You are on page 1of 23

The Role of Emotions in Marketing

Richard P. Bagozzi
University of Michigan

Mahesh Gopinath
Tulane University

Prashanth U. Nyer
Chapman University

Emotions are mental states of readiness that arise from ap- in marketing behavior; and to provide suggestions for
praisals of events or one 's own thoughts. In this article, the future research.
authors discuss the differentiation of emotions from affect,
moods, and attitudes, and outline an appraisal theory of
emotions. Next, various measurement issues are consid- THEORY AND FUNCTION OF EMOTIONS
ered. This is followed by an analysis of the role of arousal
in emotions. Emotions as markers, mediators, and mod- Little consistency can be found in the use of terminol-
erators of consumer responses are then analyzed. The ogy related to emotions. For purposes of organization and
authors turn next to the influence of emotions on cognitive discussion, we begin with a definition of emotions and
processes, which is followed by a study of the implications then turn to a framework for interpreting emotional
of emotions for volitions, goal-directed behavior, and de- behavior.
cisions to help. Emotions and customer satisfaction are
briefly explored, too. The article closes with a number of Definitions
questions for future research.
The term affect will be conceived herein as an umbrella
for a set of more specific mental processes including emo-
tions, moods, and (possibly) attitudes. Thus, affect might
be considered a general category for mental feeling
This article addresses emotional behavior in marketing.
processes, rather than a particular psychological process,
In comparison to information processing and behavioral
per se.
decision research, we know much less about the role of
By emotion, we mean a mental state of readiness that
emotions in marketing behavior. Much of what we do
arises from cognitive appraisals of events or thoughts; has
know is confined to consumer behavior, as opposed to the
a phenomenological tone; is accompanied by physiologi-
behavior of salespeople or marketing managers.
cal processes; is often expressed physically (e.g., in ges-
Nevertheless, emotions are central to the actions of
tures, posture, facial features); and may result in specific
consumers and managers alike. Our goal in this article will
actions to affirm or cope with the emotion, depending on
be to present a framework for thinking about emotions; to
its nature and meaning for the person having it. For a simi-
discuss the measurement of emotions; to review how emo-
lar perspective, see Lazarus (1991) and Oatley (1992).
tions function as causes, effects, mediators, and moderators
The line between an emotion and mood is frequently
difficult to draw but often by convention involves conceiv-
ing of a mood as being longer lasting (from a few hours up
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science.
Volume 27, No. 2, pages 184-206. to days) and lower in intensity than an emotion. Yet, excep-
Copyright 9 1999 by Academy of Marketing Science. tions to this construal can be found. Still another distinction
Bagozziet al. / THE ROLEOF EMOTIONS 185

between emotions and moods is that the former typically is strength of felt subjective experience, plus magnitude of
intentional (i.e., it has an object or referent), whereas physiological response (e.g., autonomic nervous system
moods are generally nonintentional and global or diffused activity) and extent of bodily expression (e.g., facial dis-
(Frijda 1993). Also, moods are not as directly coupled with plays), when these latter reactions accompany an emotion.
action tendencies and explicit actions as are many Probably the most important factor differentiating
emotions. emotions from moods and attitudes is the way emotions
Finally, attitudes, too, are often considered instances of arise. Emotions are said to have a specific referent (e.g., a
affect, with the same measures used on occasion to indicate consumer becomes pleased when a new detergent removes
emotions and attitudes (e.g., pleasant-unpleasant, happy- grass stains from clothing; he or she is angered by poor ser-
sad, or interested-bored semantic differential items). How- vice in a restaurant). Specifically, emotions arise in
ever, some authors take a narrower view of attitudes and response to appraisals one makes for something of rele-
define them as evaluative judgments (measured, e.g., by vance to one's well-being. By appraisal, we mean an
good-bad reactions) rather than emotional states. Cohen evaluative judgment and interpretation thereof. By some-
and Areni (1991), for instance, reserve the term affect for thing of relevance, we mean an incident or episode that
"valenced feeling states," with emotions and moods as happens to oneself (e.g., an unplanned event); a behavior
specific examples. Attitudes are evaluative judgments in one performs or a result one produces (e.g., engaging in an
their view. Nevertheless, other researchers do not make a activity or receiving or failing to receive a planned out-
distinction between affect and evaluative judgments. For come); or a change in an object, person, or thought that has
example, Eagly and Chaiken (1993) point out that Fish- personal meaning.
bein and Ajzen (1975) and other social psychologists have It is important to stress that although categories of
"regarded affect as isomorphic with evaluation itself and events or physical circumstances are frequently associated
used the terms interchangeably" (p. 12). Still others pro- with particular emotional responses, it is not the specific
pose that attitudes have two distinct, but generally highly events or physical circumstances that produce the emo-
correlated, components: affective and cognitive (or tions but rather the unique psychological appraisal made
evaluative) dimensions. Some empirical support exists for by the person evaluating and interpreting the events and
this interpretation (Bagozzi and Burnkrant 1979; Batra circumstances. Different people can have different emo-
and Ahtola 1990; Breckler and Wiggins 1989; Crites, Fab- tional reactions (or no emotional reactions at all) to the
rigar, and Petty 1994; Eagly, Mladinic, and Otto 1994). same event or happening. Note, too, that appraisals can be
It should be recognized that the terms affect, emotions, deliberative, purposive, and conscious, but also unrefiec-
moods, and attitudes have frequently been used inconsis- five, automatic, and unconscious, depending on the person
tently in the literature. We will revisit this issue when we and eliciting conditions for emotional arousal. The central
consider both measurement issues and customer satisfac- role of appraisals in the formation of emotions has come to
tion research below. For now, we stress that when reading define what are aptly called appraisal theories in psychol-
the literature, it is important to pay attention to how ogy (e.g., Frijda 1986; Lazarus 1991; Ortony, Clore, and
authors define affective (and related) terminologies and Collins 1988; Roseman 1991; Smith and Ellsworth 1985).
how they measure the variables to which the terminologies Appraisal theorists maintain that the critical determi-
refer. One's definition of terms permits an interpretation of nant of any emotion is the resultant evaluation and inter-
their meaning, but equally important is how the variables pretation that arise after comparing an actual state with a
to which the terms refer are operationalized. Some authors desired state. Two appraisals are particularly crucial at this
have defined key variables as emotions, moods, or atti- stage of emotion formation: goal relevance and goal con-
tudes but have used operationalizations corresponding to gruence (Lazarus 1991). That is, a necessary condition for
different concepts. Other authors have used operationali- an emotional response to an event or happening is that a
zations for a single variable that cut across two or more person has a personal stake in it and at the same time
instances of affect. To make clear our definition of emo- judges the event or happening to facilitate or thwart this
tions and how it differs from definitions of mood and atti- stake. Again, the appraisal can occur consciously or
tudes, we present the following point of view. unconsciously.
A distinctive feature of appraisal theories is their speci-
Organizing Framework fication of the conditions leading to discrete emotional
responses. Forced to be brief, we focus on Roseman's
Above we noted that emotions are mental states of (1991) version of appraisal theories, which differs in rela-
readiness. But so, too, are moods and attitudes. How then tively minor ways from other leading theories. Roseman
might we distinguish between these affective states? For hypothesized that particular combinations of five apprais-
one thing, the state of readiness characterized by an emo- als determine which of 16 unique emotions will be experi-
tion tends to be more intense than that characterized by enced in any given situation. Figure 1 summarizes his the-
moods or attitudes. It is more intense in the sense of ory, where the five appraisals are labeled motive
186 JOURNALOF THE ACADEMYOF MARKETINGSCIENCE SPRING 1999

FIGURE 1 Not every emotion is accounted for by Roseman's


Roseman's (1991) Appraisal framework (or by any other framework for that matter).
Theory of Emotions For instance, pride is regarded as a positive emotion in
Roseman's framework, yet excessive or exaggerated pride
(sometimes termed hubris) can invite retribution. Like-
Posiuve Emotions NegativeEmotions
Motive-Consistent Motive-lnconslstent wise, shame and guilt are thought by Roseman to be pro-
Circumstance Appetitive [ Averslve Appetitive I Aversive duced by similar appraisals, but other researchers have
caused
unexpected Sur~se
found important distinctions between shame, guilt, and
11ricertaila Hope
I
Fear Weak embarrassment (e.g., Lewis 1993). Likewise, disgust has
certain Joy [ Relief Sadness Distress, Disgust been studied extensively and found to differ from distress
uncertain Hope
eertam Joy I Relief
Frustratton Strong (e.g., Rozin, Haidt, and McCauley 1993). Nevertheless, in
Other-caused contrast to other theories of emotion that conceive of it in
uncertain Dishke Weak bipolar terms (e.g., pleasure-displeasure and high
ceruun Ldong
unee~
arousal-low arousal [Russell 1980] or high negative
Anger Strong
affect-low negative affect and high positive affect-low
Self-caused positive affect [Watson and Tellegen 1985]), Roseman's
uncertain Shame, Guilt Weak
certain Pride
framework and other appraisal theories not only allow for
uncertain many discrete emotions but specify conditions for their
Regret Strong
c~l'tain occurrence.
An elaboration of appraisal theories that is especially
SOURCE:Roseman(1991:193). Reprintedwithpermission. relevant for marketing is the treatment of goals, which may
be defined as "internal representations of desired states,
where states are broadly defined as outcomes, events, or
processes" (Austin and Vancouver 1996:338). Oatley and
Johnson-Laird (1987) proposed what they termed a com-
consistent/motive inconsistent (i.e., positive emotions ver-
municative theory of emotions wherein events are evalu-
sus negative emotions), appetitive/aversive (i.e., presence
ated in relation to a person's goals. Emotions are thought
of a reward vs. absence of a punishment), agency (i.e., out-
to function to coordinate parts of one's cognitive system so
come is perceived caused by impersonal circumstances,
as to manage responses to events and in so doing change
some other person, or the self), probability (i.e., an out-
from ongoing to new activities or to maintain desired states
come is certain or uncertain), and power (i.e., strong ver-
or activities. The self-regulation of goals is believed to be
sus weak coping potential).
the main function of emotions:
For example, pride occurs when one evaluates his or her
own performance of an action or achievement of an out-
Each goal and plan has a monitoring mechanism that
come in a positive light (e.g., a feeling of having done
evaluates events relevant to it. When a substantial
well). Here the positive emotion is motive consistent, change in probability occurs of achieving an impor-
either appetitive (e.g., having attained a positive goal) or tant goal or subgoal, the monitoring mechanism
aversive (e.g., having avoided a punishment), self- broadcasts to the whole cognitive system a signal
produced under weak or low coping potential, and either that can set it into readiness to respond to this
certain or uncertain, depending on the circumstances. Sad- change. Humans experience these signals and the
ness happens when one experiences a loss for which one states of readiness they induce as emotions. (Oatley
recognizes that nothing can be done to restore it. The loss, 1992:50)
which is of something or someone valued, is experienced
negatively and with high certainty under conditions of low According to Oatley and Johnson-Laird (1987), emotions
coping power. It is perceived to be caused by impersonal are evoked "at a significant juncture of a p l a n . . , typi-
circumstances. c a l l y . . , when the evaluation (conscious or unconscious)
One value of appraisal theories is that it is possible to of the likely success of a plan changes" (p. 35). Positive
account for most emotions. Indeed, subtle combinations of emotions (e.g., happiness, elation, joy) are associated with
appraisals yield discrete emotional responses. Anger and the attainment of a (sub)goal, which usually leads to a de-
regret, for example, differ primarily in only one type of cision to continue with the plan, whereas negative emo-
appraisal and share in the other four, namely, anger occurs tions (e.g., frustration, disappointment, anxiety) result
when a person sees another person as the source of injury from problems with ongoing plans and failures to achieve
to oneself or to another person viewed as a victim of injus- desired goals (see also Stein, Liwag, and Wade 1996).
tice, whereas regret results when one's negative outcome Emotions have implications for action and goal attain-
is attributed to actions or inactions of the self. ment. Lazarus (1991) identifies coping responses as
Bagozziet al. / THE ROLEOF EMOTIONS 187

