You are on page 1of 19

Native-View Paradigms: Multiple Cultures and Culture Conflicts in Organizations

Author(s): Kathleen L. Gregory


Source: Administrative Science Quarterly, Vol. 28, No. 3, Organizational Culture (Sep., 1983),
pp. 359-376
Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. on behalf of the Johnson Graduate School of Management,
Cornell University
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2392247 .
Accessed: 20/06/2014 18:07

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Sage Publications, Inc. and Johnson Graduate School of Management, Cornell University are collaborating
with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Administrative Science Quarterly.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Native-View Paradigms: This paper describes and critiques organizational culture
Multiple Cultures and studies done in industrial settings, some of which were
based on anthropological paradigms, including the
Culture Conflicts in Or- structural-functional and configurationist holistic
ganizations paradigms. Most failed to explore multiple "native" views.
In this paper, a multicultural model is proposed for large
Kathleen L. Gregory organizations, and problems of "cross-cultural" contact are
described. Native-view paradigms from anthropology, es-
pecially ethnoscience ethnography, are recommended for
exploring multiple perspectives in detail. An illustration
from a recent study of "Silicon Valley" technical profes-
sionals' "native" views is presented to demonstrate how
ethnoscience methods, in particular, can be applied to the
task of studying culture.

INTRODUCTION
The notion of "culture" is often associated with exotic, distant
peoples and places, with myths, rites, foreign languages and
practices. Recently, researchers have pointed out that within
our own society, organization members similarly engage in
rituals, pass along corporate myths and stories, and use arcane
jargon, and that these "informal" practices may foster or hinder
management's goals for the organization (Baker, 1980;
Schwartzand Davis, 1981; Dealand Kennedy, 1982; Petersand
Waterman, 1982). The study of organizational culture thus
becomes translated into the study of the informal or "merely"
social or symbolic side of corporate life.
In anthropology, where the concept is most fully developed,
culture concerns all aspects of a group's social behavior,
including their formal laws and technical know-how (Spradley
and McCurdy, 1975: 4). The traditionalgoal of anthropology has
been to make seemingly exotic practices of distant peoples
understandable by exploring them in context. Learning how
"native" participants make sense out of their own behavior
helps demystify apparently exotic practices. According to
Malinowski (1922: 25), the goal of ethnography is "to grasp the
native's point of view, his relation to life, to realize his vision of
? 1983 by Cornell University. his world."
0001 -8392/8~32803-0359/$00.75
Applying this anthropological approach in corporations leads
I would like to thank my colleagues who one to study participants' views about all aspects of corporate
read an earlier draft of this paper and/or experience. These would include the work itself, the technol-
offered insights, critiques, and sugges-
tions. In particular,I acknowledge Howard ogy, the formal organization structure, and everyday language,
Becker, Mike Boehm, Douglas Chene, not only myths, stories, or special jargon. That some re-
Adele Clarke, Joan Fujimora,Judith Rem-
ington, Helen Schwartzman, Leigh Star,
searchers select these for special emphasis says more about
Gwen Stern, Rachel Volberg, and Oswald the culture of the researchers than the researched, for whom
Werner. Several anonymousASQ re- all culture is equally taken for granted.
viewers also made suggestions that were
very much appreciated and led to some It is, in fact, the taken-for-granted quality of culture that
changes in the paper. I also gratefully ac-
knowledge Eleanor Wynn's encourage- presents important research and practical problems in organiza-
ment and assistance in gaining access to tions. More researchers have emphasized the homogeneity of
Silicon Valley research settings, and thank culture and its cohesive function than its divisive potential. This
those who participated as interviewees.
Tremont Research Institute, and especially paper suggests, however, that many organizations are most
its president, Elihu Gerson, provided tech- accurately viewed as multicultural.Subgroups with different
nical support during crucial early months
that was very much appreciated. This paper
occupational, divisional, ethnic, or other cultures approach
is dedicated to the memory of James P. organizational interactions with their own meanings and senses
Spradley who introduced me to the cultural of priorities. Ethnocentrism, the tendency to take for granted
perspective presented here. The views ex-
pressed in the paper are, of course, my one's own cultural view and to evaluate others' behavior in
own. terms of it, increases the tendency for misunderstandings and
359/AdministrativeScience Quarterly,28 (1983): 359-376

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
conflicts to occur. It is suggested that using "native-view"
paradigms from anthropology to study the cultures of organiza-
tion members would lead to more complete understandings of
these culture conflicts.

BACKGROUND: CULTURALPARADIGMS AND INDUS-


TRIALRESEARCH1
In organizational research, as in anthropology, there is not one
cultural paradigm, but many. Nearly every branch of organiza-
tional research has used a cultural approach or construct at
some time. Each "incarnation" has its unique history and
situation leading to particulardefinitions of culture and
methodologies. References to anthropological paradigms, with
examples from industrial research, are presented here, but
similar instances can be found in educational, medical, or other
organizational research histories.2
Culturalparadigms into which these "incarnations" fall can be
classified along three dimensions of contrast: (1) holistic-
particulate, (2) explanatory-interpretive, and (3) external-view -
native-view dimensions. This is an elaboration of Sanday's
(1979) classification of ethnographic paradigms into holistic,
1 semiotic, and behavioristic styles, where ethnography is the
"gParadigm"in this paper means conceptual process and product of cultural research.
orientation or perspective from which re-
search questions, methods, and styles of Holistic cultural paradigms lead one to view culture as an
explanation flow. A paradigm is a re-
searcher's "native"-view regarding appro-
integrated system, instead of focusing on particulartraits out of
priate research behavior and, like the other- context. The prototype for holistic cultural research is extended
native views discussed in this paper, is ethnography within an isolated social group. Usually one an-
more or less shared between those who
interact around particularresearch scenes thropologist researched a group through living with them and
(cf. Sanday, 1979; Morgan, 1980). To iden- tried to understand their behavior as meaningful in its context.
tify the tradition of organizational research The resulting ethnography sought to describe and explain the
in large, mostly private work organizations,
the word "industrial"is used to label the entire group's culture in a timeless way. This image of a robust,
kinds of institutions usually studied by coterminous, stable social group and culture persists, particu-
those who call themselves industrial larlyfor those who seek to describe the ethnography of a group
ethnographers (see Holzberg and Giovan-
nini, 1981, for a review). Different re- as a culture.
searchers use their own designations for
such settings, and in this paper the term Sanday (1979) contrasted holistic approaches according to the
"industrial"should be viewed broadly to explanatory-interpretive dimension and identified two
include corporate and, to some extent, bu-
reaucratic as well as manufacturing paradigms: (1) an explanatory structural-functional paradigm
settings. and (2) an interpretive configurationist paradigm. Explanatory
2 paradigms emphasized "uncovering causal relationships and
In educational research, "culture" has been explaining covarying patterns" (Sanday, 1979: 532), whereas
used both in the sense of "ethnicity," with
the enculturating function of schools and
configurationist paradigms were used by researchers "inter-
issues of cultural pluralism emphasized ested in describing and interpreting the whole, not in explaining
(see Kneller, 1965), and also in the sense of its origin . . " (Sanday, 1979: 531).
"school culture" or ethnography (see Wol-
cott, 1973; Gregory, 1976; Gregoryand Explanatory Holistic Paradigms
Mueller, 1980). In medical/mental health
research there have been numerous appli- The structural-functional paradigm adopts an organismic anal-
cations as well (see Goffman, 1961;
Schwartzman and Krause, 1981). Highbred ogy of a culture and explains how cultural parts function to
studies of medical education and occupa- maintain the integrity of a group's social structure. Malinowski
tions have also used a culturalapproach and Radcliffe-Brown exemplified this approach in anthropology
(see Becker et al., 1977).
(see Sanday, 1979).3 In the first half of this century, industrial
3
ethnographers applied this, and other explanatory holistic
Malinowski and Radcliffe-Brown differed
in their perspectives, with Malinowski em- paradigms, in a major "incarnation" of research from a cultural
phasizing the manner in which culture perspective.
functioned at an individuallevel, and
Radcliffe-Brown emphasizing the social The Human Relations school of management research used a
functions of culture. Both were influenced structural-functional paradigm to explore informal social organi-
by Weber and Durkheim, as were others in
other disciplines (see Harris, 1968: 464- zation or "human factors" in industry (see Mayo, 1933; Roeth-
567). lisberger and Dickson, 1939; Gardner, 1945; Warner and Low,
360/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Native-View Paradigms

