kashdan and Steger’s (2011, p. 13) words of cau-
dence and self-scrutiny in dissemination. In a tion must be heeded by scientists in both fields,
similar vein, Kashdan and Steger (2011) cau- equally: “If positive psychology is going to prog-
tioned against the rush of excitement to share ress at the scientific and applied level, context can
new knowledge in positive psychology, noting no longer be underappreciated, ignored, and
that it is critical to obtain replications and seek untreated” (Kashdan & Steger, 2011, p. 13).
alternative explanations, with the onus of respon-
sibility doubled when research offers directions
for interventions (see also Biswas-Diener, Future Directions
Kashdan, & King, 2009).
Perhaps most importantly, the core findings In concluding, we present two themes that we
derived from accumulated work in both areas are believe merit much greater attention by positive
strikingly similar. A review of 50 years of psychologists and resilience researchers alike, in
research on resilience—among children as well formulating future theories, research, and prac-
as adults—led to the simple conclusion that tice implications. The first is despite our shared
“Resilience rests, fundamentally, on relation- emphasis on the positive and salutary, we must
ships” (Luthar, 2006, p. 780). Strikingly resonant explicitly recognize that “bad is stronger than
is Zautra’s (2014) assertion, “Resilience is social, good” (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Findenauer, &
after all,” and Peterson’s (2006) “three-word Vohs, 2001, p. 323): People are generally much
summary of positive psychology: Other people more deeply affected by negative feedback such
matter” (p. 249). Reis and Aron (2008) noted that as rejection than by positive ones such as praise.
human love is part of a constellation of evolved For positive psychologists, this would imply the
regulatory mechanisms with enormous signifi- need for explicit recognition that if individuals
cance for positive adjustment, as Lambert et al. are to flourish, experiences of positive emotions
(2011) note the recurrent acknowledgement in (e.g., joy or hope) must collectively outnumber
the positive psychology literature that close rela- experiences of negative ones (such as fear, sad-
tionships are essential to individuals’ well-being ness, or guilt)—by a ratio as high as three to one
(Diener & Oishi, 2005). (Fredrickson & Losada, 2005). In parallel, even
In terms of how our science can best benefit as resilience researchers urge attention to
humanity, cognizance of the fundamental impor- strengths of families and communities, our first
tance of relationships has led resilience research- order of business must be to attend to known
ers to emphasize attention to proximal contexts in potent toxins. Research has established incontro-
any efforts to improve personal strengths. As long vertibly, for example, that chronic maltreatment
as individuals remain in interpersonal settings that is insidious and rarely overcome by other protec-
are damaging to their psychological adjustment, tive processes; yet, such forces are not always
any pull-out, short-term efforts to promote par- identified as primary and essential targets for at-
ticular skills will have limited value (Luthar & risk populations. With survival threatened, posi-
Brown, 2007; Pianta & Walsh, 1998). Increasingly, tive attributes cannot flourish.
136 S.S. Luthar et al.

Second, in operationalizing optimal outcomes R13 MH082592, R01 DA010726). Our sincere thanks to
Sasha Heinz for valuable input on prior drafts and to Nina
in both fields, the notions of generativity, or doing
L. Kumar for assistance in background research.
for the greater good, must be given much greater
priority with these attributes rated by others and
not just by the self. In positive psychology, the References
most compelling definition of “a life well lived,”
arguably, would be not just self-reported health Afifi, T. O., & MacMillan, H. L. (2011). Resilience fol-
and happiness but when adults are judged as com- lowing maltreatment: A review of protective factors.
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 266–272.
mitted to doing for others, with positive contribu- Retrieved from http://publications.cpa-apc.org/
tions to society (Bermant, Talwar, & Rozin, 2011; Aisenberg, E., & Herrenkohl, T. (2008). Community vio-
Little, 2011). Similarly, generativity can (and lence in context: Risk and resilience in children and
should) be considered a core positive outcome in families. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 23, 296–
315. doi:10.1177/0886260507312287.
operationalizing resilience among children and Algoe, S. B., Fredrickson, B. L., & Chow, S. M. (2011).
youth. We need to move beyond social conformity The future of emotions research within positive psy-
and academic grades to focusing on behavioral chology. In K. M. Sheldon, T. B. Kashdan, & M. F.