important mechanisms in this regard. When we experience categories (e.g., avoidance with fear, helping with caring,
a negative emotion (e.g., anger, sadness, fear), we are in helplessness with sadness, assault with anger, withdrawal
disequilibrium and wish to return to our normal state. with shame).
Either one or both of two coping processes are typically Finally, it has been argued that many coping responses
used: problem-focused coping, where we attempt to alle- to emotions are volitional (B agozzi 1992:186-189). The
viate the sources of distress, or emotion-focused coping, process begins with outcome-desire units and appraisals
where we either change the meaning of the source of dis- of changes or anticipated changes in goal attainment or
tress (e.g., deny that a threat exists, distance oneself from goal progress. Four appraisal classes can be identified.
the source of distress) or avoid thinking about a problem. Outcome-desire conflicts happen when one fails to
By contrast, coping with positive emotions often achieve a goal or when one experiences an unpleasant
involves sharing one's good fortune, savoring the experi- event. One or more emotional reactions occur to
ence, working to continue or increase the rewards, and outcome-desire conflicts (e.g., dissatisfaction, anger,
increasing physical activity. Positive emotions are some- shame, guilt, sadness, disappointment, disgust, regret),
times accompanied as well by higher levels of physiologi- depending on attributions of the source of goal failure or
cal arousal, expanded attention, increased optimism, the unpleasant event (i.e., self, other person, or external
enhanced recall, and a shift from self- to other-centered cause). The coping response(s) to these emotions, in turn,
orientations (e.g., becoming friendlier, caring about oth- is selected from the following: intent to remove or undo
ers), when compared, say, to sadness. Indeed, positive harm, obtain help or support, decrease outcome, reevalu-
emotions, particularly happiness, frequently stimulate ate goal, or redouble effort, if appropriate, depending on
helping or altruistic actions. Why? Schaller and Cialdini the specific emotion involved.
(1990) offer two explanations: "First, we may propose that Outcome-desire fulfillment takes place when one
positive mood leads to enhanced helping via the more achieves a goal, experiences a pleasant event, or avoids an
positive outlook and enhanced activity that appear to unpleasant event. One or more emotional reactions come
OJO about when outcome-desire fulfillment happens (e.g., sat-
spring automatically from the experience of happiness,"
and second, "we argue that happiness is associated with a isfaction, joy, elation, pleasure, pride, relief, caring, love),
motivation toward disequilibrium--toward the possible again depending on attributions of the source of good for-
attainment of additional personal rewards that transcend tune. The coping responses to these emotions include an
the basic concern over one's mood" (pp. 284-285). The intention to maintain, to increase, to share, or to enjoy the
personal rewards referred to here concern such self- outcome.
enrichment motives as affiliation, achievement, compe- Outcome-desire conflicts and fulfillment refer to out-
tence, and esteem. comes in the past or present. The following two appraisal
Closely related to coping responses are action tenden- classes go on with regard to planned outcomes. Outcome-
cies. An action tendency is "a readiness to engage in or dis- desire avoidances transpire in anticipation of unpleasant
engage from interaction with some goal object" and outcomes or goals. Fear or its variants (e.g., worry, anxi-
includes "(i)mpulses of 'moving towards,' 'moving away,' ety, distress) are the emotional reactions to this appraisal.
and 'moving against' " (Frijda, Kuipers, and ter Schure The coping responses to these emotions entail either an
1989:213). Some theorists maintain that emotions are not intention to avoid undesirable outcomes or to reinterpret
merely reactions to appraisals of events but also include the threat.
action tendencies as part of their meaning (Frijda 1986). The final class of appraisals, outcome-desire pursuits,
Others go further and maintain that action tendencies are happen in anticipation of pleasant goals or outcomes.
automatic, "prewired" responses connected to emotions Hope is the emotional reaction to such appraisals. The
(LeDoux 1996). And in Frijda's (1986) treatment, emo- coping response(s) to hope includes intentions to realize or
tions are conceived as the entire process from stimulus facilitate outcome attainment and to sustain one's commit-
event to action and arousal: ment and vigilance.
The theory of self-regulation suggests that unique voli-
Stimulus event~event coding~appraisal (evaluationof tional responses underly coping for each particular emo-
tion or class of emotions (Bagozzi 1992). In addition, the
relevance, context, and urgency/difficulty/seriousnessof specific intention enacted depends on one's degree of
self-efficacy in executing the coping responses. Somewhat
action analogous (appraisal---~emotional reactions---~coping)
event)~action readiness
" arousal responses occur for outcome-identity conflicts, fulfill-
ments, avoidances, and pursuits in social situations related
Much as emotions arise in response to patterns of apprais- to normative expectations (B agozzi 1992:191-194).
als, Frijda (1986; Frijda et al. 1989) has shown that pat- Returning to the distinction between emotions and
terns of action readiness correspond to distinct emotion moods and attitudes, we might say that in addition to the
188 JOURNALOF THE ACADEMYOF MARKETINGSCIENCE SPRING 1999

things mentioned earlier, emotions differ from moods and physiological, motor, or biological indicators would be at
attitudes in the manner in which they arise and in their rep-best considered correlates or indirect measures of
resentation in memory. Emotions occur in response to emotions.
changes in specific plans or goal-relevant events. As Oat- Other researchers who interpret emotions in broader
ley (1992) points out, emotions are manifest as "transi- terms, as either the whole process from the coding of
tions from one sequence of action and another," but moods events to action responses (e.g., Frijda 1986) or as com-
occur "when the cognitive system is maintained in an emo- plex patterns of physiological responses (e.g., Cacioppo,
tion mode for a period" (pp. 64, 91-92). Indeed, moods are Bemtson, and Klein 1992 maintain that emotional experi-
often resistant to changes in events surrounding them. One ence is a function of somatoviscerai activation, afferentia-
reason for this is that moods "depend on the dissociability tion, and cognitive operations; LeDoux 1996 emphasizes
of control emotion signals from semantic information brain processes, especially the role of the amygdala) stress
about causation" (Oatley 1992:64). In general, moods are the need for measurement processes going beyond self-
elicited by "(a) after effects of emotions; (b) organismic reports. Depending on the theorist, overt behaviors or
conditions such as illness, fatigue, previous exercise, and physiological reactions may be considered either a part of
good health, or pharmacological agents; (c) general envi- what it means to have an emotion or antecedents, concomi-
ronmental conditions and side-effects of activities: heat, tants, or possibly even effects of an emotion. More behav-
noise, environmental variety, stressful conditions" (Frijda iorally or physiologically oriented researchers obviously
1986:289). employ measures of emotions consistent with these
Like emotions, attitudes can arise from changes in interpretations.
events, but attitudes also occur in response to mundane Marketers have tended to take an empirical approach to
objects. In addition, arousal is a necessary part of emotionsthe measurement of emotions and to rely on self-reports
but not necessarily attitudes. Moreover, attitudes seem to (i.e., either unipolar or bipolar items on questionnaires). In
have the capacity to be stored during long periods of time the typical application, many items cutting across numer-
and retrieved, whereas emotions are not experienced in ous positive and negative emotions are administered to
this way (i.e., emotions are ongoing states of readiness; measure reactions to a stimulus, and such methods as fac-
tor analysis, multidimensional scaling, or cluster analysis
they are not stored and retrieved, per se, although it is pos-
sible to recreate the conditions originally producing them are used to identify the underlying emotional dimensions
in our memory and react emotionally to the thoughts so for the sample at hand. The number of items investigated in
generated, at least up to a point; emotions can, however, be this regard has been as large as 180 (Aaker, Stayman, and
classically conditioned, but it is unclear whether attitudes Vezina 1988), while a paper-and-pencil technique (basi-
can). Finally, the connection of emotions to volition and cally a single item measured continuously while viewing
action is stronger and more direct than it is for attitudes. an ad) has even been suggested to register "warmth"
Emotions directly stimulate volitions and initiate action, toward a stimulus ad (Aaker, Stayman, and Hagerty 1986;
but attitudes may require an additional motivation impe- see also Russell, Weiss, and Mendelsohn 1989).
tus, such as desire (Bagozzi 1992). Two influential studies in the measurement of emo-
how emotions are stronger predictors of behavior tional responses toward advertisements are those by Edell
than attitudes and Burke (1987) and Holbrook and Batra (1987). Edell
MEASUREMENT OF EMOTIONS and Burke (see also Burke and Edell 1989) developed a
52-item scale for measuring emotions towards ads, while
The measurement of emotions could focus on a full set Holbrook and Batra worked with a 94-item scale, which
of signs or evidence, including evaluative appraisals, sub- was later reduced to 34 items (Batra and Holbrook 1990).
jective feelings, body posture and gestures, facial expres- With so many items measuring emotions, a question
sions, physiological responses, action tendencies, and arises whether a small number of basic dimensions under-
overt actions. Whatever measurements one uses should, of lie people's responses. Edell and Burke (1987) analyzed
course, be tied to an underlying theory of emotions. the items in their scale and found three factors: upbeat feel-
Some authors (e.g., most appraisal theorists) construe ings, negative feelings, and warm feelings. Likewise, Hol-
emotions as mental states or processes, and thus it would brook and Batra (1987) used factor analysis, but in a
be prudent to directly measure the cognitive activities slightly different way. Their 94 items were first generated
comprising the emotional content of these states or a priori to measure 29 emotional indices. For example,
processes, from this perspective. Self-reports of one's sub- joyful, happy, delighted, and pleased were hypothesized to
jective experiences constitute the most frequently used indicate a joy index, and ashamed, embarrassed, and
procedures in this regard, although other methods for indi- humiliated were hypothesized to indicate a shame index.
cating emotional memory processes might be used as well Then, based on factor analyses of the 29 indices, a three-
(e.g., response time, subliminal priming). From the point factor solution for emotions was found: pleasure, arousal,
of view of mental conceptualizations of emotions, and domination. Similarly, Batra and Holbrook (1990)
Bagozzi et al. / THE ROLE OF EMOTIONS 189

factor analyzed 12 emotional indices (largely a subset of FIGURE 2


those revealed in Holbrook and Batra, with a few excep- Watson and Tellegen's Two-Factor
tions) and discovered three factors corresponding closely Structure of Affect
to those discovered by Edell and Burke. Other researchers
(e.g., Oliver 1994; Westbrook 1987), also using factor CONSENSUALMOODSTRUCTURE
analysis, have found emotional items to load on two fac-
~Gt4 pOSItIVE AFF~CCT
tors: positive affect and negative affect.
a~ve
Richins (1997) recently argued that consumption- elaled
enthu~bc
related emotions are more complex than the two- and excited
pepp
three-factor solutions observed in studies of reactions to "kindly $1rofl

ads or customer satisfaction. Moreover, because explora- Ist~d


warmhearted J
tory factor analyses often yield a small number of factors, J
she used a multidimensional scaling procedure, in con-
junction with examination of clusters based on location t lit rlist
jl
dlstrUSed
hos~ie
~ , , <;
, ~Z
and semantic similarity of emotional descriptors in two- rli~xed nervous ~ Z

dimensional space. Sixteen clusters of emotions were s


identified, each measured by 2 to 8 indicators (in Study 4): i
J

anger, discontent, worry, sadness, fear, shame, envy, lone- O~


o~nl
qu~ol
liness, romantic love, love, peacefulness, contentment, ~. 9 111111
optimism, joy, excitement, and surprise. dull
Iklepy
r
/
Although the approaches used by marketers to date
LOwPOSITIVEAFF~.C~
have been largely empirically driven (e.g., Edell and Burke
1987; Holbrook and Batra 1987; Oliver 1994; Richins,
1997; Westbrook 1987), they are consistent in certain senses SOURCE: Watson and Teilegen (1985:225). Copyright 9 1985 by the
American Psychological Association. Reprinted with permission.
with leading perspectives on emotions in psychology. For
example, Holbrook and Batra's (1987) three-factor
pleasure-arousal-domination findings are somewhat simi-
lar to Russell and Mehrabian's (1977) three-factor
affect and high negative affect-low negative affect. Rus-
pleasure-arousal-dominance model. However, some dif-
sell's interpretation is essentially the same as Watson and
ferences can be pointed out. The most important are the
Tellegen's pleasantness-unpleasantness and strong
high loadings of sadness and fear on the domination factor
engagement-disengagement axes, which are rotated 45
in Holbrook and Batra's study, in contrast to the more com-
degrees from their primary axes (but see Larsen and
mon outcome of sadness loading on or near the negative
Diener 1992).
pole of a pleasure-displeasure factor, and fear loading
The idea behind the circumplex model is that emotions
about 45 degrees away from displeasure and toward
exist in bipolar categories (e.g., happy-sad, nervous-
greater arousal (e.g., Russell, 1997).
relaxed) and can be arranged in a continuous order around
Similarly, the three factors found in Edell and Burke's the perimeter of a two-factor space. The closer emotions
(1987) study (i.e., upbeat feelings, negative feelings, and are to each other on the perimeter, the more similar they
warm feelings), correspond roughly to the high positive are. For example, excited and aroused are more similar
affect-low positive affect, high negative affect-low nega- than are content and aroused (see Figure 2). The origin or
tive affect, and pleasantness-unpleasantness dimensions, center of the circumplex is thought to represent a neutral
respectively, of Watson and Tellegen's (1985) circumplex point or adaptation level.
model; the positive-affect and negative-affect factors The circumplex model is appealing because it is intui-
observed by Oliver (1994) and Westbrook (1987) also tive, simple, and provides a description of which emotions
align to a great extent with factors on the circumplex are similar and which are dissimilar. However, it has draw-
model (see also Mano and Oliver 1993). backs. The most serious limitation is that it is based on
The circumplex structure of emotions is shown in Fig- empirical associations among experienced emotions and
ure 2. This representation of emotions has also been called has nothing to say about the conditions (e.g., appraisals)
the two-factor model, because, based on the techniques producing emotions. Then, too, the circumplex model can
used to generate it (e.g., factor analysis or multidimen- obscure subtle differences in emotions. Depending on
sional scaling), emotions can be arranged around two eliciting conditions and people's appraisals, each of the
orthogonal axes. Russell (1997) terms the axes pleasure- emotions grouped together within any particular category
displeasure and arousal-sleepiness, while Watson and Tel- on the circumplex can be distinct from its cocategory
legen (1985) label them high positive affect-low positive members. For example, it is possible to feel fearful without
190 JOURNALOF THE ACADEMYOF MARKETINGSCIENCE SPRING 1999

