You are on page 1of 9

Paper ID #24251

2018 CoNECD - The Collaborative Network for Engineering and Computing


Diversity Conference: Crystal City, Virginia Apr 29
A Review of Bias in Peer Assessment
Jacklin Hope Stonewall, Iowa State University

Jacklin Stonewall is a Ph.D. student in the Departments of Industrial and Manufacturing Systems Engi-
neering and Human Computer Interaction at Iowa State University. Her research interests include: gender
HCI, decision support systems, sustainability, and the creation of equitable cities and classrooms.
Prof. Michael Dorneich, Iowa State University
Dr. Michael C. Dorneich is an Associate Professor at Iowa State University in the Industrial and Man-
ufacturing Systems Engineering Department. He graduated in 1999 from the University of Illinois at
Urbana-Champaign with a PhD. in Industrial Engineering in the Human Factors Program. His research
interests focus on human factors, human-computer interaction, and adaptive systems that enable people
to be effective in the complex and often stressful environments found in aviation, military, robotic, and
space applications. His teaching methods include team projects and the application of team-based learning
methods into the classroom.
Cassandra Dorius
Jane Rongerude PhD, Department of Community and Regional Planning, Iowa State University
Jane Rongerude is an assistant professor in the Department of Community and Regional Planning at
Iowa State University, where she teaches courses in planning theory, community and urban revitalization,
housing, and neighborhood planning. Her research interests include the potential of planning as a tool
to interrupt local system of poverty management and pedagogical innovations related to Team Based
Learning, specifically how to integrate diverse voices within communities and classrooms. She earned
her MCP and PhD in city and regional planning from the University of California at Berkeley.

American
c Society for Engineering Education, 2018
A Review of Bias in Peer Assessment
Jacklin Stonewall1, Michael Dorneich1, PhD, Jane Rongerude2, PhD, Cassandra Dorius3, PhD
1
Industrial and Manufacturing Systems Engineering, Iowa State University
2
Community and Regional Planning, Iowa State University
3
Human Development and Family Studies, Iowa State University

The use of teams and team-centric pedagogies such as Team Based Learning (TBL) in classrooms has been
shown to increase engagement and lead to better overall learning outcomes. Active learning pedagogies
such as TBL are also promoted as promising strategies for engaging underrepresented students. For many
instructors, especially those using TBL, peer assessments are integral to the classroom environment as tools
for both monitoring team performance and ensuring accountability. However, concerns have developed
regarding the fairness of peer assessments due to student biases. Research on TBL classrooms finds that
women and students of color do not have the same experiences as their white male counterparts.
Additionally, bias has been observed in peer assessment scores with respect to race, gender, and
socioeconomic status. As more instructors recognize the benefits of teams and integrate them into their
classes, the use of peer assessments also increases, highlighting the need for a fair peer assessment process.
Through literature review, this work will present an initial description of the issues involved and identify
the extent to which bias has been observed to affect peer assessment scores. This is part of a larger project
that aims to use this information to design and evaluate fairer peer assessment processes.