1947; Richardson and Walker, 1948). In a recent review of


industrialanthropology, Holzberg and Giovannini (1981: 328)
summarized the approach in this body of industrial case study
research as follows:
The researchgoal was ethnographynot ethnology.... The re-
searchers characterizedthe enterpriseas a smallsociety inand of
itself, described its "culture"(routines,patterns,regularities,layout,
methods of keepingrecords,symbols, etc.) and mappedits social
structurein orderto delineate both formaland informalsocial
organization.
Rather than being basic research, these efforts were directed
toward enabling "scientific" managers to better control subor-
dinates by taking workers' cultural reactions into account
(Kanter, 1977: 23). The cultural control motivation of these
studies contradicts the more usual cultural relativist posture of
anthropology (see Herskovits, 1972). Further, the promanage-
ment position resulted in biased research that studied the
"irrational"behavior of lower ranking personnel and supported
unquestioningly the "rational manager" model (Kanter, 1977:
24). The strength of the Human Relations school was in its
recognition of the importance of informal social organization in
industry (see Arensberg, 1978, for further discussion; for
similar case studies, see Whyte, 1956; Moore, 1962).
Structural-functional paradigms were critiqued for leading to
static, noncontextual studies. This tendency was probably an
artifact of the small-society image they applied as a model.
Later, systems paradigms were applied to analyze industry
diachronically and within a broader social context (Holzberg and
Giovannini, 1981: 332; see also Rodin, Michaelson, and Britan,
1978). Conflict or structural inequality models (including Marxist
models) are another class of holistic explanatory paradigm that
has been applied to challenge the status-quo assumptions of
earlier research (see Nash, 1979; Kanter, 1977).4

Interpretive Holistic Paradigms


The premise of an interpretive configurationist paradigm is that
cultures can be described and compared in terms of distinct,
personality-like patterns of integrated principles. In anthropol-
ogy, this view is rooted in Boasian historical particularism, and
its introduction is credited to Benedict (1934). The paradigm
was further elaborated by Mead and others (see Sanday, 1979:
529-531). Various integrating principles have been proposed,
such as themes that run through cultures (Opler, 1945; Agar,
1979), values, norms, or cultural premises (see Kluckhohn and
4 Leighton, 1974: 303). The researcher's goal is to extract and
Morgan (1980) provided an alternative clas- delineate these principles.
sification of organization research
paradigms. Especially of interest here is his The configurationist paradigm is criticized for being simplistic,
description of how metaphors (such as reducing complex cultures to a small number of principles, and
machines, organisms, or even games) con-
strain theorizing in particular ways. In cul-
for overdrawing the extent to which principles lead to norma-
ture research, the small-society metaphor tive behavior (see Harris, 1968: 393-421). It has also been
has shaped research in important ways;
criticized for insisting that cultures are consistently and fully
however, the extent to which it is, and is
not, appropriate has not been systemati- integrated, when in reality cultures encompass discordant
cally studied. Like Morgan, I advocate apply- elements. Integration is an ongoing process that is never
ing multiple metaphors. In fact, some of
these may come directly from native
complete (Spradley and McCurdy, 1975: 593-596). The contri-
viewpoints. In philosophy of science, bution of the configurationist paradigm is in its recognition of
Feyerabend's (1975) "epistemological principles that cut across cultural domains and that may act as
anarchy" represents a position similar to
Morgan's (1980) and comes closest to the
integrators (see Spradley, 1979: 185-203, fora discussion of
posture I advocate. themes).
361/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Sanday (1979: 532) pointed out that organizational research in
the past had largely drawn from explanatory cultural paradigms
rather than from interpretive ones and suggested:
Perhapsit is time to returnto the holisticstyle as practicedby Benedict
and Mead. Itwould be interestingto see whether one couldtake a
varietyof organizational settings andcomparethem fortheirconfigura-
tionaltextureas Benedictcomparedthe Zuni,Dobuans,and Kwakiutl,
oras Meadcomparedthree New Guineasocieties forthe relationship
between sex andtemperament.
Her words appear to have influenced or prophesized a current
proliferation of corporate culture studies. Management re-
searchers are comparing companies in terms of configurations
of values. These configurations, or "cultures," are believed to
underlie characteristic behavior in organizations, leading to
successful or unsuccessful organization performance. Initially,
Ouchi (1981) compared Japanese companies' sets of values to
some American companies' values and was able to distinguish
three configurations that he called "A" (American), "J"
(Japanese), and "Z" (an American type with many Japanese-
like characteristics). More detailed analyses of Japanese and
American value configurations followed (see Pascale and
Athos, 1981). Growing conviction that cultural integration and
particularconfigurations of values contribute to company suc-
cess led researchers to typologize and compare various Ameri-
can companies according to corporate cultures (see Baker,
1980; Schwartz and Davis, 1981; Deal and Kennedy, 1982;
Peters and Waterman, 1982) and to examine the development
and transmission of value sets (see Pfeffer, 1981; Martin,
1982).
The current corporate culture studies are not substantially
different from earlier Human Relations research, in that the
goal is still to illustrate the impact of "irrational"human factors
on "rational"corporate objectives. "Rational" corporate objec-
tives correspond to management's goals for the organization.
Researchers in both areas sought to provide managers with
tools to assess and control the organizational culture of their
subordinates. This "managementcentric" motivation in corpo-
rate culture research probably contributes to the tendency in
these studies to evaluate the effectiveness of the culture with
respect to management goals. In some instances, cultural
integration has been evaluated in terms of the degree of
compliance or noncompliance with the normative view manag-
ers have of the organization, or even to define the dominant
culture as the explicit management philosophy. Managers are
then said to create organizational cultures and train employees
in them, as Baker (1980: 8) related:
Some successful corporationshave especiallydistinctiveculturesthat
they activelycultivateand manage, andthat contributesignificantlyto
theirsuccess.... IBM'stop managementcreateda culturecharac-
terizedby such shared beliefs as: (1)allemployees should be
respected andtreatedwith dignity,(2)the companyshould aimto
accomplishevery task in a superiorway, and (3)the customer should
be giventhe best service possible. IBMmanagementhas saidon many
occasions that this cultureis largelyresponsiblefor its enormous
success in the past 30 years.
But, as Arensberg (I1978:56), a veteran of the Human Relations
school reflected:
Cultureas shared meaningsand organizationas orderedbehavior,
together leadingto cooperativeresult,are not merelyplannedand
362/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Native-View Paradigms