manifestations of kindness, generosity, and self- Steger (Eds.), Designing positive psychology: Taking
stock and moving forward (pp. 115–132). New York,
lessness. If the shared goal of these two scientific NY: Oxford University Press.
disciplines is, ultimately, to promote the well- Anthony, E. J. (1974). The syndrome of the psychologi-
being of humanity, then humanitarian acts must be cally invulnerable child. In E. J. Anthony & C.
central in our own scholarly efforts—in our theo- Koupernik (Eds.), The child in his family: Children at
psychiatric risk (pp. 3–10). New York, NY: Wiley.
ries, research foci, and above all, in the messages Barnett, M. A., Scaramella, L. V., Neppl, T. K., Ontai, L.
disseminated to the public and policy makers. L., & Conger, R. D. (2010). Grandmother involvement
In summary, resilience research and positive as a protective factor for early childhood adjustment.
psychology have much in common. As both Journal of Family Psychology, 24, 635–645.
doi:10.1037/a0020829.
fields continued to mature—retaining the highest Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Findenauer, C., & Vohs, K.
standards of scientific inquiry—we face many of D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General
the same challenges. We each will need to arrive Psychology, 5, 323–370. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323.
at some prioritization of which, among dozens of Beardslee, W. R. (2002). When a parent is depressed:
How to protect your children from the effects of
criteria, must be treated as integral in defining the depression in the family. Boston, MA: Little, Brown.
“life well lived,” and must critically appraise this Beardslee, W., Gladstone, T. R. G., & O’Connor, E. E.
question at different developmental stages across (2012). Transmission and prevention of mood disor-
the life span. Notwithstanding our shared con- ders among children of affectively ill parents: A
review. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
ceptual commitment to strengths and assets, we Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 1098–1109.
must be attentive to coexisting inimical influ- Bermant, G., Talwar, C., & Rozin, P. (2011). To celebrate
ences that can powerfully thwart these. And positive psychology and extend its horizons. In K. M.
beyond the thriving of individuals, we must focus Sheldon, T. B. Kashdan, & M. F. Steger (Eds.),
Designing positive psychology: Taking stock and mov-
on what individuals do to benefit others including ing forward (pp. 430–438). New York, NY: Oxford
family, friends, and society, and on how such University Press.
generativity might best be fostered. Such a focus Biswas-Diener, R., Kashdan, T. B., & King, L. A. (2009).
will keep us true to what has been emphasized by Two traditions of happiness research, not two distinct
types of happiness. Journal of Positive Psychology, 4,
past presidents of the American Psychological 408–211. doi:10.1080/17439760902844400.
Association across many decades (Zigler, 1998): Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and
that a central aim of psychology, as a broad disci- ego-resiliency in the organization of behavior. In W. A.
pline, must be to serve the public good and to Collins (Ed.), Development of cognition, affect, and
social relations: Minnesota Symposia on Child
promote the welfare of humankind. Psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 39–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Brody, G. H., Kim, S., Murry, V. M., & Brown, A. C.
Acknowledgements We gratefully acknowledge funding (2004). Protective longitudinal paths linking child
from the National Institutes of Health (R01DA014385, competence to behavioral problems among African
7 Resilience and Positive Psychology 137

American siblings. Child Development, 75, 455–467. Fiedler, F. E. (1995). “Cognitive resources and leadership
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00686.x. performance”: A rejoinder. Applied Psychology, 44,
Burt, K. B., & Paysnick, A. A. (2012). Resilience in the tran- 50–56. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.1995.tb01386.x.
sition to adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emo-
24, 493–505. doi:10.1017/S0954579412000119. tions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300–319.
Cedeno, L. A., Elias, M. J., Kelly, S., & Chu, B. C. (2010). doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300.
School violence, adjustment, and the influence of hope Fredrickson, B. L., & Losada, M. F. (2005). Positive
on low-income, African American youth. American Affect and the Complex Dynamics of Human
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 80, 213–226. Flourishing. American Psychologist, 60, 678–686.
doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01025.x. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.7.678.