feeling hostile (see "high negative affect" in Figure 2). movie, and pleasure, contentment, irritation, or anger
Then, too, the circumplex contains categories that may not occurred toward the theater, depending on the three
correspond to emotions. For instance, surprised, drowsy, appraisal conditions. Scenarios were used to create the
and sleepy do not seem to reflect emotions. On the other appraisal conditions.
hand, the circumplex fails to represent well instances of By contrast, research not based on manipulations of
emotion important in everyday life and marketing. It does appraisal conditions or based on reactions to a single
not accommodate love, disgust, pride, hope, guilt, shame, stimulus frequently finds that emotions cluster in two and,
or embarrassment very well, to name a few. The various on occasion, three factors (e.g., Edell and Burke 1987;
two- and three-factor summaries of emotions disclosed in Holbrook and Batra 1987; Oliver 1994; Westbrook 1987).
marketing also include variables not reflective of emotions Furthermore, research examining the construct validity of
and exclude instances generally recognized as emotions. measures of discrete emotions obtained in nonexperimen-
Richins's (1997) Consumption Emotions Set (CES) tal survey settings shows that discriminant validity is often
with its 16 descriptors is appealing because it covers most lacking among measures of different positive or different
emotional reactions one encounters in consumption, and negative emotions (e.g., Bagozzi 1993) or between mea-
its measures achieved satisfactory reliability (except for sures within a particular subcategory of positive or negative
measure of envy, loneliness, peacefulness, and content- emotions, such as among measures of elation, gladness,
ment). The CES would be best used within the context of a and joy (Bagozzi 1991a).
particular theory of emotions to operationalize specific What accounts for the differences in findings between
categories of emotions hypothesized to serve as antece- experimental research based on appraisal theories and sur-
dents, consequences, or moderating variables. The CES vey research or research based on reactions to a single
could also be used to operationalize emotions in more stimulus measured by inventories of emotional items? One
empirically oriented studies, but whether measures of each possibility may be that discrete emotional reactions are
dimension would achieve discriminant validity is prob- short-lived or, once activated, stimulate other emotional
lematic. Most studies incorporating multiple instances of reactions closely related to them. Consider, for example,
both positive emotions and negative emotions find that the sadness: "[W]hen we experience loss, we rarely feel a sin-
measures load on two factors corresponding to positive gle emotion such as sadness. We grieve, are angry, anx-
and negative emotions (e.g., Bagozzi, Baumgartner, and ious, guilty, envious, even hopeful, and defensive" (Lazarus
Pieters, 1998; Oliver, 1994). 1991:250). A reason why these emotions may go in tan-
This raises the question of when one can expect discrete dem is that coping processes for sadness may involve
emotional reactions versus amalgamated groupings of, active struggle or even protest against loss, which results in
say, positive emotions and negative emotions (e.g., highly other emotions. Alternatively, the absence of discrete
correlated feelings of anger, sadness, and fear). The advan- emotions may simply reflect how difficult it is to create
tage of a theory-based approach to emotions is that spe- them. As Izard (1972) noted, pure emotions are "virtually
cific conditions can be specified for the occurrence of dis- impossible to obtain in the laboratory or in any research
tinct emotions, and these hypotheses can be tested. It is for setting" (p. 103). However, as noted above, researchers
these reasons that we used appraisal theories as our organ- have recently found that the use of scenarios in an experi-
izing framework for looking at emotions. Discrete emo- mental context can generate discrete emotional responses
tional reactions are likely to happen when one manipulates (e.g., Gopinath and Bagozzi 1999; Roseman 1991).
conditions producing specific appraisals or when naturally Another factor that might account for a coalescence of
occurring events correspond to unique appraisal condi- multiple positive emotions and multiple negative emotions
tions. Gopinath and Bagozzi (1999), for example, were in two corresponding groupings is the nature of the stimu-
able to induce independent emotional reactions toward lus under study. Most stimulus ads, products, or brands are
three targets in a moviegoing context. On the basis of complex, and the appraisals engendered are typically
Roseman's (1991) theory, distinct emotional reactions variegated, but related. Also the way in which items are
were produced as a function of three-way interactions presented on some questionnaires makes it difficult to
between motive consistency-inconsistency, appetitive- uncover discrete emotional components. When multiple
aversive, and self-other agency conditions. For example, measures of a single discrete emotional response (e.g.,
admiration, affection, dislike, and contempt resulted happy, pleased, and joyful for "joy") are interspersed
toward a group member in decision making with regard to throughout a questionnaire, this tends to reduce correla-
movie choice, and pride, shame/guilt, and regret resulted tions among items purported to indicate the same response
toward the self in group decision making, depending on and to increase correlations of these items with measures
the three appraisal conditions. Likewise, happiness, satis- of other responses. The result is predictably a reduction in
faction, annoyance, or frustration occurred toward the discriminant validity and high correlations among items
Bagozziet al. / THE ROLEOF EMOTIONS 191

measuring positive emotions and among items measuring to eating in fast-food restaurants (a common, singular
negative emotions. The alternative is to group items by the stimulus), positive and negative emotions were indepen-
emotional response they are intended to tap, which tends to dent for men and women alike.
increase correlations among measures of the same thing It is perhaps too early to give definitive recommenda-
and decrease correlations among measures of different tions on which emotional scales to employ in empirical
emotional responses. Thus, a trade-off is entailed by use of work, but for now, we think that it is advisable to recom-
either practice. mend use of unipolar scales that ask respondents to
An issue that has received little attention in marketing is express to what extent each emotion describes their own
whether to use unipolar or bipolar items to measure emo- subjective feelings, rather than bipolar scales that can
tions. The choice can influence findings and their interpre- obscure differences in emotional responses across the
tation in fundamental ways. Some leading scholars claim various dimensions. Also, at least five, preferably seven to
that emotions are in the final analysis bipolar states or nine, scale steps should be used for each item to enhance
processes. We are either happy or sad, for example, and the chances that optimal distributional properties of mea-
any other pattern (e.g., independence or concomitance) is sures will be achieved. In addition, at least three, prefera-
thought to be an artifact of measurement error. Although a bly more, items should be used for each emotional
number of studies have shown that pleasant and unpleasant subcategory.
emotions are independent (e.g., Bradburn 1969; Diener A final measurement issue we wish to raise is the fol-
and Emmons 1985; Zevon and Tellegen 1982), Green, lowing. To what extent are emotions blends of categories?
Goldman, and Salovey (1993) and Barrett and Russell For ease of discussion, we consider the categories of emo-
(1998) argue and present findings demonstrating that emo- tions presented on the circumplex. Pleasantness-
tions are bipolar, once random or both random and system- unpleasantness might combine, for instance, with arousal
atic errors are taken into account. to produce different kinds or intensities of emotion. To take
Bagozzi, Wong, and Yi (1998) challenge the conclu- a particular example, consider happiness, an instant of
sions made by Green et al. (1993) and Barrett and Russell pleasantness on the circumplex. Intense forms of happiness
(1998). They hypothesize that bipolarity, independence, occur when pleasantness combines with high arousal:
and concomitance depend on gender, culture, and the tar- elated, excited, enthusiastic, euphoric, gleeful, joyous, ec-
get of one's emotions. Briefly, Bagozzi, Wong, and Yi static, and exultant are examples. Mild forms of happiness
found that positive and negative emotions were highly occur when pleasantness combines with low arousal:
negatively correlated for American women but highly peaceful, calm, serene, or quietude of mind are examples.
positively correlated for Chinese women. The former pat- Happiness, itself, might be at an intermediate level of
tern is evidence for bipolarity (i.e., either positive or nega- arousal. Other perspectives on blended emotions, based
tive emotions occur but not both), the latter for concomi- not on a blend of arousal with emotion categories but
tance (i.e., both positive and negative emotions occur at the rather on combinations of "basic" emotions, can be found
same time). For men, the correlations between positive and in Izard (1991, 1992) and Plutchik (1980).
negative emotions were much smaller in magnitude but in What more general role, if any, does arousal play in
the same direction across ethnicity, that is, slight negative emotions (Bagozzi 1991b)? We turn to this issue next.
correlations were found for American men, slight positive
correlations for Chinese men. Thus, the relationship
between positive and negative emotions for men was AROUSAL
nearly independent. Bagozzi, Wong, and Yi argued that
differences in culture (i.e., a tendency for Americans to An early, influential point of view on emotions was pro-
view things in dichotomies or discrete categories, i.e., in fessed by James ([1890] 1950) who claimed that "bodily
opposition; and a tendency for Chinese to view things dia- changes follow directly the perception of the exciting fact,
lectically, i.e., in balance or harmony) interact with gender and.., ourfeeling of the same changes as they occur lS the
differences (i.e., a tendency for women to be more knowl- emotion" (p. 449, emphasis in original). For James, differ-
edgeable and skilled in the use of emotions than men) to ent stimuli lead to different bodily responses (e.g., sweaty
produce the divergent patterns. The above findings palms, racing heart, etc.), these physiological responses
resulted when people were asked to express how they felt are then detected as bodily sensations in our mind, and the
at the moment according to their idiosyncratic reasons, result is interpreted by us as emotional experiences. But it
which is the standard procedure used in the literature. That is important to note that James reserved this interpretation
is, the stimulus for each person could be considered het- for what he termed the "coarser" emotions (e.g., "grief, fear,
erogeneous across individuals. However, when Americans rage, love"), which involve strong bodily perturbations; he
and Chinese were asked to give their emotional reactions was less clear about what he termed the "subtler" emotions
192 JOURNALOF THE ACADEMYOF MARKETINGSCIENCE SPRING 1999