commanded; they are always partially spontaneous, responsive, both


self-realized and socially sanctioned and inspired.
Again, corporate culture researchers, like their predecessors,
are hampered by promanagement assumptions that make it
difficult for them to describe cultures as they are, before
assessing how they should be.
Native-View vs. External-View Paradigms
Cultural paradigms also vary along a dimension from emphasiz-
ing native or internal views to emphasizing external views
(Gregory, Remington, and Werner, 1978; Werner, 1981). "Na-
tive" is a technical term anthropologists use to refer to their
research subjects. The rationale for studying native views
comes from the belief that meanings are linked to behavior, and
those who take this perspective define culture as a system of
meanings. Their research goal is to discover and describe native
viewpoints, or "cultures."
Sanday (1 979: 532-536) compared two native-view or semiotic
paradigms in detail: Geertz's thick description and
Goodenough's version of ethnoscience. She pointed out that
"on close analysis the difference between Geertz and
Goodenough is not in aim but in method, focus and mode of
reporting"; thick description is interpretive, ethnoscience is
explanatory, but both aim at understanding culture from partici-
pants' points of view or conceptual worlds (Sanday, 1979: 534).
Two or more native viewpoints are likely to be, at least partially,
incommensurable to each other, and so comparing cultures is
more problematic forthosewho followa native-view paradigm,
than for those who use an external-view paradigm. An illustra-
tion of this problem is the difficulty of language translation. In
learning a particular language, one also learns to classify experi-
ence in a unique way; different languages classify and, hence,
describe experience in distinct ways. The cliched example is
the many words the Eskimo use to classify snow. Skiers and
snow removal experts no doubt have complicated classifica-
tions of their own. Even among these snow experts, snow
classifications would be different, making direct comparison
impossible. In anthropology, Sapir and Whorf explained this as
"linguistic relativity" and formally articulated this connection
between language and cultural concepts (see Whorf, 1941).
This idea is commonly referred to as the Sapir-Whorf Hypothe-
sis. Cultural translations subsume language translations, and
native-view researchers attempt to produce cultural transla-
tions, adequate for generating and interpreting social behavior
that natives would deem appropriate.
In external-view research, the researcher's or some others'
(e.g., managers') culture provides the conceptual framework
through which behavior is studied, with no expectation that
research questions oranalytic categories will conform to native
meanings. For example, actuarial studies of large-scale statisti-
cal regularities treat humans, theirartifacts, and/or behaviors as
"objects of study" and describe phenomena about which the
natives are presumably unaware (Gregory, Remington, and
Werner, 1978). Even those who view culture as meaning may
study human behavior from an external-view perspective.
Studies of attitudes, myths, and rituals can, for instance, be
conducted from an externalviewpointif the comparative
categories and frameworksdo not reflect nativepointsof view.
363/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Whiting's (1963) "Six Cultures" study, which Sanday (1979:
536) categorized as the "behaviorist style," is an example of
research from an external perspective.
Industrialresearch spans the range but has emphasized exter-
nal viewpoints or has taken a middle position more often than it
has elaborated native views. Studies in work settings that have
focused on native views include Kemnitzer's (1977) research on
railroadoperators' multiple time senses, Smith's (1977) ethno-
graphic comparison of lake to saltwater fishermen's native
views, and Spradley and Mann's (1975) research on cocktail
waitresses' cultural knowledge. Some sociological studies have
incorporated native views along with external views (see
Kanter, 1977; Van Maanen, 1977, 1980).
RESEARCHAGENDA: CONSIDERING MULTIPLENATIVE
VIEWS
Before proceeding, the meaning of culture used in the rest of
this paper requires elaboration. "Culture," as used here, is
defined as learned ways of coping with experience.5A capacity
for culture is innate in human beings, and possibly some other
species, but particularculture, by definition, is not biologically
inheritable. To a great extent, coping with experience involves
sense making and acting in terms of meanings. A culture is
conceptualized as a system of meanings that accompany the
myriad of behaviors and practices recognized as a distinct way
of life. Although it is referred to as a system, culture is only
explicit as people express it; much of the system must be
considered as being made up of implicit meanings that motivate
behavior.
Culture is an individualized capacity, but researchers have
largely studied the social acquisition and use of culture for
collective action, assuming that meanings are shared. The form
and degree of sharing are problems in need of further study
(seeWernerand Topper, 1979), and in this paper, shared culture
5 means apparently shared meanings.6 Becker (1982) provided a
This definition of culture is my own, but is description of how culture seems to be shared and to operate in
based on several others'. Spradley and a situation, which can be summarized as follows. People
McCurdy's (1975: 5) definition of culture as
"the acquired knowledge people use to interact as if they shared culture. Through trialand error,
interpret experience and generate social sometimes through conversation and negotiation, they confirm
behavior," and Werner and Topper's (1979: whether or not their meanings are similar enough to get
34) "system of knowledge that explains
the social and physical universe and pro- through social interactions appropriately. Sometimes their ex-
vides plans and decisions for coping" were pectations are confirmed; at other times they break down,
particularlyinfluential. Like those, mine is a
cultural knowledge model, as contrasted
leading to further negotiation or even conflict. From a base of
with other models based on artifacts or shared culture, people can negotiate new apparently shared
core values (see Kroeberand Kluckhohn, meanings, and do, as a matter of course. Thus, culture accrues
1952).
to social groups of every type, from families to work groups to
6
corporations.
Here, the shared qualities of culture are
underemphasized intentionally. In most Further, particularculture is taken for granted by natives, for
cases, it is the social cohesiveness of
human beings and their use of culture to whom it seems natural; actions based in other cultures often
coordinate social action that is emphasized appear "exotic" in contrast. The pervasive tendency to automat-
(see Becker, 1982). Additionally, the on- ically evaluate all phenomena from one's own cultural stand-
tological status of culture is uncertain. I
implicitly posit somie kind of reviseable point is termed ethnocentrism. Ethnocentrism can bean impor-
data-base-like model that can be partlyrep- tant cohesive force within culturalgroups, but it often leads to
resented as lexical/semantic fields (see
Werner and Topper, 1979). Notions of ac-
conflict in cross-cultural interaction. Anthropologists tradition-
quiringand sharing culture raise the same ally recognize their own and their subjects' ethnocentrism, and
questions as does "having" culture. Some couple this awareness with a posture of "culturalrelativity"that
culture, as I've defined it, can be stored
externally in books, computer data bases, denies the superiority of any particularculture (Herskovits,
and other libraries. 1972).
364/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Native-View Paradigms