Cicchetti, D. (2002). The impact of social experience on García Coll, C. T., & Marks, A. K. (Eds.). (2009).
neurobiological systems: Illustration from a construc- Immigrant stories: Ethnicity and academics in middle
tivist view of child maltreatment. Cognitive childhood. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Development, 17, 1407–1428. doi:10.1016/ Garmezy, N. (1974). The study of competence in children
S0885-2014(02)00121-1. at risk for severe psychopathology. In E. J. Anthony &
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1997). The role of self- C. Koupernik (Eds.), The child in his family: Vol. 3:
organization in the promotion of resilience in mal- Children at psychiatric risk (pp. 77–97). New York,
treated children. Development and Psychopathology, NY: Wiley.
9, 797–815. doi:10.1017/S0954579497001442. Garmezy, N., & Masten, A. S. (1986). Stress, competence,
Coley, R. L. (2001). (In)visible men: Emerging and resilience: Common frontiers for therapist and
research on low-income, unmarried, and minority psychopathologist. Behavior Therapy, 17, 500–521.
fathers. American Psychologist, 56, 743–753. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(86)80091-0.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.56.9.743. Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (1984). The study
Collishaw, S., Maughan, B., Goodman, R., & Pickles, A. of stress and competence in children: A building block for
(2004). Time trends in adolescent mental health. developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55,
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 97–111. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb00276.x.
1350–1362. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00842.x. Gillham, J. E., Brunwasser, S. M., & Freres, D. R. (2008).
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Nakamura, J. (2011). Positive Preventing depression in early adolescence: The Penn
psychology: Where did it come from, where is it Resiliency Program. In J. R. Z. Abela & B. L. Hankin
going? In K. M. Sheldon, T. B. Kashdan, & M. F. (Eds.), Handbook of depression in children and ado-
Steger (Eds.), Designing positive psychology: Taking lescents (pp. 309–332). New York, NY: Guilford
stock and moving forward (pp. 3–8). New York, NY: Press.
Oxford University Press. Gorman-Smith, D., & Tolan, P. H. (2003). Positive adapta-
Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2005). The nonobvious social tion among youth exposed to community violence. In S.
psychology of happiness. Psychological Inquiry, 16, S. Luthar (Ed.), Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation
162–167. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1604_04. in the context of childhood adversities (pp. 392–413).
Dishion, T. J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
When interventions harm: Peer groups and problem Grigorenko, E. L., & Cicchetti, D. (2012). The contribu-
behavior. American Psychologist, 54, 755–764. tion of genetic/genomic sciences to developmental
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.9.755. psychopathology [Special issue]. Development and
Dodge, K. A., & Rutter, M. (Eds.). (2011). Gene- Psychopathology, 24(4).
environment interactions in developmental psychopa- Hames, J. L., & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2011). The dog woman,
thology. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Addie Bundren, and the ninth circle: Positive psychol-
DuBois, D. L., Portillo, N., Rhodes, J. E., Silverthorn, N., ogy should be more open to the negative. In K.
& Valentine, J. C. (2011). How effective are mentoring Sheldon, T. Kashdan, & M. Steger (Eds.), Designing
programs for youth? A systematic assessment of the the future of positive psychology: Taking stock and
evidence. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, moving forward (pp. 313–323). New York, NY:
12, 57–91. doi:10.1177/1529100611414806. Oxford University Press.
Ebersöhn, L., & Ferreira, R. (2011). Coping in an HIV/ Hauser, S. T., Allen, J. P., & Golden, E. (2006). Out of the
AIDS-dominated context: Teachers promoting resil- woods: Tales of resilient teens. Cambridge, MA:
ience in schools. Health Education Research, 26, 596– Harvard University Press.
613. doi:10.1093/her/cyr016. Herrenkohl, T. I., Sousa, C., Tajima, E. A., Herrenkohl, R.
Eisenberg, N., Rg, H., Spinrad, T. L., Hofer, C., Chassin, C., & Moylan, C. A. (2008). Intersection of child
L., Zhou, Q., et al. (2010). Relations of temperament abuse and children’s exposure to domestic violence.
to maladjustment and ego resiliency in at-risk chil- Trauma, Violence, and Abuse, 9, 84–99.
dren. Social Development, 19, 577–600. doi:10.1177/1524838008314797.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00550.x. Jain, S., Buka, S. L., Subramanian, S. V., & Molnar, B. E.
