You are on page 1of 27

Copyright © 1998 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

4.09
Assessment of Memory, Learning,
and Special Aptitudes
ROBYN S. HESS
University of Nebraska at Kearney, NE, USA
and
RIK CARL D'AMATO
University of Northern Colorado, Greeley, CO, USA

4.09.1 INTRODUCTION 239


4.09.1.1 Assessment Approaches 241
4.09.1.2 Evaluation of Domain Areas 243
4.09.1.3 Intervention Approaches 244
4.09.2 ASSESSMENT OF MEMORY 245
4.09.2.1 Attention 246
4.09.2.2 Short-term and Long-term Memory 247
4.09.2.3 Memory: Implications for Intervention 249
4.09.3 ASSESSMENT OF LEARNING 249
4.09.3.1 Models of Learning 249
4.09.3.2 Learning Processes: Input and Integration 252
4.09.3.3 Academic Achievement: Output 253
4.09.3.4 Learning: Implications for Intervention 254
4.09.4 ASSESSMENT OF SPECIAL APTITUDES 254
4.09.4.1 Sensory Perception 255
4.09.4.1.1 Sensory perception: implications for intervention 256
4.09.4.2 Motor: Fine and Gross 256
4.09.4.3 Sensory-motor Integration 257
4.09.4.3.1 Motor: implications for intervention 257
4.09.4.4 Communication/Language 257
4.09.4.4.1 Communication/language: implications for intervention 259
4.09.5 FUTURE DIRECTIONS 259
4.09.6 SUMMARY 260
4.09.7 REFERENCES 261

4.09.1 INTRODUCTION traditional categories of cognition, such as


attention, memory, language, and learning are
The inner workings of the human mind and terms frequently used and considered all
the way in which people process information important to our daily adaptive functioning
has intrigued researchers for centuries. The and ability to learn new information. Yet all

239
240 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

have defied simple explanation and manipula- Psychology has made many new inroads into
tion (e.g., Gaddes & Edgell, 1994; Lezak, 1995). understanding the learning process and the
That is, as professionals we are able to identify subsequent development of corrective or adap-
when a child or adult is having difficulty tive programs for children and adults with
processing information, but the exact interrela- learning disorders and traumatic brain injuries.
tions between an individual's different capa- As assessment specialists, psychologists must
cities in areas such as attention, learning style, quickly and accurately wade through the
and sensory integration still eludes educational cumulative data available about the individual
and clinical specialists. More puzzling still is in order to select the most viable of alternative
finding effective rehabilitation strategies to hypotheses to explain the findings and offer
address deficits in memory, learning, and other appropriate interventions (D'Amato & Dean,
psychological processes. These areas are fre- 1989a; D'Amato, Rothlisberg, & Leu, in press;
quently addressed in the growing volumes of Gutkin & Reynolds, 1990). Although adminis-
neuropsychological research, but are relevant to tering a test may be a routine activity,
the practices of many traditionally trained conducting a thorough, valid assessment is an
psychologists as well (Hamsher, 1984). Not extremely complex process. The clinician is
surprisingly, all of the information needed for required to make decisions regarding which
an adequate understanding and interpretation skills to evaluate and the best instrument to use
of cognitive processes cannot be provided in a with a particular client, and to generate accurate
single chapter or obtained in an individual interpretations of the results in order to create
university course. Thus, the purpose of this the most effective intervention plan. Adding to
chapter is to provide a brief description of our the immensity of this task are the wide range of
higher cognitive processes and introduce a client variables that can impact the assessment
variety of strategies and measures for evaluating process, including motivation, environment,
these functions. culture, age, developmental level, language,
Problems in attention, memory, and learning training, educational quality, personal experi-
are not isolated to the very young or the very ence, and attitude to name just a few (Golden,
old. Adult learning problems often become Sawicki, & Franzen, 1984; Hynd & Semrud-
apparent in employment settings and after Clikeman, 1990).
injuries resulting from strokes, accidents, or The clinician must recognize that the context
diseases. Epidemiological studies suggest that in of the client may influence or even define the
the decade following the late 1990s there will be outcome of the assessment (Dana, 1993;
a dramatic increase in the number of individuals Figueroa & Garcia, 1994). For example, many
suffering from organically related disorders of the psychological, educational, and person-
resulting from the abuse of alcohol and other ality instruments available to practitioners have
types of toxic substances (Touyz, Byrne, & been criticized as culturally biased, as tradi-
Gilandas, 1994). The recent documented in- tionally not including individuals from diverse
crease in the number of head injuries caused by ethnic backgrounds in the norming sample, and
motor vehicle accidents has become an intrinsic as measuring acquired knowledge rather than
fact in today's society. High speed transporta- an individual's responsiveness to instruction or
tion and the growing prevalence of violent street the learning process (Cole & Siegel, 1990; Dana,
crimes have further increased the incidence of 1993; Figueroa & Garcia, 1994; Sattler, 1992).
head injuries (Touyz et al., 1994). So too, the Because of these problems, several researchers
growing popularity of certain contact sports believe that standardized assessment may have
(e.g., hockey, boxing), noncontact sports (e.g., questionable validity for those clients who
rock climbing, mountaineering, bicycling), and represent diverse cultural groups (Cole & Siegel,
recreational activities (e.g., skateboarding, roll- 1990; Dana, 1993; Figueroa & Garcia, 1994;
er blading) has contributed significantly to the Martinez, 1985). From the beginning, one
number of individuals suffering from traumatic robust assumption of standardized testing was
brain injury (Drew & Templer, 1992; Templer & that all individuals who take the tests would
Drew, 1992). Internal processes such as eating have had equal or comparable exposure to the
disorders, depression, diseases, epilepsy, and contents of the assessment materials prior to the
tumors can result in impaired executive func- assessment (e.g., Colvin, 1921; Dearborn, 1921;
tioning as well (Black & Strub, 1994). These Woodrow, 1921). In direct contrast to this
realities of today's society make it necessary for supposition, current statistics indicate that the
clinicians to be able to accurately evaluate a US immigrant population is not only growing
client's strengths and weaknesses in everyday rapidly but is also quickly expanding in diversity
functioning and find the key elements to (Figueroa & Garcia, 1994). These authors
fostering effective behavioral change through conclude that tests, although given high status
rehabilitation or educational improvement. in US society, are actually quite fragile because
Introduction 241

of the founding assumption regarding homo- instruments used to measure memory, learning,
geneity and general shortcomings in technical and special aptitudes as well as a brief
properties. Nevertheless, standardized tests can description of those instruments that have
be useful in evaluating current functioning strong empirical support for use with children
especially when multiple sources of information and adult populations. Any one of these areas,
and multidimensional functions are evaluated on its own, represents a very narrow picture of
to measure individual processes (Sattler, 1992). the overall functioning of an individual. How-
The responsible clinician must recognize both ever, when used in conjunction with a more
the assets and the limitations when using thorough assessment, these areas can provide
standardized measures with ethnic minority the missing pieces to the puzzle.
clients. Client difficulties may be attributed not only
The strategy the practitioner uses to accom- to intra-individual characteristics, but also to
plish an effective assessment must, of necessity, the domain of functioning (e.g., social, voca-
be based upon well-grounded, empirically tional, educational); the context or environment
validated theories of cognition and behavior. in which the client is expected to function (e.g.,
Only through the use of a theoretical framework job site, classroom, independent living); the
are specific predictions regarding performance requirements of particular assignments, jobs, or
under a given set of ecological circumstances responsibilities, task; and the strategy used to
made possible (Dean, 1985a, 1986; Rothlisberg, teach or remediate a difficulty, intervention
1992). Unfortunately, no single, diagnostic (Geil & D'Amato, 1996). Although the focus of
paradigm or theory has proven sufficient to this chapter is memory, learning, and special
explain fully the vagaries of behavior (D'Amato aptitudes, it may be helpful for the clinician to
& Rothlisberg, 1992). Psychoanalytically, be- view these processes as key components within
haviorally, and biologically based approaches, the conceptual framework presented in Figure 1.
as well as other theoretical positions, have been These areas represent extremely important
continually challenged not only to describe aspects of psychological functioning and can
behavior, but also to provide effective inter- help to complete the diagnostic picture of an
ventions for the populations whom they serve individual by providing critical information to
(D'Amato & Dean, 1989b; Gutkin & Reynolds, assist the clinician in accurate diagnosis and
1990). Prepackaged programs dealing with intervention planning.
psycholinguistic or visual-motor training, and
sensory integration training have attempted 4.09.1.1 Assessment Approaches
this, but typically failed to meet the demands of
this challenge. Gradually, the field has acknowl- Before addressing the particular areas of
edged that the effective use of assessment concern, a brief discussion of the assessment
procedures, including educational and psycho- process is warranted. Both quantitative and
logical tests, is reliant upon a theoretical qualitative assessment procedures help to pro-
foundation, which allows the incorporation of vide a breadth of information concerning
information from multiple data sources and individual functioning. A quantitative or
environments in such a manner as to increase product-oriented approach uses standard per-
the amount of effective and appropriate inter- formance data to assess individuals within and
ventions generated. across all the functional domains to be measured
A framework that is particularly useful is one by comparing the findings to a normative group
reliant on an ecological approach. From this (D'Amato, Rothlisberg, & Rhodes, 1997; Dean,
perspective, it is critical to evaluate several 1985a, 1985b; Lezak, 1995). This process detects
different aspects of clients' lives in order to whether the client's skills show a discrepancy
develop a better understanding of their func- when they are compared to other individuals
tioning within a variety of contexts. The performing within a normal range. Patterns of
purpose of this chapter is to examine the performance can also be carefully analyzed to
particular areas of attention and memory, determine the individual client's strengths and
learning processes (input and output), and the weaknesses. Data is usually considered in
special aptitudes of sensory perception, sensory- several ways: level of performance or current
motor integration, and language/communica- functioning (compared to normative stan-
tion to facilitate making informed assessment dards); pattern of performance (uniqueness of
decisions. One must possess knowledge of the strengths and weaknesses); right±left differences
cognitive processes that these tests purport to (comparing tests that evaluate both hemispheres
measure to make judgments about the useful- including both sides of the body); pathognomic
ness of any given instrument with the client's signs (indications of abnormal signs or brain
presenting issue. Furthermore, the clinician is damage); qualitative analysis (behavioral ob-
provided with an introduction to the types of servations of problem solving); intervention
242 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

• Domain (social, vocational, educational)


• Context (job site, classroom, independent living)
• Task (assignment, job, responsibility)
• Intervention (remediation, counseling, training)
Figure 1 Conceptual framework of client functioning.

planning (recommendations for appropriate cal battery (e.g., Halstead±Reitan Neuropsycho-


rehabilitation) (Hynd & Semrud-Clikeman, logical Test Battery; Reitan & Wolfson, 1993)
1990; Jarvis & Barth, 1994; Reitan & Wolfson, involve the same set of instruments for each
1985, 1993; Sattler, 1992; Selz, 1981). individual tested (Hynd & Semrud-Clikeman,
Most proponents of a quantitative approach 1990; Hynd & Willis, 1988). A standard battery
recommend a standard or fixed battery of tests. format insures that a broad array of appropriate
A fixed battery, such as a typical psychoeduca- tools is used to cover all significant domains and
tional battery (i.e., Wechsler Adult Intelligence therefore provide documented results that may
Scale-Revised [WAIS-R] or Wechsler Intelli- be interpreted with ease. In fact, a standard
gence Scale for Children-3rd Edition [WISC- battery approach may be the best choice when
III], Minnesota Multiphasic Personality and if potential litigation is an issue because this
Inventory-2nd Edition, Bender Visual-Motor method offers a normative data base to which
Gestalt Test, Woodcock±Johnson Psychoedu- client profiles can be compared and contrasted
cational Battery-Revised) or neuropsychologi- (Guilmette & Giuliano, 1991; Reitan & Wolfson,
Introduction 243

1995). Despite the apparent strengths of a Although the flexibility and individualization
standardized approach, it has been argued that apparent in this method is appealing, it requires
when developing treatment options, the use of a great deal of clinical experience to make
qualitative methods that explore the process of accurate interpretations of behaviors, and
learning or behavior may be better suited than a problems with reliability and validity are
purely quantitative or product-oriented ap- ever-present (Lezak, 1995).
proach (D'Amato, Rothlisberg, & Leu, in A third approach, and one which is likely
press). For example, if verbal instruction with used by the majority of clinicians, is the use of
verbal response is a strength for the client, a integrated data. Indeed, any time examiners
preference for left hemisphere processing might note an examinee's reaction to a task, the
be entertained and interventions utilizing a response time involved, or any problem-solving
verbal component could be tailored with that strategies employed (e.g., rehearsal, verbal
hypothesis in mind. Likewise, if a client cuing), they are inferring the underlying
demonstrated a strength in simultaneous pro- processes being used (D'Amato, Rothlisberg,
cessing of information, a global concept or & Leu, in press; Taylor, 1988; Taylor &
visual chart could be introduced before pre- Fletcher, 1990). Because all individuals show
senting the individual skills necessary to a distinctive pattern of learning and behavioral
accomplish the particular task. characteristics, it is improbable that any given
A second strategy, the qualitative approach test, or even battery of tests, in isolation, can
or process-oriented approach, uses informal capture the range of skills exhibited by that
procedures such as direct observation of individual. Furthermore, test scores that are
particular skills to analyze the specific patterns interpreted without consideration to the context
and processes in order to understand better the of the examination may be objective but are
intricacies of the client's psychological pro- meaningless in their individual application
cesses (Lezak, 1995). Practitioners utilize a (Lezak, 1995). Likewise, clinical observations
client's individual pattern of responses or unsupported by standardized and quantifiable
results to guide the assessment process. That testing may provide a rich picture of the client's
is, if a client was observed to have difficulty with current functioning but lack the comparability
memory tasks, that particular area would be necessary for many diagnostic and planning
investigated in more detail through the use of decisions. Thus it is expected that most
additional measures of memory. A decision- practitioners are integrated in their assessment
making process (i.e., whether to explore an area practices, relying on both norm-referenced
further or move on to another area of comparisons and qualitative procedures and
functioning) occurs after each item and is based observations to enrich their views of their
on clinical judgment. By employing this clients. In fact, Lezak (1995) suggests that
strategy, it is argued that a clinician is better either method is incomplete without the other.
able to understand the complexities of an
individual's performance and focus on the
impaired functional system (D'Amato, Rothlis- 4.09.1.2 Evaluation of Domain Areas
berg, & Leu, in press; Golden, 1981; Luria,
1980). Unique and individualized sets of Regardless of the position of the examiner
procedures, questions, or tasks shape the along the quantitative±qualitative continuum, it
evaluation process and might include an is helpful to conceptualize an evaluation of
individual case study approach consisting of domain areas, rather than simply focusing on
a mental status exam, observation, and symp- tests or specific problem behaviors. The follow-
tom checklists. From an educational perspec- ing domains are offered because of their
tive, a psychologist using a qualitative approach importance to daily functioning and usefulness
might gather information using work samples, to intervention development in educational and
classroom observations, or dynamic assessment vocational settings (Begali, 1994; D'Amato &
strategies (e.g., Campione & Brown, 1987; Rothlisberg, 1992; D'Amato, Rothlisberg, &
Feuerstein, Rand, & Hoffman, 1979). While Leu, in press; Gaddes & Edgell, 1994). These
the major areas traditionally covered in a domains include:
qualitative evaluation seem comprehensive (i) intelligence/cognitive abilities,
(e.g., investigations of motor functions, expres- (ii) personality/behavior/family information,
sive speech, writing, reading; see Hynd & (iii) memory and attention,
Semrud-Clikeman, 1990), this view does not (iv) learning processes,
rely on standardized batteries or clear compar- (v) academic achievement,
isons to normative populations. Instead, the (vi) sensory/perceptual systems,
selection of strategies utilized follows signifi- (vii) motor functions, and
cant clinical patient±practitioner interactions. (viii) communication/language skills.
244 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