As mentioned earlier, culturalapproaches of several types led to


ethnographic industrial studies, beginning in the late 1920s
with the Human Relations studies and currently in vogue as
corporate culture research. Perhaps due in part to the small-
society analogy from traditional ethnography, and in part to
management development orientations, these researchers
largely assumed the whole organization to be a robust unit for
culturalanalysis and so described modal accounts of "organiza-
tional cultures" that deemphasized intraculturalvariation. If
significant intraculturalvariation was observed, the culture was
said to "lack integration" (Benedict, 1934: 196). The current
practice in some corporate culture studies is to contrast
"strong" homogeneous cultures with "weak" heterogeneous
cultures that deviate from management philosophy and are said
to lack integration (Deal and Kennedy, 1982: 5; Peters and
Waterman, 1982: 75).
However, the small-homogeneous-society metaphor is often
inappropriateto those organizations that are large, internally
differentiated, rapidlychanging, and only command part-time
commitment from members. Such organizations more nearly
resemble the complex society of which they are a part. In large,
complex societies (and probably in apparently simple ones as
well), the robustness of any group as a culture is questionable.
People participate in many groups and acquire culture in all
experiences (e.g., as family members, residents, citizens, em-
ployees). As affiliations change overtime, culture from past
experiences is carried into new ones. Thus, people as culture
bearers linkgroups simultaneously through joint membership
and sequentially over their careers (see Gregory, 1980).
Societies, and many organizations, can more correctly be
viewed in terms of multiple, cross-cutting cultural contexts
changing through time, rather than as stable, bounded,
homogeneous cultures.
This multiculturalquality of society and organizations has been
described by many (see March and Simon, 1958; Likert, 1961;
Goodenough, 1978; Strauss, 1978; Van Maanen and Barley,
1982). Arensberg (1978: 57) noted how "large-scale hierarchi-
cal organizations ... put persons of diverse [professional and
subcultural] traditions into harness together." He noted the
"clashes of values and traditions" that often result and ex-
pressed his concern that a "way must be found to identify, and
to reconcile if not satisfy, such diverse and opposed traditions"
in these "modern, multiplex bureaucracies and 'plural
societies'."
Therefore, it might be more accurate to separate cultural
integration from organizational integration, and to describe
organizations, rather than cultures, as either "strong" or
"weak" in terms of integration. Organizations that lack integra-
tion may be comprised of members acting from numerous
internally consistent but externally conflicting cultures.
Ethnocentrism exacerbates the intensity of conflicts, since
each coalition takes its position forgranted or may even assume
meahings and priorities are shared. The cultures may conflict
only in a few situations, or in many.
Culturalresearch focused on the organizational dynamics re-
sulting from such "cross-cultural" contact might elaborate
organizationalprocesses that are obscuredwhen the presuma-
365/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
bly shared organizational culture is the focus. Native-view
paradigms from anthropology would be especially appropriate
for exploring the multiculturalism of organizations.
Ideally, in native-view-oriented anthropology, a cultural relativist
posture leads researchers to explore controversies in detail as
various groups view them, rather than to evaluate positions
against a standard. Instead of starting with an assumption of
shared culture (either shared among all researched group
members or shared with the researcher), meanings are ex-
plored systematically, preserving and explaining the bases of
controversies (Werner, 1981). No homogeneous units or spe-
cific characteristics of culture are defined a priori,but rather
those groups and processes recognized by native participants
are discovered and studied "in theirterms" during the research.
DISCOVERINGNATIVEVIEWS IN SILICONVALLEY
Description of the Study
The following illustrations are from a recent ethnographic study
among technical professionals in Silicon Valley computer com-
7 panies. The overall research focus was on technical profes-
Comprehensive methods sources for sionals' careers. The goal was to describe and explain how they
ethnoscience ethnography include Werner
and Topper (1979) and Spradley (1 979), and
managed their careers within and between companies, and,
those interested in learning these proce- reciprocally, how companies staffed design projects. Many
dures should consult one or both sources. approaches might have been taken to learn about technical
Here, it is only possible to provide explana-
tions necessary for following the exam-
work and culture. Since Silicon Valley is so multifaceted and
ples. A discussion of the various controver- rapidlychanging, it seemed most appropriate to discover how
sies about ethnoscience is likewise outside participants made sense of it themselves, so ethnoscience
the scope of this paper. The main criticism
is that "native" views are not subject to ethnography was used to systematically explore native
comparison as are observations from viewpoints.7
trained, "external" researchers (see
WernerandTopper, 1979; Werner, 1981). The primarysources of information were necessarily technical
8 professionals themselves; they are the experts in their own
The primary units of analysis in this study culture. About 75 technical professionals were interviewed.
were individuals, occupations, projects, and Interviewees were selected in two ways. Technical profession-
companies. It was necessary to study
several companies in depth to consider als from a wide variety of companies and backgrounds were
multiple points of view and to gain a com- interviewed as individualparticipants. Also, several companies
prehensive understanding of how particu-
lar companies are intersections for many
were selected as in-depth cases, or corporate participants, from
individuals' careers. On the other hand, which representative samples of employees were interviewed.
individual participants provided a picture of Participants were guaranteed anonymity and confidentiality in
variation across the valley. Some ethnosci-
ence ethnographies are based on a single exchange for their cooperation.8
informant's viewpoint or similarly, present
an idealized "single-mind model" drawn
During the research, a major emphasis was on learning native
from several informants. In this study, var- concepts for social categories and on identifying those with
ied points of view were presented in the contrasting orientations or cultures. Interviewees (both indi-
analysis and wider sampling was chosen
over working in-depth with a single vidual and corporate participants) were recruited according to
informant. native conceptions of representativeness, which emerged dur-
9 ing successive interviews. Criteriathey suggested included the
It should be underscored that each com- type of company one worked for (e.g., large, stable develop-
pany was taken individually in selecting the
representative sample. For example, in
ment companies, start ups, research labs, software houses),
some, marketing professionals were not amount of experience one had, seniority, sex, ethnicity, place
considered "technical" and were not inter- and type of education, and, importantly, technical occupation or
viewed. The native-view paradigm leads a
researcher to begin inductively, so random specialty. The participants identified a number of generic tech-
or stratified sampling was not an appropri- nical occupations, which were sampled, including software
ate course for selecting participants. Be-
fore interviewing, it was not possible to
engineering, hardware engineering, computer science, market-
select units for sampling or bases for ing, technical writing or documentation, and technical
stratifying. During the study, I took advan- managements
tage of native conceptions of repre-
sentativeness when asking, "Who s hould I Technical professionals' interviews lasted about one and a half
talk to here to get a representative picture
of careers?" Many native theories of sam-
hours and covered the following major topics using an open-
pling were considered (see Gregory, 1984). ended format: (1) their current jobs, projects, and employers,
366/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Native-View Paradigms