INTRODUCTION learners to engage more fully in the class [9] and increasing
interactions among students and between students and
Biases, both implicit and explicit, negatively impact the instructors [10]. Assessments can foster the development of
way people perceive members of disadvantaged groups. autonomy and maturity, as well as improve social and
Unfortunately, biases often extend into the classroom professional skills [11]. The process also encourages self-
environment [1], [2]. In one academic year, 38% of professors reflection and deeper understanding of the material, which
surveyed perceived an act of bias in their classes [2]. In small may lead to improved retention and confidence [12]. Given
group learning classrooms, these biases have been shown to the increasing prevalence of small group learning and a
manifest in many ways, including in peer assessments [3]. growing understanding of the benefits of peer assessments,
The employment of small group learning strategies (such these evaluations have become a focus of considerable
as cooperative learning or Team Based Learning) in classroom amounts of research including examinations of student
environments has been shown to increase student perceptions [13] and implementation strategies [14].
achievement, attendance, engagement, and lead to better However, both students and instructors have expressed
overall learning outcomes [4], [5], [6]. Because of these concerns about the fairness of teams and their associated peer
outcomes, team-based pedagogies and cooperative learning assessments, especially due to bias [15], [16], [17], [18].
practices have been incorporated on college campuses as a Research has shown that the experiences of women and
strategy to improve the classroom engagement of students of color in these classrooms differ from those of their
underrepresented students. Indeed, research shows that peers in terms of assessment [3]. Additionally, it is already
learning in teams positively affects objective outcomes (such understood that biased behaviors are commonly present in
as exam scores) for minority students [7], [8]. In many group higher education classrooms [2]. Because of these concerns,
learning classrooms, peer assessments are integral. interest in creating fairer peer assessments has increased. This
One of the most widely used team-centric pedagogies is review of the existing literature regarding bias in peer
team-based learning (TBL). The prevalence of TBL in the assessment is a part of a larger project undertaken to design a
United States has been steadily rising, especially in the fairer peer assessment process. The next section of this paper
medical field [6]. In TBL classrooms, permanent teams are will include a general discussion of bias in the classroom
formed to maximize heterogeneity. TBL functions on four focused on definitions, manifestations, and measurement
essential principles: 1. Properly formed teams that remain techniques. The following section will present evidence of
together for the duration of the term, 2. Readiness assurance bias in peer assessments and selected strategies for mitigating
assessments of individual’s and team’s pre-class preparation, bias in both peer assessment and the general classroom.
3. In class, team application exercises that promote learning of
material and team development, and 4. Peer assessments
designed to monitor team performance, hold individuals TYPES OF BIAS IN THE CLASSROOM
accountable for their effort and contribution, and serve to
improve overall team functioning [5]. Outside of specific Biased actions may result from the implicit and explicit
pedagogies and classroom structures, peer assessments biased attitudes of an individual. These attitudes create an
themselves have been shown to have far-reaching benefits. individual’s subjective organizational structure for how they
These assessments have been credited with empowering perceive their environment [19]. Although biased attitudes are
deeply engrained, they tend to be inconsistently expressed, measures association of target concepts with an attribute [24].
depending on the social context. Explicit attitudes in particular Two concepts (such as Black vs. White) appear in a first task
are often moderated or “censored” in sensitive social followed by two attributes (such as pleasant vs. unpleasant) in
situations [20]. a second task. When the combination of concept + attribute is
In the classroom, instructors perceive incidents of implicit highly associated, participant’s categorization of stimuli items
and explicit bias as occurring at similar frequencies [2]. will be quicker than when the combination of concept +
Additionally, women and younger faculty members have been attribute is less associated. The difference in performance
shown to be more likely to detect and report biased incidents measures among the different combinations of items measures
[2]. Classroom bias tends to target individuals’ sexual the implicit association of the concept with the attribute [24].
orientation, race, sex, and ethnicity [21]. The IAT has also facilitated an understanding of the
pervasiveness of implicit bias. Over thousands of tests,
Implicit Attitudes researchers have found that 80% of Whites and 40% of Blacks
harbor pro-White bias [25].
An implicit attitude is one that is “…activated by the mere
presence (actual or symbolic) of the attitude object and
commonly function without a person’s full awareness or Explicit Attitudes
control” [19, p. 62]. In this context, an “attitude object” is the
target of the biased attitude. Due to this lack of awareness, An explicit attitude is one that is consciously held about a
implicit attitudes are inaccessible through personal person or group. These attitudes shape responses for which
introspection [22]. These attitudes shape not only the actions individuals have the opportunity to consider the social costs
an individual takes, but can influence non-verbal behaviors, and benefits of a particular action (e.g. using a homophobic
such as body language [19]. In short, it is difficult for an slur) [19], [26]. Where implicit attitudes are difficult to self-
individual to recognize and address their own implicit biases recognize, control, and measure, explicit attitudes are overt
without outside intervention. and more readily measured by traditional assessment
In the classroom, implicit bias manifests in varying ways. measures.
One study of bias in university classrooms divided
A study on the experiences of South Asian students in
occurrences into categories of microaggressions. It was found
predominantly white classrooms found that, out of the 40
that microassaults (exclusion), microinsults (subtle verbal
snubs largely unknown to the perpetrator), and students interviewed, all had experienced an incident of
microinvalidations (negating the experiences of marginalized explicit bias. Incidences of explicit bias included: openly
groups) were the most common manifestations of implicit mocking a student’s manner of dress or religious expression
biases in the surveyed classrooms [2]. A study by Edith (such as the dastaar or hijab), asking rude or inflammatory
Samuel [16] produced similar findings on the questions (such as “Did you live in a hut?”), and publicly
microaggressions experienced in the classroom. The following ridiculing students’ abilities and accents [17]. From an
examples illustrate types of language and behavior that may instructional perspective, the incidences of explicit bias most
indicate microaggressions and implicit bias: reported by professors are the explicit use of stereotypes,
 Microassault: Ignoring the contribution of a group telling offensive jokes, and using slurs [2].
member Explicit biases are generally measured through direct self-
 Microinsult: Asking a student of color “How did you report questionnaires. However, as biased attitudes have
get accepted here?” or “But where did you really come become less socially desirable, it has been suggested that this
from?” self-reporting cannot be assumed to be accurate, due to
 Microinvalidation: Proclaiming to a student of color “I respondents moderating their responses to be more socially
don’t see race” or denying any personal implicit biases
desirable (i.e. answering what they “should” say rather than
Implicit bias is also evident in unstated assumptions about what they believe). No single measurement instrument for
other students such as needlessly offering assistance or explicit bias has emerged (in contrast with the IAT for
doubting a student’s ability to complete a task [16], [2].
measuring implicit bias). Due to the lack of one predominant
Additionally, manifestation of implicit bias may be observed
measure and concerns about the accuracy and applicability of
