You are on page 1of 6

Transitional Object Attachments in Early Childhood

and Personality Characteristics in Later Life


Keith N. Cohen and James A. Clark
University of North Dakota
The present study empirically assessed aspects of transitional object attachments
formed in early childhood and their relationship to personality characteristics in
later life. Subjects were college students enrolled in introductory psychology courses.
They were assigned to either a transitional object group or a no transitional object
group based on self- and parental reports obtained from a detailed questionnaire
focusing on early object attachments. Stringent criteria of inanimate object at-
tachments were used in selecting subjects. Subjects selected for the study were
administered the Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire. Significant personality .
differences were found between the groups. These preliminary objective findings
were discussed in terms of the role of concrete object attachments in early childhood
and areas for further research.

The importance of special objects in the world. The transitional objects and phenomena
lives of small children, though long recognized serve the child with a "resting place" in the
by parents, has recently gained growing at- struggle between inner and outer reality. In
tention among the psychological community. healthly and normal development the func-
Winnicott (1953) was the first to express the tions attributed to the object or phenomena
importance of these blankets, teddy bears, and are not lost but instead extended to play and
other cuddly toys and objects in the normal creative experiences in later life.
development of the child. He coined the terms Numerous psychoanalytic investigators have
transitional objects and transitional phenom- refined and applied Winnicott's concepts to a
ena to designate the special possessions, ac- diverse spectrum of developmental and clinical
tivities, and experiences of children during the issues. These include, for example, such mat-
early months and years of life. Whereas the ters as the earliest symbolic expressions of the
transitional object is easily recognized in the infant (Deri, 1978), creative activity (Dinnage,
form of the ubiquitous blanket or teddy bear, 1978; Rose, 1978), the separation-individu-
transitional phenomena refer to the often un- ation process (Fraiberg, 1969; Metcalf & Spitz,
noticed or unobservable repetitious behaviors, 1978), and clinical implications of vicissitudes
mannerisms, sounds, fantasies, and thoughts in the transitional process (Coppolillo, 1967;
that also serve soothing and psychologically Downey, 1978; Giovacchini, 1978; Kahne,
supportive functions for the very young child. 1967; Kaminer, 1978; Modell, 1963; Natter-
According to Winnicott, the transitional son, 1976; Parrish, 1978; Solomon, 1962; Vol-
object and transitional phenomenon help the kan, 1973; Volkan & Kavanaugh, 1978).
child manage the stresses and anxieties of sep- Although most investigators view the tran-
aration by symbolically recreating union with sitional object as healthy and important to the
the mother. These objects and phenomena are child's development, some have suggested that
theorized to serve a critical role in the devel- such possessions, particularly when too highly
opmental process of self-object differentiation. treasured by the child, reveal a pathological
During this period the young child struggles development in the mother-child relationship
to establish his or her individuality and to (Dickes, 1963, 1978; Sperling, 1963).
adapt to the reality demands of the external Investigators apart from the psychoanalytic
tradition have also focused on and discussed
aspects of early object attachments. Experi-
Requests for reprints should be sent to Keith N. Cohen, mental researchers (Gershaw & Schwarz, 1971;
who is now at the Cognitive Therapy and Diagnostic Ser- Passman & Weisberg, 1975; Weisberg & Rus-
vice, 169 Grove Street, Wellesley, Massachusetts 02181. sell, 1971), focusing on the functions such ob-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1984, Vol. 46, No. I, 106-111
Copyright 1984 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
106
TRANSITIONAL OBJECTS AND PERSONALITY 107