including reporting relationships, areas of responsibility, major


activities, and daily activities; (2) their backgrounds and careers,
including transitions, plans for the future, and nonwork ac-
tivities; and (3) their cultural knowledge about careers, staffing,
and worklife, for example, their understandings of others'
careers and jobs, companies' reputations, and so forth.
A few technical professionals were interviewed repeatedly as
key consultants. A smaller number of human resources profes-
sionals, general managers, technical recruiters ("headhunt-
ers"), coworkers, venture capitalists, and other Silicon Valley
participants were interviewed about their conceptions of tech-
nical careers and recruiting. All interviews were tape-recorded
and transcribed verbatim. Many of the interviews took place at
work and provided opportunities to make observations, which
were recorded as fieldnotes. Also, various kinds of documents
(e.g., personnel agreements, formal organization charts, com-
pany philosophies and histories) were collected as native texts.
Although the methods were ethnographic in a broad sense, the
focus was always on learning native viewpoints. Observations
were used as a source for interview questions, and discrepan-
cies between observations and interview statements were
explored in detail.
Native views were primarilydefined as cognitive systems, and
language was considered the least ambiguous representation
of native conceptual systems (Werner and Topper, 1979).
Interviews, documents, and passages of observed language
were gathered and analyzed with attention to discovering and
preserving intrinsic meanings. Data were content-analyzed
(coded and sorted) according to topics identified by participants
(such as their descriptions of companies, kinds of work, rea-
sons to change jobs, etc.). Systematic semantic structural
analyses (as developed by ethnoscientists) were used to elicit,
analyze, and represent native views as lexical/semantic fields,
data-base-like models of culture (as knowledge) consisting of
concepts linked by relations (Werner and Topper, 1979: 37).
The aim of using ethnoscience methods was to learn native
viewpoints accurately and efficiently, introducing as little bias
from my own culture as possible. Ethnoscience ethnography
gave me a set of systematic procedures for exploring lexical/
semantic fields interactively during the interviews and through
analyses of written or transcribed native texts. During inter-
views, I began with general questions to elicit a large sample of
language. This corpus was subsequently used as a base from
which to phrase follow-up questions, delving further into the
viewpoint. A very general question might be, "Tell me about
your job here, in as much detail as possible." In follow-up
questions, the structural principles of lexical/semantic fields
were capitalized on to elicit other concepts, to learn their
meanings, and to understand their relations (Spradley, 1979).
One basic relation, perhaps universal, is taxonomy or inclusion,
where one concept is a kind of another. Using this principle, I
might ask the follow-up structural question, "What kinds of
companies are there?", after discovering "company" as a
concept during a more open-ended question. The one or more
"kinds of companies" elicited, together with the cover term,
"companies,"forma contrastset. A contrastset is partof a
lexical/semanticfield and gets its name fromthe principlethat
367/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
categories (in this case concepts for various "kinds of com-
panies") stand in limited contrast to one another (Spradley,
1979: 158); they have some similarity, hence their inclusion,
and some dissimilarity, hence the need for separate terms. Any
contrast set can be represented with a tree-diagram referred to
as a taxonomy (see Figure 1). One-way arrows between terms
in the figure indicate the asymmetric quality of the taxonomic
relation. For example, a "start up" is a kindof "small company"
and, transitively, a kind of "computer product development
company." Parentheses around terms indicate a conceptual, as
opposed to an object, taxonomy (Werner and Topper, 1979). In
the study, such conceptual taxonomies, and other conceptual
structures, from numerous interviewees were compared, and
variations were explored.

(Computer Product Development Company)

(Large Company) (Small Company)

(Computer Product (Other Small


Division or Company, e.g.,
Subsidiary of a (Large (Start up) software house)
Large Corporation) Computer
Manufacturer)

Figure 1. Taxonomy of "kinds of computer product development com-


panies," a segment of one interviewee's lexicallsemantic field.

Coming to understand a native view entails learning to distin-


guish between the conceptual categories natives use. The
bases for conceptual distinctions are referred to as attributes of
the concepts. Various methods were used to identify attributes,
the simplest being asking for folk definitions and observing
definitions that occurred in the normal course of natives' writing
or conversation. For example, one informant defined a "start
up":
You go to a venture capitalist with an idea and a business plan that
involves one to ten million dollars up front and 18 months worth of
development to produce a product that will grow your company into a
fifty-million-dollar-a-year company in five years. That has to be a hig h
pressure situation. They [venture capitalists] don't want you taking off
three days a week. They want you to work eighty hours a week!
Numerous attributes (e.g., "working eighty hours a week")
were specified in this text. Further exploration of the contrast
set, "kinds of companies," led to more definitions, compari-
sons, and contrasts as meanings were fully elaborated. Such
explorations also led me to explore other contrast sets within
the overall lexical/semantic field.
368/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Native-View Paradigms

Conceptual structures (taxonomies, attribute analyses, and


others) from the same individualat various times, and from
different individuals, were compared, and controversies were
studied in detail. Culturalsharing was thus operationalized in
terms of similar lexical/semantic field representations. Since
the process is qualitative and complex, however, for practical
reasons, only selected portions of lexical/semantic fields were
subjected to detailed comparison.
The following section presents a brief example from my study,
as an illustrationof the kinds of phenomena ethnoscience
ethnography identifies. Itis not a conclusive analysis of multiple
cultures in Silicon Valley computer companies and only draws
on a small subset of the technical interviews. Although I have
not detailed the representativeness and distribution of these
native viewpoints (see Gregory, 1984), they were certainly not
uncommon examples. In these illustrations, I emphasize the
research process of discovery, and, in some cases, I have
exaggerated the relatively "exotic" appearance to underscore
the importance of checking native views within a seemingly
familiarculture. One aspect of the wider study, the presence of
multiple cultures and conflicts, is exemplified (see also Gregory,
1984).
Silicon Valley Cultures
"Silicon Valley" refers to a geographic area about 25 miles long
and 10 miles wide that extends northwest from San Jose, up
the San Francisco peninsula. It is roughly equivalent to Santa
ClaraCounty, California. Its nickname comes from the dense
concentration of high-technology companies, many of which
produce or incorporate integrated circuits on slivers of silicon
called "silicon chips." Hundreds of semiconductor, consumer
electronics, computer, and similar companies are spread along
and between U.S. Highway 101 on the northeast side of the
valley and Interstate 280 on the southwest side. The human
population of Silicon Valley is also densely concentrated and
highly differentiated. Among local inhabitants are venture
capitalists, "headhunters," entrepreneurs, technical profes-
sionals, managers, clerical workers, and production workers
involved in high-technology companies and institutes.
Change seems to be one of the few constants. Multiple
occupations, technologies, and organizations are developing
simultaneously, in contactwith each other.10AIIare enmeshed
in cooperative and/or competitive relationships within the valley
and within larger national, industry, and international markets.
As balances shift, even the near future is hard to predict. For
example, the semiconductor industry recently slowed its rate of
10
growth, leading to some lay-offs and reduced production
Klingand Gerson (1977, 1978) provided the
schedules; meanwhile, personal computer and software com-
best analyses of structural evolution of the panies are booming, competing fiercely for experienced com-
computing world. They used a "social puter scientists and software engineers, and they seem to
worlds" view from symbolic interactionist
sociology (see Strauss, 1978) to focus on conjure up new buildings overnight. Participants often use a
the multiple shifting perspectives around gold-rush metaphor to describe the valley, and it seems to fit.
the production and distribution of computer
technology. Symbolic interactionism is Pacific Microcomputer Corporation (PMC), the name I give to a
similar to ethnoscience in emphasizing fictional composite of several companies the interviewees
perspectives, but contrasts with it by plac-
ing less emphasis on learning native views work at, most closely resembles a medium-sized computer
of perspectives (see Blumer, 1969). products manufacturer several years beyond the start-up stage,
Ethnomethology is closer to ethnoscience
in elaborating perspectives (see Garfinkle,
but not yet a large, "sluggish" company. It is housed in its own
1967). recentlyconstructedbuildingjust off Interstate280. Visitorsto
369/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PMC are greeted by a young, male receptionist, are asked to
sign-in, and are given adhesive visitor's badges before being
escorted to areas where engineering work takes place. PMC
employees clip on or carry plastic badges that identify them by
picture, name, and employee number. Badges must be inserted
into black security boxes to gain entrance to several areas. PMC
zealously guards information about incipient new products by
requiring new employees (and participant observers) to sign
nondisclosure agreements and by controlling the access of
even PMC employees to information about products, if they are
not directly involved with their development. A few weeks prior
to several long-anticipated "product releases," one employee
described PMC as "pregnant with products," which vividly
captured the sense of expectation, combined with mystery,
that surrounds the development and release of new computer
products.
Most engineers, marketers, and writers work in a single huge
room where five-foot-high dividers define a maze of individual
work spaces or cubicles. A naive visitor entering this room
might observe casually dressed, collegiate-looking men and
women, sitting at desks, working at computer terminals, talking
on telephones, or simply chatting in small groups, and might
hear muffled conversations coming, presumably, from people
obscured behind partitions. To a completely naive visitor, for
example, one from a radicallydifferent culture, nearly every-
thing about PMC would appear exotic, just as the many settings
in which anthropologists customarily conduct fieldwork seem
exotic to those from the anthropologists' cultures.
A less naive observer, but one unfamiliarwith computer design-
ing work might not distinguish marketing from engineering
people, nor anticipate the existence of two competing projects,
nor understand why one PMC engineer is elated at the chance
to "alpha test the PMCB," while another is disgruntled at being
assigned to "maintain PMCA Pascal." A PMC native would be
likely to understand these and many other culturallybased
elements.
At PMC, "occupational communities" cross-cut the organiza-
tion (Van Maanen and Barley, 1982). These provide employees
with identities and significant reference groups within and
outside the company. Although not the only basis of stratifica-
tion, they are frequently a basis for folk classifications of people
at PMC and in Silicon Valley more generally. In one interview, a
software engineer suggested the following classification of
"technical professionals":
The maindistinctionsarewhat you'dcallsoftware types and hardware
types. Andthen, well Iwas going to say you'renot interestedin
marketing,but in fact a lot of people in marketingit seems to me are
convertedengineers. [People in] manufacturing,production,sales,
communications,personnel,those are alltotallyseparatecareers.
Using this and subsequent statements from the same en-
gineer, two taxonomies were constructed that correspond to
his stated understanding of these native concepts and their
relationships (see Figure 2). Inthe figure, Aand B can beviewed
as two planes in the same software engineer's multidimen-
sional lexical/semantic field. They show the complexity and
context-sensitivity of native viewpoints that may be portrayed
accurately through showing several versions. In the same

