You are on page 1of 21

Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology

© 2020 American Psychological Association 2022, Vol. 8, No. 2, 85–105


ISSN: 2332-2101 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/stl0000198

Adaptability to a Sudden Transition to Online Learning During the


COVID-19 Pandemic: Understanding the Challenges for Students

Avi Besser Gordon L. Flett


Hadassah Academic College York University
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Virgil Zeigler-Hill
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Oakland University

This cross-sectional study investigated the associations among adaptability to the


pandemic, personality, and levels of learning experiences (affective, cognitive, and
behavioral) among higher education students required to adapt to an abrupt transition
to synchronous online learning due to the global COVID-19 pandemic. A sample of
1,217 college students from Israel completed an online questionnaire after transitioning
to synchronous online learning during the COVID-19 pandemic. Comparisons of
reactions to the online condition versus more typical face-to-face learning conditions
confirmed that students had pervasive negative reactions to the online condition that
became necessary due to the pandemic. Moreover, adaptability to the pandemic was
associated broadly with more positive reactions across multiple indicators. Support was
also found for an indirect associations model such that personality traits were associ-
ated with specific reactions to online learning via their associations with adaptability.
Supplemental analyses highlighted the more positive reactions and learning adaptabil-
ity of students who reported greater feelings of belonging and mattering. The current
findings attest to the role of adaptability and the significant challenges experienced by
college students who experienced changes in their learning and life conditions due to
the need to rapidly adjust to the changes and uncertainty brought about by the
COVID-19 pandemic.

Keywords: COVID-19, adaptability, synchronous learning, university students, Israel

The COVID-19 pandemic has many elements cope with the sense of isolation and loneliness
that make it exceptionally stressful for students. that accompanies adherence to guidelines to
Safety concerns prevail due to the potential risk engage in physical isolation or social distanc-
to health and well-being for individual students ing. Stress and anxiety are also elicited by cir-
but also for their family members, friends, and cumstances that have resulted in complete dis-
colleagues. Many students have also had to ruptions of daily routines. Accordingly,

This article was published Online First October 19, 2020. students who volunteered and participated so willingly in
Avi Besser, Interdisciplinary School for Sciences, this study. Grateful thanks are also extended to the anon-
Health and Society, Department of Communication Disor- ymous reviewers for their thoughtful and constructive sug-
ders, Hadassah Academic College; Gordon L. Flett, De- gestions on a draft of this paper.
partment of Psychology, York University; Virgil Zeigler- Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
Hill, Department of Psychology, Oakland University. dressed to Avi Besser, Interdisciplinary School for Sci-
Authors declare that there is no conflict of interest. We ences, Health and Society, Department of Communication
would like to acknowledge and are grateful to the public Disorders, Hadassah Academic College, 37 Hanevi’im St.
academic colleges in Israel that approved and distributed Jerusalem, 9101001, Israel, or to Gordon L. Flett, Depart-
the survey to their students, as well as our colleagues and ment of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele Street,
friends for raising awareness of this study in their schools, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, M3J 1P3. E-mail: avibe@hac
and we would like to extend our gratitude to the 1,217 .ac.il or gflett@yorku.ca

85
86 BESSER, FLETT, AND ZEIGLER-HILL

researchers have already begun to document the Snelson, 2017). However, this is the case under
heightened anxiety and distress among people typical circumstances, yet the current situation
going through the pandemic (e.g., Casagrande, is exceedingly atypical. It must also be ac-
Favieri, Tambelli, & Forte, 2020; Flett & He- knowledged that the uncontrollable and uncer-
witt, 2020; Horesh & Brown, 2020). tain situation that is now being faced will be
Another key element is the uncertainty and entirely at odds with the need for autonomy and
ambiguity that accompanies this global health self-determination that is typically at the core of
crisis. Because it is novel, unresolved, and motivated goal striving (for a discussion, see
seemingly uncontrollable, there is uncertainty Deci & Ryan, 2012). This wholesale shift to
about the best ways to respond as well as when online learning will also be more difficult for
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and how the pandemic will be resolved and students who are sheltering in places that are
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

what life will be like following the pandemic. less optimal and more physically and psycho-
Situations of prolonged uncertainty are bound logically confining and are perhaps not very
to result in elevated worry and anxiety and this conducive to learning and performance. While
has been documented in previous research (e.g., our focus is on students, we recognize that the
Flett, Endler, & Fairlie, 1999; Lobel, Gilat, & challenges, of course, also apply to professors
Endler, 1993). and other instructors. Some of these challenges
Students also have a series of unique chal- have been documented in other recent work
lenges that are specific to them. Students must (e.g., Besser, Lotem, & Zeigler-Hill, 2020).
try to maintain their concentration on learning Given these observations and the many chal-
despite it being difficult to do so. The learning lenges that exist at present, the current article is
and achievements of students are typically un- based on research that had three primary goals.
derscored by their goals and goal orientations, First, we sought to document the extent to
yet it is not easy to maintain a focus on achieve- which an abrupt transition to synchronous on-
ment goals when life issues have become pre- line learning is impacting college students who
dominant. Another significant challenge for stu- are trying to learn as the pandemic continues to
dents is that because they must now engage in unfold. We had students rate their current learn-
online distance learning, the daily routines of ing experiences and also provide retrospective
student life have now been entirely altered. The accounts of their typical learning experiences
anxious student who tries to soothe herself or prior to the beginning of the pandemic and the
himself through affiliation with peers now has changes that have been brought about by it. The
limited opportunities to do so. measures included in this investigation were
One of the most significant changes imposed quite comprehensive. We examined a host of
on students is the focus of this article. Specifi- negative emotional reactions and mood states,
cally, many students who typically engage in both positive and negative. In addition, we also
face-to-face learning formats have now had to assessed perceived stress and feelings of lone-
make a sudden transition to online learning. liness. Our assessment extended to many differ-
While there have been extensive discussions ent aspects of the learning process, including
and analyses of the merits and challenges of not only affective reactions, but also cognitive
online learning (see Martin, Ahlgrim-Delzell, & and behavioral reactions in the typical and now
Budhrani, 2017; Olt, 2018), and research sug- atypical circumstances.
gests that students typically enjoy taking online Second, the current study examined individual
courses (see Seiver & Troja, 2014), such learn- differences in self-reported adaptability in re-
ing is best when it is planned in advance (or sponse to the pandemic as our primary and central
involves a gradual transition) and combined focus. At present, the continuing COVID-19
with other forms of learning. Ideally, students pandemic is such that we are uniquely under-
would often experience a blended learning en- going a prolonged stress sequence with no end
vironment yet this is not possible in most in- in sight. An emphasis on how the individual
stances due to the pandemic. It must also be person adapts is in keeping with interactional
acknowledged that students differ in their abil- transactional stress models that recognize the
ity to thrive and flourish rather than struggle as characteristics of the individual as mediating
a result of online learning (see Malik, Fatiman, between the attributes of the environment and
Hussain, & Sarwar, 2017; Yang, Baldwin, & the responses to stress (see Derogatis & Coons,
ADAPTABILITYAND EXPERIENCE OF ONLINE LEARNING 87

1993). The ability to adapt is central to coping traits have been shown to predict a wide array of
and problem solving with adaptability being outcomes, and they have been used extensively
related to— but distinguishable from—resil- in past coping research (see Carver & Connor-
ience (see Martin, 2017). Programmatic re- Smith, 2010; Segerstrom & Smith, 2019). These
search by Martin and colleagues has focused on personality traits are well-suited to the current
adaptability to new and uncertain circum- work given that they collectively reflect goal
stances. A measure known as the Adaptability striving, interpersonal orientations, and emo-
Scale was created by Martin, Nejad, Colmar, tional well-being. However, they are also highly
and Liem (2013) to assess levels of adaptability relevant given the original links between the
in general. This nine-item self-report inventory five-factor model and trait adaptability docu-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

has been used in numerous investigations that mented by Martin et al. (2013). These research-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

consistently show the benefits of having the ers showed that adaptability in general was as-
capacity to effectively adjust to new and uncer- sociated with higher levels of conscientiousness
tain circumstances (Collie, Holliman, & Martin, and lower levels of neuroticism. The current
2017; Holliman, Martin, & Collie, 2018; Martin study utilized the same five-factor measure used
et al., 2013; Putwain, Loderer, Gallard, & Beau- by Martin et al. (2013), which enabled us to
mont, 2020). Accordingly, the current research examine the replicability of their findings in a
examined individual differences in adaptability unique context and with a modified Adaptabil-
among students who were undergoing and re- ity Scale that captured adaptability to the spe-
quired to cope with the COVID-19 global health cific context of the COVID-19 pandemic.
pandemic. The inclusion of measures tapping adaptabil-
We utilized a version of the Adaptability ity and personality traits enabled us to assess the
Scale that took the usual scale item content and relevance of these individual difference factors
modified it so that our participants were clearly in terms of their links with perceived adaptabil-
required to report on their adaptability to the ity to the pandemic situation. Of course, it also
COVID-19 global health crisis and all of the permitted an examination to how these individ-
changes it has brought about. This decision was ual difference factors relate with the affective,
guided by our sense that it essential to begin to cognitive, and behavioral reactions to the new
document current levels of coping through learning format as expressed by our student
adaptability in response to the current chal- participants.
lenges rather than relying on well-known mea-
sures of typical coping styles from a trait per- Belongingness and Mattering as Elements
spective. Although it would have been ideal to of Relating to Other People
have a baseline measure of general adaptability
prior to the transition, this was not possible due Finally, and parenthetically, it is worth noting
to the unforeseeable need to transition to online that the measures we used to document how
learning. One limitation of this work is that we students now feel and perceive their current
did not also include the original measure, which learning situation included brief measures of
would have enabled us to distinguish between relatedness. This study focused on two interper-
general adaptability and adaptability to the pan- sonally based constructs (belongingness and
demic. mattering) that have been identified in past re-
Third, and finally, the current study investi- search as involving feelings that are central to
gated broad personality traits from the five- students’ positive emotions, learning, and per-
factor trait model in terms of their associations formance. Both belongingness and mattering
with perceived adaptability to the pandemic sit- are elements of relatedness to others. Belong-
uation and affective, cognitive, and behavioral ingness is a sense of being included and fitting
reactions to the new learning format. The well- in (e.g., being a part of a group), whereas mat-
known five-factor model of personality traits tering is the sense of being important to other
focuses on openness, conscientiousness, extra- people. Mattering is both a feeling and a need,
version, agreeableness, and neuroticism (see and it has been identified as a key but often
Costa & McCrae, 1992; Digman, 1990; Dig- overlooked positive psychology construct (see
man, & Takemoto-Chock, 1981; Goldberg, Flett, 2018a). It includes the belief of being
1990, 1992; Norman, 1963). These personality significant to others and these other people
88 BESSER, FLETT, AND ZEIGLER-HILL