(e.g., "moral, intellectual, and aesthetic feelings" (James to awareness of the meaningful stimuli. Repeated expo-
[1890] 1950:468). sure to a meaningful stimulus can lead to increased or
An equally influential theory of emotions was proposed decreased positive or negative feelings, depending on its
by Schachter and Singer (1962), who argued that emotion meaning. Mere exposure, thus, loses its utility in such
is essentially bodily arousal plus a cognitive label one pro- cases, both as a theory and a practical tactic.
vides to diagnose his or her felt arousal, a perspective con- Based loosely on the notion that emotions or moods
sistent with James's point of view. The idea is that we first induced by one stimulus become attached to another, some
experience physiological arousal, and, especially when we researchers have investigated the effects of (a) music on
are unaware or uncertain of the origin of the arousal, we length of stay and money spent in supermarkets and res-
look for evidence in the physical and social situation taurants (e.g., Milliman 1982, 1986) and (b) affective tone
accompanying the arousal to label our emotional state. of stores on purchase intentions (e.g., Donovan and Rossi-
Although this theory had considerable impact in psychol- ter 1982) and evaluations (e.g., Gardner and Simokos
ogy for nearly three decades, it has largely been discred- 1986). How can the observed attachment of affect from
ited and has not received much supporting evidence one stimulus to another be explained? Shimp (1991)
beyond Schachter and Singer's original experiment (e.g., reviews seven studies in consumer research that test vari-
Manstead and Wagner 1981; Reisenzein 1983). One ous facets of classical conditioning explanations. The idea
exception to the above observation is the frequently behind classical conditioning is that the repeated pairing
repeated finding that arousal misattributed to an extrane- of a conditioned stimulus (e.g., a new brand name) with an
ous source intensifies emotions (e.g., Cantor, Bryant, and unconditioned stimulus (e.g., an attractive spokesperson)
Zillman 1974; Dutton and Aron 1974; Zillman 1971). will eventually lead to the new brand name, on its own,
An important contribution of James ([1890] 1950) and stimulating the unconditioned response (e.g., positive
Schachter and Singer (1962) was the recognition that affect) originally induced by the unconditioned stimulus.
arousal plays an essential role in emotion. Before we Very few studies have been performed in marketing that
address arousal more fully, we should mention research in conform to the conditions required to test classical condi-
marketing that addresses the acquisition of affect, without tioning. It is unclear whether classical conditioning studies
necessarily involving concepts of arousal or information can be designed to rule out such rival explanations as
processing. Both in the practice of marketing and market- demand characteristics or cognitive interpretations of the
ing research, considerable emphasis has been placed on results. Allen and Janiszewski (1989) provide some evi-
the effects of various stimuli on consumer behavior. Retail dence that at least one type of cognitive mediation is neces-
store environment cues, advertising, background music, sary for classical conditioning to occur: namely, subject
brand names, packages, celebrity endorsers, and other awareness of the contingency between the conditioned
stimuli are frequently administered to produce emotional stimulus and unconditioned stimulus. On the other hand,
reactions in consumers. The premise is that emotions or classical conditioning, particularly for fear responses, has
moods trigger buying responses (e.g., Gardner 1985; Hill been shown to involve unconscious arousal processes con-
and Gardner 1987). nected with the amygdala (LeDoux 1996).
If not by appraisal processes or direct arousal, per se, Another way to explain the observed attachment of
how does presentation of a stimulus under repetitive con- affect from one stimulus to another is by Zillman's (1971)
ditions induce affect? A seductively simple explanation excitation-transfer model. Briefly, Zillman proposed that
was provided by Zajonc, who argued that "when objects exposure to one stimulus may produce arousal. If a second
are presented to the individual on repeated occasions, the stimulus is presented close on that also is capable of pro-
mere exposure is capable of making the individual's atti- ducing arousal on its own, the two sources of arousal may
tude toward these objects more positive" (Zajonc and Mar- combine to produce intensely experienced arousal. Under
kus 1982:125). This "mere exposure" effect has been certain conditions (e.g., unawareness of the source of
found primarily when the stimulus is simple and previ- arousal from the first stimulus, recency of the second), a
ously unknown or else has little or no semantic content person may attribute the arousal to the second stimulus.
(e.g., nonsense syllables, foreign words of Chinese char- Zillman (1983) interpreted arousal as undifferentiated
acters). One mechanism that has been offered to explain sympathetic activation.
the mere exposure effect is familiarity: we come to like An issue in need of resolution is whether emotions can
things that are familiar to us, perhaps because of feelings occur without arousal. Cognitive theories of emotions
of security (Zajonc 1968). However, a full theoretical (e.g., appraisal theories) seem to allow that emotions can
explanation for the mere exposure effect has not been be produced by cognition alone, without arousal (e.g., Par-
developed. When a person is exposed to meaningful stim- rott 1988). But does arousal always accompany the experi-
uli, it has been more difficult to produce the mere exposure ence of emotions?
effect (e.g., Obermiller 1985). This is, in part, a conse- Recent research suggests that arousal is an essential
quence of the cognitive processing that occurs in response component of emotion and is manifest in neural systems in
Bagozziet al. / THE ROLEOFEMOTIONS 193

the brain. LeDoux (1996) reviews evidence suggesting attitudes and beliefs were explained by the implications of
that there are at least five arousal systems in the brain con- spreading activation effects of arousal and coping
tributing complexly to emotional experience. Four of these responses, wherein individuals attempt to avoid negative
are in regions of the brain stem and rely respectively on and facilitate positive associations of beliefs with atti-
acetylcholine, noradrenaline, dopamine, and serotonin for tudes. In another study, Bagozzi (1996) found that for atti-
activation. A fifth (the nucleus basalis) is in the forebrain tudes toward giving blood, high arousal tends to enhance a
and also relies on acetylcholine to arouse cortical cells. halo effect from attitudes to positive beliefs and reduce the
LeDoux notes that the arousal systems act in nonspecific halo from attitudes to negative beliefs about the conse-
ways throughout the forebrain to make cells more sensitive quences of giving blood.
to incoming signals. In a sense, the nonspecific arousal Clearly, arousal is a fundamental aspect of behavior
interacts with the information processing of a particular related to emotions. We must acknowledge that appraisal
stimulus. The amygdala acts as a kind of central processor theories have not done a good, or at least complete, job of
and interacts with the prefrontal cortex (working memory incorporating arousal into their frameworks. In their
and attention), hippocampus (long-term explicit memory), defense, however, we should mention the following.
and sensory cortex (perception and short-term storage) to Appraisal theorists recognize that the intensity of emo-
influence emotional responses. The amygdala not only tional experience consists of two components: arousal and
influences cortical areas of the brain but also receives input self-control (e.g., Frijda 1994:120). Likewise, researchers
from arousal networks (which themselves also influence accept that "autonomic nervous system and other physio-
the forebrain) and feedback from bodily expression of logical processes" at least accompany subjectively felt
emotions. In addition, signals from the amygdala are sent to emotions (e.g., Oatley 1992: 21) and that"[i]f the criterion
muscles and internal organs and glands. of physiological activity was eliminated from the defini-
Most of the research to date into the role of arousat sys- tion, the concept of emotion would be left without one of
tems and the amygdala in emotional behavior has been the important response boundaries with which to distin-
limited to a small number of emotions (e.g., fear). But it is guish it from nonemotion" (Lazarus 1991:58-59). But is
believed that each emotional response is mediated by sepa- there more to arousal in emotion than this?
rate neural systems, although each may overlap or resem- Some psychologists and marketers have been quick to
ble each other in many respects. In sum, LeDoux (1996) dismiss Zajonc's (1980) claim that "preferences need no
and other brain researchers (e.g., Damasio 1994) construe inferences" (see Cohen and Areni 1991:215-216; Lazarus
emotions as biological functions of the nervous system 1982; Marcel 1983; Zajonc 1984). However, we believe it
(see also Zajonc 1998). is important, at the present, to recognize that emotional
meanings can be processed subconsciously, emotions can
To our knowledge, there has been little work to either
be activated automatically, and responses to emotions
integrate or reconcile cognitive theories of emotions with
(e.g., coping, action tendencies, actions) also can occur
neural and biological theories. Much remains to be done in
automatically. We leave open the possibility that "emotion
psychological research before we can make definitive
and cognition are best thought of as separate but interact-
statements about the precise role of arousal in emotional
ing mental functions mediated by separate but interacting
experience and behavior.
brain systems" (LeDoux 1996:69; see also Oatley 1992,
At least three studies have examined limited aspects of chap. I). It appears that arousal is a key part of emotional
arousal in marketing-related contexts. Sanbonmatsu and functions in the brain that underlies much of its automatic-
Kardes (1988) found that arousal may govern attitude for- ity. Cognitive appraisals and arousal need to be better
mation in persuasive message settings. Attitudes were incorporated into our theories of emotion.
based on peripheral cues when respondents were highly
aroused but on argument strength when they were moder-
ately aroused. It is unclear whether arousal functioned EMOTIONS AS MARKERS, MEDIATORS,
here to reduce information-processing capacity of external AND MODERATORS OF CONSUMER
arguments or focused attention on internal reactions. RESPONSES
Bagozzi (1994) found that consistent with predictions
by knowledge-assembly theory (Hayes-Roth 1977), Based on content, most advertisements can be divided
arousal transformed a two-dimensional, affective- into two categories: (a) thinking ads, where focus is placed
cognitive representation of evaluations of giving blood on either factual information (e.g., product attributes) or
into a one-dimensional, unitized representation. Likewise, utilitarian consequences of product/service use (e.g., sav-
arousal increased the association between attitudes and ings in time or money) or (b) feeling ads, where concentra-
positive beliefs about the consequences of giving blood tion is placed on the emotions one wiI1experience through
and decreased the association between attitudes and nega- use or ownership of a product (see Puto and Wells's [ 1984]
tive beliefs. These predictions on the associations between similar distinction between informational and
194 JOURNALOF THE ACADEMYOF MARKETINGSCIENCE SPRING 1999

transformational advertising). Rather than focusing on the A~ than thoughts (e.g., how informative or useful the ad
stimulus, per se, it is more important to emphasize the is), but under higher involvement viewing, both feelings
processes and experiences comprising a person's response and thoughts may be important (e.g., Miniard, Bhatla, and
to ads, to better understand the emotional meaning of ads Rose 1990). Presumably, high involvement promotes cog-
(e.g., Friestad and Thorson 1986). nitive processing of the usefulness of the ad and its
Paralleling the above differentiation between types of content.
ads and the emotional-cognitive division in mental A majority of research has addressed the effects of A~,
processes mentioned earlier, Batra and Ray (1986) devel- especially on attitudes toward the brand (Ab). Classic atti-
oped a framework and coding scheme for classifying tude theory maintains that A bis a function of beliefs about
affective responses to ads, as a complement to cognitive brand attributes or consequences of product use. Mitchell
responses. Specifically, Batra and Ray (1986) identified and Olson (1981) and Shimp (1981) were the first to find
three positive affective response categories: surgency- that A~ provided additional explanatory power for A bover
elation-vigor/activation (SEVA), deactivation, and social and above brand beliefs (see also Edell and Burke 1987).
affection feelings. The SEVA feelings refer to upbeat, Batra and Ray (1986) found, however, that affective
happy mood reactions (e.g., "the ad's music was 'catchy,' responses toward the ad influenced A b only indirectly
the ad was 'fun to watch or breezy,' o r . . . a likable use of through Am.
humor"); deactivation includes soothing, relaxing, quiet, Some research has addressed the conditions under
or pleasing reactions; and social affection encompasses which Aadinfluences A~. For example, Brown and Stayman
feelings of warmth, tenderness, and caring (Batra and Ray (1992) revealed in their meta-analysis that the effects of
1986:241). The three positive affective responses were Aadon Abare greater for novel than well-known brands and
used along with six cognitive responses (i.e., support argu- for durable and other goods versus nondurables. Some evi-
ments, counterarguments, execution discounting, execu- dence also exists showing that Aadinfluences A bindirectly
tion bolstering, neutral distracters, and other reactions) in through its effect on beliefs about the brand (MacKenzie et
a study of the impact of television commercials on con- al. 1986). Finally, Stayman and Aaker (1988) showed that
sumers. A total of 12 percent of reactions to ads were clas- repetition governs the feelings to Aa~ relationship. Under
sified as positive affect: SEVA (3.7%), deactivation levels of low versus high repetition, feelings have a
(2.5%), and social affect (6.1%). stronger effect on Aad. This may be a consequence of
One use of emotional reactions in the above sense greater information processing under high versus low
might be as markers or indicators of the effectiveness of repetition.
advertising copy, particularly with respect to the overall In addition to the transfer of affect from ad emotions to
persuasiveness of the ad, the appeal of spokespersons, A d, research shows that brand names and feelings toward
evaluation of particular product claims, and appraisals of ads can become linked in memory. Stayman and Batra
other aspects of the execution (Wiles and Cornwell 1990). (1991) found that respondents exposed to an affective, as
Also, the program surrounding an ad (e.g., happy versus opposed to an argument, ad were able to retrieve brand atti-
sad content) has been found to have main effects on one's tudes faster, when primed with the brand name. In addi-
evaluation of an ad and recall (e.g., Goldberg and Gorn tion, the positive retrieved affect had a stronger influence
1987). Future research is needed to identify how program on choice in low- than in high-involvement contexts. In a
content and advertising appeals interact to influence con- second study, Stayman and Batra demonstrated that view-
sumer emotional responses. ers of an ad who were in a positive affective state more
A research question that has received quite a bit of strongly evoked the affect when given the brand name as a
attention in recent years is how and to what extent emo- subsequent retrieval cue than viewers exposed to the ad
tional reactions to ads influence consumer decision mak- while not in a positive affect state.
ing. Most often these reactions have been measured as atti- Olney, Holbrook, and B atra (1991 ) investigated a hier-
tudes (e.g., liking) toward the ad (e.g., Brown and Stayman archical model explaining advertising viewing time. The
1992; Mitchell and Olson 1981; Shimp 1981). effects of ad emotions (i.e., pleasure and arousal) were
Attitude toward the ad (A d) is thought to be a function mediated by A d and reactions to ad content. Interestingly,
of feelings (and thoughts) about the ad itself (e.g., Batra arousal had both indirect and direct effects on viewing
and Ray 1986; MacKenzie, Lutz, and Belch 1986). In fact, time, even after controlling for ad content and Aad.Arousal
Batra and Ray (1986) found that the three affective was measured by self-reports.
responses discussed above significantly predicted A deven Emotions have been found to serve as moderators in
after controlling for the effects of cognitive responses. A their impact on A b. Batra and Stayman (1990), in one of the
number of researchers have examined the conditions few studies to examine mood and print ads, found that
under which emotions influence A~. For instance, under positive moods enhance A b through their interaction with
low-involvement viewing conditions, feelings about the two cognitive processes: "(1) a bias against the generation
ad have been found to be more important determinants of of negative thoughts (such as evoked by weak arguments),
Bagozziet al. f THEROLEOF EMOTIONS 195