Farber, E. A., & Egeland, B. (1987). Invulnerability in (2012). Protective factors for youth exposed to vio-
abused and neglected children. In E. J. Anthony & B. lence: Role of developmental assets in building emo-
J. Cohler (Eds.), The invulnerable child (pp. 253–288). tional resilience. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice,
New York, NY: Guilford Press. 10, 107–129. doi:10.1177/1541204011424735.
138 S.S. Luthar et al.

Kagan, J., Snidman, N., & Arcus, D. (1998). Childhood deriv- Luthar, S. S., & Barkin, S. H. (2012). Are affluent youth
atives of reactivity in infancy. Child Development, 69, truly “at risk”? Vulnerability and resilience across
1483–1493. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06171.x. three diverse samples. Development and
Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (2011). Challenges, pit- Psychopathology, 24, 429–449. doi:10.1017/
falls, and aspirations for positive psychology. In K. M. S0954579412000089.
Sheldon, T. B. Kashdan, & M. F. Steger (Eds.), Luthar, S. S., & Brown, P. J. (2007). Maximizing resil-
Designing positive psychology: Taking stock and mov- ience through diverse levels of inquiry: Prevailing
ing forward (pp. 9–21). New York, NY: Oxford paradigms, possibilities, and priorities for the future.
University Press. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 931–955.
Keyes, C. L. M., & Lopez, S. J. (2002). Toward a science doi:10.1017/S0954579407000454.
of mental health: Positive directions in diagnosis and Luthar, S., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). The construct of resil-
interventions. In C. R. Synder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), ience: Implications for interventions and social poli-
The handbook of positive psychology (pp. 45–59). cies. Development and Psychopathology, 12,
New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 857–885.
Kim, J., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Manly, J. T. Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The con-
(2009). Child maltreatment and trajectories of person- struct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guide-
ality and behavioral functioning: Implications for the lines for future research. Child Development, 71,
development of personality disorder. Development 543–562. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00164.
and Psychopathology, 21, 889–912. doi:10.1017/ Luthar, S. S., Doernberger, C. H., & Zigler, E. (1993).
S0954579409000480. Resilience is not a unidimensional construct: Insights
Kim-Cohen, J., & Turkewitz, R. (2012). Resilience and from a prospective study of inner-city adolescents.
measured gene-environment interactions. Development Development and Psychopathology, 5, 703–717.
and Psychopathology, 24, 1297–1306. doi:10.1017/ doi:10.1017/S0954579400006246.
S0954579412000715. Maggi, S., Roberts, W., MacLennan, D., & D’Angiulli, A.
Koenen, K. C., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., & (2011). Community resilience, quality childcare, and
Purcell, S. (2003). Domestic violence is associated preschoolers’ mental health: A three-city comparison.
with environmental suppression of IQ in young chil- Social Science and Medicine, 73, 1080–1087.
dren. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 297– doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.06.052, 10.1016%2Fj.
311. doi:10.1017/S0954579403000166. socscimed.2011.06.052.
Lambert, N. M., Fincham, F. D., Gwinn, A. M., & Ajayi, Martin, A., Ryan, R. M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2010).
C. A. (2011). Positive relationship science: A new When fathers’ supportiveness matters most: Maternal
frontier for positive psychology? In K. M. Sheldon, T. and paternal parenting and school readiness. Journal
B. Kashdan, & M. F. Steger (Eds.), Designing positive of Family Psychology, 24, 145–155. doi:10.1037/
psychology: Taking stock and moving forward (pp. a0018073.
280–292). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience pro-
Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). The neighborhoods cesses in development. American Psychologist, 56,
they live in: The effects of neighborhood residence on 227–238. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.227.
child and adolescent outcomes. Psychological Bulletin, Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2012). Risk and resilience
126, 309–337. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.309. in development and psychopathology: The legacy of
Little, B. R. (2011). Personality science and the northern Norman Garmezy. Development and Psychopathology,
tilt: As positive as possible under the circumstances. In 24, 333–334. doi:10.1017/S0954579412000016.