showing that they pay attention to the individ- sessments of face-to-face learning with current
ual. It can also be a feeling that arises from assessments of synchronous online learning.
having others depend on oneself (see Rosenberg Second, the current study evaluated the rele-
& McCullough, 1981). There is an extensive vance of individual differences in self-reported
and growing literature that documents how stu- adaptability in response to the pandemic. It was
dents do much better when they have developed hypothesized and anticipated that students with
a sense that they belong and they matter in higher levels of adaptability, relative to students
general, but especially in their places of learn- with lower levels of adaptability, would report
ing (see Allen, Kern, Vella-Brodrick, Hattie, & more favorable responses to synchronous online
Waters, 2018; Flett, 2018a; Kivlighan et al., learning.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2018). Connection to others is key when it Finally, the current study also evaluated the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

comes to online learning (see Seiver & Troja, relevance of broad individual difference factors
2014). The benefits of mattering as a resource from the five-factor trait model; these traits
during the pandemic have been described at were examined in terms of their links with per-
length (see Flett & Zangeneh, 2020). ceived adaptability to the pandemic situation
The eminent self-esteem researcher Morris and affective, cognitive, and behavioral reac-
Rosenberg argued that mattering is one of the tions to the new learning format. Given that
core elements of self and identity (Rosenberg, broad personality traits can be regarded as distal
1985; Rosenberg & McCullough, 1981). The influences while adaptability to the pandemic is
benefits of mattering have resulted in recent a very proximal influence, the current analysis
calls to make mattering a central focus of men- also evaluated as part of this third goal an indi-
tal health promotion in schools and universities rect associations model focused on the associa-
(Flett, 2018b; Flett, Khan, & Su, 2019). Matter- tions that broad personality variables had with
ing has been linked with measures of academic current feelings and reactions in synchronous
resilience (Flett, Su, Ma, & Guo, 2016). These online learning via personality variables’ asso-
components of feeling valued can be examined ciations with adaptability during the COVID-
in general, but it is also possible to examine 19. This indirect effects model focus was based
these components in specific contexts, including on our expectation that there would indeed be
the college environment (see France & Finney, significant associations between the personality
2010; Tovar, Simon, & Lee, 2009). Belonging traits and adaptability and between adaptability
and mattering tap into core elements of the self and reactions and responses to the pandemic.
and it should be the case that students who see Our participants were students who have
themselves as more adaptable during the pan- been through multiple past experiences that
demic crisis should have a strong, positive sense have required them to adapt, yet they now find
of self that is built, at least in part, on the themselves in a totally unique and uncertain
scaffold provided by knowing that they belong situation that is different from their past expe-
and they matter to other people. However, it is riences. The current research was conducted
important to recognize that belonging and mat- with students from Israel who are typically
tering are double-edged; that is, it is great to older than the college and university students
have this sense of fit and significance but ab- who are described in research studies conducted
jectly horrible to feel excluded, unimportant, in the United States and Canada. These are
unheard, and invisible. students who have spent 3 years in the military
beginning at about 19 years of age, followed by
The Current Study a year of work, and then 6 months traveling for
“the big travel.” Moreover, there is an ongoing
In summary, the current research on college exposure to the Israeli–Palestinian conflict in-
students undergoing the COVID-19 global cluding terror attacks that involve negative
health crisis reflected three interrelated goals. mental health consequences. In short, these stu-
First, we sought to document how students re- dents have experienced a host of novel situa-
spond to changes in their learning situations that tions that represent opportunities to be adapt-
were implemented in response to the pandemic able in general. When viewed from this
and the need to engage in social distancing. This perspective, those students who are finding it
involved direct comparisons of retrospective as- difficult to adapt to the pandemic are likely
ADAPTABILITYAND EXPERIENCE OF ONLINE LEARNING 89

experiencing feelings and a form of stress un- through a number of possible options to assist
like their past experiences. me in this new situation.” Each item from the
Adaptability Scale was designed to reflect the
following criteria: (a) appropriate cognitive, be-
Method
havioral, or affective adjustment in response to
(b) uncertainty and/or novelty that has (c) a
Participants
constructive purpose or outcome. Martin et al.
The participants were undergraduate students (2013) advised that adaptability can be opera-
from five public higher education institutions in tionalized as a higher-order factor (indicated by
Israel (i.e., academic colleges). A total of 1,857 a cognitive– behavioral factor [six items] and an
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

students entered the secure online questionnaire affective factor [three items]) or as a first-order
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

with 1,217 of those students actually complet- factor (indicated by nine items). In the interest
ing the questionnaires (945 women [77.6%] and of parsimony, we adopted the latter operation-
272 men [22.4%]). Their mean age was 27.42 alization and focused on the nine-item compos-
years (mode and median ⫽ 25 years [SD ⫽ ite score for adaptability (␣ ⫽ .92), which is
7.703]). As such, our participants were older consistent with the results of a principal com-
than typical college student samples and most ponent analysis of the modified version indicat-
were adults rather than emerging adults. ing that the nine items were represented by one
In the present sample, 45.5% of the partici- factor (Eigenvalue ⫽ 5.56) with loadings rang-
pants were in their first academic year, 27.4% ing from .71 to .85 and accounting for 61.74%
were in their second year, and 21.9% were in of the variance. Participants were asked to rate
their third year. The academic majors of the their level of agreement with each item using
participants were as follows: 36.6% social sci- scales that ranged from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to
ences, 21.8% sciences, 5.7% art, 3.7% law, 7 (Strongly Agree). This instrument has been
4.6% humanities, and 27.7% management stud- shown to demonstrate adequate psychometric
ies. Participants reported that they participated properties (e.g., Martin et al., 2013).
in a mean of 6.77 synchronous online course Personality. Personality features were as-
sessions per week (1 to 15 course sessions with sessed using the 40-item International English
mode and median ⫽ 6 courses per week, SD ⫽ Big-Five Mini-Markers instrument (Thompson,
2.88), which is approximately an average of 2008), which was based on prior work by Gold-
about 12 hr per week in front of a computer berg (e.g., Goldberg, 1992; Hampson & Gold-
screen. The sample consisted predominantly of berg, 2006). Participants rated the extent to
participants who were single (70.4%), Jewish which 40 trait adjectives were accurate descrip-
(83.3%), and currently unemployed or on tors of themselves in order to capture the fol-
forced/unpaid vacation due to the COVID-19 lowing personality features: extraversion (8
pandemic (67.1%). The self-reported current items; e.g., Talkative [␣ ⫽ .75]), openness to
economic status of these participants was 4.8% experience (8 items; e.g., Creative [␣ ⫽ .76]),
very good, 22.0% good, 44.1% moderate, neuroticism (8 items; e.g., Moody [␣ ⫽ .74]),
20.0% not good, 5.7% bad, and 3.3% very bad. conscientiousness (8 items; e.g., Efficient [␣ ⫽
.85]), and agreeableness (8 items; e.g., Warm
Materials [␣ ⫽ .74]). Participants were asked to rate how
accurately each item described themselves us-
Adaptability. Adaptability was measured ing scales that ranged from 1 (Very Inaccurate)
using a modified version of the Adaptability to 7 (Very Accurate). This instrument has been
Scale (Martin et al., 2013). The modifications shown to demonstrate adequate psychometric
involved slightly rewording each of the nine properties (e.g., Thompson, 2008).
items of the Adaptability Scale to focus on the Learning experiences. We measured the
COVID-19 pandemic situation rather than the extent to which participants reported learning ex-
decontextualized form of adaptability that was periences since shifting to synchronic teaching
the focus of the original instrument. For exam- (i.e., current experience) as well as their typical
ple, the item “I am able to think through a level of same experience during traditional face-
number of possible options to assist me in a new to-face teaching (i.e., general experience). The
situation” was altered to read “I am able to think learning experiences measured were: Stressfulness
90 BESSER, FLETT, AND ZEIGLER-HILL

and Isolation (stress and loneliness), Negative Mood institutions in Israel were required to sign an
(anxiety, frustration, helplessness, and agreement with the Israeli Finance Ministry that
boredom[␣face-to-face ⫽ .80 and ␣ synchronous online ⫽ required them to move almost immediately to
.86]), Positive Mood (optimism, satisfaction and en- online teaching. Institutions postponed the
joyment [␣face-to-face ⫽ .80 and ␣ synchronous online ⫽ opening of the second semester for a week to
.85]), Learning: Cognitive (attentive and focused), allow for preparations of synchronous online
Depth of Learning (know and understand, and able teaching resources and core curriculum plan-
to apply), Motivation (responsibility and commit- ning. Faculty were required to learn the peda-
ment to learning), and Performance (meeting my gogical and technological aspects associated
standard and expectations). The Cronbach Alpha’s with the transition, which required becoming
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

for these six items were ␣face-to-face ⫽ .90 and family with platforms used for online teaching
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