The areas of intelligence and personality assess- Table 1 Subdomains of attention and memory,
ment are covered in more depth in Chapters 8 learning process, and special aptitudes.
and 12 of this volume. The remaining areas are
divided into the subareas presented in Table 1 to Attention and memory
provide a better understanding of the types of Attention
skills encompassed in each of these domains. Concentration or vigilance
Given the complexity of these areas, all should Visual memory
be considered both formally and informally. Verbal memory
Recall
Direct observations and interviews with the
Recognition
client and family members are vital components Short-term memory
in evaluating any individual's performance. The Long-term memory
selection of tests utilized to evaluate these
Learning processes (input and output)
abilities will vary greatly depending on the
unique needs of the individual, considered in Visual processing
tandem with the reason for referral. Motoric processing
Data on the functioning of these domains Auditory processing
Linguistic/verbal processing
provides useful information for the clinical
Simultaneous processing
psychologist. As demonstrated in Table 2, a Sequential processing
variety of testing instruments are appropriate in Academic achievement
each of these areas. It should be noted that
Sensory/perceptual
different authors have suggested various subsets
of domains for analysis as well as recommend- Visual
ing literally hundreds of other measures as Auditory
appropriate for children and adults (Batchelor, Tactile-kinesthetic
Integrated
1996a; Begali, 1992; Dean & Gray, 1990;
Gaddes & Edgell, 1994; Hynd & Willis, 1988; Motor functions
Lezak, 1995). Thus the instruments categorized Strength
in Table 2 represent only a sampling of available Speed
measures. The practitioner must take responsi- Coordination
bility for carefully matching the individual with Lateral preference
potential assessment options, after considering Sensory-motor integration
the distinct features of the instruments and the Communication/language skills
unique needs of the client. Receptive vocabulary
Expressive vocabulary
Speech/language
4.09.1.3 Intervention Approaches Written language

The referral question for any client is rarely


ªhow is this individual functioning today?º; Source: Adapted from D'Amato & Rothlisberg (in press) and
D'Amato, Rothlisberg & Rhodes (1997).
instead the referral source is most often
interested in the extent of decline following an
injury or illness, the expected future perfor-
mance in school or work settings, or how to
maximize a client's potential given certain social, and recreational settings despite difficul-
difficulties (e.g., head injury, learning disability; ties related to their deficits. To reach this end,
Long, 1996). Several decisions must be made in intervention strategies may focus on:
relation to the intervention strategy and will be (i) remediating or retraining impaired cogni-
reliant on the quality of the information tive processes (if there is a reason to believe that
provided by the assessment. Intervention may the process can be improved with practice),
be conceptualized using one of three ap- (ii) helping the client to develop new skills to
proaches: remediation (retraining a previously compensate for residual deficits,
learned skill), compensation (learning to use (iii) creating classroom or workplace adapta-
other strengths to offset a lost skill), or a tions and other environmental compensations
combination of both (D'Amato & Rothlisberg, that permit effective performance despite resi-
1996). In particular, it is critical to determine the dual deficits,
level of intervention on which to focus one's (iv) choosing instructional or therapeutic
efforts and the ideal combination of strategies procedures that best fit the client's profile of
that will work best with an individual. Reha- strengths and weaknesses, and
bilitative efforts emphasize enabling clients to (v) promoting improved metacognitive
reach their goals in educational, vocational, awareness of strengths and needs so that the
Assessment of Memory 245

Table 2 Common instruments and procedures used client can become an active participant in
to evaluate attention and memory, learning processes selecting goals and interventions strategies
and special aptitudes. (Ylvisaker, Szekeres, & Hartwick, 1994).
Attention and memory
Test of Variables of Attention (TOVATM) 4.09.2 ASSESSMENT OF MEMORY
Visual Search and Attention Test
Tests of Memory and Learning Memory is one of the most important
Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised cognitive functions to be assessed. It is a highly
Wide range assessment of memory and learning complex cognitive function that encompasses
Learning processes: input and integration several relatively discrete stages: reception and
registration of sensory stimuli, temporary short-
Detroit Tests of Learning Aptitude-3 term storage of information, storage of the
Swanson's Cognitive Processing Test information in a more permanent form (long-
Children's Auditory Verbal Learning Test
term memory), and recall and retrieval of
Tactile Performance Test (Halstead±Reitan
Battery) previously stored information (Shiffrin &
Speech±Sounds Perception Test (Halstead±Reitan Atkinson, 1969; Taylor, Fletcher, & Satz,
Battery) 1984). Functioning at each stage depends upon
Wisconsin Card Sort the integrity of the previous steps, with any
interruption in the hierarchy having the poten-
Academic achievement: output
tial to interfere with memory storage or
Woodcock±Johnson Psycho-educational Battery- retrieval. For example, difficulties with atten-
Revised: Achievement tion, which most closely relates to the first stage
Peabody Individual Achievement Test-Revised of memory, would obviously lead to problems
Wechsler Individual Achievement Test in short- and long-term storage as well as later
Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement retrieval of the information. A further source of
Keymath-revised complexity in understanding and measuring this
Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised
Test of Reading Comprehension-3
skill is the variety of theoretical approaches
Test of Written Language-3 from which memory can be conceptualized,
including information processing, neuropsy-
Sensory perception chological, and behavioral perspectives.
Observations Memory testing is very useful for assessing
Developmental history the possibility of organic disease, in helping to
Mental status examination differentiate between organic and psychiatric
Motor-free Visual Perception Test disorders, and in determining the functional
Vision and hearing screening significance of a memory problem (Black &
Strub, 1994). Most of the major neurobeha-
Motor (fine and gross)
vioral disorders such as dementia, confusional
Bender Visual-Motor Gestalt Test states, amnesia, material-specific memory/
Detroit Test of Learning Aptitude-3 (Motoric learning defects, and attentional dysfunction
Composite) are those in which disturbances of memory and
Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration attention are the prominent clinical features
Finger Oscillation Test (Hamsher, 1984). However, individuals with
Grip Strength Test
K-ABC Nonverbal Scale (e.g., Hand Movements
depression, post-traumatic stress disorder, dis-
subtest) sociative disorders (e.g., dissociative amnesia,
McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities (Motor dissociative identity disorders) might also
Scale) demonstrate attention and memory deficits
WISC-III and WAIS-R (Block Design, Object (American Psychiatric Association, 1994).
Assembly, Coding subtests) Memory skills represent a difficult area to
Bruininks±Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency address because of the variety of levels (e.g.,
Communication/language skills working, short-term, long-term) and the poten-
tial implications of a deficit. So too, memory to
Revised Token Test some degree is modality specific; that is, for
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised example, some individuals may have impaired
Test of Adolescent Language verbal memory but intact visual memory. Thus,
Test of Language Development-2 (Primary and it is important to look at various components of
Intermediate)
Test of Language Competence
memory rather than obtaining a simple global
memory score. Tests that provide a single
memory score offer a myopic and problematic
Source: Adapted from D'Amato, Rothlisberg, & Rhodes (1997). view of the multifaceted quality of memory.
246 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

4.09.2.1 Attention errors, which are rarely made by those without


attention or vigilance difficulties (Black &
One of the key components to memory and Strub, 1994). More formal measures such as
learning is the ability to attend selectively to the Visual Search and Attention Test (Trenerry,
relevant information that we are presented with Crosson, DeBoe, & Leber, 1990) can be used for
during the course of daily functioning. Atten- adults. This test purports to measure sustained
tion is an extremely important basic function attention and visual scanning. The test consists
which refers to the client's ability to maintain of four 60-second trials and is made up of four
awareness and to focus on a specified environ- tasks which become increasingly complex. The
mental stimulus, while screening out other respondent is required to cross out letters or
stimuli that are potentially distracting (Black symbols that match a target. Normative tables
& Strub, 1994). Being able to attend has three are provided and arranged in four 10-year age
major benefits for an individual: accuracy, bands, an 18±19 year age band, and a 60+ age
speed, and maintenance of mental processing band, and the statistical properties appear to be
(LaBerge, 1995). Attention deficits appear as adequate (Hooper, 1995). Based on how the
distractibility or impaired ability for focused stimuli are presented, these tasks can provide a
behavior, regardless of the individual's inten- measure of either visual or auditory vigilance.
tion (Lezak, 1995). Intact attention is a Vigilance tests are often referred to as
necessary condition of concentration which continuous performance tests (CPTs), which
requires an individual to sustain attention over are automated tasks, now computer-adminis-
an extended period of time. Concentration tered, that purport to measure sustained
problems may be due to a simple attentional attention (Greenberg & Waldman, 1993; Las-
disturbance, or to inability to maintain a siter, D'Amato, Raggio, Whitten, & Bardos,
purposeful attentional focus or, as is often the 1994; Rosvold, Mirsky, Sarason, Bransome, &
case, to both problems. This skill is important Beck, 1956). CPTs have become a popular tool
for adequate performance on any cognitive for clinicians to measure attentional perfor-
task, and can be impaired as a result of either mance, response inhibition, and medication
an organic or emotional disorder (D'Amato, monitoring in both children and adults (Eliason
1990; Dean, 1985a). Several psychological & Richman, 1987; Lassiter et al., 1994). Many
difficulties have been associated with atten- versions of CPT have been developed since the
tional problems such as impulsivity, distract- original but the basic methodology of these
ibility, and poor social judgment. Tests that tasks remains fairly constant. Clients are
require mental effort and persistence can presented with a variety of stimuli that are
measure an individual's ability to select, sustain, displayed for a short period of time, and are
and shift attention (Slomka & Tarter, 1993). By instructed to respond to a predefined ªtargetº
comparing performance on various types of stimulus. A number of different indices can be
tasks, the practitioner is able to distinguish a recorded with these tasks including omissions
global attention deficit from the more discrete, errors (i.e., failing to detect target stimulus),
task-specific problems of concentration and commissions (i.e., responding to nontarget
tracking. stimulus), and response times for correct
It is important to clarify the nature of an detections (Greenberg & Waldman, 1993).
attention problem by observing people's general Commission errors are considered to be in-
behavior as well as their performance on tests dicative of impulsivity and omission errors are
involving concentration. An interview with thought to denote inattention (Eliason &
family members can provide important infor- Richman, 1987; Lassiter et al., 1994). Examples
mation about attentiveness and susceptibility to of these types of tests include the CPT-2
distraction. So too, formal or quantitative (Lindgren & Lyon, 1983), the Raggio Evaluation
measures of attention and short-term memory of Attention Deficit Disorder (Raggio, 1991),
can be derived from the Digit Span and Coding and the Test of Variables of Attention (TO-
or Digit Symbol subtests of the WISC-III or VATM; Greenberg, 1993).
WAIS-R tests. Some of the more specific, The TOVATM is a nonlanguage-based, visual
informal measures of attention and concentra- continuous performance test. This test runs for
tion might include observation, a digit span 23 minutes on a fixed-interval schedule and
task, and a vigilance task as outlined by Strub presents two easily discriminated visual stimuli
and Black (1993). The individual is given orally for 100 milliseconds every two seconds. It was
a series of random letters with the letter ªAº designed for use in the diagnosis and monitoring
occurring with greater frequency than the other of pharmacotherapy of children and adults with
letters. The individual is instructed to signal attention deficit disorders and can be used with
whenever the targeted letter (i.e., A) is heard. individuals age five to adulthood. The test does
The individual's performance is scored for not require right-left discrimination and has
Assessment of Memory 247