leading to a more favorable evaluation of message argu- in a study by Wegener, Petty, and Smith (1995). Wegener
ments, and (2) a reduction in total cognitive elaboration, et al. (1995) discovered that happy (versus sad) moods
making processing more heuristic than systematic" (pp. lead to the processing of more arguments in a message
212-213). An interesting finding was that positive moods when a "proattitudinal/uplifting" position was taken, but
seem to reduce counterargumentation when weak argu- happy (versus sad) moods lead to less processing of argu-
ments are used in ads (see also Worth and Mackie 1987). ments when a "counterattitudinal/depression" position
Batra and Stephens (1994) also investigated the moderat- was taken. Wegener et al. (1995) explain the findings, in
ing effects of mood on A~. Specifically, they showed that part, by suggesting that when in a happy mood, people try
mood and motivation (the latter conceived as degree of to maintain their mood and thus process less of the coun-
relevance of the product category for consumers) inter- terattitudinal/depression content.
acted to govern A b when consumers watched television In addition, interactions of emotions sometimes occur
ads. The greatest impact on A~ occurred when positive with other variables, such as motivational or ability fac-
moods ensued under conditions of low motivation. The tors. For instance, Wegener, Petty, and Klein (1994) found
rationale is that positive moods and low motivation sup- that for people high in need for cognition, messages
press counterargumentation (and therefore lead to more framed positively (i.e., good things will happen if people
favorable Ab) in comparison with high-motivation adopt the advocacy) were more persuasive when the audi-
conditions. ence was happy (versus sad), but messages framed nega-
Following the elaboration likelihood model (ELM) tively (i.e., bad things will happen if people do not adopt
(e.g., Petty and Cacioppo 1986), we might expect emo- the advocacy) were more persuasive when the audience
tions to have one or both of two effects. Emotions might was sad (versus happy). Wegener et al. (1994) explained
operate centrally to influence cognitive processes (e.g., these findings by claiming that good outcomes seem better
with regard to argumentation), or emotions might function and more likely to occur while in a good (versus sad)
peripherally (perhaps through associative or affect transfer mood, and bad outcomes seem worse and more likely to
mechanisms). Early predictions under the ELM took an occur while in a sad (versus happy) mood.
either-or perspective and stressed that when the processing Finally, even when the likelihood of information pro-
of information in a communication is low (e.g., due to low cessing is low (due, e.g., to low need for cognition or low
motivation, distraction, low need for cognition, weak argu- relevance of a product for a consumer), the mood of the
ments), emotional content in the communication (e.g., an
audience can have a direct effect on A b,but when the likeli-
attractive spokesperson) is processed directly and trans-
hood of information processing is high, the mood of the
fers to, or influences, attitude toward the product or mes-
audience affected message-generated thoughts consistent
sage. When issue-relevant thinking is high, attitude
with the mood (Petty, Schumann, Richman, and
change is thought to be a function of the balance of pros
Strathman 1993). Mood was induced by a television pro-
and cons in the communication, a largely rational process,
gram or music.
and emotion may not be a factor.
Over time, the role of emotion in persuasive communi-
cation has been found to be more complex than the simple THE EFFECTS OF EMOTION 1:
central versus peripheral processing alternatives specified INFLUENCES ON COGNITIVE
in the ELM (e.g., Wegener and Petty 1996). For example, PROCESSES
when the likelihood of information processing is moder-
ate, emotions have been found to affect the extent to which A person's emotional state can influence various
arguments in a communication become elaborated. Posi- aspects of information processing including encoding and
tive (versus neutral) moods tend to lead to less processing retrieval of information, different strategies used to
of arguments (e.g., Bless, Bohner, Schwarz, and Strack process information, evaluations and judgments, and crea-
1990; see also discussion below on the effects of emotions tive thinking. In this section we examine the influence of
on cognitive processes). By contrast, when people process affective states on various aspects of cognitive processes.
the arguments in a message closely, mood might bias
information processing or even function as an argument
itself (Forgas 1995). Another explanation for mood effects Emotion/Mood Effects on Memory
is that sad or neutral (versus happy) moods lead to more
effortful processing, which is believed to be done sponta- The influence of mood states on memory can be
neously (e.g., Bohner, Chaiken, and Hunyadi 1994). Note broadly classified into three categories: retrieval effects,
that this prediction seems to conflict with the observations encoding effects, and state-dependent learning effects. In
made by Schaller and Cialdini (1990), who analyzed the following section, we review some of the studies that
mostly nonpersuasive communication studies. Some clari- have investigated these effects and discuss the mechanism
fication for the discrepancy in interpretations can be seen by which affect influences memory.
196 JOURNALOF THE ACADEMYOF MARKETINGSCIENCE SPRING 1999

Retrieval effects. Affect has been shown to influence re- information, and they subsequently recalled the negative
trieval of information, whereby persons in a positive mood information better. Conversely, happy individuals spent
state at the time of retrieval have been found to show supe- more time on the positive information and recalled that
rior recall of positive material learned during encoding, better at a later time. The greater levels of associations
relative to neutral or negative material (Isen, Shalker, evoked by mood-congruent material may have caused a
Clark, and Karp 1978; Laird, Wagener, Halal, and Szegda more extensive elaboration, which, in turn, requires more
1982; Nasby and Yando 1982; Teasdale and Russell 1983). time. However, Isen et al. (1978) and Srull (1983) failed to
For example, Isen et al. (1978) had respondents study posi- find any encoding effects of affect.
tive, negative, and neutral words. Either positive, neutral,
or negative mood states were induced in these respon- State-dependent learning effects. A third memory ef-
dents. Respondents in the positive-mood condition re- fect of mood is the state-dependent learning effect of af-
trieved more positive words compared with neutral or fect, where any material regardless of its affective valence
negative words. Isen et al. (1978) suggest that thinking learned under a particular mood state is recalled better
about mood-incongruent material involves shifting one's when the person is again in that affective state (Bartlett,
focus, which is cognitively taxing, and therefore people Burleson, and Santrock 1982; Bartlett and Santrock 1979;
are more likely to focus on mood-congruent material. An- Bower, Monteiro, and Gilligan 1978; Bower, Gilligan, and
other mechanism proposed to explain the retrieval effects Monteiro 1981). Bower et al. (1978) had respondents learn
of positive affect suggests that positive mood at the time of two sets of words, one while they were in a positive affec-
retrieval functions as a cue that primes the positive mate- tive state and the other in a negative mood. When respon-
rial in memory, making these material more accessible dents who learned the two lists in different moods recalled
(Isen 1989; Isen et al. 1978; also see Tulving and Pearl- the words in the wrong mood (e.g., when words learned in
stone 1966 for a discussion on the effects of priming on ac- a positive mood were recalled while respondents were in a
cessibility). The easier accessibility of positive material negative mood), they experienced interference and the av-
may then influence other cognitive processes such as erage recall rate was less than 50 percent. When respon-
evaluations and decision making, and also subsequent be- dents who learned two lists in different moods recalled the
haviors. While retrieval effects have been replicated by words in the correct mood, the average recall rate was
many researchers using different mood induction and test- more than 70 percent. Control respondents who learned
ing techniques, a few prominent studies have failed to de- and recalled both lists while in the same mood showed an
tect retrieval effects (Bower, Monteiro, and Gilligan 1978; average recall rate between 50 and 60 percent. Bower and
Bower, Gilligan, and Monteiro 1981), leading Isen (1984) Cohen (1982) suggest that the respondents' mood at the
to speculate that this failure may have been caused by the time of learning becomes associated with the learned ma-
specific material and induction methods (such as hypno- terial and that these associations facilitate the recall of
sis) used in these studies. learned material when the mood state at recall matches the
mood state at encoding (also see Bower 1981 for a descrip-
Encoding effects. Mood states have also been shown to tion of his semantic-network theory).
exhibit encoding effects whereby the affective state at the Evidence for mood state-dependent learning has been
time of learning is associated with superior memory for ambiguous. Many studies have failed to find any state-
similarly valenced material (Bower and Cohen 1982; For-
dependent effects of mood state (Bower and Mayer 1985;
gas and Bower 1987). Nasby and Yando (1982) found that
Isen et al. 1978; Laird et al. 1982; Nasby and Yando 1982).
positive mood at the time of learning led to an improved re-
Eich and Birnbaum (1982) and Isen (1984, 1989) have
call of positive material at a later point in time regardless of
suggested that when the material to be learned has seman-
the mood state at the time of recall. Bower et al. (1981)
tic meaning, the stimulus will be encoded according to this
found evidence for the encoding effect of both positive and meaning, and the influence of the mood state in the encod-
negative affect. Respondents were made to feel happy or
ing and subsequent retrieval processes will be minimal.
sad and then read descriptions of various psychiatric inter- However, when the stimulus lacks meaning, contextual
views. Happy respondents learned many more happy facts cues such as affective states at the time of learning may be
than sad facts, while sad respondents learned many more more strongly encoded with the learned material. At the
sad facts than happy facts. time of retrieval, these memory items, which have few
How can the encoding effects of mood be explained? semantic associations, are more primed by the matching
Bower and colleagues (e.g., Bower and Cohen 1982) have affective state at recall.
suggested that mood-congruent material is likely to be
more semantically elaborated relative to mood- Asymmetric effects of positive and negative moods.
incongruent material. Forgas and Bower (1987) found that While positive affective states have been shown to have
in impression formation situations, sad individuals spent significant influences on recall, negative affect has some-
more time examining negative rather than positive times been found to have either no effect or a much smaller
Bagozziet al. / THE ROLEOF EMOTIONS 197

effect on the recall of negative material from memory. states, compared with those in neutral or negative mood
Asymmetric effects of positive and negative moods have states, tend to be better at integrating information, finding
been found for retrieval effects (Isen et al. 1978; Nasby and relationships among stimuli, and at finding creative solu-
Yando 1982; Teasdale and Fogarty 1979), encoding effects tions (Isen and Daubman 1984; Isen, Daubman, and
(Nasby and Yando 1982), and state-dependent learning ef- Nowicki, 1987; Isen, Johnson, Mertz, and Robinson 1985;
fects (Bartlett and Santrock 1979; Bartlett et al. 1982). Isen Isen, Niedenthal, and Cantor 1992; Murray, Sujan, Hirt,
(1984) speculated that positive affect is structured in a and Sujan 1990). For example, respondents in positive
broad and extensive manner (i.e., highly interconnected mood conditions tended to group a wider range of neutral
with other memories), while negative affect is more nar- stimuli together (Isen and Daubman 1984). They also
rowly and less well connected with other material, and that rated words such as cane, ring, and purse as being better
specific negative affective states such as anger and sadness exemplars of the category "clothing," than did the neutral-
may be organized separately in memory. This, in turn, mood respondents. Murray et al. (1990) found that
would make it difficult for any given negative mood to act positive-mood respondents, compared with respondents
as an effective retrieval cue. It is not readily apparent why in other mood states, formed broader categories when
negative affect would be less well connected in memory focusing on similarities among exemplars and narrower
and positive affect more widely interconnected as Isen categories when focusing on differences, prompting them
suggests, considering that negative affect usually signals to suggest that what positive mood promotes is not broader
problematic environmental conditions that may require categorization but rather cognitive flexibility. Isen et al.
problem solving (Schwarz and Clore 1983; Wegener et al. (1985) found that positive-mood respondents tended to
1995). One of the side effects of this hypothesized in- give more unusual responses to neutral words in word
terconnectedness of positive memories is the greater crea- association tests. For example, in response to the word
tiveness and cognitive flexibility demonstrated by people house, positive-mood respondents were more likely than
in positive moods, a topic we review briefly later in this were neutral-mood respondents to mention unusual first
article. associates such as security, residence, and apartment, sug-
Mood maintenance and repair have also been put for- gesting that positive-mood states may influence cognitive
ward as explanations for the asymmetric effects of positive organization, resulting in more flexible interpretation of
and negative moods. Isen (1984) has suggested that happy relationships among stimuli. This cognitive flexibility also
individuals attempt to prolong their positive affective state results in enhanced creativity. Isen et al. (1987) found that
by focusing on the positive aspects of their stimulus (mood respondents in positive-mood states outperformed those in
maintenance), while individuals in a negative mood try to neutral- and negative-mood states on tests requiring crea-
improve their situation by not focusing on negative memo- tive solutions. These researchers suggest that positive-
ries (mood repair). Isen (1989) noted that in some of the mood respondents were better at creative problem solving
studies showing symmetrical effects of positive and nega- since such tasks required the ability to see relatedness
tive mood (Bower et al. 1978, 1981), respondents were among seemingly unrelated stimuli, and as we have seen
instructed to maintain their induced moods, and this may earlier, positive affect results in more flexible cognitive
have discouraged them from engaging in mood repair organization.
strategies. However, the mood repair explanation is not
without problems. If sad respondents engage in mood Mood Effects on Evaluation
repair, why is there no evidence for mood-incongruent
recall effects? After all, an effective strategy to improve a One of the best-recognized and most robust effects of
depressed mood state is to engage in pleasant thoughts and mood is its influence on evaluation. Individuals in
memories. Yet, evidence seems to show that negative positive-mood states have been shown to evaluate stimuli
mood inhibits the recall of positive memories (e.g., Isen et more positively than individuals in neutral- or negative-
al. 1978). The competing explanations of memory struc- mood states, whether the stimuli being studied are other
ture differences and mood maintenance/repair have also people (Clore and Byrne 1974; Forgas and Bower 1987),
been used to explain differences in information- consumer goods (Isen et al. 1978; Srull 1983), life satis-
processing strategies of happy and sad individuals, a topic faction (Schwarz and Clore 1983), or past life events
we discuss later in this article. (Clark and Teasdale 1982). The reliability of mood effects
on evaluation is perhaps best illustrated by the fact that
evaluations of the pleasantness of neutral/ambiguous
Mood, Categorization, and Creativity stimuli are used as checks for mood manipulation (e.g.,
Isen et al. 1985, 1987). Isen et al. (1978) found that respon-
Various studies investigating the influence of mood on dents in whom positive mood was induced were more
categorization have found that people in positive mood likely to rate their cars and televisions more favorably
198 JOURNALOFTHEACADEMYOF MARKETINGSCIENCE SPRING1999