␣synchronous online ⫽ .92. We also assessed relatedness such as Zoom and Google Meet. In addition, it
experiences with six items (i.e., “I feel accepted,” is important to note that most university courses
“I feel like belong,” “I feel ignored,” “I feel un- in Israel have traditionally been taught using
important,” “I feel like someone special,” and “I face-to-face on-campus with very few courses
feel like I matter”). Two items each were designed offered in an asynchronous online format or
to assess perceived belongingness, mattering, and using a mixed teaching (hybrid) format such as
antimattering. Mattering and antimattering were as- flipped classrooms. Thus, most of the students
sessed as separate measures in light of evidence and faculty in Israel had little, if any, experience
suggesting that mattering and antimattering are not with online learning in general and synchronous
simply polar opposites representing the same dimen- online learning in particular. Faculty members
sion (see Flett, 2018a, for a discussion). Four items were required to convert their courses to these
(␣face-to-face ⫽ .80 and ␣ synchronous online ⫽ platforms with a degree of urgency. In essence,
.81) were from a scale of relatedness constructed this constituted an emergency pivot to online
by Furrer and Skinner (2003) and two additional learning by students and faculty who were not
highly face-valid items were written for the pur- expecting or anticipating this need.
poses of the current study (i.e., “I feel like I Recruitment of the participants was as fol-
belong” and “I feel like I matter”). Previously, lows: An e-mail with a link to a secure online
Flett (2018a) noted that while the scale created by questionnaire was sent by the internal systems
Furrer and Skinner (2003) was designed to tap of the five public higher education institutions
relatedness and belongingness, three of the four to their undergraduate students, at the end of the
items are worded in a manner that actually reflects fifth week of online synchronous learning, re-
feelings of mattering or not mattering (i.e., anti- questing volunteers for a study concerning “ex-
mattering). It was further noted that despite the periences with online synchronous learning.”
fact that belongingness and mattering are substan- This corresponds to the first week of the second
tially correlated, they represent distinct variables semester (or trimester) of the academic year in
to the extent that it is possible to have a sense of Israel so most students would have already had
belonging but not a sense of mattering (e.g., feel- at least one full semester of traditional face-to-
ing part of a group but not highly valued or sig- face learning before the transition to online syn-
nificant within the group). Mattering and belong- chronous learning.
ingness were assessed within the context of the The secure online questionnaire included the
online learning versus face-to-face learning in the informed consent, demographic background, in-
current study. Participants responded to all items formation about personality characteristics,
using a response scale that ranged from 1 (not at adaptability, current level of perceived learning
all) to 7 (very much). experiences during the transition to online syn-
chronous teaching, and general learning experi-
Procedure ences during previous periods of traditional
face-to-face learning. Participation in this
The decision on social remoteness and reso- study was voluntary and participants were
lution was made by the Israeli government a aware that they could withdraw from the
week before the second semester opened while study at any time. All participants provided
students in Israel were in the exam period dur- their signed, informed consent. No social se-
ing the end of the first semester. Academic curity numbers or other identifying data were
ADAPTABILITYAND EXPERIENCE OF ONLINE LEARNING 91

collected nor were any invasive examinations ables and with learning experiences in synchro-
conducted by the researchers. This project nous online learning (see Table 3).
was conducted with the Ethics Committee of Multivariate analyses.
the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Ha- The unique contribution of adaptability.
dassah Academic College. Using a series of hierarchical multiple regres-
sions, we estimated the unique variance statis-
Analytic Strategy tically predicted by each measure above and
Mean differences. Paired samples t tests beyond the others (see Table 4). That is, we
were used to examine mean differences for par- ascertained whether personality variables added
ticipants’ reported learning experiences in face- significantly to the predictions of experiences of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

synchronous online learning beyond the ratings


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to-face versus synchronous online learning


methods (see Table 1). of the same experience in face-to-face learning,
Univariate analyses. Zero-order Pearson and whether adaptability added significantly
correlations were used to examine (a) associa- above and beyond personality variables.
tions of personality variables with learning ex- The indirect associations model. The hy-
periences in synchronous online learning (see potheses for the present study were consistent
Table 2) and (b) associations of adaptability to with an indirect effects model of personality: we
the COVID-19 pandemic with personality vari- examined the indirect associations that person-

Table 1
Mean Differences for Learning Experiences in Face to Face vs. Synchronous Online Learning
Learning method face-to face Effect
synchronous online size
Learning experiences M SD M SD t(1,216) Cohen’s d
Stressfulness and isolation
Stress 3.52 1.95 4.32 2.23 ⫺9.72ⴱⴱⴱ 0.28
Loneliness 2.07 1.62 4.51 2.19 ⫺29.90ⴱⴱⴱ 0.86
Negative mood
Anxiety 2.56 1.74 3.71 2.32 ⫺16.11ⴱⴱⴱ 0.46
Frustration 2.97 1.74 4.74 2.14 ⫺22.23ⴱⴱⴱ 0.64
Helplessness 2.39 1.61 4.30 2.27 ⫺25.58ⴱⴱⴱ 0.73
Boredom 3.09 1.73 4.73 1.98 ⫺21.20ⴱⴱⴱ 0.61
Positive mood
Optimism 5.21 1.64 3.76 1.96 20.21ⴱⴱⴱ 0.58
Satisfaction 5.25 1.54 3.40 2.01 24.71ⴱⴱⴱ 0.71
Enjoyment 5.17 1.67 3.36 1.99 23.59ⴱⴱⴱ 0.67
Relatedness
Belongingness 10.44 3.15 7.44 3.68 25.15ⴱⴱⴱ 0.72
Mattering 7.96 3.50 5.75 3.24 20.50ⴱⴱⴱ 0.59
Antimattering 4.14 2.85 6.14 3.82 ⫺17.52ⴱⴱⴱ 0.50
Learning
Cognitive
Attentive and focused 5.35 1.54 3.39 1.97 25.20ⴱⴱⴱ 0.73
Depth of learning
Know and understand 5.49 1.35 3.75 1.93 25.43ⴱⴱⴱ 0.73
Able to apply 5.70 1.34 3.66 2.02 28.57ⴱⴱⴱ 0.82
Motivation
Responsibility 5.99 1.34 4.47 2.12 21.02ⴱⴱⴱ 0.60
Commitment 6.04 1.30 4.24 2.14 24.10ⴱⴱⴱ 0.72
Performance
Meeting my standards and expectations 5.49 1.45 3.70 1.99 24.78ⴱⴱⴱ 0.71
Note. N ⫽ 1,217.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .0001.
92 BESSER, FLETT, AND ZEIGLER-HILL

Table 2
Correlations of Personality Variables With Learning Experiences in Synchronous Online Learning
Personality variables
Learning experiences in synchronous
online learning Extraversion Openness Neuroticism Conscientiousness Agreeable
Stress ⫺.04 ⫺.03 .16ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.06ⴱ ⫺.05
Loneliness .01 ⫺.03 .18ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.04 ⫺.01
Negative mood ⫺.04 ⫺.07ⴱⴱ .25ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.06ⴱ ⫺.08ⴱⴱ
Positive mood .07ⴱ .06ⴱ ⫺.14ⴱⴱⴱ .02 .06ⴱ
Belongingness .18ⴱⴱⴱ .14ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.16ⴱⴱⴱ .11ⴱⴱⴱ .14ⴱⴱⴱ
Mattering .17ⴱⴱⴱ .14ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.12ⴱⴱⴱ .04 .10ⴱⴱⴱ
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Antimattering ⫺.07ⴱ ⫺.05 .13ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.10ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.12ⴱⴱⴱ


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Attentive and focused .01 .05 ⫺.13ⴱⴱⴱ .08ⴱⴱ .06ⴱ


Depth of learning .05 .09ⴱⴱ ⫺.13ⴱⴱⴱ .09ⴱⴱ .05
Motivation .03 .06ⴱ ⫺.10ⴱⴱⴱ .15ⴱⴱⴱ .07ⴱ
Meeting my standards and expectations .06ⴱ .07ⴱⴱ ⫺.14ⴱⴱⴱ .12ⴱⴱⴱ .03
Note. N ⫽ 1,217.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001. ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .0001.

ality variables had with experiences in synchro- the ␹2/df ratio; (b) the root mean square error of
nous online learning via adaptability during the approximation (RMSEA); (c) the CFI; the (d)
COVID-19 pandemic. Accordingly, we tested GFI and (d) AGFI. A model in which ␹2/df
the fit of the proposed indirect effects model to was⬍ ⫽ 2, CFI, GFI and AGFI were greater
the empirical data for eight models correspond- than 0.95, and the RMSEA index was between
ing to each of the experience measures, using 0.00 and 0.08 (Hu & Bentler, 1999) was
path analyses in AMOS (Arbuckle, 2014). In deemed acceptable. These moderately stringent
addition to the overall nonsignificant chi-square acceptance criteria clearly reject inadequate or
(␹2 test of exact fit), the following fit indices poorly specified models, while accepting mod-
were used to evaluate the proposed models: (a) els for consideration that meet real-world crite-