in body language – such as an inability to make or maintain
eye contact, or physically distancing oneself from a member traditional measures [27], some researchers have created
of a disadvantaged group [23]. study-specific measures for assessing explicit bias (e.g. Pantos
Unfortunately, implicit bias is not reserved for student- & Perkins, [28] and Devine [29]). One such study-specific
student interactions. This type of bias has also been seen in the measure is the “Shoulds and Woulds” scale which measures
marks given to students by instructors. Female students have the extent to which individuals predict they would act with
been shown to receive lower class participation scores than more prejudice than they should [29]. Nonetheless, there are a
male students, despite no evidence for this disparity in other number of validated explicit bias measures in use.
aspects of the course (e.g. exam and homework scores) [17]. One of the oldest measures of explicit bias is the
The most common method of measuring implicit bias is Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSD). The full
through the use of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). version of the MCSD consists of a 33-item questionnaire that
Developed in 1998 at the University of Washington, the IAT
assesses beliefs and the extent to which the individual students consistently underestimate themselves [47], [48], [49]
moderates these beliefs to be more socially desirable [30]. while male students consistently overestimate themselves [47],
Other measures focus on attitudes toward specific groups. The [48].
widely used Modern Racism Scale (MRS) is a self-report Many researchers have proposed explanations for these
measure of the extent of an individual’s racist beliefs [31]. differences in results. Falchikov and Malkin [50] suggested
This scale measures abstract ideas (i.e. affirmative action) that they may be due to gender-based communication
rather than attitudes toward specific individuals or groups differences and socialization or the gender association of the
[32]. Similarly, the Modern Homonegativity Scale (MHS) task or class (i.e. women in a traditionally masculine topic of
measures contemporary attitudes toward lesbians and gay men study). Another explanation is the influence of ability. In
[33]. The Attitudes Toward Women (ATW) scale measures many studies where women have received overall higher peer
attitudes towards the roles of women [34]. However, this scale evaluation scores, they also had higher GPAs [43], [51]. It has
has fallen out of favor for its dated content [35]. been suggested that examining peer evaluation scores in
conjunction with GPA might clarify this effect [43]. More
BIAS IN PEER ASSESSMENT recently, the mixed results have been suggested to be affected
by cultural differences unaccounted for in analysis [18]. As
Bias in peer assessment can be directed at many different
the association of behaviors or personality traits with gender
social and demographic groups. While the biases discussed
varies by culture, this is a plausible explanation.
hitherto have focused on gender, race, and ethnicity, peer
Studies of racial bias in peer assessment have also
assessment bias has also been observed due to social style,
returned mixed results. Multiple researchers have found that
socioeconomic status, native language, and peer group
individuals tend to be rated higher by members of their own
affiliation.
race than of other races [52], [53], [54], [55]. However, other
Bias due to gender has been studied extensively.
work has demonstrated that this may not be the case. In an
However, the results have been mixed for both same sex and
extensive re-analysis of Kraiger and Ford’s [55] military data,
opposite sex ratings [36], [12]. A study of oral presentation
Black raters were shown to give higher ratings to White ratees
assessments found same sex bias where men rated other men
than to Black ratees [56]. Further analysis of these data
slightly higher, however ratings given by women were
indicated that Black recruits consistently received lower
unaffected [12]. Conversely, other researchers found that
ratings than White recruits from both Black and White raters
ratings between members of the same sex were consistently
[56]. Similarly, peer ratings in a sophomore level engineering
more prone to devaluation than when rating the opposite sex
class demonstrated that minority students received lower
[37]. Males have been found to award the highest scores to
ratings than non-minority students [57]. Finally, in some
females and females award the highest scores to males [38],
cases, no significant evidence of racial bias has been produced
[18]. Recently, though, a study of the implementation of TBL
[58], [59].
in general education classes showed that while gender bias in
the assessment scores was not observed, women did more Recently, a study of peer assessment in large general
work in team activities, suggesting that their extra work was education classes taught using TBL found convincing
going unrewarded [3]. evidence of racial bias [3]. In these classes, students of color
Results have again been mixed when analyzing the contributed the same number of answers and suggestions as
overall ratings received by male and female students. Multiple their peers. However, they received significantly lower peer
researchers have found that female students receive lower evaluation scores than White students on three out of four
ratings and fewer positive qualitative comments than their areas of assessment [3].
male peers [39], [40], [41]. However, it has also been found Other factors affecting the fairness of peer assessment
that males receive lower ratings than their female peers [42], have received less attention than gender and race. A survey of
[43], [44]. The mixed results extend to analyses of the ratings 232 undergraduates in the United States revealed that 32%
given by male and female students. Women have, in some would evaluate oral presentations by less wealthy students
cases, been found to give higher evaluation scores than men more harshly than they would evaluate more wealthy students.
[42]. Conversely, another study found men give higher This effect was particularly strong for female raters.
evaluation scores and women give lower marks [40]. Additionally, students who held more conservative attitudes
Student attitudes toward peer assessment are also were more likely to give harsher ratings to less-wealthy
inconsistent. A study of Australian undergraduates found no students [60]. Bias due to social style has been observed in
gender differences in satisfaction with the peer assessment both engineering and business classrooms with students who
process [45]. However, more recent research has shown that exhibit an “expressive” social style receiving significantly
male students report more positive attitudes about peer higher marks than students with other social styles [61], [43].
assessment than female students [10], [46]. Evidence has also suggested that peer ratings may be biased
One of the few consistent findings for gender effects by friendship [62], [12] and language similarity (native
relates to ratings given by students to themselves. Female
speakers receiving higher ratings than non-native speakers)
[12] Perspective Taking
MITIGATION Perspective taking involves imagining oneself as the
Significant research has been undertaken on bias target of a particular bias and contemplating the resulting
mitigation techniques and methods. However, for the purpose psychological experience [71]. This strategy works by
of this paper, the techniques presented will be limited to those increasing the closeness between the individual and the
most applicable to peer assessment in a team-based classroom targeted group [72]. Perspective taking has been shown to be
environment. Additionally, focus will be placed on techniques very effective in reducing implicit racial bias as measured by
specifically intended to reduce implicit bias. pre- and post-intervention IAT scores [71].
As implicit bias is inaccessible through personal
introspection, it can be difficult to mitigate. Devine et al [29] Increase Positive Contact
posit that to begin breaking the “habit of bias”, one must first Contact is one of the greatest influences on the strength of
be made aware of the bias and feel concern about how the bias biases: higher levels of self-reported contact with a group
affects others [63], [64]. Awareness of implicit bias is result in lower implicit and explicit bias against that group
generally accomplished by presenting evidence of bias via the [73], [74]. Positive contact is generated through interactions
Implicit Association Test (IAT) [65], [66]. with the stigmatized group over a period of time. While