negligible practice effects. Recently, the authors lasting memory (short-term or long-term).
of the TOVATM have created developmental Furthermore, these different memory functions
norms for children aged 6 to 16 which are must be systematically reviewed through visual
available for few other CPT versions (Green- and aural modalities using both recall and
berg & Waldman, 1993). This type of tool may recognition tasks. Lezak (1995) suggests that at
also be useful in assisting the clinician in the a minimum, the memory examination should
differential diagnosis of children and adoles- include: immediate retention tasks, including
cents experiencing externalizing problems (e.g., short-term memory with interference; learning
attention deficit disorder, oppositional defiant in terms of extent of recent memory, learning
disorder, conduct disorder, and aggression) capacity, and how well newly learned material is
and/or learning disabilities (Eliason & Rich- retained; and efficiency of retrieval of both
man, 1987; Greenberg & Waldman, 1993). recently learned and long-stored information
Despite the technological advances and newly (i.e., remote memory).
defined norms, CPTs present a quandary to Informal methods of assessment include tests
practitioners because of the variety of attributes of immediate recall such as digit repetition and/
that the tests reportedly measure. Some see these or sentence repetition, interviewing for infor-
tests as measuring attention and impulsivity mation from remote memory (e.g., ªwhere were
(Klee & Garfinkel, 1983), educational achieve- you born?º), and new learning ability (e.g.,
ment (Campbell, D'Amato, Raggio, & Ste- immediate recall for a verbal story, asking the
phens, 1991), behavior (Lassiter et al. 1994), individual to remember four unrelated words
general neuropsychological functioning (Hal- for a span of 5, 10, and 30 minutes). During this
perin, Sharma, Greenblatt, & Schwartz, 1991), last task, the examiner can provide recognition
and information processing (Swanson, 1981). cues if the individual is having difficulty
Given this variance, a practitioner is left with the remembering the words. It is expected that
question of how to interpret the test results of a those without difficulties will remember all
particular client. While research supports many words, while those with brain damage might be
of these claims, different versions of the CPTs expected to remember one (Black & Strub,
have been used in these studies, with different 1994). For aphasic clients or those with other
samples of children and adults. So too, the speech or language problems, an informal
validity of CPTs have been related to material measure of visual memory can be completed
collected from teachers, parents, and peers, and by hiding five objects around the interview
from standardized intelligence, achievement, room as the client names each item as it is
and personality tests. While it is obvious that hidden. After 10 minutes, the client is asked for
CPTs measure issues critical to learning and name and location of each item. Reportedly,
memory, the specificity of these instruments both normal and lower IQ clients should be able
remains unclear. In conclusion, Morris (1996) to find all five objects, with slightly lower
noted that many of the measures that purported performance for older patients (approximately
specifically to measure sustained attention often four objects) (Black & Strub, 1994; Simpson,
measured other variables, and thus many of Black, & Strub, 1986). These memory tasks
these tests have poor construct validation and should be supplemented with observations and
may be more appropriately viewed as multi- interviews with family members. So too, if an
dimensional in nature. As an alternative, ability measure such as the WISC-III or WAIS-
Barkley (1996) advocates the use of more natural R is administered, performance on Digit Span
tasks to study attention in an individual. He can provide information on immediate verbal
concludes that CPT-type tasks are unrelated to retention and the information subtest can be an
our daily functioning and thus, an individual's indicator of the extent of remote memory in an
performance on such tasks is irrelevant. In individual.
response to this concern, several investigators To complete an assessment of the major
have reportedly used television viewing, perfor- dimensions of memory, Lezak (1995) has
mance on classroom tasks, video games, and suggested including:
driving performance as a means of studying (i) a test of configural recall and attention
attention and its deficits in various groups of such as the visual reproduction subtest on the
children and young adults (Barkley, 1996). Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, 1987) or the
Benton Visual Retention Test (Benton-Sivan,
4.09.2.2 Short-term and Long-term Memory 1992);
(ii) a paragraph for recall to examine learn-
As already noted, each component of the ing and retention of meaningful verbal material;
memory process is reliant upon the previous and
steps. If information in sensory storage under- (iii) a test of learning ability that gives a
goes additional processing, it becomes a more learning curve and includes a recognition trial,
248 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

such as Rey's Auditory-Verbal Learning Test rapid intellectual, academic, and physical (in-
(for review see Lezak, 1995). cluding neurological) development has ended
These techniques should be integrated into (Reynolds & Bigler, 1994). In adults, memory
the general clinical interview to create a varied dysfunction is associated with a variety of well-
testing format, to enable the practitioner to use defined disorders, and in many individuals is
nonmemory tasks as interference activities, and one of the earliest and key symptoms such as in
to reduce stress in those clients who have Korsakoff's disease and various other demen-
memory impairments and are concerned about tias including Alzheimer's disease. Because of
their abilities (Black & Strub, 1994). the key role of evaluating memory in the clinical
There are numerous formal instruments setting, there are a number of instruments
available which measure different dimensions designed for memory assessment in older
of memory. For children and adolescents, the populations including the Doors and People: A
Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learn- Test of Visual and Verbal Recall and Recognition
ing (Sheslow & Adams, 1990), and the Test of (Baddeley, Emslie, & Nimmo-Smith, 1994), the
Memory and Learning (TOMAL; Reynolds & Memory Assessment Scales (MAS; Williams,
Bigler, 1994) can be used to evaluate individual 1991), and the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised
strengths and weaknesses in the areas of (WMS-R; Wechsler, 1987).
memory and attention. In particular, the The WMS-R (Wechsler, 1987) provides an
TOMAL represents a reliable, empirically extensive measure of several dimensions of
sound measure for children and adolescents. memory. It consists of eight short-term memory
The TOMAL consists of four core indexes tests, four delayed-recall subtests, and a brief
comprising Verbal Memory, Nonverbal Mem- screening measure of mental status (i.e., in-
ory, Composite Memory, and Delayed Recall. formation and orientation questions). The eight
Supplementary indexes for Learning, Attention short-term memory tests yield four composite
and Concentration, Sequential Memory, Free scores: Verbal Memory, Visual Memory, Total
Recall, and Associative Recall are also pro- General Memory, and Attention/Concentra-
vided. Subtests include Memory for Stories, tion. The delayed-recall measures can be
Facial Memory, Word Selective Reminding, combined to derive a fifth composite score,
Visual Selective Reminding, Object Recall, Delayed Recall. The test is intended for use for
Abstract Visual Memory, Digits Forward, individuals ranging in age from 16 to 74 and
Visual Sequential Memory, Paired Recall, requires approximately 50 minutes to adminis-
Memory-for-Location, Manual Imitation, Let- ter. The psychometric properties of the WMS-R
ters Forward, Digits Backward, and Letters are questionable in terms of low reliability
Backward. The TOMAL was standardized for coefficients for the composite scores (average
children aged 5 to 19. r = 0.74), but provides stronger support for the
The TOMAL boasts many unique features, General Memory and Attention/Concentration
including a great variety of memory indexes (average r = 0.81) scores. Although the WMS-
(Reynolds & Bigler, 1994). While some of the R demonstrated satisfactory discrimination
subtests appear similar to other memory power between various clinical groups, factor
measures, some unique features of this test analyses supported a two-factor rather than the
include a learning index where teaching is hypothesized five-factor model. Huebner (1992)
permissible, a sequential memory index, and concluded that this instrument must be used
an attention and concentration index. Delayed cautiously in making clinical decisions about
recall subtests are also available and are offered individuals and interpretation should be re-
as an evaluation of forgetting or memory decay. stricted to General Memory and Attention/
It is possible to compare the examinee's own Concentration ability.
personal learning curve with a standardized For adolescents and adults, the MAS (Wil-
learning curve. The test is easy to administer and liams, 1991) also provides a valid, reliable, and
generally user-friendly. Its psychometric prop- comprehensive measure of memory function-
erties appear to be well-developed. In the ing. The MAS was standardized for use with
TOMAL subtests, 63% of the reliability adults aged 18 to 90. The major functions
coefficients are at or exceed 0.9, 31% are measured by the MAS include: verbal and
between 0.8 and 0.89, and only 6% fall below nonverbal learning and immediate memory;
0.8. Test±retest coefficients range from 0.71 to verbal and nonverbal attention, concentration,
0.91. Support for the validity of this instrument and short-term memory; and memory for verbal
was determined through indices of content and nonverbal material following delay. In
validity, construct validity (e.g., factor analytic addition, measures of recognition, intrusions
studies), and criterion-related validity. during verbal learning recall, and retrieval
Assessment of memory dysfunction in adults strategies are also available. The test consists
is easier than in children because their period of of 12 subtests based on seven memory tasks.
Assessment of Learning 249

Five of the subtests assess the retention of classical and operant conditioning; Baldwin &
information learned in a subtest administered Baldwin, 1986), cognitive (e.g., information
earlier in the sequence. Total testing time is processing; Pressley & Levin, 1983), or social
approximately one hour. Test-retest reliability (e.g., social learning and modeling; Bandura,
for the MAS was estimated using general- 1977) perspectives, to name but a few. Further-
izability coefficients and these correlations more, the distinction drawn between measures
averaged 0.85 for the subtests, 0.9 for the of memory and measures of learning is tenuous
summary scales (i.e., Short-Term Memory, at best since all instruments that evaluate an
Verbal Memory, and Visual Memory), and individual's learning process will automatically
0.95 for the global memory scale. The validity of include aspects of memory functioning. Because
the MAS was established using three types of learning takes center stage as one of our
studies: convergent and discriminant validity, functions of daily living, it represents a critical
factorial validity, and group differentiation. area to evaluate. By fully evaluating several
Despite these strengths, Berk (1995) concluded components of learning, clinicians can deter-
that clinicians should use caution in interpreting mine where the process is breaking down and
the scores until some technical problems (e.g., provide recommendations for rehabilitation.
inadequate samples, lack of evidence for An understanding of how a client best learns
content validity) can be corrected. also has important implications for the types of
therapeutic strategies that will most likely be
4.09.2.3 Memory: Implications for Intervention successful. For example, if an adolescent has
difficulty processing and remembering auditory
Because memory is multifaceted, interven- information, talk therapy may not be the best
tions in the memory domain must also be approach. Supplementing discussion with role
multidimensional. Interventions may be divided play, videos, and other visual cues may be
several ways; those involving language, those necessary to facilitate the client's acquisition of
that are nonverbal, those requiring long, short, new knowledge.
or intermediate memory, and those that use a Difficulties in learning can be attributed to a
combined approach to aid in retention (Gaddes number of disorders, including the general
& Edgell, 1994; Lezak, 1995). Strategy selection category of learning disorders (e.g., dyslexia,
depends on accessing the strengths of the clients dyscalculia), traumatic brain injuries, drug and
or, in the case of injury or disease, accessing alcohol abuse, and medical disorders (e.g.,
those parts of the brain which have been least strokes, Alzheimer's disease). Indeed, all of the
impacted. For example, learners who have variables that can affect attention and memory
difficulty with nonverbal memory tasks but will also impact learning. Furthermore, certain
have retained verbal skills may benefit from chronic medical conditions can play a role in
memory interventions that use language. Mne- learning difficulties. For example, childhood
monic devices may be used to assist in recall of diabetes is associated with subtle problems with
information if a series of problem-solving steps respect to visuospatial and visuomotor proces-
is required (Mastropieri & Scruggs, 1989). For sing (e.g., Rovet, Ehrlich, & Hoppe, 1988),
those with more difficulty remembering, some verbal abilities (Kovacs, Goldston, & Ivengar,
simple techniques such as writing all meetings in 1992), and memory and attention problems,
an appointment book or using grocery shopping which translate into increased risk for difficulties
lists and daily ªto doº lists are practical. in academic achievement, (Kovacs et al., 1992;
Rovet, Ehrlich, Czuchta & Akler, 1993). So too,
4.09.3 ASSESSMENT OF LEARNING sickle cell anemia, an inherited disorder in people
of African descent, often produces some subtle
Memory is a ubiquitous component of daily cognitive impairments that can affect school
life and is fundamental to the process of achievement negatively (Brown, Armstrong, &
learning. One must be able to remember in Eckman, 1993). Among individuals with trau-
order to demonstrate learning. The classic matic brain injuries, learning process problems
definition of learning describes it as changes may be reflected as an uncertainty as to whether
in behavior as a result of experience. Some have a concept has been learned or not (Cohen, 1991).
even considered this definition of learning as
also defining memory (e.g., see Kolb & 4.09.3.1 Models of Learning
Whishaw, 1990). Despite this relatively simple
definition, the learning process itself defies easy Information-processing theories have proved
explanation. Learning can be approached from extremely useful in conceptualizing learning
a neuropsychological (e.g., planning, attention, because this model can be applied to any given
simultaneous, successive [PASS] model; Das, cognitive task and allows the practitioner to
Naglieri, & Kirby, 1994), behavioral (e.g., specify where the learning process is breaking
250 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