370/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Native-View Paradigms

A
(Technical Professional)

(Hardware oftware
Type) / eype)

(Engineer) (Computer
(Technician) (Software Scientist)
Engineer)

B
(Technical Professional)

(Person in Marketing)

(Engineer)

(Converted
Engineer)

Figure 2. Two alternate taxonomies of "kinds of technical professionals,"


from a single native viewpoint.

interview, the software engineer defined a limited contrast


between "computer scientists" and "software engineers" in
the following way:
The difference is really,computerscientists are moreinterestedinthe
academicside of it,andthey willwantto writea paperaboutwhat they
did,anddon'treallycaredeep down intheir heartswhether customers
have any use for anythingthat they're doing.Software engineers may
have exactlythe same training.Theycouldtakethe same courses, but
they see themselves as builders,and want to get something built.
This partialnative text identifies attributes he considers relevant
for distinguishing between the two conceptual categories. A
different software engineer, or a computer scientist might
distinguish between the terms on other bases, but would
probably consider his comparison appropriate. A newcomer to
the setting would be likely to misuse the terms through failing
to recognize the subtle distinctions between them. One un-
familiarwith computer-designing work would lack any aware-
ness of the terms or the social categories to which they refer.
PMC employees sometimes alluded to different occupation-
based or suborganization-basedcultures."Hardware,"
371/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
"Software," "Engineering," "Marketing," "PAC Division," or
"Scientist" orientations were often mentioned. These would
presumably be represented by sizeable conceptual dis-
similarities. The software engineer, quoted above, elaborated
"engineer" and "scientist" perspectives in the following
way:11
Indevelopment[work]there's too much pressureto get something
done, and sometimes the onlyway to solve a problemis to thinkupa
new idea. Butan engineer's pointof view is you don'twant to do that.
Youwant to use well-knowntechniques that are sure to work.Onlyif
there's a problemthat no one knows how to solve do you have to
inventa new thing.There's a riskto invention.
Scientists are the other way. They don'twant to do what's been done
before because they won't be able to publisha paper.So, even if
there's a perfectlygood way to do it,you'dbetterthinkof anotherway.
In a similar kind of example, a "marketeer" who describes
himself further as "a converted engineer now in product
marketing" contrasted marketing, engineering, and sales
orientations at PMC:
Sales people want to plug holes. They see that they lost this sale
because they didn'thave this [productor service]. Marketingis trying
to lookahead so they will have time to respond[to competition].
Engineeringwillbe interestedin what's coming interms of where is
the system nowand where shouldit be a fewyears fromnowand how
do we get there. Itseems that for sales, onlythis month is important;
for marketing,onlythis yearis important;forengineering,nothingthis
year is important.
Just as more widely-held cultures produce ethnocentricity,
each occupational culture justifies its own centrality and em-
phasizes local priorities, as the "marketeer" elaborated:
Eachfunctionalareathinksof themselvesas beingthe company,orat
least at the center of it. Engineeringsees themselves as beingcentral,
with marketingtakingtheirstuff and handingit to sales. Marketing
sees themselves as coordinatingallthe other pieces. They'rethe ones
that set the tone for the companyin general.Sales people thinkif you
don'tget this problemsolved bythe end of the week, then we're going
to lose every sale.
Employees holding such contrasting viewpoints may try to
11
interact and coordinate their actions to produce computer
Particular"native" orientations have been products but may find that their conflicting cultures complicate
noted by other researchers. This example
provides evidence to support distinct "sci- these attempts or even make direct coordination unproductive.
entist" and "engineer" orientations (cf. These particularinterviewees were unusual in their ability to
Ritti, 1971; Bailyn, 1980). In the next exam-
ple, the contrast between marketing and
articulate conflicts. Inactual practice, such conflicts remain tacit
engineering viewpoints supports similar and can lead to misunderstanding and wasted time and effort,
findings of Lawrence and Lorsch (1967). as the software-engineer indicated in discussing "research"
However, the principle here is broader and
leads researchers to consider culturalorien- and "development" orientations:
tations based on a wide variety of structural
and historical experiences. For example,
They frustrateeach other totally.There's no communicationbetween
technical writers may bring creative- the two if they tryto worktogether. Instead,you set up a research
writing, artistic viewpoints to their work if grouphere and a developmentgroupthere andtrynot to workclosely
they were-chosen from those back- together because you can'tget alongverywell.
grounds. Computer companies sometimes
intentionally import (technically) "naive" Infact, at [PMC]we don't haveany research-a slightexaggeration-
users as employees. Ethnic backgrounds we-have had, fromtime to time, groupsthat didn'tlast very longthat
provide another dimension. Overall com-
pany cultures, such as being an "ex-HP were doingresearch.None lasted morethantwo months.Theywould
manager" or being an "originalemployee" do something totallyuseless, totallya flop, andthey wouldjust sort of
often produce orientations that can lead to dissolve it. Or,they didsomething so successful itwouldturnout to be
cultural conflicts. The inductive, native- productdevelopment,andthe same guys that didthe researchwanted
view approach described here aims to un-
cover any bases for culturalorientations, to do the productdevelopment,so the groupvacatedthat sphere
not only structurally predicted ones based again.We never haveongoingresearch.Withinthe companythere are
on supposedly rational interests. r
only two people we have now who arerea esearching-type people.