compared with respondents in neutral-mood states. In the Kommer, and Wagner (1987). We discuss this and other
case of memory-based evaluations, if the recalled infor- related issues in greater detail later in this article.
mation is biased by the mood (as discussed in the follow-
ing section), then evaluations that follow will be biased Mood Effects on Information Processing
too. On-line processing of evaluations can also be influ-
enced by mood states through the retrieval of information It has been suggested that the mood maintenance strat-
congruent with the mood (Clore, Schwarz, and Conway egy used by people in positive moods may also cause them
1994). to avoid investing cognitive effort in tasks unless doing so
A competing explanation based on the feelings-as- promises to maintain or enhance their positive mood (e.g.,
information model (see Schwarz 1990; Schwarz and Bless Isen 1987; Wegener et al. 1995). Consequently, people in
1991; Schwarz and Clore 1983) suggests that individuals positive-mood states may not be motivated to engage in
may assume that their mood states are affective reactions systematic processing of information and may use heuris-
to the object being evaluated and thus base their evalua- tic processing instead. Positive affect usually denotes a
tions on their affective states. For example, a happy indi- benign environment that does not require any action. On
vidual when asked to evaluate a painting may ask the ques- the other hand, negative affective states act as information
tion, "How do I feel?" and infer that his or her positive signaling that the environment poses a problem and may
mood is a reaction to the painting and therefore come to the motivate people to engage in systematic processing, which
conclusion that he or she likes the painting. The feelings- is usually better suited to handling threatening situations
as-information hypothesis suggests that when individuals (Schwarz 1990; Schwarz and Clore 1983). Various stud-
attribute their mood state to something else other than the ies have found evidence suggesting the use of heuristic
object being evaluated, the effect of mood on evaluation processing by people in positive moods and systematic
should disappear. Schwarz and Clore (1983) found sup- processing by people in negative moods (e.g., Bless et al.
port for this hypothesis when they showed that although 1990, 1996; Mackie and Worth 1989). Bless et al. (1990)
people called on sunny days reported more life satisfaction presented happy and sad individuals with either strong or
than people called on cloudy days, the differences disap- weak counter-attitudinal arguments. Sad individuals were
peared when the interviewer casually mentioned the influenced only by strong arguments, while happy indi-
weather to the individuals. Presumably, the casual mention viduals were equally influenced by strong and weak argu-
of the weather made people attribute their mood to the ments. These effects have been consistently replicated and
weather, and hence the mood lost any diagnostic value in have been interpreted as providing evidence for reduced
evaluating life satisfaction. In other words, people may use systematic processing by individuals in positive affective
their moods as the basis for forming evaluations of objects states. The two major mechanisms that have been used to
unless the diagnostic value of the mood is discounted. explain mood effects on information-processing strategies
Clore et al. (1994) compared the two explanations for are the same as those used to explain the asymmetric
mood effects on evaluation and suggested that individuals effects of mood effects on memory, namely, the highly
may use feelings as information when the evaluation task interconnected nature of positive memories and mood
is affective in nature, when other information is lacking, maintenance.
when the information is complex, or when there are time In discussing the asymmetric effects of positive and
constraints. negative affect on memory, we had briefly discussed Isen's
Although mood effects on evaluation have been repli- (1984) contention that positive concepts are more highly
cated often, a few prominent studies have shown that mood interconnected in memory relative to negative ideas. Isen
states will not influence evaluation when the object being (1987) and Mackie and Worth (1989) have argued that
evaluated is highly familiar and for which past evaluations since positive memories are highly interconnected, posi-
exist in memory (e.g., Salovey and Birnbaum 1989; Srull tive mood will prime and activate some related and many
1983, 1984). Srull (1984, Experiment 3) found that the unrelated positive memories, thus leading to cognitive
evaluations of a car made by novices, but not by experts, capacity constraints. Because individuals do not have the
were influenced by mood state. Novices, by definition, are cognitive resources to engage in systematic processing,
unfamiliar with the product category and are more likely to they resort to the less demanding heuristic processing.
engage in on-line evaluations, which are more susceptible A second explanation for mood effects on cognitive
to mood influences. Experts, on the other hand, have prior processing is based on the concept of mood maintenance.
evaluations available in memory and so do not engage in Isen (1987) has suggested that individuals in a positive
on-line evaluations and are less likely to be influenced by affective state are motivated to maintain their mood and
mood. Similar findings have been obtained by Salovey and may avoid cognitive activity that could interfere with their
Birnbaum (1989, Experiment 3) and Schwarz, Strack, positive mood. Thus, individuals in a happy mood are
Bagozziet al. / THEROLEOFEMOTIONS 199

unlikely to engage in systematic processing of information when the judgments require a high degree of constructive
(also see Bohner, Crow, Erb, and Schwarz 1992). (on-line) processing. Affect does not influence those judg-
Another motivation-based explanation for the reduced ments that call for the retrieval of preexisting evaluations
processing under positive mood has been offered by or for information processing that is highly specific and
Schwarz and colleagues (e.g., Schwarz and Clore 1983) not requiring constructive processing. Forgas (1994,
using their feelings-as-information hypothesis. Negative 1995) suggests that direct-access processing (a low-affect
affective states inform people that they may be facing a infusion strategy) is used when the evaluative target is
problem and this may provoke systematic processing of familiar and when there are no strong cognitive, affective,
information that is better suited to problem solving. Posi- or situational factors that call for systematic processing.
tive mood, on the other hand, informs the individual that Accordingly, individuals who evaluate very familiar
the environment is benign and thus he or she may not be objects would be using direct-access processing, and
motivated to engage in effortful cognitive processing. A hence mood effects would not be found in such cases (cf.
fourth hypothesis suggests that the motivation to simplify Srull 1984). Another low-affect infusion strategy is moti-
processing is not what causes the reliance on heuristic vated processing, where the information search and the
processing; rather, it is the increased use of heuristic pro- evaluative outcome are guided by prior motivational goals
cessing by these happy-mood individuals that results in such as mood repair (see Forgas 1995 for a detailed discus-
simplified processing (e.g., Bless et al. 1996). sion). When the evaluative target is simple, the personal
Although the increased reliance on heuristics by indi- relevance is low, the individual has limited cognitive
viduals in positive moods has been replicated many times, capacity, or the accuracy requirements are not high, the
there have been studies that have shown that positive individual may resort to heuristic processing, a high-affect
moods could lead to both increased or diminished levels of infusion strategy. Under these circumstances, evaluations
cognitive processing (e.g., Martin, Ward, Achee, and Wyer may be based on the existing mood, as in the feelings-as-
1993). Wegener et al. (1995) have presented empirical information hypothesis (cf. Schwarz and Bless 1991).
support for their hypothesis that positive moods could lead When the judgmental situation requires the individual to
to either lesser or greater levels of information processing, learn and process novel information, and when he or she
depending on whether happy individuals believe that sys- has adequate cognitive capacity and motivation to process
tematic processing will lead to the maintenance or destruc- the information, substantive processing is predicted to
tion of their positive mood. People in a positive-mood take place. According to Forgas (1995), this default strat-
state, who believe that systematic processing of a message egy, which is used if other less effortful strategies are
would help maintain their mood, may engage in more inadequate, involves constructive processing and is one
detailed processing. However, if they view systematic that is susceptible to affect infusion (cf. Srull 1984).
processing of that message as a threat to their positive-
mood state, they would avoid elaborate processing. This
THE EFFECTS OF EMOTIONS 2:
hedonic contingency mood management is especially INFLUENCE ON VOLITIONS,
likely to be seen in positive-mood individuals who have GOAL-DIRECTED BEHAVIOR,
the most to lose by incorrectly engaging in systematic AND DECISIONS TO HELP
processing. Sad individuals, by contrast, have less to lose,
since processing messages might make them feel better Sometimes emotions spur one onto action; at other
(Wegener and Petty 1994). Wegener et al. (1995) suggest times emotions inhibit or constrain action. But only
that most of the previous research in which positive moods recently have researchers devoted much attention to study-
have been shown to lead to reduced message processing ing how this occurs.
have involved counterattitudinal or otherwise unpleasant Bagozzi, Baumgartner, and Pieters (1998) investigated
cognitions (e.g., Mackie and Worth 1989). Since the sys- the role of anticipatory emotions in goal striving. In a
tematic processing of counterattitudinal or unpleasant panel study of consumers' efforts to lose or maintain their
messages is unlikely to help maintain a positive mood, body weight, people first reacted to the possibility of
individuals choose not to use systematic processing. achieving or not achieving their goals with well-defined
Forgas (1994, 1995), building on Fiedler's (1990) positive and negative anticipatory emotions. The anticipa-
dual-force model, has recently presented the affect infu- tory emotions then energized volitions in the form of
sion model, which suggests that the various explanations intentions, plans, and the decision to expend energy in the
for mood effects on memory and information processing service of goal striving. Volitions, in turn, were shown to
(such as mood maintenance, feelings as information, influence goal-directed behaviors related to exercising
affect priming) are not necessarily competing models but and dieting. Next, the intensity of execution of these
could rather be complementary explanations. He suggests behaviors contributed to degree of goal attainment. Goal-
that affect infusion into judgmental processes takes place outcome emotions subsequently followed.
200 JOURNALOF THE ACADEMYOF MARKETINGSCIENCE SPRING 1999