down. Silver (1993) proposed an information- & Dawson, 1978). More recently, neuropsycho-
processing model based on four steps: input logical models have been applied to ATIs and
(how information from the sense organs enters offer promise for identifying aptitudes and
the brain), integration (interpreting and proces- prescribing treatments (D'Amato, 1990; Hart-
sing the information), storage (storing the lage & Telzrow, 1983). One of the major
information for later retrieval), and output techniques that Cronbach and Snow (1977)
(expressing information via language or muscle suggested for matching treatment approaches
activity). Learning is reliant upon each of the with learner aptitudes was ªcapitalization of
first three steps and is observed or inferred from strengths.º Our increasing knowledge of how
the fourth step. Other models of information the brain functions allows clinicians to obtain a
processing highlight the importance of the more detailed understanding of how a client
working memory in skill acquisition and learns new information. For example, although
learning (Baddeley, 1986; Just & Carpenter, the cerebral hemispheres act in concert, the right
1992; Swanson, 1995). Working memory has hemisphere seems to be specialized for holistic,
traditionally been defined as a system of limited spatial, and/or nonverbal reasoning whereas the
capacity for the temporary maintenance and left shows a preference for verbal, serial, and/or
manipulation of information (e.g., Baddeley, analytic type tasks (Gaddes & Edgell, 1994;
1986; Just & Carpenter, 1992) and most closely Lezak, 1995; Reynolds, 1981a; Walsh, 1978).
corresponds to the integration step in Silver's Similarly, models of cognitive processing have
model. Tasks that measure working memory are been proposed that agree with the specialization
those that require the client to remember a small of how scientists think the brain processes
amount of material for a short time while information; some have called this preferential
simultaneously carrying out further operations. processing styles (D'Amato, 1990). For exam-
In daily life, these tasks might include remem- ple, simultaneous processing ability has been
bering a person's address while listening to affiliated with the right hemisphere because of
instructions about how to reach a specific its holistic nature; it deals with the synthesis of
destination (Swanson, 1995). When viewed parts into wholes and is often implicitly spatial
from this perspective, working memory differs (Das, Kirby, & Jarman, 1979). In contrast, the
from the related concept of short-term memory left hemisphere processes information using a
which is typically described as remembering more successive/sequential method, considering
small amounts of material and reproducing it serial or temporal order of input (Dean, 1984,
without integrating or transforming the infor- 1986). Models of brain organization have also
mation in any way (e.g., repeating back a series been proposed that attempt to explain the
of numbers) (Cantor, Engle, & Hamilton, 1991; diversity and complexity of behavior.
Just & Carpenter, 1992). Working memory An expansion of the hemispheric specializa-
appears to be extremely important to an tion approach is offered in the planning,
individual's ability to learn, and in adult attention, simultaneous, successive (PASS)
samples has correlations of 0.55±0.92 with cognitive processing model (Das et al., 1994)
reading and intelligence measures (e.g., Dane- which proposes four processing components.
man & Carpenter, 1980; Kyllonen & Christal, This model is based on the neuropsychological
1990). model of Luria (1970, 1973, 1980; Reynolds,
In an effort to promote the notion that input 1981a) and presents a comprehensive theoretical
and integration of stimuli can impact subse- model by which cognitive processes can be
quent learning, Cronbach and Snow (1977) have examined. On the basis of his clinical investiga-
advanced a theory suggesting that some types of tions with brain-injured patients, Luria (1973)
individuals might benefit from one form of suggested that there are three functional units
treatment, whereas others might benefit from that provide three classes of cognitive processes
another type of treatment: an aptitude by (i.e., memory, conceptual, and perceptual)
treatment interaction (ATI). Many researchers responsible for all mental activity. Figure 2
and educators alike believe that matching provides a graphic presentation of the PASS
learner characteristics with treatment ap- model of cognitive processing. The functional
proaches can enhance learning (e.g., Cronbach units work in concert to produce behavior and
& Snow, 1977; Resnick, 1976; Reynolds, 1981b). provide arousal and attentional (first unit),
However, subsequent studies have demon- simultaneous-successive (second unit), and
strated little support for this theory (e.g., Arter planning (third unit) cognitive processes. The
& Jenkins, 1977; Tarver & Dawson, 1978). PASS model separates the second unit into two
Initially, theories of input examined learner individual processes (i.e., simultaneous and
modalities (e.g., visual, auditory, kinesthetic), sequential). Instruments can be used to measure
which were later deemed to be too simplistic individual strengths in these different styles of
(Arter & Jenkins, 1977; Kaufman, 1994; Tarver processing.
Assessment of Learning 251

Input Output

Serial Concurrent Serial Concurrent

First Third
Functional Unit Functional Unit

AROUSAL/
ATTENTION PLANNING
Knowledge Base

Knowledge Base
Conceptual

Conceptual
Perceptual

Perceptual
Memory

Memory

Brain Stem Frontal

Occipital, Parietal & Temporal


Functional Unit
Second

Memory Conceptual Perceptual

SIMULTANEOUS & SUCCESSIVE

Figure 2 PASS model of cognitive processing. (Assessment of Cognitive Processes: The Pass Theory of
Intelligence (p. 21), by J. P. Das, J. A. Naglieri, and J. R. Kirby, 1994, New York: Allyn & Bacon. Copyright
1994, by Allyn & Bacon. Reprinted with permission.)

Knowledge of the brain and theories govern- beyond the product to determine the influence
ing information processing can determine the of related factors. These factors can include the
types of data collected during the assessment nature of the stimuli used (visual, verbal,
phase. For example, instead of simply observing tactile), the method of presentation (visual,
whether the individual was successful at a task verbal, concrete, social), the type of response
or set of measures, the practitioner looks desired (verbal, motor, constructional), and the
252 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

response time allowed (timed, untimed; Cooley lisberg, & Rhodes, 1997). These specialized
& Morris, 1990). Other researchers have measures of performance in learning do not fall
advocated a move to an even more intense neatly within the traditional domains of
examination of processing through the use of intelligence, achievement, or neuropsychologi-
dynamic assessment strategies (Campione & cal processing. These tests, including the Detroit
Brown, 1987; Feuerstein et al., 1979; Palincsar, Tests of Learning Aptitude-3 (DTLA-3; Ham-
Brown, & Campione, 1991). Theoretically, this mill, 1991), the Swanson Cognitive Processing
strategy allows the examiner to obtain informa- Test (S-CPT; Swanson, 1996), the Children's
tion about the client's responsiveness to hints or Auditory Verbal Learning Test-2 (Talley, 1993),
probes, and thus elicits processing potential and others can offer valuable information
(Swanson, 1995). When an examinee is having concerning how individuals attend to and deal
difficulty, the examiner attempts to move the with new information. While practitioners have
individual from failure to success by modifying used these instruments to document clients'
the format, providing more trials, providing strengths and weaknesses, diagnose problems,
information on successful strategies, or offering and chart the course of disorders, these
increasingly more direct cues, hints, or prompts instruments offer more practical information
(Swanson, 1995). This approach allows the concerning rehabilitation or program planning
examiner an opportunity to evaluate perfor- than for diagnostic activities.
mance change in the examinee with and without Neuropsychological tests have also been seen
assistance. However, there is little if any by some to evaluate variables related to
standardized information available on this learning processes. In fact, the Halstead Reitan
technique and it has been criticized for its Neuropsychological Test Battery (Reitan &
clinical nature and poor reliability (e.g., Pa- Wolfson, 1985, 1993) reportedly measures
lincsar et al., 1991). problem solving, tactual discrimination, sen-
sory recognition, spatial memory, verbal-audi-
4.09.3.2 Learning Processes: Input and tory discrimination, attention, nonverbal
Integration auditory discrimination, psychomotor speed,
and manual dexterity as well as several other
Generally, three approaches have been uti- skills (D'Amato, 1990; Dean, 1985a, 1985b,
lized when evaluating how individuals prefer- 1986; Lezak, 1995). Individuals interested in a
entially process information. The first neuropsychological approach to processing
approach, seen as the traditional approach, should consult some of the recommended
employs established measures (such as the references and Chapters 10 and 11, this volume.
WISC-III) with the practitioner seeking to The area of learning processing is more
understand information processing through difficult to evaluate and is often subsumed
an analysis of common test results such as within the intelligence or achievement domains.
reviewing global scores, subtests, and clusters of One measure that has a long history in the
subtests (Kaufman, 1990, 1994). For example, a evaluation of processing styles is the DTLA-3.
pattern of strengths on the Picture Completion, This instrument was designed for use with
Block Design, Object Assembly and corre- individuals aged 6 to 17. More recently, two
sponding weaknesses in Picture Arrangement other versions of this test, the Detroit Tests of
and Coding might suggest meaningful differ- Learning Aptitude-Adult (Hammill & Bryant,
ences in a client's mental processing style (i.e., 1991a) and Detroit Test of Learning Aptitude-
right hemispheric functioning vs. left hemi- Primary (2nd ed.; Hammill & Bryant, 1991b)
spheric functioning from cerebral specialization have expanded the usefulness of this instrument
theory, or simultaneous vs. successive coding to include individuals from age 2 to 79. The
from Luria theory). The second view of DTLA-3 consists of 11 subtests comprising:
information processing, considered the infor- Word Opposites, Design Sequences, Sentence
mal approach, considers observations, check- Imitations, Reversed Letters, Story Construc-
lists, and learning style inventories to tion, Design Reproduction, Basic Information,
understand how individuals learn. From this Symbolic Relations, Word Sequences, Story
view, individuals who seem to profit most from Sequences, Picture Fragments; and 16 composite
visual clues may be seen as visual learners, and scores: General Mental Ability Composite,
might be taught utilizing overheads, visual Optimal Level Composite, Domain Composites
diagrams, and worksheets. The final approach (Verbal, Nonverbal, Attention-Enhanced,
to understanding processing stems from the Attention-Reduced, Motor-Enhanced, Motor-
administration and analysis of the many unique Reduced), Theoretical Composites (Fluid In-
measures that have been offered as learning style telligence, Crystallized Intelligence, Associative
or processing tests. This approach is seen as a Level, Cognitive Level, Simultaneous Proces-
nontraditional test approach (D'Amato, Roth- sing, Successive Processing, Verbal Scale, and
Assessment of Learning 253

Performance Scale). The testing time is esti- measure of achievement (e.g., Anastasi, 1988;
mated to vary from 50 minutes to two hours. The Dean, 1977, 1983), it would seem that the
internal consistency reliabilities for the subtests operationalization of the two areas allows for a
are sufficiently high; however, the data on comparison of more generic problem-solving
stability are limited. It was also noted that the and verbal tasks to those directly involved in
factor analysis does not support the construct scholastic performance. Thus, a measure of
validity of the different composites. In fact, only ability may be conceived of as attempting to
four factors (one being a residual or difficult to address the concept of underlying skills or
interpret category) were identified in the manual. capacities, whereas the measure of achievement
Despite these concerns, Poteat (1995) notes that is tied to the notion of the individual's
the DTLA-3 can be recommended as an adjunct proficiency in applying that ability in a
to some of the better developed measures of functional way to real world skills (e.g.,
intelligence and it provides some potentially academics). A measure of academic achieve-
valuable information about diverse abilities. The ment can help provide information as to the
DTLA has been especially helpful when evalu- degree of impairment experienced by indivi-
ating children who suffer from learning dis- duals, especially among children and adoles-
abilities or traumatic brain injuries. The Detroit cents. New learning, however, is not isolated to
Tests of Learning Aptitude-Adult (Hammill & the school years. Adults required to learn new
Bryant, 1991a) comprises 12 subtests and 16 skills as part of job training, vocational
composites and measures areas similar to the rehabilitation, or after brain injuries are all
DTLA-3. Internal consistency reliability of all placed in very real learning situations. It is
scores approximates 0.9 for all ages. This critical to have an understanding of the client's
instrument represents a useful tool in practice basic skills in order to facilitate vocational,
because of the type of information it can provide academic, and intervention decision making.
regarding client cognitive functioning in relation Assessment of academic achievement can
to learning new information. occur through a blend of formal and informal
A very recent contribution to the field of measures as well. For example, in a school
cognitive processing is the S-CPT (Swanson, setting, reviewing student clients' work samples,
1996) which purports to measure different interviewing the students and teacher about
aspects of intellectual functioning and informa- their learning and the classroom, and classroom
tion processing potential. The battery, designed observations can provide essential information.
for use with persons age five to adulthood, So too, curriculum-based measurement, where
draws from the work on information processing informal reading, writing, and math probes
theory and dynamic assessment. The subtests in (Shinn, 1989) are obtained to determine the
this measure are as follows: Rhyming Words, clients' current level of functioning and progress
Visual Matrix, Auditory Digit Sequencing, during intervention phases, are particularly
Mapping and Directions, Story Retelling, useful for monitoring the effectiveness of
Picture Sequencing, Phrase Recall, Spatial treatment approaches to learning difficulties
Organization, Semantic Association, Semantic (Fuchs, 1994). There are also several types of
Categorization, and Nonverbal Sequencing. norm-referenced instruments that are available,
This standardized test battery can be adminis- which, because of the availability of a standard
tered in an abbreviated form (five subtests) or in normative base, permit comparison across a
a complete form under traditional or interactive wide variety of curricular contexts (D'Amato,
testing conditions. Normative data for the S- Rothlisberg, & Rhodes, 1997). Some of these
CPT were gathered on 1611 children and adults. instruments measure a particular area of
The author reports high levels of internal achievement such as math or reading (e.g.,
reliability and high construct and criterion- Keymath Revised, Connolly, 1988; Test of
related validity (Swanson, 1995). This instru- Reading Comprehension-3, Brown, Hammill,
ment may offer a promising alternative to & Wiederholt, 1995; Test of Written Language-
product-oriented evaluation strategies while 3, Hammill & Larsen, 1996) while others
still allowing for normative comparison. provide a broad-based screening of a number
of academic areas (e.g., Peabody Individual
Achievement Test-Revised [PIAT-R], Mark-
4.09.3.3 Academic Achievement: Output wardt, 1989; Woodcock±Johnson Psychoeduca-
tional Battery-Revised [WJPB-R], Woodcock &
It is likely that those individuals who Johnson, 1989). These broad-based tests all
experience difficulties in processing or learning have a similar organizational structure. For
will display academic difficulties as well. example, measures in a particular area, such as
Although some might hold that there is little reading, are typically divided into basic skill
difference in the measure of ability and the areas (e.g., reading decoding) and some form of
254 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