jects serve for the child, have referred to these (Passman & Weisberg, 1975), behavioral difc-
possessions as "security blankets." Bowlby ferences were found between children who
(1969), offering an ethological perspective, were attached to objects and those who were
simply denned these objects as "substitutes" not. Those children attached to a blanket were
for the mother and attributed no other special able to play alone in a novel environment with
meaning to them. little stress comparable to children unattached
Empirical studies of the phenomena de- to a blanket but with their mother present.
scribed by Winnicott have lagged considerably The unattached children on the other hand,
behind the vast theoretical formulations of- when not in the mother's presence, exhibited
fered. Many of the more systematized inves- greater distress even when exposed to blankets,
tigations have focused attention on object at- familiar toys, or no objects at all. From the
tachment as linked to social factors. Gaddini study, it appeared that the blanket and the
and Gaddini (1970) and Hong and Townes mother possess similar supportive functions.
(1976) in cross-cultural studies found object Although child-rearing practices and greater
attachment to be more prevalent (54%-61%) physical contact with the mother appear re-
among children of upper socioeconomic lated to low frequency of object attachment
groups. Factors such as breast feeding, weaning (Gaddini & Gaddini, 1970; Hong & Townes,
at a later age, and sleeping in close proximity 1976), children experiencing maternal depri-
to the mother were demonstrated to be related vation as seen among institutionalized infants
to the lower frequency (5%-18%) of transi- have shown a conspicuous absence of object
tional object attachments among rural, lower attachments (Provence & Ritvo, 1961). A ret-
socioeconomic groups in Italy and Korea. rospective study by Horton, Louy, and Cop-
Other investigators, often using differing polillo (1974) also found a very low frequency
criteria in denning transitional object attach- of prior transitional object attachments among
ments and varying sampling and data col- a group of personality disordered adults.
lecting methods, have also studied these phe- Hong (1978), in a comprehensive review
nomena of early childhood. From these sur- and integration of the various bodies of lit-
veys, the frequency of object attachment has erature on transitional object attachments,
been found by most accounts to exceed 50% viewed the various positions and findings,
among children (Busch, Nagera, McKnight, though at times conflicting, as adding to the
& Pezzarossi, 1973; Ekecrantz & Rudhe, 1972; richness of the phenomena being studied. He
Passman & Halonen, 1979; Rudhe & Eke- suggested that attachment objects need to be
crantz, 1974; Sherman, Hertzig, Austrian, & considered in terms of their origin and func-
Shapiro, 1981). The persistence of these object tion as well as their intrapsychic components.
attachments are frequently found to extend All these aspects, he stated, .provide "a mul-
well beyond early childhood (Busch et al., tidimensional view of the significance of tran-
19,73; Passman & Halonen, 1979), with one sitional phenomena" (p. 76).
study (Sherman et al., 1981) reporting almost The purpose of the present study has been
half of the children who initially formed such to empirically assess aspects of early object
attachments to still be displaying them at 9 attachments and personality characteristics.
years of age. The emphasis has been to examine the nature
Relations have generally not been demon- of the transitional object as representing early
strated between object attachment and sex of developmental processes that expand and in-
the child, birth order, parents' marital status, fluence subsequent aspects of psychological
behavioral characteristics, or ego development and interpersonal functioning. Whereas tran-
(Ekecrantz & Rudhe, 1972; Sherman et al., sitional objects can have a wide range of special
1981; Stevenson, 1954). Boniface and Graham or personal meanings, their use and signifi-
(1979), however, discovered relations between cance might well be suspected of revealing
the presence of an attachment and thumb general characteristics in adaptive and per-
sucking, sucking objects other than the at- sonality style. This investigation specifically
tachment object, and parents' ratings of their examines the relationship between objectively,
child's independence. measured personality characteristics in young
In a well-designed experimental study adulthood and the presence or absence of
108 KEITH N. COHEN AND JAMES A. CLARK