It has been suggested that prejudice interventions yield contact over time tends to be the most effective at mitigating
the best outcomes when implemented in three stages: 1. bias, even short periods of positive contact (such as guest
Recognize the bias 2. Apply bias mitigation techniques 3. speakers from targeted groups) have been shown to be
Allow time to practice the techniques and reflect on changes effective [75].
[29], [25]. From the literature, Devine et al compiled the most Research on the impact of mitigation strategies on biased
prominent bias mitigation strategies as a “prejudice habit- peer assessments in higher education is limited. However, a
breaking intervention”. Five prominent bias mitigation study that combined standard peer assessment rater training
techniques that have been developed are stereotype with bias reduction training produced a significant reduction
replacement, counter-stereotype imaging, individuation, in bias against individuals with differing social styles [76].
perspective taking, and increasing positive contact. While many of these strategies have been employed in
classrooms, higher education instructors often try to mitigate
Stereotype Replacement biased actions in other ways. A study of university professors
This technique involves recognizing a stereotypical found that few employed specific strategies and instead relied
response (e.g. “women aren’t good at math”) and consciously on using the bias as a discussion topic, providing a rebuttal or
replacing it with a rational, non-biased response (e.g. “male counterevidence, and direct confrontation [2].
and female math scores show no difference when accounting
for math classes taken”) [67]. Stereotype replacement involves CONCLUSION
significant self-reflection where an individual tries to
Biased actions are the result of biased attitudes, which
determine why a stereotypical response occurred in order to
implicitly and explicitly shape the way an individual perceives
determine how a biased response could be avoided in the
their environment. In the classroom, implicit bias tends to
future [29], [68].
manifest as microaggressions, negative assumptions about
other students, and distancing body language, but without the
Counter-Stereotype Imaging
awareness of the perpetrator. Explicit bias also manifests in
Counter-Stereotype imaging involves providing an
words, interactions, and body language, but includes
individual with a detailed description or picture of a counter-
deliberate action and deeply held conscious beliefs. Both types
stereotype. The counter-stereotype may be abstract (e.g.
of bias may affect the student’s ability to fairly assess their
“female leaders”) or specific (e.g. “Sheryl Sandberg”). While
peer’s effort and contribution.
many of the techniques discussed in this section have been
In peer assessments, evidence has been found to suggest
developed specifically in relation to implicit bias, counter-
that biased grades may be given based on pre-conceived
stereotype imaging has been shown to be effective at
notions of gender, race or ethnicity, and socioeconomic status.
mitigating both implicit and explicit bias [69].
However, the results for both gender and racial bias are mixed
and the extent to which biased peer grading occurs is still
Individuation
unclear.
Individuation prevents biased actions by learning
As instructors and students have expressed a desire for
specifics about others in order to view them as an individual as
fairer educational experiences, actions can and have been
opposed to an incidence of a stereotype. Using this technique,
taken to mitigate bias in the classroom and in peer assessment.
the lines between the “in-group” and “out-group” are
These actions typically involve bias reduction strategies and,
obscured, leading to fairer behavior [70].
in the case of peer assessment, rater training combined with [7] R. E. Slavin, & E. Oickle, "Effects of cooperative learning
reduction strategies. teams on student achievement and race relations:
This review of the literature is the beginning of a larger Treatment by race interactions," Sociology of Education,
project focused on creating fairer peer assessments by pp. 174-180, 1981.
teaching students techniques to address their own biases. With
this knowledge of where bias exists and the strategies used to [8] L. Springer, M. E. Stanne, & S. S. Donovan, “Effects of
small-group learning on undergraduates in science,
mitigate it, the research team will develop a comprehensive
mathematics, engineering, and technology: A meta-
peer assessment and bias mitigation methodology. This
analysis,” Review of educational research, vol. 69, no. 1,
methodology will then be tested in classrooms and evaluated pp. 21-51, 1999.
for its effectiveness in reducing the effects of bias on peer
assessment marks. This process will focus on implementing a [9] L. A. Stefani, “Peer, self and tutor assessment: Relative
“bias intervention” structured to help students overcome reliabilities,” Studies in Higher Education, vol. 19 no. 1,
specific biases related to race, gender, and international pp. 69-75, 1994.
student status. In university settings, international students
tend to be frequent targets of bias, regardless of race [16], [10] M. L. Wen, & C. C. Tsai, “University students’
[17]. These interventions will draw heavily from the literature perceptions of and attitudes toward (online) peer
on validated mitigation techniques reviewed here. After assessment,” Higher Education, vol. 51, no. 1, pp. 27-44,
implementing the bias intervention, measures of bias as well 2006.
as peer assessment scores will be recorded throughout the
[11] K. Topping, “Peer assessment between students in
semester.
colleges and universities,” Review of educational
Research, vol. 68, no. 3, pp. 249-276, 1998.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
[12] A. M. Langan, C. P. Wheater, E. M. Shaw, B. J. Haines,
This material is based upon work supported by a Miller Grant W. R. Cullen, J. C. Boyle, D. Penney, J. A. Oldekop, C.
Ashcroft, and L. Lockey, “Peer Assessment of Oral
from Iowa State University.
Presentations: Effects of Student Gender, University
Affiliation and Participation in the Development of
REFERENCES Assessment Criteria,” Assessment & Evaluation in Higher
[1] A. Marcus, L.C. Mullins, K.P. Brackett., Z. Tang, A.M. Education, vol. 30, no. 1, pp. 21–34, 2005.
Allen, & D.W. Pruett, “Perceptions of racism on
campus,” College Student Journal, vol. 37, pp. 611–626, [13] A. Planas Lladó, L. F. Soley, R. M. Fraguell Sansbelló, G
2003. A. Pujolras, J.P. Planella, N. Roura-Pascual,& L. M.
Moreno, “Student perceptions of peer assessment: an
[2] G.A. Boysen, & D.L. Vogel, “Bias in the classroom: interdisciplinary study,” Assessment & Evaluation in
Types, frequencies, and responses,” Teaching of Higher Education, vol. 39, no. 5, pp. 592-610, 2014.
Psychology, vol. 36, no.1, pp. 12-17, 2009.
[14] A. M. La Greca, B. Lai, S. Chan, & W. Herge, “Peer
[3] C. Wayland, L. Walker, & A. Ferrara, “Opening the Black Assessment Strategies,” in Diagnostic and Behavioral
Box: How Gender and Race Mediate Collaborative Assessment in Children and Adolescents: A Clinical
Learning in the Classroom,” in the Southeastern Women’s Guide, 2013.
Studies Association Annual Meeting, Wilmington, NC,
USA, 2014. [15] D. Magin, & P. Helmore, “Peer and teacher assessments
of oral presentations: How reliable are they?,” Studies in
[4] L.K. Michaelsen, A.B. Knight, & L.D. Fink, Team-based Higher Education, vol. 26, no. 3, pp. 287–298, 2001.
learning: A transformative use of small groups in higher
education. Sterling, VA: Stylus, 2004. [16] E. Samuel, “Racism in peer-group interactions: South
Asian students' experiences in Canadian
[5] L.K. Michaelsen & M. Sweet, “Team‐based academe,” Journal of College Student Development, vol.
learning,” New directions for teaching and learning, vol. 45, no. 4, pp. 407-424, 2004.
128, pp. 41-51, 2011.
[17] D. Dancer, & P. Kamvounias, “Student involvement in
[6] R. E. Allen, J. Copeland, A.S. Franks, R. Karimi, M. assessment: A project designed to assess class
McCollum, D. J. Riese, & A. Y. Lin, (2013). “Team- participation fairly and reliably,” Assessment &
based learning in US colleges and schools of Evaluation in Higher Education, vol. 30, no. 4, pp. 445-
pharmacy,” American journal of pharmaceutical 454, 2005.
education, vol. 77, no.6, pp. 115, 2013.
[18] V. Aryadoust, “Gender and academic major bias in peer [30] D. P. Crowne, & D. Marlowe, “A new scale of social
assessment of oral presentations,” Language Assessment desirability independent of psychopathology,” Journal of
Quarterly, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 1-24, 2016. consulting psychology, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 349, 1960.