applied skill area (e.g., reading comprehension) tage may not give a good indication of the
so that variations in the aspects of the academic expectations for student performance in the
tasks can be noted. The difference between classroom where recall and more integrated
measures often lies in the method by which they answers are the norm (D'Amato, Rothlisberg, &
obtain their information (e.g., whether visual- Rhodes, 1997).
motor or oral responses are required); that is,
whether they require the examinee to indicate
the response through nonverbal (e.g., pointing) 4.09.3.4 Learning: Implications for Intervention
or verbal output.
A number of authors have related how
The achievement test that is designed for the
knowledge of the way individuals process
broadest range of individuals is the WJPB-R
information can contribute to the development
with norms ranging from 2 to 95 years of age.
of treatment based on neuropsychological
The WJPB-R consists of both a cognitive and an
processes (D'Amato, 1990; Reynolds, 1981b,
achievement component. The tests of achieve-
1986; Telzrow, 1985). For example, when
ment are divided into a standard battery
learning how to read, individuals who display
consisting of four broad areas: Reading (Letter-
a simultaneous/visual spatial strength in pro-
Word Identification, Passage Comprehension),
cessing might benefit from being taught using a
Mathematics (Calculations, Applied Problems),
whole word approach whereas individuals who
Written Language (Dictation, Writing Sam-
display a strength in sequential/auditory pro-
ples), and Broad Knowledge (Science, Social
cessing can be taught using a phonetic approach
Studies, Humanities). A supplemental battery is
(Whitten, D'Amato, & Chittooran, 1992). For
also available to expand the standard battery
both children and adults, cognitive rehabilita-
coverage. It includes Word Attack, Reading
tion is an emerging discipline which includes the
Vocabulary, Quantitative Concepts, Proofing,
retraining or use of compensatory strategies in
and Writing Fluency. Employing one or more of
thinking and problem-solving skills (Wedding,
the supplemental subtests gives the examiner the
Horton, & Webster, 1986). Cognitive retraining
option of computing additional areas of
can include assistance in strategy development
achievement such as Basic Reading Skills and
for attention and concentration, memory,
Reading Comprehension which is consistent
language, perceptual and cognitive deficits,
with the language of the Individuals with
and social behavior. Thus, the term cognitive
Disabilities Educational Act of 1990 and some
retraining encompasses all areas of functioning
state legislative guidelines for identifying spe-
that may have been negatively impacted by
cific areas of learning disability. This instrument
neuropsychological disorders or traumatic
is statistically sound and ample amounts of
brain injury (D'Amato, Rothlisberg, & Leu,
research have been conducted and support the
in press; Gray & Dean, 1989). Assisting learners
use of this test.
with cognitive remediation or compensation
Another measure of achievement, the PIAT-
often includes the use of metacognitive strate-
R (Markwardt, 1989) has also been supported as
gies. Metacognition includes analyzing the
a well-developed and psychometrically sound
processes an individual uses to generate an idea
instrument (Williams & Vincent, 1991). It
or thought. By receiving assistance in breaking
consists of five subtest scores (General Informa-
down problems and understanding the pro-
tion, Reading Recognition, Reading Compre-
cesses needed to solve problems, clients may
hension, Mathematics, Spelling) that are
learn how to generalize the process to many
provided in addition to the Total Reading
problem types and improve overall learning and
and Total Test scores. A Written Expression
functioning. Although cognitive retraining is
and optional Written Language score are also
time consuming, the generalizability of the
available. The PIAT-R was normed for indivi-
strategies has been seen as appropriate to many
duals aged 5±18 years. It is different from other
settings (Gray & Dean, 1989; Kavale, Forness,
tests in that it includes a larger pictorial
& Bender, 1988).
component in its item types, letting children
avoid the need for verbal reply, and instead
expecting them to point at the correct answer 4.09.4 ASSESSMENT OF SPECIAL
(out of four) for reading, spelling, and mathe- APTITUDES
matics items. Since the task demands for
recognition of information do not appear to In determining the basis for a client's
be the same as for recall, this response format difficulty, it is critical to explore the building
may aid children with retrieval difficulties or blocks of memory and learning to obtain an
those that have developed some background understanding of how the individual processes
knowledge of the area in question. It should be information (sensory input). For instance,
noted, though, that this response-type advan- sensory and perceptual skills are essential to
Assessment of Special Aptitudes 255

receiving stimuli from the environment and In the assessment of sensory perception, it is
making sense of what is received. So too, a important to evaluate visual, auditory, and
clinician must examine the output or produc- haptic (tactile) functions. For children and older
tion that the client demonstrates in response to adults it is especially important that actual
stimuli via action (e.g., motor skills) or com- sensory deficits have been ruled out through the
munication (e.g., spoken language, writing). administration of a thorough vision and hearing
That is, clients may understand a task, but, test. If these senses appear to be intact, an in-
because of integration difficulties or language depth evaluation of functioning is warranted.
impairments, be unable to demonstrate their Sensory perception can be evaluated informally
knowledge. For example, the reproduction of a through clinical observations, formally through
visual stimulus in response to a request involves standardized tests, or via other methods of data
both perceptual discrimination and fine motor collection. However, at times these strategies
development, as well as the ability to integrate may prove inconclusive regarding the etiology
visual, tactile, and auditory skills. Therefore, of performance difficulties and more formal
inadequate performance in copying geometric assessment is necessary to evaluate a client's
designs developed to assess these skills may stem functioning.
from: a misperception, or faulty interpretation Several instruments are available to measure
of the input information; problems in executing a client's functioning within this domain. Most
the fine motor response, or output; and/or are inexpensive and relatively quick to admin-
difficulties integrating the input and output, ister. Within the visual modality, the Motor-
otherwise known as integrative or central Free Visual Perception Test (MFVPT; Colar-
processing difficulties. By evaluating the do- usso & Hammill, 1996) allows the clinician to
mains of sensory perception, sensory-motor evaluate visual perception without motor
integration, and communication/language, the involvement in children. This 36-item measure
practitioner is in a better position to understand assesses five facets of visual perception: spatial
the client's ability to receive information relations, visual discrimination, figure±ground,
adequately, integrate these basic skills, and visual closure, and visual memory. The MFVPT
demonstrate the products of memory and is intended for children four to eight years of
learning processes. age. The MFVPT can offer information
essential for the differential diagnosis of motor
4.09.4.1 Sensory Perception vs. visual processing problems. However, when
used in isolation from other measures or
Perception of stimuli is a complex process techniques, the MFVPT offers information
involving many different aspects of brain regarding visual processing difficulties but is
functioning (Lezak, 1983). Typically, percep- unable to rule out motor concerns. For adults,
tion includes recognizing features and relation- the Benton Revised Visual Retention Test
ships among features. It is affected by context (Benton-Sivan, 1992) is a widely used measure
(figure±ground) and intensity, duration, sig- of visuoperceptual ability, constructional skills,
nificance, and familiarity of the stimuli (Ylvi- and immediate visual memory (Youngjohn,
saker, Szekeres, & Hartwick, 1994). Sensory Larrabee, & Crook, 1993). Clients are required
perception skills are vital to an individual's to reproduce abstract geometric designs from
understanding and response to the environment memory.
because they form the basis of each individual's Some clients have difficulty discriminating
interaction with the world (D'Amato, Rothlis- sounds even when thresholds for sound
berg, & Rhodes, 1997; Lezak, 1995). Difficulties perceptions are intact (Lezak, 1995). Auditory
may manifest themselves in the individual's discrimination can be tested by having the
ability to use information gained through the client repeat words and phrases spoken by the
senses. For example, a client may be able to hear clinician, or by asking the client whether two
sounds well, but have trouble understanding spoken words are the same or different. On this
what is heard (auditory processing). Likewise, a task, the clinician will want to use word pairs
client may be able to see words clearly but have that sound alike such as ªcatº and ªcapº along
problems reproducing them when writing with identical word pairs (Lezak, 1995). This
(visual-motor difficulties). Sensory perception technique has been formalized through the
tasks often form the foundation for the later development of Wepman's Auditory Discrimi-
performance of higher order cognitive skills. nation Test (Wepman & Reynolds, 1987) which
Without the ability to accurately sense and allows the clinician to determine whether the
perceive cues from the environment, the learner client is able to discriminate similar sounding
is placed in the position of trying to decode a words adequately. Although the test was
message when the code is scrambled and often originally devised to identify auditory discri-
changing. mination problems in young school children,
256 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

and the present norms were developed on Movement also can consist of both discrete and
samples of four to eight year olds, norms for continuous patterns. Movements that are dis-
the oldest age group (8±0 to 8±11) are adequate crete might involve something as simple as
for adults since auditory discrimination is lifting a finger, while continuous movements
generally fully developed by this age (Lezak, include an integrated set of skills like skipping.
1995). Movements may be disrupted if damage exists in
The perception of tactile stimuli is regularly the premotor cortex where the ªkinesthetic
measured as a component of a thorough melodyº is believed to be formed. If this occurs,
neuropsychological examination, but less often the individual may not be able to perform serial-
in nonspecialized clinical settings. Informal continuous movements but may be able to
strategies for evaluating this area include asking demonstrate the specific discrete movements.
clients to indicate whether they feel the sharp or Because of the complexity of motor patterns, the
the dull end of a pin, pressure from one or two individual's posture, movement in isolation,
points (applied simultaneously and close to- and movement in serial order should be assessed
gether), or pressure from a graded set of plastic for possible intervention. This can be accom-
hairs, the ªVon Frey hairsº (Lezak, 1995). The plished by observing individuals completing
eyes should be closed or the hand kept out of tasks such as writing their name (uses one hand),
sight when tactile sensory functions are tested. tying their shoes (uses both hands), and also
More formal measures include the Tactile Form performing novel tasks such as repeated tapping
Perception Test (Benton, Hamsher, Varney, & or clapping patterns. It should be noted if there
Spreen, 1983) and the Tactual Performance Test is difficulty integrating the use of both hands.
(Reitan & Davison, 1974). Deficits in tactile Both fine and gross motor skills should always
senses are often associated with damage to the be evaluated.
right hemisphere of the brain and may have Although informal methods will yield a great
important implications for a client's vocational deal of information regarding a client's fine and
functioning (Lezak, 1995). gross motor functioning, several standardized
instruments are also available which measure
various specific or broad components of
4.09.4.1.1 Sensory perception: implications for motoric functioning. For example, the
intervention Bruininks±Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency
If the client is having difficulty in one or (Bruininks, 1978) provides a comprehensive
more areas of sensory perception, this informa- picture of an individual's motor development.
tion is critical for intervention planning. That The instrument was designed for children aged
is, the client's unique pattern of receiving 4±5 to 14±15 and can be administered in 15±60
information from the environment can be used minutes, depending on whether the complete or
to create effective education, rehabilitation, or short form of the battery is used. Three
therapeutic intervention. If a client is weak in composite scores are provided in the areas of:
auditory processing but strong in visual Gross Motor Development (Running speed and
processing, for example, visual cues such as agility, Balance, Bilateral coordination,
drawings, videos, or demonstrations may be the Strength), Gross and Fine Motor Development
most effective means for training them in new (Upper-limb coordination), and Fine Motor
skills. Development (Response speed, Visual-motor
control, Upper-limb speed and dexterity).
Specific fine motor abilities can also be
4.09.4.2 Motor: Fine and Gross measured by using the Finger Tapping Test
and the Grip Strength Test which are both a part
The motor domain involves a range of both of the Halstead-Reitan Battery (Reitan &
fine and gross motor movement. Fine motor Wolfson 1993).
skill is commonly thought of as movement To measure a client's lateral preference, the
which does not involve the entire body. Writing, Lateral Preference Schedule (Dean, 1988) can be
opening a letter, or tying a shoe are all examples administered to obtain a better understanding
of fine motor movements. Gross motor move- of clients' lateral preference in the use of their
ment involves large extremities and often the eyes, ears, arms, hands, and feet (Rothlisberg,
entire body. Activities such as walking or sitting 1991). Atypical patterns of lateral preference
down involve gross motor capacities. Inten- have been hypothesized to indicate potential
tional movement, using fine and gross motor predictors of reading difficulty (Bemporad &
skills, involves a series of brain-based systems Kinsbourne, 1983; Dean, Schwartz, & Smith,
and is learned with repetition. With repeated 1981). Determining lateral preference can be
action, the movement becomes rote or, as Luria useful in interpreting assessment findings and in
(1973) described it, a ªkinesthetic melody.º creating a plan for rehabilitation (Lezak, 1995).
Assessment of Special Aptitudes 257

4.09.4.3 Sensory-motor Integration performance (Hartlage & Golden, 1990). In the


personality area, performance on the Bender
An additional component of our motoric may also be used to develop hypotheses
functioning is the ability to integrate what is regarding impaired performance due to poor
received by the senses with what is produced planning, impulsivity, or compulsivity. Extre-
through action. For example, an individual may mely large or small figures, heavily reinforced
be able to perceive letters correctly and have lines, and second attempts are examples of the
adequate fine motor control, but still have item reproduction difficulties which are thought
difficulty correctly copying material presented to indicate emotional concerns on the part of the
in visual form. Numerous paper-and-pencil individual.
tests have been developed to assess motor The VMI is an individual or group adminis-
function as it relates to visual-motor integra- tered test that involves copying a sequence of 24,
tion. Two of the most popular measures for this increasingly complex, geometric figures. The
purpose are the Bender Visual-Motor Gestalt test requires a relatively short administration
(Bender, 1938) and the Developmental Test of time and is designed primarily for ages 4 to 13.
Visual-Motor Integration (VMI; Beery, 1989). The VMI offers several advantages as a tool for
The Visual-Motor Gestalt Test (Bender, assessment and is widely used in psychological
1938) is an individually administered test evaluation and research. The most common use
containing nine geometric figures which the of the VMI, now in its third edition, seems to be
client copies on to a blank sheet of paper. While in assisting with the diagnosis of children who
historically this test was seen as a general are suspected of having learning problems due
measure of organicity, it is more appropriately to visual-motor difficulties. The VMI is also
used as a measure of visual-motor skills. frequently employed when investigating the
Standard scores are provided in the develop- reliability and validity of other tests of visual-
mental scoring system for children ages 5±0 to motor integration, such as the Bender, self
11±11, although it is frequently used with adults drawing tasks, progressive matrices, and neu-
as well. Most commonly known as the ªBend- ropsychological tests (Goldstein, Smith, &
er,º this measure is perhaps the best known and Waldrep, 1986; Palisano & Dichter, 1989).
most widely used visual-motor assessment Because the VMI does not require a verbal
procedure available today (Bender, 1938; response, it has also been used to assess visual-
Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1990). As a component motor processes among non-English-speaking
of a comprehensive assessment battery, perfor- children (Brand, 1991; Frey & Pinelli, 1991).
mance on the Bender has long been thought to
reveal visual-motor difficulties that may be
associated with cerebral impairment (Sattler, 4.09.4.3.1 Motor: implications for intervention
1992). Traditionally used to assess an indivi- Motor problems and sensory-motor integra-
dual's constructional praxic skills, the Bender tion difficulties can impair a client's ability to
provides an evaluation of motor integration write or to learn new skills requiring motor
employed in the execution of complex learned coordination, and generally can have a negative
movements (Hartlage & Golden, 1990). The impact on daily functioning. In the classroom
information generated through this process setting, possible suggestions for accommodat-
may then be compared with levels of perfor- ing these difficulties might include modifying
mance across other measures of functioning. instructions to compensate for motor difficul-
Alternate uses of the Bender include its ties (e.g., allowing pointing to the correct choice
administration as a memory test as well as a rather than writing, allowing students to tape
copying test. This dual administration process record notes or copy them from others). In a
can be employed to assess different mental rehabilitation setting, the client may need to
functions (short-term visual memory and visual learn alternative methods for writing such as
perception) which utilize the same modalities in using word processing programs on a computer
perception and task execution (Sattler, 1992). or, if serious difficulties exist, using voice-
An additional technique available when inter- activated programs. Consultation with an
preting the Bender performance is to have occupational therapist or a physical therapist
individuals compare the figure which they will be extremely helpful in treatment planning
produced with the corresponding stimulus when motor difficulties are evident.
design. If the client is unable to recognize
obvious differences between the two designs, a
perceptual deficit may be involved. Likewise, if 4.09.4.4 Communication/Language
the client is able to detect a difference between
the two figures, but is unable to make them Language is the basic tool of human
alike, motor involvement may be influencing communication and hence essential to evaluate
258 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