K. M. Sheldon, T. B. Kashdan, & M. F. Steger (Eds.), Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (2012). Resilience in devel-
Designing positive psychology: Taking stock and mov- opmental psychopathology: Contributions of the
ing forward (pp. 228–248). New York, NY: Oxford Project Competence Longitudinal Study. Development
University Press. and Psychopathology, 24, 345–361. doi:10.1017/
Lopez, S. J., & Snyder, C. R. (Eds.). (2009). Oxford hand- S095457941200003X.
book of positive psychology. New York, NY: Oxford McCrae, R. (2011). Personality traits and the potential of
University Press. positive psychology. In K. M. Sheldon, T. B. Kashdan,
Luthar, S. S. (1991). Vulnerability and resilience: A study & M. F. Steger (Eds.), Designing positive psychology:
of high-risk adolescents. Child Development, 62, 600– Taking stock and moving forward (pp. 192–206). New
616. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01555.x. York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Luthar, S. S. (Ed.). (2003). Resilience and vulnerability: Mersky, J. P., Berger, L. M., Reynolds, A. J., & Gromoske,
Adaptation in the context of childhood adversities. A. N. (2009). Risk factors for child and adolescent
New York: Cambridge University Press. maltreatment: A longitudinal investigation of a cohort
Luthar, S. S. (2006). Resilience in development: A syn- of inner-city youth. Child Maltreatment, 14, 73–88.
thesis of research across five decades. In D. Cicchetti doi:10.1177/1077559508318399.
& D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathol- Murphy, L. B., & Moriarty, A. E. (1976). Vulnerability,
ogy, Vol. 3: Risk, disorder, and adaptation (2nd ed., coping, and growth: From infancy to adolescence.
pp. 739–795). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
7 Resilience and Positive Psychology 139

Murry, V., Bynum, M. S., Brody, G. H., Willert, A., & Rhodes, J., & Lowe, S. R. (2008). Youth mentoring and
Stephens, D. (2001). African American single mothers resilience: Implications for practice. Child Care in
and children in context: A review of studies on risk Practice, 14, 9–17. doi:10.1080/13575270701733666.
and resilience. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Rios, R., Aiken, L. S., & Zautra, A. J. (2012).
Review, 4, 133–155. doi:10.1023/A:1011381114782. Neighborhood contexts and the mediating role of
Neal, J. W., Neal, Z. P., Atkins, M. S., Henry, D. B., & neighborhood social cohesion on health and psycho-
Frazier, S. L. (2011). Channels of change: Contrasting logical distress among Hispanic and non-Hispanic
network mechanisms in the use of interventions. residents. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 43, 50–61.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 47, 277– doi:10.1007/s12160-011-9306-9.
286. doi:10.1007/s10464-010-9403-0. Roberts, M. C., Brown, K. J., Johnson, R. J., & Reinke,
Noftle, E. E., Schnitker, S. A., & Robins, R. W. (2011). J. (2002). Positive psychology for children:
Character and personality: Connections between posi- Development, prevention, and promotion. In C. R.
tive psychology and personality psychology. In K. M. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive
Sheldon, T. B. Kashdan, & M. F. Steger (Eds.), psychology (pp. 663–671). New York, NY: Oxford
Designing the future of positive psychology: Taking University Press.
stock and moving forward (pp. 207–227). New York: Rogosch, F. A., Dackis, M. N., & Cicchetti, D. (2011).
Oxford University Press. Child maltreatment and allostatic load: Consequences
Obradović, J., Long, J. D., Cutili, J. J., Chan, C., Hinz, E., for physical and mental health in children from low-
Heistad, D., et al. (2009). Academic achievement of income families. Development and Psychopathology,
homeless and highly mobile children in an urban 23, 1107–1124. doi:10.1017/S0954579411000587.
school district: Longitudinal evidence on risk, growth, Rutter, M. (1979). Protective factors in children's
and resilience. Development and Psychopathology, 21, responses to stress and disadvantage. In M. W. Kent &
493–518. doi:10.1017/S0954579409000273. J. E. Rolf (Eds.), Primary prevention in psychopathol-
Oishi, S., & Kurtz, J. L. (2011). The positive psychology ogy, Vol. 3: Social competence in children (pp. 49–74).
of positive emotions: An avuncular view. In K. M. Hanover, NH: University Press of New England.