[19] J. F. Dovidio, K. Kawakami, & S. L. Gaertner, “Implicit [31] J. B. McConahay, (1986). “Modern racism, ambivalence,
and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction,” Journal and the Modern Racism Scale,” in Prejudice,
of personality and social psychology, vol. 82, no. 1, pp. discrimination, and racism, J. F. Dovidio & S. L.
62, 2002. Gaertner, Eds. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 1986, pp.
91-125.
[20] J. F. Dovidio, & R. H. Fazio, (1992). “New technologies
for the direct and indirect assessment of attitudes,” In [32] J. L. Snowden, “Explicit and Implicit Bias Measures:
Questions about survey questions: Meaning, memory, Their Relation and Utility as Predictors of Criminal
attitudes, and social interaction, J. Tanur, Ed. New York: Verdict Tendency,” PhD dissertation, University of North
Russell Sage Foundation, 1992, pp. 204-237. Carolina at Wilmington, 2005.

[21] G. A. Boysen, D. L. Vogel, M. A. Cope, & A. Hubbard, [33] A. M. Morrison, & T. G. Morrison, “Development and
“Incidents of bias in college classrooms: Instructor and validation of a scale measuring modern prejudice toward
student perceptions,” Journal of Diversity in Higher gay men and lesbian women,” Journal of
Education, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 219, 2009. homosexuality, vol. 43, no. 2, pp. 15-37, 2003.