Table 3
Correlations of Adaptability to the COVID⫺19 Pandemic With Personality
Variables and Learning Experiences in Synchronous Online Learning
Adaptability to the
Personality variables COVID-19 pandemic
Extraversion .22ⴱⴱⴱ
Openness .21ⴱⴱⴱ
Neuroticism ⫺.36ⴱⴱⴱ
Conscientiousness .27ⴱⴱⴱ
Agreeable .25ⴱⴱⴱ
Learning experiences in synchronous online learning
Stress ⫺.28ⴱⴱⴱ
Loneliness ⫺.30ⴱⴱⴱ
Negative mood ⫺.43ⴱⴱⴱ
Positive mood .37ⴱⴱⴱ
Belongingness .31ⴱⴱⴱ
Mattering .27ⴱⴱⴱ
Antimattering ⫺.27ⴱⴱⴱ
Attentive and focused .32ⴱⴱⴱ
Depth of learning .36ⴱⴱⴱ
Motivation .31ⴱⴱⴱ
Meeting my standards and expectations .36ⴱⴱⴱ
Note. N ⫽ 1,217.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .0001.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Table 4
Summary of the Series of Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analyses
Learning experiences in synchronous online learning
Meeting my
Negative Positive Attentive and Depth of standards and
Stress Loneliness mood mood Belongingness Mattering Antimattering focused learning Motivation expectations
Step 1:
Face-to-face levels of learning experience .04 ⫺.10ⴱⴱ ⫺.02 ⫺.10ⴱⴱⴱ .27ⴱⴱⴱ .38ⴱⴱⴱ .31ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.17ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.10ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.02 ⫺.04
R2 .002 .01 0.0 .01 .07 .14 .10 .03 .01 .00 .00
F for ⌬R2 2.13 11.86ⴱⴱ .27 12.21ⴱⴱⴱ 91.82ⴱⴱⴱ 201.46ⴱⴱⴱ 130.75ⴱⴱⴱ 35.83ⴱⴱⴱ 12.46ⴱⴱⴱ .30 2.00
Step 2: Personality variables
Extraversion ⫺.02 .01 ⫺.01 .07 .08ⴱ .05 .01 ⫺.01 .04 .01 .04
Openness ⫺.02 ⫺.05 ⫺.07 .05 .09ⴱ .04 ⫺.01 .04 .10ⴱⴱ .04 .07ⴱ
Neuroticism .15ⴱⴱⴱ .20ⴱⴱⴱ .26ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.14ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.12ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.10ⴱⴱⴱ .08ⴱ ⫺.12ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.12ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.09ⴱ ⫺.13ⴱⴱⴱ
Conscientiousness ⫺.03 ⫺.05 ⫺.03 .00 .03 ⫺.01 ⫺.03 .09ⴱⴱ .09ⴱ .14ⴱⴱⴱ .13ⴱⴱⴱ
Agreeable ⫺.01 .02 ⫺.02 .04 .04 .03 ⫺.05 .03 ⫺.01 .01 ⫺.04
⌬R2 .03 .04 .07 .03 .11 .16 .11 .03 .04 .03 .04
F for ⌬R2 6.83ⴱⴱⴱ 10.68ⴱⴱⴱ 19.87ⴱⴱⴱ 8.54ⴱⴱⴱ 10.12ⴱⴱⴱ 5.31ⴱⴱⴱ 3.08ⴱ 7.87ⴱⴱⴱ 9.65ⴱⴱⴱ 8.26ⴱⴱⴱ 10.88ⴱⴱⴱ
Step 3:
Adaptability ⫺.30ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.26ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.42ⴱⴱⴱ .38ⴱⴱⴱ .27ⴱⴱⴱ .27ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.27ⴱⴱⴱ .31ⴱⴱⴱ .36ⴱⴱⴱ .31ⴱⴱⴱ .36ⴱⴱⴱ
⌬R2 .10 .05 .20 .11 .16 .21 .16 .07 .10 .07 .10
F for ⌬R2 89.88ⴱⴱⴱ 68.30ⴱⴱⴱ 200.14ⴱⴱⴱ 159.14ⴱⴱⴱ 77.10ⴱⴱⴱ 82.47ⴱⴱⴱ 79.65ⴱⴱⴱ 100.29ⴱⴱⴱ 141.39ⴱⴱⴱ 96.29ⴱⴱⴱ 137.24ⴱⴱⴱ
Note. N ⫽ 1,217.
ADAPTABILITYAND EXPERIENCE OF ONLINE LEARNING

ⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001. p ⬍ .0001.
93
94 BESSER, FLETT, AND ZEIGLER-HILL

ria for reasonable fit and representation of the Results


data (Kelloway, 1998). Results of path coeffi-
cients for these models are presented in Table 5 Mean Differences
and fit indices presented in Table 6.
Next, Pearson correlations were also calcu- As can be seen in Table 1, participants re-
lated as supplementary analyses to examine the ported significantly higher levels of stress and
associations of experiences of relatedness vari- isolation as well as negative mood in synchro-
ables (i.e., mattering and belonging) and the nous online learning experience compared to
learning experiences in synchronous online their experience in previous traditional face-to
learning (see Table 7). Finally, additional sup- face learning. Moreover, they reported signifi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

plementary analyses conducted for the purposes cantly lower levels of positive mood, related-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of providing additional information revealed ness, concentration and focus, motivation, and
modest negative correlations between adapt- performance. Overall, participants experienced
ability and course sessions per week, r ⫽ ⫺.09, synchronous online learning as substantially
p ⬍ .01 and current economic status, r ⫽ ⫺.16, less positive in all aspects of the two learning
p ⬍ .001. However, controlling for course ses- experiences. The effect sizes for these paired
sions per week and current economic status did samples t tests were computed according to
not significantly alter the results reported below. Cohen (1988). As can be seen in Table 1, the
As a result, we decided not to include these two results indicate that the effect sizes ranged from
variables in the final analyses. In addition, sim- small (Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.28) to high (Cohen’s d ⫽
ilar associations were found for men and wom- 0.86), and overall, they were medium in mag-
en. Further, controlling for gender did not sig- nitude (mean Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.65, SD ⫽ .14; see
nificantly alter the results reported below. As a Chen, Cohen, & Chen, 2010).
result, we decided not to include gender in the To reduce the number of scales, since all
final analyses because it did not appear to play negative mood scales and positive mood scales
an important role in the results nor did we have were significantly different for the learning
any hypotheses concerning the role of gender. methods and had high internal reliability we

Table 5
Path Coefficients for the Series of the Models of the Indirect Associations
Estimate
Effect B SE ␤ t
Personality variables on adaptability to COVID-19
Extraversion ¡ Adaptability .28 .06 .13 5.21ⴱⴱⴱ
Openness ¡ Adaptability .32 .06 .14 5.41ⴱⴱⴱ
Neuroticism ¡ Adaptability ⫺.63 .05 ⫺.31 ⫺12.17ⴱⴱⴱ
Conscientiousness ¡ Adaptability .36 .06 .17 6.49ⴱⴱⴱ
Agreeable ¡ Adaptability .27 .08 .10 3.48ⴱⴱ
Adaptability to COVID-19 on learning experiences in synchronous online learning
Adaptability ¡ Stress ⫺.06 .00 ⫺.30 ⫺10.93ⴱⴱⴱ
Adaptability ¡ Loneliness ⫺.16 .01 ⫺.24 ⫺8.26ⴱⴱⴱ
Adaptability ¡ Negative mood ⫺.24 .02 ⫺.39 ⫺14.33ⴱⴱⴱ
Adaptability ¡ Positive mood ⫺.16 .01 .38 14.18ⴱⴱⴱ
Adaptability ¡ Belongingness .09 .01 .29 11.17ⴱⴱⴱ
Adaptability ¡ Mattering .07 .01 .26 10.31ⴱⴱⴱ
Adaptability ¡ Antimattering ⫺.08 .01 ⫺.25 ⫺9.53ⴱⴱⴱ
Adaptability ¡ Attentive and focused .05 .01 .31 11.71ⴱⴱⴱ
Adaptability ¡ Depth of learning .11 .01 .37 13.84ⴱⴱⴱ
Adaptability ¡ Motivation .11 .01 .31 11.47ⴱⴱⴱ
Adaptability ¡ Meeting my standards and expectation .06 .04 .37 13.68ⴱⴱⴱ
Note. N ⫽ 1,217.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .0001.
ADAPTABILITYAND EXPERIENCE OF ONLINE LEARNING 95

Table 6
Summary of Fit Indices for the Series of the Models of the Indirect Associations
Models for learning experiences in
synchronous online learning ␹2 df p CFI GFI AGFI RMSEA
Stress 7.14 5 .21 1 1 .99 0.02 (0.000–0.05)
Loneliness 5.56 4ⴱ .24 1 1 .99 0.02 (0.000–0.05)
Negative mood 7.52 4ⴱ .11 1 1 .99 0.03 (0.000–0.06)
Positive mood 6.55 5 .26 1 1 .99 0.005 (0.000–0.01)
Belongingness 6.25 5 .28 1 1 .99 0.014 (0.000–0.04)
Mattering 4.81 5 .44 1 1 .99 0.000 (0.000–004)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Antimattering 5.37 5 .37 1 1 .99 0.01 (0.000–0.04)


Attentive and focused 5.92 5 .31 1 1 .99 0.008 (0.000–0.04)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Depth of learning 4.2 5 .52 1 1 .99 0.000 (0.000–0.04)


Motivation 9.06 5 .11 .99 1 .99 0.03 (0.000–0.05)
Meeting my standards and expectations 10.09 5 .07 1 1 .99 0.03 (0.000–0.06)

Modification indices indicate that the path from Neuroticism to Loneliness should be kept in the model for loneliness (B ⫽
.04, ␤ ⫽ .11, t ⫽ 3.70, p ⬍ .0001) as well as for the model of Negative mood (B ⫽ .12, ␤ ⫽ .15, t ⫽ 4.36, p ⬍ .0001).