Sheldon, T. B. Kashdan, & M. F. Steger (Eds.), Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective
Designing positive psychology: Taking stock and mov- mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57,
ing forward (pp. 101–114). New York, NY: Oxford 316–331. doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.1987.tb03541.x.
University Press. Rutter, M. (1998). Developmental catch-up, and deficit,
Pargament, K. I., & Cummings, J. (2010). Anchored by following adoption after severe early privation.
faith: Religion as a resilience factor. In J. W. Reich, A. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 465–
J. Zautra, & J. S. Hall (Eds.), Handbook of adult resil- 476. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00343.
ience (pp. 193–210). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Rutter, M. (2000). Resilience reconsidered: Conceptual
Park, C. L. (2010). Stress-related growth and thriving considerations, empirical findings, and policy implica-
through coping: The roles of personality and cognitive tions. In J. P. Shonkoff & S. J. Meisels (Eds.), Handbook
processes. Journal of Social Issues, 54, 267–277. of early childhood intervention (2nd ed., pp. 651–682).
doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1998.tb01218.x. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Peck, S. C., Roeser, R. W., Zarrett, N., & Eccles, J. S. Rutter, M. (2012). Resilience as a dynamic concept.
(2008). Exploring the roles of extracurricular activity Development and Psychopathology, 24, 335–344.
quantity and quality in the educational resilience of doi:10.1017/S0954579412000028.
vulnerable adolescents: Variable- and pattern-centered Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it?
approaches. Journal of Social Issues, 64, 135–155. Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-
doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00552.x. being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Peterson, C. (2006). A primer in positive psychology. New 57, 1069–1081. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069.
York, NY: Oxford University Press. Sameroff, A. J., & Rosenblum, K. L. (2006). Psychosocial
Peterson, C., & Park, N. (2003). Positive psychology as constraints on development of resilience. Annals of the
the evenhanded positive psychologist views it. New York Academy of Science, 1094, 116–124.
Psychological Inquiry, 14, 141–146. doi:10.1207/ doi:10.1196/annals.1376.010.
S15327965PLI1402_03. Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1993). Crime in the mak-
Peterson, C., Park, N., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2006). ing: Pathways and turning points through life.
Greater strengths of character and recovery from ill- Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
ness. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1, 17–26. Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, C. (2000). Positive
doi:10.1080/17439760500372739. psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist,
Pianta, R. C., & Walsh, D. I. (1998). Applying the con- 55, 5–14. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5.
struct of resilience in schools: Cautions from a devel- Sheldon, K. M., Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (Eds.).
opmental systems perspective. School Psychology (2011). Designing positive psychology: Taking stock
Review, 27, 407–417. Retrieved from http://www. and moving forward. New York, NY: Oxford
nasponline.org/publications/spr. University Press.
Reis, H. T., & Aron, A. (2008). Love: What is it, why does it Sherman, B. J., Duarte, C. S., & Verdeli, H. (2011).
matter, and how does it operate? Psychological Science, Internalizing and externalizing problems in adoles-
3, 80–86. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00065.x. cents from Bahia, Brazil: Sociodemographic correlates
140 S.S. Luthar et al.

and family environment in boys and girls. International Vanderbilt-Adriance, E., & Shaw, D. S. (2008).
Journal of Mental Health, 40(3), 55–76. doi:10.2753/ Conceptualizing and re-evaluating resilience across
IMH0020-7411400304. levels of risk, time, and domains of competence.
Shiner, R. L., & Masten, A. S. (2012). Childhood person- Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 11,
ality as a harbinger of competence and resilience in 30–58. doi:10.1007/s10567-008-0031-2.
adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 24, Véronneau, M.-H., & Dishion, T. J. (2010). Predicting
507–528. doi:10.1017/ S0954579412000120. change in early adolescent problem behavior in the
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (Eds.). (2000). From middle school years: A mesosystemic perspective on
neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early parenting and peer experiences. Journal of Abnormal
childhood development. Washington, DC: National Child Psychology, 38, 1125–1137. doi:10.1007/
Academy Press. s10802-010-9431-0.