when working with any client (Black & Strub, produce approximately 20 animal names and a
1994). It should be viewed as a key skill because total of 40±60 words with performance depend-
it serves as a primary means of conveying ing to some degree on the client's intelligence,
information from the individual to others and education, and social/linguistic background
from others to the individual. Thus, commu- (Black & Strub, 1994). Additional methods of
nication difficulties have the power to influence examining expressive language include having
all areas of life (D'Amato, Rothlisberg, & the client repeat back meaningful verbal phrases
Rhodes, 1997). Difficulties or dysfunction or sentences of increasing length and semantic
found on tests of higher level functioning complexity. Word finding and naming difficul-
(e.g., learning processes) may well be secondary ties can be detected by the client's responses to
to a language disorder. Accordingly, language the open-ended questions or by having the client
should be evaluated early in the course of an describe a picture containing a series of objects
assessment to rule out problems in this area of or actions (Black & Strub, 1994).
functioning (Black & Strub, 1994; Lezak, Another major area of language functioning
1995). Another obvious reason to evaluate is receptive language or an individual's ability to
language and communication skills is that understand what has been said. A comprehen-
language disorders occur as the result of a wide sive assessment should include an evaluation of
range of neurologic diseases and can manifest the individual's ability to analyze and integrate
in a variety of forms of aphasia (e.g., information presented in a verbal format, since
Wernicke's, anomia, global, alexia, agraphia; a common difficulty among those experiencing
Kolb & Whishaw, 1990). To aid in the traumatic head injury is a decreased capacity to
interpretation of test findings, it is important coordinate the social aspects of language
for the clinician to be familiar with the various (Ylvisaker, Szekeres, Haarbauer-Krupa, Ur-
classic clinical aphasia presentations (see banczyk, & Feeney, 1994). It is not sufficient to
Gaddes & Edgell, 1994; Kolb & Whishaw, evaluate language comprehension based on
1990; Lezak, 1995). open-ended questions because this method
The language evaluation should be systematic relies on expressive skills and does not examine
and include an assessment of a range of comprehension in isolation (Black & Strub,
relatively specific language functions. Assess- 1994). Language comprehension can be eval-
ment must evaluate both receptive and expres- uated informally by asking the client to point to
sive verbal and nonverbal abilities to determine common objects in the room or by asking a
if adaptations are needed to enhance the series of increasingly complex questions that
individual in academic, vocational, and social require only a ªyesº or ªnoº response (e.g. ªDo
situations. As part of an informal evaluation of dogs have four legs?º) (Black & Strub, 1994). An
expressive language, the clinician will want to evaluation of a client's reading and writing skills
evaluate spontaneous speech and verbal fluency could also be included in an evaluation of
by asking the client open-ended questions language and communication (Black & Strub,
(Black & Strub, 1994). While the client is 1994). The client can be asked to read sentences
responding, the clinician can listen carefully for of increasing difficulty, spell words to dictation,
abnormal articulation, dysarthria (incoordina- and compose a paragraph in response to a
tion of the speech apparatus), verbal apraxia prompt (e.g., ªTell me how to change a tire.º)
(difficulty carrying out purposeful speech), (Black & Strub, 1994).
dysfluency, loss of prosody (melodic intona- In addition to the informal methods, several
tion), and disturbances of syntax or paraphasic instruments are available that can prove useful
errors (production of unintended syllables, for the clinical evaluation of language. There are
words, or phrases) (Black & Strub, 1994; Kolb a number of aphasia tests and batteries (e.g.,
& Whishaw, 1990). Another important compo- Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, Good-
nent of language, pragmatics, can also be glass & Kaplan, 1983; Multilingual Aphasia
evaluated. Pragmatics refers to the knowledge Examination, Benton & Hamsher, 1989) which
and activities of socially appropriate commu- involve lengthy, well-controlled procedures and
nication, which takes in much of the nonverbal are best left to speech pathologists who have
aspects of communication, such as gestures, more extensive training in the specialized
loudness of speech, as well as verbal appro- techniques of aphasia examinations (Lezak,
priateness (Sohlberg & Mateer, 1990). 1995). As an alternative, aphasia screening tests
Evaluation of verbal fluency can be accom- can be used to indicate the presence of an
plished by counting the number of words the aphasic disorder and may even highlight its
client is able to produce without repetition specific characteristics, but do not provide the
within a restricted category (e.g., animals or fine discriminations of the complete aphasia test
words beginning with a particular letter) and batteries. Furthermore, these screening tests do
time (e.g., 60 seconds). The average adult should not require technical knowledge of speech
Future Directions 259

pathology for adequate administration or (on the primary form only). Overall, the TOLD-
determination of whether a significant aphasic 2 instruments are reliable and valid as language
disorder is present (Lezak, 1995). One of the screening tools for younger clients (Wochnick
most comprehensive aphasic screening tests Fodness, McNeilly, & Bradley-Johnson, 1991;
available is the Revised Token Test (McNeil & Westby, 1988). Some tests have been developed
Prescott, 1978). This expanded version of the to measure more complex language usage, such
original Token Test (De Renzi & Vignolo, 1962) as the TLC (Wiig & Secord, 1989) which
contains 10 10-item subtests. McNeil and purports to measure metalinguistic abilities.
Prescott (1978) sought to ameliorate psycho- The four subtests involve producing multiple
metric weaknesses of the original with this meanings for ambiguous sentences, recognizing
revision as well as seeking to develop an inferences on the basis of incomplete informa-
evaluative system for describing the nature tion, creating sentences given three words and a
and quantifying the degree of language deficit in context, and recognizing the meaning of
order to facilitate treatment planning. Using figurative language. This type of test may be
tokens of various shapes and sizes, the clinician useful for identifying subtle problems in
gives the client a series of increasingly complex language usage (Crosson, 1996).
instructions to follow. Though simple to
administer, this instrument is reportedly very
sensitive to disrupted linguistic processes that 4.09.4.4.1 Communication/language:
are central to the aphasic disability (Lezak, implications for intervention
1995). For clients who are experiencing difficulty
Clinicians wanting a basic measure of with either or both receptive and expressive
different aspects of communication and lan- language, the clinician can modify verbal
guage may wish to consider using the Peabody interaction by shortening the length of informa-
Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R; tion presented or presenting information in
Dunn & Dunn, 1981), the Test of Language steps. Additional ideas for the school-age client
Development (TOLD-2; Hammill & Newcomer, might include recommending that the teacher
1988), and the Clinical Evaluation of Language repeat directions and have the student also
Fundamentals-Revised (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, repeat and explain the directions back to the
1987) or the Test of Language Competence teacher, pairing verbal instructions with non-
(TLC; Wiig & Secord, 1989). Unfortunately, verbal cues, and using nonverbal cues. Many of
most of these tests are normed exclusively on these strategies could be adapted to adults in
children and adolescents and, therefore, have rehabilitation and other types of therapeutic
limited application to adults. One of the most settings as well. The clinician must be careful to
frequently used tests, and one which has adult check frequently with clients to ensure under-
norms, is the PPVT-R. This test measures standing and to assist the clients and their
receptive vocabulary only and was normed for families to adjust to these communication or
individuals aged two and a half to adulthood. language deficits.
The PPVT-R is untimed and requires the
examinee to select from each plate of four
pictures the one that best represents the target 4.09.5 FUTURE DIRECTIONS
word. The test requires no reading ability, nor is
the ability to point or provide an oral response Although knowledge about how individuals
essential (Shea, 1989). The PPVT-R can help to process information has grown exponentially
establish the level of verbal understanding a since the late 1970s, researchers and practi-
client has when expressive language is not tioners alike are left with many questions
required. Comparing such receptive skills with regarding how individuals remember and learn.
those expressive skills needed for other tests How do age, gender, and ethnicity impact a
may help in developing hypotheses about the client's functioning on these specific instru-
qualitative nature of verbal performance and in ments? Do individuals with brain damage
framing potential treatment (D'Amato, Roth- process information differently than individuals
lisberg, & Rhodes, 1997). with ªnormalº brain functioning? How do the
The TOLD-2 is available in a form designed results of an assessment translate into effective
for primary ages (4±0 to 8±11) and intermediate treatment strategies that will help individuals
ages (8±6 to 12±11). This test purports to function better on the job? Despite these
measure receptive and expressive language questions and more, as a field we do know that
proficiency. The results for the TOLD-2 provide current measures of processing can allow
quotients for an Overall Spoken Language practitioners to make predictions with a reason-
score, and for the composites of Listening, able level of confidence. However, we must also
Speaking, Semantics, Syntax, and Phonology recognize that future research investigating the
260 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

prediction accuracy of various tests is needed to In order to generate an accurate diagnosis or


expand the range and precision of clinical provide the most sound recommendations, a
prediction (Long, 1996). Indeed, limited re- complete understanding of the client is neces-
search exists that evaluates the effect of various sary. A practitioner could spend hours evaluat-
treatment approaches on success in clinical ing each of the areas outlined with careful
pediatric or adult populations (Batchelor, consideration of all subdomains using a multi-
1996b; Ris & Noll, 1994). As our knowledge dimensional approach. Although this approach
of the nervous system is expanded, and we begin would yield a bounty of information, it may not
to understand the intricacies of the brain's be practical given time limitations and insurance
organization, we can begin to see how percep- policy guidelines. The key for the clinician is to
tions are formed, information stored and find a balance between finding out the most
integrated, and action taken. Until that time, important information about client functioning
the explanation for behavior and certain through the use of instruments with the best
learning difficulties can only be inferred predictive ability and spending a limited amount
(D'Amato, Rothlisberg, & Leu, in press). of time on assessment. By creating an efficient
Another component complicating our en- and effective assessment approach, more time is
hanced understanding of information proces- available to implement treatment. The task of
sing is the need for common language and goals generating recommendations for interventions
between neurologists, psychologists, educa- that are likely to enhance client functioning is of
tional researchers, and vocational rehabilitation central importance to the issue of assessment.
specialists. Bigler (1996) notes that it is To this end, continued information is needed on
the relationship between assessed cognitive
essential that physicians and psychologists work processing and predicted future performance
toward some common understanding of normal in real-world settings (Sbordone & Long, 1996).
and abnormal behavioral manifestations of brain Furthermore, we must gain knowledge about
functioning, particularly aspects of complex atten- the most effective intervention strategies for all
tion and integration of sensory experiences, mem- types of individuals. Future research will enable
ory, motivation, organization of verbal and
nonverbal cognition, abstract thinking, problem
us to understand how the brain processes
solving, executive functions, and self-monitoring information and what treatments are effective
of behavior. Without agreement on a detailed and with what types of clients. Indeed, this addi-
relatively comprehensive model of neurobehavior- tional knowledge may allow us to match client
al development, psychologists will be limited in subtypes with specific treatments which will
their ability to develop appropriate assessment and increase the effectiveness and efficacy of
intervention strategies. (p. 50) psychological services.

Unfortunately, outcomes in rehabilitation


research have also suffered from a lack of 4.09.6 SUMMARY
consensus with importance placed on different
variables depending on the orientation of the The assessment of children, adolescents, and
author and audience (Batchelor, 1996b). For adults encompasses a wide range of domains
example, clinical researchers have examined from which a clinician may view a client's
pre- and postperformance measures of cogni- functioning. Consideration must be given to the
tive, motivational, and behavioral functions many layers of the client context (e.g., family
(Ris & Noll, 1994), while service providers have support, socioeconomic status, domain of
focused on outcome constructs such as employ- functioning), client characteristics (e.g., motiva-
ment and independent living (e.g., Adunsky, tion, education level, ethnicity, age), as well as
Hershkowitz, Rabbi, Asher-Sivron, & Ohry, the specific cognitive processes under question
1992). Concurrently, third-party payers are (e.g., memory, sensory perception). Although
interested in length of stay, cost, and effective- often overlooked or subsumed within the
ness in allocation of resources (Fratalli, 1993), broader arenas of intelligence or achievement,
while families and consumers are interested in the specific areas of memory, learning, and
quality of care. Batchelor (1996b) concluded special aptitudes are critical to our daily
that the majority of research has emphasized functioning. That is, one will have difficulty
short-term outcomes and the meaningful ques- demonstrating intelligence or learning new
tions generated by service providers, consu- tasks, if there is a severe memory deficit or
mers, and third-party payers have been difficulty in accurately perceiving stimuli. The
overlooked and present an important direction descriptions of the domains presented in this
for the field to pursue. chapter have offered insight into the field's
In terms of the practical aspects of assess- current understanding of these systems as well
ment, the practitioner's task is not an easy one. as the breadth of evaluation strategies available
References 261