[22] C. Staats, K. Capatosto, R. A. Wright, & D. Contractor, [34] J. T. Spence, R. Helmreich, & J. Stapp, “A short version
“State of the science: Implicit bias review 2014,” Kirwan of the Attitudes toward Women Scale (AWS),” Bulletin of
Institute for the Study of Race and Ethnicity, vol. 14, pp. the Psychonomic Society, vol. 2, no.4, pp. 219-220, 1973.
129-142, 2014
[35] J. M. Twenge, “Attitudes toward women, 1970–
[23] M. Chen, & J. A. Bargh, “Nonconscious behavioral 1995,” Psychology of Women Quarterly, vol. 21, no. 1,
confirmation processes: The self-fulfilling consequences pp. 35-51, 1997.
of automatic stereotype activation,” Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 33, no. 5, pp. 541- [36] A. H. Eagly, & L. L. Carli, “Sex of researchers and sex-
560, 1997. typed communications as determinants of sex differences
in influenceability: A meta-analysis of social influence
[24] A. G. Greenwald, D. E. McGhee, & J. L. Schwartz, studies,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 90, no. 1, pg. 1,
“Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: 1981.
the implicit association test,” Journal of personality and
social psychology, vol. 74, no. 6, pp. 1464, 1998. [37] G. O'Neill, “Self, teacher and faculty assessments of
student teaching performance: a second scenario,” The
[25] J. Wald, “Can “de-biasing” strategies help to reduce Alberta Journal of Educational Research, vol. 31, no. 2,
racial disparities in school discipline? A summary of the pp. 88-98, 1985.
literature,” Discipline Disparities: A Research-to-Practice
Collaborative, 2014. [38] R. F. Van-Trieste, “The relation between Puerto Rican
university students’ attitudes toward Americans and the
[26] T. D. Wilson, S. Lindsey, & T. Y. Schooler, “A model of students’ achievement in English as a second language,”
dual attitudes,” Psychological Review, vol. 107, pp. 101– Homines, vol. 13, no. 4, pp. 94–112, 1990.
126, 2000.
[39] F. Pajares, & M. J. Johnson, “Self‐efficacy beliefs and the
[27] J. S. Nevid, & N. McClelland, “Measurement of implicit writing performance of entering high school
and explicit attitudes toward Barack Obama,” Psychology students,” Psychology in the Schools, vol. 33, no. 2, pp.
& Marketing, vol. 27, no. 10, pp. 989-1000, 2010. 163-175, 1996.