372/ASQ,September1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Native-View Paradigms

One of them has been turned into an engineer. Several of them came
from research, but they all became engineers."
Usually, individuals' careers proceed within single occupational
communities.12 But, both these interviewees had been "con-
verted," one from an engineer into a product marketer, and the
other from a computer scientist into a software engineer. The
term "converted" is revealing of the difficulty with which such
changes are made. The difficulty lies in changing cultural
orientations, as the software engineer explained:
We hire people, including me, from research who come here and are
not used to this notion of when you develop a product you always want
to avoid thinking up new ideas. It's hard not to think up new ideas.
That's the way I've been trained. And, it takes some learning and some
bad experiences to stop doing that. The rewards are getting a product
out. The rewards aren't in having a paper to write.
Once the change has been successfully negotiated, however,
the "convert" sometimes retains a sensitivity to the abandoned
perspective and is able to make contrasts between at least
these "familiar"cultures explicit. Converts become more adept
at cross-cultural "mediation" using their insights. In fact, the
marketeer reported experiences in which he acted as a trans-
lator or culture broker between marketing and engineering. This
is a naturallyoccurring instance of the conflict-resolution value
to be gained from understanding another's viewpoint. The
native-view paradigm provides a systematic way for re-
searchers, and possibly participants as well, to gain the advan-
tages of such "conversions" without literallychanging jobs.
At PMC, the overlapping of cultures creates numerous bases
for group segmentation or cohesion. Some culture is widely
shared, at the organizational or even regional/industrylevel, and
creates a basis for understanding. For example, innovation in
general and new products in particularare highly valued, as was
pointed out earlier. The opportunity to work on "the newest,
whizziest product" is sought by many Silicon Valley employees.
Those who share this basic understanding would know why
the engineer mentioned earlierwas elated at a chance to "alpha
test" (to give an early in-house trialto a product) a soon-to-be-
released new product. Conversely, "maintaining" last year's
product is assigned to junior, low-status employees, if it is
assigned to anyone at all. At PMC, the life-cycle of computer
products was so compressed that few stayed on the market
long enough to require maintenance.
But the value placed on innovation is not enough to guarantee
shared priorities. Values are often expressed differently when
they intersect with cross-cutting occupational cultures. For
example, the positive value placed on innovation is expressed in
different ways by engineers and scientists. Engineers concen-
trate on developing new products, specifically on "getting it out
the door" (releasing it to the market), whereas scientists
emphasize developing new technology. As was pointed out,
these goals are sometimes diametrically opposed.

CONCLUSIONS AND APPLICATIONS


12 Much past organizational culture research emphasized shared
For a similar discussion of the mechanism culture and suffered from managementcentric biases. A multi-
of occupational commitment, see Becker
(1960) or Van Maanen and Barley (1982:
culturalimage of organizationsleads one to considerboth
21). cohesive and divisivefunctionsof cultureandis recommended
373/ASQ,September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
to researchers. These multiple cultures are not simply subcul-
tures, dominated by an organizational culture, but usually
cross-cut several organizations, as do occupational cultures. If,
in doing basic research, researchers adopt a cultural relativist
posture, more accurate models of varied native views might be
created.
From the perspective of the organization, researchers can
relate the terms "culture" and "organization" in one of two
senses: (1) culture coterminous with the focal organization's
boundaries (i.e., organizational culture) and/or (2) noncotermi-
nous culture, including cultural context (e.g., national, geo-
graphic, or industrial cultures); culture that cross-cuts the
organization (e.g., occupational or ethnic cultures); or subcul-
tures of the focal organization (e.g., departmental or project
cultures). The researcher's choice would depend on the
theoretical or empirical grounds for a particularstudy.
Finally, ratherthan being basic research, many Human Rela-
tions and corporate culture studies had culture evaluation and
control as goals. These researchers sought means to encour-
age lower rankingemployees to conform to management's
values instead of exploring indigenous value configurations in a
balanced way. They found, however, that among other attri-
butes, excellent companies were characterized by "people
values." Management in these companies emphasized internal
communication and respect for employees at all levels (Peters
and Waterman, 1982: 235). Ethnoscience ethnography can
complement "people values" by providing management with a
tool for exploring organizational participants' perspectives, in
their own terms. This practice would facilitate upward com-
munication and culture-conflict resolution. Further, holding a
posture of cultural relativityduring the exploration process
would express "respect" for employees in the fullest sense of
the term.13 Later, if management attempts to alter employees'
behaviorthrough "redirecting" culturalvalues, successful ef-
model for downward and horizontal com- forts will require understanding existing cultures in detail,
munication, as well as upward communica-
tion, and could provide a process for teach-
especially since the "same" basic values may lead to conflicting
ing as well as learning cultural viewpoints. priorities. Native-view paradigms, and particularlyethnoscience
Culturaltranslation is always conducted in- ethnography, can provide both basic and applied researchers
teractively. If the goal is to reduce cultural
conflict, the process should be mutual and with solid bases for understanding the complexity of organiza-
symmetric, in so far as this is possible. tional cultures.

REFERENCES
Agar, Michael Baker, Edwin Benedict, Ruth
1979 "Themes revisited: Some prob- 1980 "Managing organizational cul- 1934 Patterns of Culture. NewYork:
lems in cognitive anthropol- ture." Management Review, Macmillan.
ogy." Discourse Processes, 2: July: 8-13. Blumer, H.
11-32. Becker, Howard S. 1969 Symbolic Interactionism. En-
Arensberg, Conrad M. 1960 "Notes on the concept of glewood Cliffs, NJ:
1978 "Theoretical contributions of commitment." American Jour- Prentice-Hall.
industrial and development nal of Sociology, 57: 470-477. Deal, Terrence E., and Allan A.
studies." In Elizabeth M. Eddy 1982 "Cultures: A sociological Kennedy
and William L. Partridge (eds.), view." Yale Review, 71 (Sum- 1982 Corporate Cultures: The Rites
Applied.Anthropology in mer): 513-527. and Rituals of Corporate Life.
America: 49-78. New York:
Becker, Howard S., Blanche Geer, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Columbia University Press.
Everett Hughes, and Anselm Feyerabend, Paul
Bailyn, Lotte Strauss 1975 Against Method: Outline of an
1980 Livingwith Technology: Issues 1977 Boys in White: Student Culture Anarchistic Theory of Knowl-
at Mid-Career. Cambridge, MA: in Medical School, New edge. London: Verso.
MITPress. Brunswick, NJ: Transaction
Books.

374/ASQ, September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Native-View Paradigms

Gardner, Burleigh Holzberg, Carol, and Maureen Moore, Wilbert E.