Anticipatory emotions functioned in the following sales negotiation). With embarrassment, the threat is to the
manner. Respondents indicated which emotions they "presented self," and one feels foolish and awkward; with
anticipated they would experience if they were to achieve shame, the threat is to the "core self," and one feels quite
their goal (i.e., excitement, delight, happiness, gladness, pained, perhaps even humiliated or devastated. Verbeke
satisfaction, pride, and self-assurance) and which emo- and B agozzi (1998) found that salespersons' propensity to
tions they anticipated they would experience if they were experience shame and embarrassment led to particular
to fail to achieve their goal (i.e., anger, frustration, guilt, protective or coping responses: the greater the shame and
shame, sadness, disappointment, depression, worry, embarrassment, the greater the impulse to hide, to with-
uncomfortableness, and fear). The key processes are simi- draw, and to avoid contact with customers. These coping
lar to forward-looking counterfactual thinking processes responses, in turn, negatively influenced sales
(e.g., Roese and Olson 1995) or what Gleicher et al. (1995) performance.
term "prefactuals" The decision maker begins by "imag- The study by Verbeke and Bagozzi (1998) illustrates
ining the possible" (i.e., identifying and evaluating the dysfunctional effects of negative emotions (see also Ver-
consequences occurring if one were to achieve one's goal beke and Bagozzi 1999). Yet, negative emotions can have
or not). The alternative consequences to imagined goal pro-social consequences in certain instances. Bagozzi and
success and goal failure serve as input to appraisal Moore (1994) investigated the role of negative emotions
processes and felt emotional experiences. People are then and empathy on decisions to give help to abused children.
motivated to choose actions promoting the positive affect In Study 1, the effects of a negative emotional-appeal ad
and avoiding the negative affect associated with goal were compared with those of a rational-appeal ad; in
attainment and goal failure, respectively. Study 2, the effects of three levels of intensity of negative-
Four other studies support the above role for prefactuals emotion appeals were examined. The negative emotional
in decision making. Boninger, Gleicher, Hetts, Armor, and ads featured a young boy in his home running away from
Moore (1994) discovered that prefactuals with regard to his father in terror. Both studies demonstrated that four
the purchase of insurance in a laboratory game influenced negative emotions (anger, sadness, fear, tension) and four
subsequent action to take insurance. Gleicher et al. (1995) aspects of empathy (perspective taking, compassion/pity,
found that prefactuals concerning condom use affected protection motivation, fantasy elaboration) mediated the
positive attitudes; however, intentions to use condoms effects of viewing negative emotional ads on decisions to
were not influenced by prefactuals, although the differ- help. Stronger felt negative emotions in the audience led to
ence in means between experimental and control groups greater feelings of empathy, and this, in turn, enhanced the
was in the predicted direction. Perugini and Bagozzi decision to help victims of child abuse.
(1998) also showed that anticipated emotions were instru-
A final area where emotions play a key role in promot-
mental in influencing desires, volitions, and behavior in a
ing action is in goal setting. We can think of goal setting in
study of exercising and an investigation of studying behav-
terms of a hierarchy of three classes of goals (Bagozzi and
iors (see also Parker, Manstead, and Stradling 1995; Rich-
Dholakia 1998; Pieters, Baumgartner, and Allen 1995). A
ard, van der Pligt, and de Vries 1995). Finally, Brown,
focal or basic-level goal answers the question, "What is it
Cron, and Slocum (1997) replicated the model proposed
that I strive for?" For example, a consumer may have a goal
by Bagozzi, Baumgartner, and Pieters (1998) in a study of
to lose body weight. The means for achieving this goal can
goal pursuit by salespeople.
Although emotions often function in broad categories be interpreted as subordinate goals and answer the ques-
of positive and negative affect, specific subcategories may tion, "How can I achieve that for which I strive?" Various
have special relevance in certain contexts. Verbeke and types of exercising behaviors and dieting activities are
Bagozzi (1998) studied the behavior of 458 salespeople in examples of subordinate goals for the focal goal of losing
Europe who sold financial services and found that two weight. Superordinate goals constitute reasons for pursu-
negative emotions were particularly important: shame and ing a focal goal and answer the question, "Why do I want to
embarrassment. These self-conscious emotions encom- achieve that for which I strive?" Bagozzi and Edwards
pass personal standards with regard to acceptable (1998) investigated the hierarchical structure for super-
thoughts, feelings, and actions. Shame occurs when one ordinate goals governing body weight maintenance and
fails to live up to an ideal and another person whose found that happiness was an important goal toward which
approval is important to oneself judges this failure criti- the focal goal and other more concrete superordinate goals
cally. Embarrassment is generated either when the self pointed (other superordinate goals included, for example,
does something inappropriate in public or the other with social acceptance, self-esteem, look good, feel good, and
whom one interacts does something inappropriate, but in fit into clothes). Happiness, or more generally personal
contrast to shame, the threat under embarrassment is less welfare, seems to be a central motive spurring one onto
fundamental and involves relatively trivial and even action. We turn now to the role of happiness and other
humorous behaviors (e.g., passing gas audibly while in a emotions in customer satisfaction.
Bagozzi et al. / THE ROLEOF EMOTIONS 201

EMOTIONS AND CUSTOMER under which satisfaction exists uniquely from many other
SATISFACTION positive emotions. We suspect that previous studies find-
ing discriminant validity for measures of satisfaction can
Early research viewed customer satisfaction as a key be explained by the way items were presented on the ques-
outcome of product/service purchase, whereby a compari- tionnaire (e.g., separation of measures of satisfaction from
son is made between expectations of performance and measures of other positive emotions) or the lack of inclu-
actual performance, and satisfaction arises when actual sion of a sufficient number of positive emotions. No study
performance is greater than or equal to expected perfor- to date has performed either a rigorous experimental or
mance, and dissatisfaction occurs otherwise (e.g., Oliver survey (e.g., multitrait-multimethod) examination of con-
1980; Yi 1990). Westbrook (1987) explored the influence struct validity of measures of satisfaction, joy, and related
of positive affect and negative affect on satisfaction, along positive emotions.
with expectation beliefs and disconfirmation beliefs. Sat- The centrality of satisfaction in marketing studies is
isfaction with automobiles was a function of positive perhaps more due to being the first emotion to receive
affect, negative affect, expectations of receiving benefits scrutiny in postpurchase behavior research than to consti-
and liabilities, and disconfirmation beliefs. Satisfaction tuting a unique, fundamental construct in and of itself.
with cable television was determined by positive affect, Indeed, it is likely that---depending on the situation, prod-
negative affect, and disconfirmation beliefs. Westbrook uct, and person---other positive and negative emotions are
and Oliver (1991) found that three emotional responses more important outcomes of purchase. Under certain con-
were important antecedents of satisfaction of newly pur- ditions, frustration, anger, disappointment, alienation, dis-
chased automobiles: pleasant surprise, interest, and gust, anxiety, alarm, guilt, shame, joy, happiness, hope,
hostility. pride, jubilation, excitement, relief, amusement, and
Oliver (1993) expanded the determinants of satisfac- pleasure, among many other negative and positive emo-
tion to include positive affect (interest and joy) and nega- tions, might be more valid reactions consumers have to
tive affect (anger, disgust, contempt, shame, guilt, fear, purchases. By the same token, the implications of emo-
sadness), as well as disconfirmation beliefs. In addition, tional reactions in purchase situations on complaint
attribute satisfactions and attribute dissatisfactions were behaviors, word-of-mouth communication, repurchase,
hypothesized as direct determinants of satisfaction, as well and related actions may differ for various positive and
as indirect (through positive and negative affect) determi- negative emotions and be of more relevance than reactions
nants. Satisfaction with automobiles was found to be a to satisfaction or dissatisfaction, per se.
function of attribute satisfaction, attribute dissatisfaction, Research by Nyer (1997a, 1997b) addresses appraisal
positive affect, negative affect, and disconfirmation. Satis- theories and their role in postconsumption responses.
faction with a required university course was also found to Nyer found that such postconsumption responses as repur-
be determined by these antecedents, except attribute chase intentions, word-of-mouth intentions, and other
dissatisfaction. reactions are predicted best by using measures of satisfac-
tion plus measures of other emotions. Other studies of note
The investigation of the impact of emotions on post-
purchase reactions is an important development in market- investigating the role of specific emotions in customer sat-
ing. However, it is unclear whether satisfaction is phenome- isfaction include those done by Dubt, B~langer, and
nologically distinct from many other positive emotions. Trudeau (1996); Folkes, Koletsky, and Graham (1987);
Satisfaction is neither a basic emotion nor a central and Taylor (1994). More information for first paper
emotional category in leading theories of emotions (e.g., We are uncertain whether a single, summary emotional
Frijda 1986; Lazarus 1991; Oatley 1992; Roseman 1991; response such as satisfaction is feasible or even desirable.
Smith and Ellsworth 1985). Furthermore, Shaver, But if one is to be discovered, it may lie more in emotions
more closely connected to human welfare or emotional
Schwartz, Kirson, and O'Connor (1987) found that satis-
well-being (e.g., Diener and Larsen 1993). In this regard,
faction shares much common variance with such positive
emotions as happiness, joy, gladness, elation, delight, and for example, a case could be made for happiness as a fun-
enjoyment, among others. Likewise, Nyer (1997b) discov- damental emotion related to the purchase of goods and ser-
ered that measures of joy and satisfaction loaded on one vices, in particular, and emotional well-being, in general
factor. (Bagozzi forthcoming).
Although we leave open the possibility that measures of
satisfaction can achieve discriminant validity from mea- SOCIAL BASES OF EMOTIONS
sures of joy, happiness, and other positive emotions, we
believe that this will be very difficult to produce in prac- Although people can experience emotions privately,
tice. Also, no theory exists for specifying the conditions such as in response to physical danger, a case can be made
One of the points to acchieve with the
new data is discriminant validity in paper of Happiness
202 JOURNALOF THE ACADEMYOF MARKETINGSCIENCE SPRING 1999

that emotions are most often interpersonal or group-based discrete emotions for consumer behavior? When do emo-
responses. Unfortunately, the vast majority of research tions function in clusters or as homogeneous groups, such
into emotional behavior has had an individualistic slant to as positive and negative categories?
it (e.g., Parkinson 1995). Marketing relationships seem to Area No. 6. How do emotions function to influence the
be contexts where more social conceptualizations of emo- behavior of salespeople and managers? When are they
tions would be worth pursuing. For example, Ruth, Otnes, functional or dysfunctional?
and Brunel (1998) review studies where discrete emotions Area No. 7. How do consumers and managers control
are central in gift exchanges, and they show how appraisals their emotions to advantage? What role do emotions play
lead to emotions and how emotions relate to interpersonal in self-regulation?
relationships and disposition of gifts. A related area in Area No. 8. How should emotions be measured in mar-
need of study is the management of emotions by organiza- keting? When are self-reports appropriate and inappropri-
tions and by the self (e.g., Bagozzi 1992; Hochschild ate? How can physiological measures be incorporated?
1983; Locke 1996). Finally, cultural aspects of emotions What are the consequences of treating emotions as unipo-
deserve further inquiry (e.g., Markus and Kitayama 1994). lar versus bipolar responses?
Area No. 9. What are the distinctions between and rela-
tionships among emotions, affect, feelings, evaluations,
CONCLUSIONS
moods, and attitudes?
Emotions are ubiquitous throughout marketing. They Area No. 10. How are distinct emotions related to each
influence information processing, mediate responses to other? Under what conditions, for example, does frustra-
persuasive appeals, measure the effects of marketing stim- tion lead to dissatisfaction? Shame lead to anger? Or love
uli, initiate goal setting, enact goal-directed behaviors, and lead to happiness?
serve as ends and measures of consumer welfare. Yet, we Area No. 11. An area neglected by marketers, but at the
are only beginning to understand the role of emotions in heart of the discipline, is the role of emotions in marketing
marketing. exchanges and relationships. How do emotions initiate,
The following areas constitute opportunities for future maintain, or sever marketing relationships? Can emotions
research: and marketing relationships be studied more dynamically
Area No. 1. Exactly how are appraisals conducted and as they evolve in real time and in context? What are the
how do they lead to emotional reactions in consumers? In implications of treating emotions in marketing as social
what sense are appraisals conscious and purposive versus p h e n o m e n a as opposed to strictly intrapsychic
automatic? What is the role of the amygdala, hippocam- phenomena?
pus, and other neural systems in appraisals? What are the Area No. 12. Finally, is customer satisfaction a unique
essential elements of cognitive appraisals in emotional phenomenon or is it a subcategory of positive emotions?
behavior and how can they be influenced by marketing Likewise, is dissatisfaction a unique phenomenon, the
stimuli? polar opposite of satisfaction, or a subcategory of negative
Area No. 2. Related to No. 1, but also more generally emotions? How do satisfaction/dissatisfaction and other
throughout the processes involved in emotional responses, emotions relate to customer loyalty, complaint behavior,
what role does arousal play? What is physiological arousal and word-of-mouth behavior? What role do emotions play
and how does it relate to appraisals, coping responses, in the diffusion of innovations?
action tendencies, and behavior? What do self-reports of
arousal really indicate? Is arousal an essential component ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
of emotions? If so, how can marketers develop theories of
emotions, better incorporating arousal, and how should The authors would like to thank Professor Julie Ruth
arousal be measured? for her comments on this article.
Area No. 3. How do emotions affect information pro-
cessing in consumer decision making? In what ways do
emotions influence the encoding, storage, and retrieval of REFERENCES
information? What contribution do emotions make to con-
sumer decision making and choice? Aakcr,DavidA., DouglasM. Stayman,and MichaelR. Hagerty.1986.
Area No. 4. Are emotional reactions universal? In what "Warmthin Advertising:Measurement,Impact and SequenceEf-
fects."Journal of ConsumerResearch 12 (March):365-381.
ways do emotions differ across cultures? What role do , - - , and RichardVezina. 1988."IdentifyingFeelingsElic-
socialization, ethnicity, and culture play in the representa- ited by Advertising."Psychology & Marketing 5 (Spring): 1-16.
tion, experience, and effects of emotions? Allen, ChrisT. and ChrisA. Janiszewski.1989."Assessingthe Roleof
ContingencyAwarenessin AttitudinalConditioningWith Implica-
Area No. 5. Under what conditions do emotions func- tions for AdvertisingResearch."Journal of Marketing Research 26
tion in discrete categories and what are the implications of (February):30-43.
Bagozzi et al. / THE ROLE OF EMOTIONS 203