used a composite score for each in further anal- such that high levels of neuroticism were asso-
yses. In addition, we used a composite score of ciated with more negative experiences. Overall,
the two scales for depth of learning and for the the associations that personality features had
two scales of motivation. These decisions had with learning experiences were relatively small
the effect of reducing the number of outcome in magnitude.
variables (from 18 to 11) and subsequently the The results in Table 3 indicate that adapt-
number of models to be examined and related ability to the COVID-19 pandemic was sig-
errors associated with a larger number of anal- nificantly associated with both the personality
yses. variables and the learning experiences in syn-
Univariate Analyses chronous online learning. High levels of
adaptability to the COVID-19 pandemic were
As can be seen in Table 2, according to the linked with constructive learning experiences
zero-order correlations, among the personality outcomes. The associations that adaptability
variables, Neuroticism was found to have sig- to the COVID-19 pandemic has with both
nificant associations with all of the learning personality and learning experiences in syn-
experiences in synchronous online learning chronous online learning were stronger than

Table 7
Correlations of Experiences of Relatedness and the Other Learning Experiences
in Synchronous Online Learning
Relatedness in synchronous online learning
Learning experiences in synchronous
online learning Belongingness Mattering Antimattering
ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
Stress ⫺.31 ⫺.28 .32ⴱⴱⴱ
Loneliness ⫺.39ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.29ⴱⴱⴱ .37ⴱⴱⴱ
Negative mood ⫺.45ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.41ⴱⴱⴱ .48ⴱⴱⴱ
Positive mood .46ⴱⴱⴱ .47ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.44ⴱⴱⴱ
Attentive and focused .41ⴱⴱⴱ .41ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.39ⴱⴱⴱ
Depth of learning .48ⴱⴱⴱ .46ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.46ⴱⴱⴱ
Motivation .41ⴱⴱⴱ .37ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.36ⴱⴱⴱ
Meeting my standards and expectations .44ⴱⴱⴱ .41ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.40ⴱⴱⴱ
Note. N ⫽ 1,217.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .0001.
96 BESSER, FLETT, AND ZEIGLER-HILL

the ones that personality had with learning ations between personality variables and syn-
outcomes. chronous online learning experience outcomes.
These findings may support the assumed in- Figure 1 depicts the indirect associations path
direct associations that personality variables model that was evaluated.
may have with learning experiences via the We examined the models while controlling
adaptability to the COVID-19 pandemic. for the associations (covariation) among the
personality variables and their associations with
Multivariate Analyses the ratings of the same experience in face-to-
face learning and its association with adaptabil-
The unique contribution of adaptability. ity to the COVID-19 pandemic and with the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Using a series of hierarchical multiple regres- synchronous online learning experience out-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sion analyses, controlling for the associations come. Path analyses indicated (see Table 5) that
(covariation) among the personality variables high neuroticism, but low extraversion, openness,
(entered together in Step 2) and the ratings of conscientiousness, and agreeableness personality
the same experience in face-to-face learning variables scores were related to lower learning
(entered in Step 1) and its association with experiences in synchronous online learning via
adaptability to the COVID-19 pandemic (en- lower levels of adaptability to the COVID-19 pan-
tered in Step 3) and with the synchronous online demic. Adaptability to the COVID-19 pandemic
learning experience predicted outcome, we ex- was significantly associated with synchronous
amined the unique variance statistically pre- online learning experiences outcomes across an
dicted by each measure above and beyond the extensive series of analyses. As can be seen in
others. Table 4 reports the R-squared changes Table 6, the indirect effect models fit the em-
for each measure; here we indicate the unique pirical data very well. It was found for two of
variance statistically predicted by each measure the models (Loneliness and Negative Mood)
above and beyond the others. As can be seen, that modification indices indicated that Neurot-
personality variables added significantly to the icism had both significant direct and indirect
predictions of experiences of synchronous on- associations.
line learning beyond the ratings of the same Overall, the results presented in Table 5 in-
experience in face-to-face learning, and adapt- dicate that personality variables were associated
ability added significantly above and beyond significantly with adaptability to the COVID-19
personality variables. Adaptability was most pandemic, which was significantly associated
predictive of substantial unique variance in re- with experiences of synchronous online learn-
ported levels of negative mood and positive ing.
mood, but it is evident from Table 4 that adapt-
ability predicted significant unique variance Summary of Main Findings
across all of the measures. The variance infla-
tion factor (VIF) values for this analysis were Overall, compared to their experience in pre-
less than 1.19, which suggests that multicol- vious traditional face-to face learning, partici-
linearity was not an issue (Darlington & Hayes, pants experienced the synchronous online learn-
2017). ing as substantially less positive in all aspects of
Results of the univariate zero-order associa- the two learning experiences (significantly
tions and the above multivariate analyses indi- higher levels of stress and isolation as well as
cated that personality variables have low or no negative mood and significantly lower levels of
direct associations with synchronous online positive mood, relatedness, concentration and
learning experience outcomes. In the following focus, motivation, and performance). Multivar-
section, we will explore the assumed indirect iate analyses indicated that personality variables
associations of personality variables and syn- were associated significantly with adaptability
chronous online learning experience outcomes to the COVID-19 pandemic, which was signif-
via their associations with adaptability to the icantly associated with each of the experiences
COVID-19 pandemic.1
The indirect associations model. Path 1
According to Zhao, Lynch, Jr., & Chen (2010), there
analyses were used to examine the role of adapt- need not be a significant zero-order effect of predictor on
ability to the COVID-19 in the indirect associ- outcome to establish indirect effect.
ADAPTABILITYAND EXPERIENCE OF ONLINE LEARNING 97
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 1. The indirect associations of personality variables with experiences in synchronous


online learning through adaptability to the COVID-19 pandemic.

of synchronous online learning beyond levels of tive (i.e., belonging and mattering). We in-
the experience in previous traditional face-to cluded a focus on these psychosocial variables
face learning. involving feelings of belonging and mattering
or not mattering given the isolation associated
Supplemental Analyses with living conditions during the pandemic.
Second, our central focus was on individual
Finally, as can be seen in Table 7, significant
differences in adaptability to the pandemic as a
associations were found for the experiences of
relatedness variables and the other learning ex- potentially important factor in how students
periences in synchronous online learning. High coped with the transition to synchronous online
levels of belongingness and mattering were as- learning. This research was based on a modified
sociated broadly with more positive experiences version of the Adaptability Scale that specifi-
in synchronous online learning while feelings of cally measured self-reported adaptability to the
not mattering (i.e., antimattering) were associ- pandemic period.
ated broadly with negative experiences. Feel- Finally, this study also considered the extent
ings of belonging and mattering were linked to which the broad personality trait dimensions
with higher scores on positive indicators (e.g., that comprise the five-factor model relate to the
positive mood) and lower scores on negative reported emotions and learning experiences in
indicators (e.g., negative mood). synchronous online learning formats via adapt-
ability. The inclusion of these dimensions also
Discussion enabled us to test a model with adaptability
conceptualized as more proximal and personal-
The current study had three interrelated ity traits as distal.
goals. First, we contrasted student retrospective Our comparisons of the student responses and
reports of their experiences in the typical face- reactions to the two learning formats provided an
to-face learning format versus synchronous on- overall picture that illuminated the various ways
line learning. This comparison examined indi- in which many students were struggling with
cators of emotion and mood (both positive and the abrupt and unforeseeable shift in educa-
negative) as well as variables reflecting per- tional format necessitated by the global health
ceived self-worth from a relatedness perspec- pandemic. It was found across all of our mea-
98 BESSER, FLETT, AND ZEIGLER-HILL

sures that students as a group seemingly had a ing. Collectively, these associations accord with
pattern of responses indicating more positive the analyses of the complex nature of loneliness
reactions to the face-to-face format versus the and the pervasive negative impacts that tend to
synchronous online format. We describe these accompany it (see Cacioppo & Patrick, 2008).
differences in detail below with the caveat that The ratings for belongingness and mattering
we were, of course, not able to assess ratings were in keeping with reported levels of loneli-
while face-to-face class formats prevailed so ness. Our participants reported substantially
these ratings are retrospective accounts of how lower feelings of belonging and mattering and
things used to be from the students’ perspec- higher feelings of not mattering after the tran-
tives; as such, there is always a concern about sition. These results were found within the con-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

response bias with retrospective ratings. An- text of providing ratings of the learning formats
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

other caveat is that there is no doubt that the but it is likely that the responses provided reflect
ratings given to learning through an online syn- a generalized sense of feelings of belonging and
chronous format also must reflect to some de- mattering as a student at the college. This was
gree how students are adapting to the pandemic not particularly surprising given that these feel-
situation and the natural reactivity that ensues ings are best served through actual interactions
when autonomy and self-determination needs with other people who can make us feel valued
are thwarted by controlling, constricting life and connected with them. We will discuss the
circumstances. results for belonging and mattering in more
These considerations notwithstanding, the detail below. Still, it is worth noting that the
differences that were detected were of such differences found in the current situation sug-
magnitude that it is clear that the sudden onset gest that contextual factors can clearly impact
of synchronous online learning necessitated by the degree to which students can feel a sense of
the pandemic was comparatively undesirable relatedness to their fellow students and course
for many students. Our analyses found that cur- instructors.
rent conditions evoked stronger negative moods Robust differences were found following sta-
(i.e., anxiety, frustration, boredom) and lower tistical analyses that focused on the learning
positive moods. Our positive mood indicators variables. The overall pattern of findings sug-
included not only levels of enjoyment, but also gests that students who experienced an abrupt
satisfaction and optimism. Given the role of shift to a synchronous online format reported
confidence, hope, and optimism in resilience negative experiences, relative to the face-to-
and positive educational outcomes (see Feld- face format. As a group, these students reported
man & Kubota, 2015; Martin & Marsh, 2006; comparatively less learning and greater atten-
Rand, 2009), the substantially lower levels of tional difficulties with the shift to the online
optimism found with synchronous online learn- format. The results also provide clear indica-
ing, relative to face-to-face learning, should per- tions of motivational deficits and some degree
haps to be underscored in terms of their poten- of disengagement. Moreover, the analyses sug-
tial implications. gest that students are experiencing a greater
It is also important to emphasize that in line discrepancy between how they were actually
with concerns about the impact of physical and doing versus how they would ideally like to be
social isolation, one of the largest differences doing in terms of their expectations and stan-
detected was for reports of loneliness in the dards.
synchronous online condition. We highlight this
result not only because of widely expressed The Relevance of Adaptability
concerns about levels of loneliness among peo-
ple coping with the pandemic, but also other Given the significant challenges that these
indications within our data of the destructive- data reflect, it is incumbent upon students to
ness of loneliness. Additional analyses not re- find ways to adapt to the prevailing situation.
ported here due to space limitations confirm that Our analyses showed that our modified version
loneliness is linked with a host of negative of the Adaptability Scale was successful in
emotions (e.g., helplessness, frustration) and a terms of this adapted measure having acceptable
paucity of positive emotions. Loneliness was psychometric properties and yielding meaning-
also associated with negative reports of learn- ful findings. Psychometric analyses confirmed
ADAPTABILITYAND EXPERIENCE OF ONLINE LEARNING 99