1945 Human Relations in Industry. Giovannini 1962 The Conduct of the Corpora-
Chicago: Irwin. 1981 "Anthropology and industry: tion. New York: Vintage.
Reappraisal and new direc- Morgan, Gareth
Garfinkle, H.
tions." Annual Review of An- 1980 'Paradigms, metaphors, and
1967 Studies in Ethnomethodology.
thropology, 10: 317-360. puzzle solving in organization
Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice-Hall. Kanter, Rosabeth M. theory." Administrative Sci-
1977 Men and Women of the Corpo- ence Quarterly, 25: 605-622.
Goffman, Erving
ration. New York: Basic Books. Nash, June C.
1961 Asylums. Garden City, NY:
Doubleday. Kemnitzer, L. S. 1979 We Eat the Mines and the
1977 "Anotherviewoftimeandthe Mines Eat Us: Dependency and
Goodenough, Ward
railroader."Anthropological Exploitation in BolivianTin
1978 'Multiculturalism as the normal
Quarterly, 50(1): 25-29. Mines. New York: Columbia
human experience." In
University Press.
Elizabeth M. Eddy and William Kling, Rob, and Elihu Gerson
L. Partridge (eds.), Applied An- 1977 "The social dynamics of tech- Opler, Morris
thropology in America: 79-87. nical innovation in the comput- 1945 'Themes as dynamic forces in
New York: Columbia University ing world." Symbolic Interac- culture." American Journal of
Press. tion, 1(1): 132-146. Sociology, 53: 198-206.
1978 "Patterns of segmentation and
Gregory, Kathleen L. Ouchi, William
intersection in the computing
1976 'Stop throwing books out my 1981 TheoryZ. Reading, MA:
world." Symbolic Interaction, Addison-Wesley.
window: An ethnography of jr.
1(2): 24-43.
high school teaching." Honors Pascale, Richard T., and Anthony G.
thesis, Department of An- Kluckhohn, Clyde, and Dorothea Athos
thropology, MacalesterCol- Leighton 1981 The Art of Japanese Manage-
lege, St. Paul, MN. 1974 The Navajo, rev. ed. Cam- ment. New York: Simon and
1980 'Work organizations and bridge, MA: HarvardUniversity Sch uster.
careers: A theoretical Press.
framework for anthropology Peters, Thomas J., and Robert H.
Kneller, George F. Waterma n, J r.
with three cross-cultural appli- 1965 Educational Anthropology.
cations." Unpublished second 1982 In Search of Excellence. New
New York: Wiley. York: Harperand Row.
year paper, Department of An-
thropology, Northwestern Uni- Kroeber, Alfred L., and Clyde Pfeffer, Jeffrey
versity, Evanston, IL. Kluckhohn 1981 'Management as symbolic ac-
1984 'An ethnographic study of 1952 Culture: A CriticalReview of tion: The creation and mainte-
technical professionals' Concepts. New York:Vintage. nance of organizational
careers in Silicon Valley com- Lawrence, Paul R., and Jay W. paradigms." In LarryL. Cum-
puter companies." Ph.D. Lorsch mings and BarryM. Staw (eds.),
dissertation (in progress), 1967 Organization and Environment: Research in Organizational Be-
Department of Anthropology, Managing Differentiation and havior, 3: 1-52. Greenwich,
Northwestern University, Integration. Boston: Division of CT: JAI Press.
Evanston, IL. Research, HarvardUniversity Richardson, Friedrich, and C.
Gregory, Kathleen L., and Sigfried Graduate School of Business Walker
Mueller Administration. 1948 Human Relations in an Expand-
1980 "Leif Ericson Elementary Likert, Rensis ing Company. New Haven, CT:
School." In Why Do Some 1961 New Patterns of Management. Yale University Labor Man-
Urban Schools Succeed?: The New York: McGraw-Hill. agement Center.
Phi Delta KappaStudy of Ex-
ceptional Urban Elementary Malinowski, Bronslaw Ritti, Richard
Schools: 60-74. Bloomington, 1922 Argonauts of the Western 1971 The Engineer in the Industrial
IN: Phi Delta Kappa. Pacific. London: Routledge and Corporation. New York: Co-
Kegan Paul. lumbia University Press.
Gregory, Kathleen L., Judith Rem-
ington, and Oswald Werner March, James G., and Herbert A. Rodin, Miriam, Karen Michaelson,
1978 "Ways of knowing: The case Simon and Gerald Britan
for radicalethnoscience." 1958 Organizations. New York: 1978 "Systems theory in anthropol-
Paper presented at the Annual Wiley. ogy." CurrentAnthropology,
Meeting of the American An- Martin, Joanne 19(4): 747-762.
thropological Association, Los 1982 "Stories and scripts in organiza- Roethlisberger, F. J., and W. J.
Angeles, CA. tional settings." In Albert H. Dickson
Harris, Marvin Hastorf and Alice M. Isen (eds.), 1939 Management and the Worker:
1968 The Rise of Anthropological Cognitive Social Psychology: An Account of a Research Pro-
Theory. NewYork: Crowell. 255-305. New York: gram Conducted by a Western
Elsevier-North Holland. Electric Company, Hawthorne
Herskovits, Melville J. Works, Chicago. Cambridge,
1972 CulturalRelativism: Perspec- Mayo, Elton
1933 The Human Problems of an In- MA: HarvardUniversity Press.
tives in CulturalPluralism. New
York: Vintage. dustrial Civilization.New York:
Macmillan.

375/ASQ, September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Sanday, Peggy R. Spradley, James P., and David W. Werner, Oswald, Martin Topper and
1979 "The ethnographic McCu rdy Associates
paradigm(s)." Administrative 1975 Anthropology: The Cultural 1979 Handbook of Ethnoscience:
Science Quarterly, 24: 527- Perspective. New York: Wiley. Ethnographies and Ency-
538. Strauss, Anselm clopaedias. Evanston, IL: De-
Schwartz, Howard, and Stanley 1978 'A social world perspective." partment of Anthropology,
Davis Studies in Symbolic Interaction, Northwestern University.
1981 'Matching corporate culture to 1: 119-128. Whiting, Beatrice (ed.)
business strategy." Organiza- 1963 Six Cultures. New York: Wiley.
Van Maanen, John (ed.)
tional Dynamics, Summer: 1977 Organizational Careers: Some Whorf, Benjamin
30-48.
New Perspectives. New York: 1941 "The relation of habitual
Schwartzman, Helen B., and Merton Wiley. thought and behavior to lan-
S. Krause (eds.) 1980 "Career games." In C. B. Derr guage." In Leslie Spier (ed.),
1981 Organizational Culture and Ser- (ed.), Work, Family and the Language, Culture and Person-
vice Delivery. Chicago, IL:Insti- Career: 1 1 1-143. New York: ality: 75-93. Menasha, WI:
tute forJuvenile Research, Il- Praeger. Sapir Memorial Publication
linois Department of Mental Van Maanen, John, and Stephen R. Fund.
Health.
Barley Whyte, William H., Jr.
Smith, M. E. 1982 Occupational Communities: 1956 The Organization Man. New
1977 "Don't call myboatashipl" Culture and Control in Organiza- York: Doubleday.
Anthropplogical Quarterly, tions. TR-10 (Technical Report). Wolcott, Harry F.
50(1): 9-17. Cambridge, MA: Sloan School 1973 The Man in the Principal's Of-
of Management, MIT.
Spradley, James P. fice: An Ethnography. New
1979 The Ethnographic Interview. Warner, W. L., and J. Low York: Holt, Rinehart and
New York: Holt, Rinehart and 1947 The Social System of the Mod- Winston.
Winston. ern Factory: The Strike, a Social
Analysis. New Haven, CT: Yale
Spradley, James P., and Brenda
University Press.
Mann
1975 The Cocktail Waitress: Wom- Werner, Oswald
an's Work in a Man's World. 1981 'An anthropological approach
New York: Wiley. to fieldwork: Ethnoscience." In
Lenore Borzak (ed.), Field
Study: 177-206. Beverly Hills,
CA: Sage.

376/ASQ, September 1983

This content downloaded from 151.230.89.127 on Fri, 20 Jun 2014 18:07:08 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like