transitional object attachments in early child- oped through extensive factor analytic research that re-
hood. sulted in 16 primary scales or factors of personality.
The 16 PF was used in the present study to assess dif-
ferential aspects of the two previously defined groups: the
Method transitional object group and the no transitional object
group. The data were analyzed using multiple regression
Subjects analysis, which in this case is equivalent to a discriminant
function analysis. The purpose was to find the personality
The subjects included in this study were 37 undergrad-
traits on which the two groups differed. The multiple
uate students selected from an initial pool of 278 subjects
regression technique assessed the contribution of each per-
enrolled in introductory psychology courses. This larger
sonality factor in predicting variation in the dependent
pool of subjects all completed a 10-item questionnaire
variable, transitional object attachment.
designed to explore the nature of special inanimate object
attachments they may or may not have had as small chil-
dren. The questionnaire focuses on descriptive qualities Results
of the object and general adaptive functions it served.
From the self-reports of the 278 initial subjects, 107 Relationship of Personality Variables to
persons were selected. Of these subjects, 55 had what were Transitional Object Attachment
considered to be primary transitional object attachments,
and 52 gave no indication of early object attachments. In the analysis of the primary 16 factors,
The remaining subjects were eliminated due to responses significant effects were found on the Reserved
that were either too vague or not suggestive of primary
transitional object attachments. From the subjects selected,
(sizothymia) versus Outgoing (affectothymia)
64 were willing to participate in the study. factor, F(l, 34) = 15.97, p < .0003, and the
Parental reports were obtained from these subjects as Relaxed (low ergic tension) versus Tense (high
a further part of the screening process to confirm the ergic tension) factor, f\l, 34) = 23.87, p <
retrospective accounts of the subjects. These reports helped .0001. These predictor variables considered in
establish the onset of the object usage within the first year
as well as corroborated its very special importance to the a regression equation (see Table 1) produced
child beyond that of other objects. If any significant dis- a squared multiple correlation indicating that
crepancies between self- and parental reports existed, the 53.7% of the variance was accounted for in
subject was eliminated. the dependent variable (transitional object
The criteria (based on those proposed by Busch et al.,
1973) used for determining those with primary transitional
group vs. no transitional object group). Pear-
objects in early childhood from those without such at- son correlation coefficients for the significant
tachments were the following: factors also produced significant relationships
1. Transitional object group: This group consisted of both on the Reserved factor (r - .46, p < .004)
those persons indicating a strong, singular attachment to and the Tense factor (r = .57, p < .0003). The
a special object (e.g., blanket, pillow, diaper) that occurred
rather early in life, during the first year, which persisted effects of the remaining 14 variables did not
for a reasonable length of time (approximately 1 year, significantly add to the predictive power when
often persisting for considerably longer). These involved considered in the regression equation, F(14,
elaborate reports of loved objects that were highly treasured 20) = .26, ns.
and affectionately and nostalgically remembered. They of-
ten had special qualities (e.g., softness, smell) that served
Inspection of the means for the transitional
important supportive and soothing functions. object group and the no transitional object
2. No transitional object group: This group consisted group on the Reserved versus Outgoing di-
of those persons indicating no clear attachments of the mension reveal that it was the no transitional
sort described above. object group that deviated from the norm on
In the final analysis, 37 subjects remained. Nineteen (8
female, 11 male) had transitional objects based on the
specified criteria and consistency between self- and parental Table 1
reports, whereas 18 (9 female, 9 male) show no evidence Multiple Regression Predicting Object Attachment
of transitional object attachments. from Primary Personality Factors
Source df SS MS P<
Measures
Reserved vs.
Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire: Form A. The Outgoing 1 2.0085 2.0085 15.97 .0003
Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire (16 PF; Cattell, Relaxed vs.
Eber, & Tatsouka, 1970) is a personality inventory con- Tense 1 3.0017 3.0017 23.87 .0001
sisting of 187 items. The 16 PF was selected for the study Error
as it represents an objective personality instrument that (residual) 34 4.2760 .1257
measures a broad array of personality variables applicable
to nonclinical populations. The questionnaire was devel- Note. Total SS = 9.2432. R2 = .5373, p < .0001.
TRANSITIONAL OBJECTS AND PERSONALITY 109

that factor (Z = 4.08, p < .0001). This de- jective. Test items composing the Reserve di-
viation was in the Reserved direction. On the mension depict the individual as one who
Relaxed versus Tense dimension, it was the would prefer to live alone in the deep woods
transitional object group that deviated from rather than in a sociable suburb, not involve
the norm (Z = 3.43, p < .001). This occurred themselves in charitable work or political
in the Tense direction (see Table 2). causes, do carpentry or laboratory work rather
than be involved in more interpersonally de-
Discussion manding activities (Cattell et al., 1970).
The present set of findings appear to support
The major findings of the present study re- the hypothesis that transitional object attach-
vealed significant relationships between spe- ments as early and highly significant markers
cific personality characteristics and the pres- of intrapsychic developmental functioning re-
ence or absence of transitional object attach- veal certain generalizable characteristics about
ments in early childhood. personality development and style. These seem
The personality factor related to the pres- to suggest the importance and impact that the
ence of strong and persistent attachments to use of specific object attachments or their ab-
an inanimate object was the Tense factor of sence may have on the negotiation and reso-
the 16 PR This factor indicates excitability* lution of both development issues present in
restlessness, and impatience. The scale is most early childhood as well as on the approach
indicative of anxiety experiences and reactions. and negotiation of subsequent interactions
Examination of the test items helps elucidate with one's environment. There are many lim-
some of the qualitative features of the per- itations that caution against too greatly gen-
sonality characteristics defined by this factor eralizing from these findings, as it is not known
of the 16 PR The Tension factor is composed what full array of factors lead to early object
of items that reflect admissions of feeling tense, attachments or, for that matter, what causal
difficulty calming down, low tolerance for crit- relationships exist between object attachments
icism, ease in being angered, and inability to and personality factors. However, the person-
hold back remarks (Cattell, Eber, & Tatsouka, ality characteristics that have been found be-
1970). tween groups are noteworthy and therefore
The personality characteristic related to the raise some interesting issues for consideration.
absence of an early object attachment was the First, one must consider the seeming dis-
Reserved factor of the 16 PR This scale reflects parity between the transitional object group's
traits of aloofness and detachment as well as endorsement of tension items on the 16 PF
tendencies to be rather rigid, precise, and ob- and the theorized notions of the transitional
object as a healthy, universal adaptation (Win-
Table 2 nicott, 1953) that serves soothing structure-
Means and Standard Deviations for the enhancing functions (Tolpin, 1971) for the in-
Transitional Object Group (TO) and No dividual both in early life and also throughout
Transitional Object Group (NTO) on Two later life experiences. The meaning of these
Personality Factors findings might best be viewed at this stage in
light of several possible interpretations of the
Population data. For example, one can view these findings
Factor parameters* TO NTO
as supporting true tension and anxiety char-
Reserved vs. acteristics or alternatively as tendencies toward
Outgoing high arousal states and greater capacity for
M 10.93 10.32 7.61 intensity of experience or possibly as a greater
SD 3.45 2.75 2.59
Relaxed vs. willingness to self-disclose negative aspects of
Tense oneself. All remain as hypotheses in need of
M 13.43 17.21 12.50 further investigation.
SD 4.81 3.78 3.24 A second consideration, again one requiring
•Normative data for Sixteen Personality Factor Ques- several possible explanations, is why individ-
tionnaire on College Students (Males and Females: Form ual's reporting no involvement or attachment
A; Cattell et al., 1970). N = 4,272. with transitional objects revealed reserved
110 KEITH N. COHEN AND JAMES A. CLARK