that the nine items represented one broad adapt- learning formats. Higher levels of adaptability
ability factor despite the measure having three were associated with lower scores on the nega-
subsets of items reflecting affective, behavioral, tive mood composite and higher scores on the
and cognitive adaptability. positive mood composite. Greater adaptability
Our first focus was on how individual differ- was associated significantly with lower levels of
ences in adaptability to the pandemic were re- loneliness. The apparent advantages of adapt-
lated to the personality traits that comprise the ability extended to higher reported levels of
five-factor model. We found modest but signif- attention and focus, greater depth of learning,
icant associations with all five factors, and im- and indices reflecting greater motivation to
portantly, all five factors accounted for signifi- learn. These results accord with past evidence
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cant unique variance in levels of adaptability of the role of adaptability in motivated engage-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

when examined in a regression analysis. Stu- ment, learning, and performance (e.g., Burns,
dents who reported higher levels of adaptability Martin, & Collie, 2018; Collie et al., 2017;
also had lower levels of neuroticism and higher Martin, Yu, Ginns, & Papworth, 2017), but our
levels of conscientiousness, openness, agree- results are still quite unique due to our specific
ableness, and extraversion. This both replicates focus on adaptability to the global health pan-
and extends past work by Martin et al. (2013), demic and all of the challenges and concerns
which found that these broad traits were asso- associated with the COVID-19 pandemic.
ciated in a similar manner with adaptability in As noted earlier, the current study has to rely
general. The most robust associations in the on the concurrent assessment of participants’
current research were with lower neuroticism pandemic experiences and retrospective assess-
and greater conscientiousness and this outcome ment of experiences with a face-to-face learning
replicates the findings reported by Martin et al. mode. We conducted analyses that examined
(2013). the link between adaptability and ratings on
The obtained link with extraversion and synchronous online learning after controlling
adapting to the pandemic is not particularly for retrospective accounts of those same expe-
robust but it is interesting given the natural riences in face-to-face classroom situations. All
tendency to presume that socially isolated ex- of the associations between adaptability and
traverts are likely to suffer disproportionately pandemic-related learning experiences were
during the pandemic. This is still likely for a still evident after controlling for perceptions of
subset of these individuals, but this should be earlier experiences. It was once again the case
mitigated for those extraverted students who that higher levels of adaptability appeared to be
have higher levels of adaptability. serving students well in terms of their more
The findings with openness deserve to be recent learning experiences. Of course, we can-
briefly highlighted given that it might be ex- not rule out that adaptability and these reports
pected that openness to new experiences could are not linked causally and, indeed, it is possible
extend to greater tolerance for uncertainty that that reciprocal relations exist.
could conceivably be reflected in more positive Our analyses showed that adaptability was
reactions to synchronous online learning. How- associated with all of the outcome measures
ever, there were only small associations be- even when personality and retrospective ratings
tween openness and the learning variables (e.g., of face-to-face learning were included in the
depth of understanding) and a small positive model. It is worth noting that adaptability was
association between openness and adaptability. clearly most predictive of variance in levels of
This pattern of results may reflect how openness positive and negative mood. This pattern of
is measured and the complexities of the open- results could signify that while adaptability was
ness construct that see it associated with ambi- assessed in terms of cognitive, behavioral, and
guity intolerance in certain defined contexts emotional adaptability, the unique circum-
(see Bardi, Guerra, & Ramdeny, 2009; McCrae, stances students found themselves in were more
1996). strongly reflected in emotional reactions, and
The associations detected between adaptabil- being able to adapt emotionally may have been
ity and learning-related ratings clearly attest to especially important. This interpretation is in
the relevance of adaptability in terms of the keeping with the related work by Bonanno and
perceived capability of students to adapt to new colleagues (see Bonanno, Papa, Lalande, West-
100 BESSER, FLETT, AND ZEIGLER-HILL

phal, & Coifman, 2004; Westphal, Seivert, & solving as well as facilitate an atmosphere of
Bonanno, 2010; Zhu & Bonanno, 2017), which hopefulness could prove quite effective.
highlights the importance of expressive flexibil- Unfortunately, at present, there is not much
ity. The premise of this work is that it is highly evidence in the published literature that there
adaptive to be flexible in the modulation of has been an extensive focus thus far on promot-
emotional expressions in response to situational ing adaptability among students. Perhaps one of
demands, such as the unique situational and the greatest lessons of the pandemic is that there
educational demands that faced our participants. is much to be said for establishing practices and
Proponents of this work regard this capacity as programs that are designed to proactively en-
perhaps most needed, relative to other emotion- hance adaptability among students and, more
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

related capabilities, and the current results in- generally, among other people in the commu-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

volving adaptability as it relates to experienced nity. It is important to reiterate that adaptability


moods fit these claims. is different from resilience because adaptability
It would be useful for future research in sim- is about coping and thriving in new circum-
ilar situations to examine the nature of individ- stances rather than having the ability to bounce
ual differences in self-reported levels of adapt- back from setbacks. The logical question that
ability. A key goal is to get a better follows from our study is “What can colleges
understanding of how individual participants and universities do at a broad level and what can
interpret and understand adaptability. Several individual instructors do at a more specific level
possibilities may be involved here. For instance, in order to promote adaptability?” Our sense is
it is possible that perceptions of adaptability are that there is merit in considering the promotion
linked with implicit beliefs and narratives about of adaptability from a possible selves frame-
adaptability, given the role of implicit beliefs work informed by the research and theory of
about personal histories (see Ross, 1989). It scholars such as Markus and Oyserman (e.g.,
would also be potentially revealing to determine Markus & Nurius, 1986, 1987; Oyserman &
whether self-reported adaptability is reflected in Fryberg, 2006; Oyserman, Gant, & Ager, 1995).
daily cognitive, affective, and behavioral reac- Students who are relatively low in adaptability
tions. need to embrace a possible self that includes
Our working model was one in which the link being able to adjust well to new and uncertain
between more distal personality factors and circumstances. A key component of adaptability
more positive reactions to online learning was is the belief that one has or can develop the
via reported levels of adaptability. Substantial capacity to be adaptable and can demonstrate
support for this indirect associations model was this capability. It is known from work on the
obtained (see Figure 1), though it is important to promotion of possible selves that the develop-
reiterate that these results were based entirely ment of a positive possible self is more likely
on cross-sectional data (see Maxwell & Cole, when certain factors are in place and these fac-
2007, for an extended discussion of the limita- tors seem highly relevant to promoting a sense
tions inherent in the examination of indirect of being adaptable. These factors include plau-
associations using cross-sectional data). These sible strategies and resources, a fit with their
results have potentially important conceptual sense of who students are in terms of their core
implications in terms of the need to incorporate identities, and supportive relationships that en-
adaptability mechanisms into models that link courage the development of the possible self.
broad personality traits and various learning Another key aspect is framing any struggles in
outcomes, including motivation and perfor- realizing the possible self as normative and ex-
mance indicators. These results also have some perienced commonly by others as well (see
noteworthy practical implications. Given our Oyserman, Bybee, & Terry, 2006; Oyserman,
model that highlights the role of adaptability, it Terry, & Bybee, 2002). On a similar note, it is
is evident that colleges and universities can important to retain a sense of hope and opti-
better prepare their students for success by en- mism, and adaptability should be enhanced by
gaging in prevention efforts designed to pro- taking the type of approach advocated by Sny-
mote increased levels of adaptability among der (1991) in his work on hope. This work
their students. Training exercises designed to involves an emphasis on both “the will and the
instill flexible thinking, coping, and problem- ways” because hope involves both a sense of
ADAPTABILITYAND EXPERIENCE OF ONLINE LEARNING 101

agency and pathways (i.e., specific strategies) and mattering were associated with elevated
that can be put into practice. levels of adaptability in accordance with previ-
This focus on the promotion of adaptability ous research linking belongingness and matter-
should emphasize adaptability as a general ori- ing with adaptability, buoyancy, and resilience
entation activated in response to unforeseeable (see Flett et al., 2016; Skinner, Zimmer-
and unexpected challenges such as those Gembeck, & Connell, 1998; Wessel, Ryan, &
brought about by the pandemic, but adaptability Oswald, 2008). Other associations detected in
can also be a general capacity or resource de- the current study indicate that increases in levels
signed to assist students with other transitions of belonging and mattering may be pathways to
that may reflect their personal life circum- improvements in students’ levels of attention,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