to probe the diverse nature of cognitive search Monograph, No. 3. New York: American
processes. Once the practitioner identifies the Orthopsychiatric Association.
Benton, A. L., & Hamsher, K. deS. (1989). Multilingual
assessment needs of the individual client, and a Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.
decision is made as to the components most Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. deS., Varney, N. R., & Spreen,
relevant for exploring the referral question, the O. (1983). Contributions to neuropsychological assess-
process can begin. By generating quality data, ment. New York: Oxford University Press.
Benton-Sivan, A. (1992). The Revised Visual Retention Test
our ability to predict outcomes and provide (5th ed.). New York: The Psychological Corporation.
effective potential intervention strategies is Berk, R. A. (1995). Review of the Memory Assessment
increased. Indeed, the goal of any assessment Scale. In J. C. Conoley & J. C. Impara (Eds). The twelfth
is to respond correctly to the question presented mental measurement yearbook (pp. 593±594). Lincoln,
by the referral source, provide accurate predic- NE: Buros.
Bigler, E. D. (1996). Bridging the gap between psychology
tions of future outcomes, and generate effective and neurology: Future trends in pediatric neuropsychol-
strategies for improving the client's adaptation ogy. In E. S. Bachelor & R. S. Dean (Eds.), Pediatric
or functioning. neuropsychology: Interfacing assessment and treatment
for rehabilitation (pp. 27±54). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Black, F. W., & Strub, R. L. (1994). The bedside and office
mental status examination. In S. Touyz, D. Byrne, & A.
4.09.7 REFERENCES Gilandas (Eds.), Neuropsychology in clinical practice
Adunsky, A., Hershkowitz, M., Rabbi, R., Asher-Sivron, (pp. 38±60) Boston: Academic Press.
L., & Ohry, A. (1992). Functional recovery in young Brand, H. J. (1991). Correlation for scores on revised tests
stroke patients. Archives of Physical Medicine and of visual-motor integration and copying test in a South
Rehabilitation, 73, 859±862. African sample. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 73,
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and 225±226.
statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washing- Brown, R. T., Armstrong, F. D., & Eckman, J. R. (1993).
ton DC: Author. Neurocognitive aspects of pediatric sickle cell disease.
Anastasi, A. (1988). Psychological testing (6th ed.). New Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 33±45.
York: Macmillan. Brown, V. L., Hammill, D. D., & Wiederholt, J. L. (1995).
Arter, J. A., & Jenkins, J. R. (1977). Examining the benefits Test of Reading Comprehension-3 (TORC-3). Austin,
and prevalence of modality considerations in special TX: PRO-ED.
education. The Journal of Special Education, 11, Bruininks, R. H. (1978). Bruininks±Oseretsky Test of
291±298. Motor Proficiency. Circle Pines, MN: American Gui-
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK: dance Service.
Oxford University Press. Campbell, J. W., D'Amato, R. C., Raggio, D. J., &
Baddeley, A., Emslie, H., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994). Stephens, K. D. (1991). Construct validity of the
Doors and People: A Test of Visual and Verbal Recall and computerized Continuous Performance Test with mea-
Recognition. Suffolk, UK: Thames Valley Test Co. sures of intelligence, achievement, and behavior. Journal
Baldwin, J. D., & Baldwin, J. I. (1986). Behavior principles of School Psychology, 29, 143±150.
in everyday life (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice- Campione, J. C., & Brown, A. L. (1987). Linking dynamic
Hall. assessment with school achievement. In C. S. Lidz (Ed.),
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Dynamic assessment: Foundations and fundamentals
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. (pp. 82±115). New York: Guilford.
Barkley, R. A. (1996). Critical issues in research on Cantor, J., Engle, R. W., & Hamilton, G. (1991). Short-
attention. In G. R. Lyon & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), term memory, working memory, and verbal abilities:
Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 45±56). How do they relate? Intelligence, 15, 229±246.
Baltimore: Brookes. Cohen, S. B. (1991). Adapting educational programs for
Batchelor, E. S. (1996a). Neuropsychological assessment of students with head injuries. Journal of Head Trauma
children. In E. S. Bachelor & R. S. Dean (Eds.), Pediatric Rehabilitation, 1, 56±63.
neuropsychology: Interfacing assessment and treatment Colarusso, R. P., & Hammill, D. D. (1996). Motor-Free
for rehabilitation (pp. 9±26). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Visual Perception Test-Revised (MFPT-R). Novato, CA:
Batchelor, E. S. (1996b). Future considerations for Academic Therapy.
rehabilitation research and outcome studies. In E. S. Cole E., & Siegel, J. A. (1990). School psychology in a
Bachelor & R. S. Dean (Eds.), Pediatric neuropsychol- multicultural community: Responding to childrens'
ogy: Interfacing assessment and treatment for rehabilita- needs. In E. Cole & J. A. Siegel (Eds.), Effective
tion (pp. 347±352). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. consultation in school psychology (pp. 141±169). Toronto,
Beery, K. E. (1989). Developmental Test of Visual-Motor ON: Hogrefe & Huber.
Integration. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Colvin, S. S. (1921). Intelligence and its measurement: A
Resources. symposium (IV). Journal of Educational Psychology, 12,
Begali, V. (1992). Head injury in children and adolescents: A 136±139.
resource and review for school and allied professionals Connolly, A. J. (1988). Keymath-revised: A diagnostic
(2nd ed.). Brandon, VT: Clinical Psychology Publishing inventory of essential mathematics. Circle Pines, MN:
Company. American Guidance Service.
Begali, V. (1994). The role of the school psychologist. In R. Cooley, E. L., & Morris, R. D. (1990). Attention in
C. Savage & G. F. Wolcott (Eds.), Educational dimen- children: A neuropsychology based model of assessment.
sions of acquired brain injury (pp. 453±473). Austin, TX: Developmental Neuropsychology, 6, 239±274.
PRO-ED. Cronbach, L. J., & Snow, R. E. (1977). Aptitudes and
Bemporad, B., & Kinsbourne, M. (1983). Sinistrality and instructional methods. A handbook for research on
dylexia: A possible relationship between subtypes. Topics interactions. New York: Irvington.
in Learning and Learning Disabilities, 3(1), 48±65. Crosson, B. (1996). Assessment of subtle language deficits
Bender, L. (1938). A visual motor gestalt test and its in neuropsychological batteries: Strategies and implica-
clinical use. American Orthopsychiatric Association Re- tions. In R. J. Sbordone & C. J. Long (Eds.), Ecological
262 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

validity of neuropsychological testing (pp. 243±259). difficulties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
Delray Beach, FL: GR Press/St Lucie Press. ogy, 49, 227±235.
D'Amato, R. C. (1990). A neuropsychological approach to Dearborn, W. F. (1921). Intelligence and its measurement:
school psychology. School Psychology Quarterly, 5, A symposium (XII). Journal of Educational Psychology,
141±160. 12, 210±212.
D'Amato, R. C., & Dean, R. S. (Eds.) (1989a). The school De Renzi, E., & Vignolo, L. A. (1962). The Token Test: A
psychologist in nontraditional settings: Integrating clients, sensitive test to detect disturbances in aphasics. Brain, 85,
services, and settings. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 665±678.
D'Amato, R. C., & Dean, R. S. (1989b). The past, present, Drew, R. H., & Templer, D. I. (1992). Contact sports. In D.
and future of school psychology in nontraditional I. Templer, L. C. Hartlage, & W. G. Cannon (Eds.),
settings. In R. C. D'Amato & R. S. Dean (Eds.), The Preventable brain damage: Brain vulnerability and health
school psychologist in nontraditional settings: Integrating (pp. 15±29). New York: Springer.
clients, services, and settings (pp. 185±209). Hillsdale, NJ: Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture
Erlbaum. Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American
D'Amato, R. C., & Rothlisberg, B. A. (1992). Psychological Guidance Service.
perspectives on intervention: A case study approach to Eliason, M. J., & Richman, L. C. (1987). The Continuous
prescriptions for change. New York: Longman. Performance Test in learning disabled and nondisabled
D'Amato, R. C., & Rothlisberg, B. A. (1996). How children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20, 614±619.
education should respond to students with traumatic Feuerstein, R., Rand, Y., & Hoffman, M. (1979). The
brain injuries. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, dynamic assessment of retarded performers: The Learning
670±683. Potential Assessment Device: Theory, instruments, and
D'Amato, R. C., Rothlisberg, B. A., & Leu, P. W. (in techniques. Baltimore: University Park.
press). Neuropsychological assessment for intervention. Figueroa, R. A., & Garcia, E. (1994). Issues in testing
In C. R. Reynolds & T. B. Gutkin (Eds.), The handbook students from culturally and linguistically diverse back-
of school psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Wiley. grounds. Multicultural Education, 2, 10±19.
D'Amato, R. C., Rothlisberg, B. A., & Rhodes, R. L. Frattali, C. M. (1993). Perspectives on functional assess-
(1997). Utilizing a neuropsychological paradigm for ment: Its use for policy making. Disability and Rehabi-
understanding common educational and psychological litation, 15, 1±9.
tests. In C. R. Reynolds & E. Fletcher-Janzen (Eds.), Frey, P. D., & Pinelli, B. (1991). Visual discrimination and
Handbook of clinical child neuropsychology (2nd ed.). visuomotor integration among two classes of Brazilian
New York: Plenum. children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72, 847±850.
Dana, R. H. (1993). Multicultural assessment perspectives Fuchs, L. S. (1994). Integrating curriculum-based measure-
for professional psychology. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. ment with instructional planning for students with
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual learning disabilities. In N. C. Jordan & J. Goldsmith-
differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Phillips (Eds.), Learning disabilities: New directions for
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450±466. assessment and intervention (pp. 177±195). Boston: Allyn
Das, J. P., Kirby, J., & Jarman, R. F. (1979). Simultaneous & Bacon.
and successive cognitive processes. New York: Academic Gaddes, W. H., & Edgell, D. (1994). Learning disabilities
Press. and brain function: A neuropsychological approach (3rd
Das, J. P., Naglieri, J. A., & Kirby, J. R. (1994). Assessment ed.). New York: Springer-Verlag.
of cognitive processes. The PASS theory of intelligence Geil, M., & D'Amato, R. C. (1996). Contemporary
New York: Allyn & Bacon. ecological neuropsychology: An alternative to the medical
Dean, R. S. (1977). Canonical analysis of a jangle fallacy. model for conceptualizing learning disabilities. Manu-
Multivariate Experimental Clinical Research, 3, 17±20. script submitted for publication.
Dean, R. S. (1983). Intelligence-achievement discrepancies Golden, C. J. (1981). The Luria±Nebraska Children's
in diagnosing pediatric learning disabilities. Clinical Battery: Theory and formulation. In G. W. Hynd & J.
Neuropsychology, 3, 58±62. E. Obrzut (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment and the
Dean, R. S. (1984). Functional lateralization of the brain. school-aged child: Issues and procedures (pp. 277±302).
Journal of Special Education, 18, 239±256. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Dean, R. S. (1985a). Neuropsychological assessment. In Golden, C. J., Sawicki, R. F., & Franzen, M. D. (1984).
R. Michels, J. O. Cavenar, H. K. H. Brodie, A. M. Test construction. In G. Goldstein & M. Hersen (Eds.),
Cooper, S. B. Guze, L. L. Judd, G. L. Klerman, & A. J. Handbook of psychological assessment (pp. 19±37). New
Solnit (Eds.), Psychiatry (pp. 1±16). Philadelphia: York: Pergamon.
Lippincott. Goldstein, D. J., Smith, K. B., & Waldrep, E. E. (1986).
Dean, R. S. (1985b). Foundation and rationale for Factor analytic study of the Kaufman Assessment
neuropsychological bases of individual differences. In Battery for Children. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
L. C. Hartlage & C. F. Telzrow (Eds.), The neuropsy- 42, 890±894.
chology of individual differences: A developmental per- Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). Boston Diagnostic
spective (pp. 7±39). New York: Plenum. Aphasia Examination (BDAE). Philadelphia: Lea and
Dean, R. S. (1986). Perspectives on the future of Febiger. Distributed by Psychological Assessment Re-
neuropsychological assessment. In B. S. Plake & J. C. sources, Odessa, FL.
Witt (Eds.), Buros-Nebraska series on measurement and Gray, J. W., & Dean, R. S. (1989). Approaches to the
testing: Future of testing and measurement (pp. 203±241). cognitive rehabilitation of children with neuropsycholo-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. gical impairment. In C. R. Reynolds & F. Fletcher-
Dean, R. S. (1988). Lateral Preference Schedule. Odessa, Janzen (Eds.), Handbook of clinical child neuropsychology
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. (pp. 397±408). New York: Plenum.
Dean, R. S., & Gray, J. W. (1990). Traditional approaches Greenberg, L. (1993). Test of variables of attention
to neuropsychological assessment. In C. R. Reynolds & (T.O.V.A.TM). Wood Dale, IL: Stoetling.
R. W. Kamphaus (Eds.), Handbook of psychological and Greenberg, L. M., & Waldman, I. D. (1993). Develop-
educational assessment of children: Intelligence and mental normative data on the test of variables of
achievement (pp. 371±388). New York: Guilford Press. attention (T.O.V.A.TM). Journal of Child Psychology
Dean, R. S., Schwartz, N. H., & Smith, L. S. (1981). and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 34, 1019±1030.
Lateral preference patterns as a discriminator of learning Guilmette, T. J., & Giuliano, A. J. (1991). Taking the
References 263