[28] A. J. Pantos, & A. W. Perkins, “Measuring implicit and [40]D. B. Kaufman, R. M. Felder, & H. Fuller, (2000).
explicit attitudes toward foreign accented Accounting for individual effort in cooperative learning
speech,” Journal of Language and Social teams. Journal of Engineering Education, 89(2), 133-140,
Psychology, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 3-20, 2013. 2000.

[29] P.G. Devine, P. S. Forscher, A. J. Austin, & W. T. Cox, [41] R. E. Bryan, A. J. Krych, S. W. Carmichael, T. R.
“Long-term reduction in implicit race bias: A prejudice Viggiano, W. Pawlina, “Assessing Professionalism in
habit-breaking intervention,” Journal of experimental Early Medical Education: Experience with Peer
social psychology, vol. 48, no. 6, pp. 1267-1278, 2012. Evaluation and Self- Evaluation in the Gross Anatomy
Course,” Annals-Academy of Medicine Singapore, vol. [55] K. Kraiger, & J. K. Ford, “A meta-analysis of ratee race
34, no. 8, pp. 486–491, 2005. effects in performance ratings,” Journal of Applied
Psychology, vol. 70, no. 1, pp. 56-65, 1985.
[42] W. R. Sherrard, F. Raafat, & R. R. Weaver, “An
empirical study of peer bias in evaluations: Students [56] P. R. Sackett, & C. L. DuBois, “Rater-ratee race effects
rating students,” Journal of Education for Business, vol. on performance evaluation: Challenging meta-analytic
70, no. 1, pp. 43-47, 1994. conclusions,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 76, no.
6, pp. 873, 1991.
[43] G. L. May, & L. E. Gueldenzoph, “The Effect of Social
Style on Peer Evaluation Ratings in Project Teams,” [57] Kaufman, D. B., R. M. Felder, & H. Fuller, “Peer ratings
Journal of Business Communication, vol. 43 no. 1, pp. 4– in cooperative learning teams,” in Proceedings of the
20, 2006. 1999 Annual ASEE Meeting, pp. 1-11.

[44] R. Tucker, “Sex does not matter: gender bias and gender [58] F. L. Schmidt, & R. H. Johnson, “Effect of race on peer
differences in peer assessments of contributions to group ratings in an industrial situation,” Journal of Applied
work,” Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Psychology, vol. 57, no. 3, pp. 237, 1973.
Education, vol. 39, no. 3, pp. 293-309, 2014.
[59] E. D. Pulakos, L. A. White, S. H. Oppler, & W. C.
[45] T. Gatfield, “Examining student satisfaction with group Borman, “Examination of race and sex effects on
projects and peer assessment,” Assessment & Evaluation performance ratings,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol.
in Higher Education, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 365-377, 1999. 74, no. 5, pp. 770, 1989.

[46] K. J. Topping, “Methodological quandaries in studying [60] D. Moorman, & D. Wicks-Smith, “Poverty discrimination
process and outcomes in peer assessment,” Learning and revealed through student peer evaluations,” College
Instruction, vol. 20, no. 4, pp. 339-343, 2010. Student Journal, vol. 46, no. 1, pp. 141-149, 2012.

[47] C. Rees, “Self‐assessment scores and gender,” Medical [61] H. O. Valencia, A. G. Carrillo, & M. M. G. Benítez, “The
Education, vol. 37, no. 6, pp. 572-573, 2003. Iinfluence of social style in evaluating academic
presentations ofengineering projects,” JOTSE: Journal of
[48] D. S. Lind, S. Rekkas, V. Bui, T. Lam, E. Beierle, and E. technology and science education, vol. 2, no. 2, 2012.
M. Copeland, “Competency- Based Student Self-
Assessment on a Surgery Rotation.” Journal of Surgical [62] S. Brown, & P. Knight, Assessing learners in higher
Research, vol. 105, no. 1, pp. 31–34, 2002. education, Psychology Press, 1994.