traits on the 16 PF. The absence of an ob- Dickes, R. (1978). Parents, transitional objects, and child-
servable and concrete object attachment, an hood fetishes. In S. Grolnick, L. Barkin, & W. Muen-.
sterbergen (Eds.), Between reality and fantasy: Transi-
attachment which is theorized to mark the tional objects and phenomena (pp. 305-319). New "Vbrk:
intrapsychic process of self and object differ- Jason Aronson.
entiation and the successful negotiation of the Dinnage, R. (1978). A bit of light. In S. Grolnick, L.
demands of the external world, may represent , Barkin, & W. Muensterberger (Eds.), Between reality
and fantasy: Transitional objects and phenomena (pp.
internal incapacities in the ability to meet such 363-378). New York: Jason Aronson.
requirements. Yet it may also be possible to Downey, W. (1978). Transitional phenomena in the analysis
interpret these characteristics as reflecting of early adolescent males. Psychoanalytic Study of the
more positive features such as self-sufficiency Child, 33, 19-46.
and resourcefulness. Again, the data is not Ekecrantz, L., & Rudhe, L. (1972). Transitional phenom-
ena: Frequency, forms and functions of specially loved
conclusive in supporting one definitive expla- objects. Ada Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 48, 261-273.
nation. Fraiberg, S. (1969). Libidinal object constancy and mental
The findings from this study, although pow- representation. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 24,
erful statistically, should be viewed as only 9-47.
preliminary sources of objective data that have Gaddini, R., & Gaddini, E. (1970). Transitional objects
and the process of individuation: A study in three dif-
not as yet been cross-validated. Moreover, the ferent social groups. Journal of the American Academy
data raise more questions and issues for con- of Child Psychiatry, 9, 347-365.
sideration than they provide conclusions and Gershaw, N., & Schwarz, J. (1971). The effects of a familiar
answers to the developmental and personality toy and mother's presence on exploratory and attach-
ment behavior in young children. Child Development,
matters being investigated. 42, 1662-1666.
Further objective research along the lines Giovacchini, P. (1978). The impact of delusion and the
pursued in this study with accompanying be- delusion of impact: Ego defect and the transitional phe-
havioral data collected through interviews and nomenon. In S. Grolnick, L. Barkin, & W. Muenster-
berger (Eds.), Between reality and fantasy: Transitional
other interpersonal modalities (which can fo- objects and phenomena (pp. 323-343). New \brk: Jason
cus on the present capacity to engage in tran- Aronson.
sitional experiences) can help expand the data Hong, K. (1978). The transitional phenomena: A theoretical
base needed in more fully gaining an under- integration. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 33, 47-
standing of this fascinating "third area" of ex- 79.
Hong, K., & Townes, B. (1976). Infants' attachment to
perience that Winnicott (1953) has so richly inanimate objects. Journal of the Academy of Child
described. Psychiatry, 15, 49-61.
Horton, P., Louy, X, & Coppolillo, H. (1974). Personality
disorder and transitional relatedness. Archives of General
References Psychiatry, 30, 618-622.
Kahne, M. (1967). On the persistence of transitional phe-
Boniface, D., & Graham, P. (1979). The three year old nomena into adult life. International Journal of Psycho-
and his attachments to a special soft object. Journal of Analysis, 48, 247-258.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 217, 217-224. Kaminer, H. (1978). Transitional object components in
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. self and object relations. In S. Grolnick, L. Barkin, &
London: Hogarth Press. W. Muensterberger (Eds.), Between reality and fantasy:
Busch, R, Nagera, H., McKnight, J., & Pezzarossi, G. Transitional objects and phenomena (pp. 233-243). New
(1973). Primary transitional objects. Journal of the \fork: Jason Aronson.
American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 12, 193-214.
Cattell, R., Eber, H., & Tatsouka, M. (1970). Handbook Metcalf, D., & Spitz, R. (1978). The transitional object:
for the Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire. Critical developmental period and organizer of the psy-
che. In S. Grolnick, L. Barkin, & W. Muensterberger
Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability
(Eds.), Between reality and fantasy: Transitional objects
Testing.
Coppolillo, H. P. (1967). Maturational aspects of the tran- and phenomena (pp. 97-108). New \brk: Jason Aronson.
sitional phenomena. International Journal of Psycho- Modell, A. (1963). Primitive object relationship and the
analysis, 48, 237-246. predisposition to schizophrenia. International Journal
Deri, S. (1978). Transitional phenomena: Vicissitudes of of Psycho-Analysis, 44, 282-292.
symbolization and creativity. In S. Grolnick, L. Barkin, Natterson, J. M. (1976). The self as a transitional object:
& W. Muensterberger (Eds.), Between reality and fantasy: Its relationship to narcissism and homosexuality. In-
Transitional objects and phenomena (pp. 43-60). New ternational Journal of Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 5,
\fork: Jason Aronsoh. 131-144.
Dickes, R. (1963). Fetishistic behavior: A contribution to Parrish, D. (1978). Transitional objects and phenomena
its complex development and significance. Journal of in a case of twinship. In S. Grolnick, L. Barkin, & W.
the American Psychoanalytic Association, 11, 303-330. Muensterberger (Eds.), Between reality and fantasy:
TRANSITIONAL OBJECTS AND PERSONALITY 111