Staudinger, U. M., Freund, A. M., Linden, M., & Maas, I. Walsh, F. (2007). Traumatic loss and major
(1999). Self, personality and life regulation: Facets of disasters: Strengthening family and community resil-
psychological resilience in old age. In P. B. Baltes & ience. Family Process, 46, 207–227. doi:10.1111/
K. U. Mayer (Eds.), The Berlin aging study: Aging j.1545-5300.2007.00205.x.
from 70 to 100 (pp. 302–328). New York, NY: Werner, E. E. (2012). Children and war: Risk, resilience,
Cambridge University Press. and recovery. Development and Psychopathology, 24,
Steinberg, L. (2001). We know some things: Adolescent- 553–558. doi:10.1017/S0954579412000156.
parent relationships in retrospect and prospect. Journal Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. (1982). Vulnerable but invin-
of Research on Adolescence, 11, 1–19. doi:10.1111/ cible: A longitudinal study of resilient children. New
1532-7795.00001. York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Stoiber, K. C., & Gettinger, M. (2011). Functional assess- Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the
ment and positive support strategies for promoting odds: High risk children from birth to adulthood.
resilience: Effects on teachers and high-risk children. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Psychology in the Schools, 48, 686–706. doi:10.1002/ Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (2001). Journeys from child-
pits.20587. hood to midlife: Risk, resilience, and recovery. Ithaca,
Stormshak, E. A., Comeau, C. A., & Shepard, S. A. (2004). NY: Cornell University Press.
The relative contribution of sibling deviance and peer Williams, J., & Nelson-Gardell, D. (2012). Predicting
deviance in the prediction of substance use across middle resilience in sexually abused adolescents. Child
childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, Abuse and Neglect, 36, 53–63. doi:10.1016/j.
635–649. doi:10.1023/B:JACP.0000047212.49463.c7. chiabu.2011.07.004.
Synder, C. R., & Lopez, S. J. (Eds.). (2002). The hand- Wong, P. T. P. (2007). The promises and perils of positive
book of positive psychology. New York, NY: Oxford psychology. PsycCRITIQUES, Contemporary
University Press. Psychology: APA Review of Books.
Thompson, R., & Tabone, J. K. (2010). The impact of Yates, T., & Grey, I. K. (2012). Adapting to aging out:
early alleged maltreatment on behavioral trajectories. Profiles of risk and resilience among emancipated fos-
Child Abuse and Neglect, 34, 907–916. doi:10.1016/j. ter youth. Development and Psychopathology, 24,
chiabu.2010.06.006. 475–492. doi:10.1017/S0954579412000107.
Tiet, Q. Q., Huizinga, D., & Byrnes, H. F. (2010). Yates, T. M., & Masten, A. S. (2004). Fostering the future:
Predictors of resilience among inner city youths. Resilience theory and the practice of positive psychol-
Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19, 360–378. ogy. In P. A. Linley & S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive psy-
doi:10.1007/s10826-009-9307-5. chology in practice (pp. 521–539). Hoboken, NJ:
Vaillant, G. E. (2012). Triumphs of experience: The men Wiley.
of the Harvard Grant Study. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Zautra, A. J. (2014). Resilience is social, after all. In M.
University Press. Kent, M. C. Davis, & J. W. Reich (Eds.), The resil-
Valdez, C. R., Mills, C. L., Barrueco, S., Leis, J., & Riley, ience handbook: Approaches to stress and trauma
A. W. (2011). A pilot study of a family-focused inter- (pp. 185–196). New York: Rutledge.
vention for children and families affected by maternal Zigler, E. (1998). A place of value for applied and policy
depression. Journal of Family Therapy, 33, 3–19. studies. Child Development, 69, 1467–8624.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-6427.2010.00529.x. Zimmerman, M. A., & Brenner, A. B. (2010). Resilience in
Van Ryzin, M. (2010). The secondary school advisor as adolescence: Overcoming neighborhood disadvantage.
mentor and secondary attachment figure. Journal of In J. W. Reich, A. J. Zautra, & J. S. Hall (Eds.),
Community Psychology, 38, 131–154. doi:10.1002/ Handbook of adult resilience (pp. 283–308). New York,
jcop.20356. NY: Guilford Press.

View publication stats

You might also like