[49] M. Das, “Self and tutor evaluations in problem‐based [63] P. G. Devine, & M. J. Monteith, “The role of
learning tutorials: is there a relationship?” Medical discrepancy-associated affect in prejudice
education, vol. 32, no. 4, pp. 411-418, 1998. reduction,” Affect, cognition, and stereotyping:
Interactive processes in group perception, pp. 317-344,
[50] N. Falchikov, & D. Magin, “Detecting gender bias in peer 1993.
marking of students’ group process work,” Assessment &
Evaluation in Higher Education, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 385- [64] E. A. Plant, P. G. Devine, W. T. Cox, C. Columb, S. L.
396, 1997. Miller, J. Goplen, & B. M. Peruche, “The Obama effect:
Decreasing implicit prejudice and stereotyping,” Journal
[51] D. F. Baker, “Peer Assessment in Small Groups: A of Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 45, no. 4, pp.
Comparison of Methods,” Journal of Management 961-964, 2009.
Education, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 183–209, 2008.
[65] M. J. Monteith, C. I. Voils, & L. Ashburn-Nardo,
[52] J. A. Cox, & J. D. Krumboltz, “Racial bias in peer ratings “Taking a look underground: Detecting, interpreting, and
of basic airmen,” Sociometry, vol. 21, no. 4, pp. 292-299, reacting to implicit racial biases,” Social Cognition,
1958. vol. 19, no. 4, pp. 395-417, 2001.
[53] J. E. Dejung, & H. Kaplan, “Some differential effects of [66] K. Lee, P. C. Quinn, & G. D. Heyman, “Rethinking the
race of rater and ratee on early peer ratings of combat emergence and development of implicit racial bias: A
aptitude,” Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 46, no. 5, perceptual-social linkage hypothesis,” New perspectives
pp. 370, 1962. on human development, pp. 27-46, 2017.

[54] F. J. Landy, & J. L. Farr, “Performance [67] E. Fine, A. Wendt, & M. Carnes, “Gendered
rating,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 87, no. 1, pp. 72, expectations: are we unintentionally undermining our
1980. efforts to diversify STEM fields?,” Crossroads, The ACM
Magazine for Students, vol. 20, no. 4, pp. 46-51, 2014.
[68] M. J. Monteith, “Self-regulation of prejudiced responses:
Implications for progress in prejudice-reduction
efforts,” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, vol. 65, no. 3, pp. 469, 1993.

[69] I.V. Blair, J.E. Ma, A. P. Lenton, “Imagining stereotypes


away: The moderation of implicit stereotypes through
mental imagery,” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, vol. 81, pp. 828–841, 2001.

[70] D. A. Wilder, “Reduction of intergroup discrimination


through individuation of the out-group,” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 36, no. 12, pp.
1361-1374, 1978

[71] A. R. Todd, G. V. Bodenhausen, J. A. Richeson, & A. D.


Galinsky, “Perspective taking combats automatic
expressions of racial bias,” Journal of personality and
social psychology, vol. 100, no. 6, pp. 1027, 2001.

[72] A. D. Galinsky, & G. B. Moskowitz, “Perspective-taking:


decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility,
and in-group favoritism,” Journal of personality and
social psychology, vol. 78, no. 4, pp. 708, 2000.

[73] G. M. Herek, J. P. Capitanio, “Some of my best friends:


Intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and
heterosexuals’ attitudes toward gay men and
lesbians,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
vol. 22, pp. 412-424, 1996.

[74] S. E. Burke, J. F. Dovidio, J. M. Przedworski, R. R.


Hardeman, S. P. Perry, S. M. Phelan, D. B. Nelson, “Do
Contact and Empathy Mitigate Bias Against Gay and
Lesbian People Among Heterosexual Medical Students?
A report from Medical Student CHANGES,” Academic
medicine: journal of the Association of American Medical
Colleges, vol. 90, no. 5, pp. 645, 2015.

[75] L. Kelley, C. Chou, S. Dibble, P. Robertson, “A critical


intervention in lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender
health: Knowledge and attitude outcomes among second-
year medical students,” Teaching and Learning in
Medicine, vol. 20, pp. 248 – 253, 2008.

[76] G. L. May, “The effect of rater training on reducing


social style bias in peer evaluation,” Business
Communication Quarterly, vol. 71, no. 3, pp. 297-313,
2008.

You might also like