stances. Students who have developed a capac- motivation, learning, and meeting expectations,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ity to be adaptable should be better able to cope and this accords with evidence for the role of
with personal disasters but also situations that mattering and belongingness in positive learn-
many students should face in the future such as ing orientations and outcomes (see Flett, 2018a;
needing to adjust to a new job that can amount Furrer & Skinner, 2003; King, 2015; Skinner,
to a very different work experience than what Furrer, Marchand, & Kindermann, 2008). More
was anticipated. generally, these results signify the potential use-
Given the relevance of adaptability according fulness and relevance of positive psychology
to our current results, it is important to conduct constructs in adaptability in general and in the
broad research in future investigations to iden- learning adaptability of students.
tify personal and contextual factors that predict Collectively, these results are entirely in
adaptability. Although the association was mod- keeping with arguments about the need for col-
est in overall magnitude, here it is worth noting leges and universities to find authentic ways to
that there was a tendency in the current study promote these interpersonally based feelings
for lower levels of adaptability to be reported by among their students (see Flett et al., 2019). The
our participants with lower levels of economic results for mattering and belonging in the cur-
status. This is noteworthy given concerns ex- rent study were with respect to the two different
pressed by public health officials about the ad- learning conditions, but there is much that can
ditional challenges and risks of COVID-19 ex- be done by institutions and by individual in-
posure for people of lower socioeconomic structors to promote relatedness and feelings of
status. It may be the case that not only is con- mattering and belongingness among their stu-
tracting the virus more likely for people with dents. When it comes to online synchronous
lower economic standing, they may also feel online learning opportunities, this could be as
less capable of adapting to the challenges asso- simple as providing regular opportunities for
ciated with physical and social isolation, addi- students to express their needs so that they have
tional economic burden, and profound disrup- a clear sense of having a voice, and being sure
tions to daily routines. Financial considerations to provide safe opportunities for students who
would almost certainly add to the challenges are typically reserved to participate. In some
facing many students. instances, these students with elements of social
Our secondary focus on belonging and mat- anxiety may be better served by opportunities to
tering was an emphasis in the current research individually connect with their instructors.
for the various reasons outlined earlier. Our There are also implications here in terms of how
results illustrate why it may be beneficial to find feelings of not mattering relate to a sense of
practical ways to boost the extent to which being visible. Many students who feel like they
students develop an enhanced sense of belong- do not matter feel like they are invisible to
ing and mattering and decrease the degree to others. It is important to keep in mind that while
which marginalizing experiences make students students may be visible online in an objective
feel like they do not matter. Analyses indicated sense, they may not feel psychologically visi-
that belongingness and mattering had robust ble, especially if their various psychological
links with emotional reactions to online syn- needs are not being met. The usual needs to see
chronous learning such that both were associ- oneself as competent, autonomous, and con-
ated with lower levels of negative moods and nected to others include a need to feel a sense of
higher levels of positive moods. Both belonging belonging and mattering by being someone who
102 BESSER, FLETT, AND ZEIGLER-HILL

is valued by others and to be both seen and thrive in uncertain and ambiguous times that
heard. may include an element of threat. Adaptability
The current research investigation yielded is associated in a predictable manner with the
many novel findings as students attempt to cope traits that comprise the five-factor model of
with the challenges of an extremely unique and personality (i.e., lower neuroticism and higher
uncertain situation but, of course, the usual cau- levels of conscientiousness, openness, agree-
tions and concerns related to research investi- ableness, and extraversion) and the students
gations still apply. As suggested earlier, causal who are higher in adaptability are both less
inferences are not warranted due to the cross- upset and more positive when asked to evaluate
sectional nature of this research. One byproduct this transition in learning conditions. It seems
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of this complex period is that it points to the more generally that the adaptability construct is
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

need for a focus on complexities when inter- a broad orientation but also can be applied in
preting research findings. Ideally, research of useful ways to specific contexts. As such, adapt-
this nature will include a longitudinal compo- ability merits much more attention in models of
nent that includes a focus on long-term out- coping and self-regulation. The potential of
comes experienced by students (for a discus- adaptability as a core construct that is relevant
sion, see Chen & Bonanno, 2020). Second, our to daily life is particularly apparent as the un-
results are based solely on self-report data and certainly and need to adapt to the pandemic as
future research of this nature would be but- an evolving context continues to be highly sa-
tressed by the inclusion of observer ratings, lient around the world.
especially in terms of the broad personality fac-
tors. Third, it is important to note issues per-
taining to the generalizability of our findings. It References
is typically the case that college students from
Israel are older than in many other countries. Allen, K., Kern, M. L., Vella-Brodrick, D., Hattie, J.,
Our participants had an average age of over 27 & Waters, L. (2018). What schools need to know
years old and had military training, so they are about fostering school belonging: A meta-analysis.
Educational Psychology Review, 30, 1–34. http://
older and have a broader set of experiences than dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-016-9389-8
is typical for college students in many other Arbuckle, J. L. (2014). IBM SPSS Amos 19 user’s
countries. Although we expect that the current guide. Crawfordville, FL: Amos Development
results are quite generalizable to students from Corporation.
other countries, generalizability needs to be Bardi, A., Guerra, V. M., & Ramdeny, G. S. D.
evaluated rather than assumed. However, it is (2009). Openness and ambiguity intolerance: Their
our sense that our results would have been sim- differential relations to well-being in the context of
ilar if evaluated in other samples. Moreover, an academic life transition. Personality and Indi-
themes such as adaptability, belonging, and vidual Differences, 47, 219–223. http://dx.doi.org/
mattering should also be highly relevant among 10.1016/j.paid.2009.03.003
Besser, A., Lotem, S., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2020).
elementary and high school students needing to Psychological stress and vocal symptoms among
adapt to abrupt transitions in their learning en- university professors in Israel: Implications of the
vironments. Reactions to distance learning shift to online synchronous teaching during the
could vary considerably as a function of these COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Voice. Advance
factors. online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
In summary, the results of the current study .jvoice.2020.05.028
illustrated the challenges for students and the Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M.,
individual differences among them in their & Coifman, K. (2004). The importance of being
adaptability to a sudden, abrupt transition away flexible: The ability to both enhance and suppress
from traditional learning approaches to an on- emotional expression predicts long-term adjust-
ment. Psychological Science, 15, 482– 487. http://
line learning approach that was made necessary dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00705.x
by the growth of the coronavirus and associated Burns, E. C., Martin, A. J., & Collie, R. J. (2018).
safety concerns. The current findings serve as a Adaptability, personal best (PB) goals setting, and
reminder that there are salient individual differ- gains in students’ academic outcomes: A longitu-
ences in adaptability and students with higher dinal examination from a social cognitive perspec-
levels of this capability may be better able to tive. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 53,
ADAPTABILITYAND EXPERIENCE OF ONLINE LEARNING 103

57–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018 Digman, J. M., & Takemoto-Chock, N. K. (1981).


.02.001 Factors in the natural language of personality: Re-
Cacioppo, J. T., & Patrick, W. (2008). Loneliness: analysis, comparison, and interpretation of six ma-
Human nature and the need for social connection. jor studies. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 16,
New York, NY: Norton. 149–170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s1532790
Carver, C. S., & Connor-Smith, J. (2010). Personality 6mbr1602_2
and coping. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, Feldman, D. B., & Kubota, M. (2015). Hope, self-
679–704. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych efficacy, optimism, and academic achievement:
.093008.100352 Distinguishing constructs and levels of specificity
Casagrande, M., Favieri, F., Tambelli, R., & Forte, G. in predicting college grade-point average. Learn-
(2020). The enemy who sealed the world: Effects ing and Individual Differences, 37, 210–216.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

quarantine due to the COVID-19 on sleep quality, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.11.022


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

anxiety, and psychological distress in the Italian Flett, G. L. (2018a). The psychology of mattering:
population. Sleep Medicine, 75, 12–20. http://dx Understanding the human need to be significant.
.doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2020.05.011 Cambridge, MA: Academic Press/Elsevier. Re-
Chen, H., Cohen, P., & Chen, S. (2010). How big is trieved from https://www.elsevier.com/books/the-
a big odds ratio? Interpreting the magnitudes of psychology-of-mattering/flett/978-0-12-809415-0
odds ratios in epidemiological studies. Communi- Flett, G. L. (2018b). Resilience to interpersonal
cations in Statistics Simulation and Computation, stress: Why mattering matters when building the
39, 860– 864. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0361 foundation of mentally healthy schools. In A. Le-
0911003650383 schied, D. H. Saklofske, & G. L. Flett (Eds.), The
Chen, S., & Bonanno, G. A. (2020). Psychological handbook of school-based mental health promo-
adjustment during the global outbreak of COVID- tion: An evidence informed framework for imple-
19: A resilience perspective. Psychological Trau- mentation (pp. 383– 410). New York, NY: Spring-
ma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, er. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-89842-
12(S1), S51–S54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
1_20
tra0000685
Flett, G. L., Endler, N. S., & Fairlie, P. (1999). The
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the
interaction model of anxiety and the threat of Que-
behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
bec’s separation from Canada. Journal of Person-
baum.
ality and Social Psychology, 76, 143–150. http://
Collie, R. J., Holliman, A. J., & Martin, A. J. (2017).
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.1.143
Adaptability, engagement and academic achieve-
ment at university. Educational Psychology, 37, Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2020). The perfection-
632– 647. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01443410 ism pandemic meets COVID-19: Understanding
.2016.1231296 the stress, distress, and problems in living for
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal per- perfectionists during the global health crisis. Jour-
sonality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO nal of Concurrent Disorders, 2, 80–105.
Personality Inventory. Psychological Assessment, Flett, G. L., Khan, A., & Su, C. (2019). Mattering and
4, 5–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.5 psychological well-being in college and university
Darlington, R. B., & Hayes, A. F. (2017). Regression students: A review and recommendations for cam-
analysis and linear models: Concepts, application, pus-based initiatives. International Journal of
and implementation. New York, NY: The Guilford Mental Health and Addiction, 17, 667– 680. http://
Press. dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11469-019-00073-6
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2012). Self-determina- Flett, G. L., Su, C., Ma, L., & Guo, L. (2016).
tion theory. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. W. Krug- Mattering as a unique resilience factor in Chinese
lanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of the- children: A comparative analysis of predictors of
ories of social psychology (pp. 416– 437). depression. International Journal of Child and Ad-
Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. http://dx.doi.org/10 olescent Resilience, 4, 91–102.
.4135/9781446249215.n21 Flett, G. L., & Zangeneh, M. (2020). Mattering as a
Derogatis, L. R., & Coons, H. I. (1993). Self-report vital support for people during the COVID-19 pan-
measures of stress. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz demic: The benefits of feeling and knowing that
(Eds.), Handbook of stress: Theoretical and clini- someone cares during times of crisis. Journal of
cal aspects (2nd ed., pp. 200–233). Toronto, ON: Concurrent Disorders, 2, 106–123.
The Free Press. France, M. K., & Finney, S. J. (2010). Conceptual-
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emer- ization and utilization of university mattering: A
gence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of construct validity study. Measurement and Evalu-
Psychology, 41, 417– 440. http://dx.doi.org/10 ation in Counseling and Development, 43, 48– 65.
.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002221 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0748175610362369
104 BESSER, FLETT, AND ZEIGLER-HILL

Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness education levels. Bulletin of Education and Re-
as a factor in children’s academic engagement and search, 39, 183–195.
performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves.
95, 148–162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663 American Psychologist, 41, 954–969. http://dx.doi
.95.1.148 .org/10.1037/0003-066X.41.9.954
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1987). Possible selves:
of personality”: The big-five factor structure. Jour- The interface between motivation and the self-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, concept. In K. Yardley & T. Honess (Eds.), Self
1216–1229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514 and identity: Psychosocial perspectives (pp. 157–
.59.6.1216 172). New York, NY: Wiley.
Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers Martin, A. J. (2017). Adaptability—What it is and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

for the big-five factor structure. Psychological As- what it is not: Comment on Chandra and Leong
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sessment, 4, 26– 42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ (2016). American Psychologist, 72, 696– 698.
1040-3590.4.1.26 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000163
Hampson, S. E., & Goldberg, L. R. (2006). A first Martin, A. J., & Marsh, H. W. (2006). Academic
large cohort study of personality trait stability over resilience and its psychological and educational
the 40 years between elementary school and correlates: A construct validity approach. Psychol-
midlife. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- ogy in the Schools, 43, 267–281. http://dx.doi.org/
chology, 91, 763–779. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 10.1002/pits.20149
0022-3514.91.4.763 Martin, A. J., Nejad, H. G., Colmar, S., & Liem,
Holliman, A., Martin, A. J., & Collie, R. (2018). G. A. D. (2013). Adaptability: How students’ re-
Adaptability, engagement, and degree completion: sponses to uncertainty and novelty predict their
A longitudinal investigation of university students. academic and non-academic outcomes. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 38, 785–799. http://dx Educational Psychology, 105, 728–746. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2018.1426835 .doi.org/10.1037/a0032794
Horesh, D., & Brown, A. D. (2020). Traumatic stress Martin, A. J., Yu, K., Ginns, P., & Papworth, B.
in the age of COVID-19: A call to close critical (2017). Young people’s academic buoyancy and
gaps and adapt to new realities. Psychological adaptability: A cross-cultural comparison of China
Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, with North America and the United Kingdom.
12, 331–335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tra0000592 Educational Psychology, 37, 930–946. http://dx
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit .doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2016.1202904
indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conven- Martin, F., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L., & Budhrani, K.
tional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural (2017). Systematic review of two decades (1995 to
Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. http://dx.doi.org/10 2014) of research on synchronous online learning.
.1080/10705519909540118 American Journal of Distance Education, 31,
Kelloway, E. K. (1998). Using LISREL for structural 3–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08923647.2017
equation modeling: A researcher’s guide. New- .1264807
bury Park, CA: SAGE. Maxwell, S. E., & Cole, D. A. (2007). Bias in cross-
King, R. B. (2015). Sense of relatedness boosts en- sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psy-
gagement, achievement, and well-being: A latent chological Methods, 12, 23– 44. http://dx.doi.org/
growth model study. Contemporary Educational 10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.23
Psychology, 42, 26–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j McCrae, R. R. (1996). Social consequences of expe-
.cedpsych.2015.04.002 riential openness. Psychological Bulletin, 120,
Kivlighan, D. M., III, Abbas, M., Gloria, A. M., 323–337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909
Aguinaga, A., Frank, C., & Frost, N. D. (2018). .120.3.323
Are belongingness and hope essential features of Norman, W. T. (1963). Toward an adequate taxon-
academic enhancement groups? A psychosociocul- omy of personality attributes: Replicated factors
tural perspective. Journal of Counseling Psychol- structure in peer nomination personality ratings.
ogy, 65, 204–213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66,
cou0000266 574–583. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0040291
Lobel, T. E., Gilat, I., & Endler, N. S. (1993). The Olt, P. A. (2018). Virtually there: Distant freshmen
Gulf War: Distressful reactions to SCUD missile blended in classes through synchronous online ed-
attacks. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 6, 9–23. ucation. Innovative Higher Education, 43, 381–
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10615809308249529 395. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10755-018-9437-z
Malik, M., Fatiman, G., Hussain, A., & Sarwar, A. Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., & Terry, K. (2006). Pos-
(2017). E-learning: Students’ perspectives about sible selves and academic outcomes: How and
asynchronous and synchronous resources at higher when possible selves impel action. Journal of Per-
ADAPTABILITYAND EXPERIENCE OF ONLINE LEARNING 105

sonality and Social Psychology, 91, 188–204. namic? Journal of Educational Psychology, 100,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.188 765–781. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012840
Oyserman, D., & Fryberg, S. (2006). The possible Skinner, E. A., Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Connell,
selves of diverse adolescents: Content and function J. P. (1998). Individual differences in the develop-
across gender, race and national origin. In C. Dun- ment of perceived control. Monographs of the So-
kel & J. Kerpelman (Eds.), Possible selves: The- ciety for Research in Child Development, 63(2/3),
ory, research and applications (pp. 17–39). Haup- i⫹iii⫹v-vi⫹1–231. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
page, NY: Nova Science Publishers. 1166220
Oyserman, D., Gant, L., & Ager, J. (1995). A socially Snyder, C. R., Harris, C., Anderson, J. R., Holleran,
contextualized model of African American iden- S. A., Irving, L. M., Sigmon, S. T., . . . Harney, P.
tity: Possible selves and school persistence. Jour- (1991). The will and the ways: Development and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, validation of an individual-differences measure of


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1216–1232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514 hope. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-


.69.6.1216 ogy, 60, 570–585. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
Oyserman, D., Terry, K., & Bybee, D. (2002). A 3514.60.4.570
possible selves intervention to enhance school in- Thompson, E. R. (2008). Development and valida-
volvement. Journal of Adolescence, 25, 313–326. tion of an international English Big-Five mini-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jado.2002.0474 markers. Personality and Individual Differences,
Putwain, D. W., Loderer, K., Gallard, D., & Beau- 45, 542–548. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008
mont, J. (2020). School-related subjective well- .06.013
being promotes subsequent adaptability, achieve- Tovar, E., Simon, M. A., & Lee, H. B. (2009).
ment, and positive behavioural conduct. British Development and validation of the College Mat-
Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 92–108. tering Inventory with diverse urban college stu-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12266 dents. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling
Rand, K. L. (2009). Hope and optimism: Latent and Development, 42, 154–178. http://dx.doi.org/
structures and influences on grade expectancy and 10.1177/0748175609344091
academic performance. Journal of Personality, 77, Wessel, J. L., Ryan, A. M., & Oswald, F. L. (2008).
231–260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494 The relationship between objective and perceived
.2008.00544.x fit with academic major, adaptability, and major-
Rosenberg, M. (1985). Self-concept and psycholog- related outcomes. Journal of Vocational Behavior,
ical well-being in adolescence. In R. L. Leahy 72, 363–376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2007
(Ed.), The development of the self (pp. 205–246). .11.003
Toronto, ON: Academic Press. Westphal, M., Seivert, N. H., & Bonanno, G. A.
Rosenberg, M., & McCullough, C. B. (1981). Mat- (2010). Expressive flexibility. Emotion, 10, 92–
tering: Inferred significance and mental health 100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018420
among adolescents. Research in Community & Yang, D., Baldwin, S., & Snelson, C. (2017). Persis-
Mental Health, 2, 163–182. tence factors revealed: Students’ reflections on
Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the completing a fully online program. Distance Edu-
construction of personal histories. Psychological cation, 38, 23–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Review, 96, 341–357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ 01587919.2017.1299561
0033-295X.96.2.341 Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., Jr., & Chen, Q. (2010).
Segerstrom, S. C., & Smith, G. T. (2019). Personality Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and truths
and coping: Individual differences in responses to about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer
emotions. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 651– Research, 37, 197–206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/
671. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych- 651257
010418-102917 Zhu, Z., & Bonanno, G. A. (2017). Affective flexi-
Seiver, J. G., & Troja, A. (2014). Satisfaction and bility: Relations to expressive flexibility, feedback,
success in online learning as a function of the and depression. Clinical Psychological Science, 5,
needs for affiliation, autonomy, and mastery. Dis- 930–942. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/216770
tance Education, 35, 90–105. http://dx.doi.org/10 2617717337
.1080/01587919.2014.891427
Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kinder- Received May 7, 2020
mann, T. (2008). Engagement and disaffection in Revision received August 13, 2020
the classroom: Part of a larger motivational dy- Accepted August 21, 2020 䡲

You might also like