Transitional objects and phenomena (pp. 271-287), New Sperling, M. (1963). Fetishism in children. Psychoanalytic
York; Jason Aronson. Quarterly, 32, 374-392.
Passman, R. H., & Halonen, J. S. (1979), A developmental Stevenson, O. (1954). The first treasured possession. Psy-
survey of young children's attachment to inanimate choanalytic Study of the Child, 9, 374-392.
objects. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 134, 165-178. Tolpin, M. (1971), On the beginnings of a cohesive self:
Passman, R., & Weisberg, P. (1975). Mothers and blankets An application of the concept of transmuting inter-
as agents for promoting play and exploration by young nalization to the study of the transitional object and
children in a novel environment. Developmental Psy- signal anxiety. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 26,
chology, 11, 170-177. 316-354.
Provence, S., & Ritvo, S. (1961). Effect of deprivation on Volkan, V. (1973). Transitional fantasies in the analysis of
institutionalized infants: Disturbances in development a narcissistic personality. Journal of the American Psy-
in relationship to inanimate objects. Psychoanalytic choanalytic Association, 21, 351-376.
Study of the Child, 16, 189-205. Volkan, V., & Kavanaugh, J, (1978). The cat people. In
Rose, G. (1978). The creativity of everyday life. In S. S. Grolnick, L. Barkin, & W. Muensterberger, (Eds.),
Grolnick, L. Barkin, & W. Muensterberger (Eds.), Be- Between reality and fantasy: Transitional objects and
tween reality and fantasy: Transitional objects and phe- phenomena (pp. 289-303). New York: Jason Aronson.
nomena (pp. 345-362). New 'Vbrk: Jason Aronson. Weisberg, P., & Russell, J. (1971). Proximity and inter-
Rudhe, L., & Ekecrantz, L. (1974). Transitional phenom- actional behavior of young Children to their "security"
ena. Ada Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 50, 381-400. blankets. Child Development, 42, 1575-1579.
Sherman, M,, Hertzig, M., Austrian, R., & Shapiro, T. Winnicott, D. W. (1953). Transitional object and transi-
(1981). Treasured objects in school-aged children. Pe- tional phenomena. International Journal of Psycho-
diatrics, 68, 379-386. Analysis, 34, 89-97.
Solomon, J. C, (1962). Fixed idea as an internalized tran-
sitional object. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 16,
632-644. Received December 1, 1982

You might also like