stand: Issues and strategies in forensic neuropsychology. is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelli-
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, 197±219. gence, 14, 389±433.
Gutkin, T. B., & Reynolds, C. R. (Eds.) (1990). The LaBerge, D. (1995). Attentional processing: The brain's art
handbook of school psychology (2nd ed.). New York: of mindfulness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Wiley. Press.
Halperin, J. M., Sharma, V., Greenblatt, E., & Schwartz, S. Lassiter, K. S., D'Amato, R. C., Raggio, D. J., Whitten, J.
(1991). Assessment of the Continuous Performance Test: C. M., & Bardos, A. N. (1994). The construct specificity
Reliability and validity in a nonreferred sample. Psy- of the Continuous Performance Test: Does inattention
chological Assessment, 3, 603±608. relate to behavior and achievement? Developmental
Hammill, D. D. (1991). Detroit Tests of Learning Aptitude Neuropsychology, 10, 179±188.
(DTLA-3) (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment (2nd
Hammill, D. D., & Bryant, B. R. (1991a). Detroit Tests of ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Learning Aptitude-Adult (DTLA-A). Austin, TX: PRO- Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd
ED. ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hammill, D. D., & Bryant, B. R. (1991b). Detroit Tests of Lindgren, S. D., & Lyon, D. (1983). PACE: Pediatric
Learning Aptitude-Primary (DTLA-P:2) (2nd ed.). assessment of cognitive efficiency. Iowa City, IA: Uni-
Austin, TX: PRO-ED. versity of Iowa, Department of Pediatrics.
Hammill, D. D., & Larsen, S. C. (1996). Test of Written Long, C. J. (1996). Neuropsychological tests: A look at our
Language-3 (TOWL-3). Austin, TX: PRO-ED. past and the impact that ecological issues may have on
Hammill, D. D., & Newcomer, P. L. (1988). Test of our future. In R. J. Sbordone & C. J. Long (Eds.),
Language Development Intermediate (TOLD-2) (2nd Ecological validity of neuropsychological testing
ed.). Austin, TX: PRO-ED. (pp. 1±14). Delray Beach, FL: GR Press/St Lucie Press.
Hamsher, K. de S. (1984). Specialized neuropsychological Luria, A. R. (1970). The functional organization of the
assessment methods. In G. Goldstein & M. Hersen brain. Scientific American, 222(3), 66±78.
(Eds.). Handbook of psychological assessment Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain: An introduction to
(pp. 235±256). New York: Pergamon. neuropsychology. New York: Basic Books.
Hartlage, L. C., & Golden, C. J. (1990). Neuropsycholo- Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man (2nd
gical assessment techniques. In T. B. Gutkin & C. R. ed.). New York: Basic Books.
Reynolds (Eds.), The handbook of school psychology (2nd Markwardt, (1989). Peabody Individual Achievement Test-
ed., pp. 431±457). New York: Wiley. Revised (PIAT-R). Circle Pines, MN: American Gui-
Hartlage, L. C., & Telzrow, C. F. (1983). The neuropsy- dance Service.
chological basis of educational intervention. Journal of Martinez, M. A. (1985). Toward a bilingual school
Learning Disabilities, 16, 521±528. psychology model. Educational Psychology, 20, 143±152.
Hooper, S. R. (1995). Review of the Visual Search and Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (1989). Constructing
Attention Test. In J. C. Conoley & J. C. Impara (Eds.), more meaningful relationships: Mnemonic instruction
The twelfth mental measurements yearbook for special populations. Educational Psychology Review,
(pp. 1081±1082). Lincoln, NE: Buros. 1, 83±111.
Huebner, E. S. (1992). Review of the Wechsler Memory McNeil, M. M., & Prescott, T. E. (1978). Revised Token
Scale-Revised. In J. J. Kramer & J. C. Conoley (Eds.), Test. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
The eleventh mental measurement yearbook Morris, R. D. (1996). Relationships and distinctions
(pp. 1023±1024). Lincoln, NE: Buros. among the concepts of attention, memory, and executive
Hynd, G. W., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (1990). Neuropsy- function: A developmental perspective. In G. R. Lyon &
chological assessment. In A. S. Kaufman (Ed.), Assessing N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Attention, memory, and execu-
adolescent and adult intelligence (pp. 638±695). Boston: tive function (pp. 11±16). Baltimore: Brookes.
Allyn & Bacon. Palincsar, A., Brown, A. L., & Campione, J. C. (1991).
Hynd, G. W., & Willis, W. G. (1988). Pediatric neuropsy- Dynamic assessment. In H. L. Swanson (Ed.), Handbook
chology, Boston: Allyn & Bacon. on the assessment of learning disabilities: Theory,
Jarvis, P. E., & Barth, J. T. (1994). The Halstead-Reitan research, and practice (pp. 75±95). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Neuropsychological Battery: A guide to interpretation and Palisano, R. J., & Dichter, C. G. (1989). Comparison of
clinical applications. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assess- two tests of visual-motor development used to assess
ment Resources. children with learning disabilities. Perceptual and Motor
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory Skills, 68, 1099±1103.
of comprehension: Individual differences in working Poteat, G. M. (1995). Review of the Detroit Tests of
memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122±149. Learning Aptitude, Third Edition. In J. C. Conoley & J.
Kaufman, A. S. (1990). Assessing adolescent and adult C. Impara (Eds.), The twelfth mental measurement
intelligence. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. yearbook (pp. 277±278). Lincoln, NE: Buros.
Kaufman, A. S. (1994). Intelligent testing with the WISC- Pressley, M., & Levin, J. R. (Eds.) (1983). Cognitive
III. New York: Wiley. strategy research: Psychological foundations. New York:
Kavale, K. A., Forness, R. F., & Bender, M. (1988). Springer-Verlag.
Handbook of learning disabilities: Volume II: Methods Raggio, D. (1991). Raggio Evaluation of Attention Deficit
and interventions. Boston: College-Hill. Disorder (Computerized test). Jackson, MS: University
Klee, S. H., & Garfinkel, B. D. (1983). The computerized of Mississippi Medical Center, Infant and Child Devel-
Continuous Performance Task: A new measure of opment Clinic.
inattention. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11, Reitan, R. M., & Davison, L. A. (1974). Clinical
489±495. neuropsychology: Current status and applications. New
Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (1990). Fundamentals of human York: Winston/Wiley.
neuropsychology (3rd ed.). New York: Freeman. Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead±Reitan
Kovacs, M., Goldston, D., & Ivengar, S. (1992). Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and clinical
Intellectual development and academic performance of interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.
children with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus: A Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1993). The Halstead±Reitan
longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and clinical
676±684. interpretation (2nd ed.). Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology
Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability Press.
264 Assessment of Memory, Learning, and Special Aptitudes

Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1995, October). Cognitive Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America: Learn-
and emotional consequences of mild head injury. Paper ing Disabilities, 2, 181±192.
presented at the fall conference of the Colorado Simpson, N., Black, F. W., & Strub, R. L. (1986). Memory
Neuropsychological Society, Colorado Springs, CO. assessment using the Strub-Black mental status exam-
Resnick, L. B. (Ed.) (1976). The nature of intelligence. ination and the Wechsler Memory Scale. Journal of
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Clinical Psychology, 42, 147±155.
Reynolds, C. R. (1981a). The neuropsychological basis of Slomka, G. T., & Tarter, R. E. (1993). Neuropsychological
intelligence. In G. W. Hynd & J. E. Obrzut (Eds.), assessment. In T. H. Ollendick & M. Hersen (Eds.),
Neuropsychological assessment and the school-aged child: Handbook of child and adolescent assessment
Issues and procedures (pp. 87±124). New York: Grune & (pp. 208±223). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Stratton. Sohlberg, M. M., & Mateer, C. A. (1990). Evaluation and
Reynolds, C. R. (1981b). Neuropsychological assessment treatment of communicative skills. In J. S. Kreutzer & P.
and the habilitation of learning: Considerations in the Wehman (Eds.), Community integration following trau-
search for the aptitude x treatment interaction. School matic brain injury. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Psychology Review, 10, 343±349. Strub, R. L., & Black, F. W. (1993). The mental status
Reynolds, C. R. (1986). Transactional models of intellec- examination in neurology (3rd ed.). Philadelphia: F. A.
tual development, yes. Deficit models of process Davis.
remediation, no. School Psychology Review, 15, 256±260. Swanson, H. L. (1981). Vigilance deficits in learning
Reynolds, C. R., & Bigler, E. D. (1994). Test of memory and disabled children: A signal detection analysis. Journal
learning. Austin, TX: PRO-ED. of Psychology and Psychiatry, 2, 339±398.
Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (1990). Handbook Swanson, H. L. (1995). Using the Cognitive Processing Test
of psychological and educational assessment of children: to assess ability: Development of a dynamic assessment
Intelligence and achievement. New York: Guilford measure. School Psychology Review, 24, 672±693.
Press. Swanson, H. L. (1996). Swanson Cognitive Processing Test
Ris, D., & Noll, R. B. (1994). Long-term neurobehavioral (S-CPT). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
outcome in pediatric brain-tumor patients: Review and Talley, J. L. (1993). Children's Auditory Verbal Learning
methodological critique. Journal of Clinical and Experi- Test-2 (CAVLT-2). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assess-
mental Neuropsychology, 16(1), 21±42. ment Resources.
Rosvold, H., Mirsky, A., Sarason, I., Bransome, L., & Tarver, S. G., & Dawson, M. M. (1978). Modality
Beck, L. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain preference and the teaching of reading: A review. Journal
damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 343±350. of Learning Disabilities, 11, 5±17.
Rothlisberg, B. A. (1991). Factor stability of the Lateral Taylor, H. G. (1988). Learning disabilities. In E. J. Mash &
Preference Schedule. International Journal of Neu- L. G. Terdal (Eds.), Behavioral assessment of childhood
roscience, 61, 83±85. disorders (2nd ed., pp. 402±450). New York: Guilford
Rothlisberg, B. A. (1992). Integrating psychological ap- Press.
proaches to intervention. In R. C. D'Amato & B. A. Taylor, H. G., & Fletcher, J. M. (1990). Neuropsycholo-
Rothlisberg (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on inter- gical assessment of children. In G. Goldstein & M.
vention: A case study approach to prescriptions for change Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of psychological assessment
(pp. 190±198). New York: Longman. (2nd ed., pp. 228±255). New York: Pergamon.
Rovet, J. F., Ehrlich, R. M., Czuchta, D., & Akler, M. Taylor, H. G., Fletcher, J. M., & Satz, P. (1984).
(1993). Psychoeducational characteristics of children and Neuropsychological assessment in children. In G. Gold-
adolescents with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. stein & M. Hersen (Eds.). Handbook of psychological
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 7±22. assessment (pp. 211±234). New York: Pergamon.
Rovet, J. F., Ehrlich, R. M., & Hoppe, M. (1988). Specific Telzrow, C. F. (1985). The science and speculation of
intellectual deficits in children with early onset diabetes rehabilitation in developmental neuropsychological
mellitus, Child Development, 59, 226±234. disorders. In L. C. Hartlage & C. F. Telzrow (Eds.),
Sattler, J. M. (1992). Assessment of children (3rd ed., rev.). The neuropsychology of individual differences: A
San Diego, CA: Sattler. developmental perspective (pp. 271±307). New York:
Sbordone, R. J., & Long, C. J. (Eds.) (1996). Ecological Plenum.
validity of neuropsychological testing. Delray Beach, FL: Templer, D. I., & Drew, R. H. (1992). Noncontact sports.
GR Press/St Lucie Press. In D. I. Templer, L. C. Hartlage, & W. G. Cannon
Selz, M. (1981). Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test (Eds.), Preventable brain damage: Brain vulnerability and
batteries for children. In G. W. Hynd & J. E. Obrzut health (pp. 30±40). New York: Springer.
(Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment and the school- Touyz, S., Byrne, D., & Gilandas, A. (1994). Neuropsychol-
aged child: Issues and procedures (pp. 195±235). New ogy in clinical practice. Boston: Academic Press.
York: Grune & Stratton. Trenerry, M. R., Crosson, B., DeBoe, J., & Leber, W. R.
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1987). Clinical (1990). Visual search and attention test. Odessa, FL:
Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Revised (CELF- Psychological Assessment Resources.
R). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp. Walsh, K. W. (1978). Neuropsychology: A clinical approach.
Shea, V. (1989). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. New York: Churchill Livingstone.
In C. S. Newmark (Ed.), Major psychological assessment Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised man-
instruments (Vol. II, pp. 271±283). Boston: Allyn & ual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Bacon. Wedding, D., Horton, A. M., & Webster, J. S. (1986). The
Sheslow, D., & Adams, W. (1990). Wide Range Assessment neuropsychology handbook: Behavioral and clinical per-
of Memory and Learning (WRAML). Wilmington, DE: spectives. New York: Springer.
Jastak. Wepman, J. M., & Reynolds, W. M. (1987). Wepman's
Shiffrin, R. M., & Atkinson, R. C. (1969). Storage and Auditory Discrimination Test (2nd ed.) Los Angeles:
retrieval processes in long-term memory. Psychological Western Psychological Services.
Review, 76, 179±193. Westby, C. (1988). Test review: Test of Language
Shinn, M. R. (Ed.) (1989). Curriculum-based measurement: Development-2 Primary, Test of Language
Assessing special children. New York: Guilford. Development-2 Intermediate. The Reading Teacher, 42,
Silver, L. B. (1993). Introduction and overview to the 236±237.
clinical concepts of learning disabilities. Child and Whitten, J. C., D'Amato, R. C., & Chittooran, M. M.
References 265

(1992). A neuropsychological approach to intervention. Woodcock, R., & Johnson, M. B. (1989). Woodcock±
In R. C. D'Amato & B. A. Rothlisberg (Eds.), Johnson Psychoeducational Battery-Revised (WJPB-R).
Psychological perspectives on intervention: A case study Chicago: Riverside.
approach to prescriptions for change (pp. 112±136). White Woodrow, H. (1921). Intelligence and its measurement: A
Plains, NY: Longman. symposium (XI). Journal of Educational Psychology, 12,
Wiig, E. H., & Second, W. (1989). Test of Language 207±210.
Competence-Expanded Edition (TLC). San Antonio, Ylvisaker, M., Szekeres, S. F., Haarbauer-Krupa, J.,
TX: The Psychological Corporation. Urbanczyk, B., & Feeney, T. J. (1994). Speech and
Williams, J. M. (1991). Memory Assessment Scales (MAS). language intervention. In R. C. Savage & G. F. Wolcott
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. (Eds.), Educational dimensions of acquired brain injury
Williams, R. E., & Vincent, K. R. (1991). Review of the (pp. 185±235). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Peabody Individual Achievement Test-Revised. In D. J. Ylvisaker, M., Szekeres, S. F., & Hartwick, P. (1994). A
Keyser & R. C. Sweetland (Eds.), Test critiques (Vol. 8, framework for cognitive intervention. In R. C. Savage
pp. 557±562). Kansas City, MO: Test Corporation of & G. F. Wolcott (Eds.), Educational dimensions of
America. acquired brain injury (pp. 35±67). Austin, TX: PRO-
Wochnick Fodness, R., McNeilly, J., & Bradley-Johnson, ED.
S. (1991). Test±retest reliability of the Test of Language Youngjohn, J. R., Larrabee, G. J., & Crook, T. H. (1993).
Development-2: Primary and Test of Language New adult age- and education-correction norms for the
Development-2: Intermediate. Journal of School Psychol- Benton Visual Retention Test. The Clinical Neuropsy-
ogy, 29, 161±165. chologist, 7, 155±160.

You might also like