Austin, James T. and Jeffrey B. Vancouver. 1996. "Goal Constructs in Message Content and Context Cues and on Subsequent Behaviour?'
Psychology: Structure, Process, and Content?' PsychologicalBulle- European Journal of Social Psychology 22:511-530.
tin 120:338-3759 , Shelly Chaiken, and Piroska Hunyadi. 1994. "The Role of Mood
Bagozzi, Richard P. 1991a. "Further Thoughts on the Validity of Mea- and Message Ambiguity in the Interplay of Heuristic and Systematic
sures of Elation, Gladness, and Joy." Journal of Personality and So- Processing." EuropeanJournal of Social Psychology 24:207-221
cial Psychology 61:98-104. Boninger, David S., Faith Gleicher, John Hetts, David Armor, and E.
-. 199 lb. "The Role of Psychophysiology in Consumer Research." Moore9 1994. "The Influence of Counterfactual Thinking on Inten-
In Handbook of ConsumerBehavior. Eds. Thomas S. Robertson and tions and Behavior9 Unpublished raw data, reported in Gleicher et al.
Harold H. Kassarjian. Englewood Cliffs, N J: Prentice Hall, 124-161. (1995)9
-. 1992. "The Self-Regulation of Attitudes, Intentions, and Behav- Bower, Gordan H. 1981. "Mood and Memory?' American Psycludogist
ior." Social Psychology Quarterly 55:178-204. 36 (2): 129-1489
9 1993. "An Examination of the Psychometric Properties of ~and Paul R. Cohen. 1982. "Emotional Influences in Memory and
Measures of Negative Affect in the PANAS-X Scales." Journal of Thinking: Data and Theory?' In Affect and Cognition.Eds. Margaret
Personality and Social Psychology 65:836-851. Clark and Susan T. Fiske. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 291-
9 1994. "The Effects of Arousal on the Organization of Positive 331.
and Negative Affect and Cognitions: Application to Attitude The- and John D. Mayer9 1985. "Failure to Replicate Mood-
ory." Structural Equation Modeling 1:222-252. Dependent Retrieval:' Bulletin of the PsychonomicSociety23:39-42.
9 1996. "The Role of Arousal in the Creation and Control of the , Kenneth P. Monteiro, and Stephen G. Gilligan. 1978. "Emo-
Halo Effect in Attitude Models." Psychology & Marketing 13:235- tional Mood as a Context for Learning and Recall?' Journal of Verbal
264. Learning and VerbalBehavior 17:573-585.
- - . Forthcoming. "Happiness?' In Encyclopedia of Human Emo- , Stephen G. Gilligan, and Kenneth P. Monteiro. 1981. "Selectiv-
tions. Eds. David Levinson, James Ponzetti, and Peter Jorgensen. ity of Learning Caused by Affective States?' Journalof Experimental
New York: Macmillan. Psychology: General 110 (4): 451-473.
- and Robert E. Burnkrant. 1979. "Attitude Organization and the
-
Bradburn, Norman M. 1969. The Structureof Psychological Well-Being.
Attitude-Behavior Relationship?' Journal of Personality and Social Chicago: Aldine.
Psychology 37:913-9299 Breckler, Steven J. and Elizabeth C. Wiggins. 1989. "Affect Versus
- and Utpal Dholakia. 1998. "Goal-Setting and Goal-Striving in
- Evaluation in the Structure of Attitudes." Journal of Experimental
Consumer Behavior." Unpublished working paper, University of Social Psychology25:253-2719
Michigan. Brown, Steven P. and Douglas M. Stayman. 1992. "Antecedents and
--and Elizabeth A. Edwards. 1998. "Goal Setting and Goal Pursuit Consequences of Attitude Toward the Ad: A Meta-Analysis?' Jour-
in the Regulation of Body Weight?' Psychologyand Health 13:593- nal of Consumer Research 19 (June): 34-51.
621. , William L. Cron, and John W. Slocum Jr. 1997. "Effects of
- and David J. Moore. 1994. "Public Service Advertisements:
- Goal-Directed Emotions on Salesperson Volitions, Behavior, and
Emotion and Empathy Guide Prosocial Behavior9 Journal of Mar- Performance: A longitudinal Study." JournalofMarketing 61:39-50.
keting 58:56-70. Burke, Marian C. and Julie A. EdeU. 1989. "The Impact of Feelings on
, Hans Baumgartner, and Rik Pieters. 19989"Goal-Directed Emo- Ad-Based Affect and Cognition?' Journal of Marketing Research
tions:' Cognition and Emotion t 2:1-26. 26:69-83.
, Nancy Wong, and Youjae Yi. 1998. "The Representation of Af- Cacioppo, John T., Gary G. Berntson, and David J. Klein. 1992. "What Is
fect in Independent- and Interdependent-Based Cultures?' Unpub- an Emotion? The Role of Somatovisceral Afference, With Special
lished working paper, University of Michigan. Emphasis on Somatovisceral 'Illusions.' "Review of Personality and
Barrett, Lisa Feldman and James A. Russell9 1998. "Independence and Social Psychology 14:63-98.
Bipolarity in the Structure of Current Affect9 Journalof Personality Cantor, Joanne R., Jennings Bryant, and Dolf Zillman. 1974. "Enhance-
and Social Psychology 74:967-984. ment of Humor Appreciation by Transferred Excitation?' Journal of
Bartlett, James C. and John W. Santrock. 1979. "Affect-Dependent Epi- Personality and Social Psychology 30:812-821.
sodic Memory in Young Children:' ChildDevelopment 50:513-518. Clark, Margaret D. and John D. Teasdale. 1982. "Diurnal Variation in
, Georgia Burleson, and John W. Santrock. 1982. "Emotional Clinical Depression and Accessibility of Memories of Positive and
Mood and Memory in Young Children:' Journal of Experimental Negative Experiences:' Journal of Abnormal Psychology 91:87-95.
Chdd Psyctudogy 34:59-76. Clore, Gerald L. and Donn Byrne. 1974. "A Reinforcement Affect
Batra, Rajeev and Olli T. Ahtola. 1990. "Measuring the Hedonic and Model of Attraction." In Foundations oflnterpersonal Attraction9
Utilitarian Sources of Consumer Attitudes?' Marketing Letters Ed. T. L. Huston. New York: Academic Press, 143-170.
2:159-1709 , Norbert Schwarz, and Michael Conway. 1994. "Affective
--and Morris B. Holbrook. 1990. "Developing a Typology of Af- Causes and Consequences of Social Information Processing?' In
fective Responses to Advertising?' Psychology & Marketing 7 Handbook of Social Cognition, 2nd ed., Vol. 1. Eds. Robert Wyer,
(Spring): 11-25. Thomas Srull, and Alice Isen. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum,
- and Michael L. Ray. 1986. "Affective Responses Mediating Ac-
-
323-417.
ceptance of Advertising:' Journal of Consumer Research 13 (Sep- Cohen, Joel B. and Charles S. Areni. 1991. "Affect and Consumer Behav-
tember): 234-249. ior." In Handbook of ConsumerBehavior.Eds. Thomas S. Robertson
--and Douglas M. Stayman. 1990. "The Role of Mood in Advertis- and Harold H. Kassarjian. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 188-
ing Effectiveness?' Journal of Consumer Research 17:203-2149 240.
- and Debra Stephens. 1994. "Attitudinal Effects of Ad-Evoked
-
Crites Jr., Stephen L., Leandre R. Fabrigar, and Richard E. Petty. 1994.
Moods and Emotions: The Moderating Role of Motivation?' Psychol- "Measuring the Affective and Cognitive Properties of Attitudes:
ogy & Marketing 11:199-215. Conceptual and Methodological Issues ?' Personalityand Social Psy-
Bless, Herbert, Gerd Bohner, Norbert Schwarz, and Fritz Strack. 1990. chology Bulletin 20 (December): 619-634.
"Mood and Persuasion: A Cognitive Response Analysis:' Personal- Damasio, Antonio R. 1994. Descartes'Error: Emotion, Reason, and the
ity and Social PsychologyBulletin 16 (2): 331-345. Human Brain. New York: Avon.
9Gerald L. Clore, Norbert Schwarz, Verena Golisano, Christina Diener, Ed and Robert A. Emmons 1985. "The Independence of Positive
Rabe, and Marcus Wolk. 1996. "Mood and the Use of Scripts: Does a and Negative Affect?' Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
Happy Mood Really Lead to Mindlessness?" Journal of Personality 47:1105-1117.
and Social Psychology71 (4): 665-679. ~and Randy J. Larsen. 1993. "The Experience of Emotional Well-
Bohner, Gerd, Kimberly Crow, Hans-Peter Erb, and Norbert Schwarz. Being?' In Handbook of Emotions. Eds. Michael Lewis and Jeannette
1992. "Affect and Persuasmn: Mood Effects on the Processing of M. Haviland. New York: Guilford, 405-415.
204 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE SPRING 1999

Donovan, Robert J. and John R. Rossiter. 1982. "Store Atmospherics: Hayes-Roth9 B. 1977. "Evolution of Cognitive Structures and Pro-
An Environmental Psychology Approach." Journal of Retailing cesses" Psychological Review 84:260-278.
58:34-57. Hill, Ronald P. and MerylP. Gardner. 1987. "The Buying Process: Effects
Dubt, Laurette, Marie-Clande Btlanger, and Elyse Trudeau. 1996. "The of and on Consumer Mood States." Advances in Consumer Research
Role of Emotions in Health Care Satisfaction." Journal of Health 14:408410.
Care Marketing 16 (Summer): 45-51. Hochschild, Arlie R. 1983. The Managed Heart. Berkeley: University of
Dutton, Donald G. and Arthur P. Aron. 1974. "Some Evidence for California Press9
Heightened Sexual Attraction Under Conditions of High Anxiety." Holbrook, Morris B. and Rajeev Batra. 1987. "Assessing the Role of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 30:510-517. Emotions as Mediators of Consumer Responses to Advertising."
Eagly, Alice H. and Shelly Chaiken. 19939 The Psychology of Attitudes. Journal of Consumer Research 14:404-420.
New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Isen, Alice M. 1984. "Toward Understanding the Role of Affect in Cogni-
9Antonio Mladinic, and Stacey Otto. 1994. Cognitive and Affec- tion?' In Handbook of Social Cognition, Vol. 3. Eds. Robert Wyer,
tire Bases of Attitudes Toward Social Groups and Social Policies." Thomas Srull, and Alice lsen. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbanm,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 30:113-137. 179-2369
Edell, Julie A. and Marian Chapman Burke. 1987. "The Power of Feel- 9 1987. "Positive Affect, Cognitive Processes, and Social Behav-
ings in Understanding Advertising Effects." Journal of Consumer ior?' In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 20. Ed.
Research 14 (December): 421-433. Leonard Berkowitz. New York: Academic Press, 203-253.
Eich, Eric J. and Isabel M. Bimbaum. 1982. "Repetition, Cueing and - - . 1989. "Some Ways in Which Affect Influences Cognitive Pro-
State-Dependent Memory." Memory and Cognition 10:103-114. cesses: Implications for Advertising and Consumer Behavior" In
Fiedler, Klaus9 1990. "Mood-dependent Selectivity in Social Cognition." Cognitive and Affective Responses to Advertising9Eds. Patricia Caf-
In European Review of SocialPsychology,Vol. 1. Eds. Wolfgang Stroebe ferata and Alice M. Tybout. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books, 91-
and Miles Hewstone. New York: John Wiley, 1-32. 117.
Fishbein, Martin and Icek Ajzen. 1975. Belief, Attitude, Intention, and - - and Kimberly A. Danbman. 1984. "The Influence of Affect on
Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading, MA: Categorization" Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47
Addison-Wesley. (6): 1206-1217.
Folkes, Valerie, Susan Koletsky, and John L. Graham. 1987. "A Field , Thomas E. Shalker, Margaret Clark, and Lynn Karp. 1978. "Af-
Study of Causal Inferences and Consumer Reaction: The View From fect, Accessibility of Material in Memory9 and Behavior: A Cognitive
the Airport." Journal of Consumer Research 13 (March): 534-539. Loop?" Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36 (1): 1-12.
Forgas, Joseph P. 1994. "The Role ofF,motion in Social Judgments: An , Mitzi M. Johnson, Elizabeth Mertz9 and Gregory F. Robinson.
Introductory Review and an Affect Infusion Model (AIM)." Euro- 1985. "The Influence of Positive Affect on the Unusualness of Word
pean Journal of Social Psychology 24:1-24. Associations" Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48 (6):
. 1995. "Mood and Judgment: The Affect Infusion Model 1-14.
(AIM)" Psychological Bulletin 117 (1): 39-66. 9 Kimberly A. Daubman, and Gary R Nowicki. 1987. "Positive
Affect Facilitates Creative Problem Solving." Journal of Personality
- - and Gordon H. Bower9 1987. "Mood Effects on Person-
and Social Psychology 52 (6): 1122-1131.
Perception Judgments." Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy 53 (1): 53-60. , Paula M9Niedenthal, and Nancy Cantor. 1992. "An Influence of
Friestad, Marian and Esther Thorson. 1986. "Emotion-Eliciting Adver- Positive Affect on Social Categorization." Motivation and Emotion
16 (1): 65-78.
tising: Effect on Long-term Memory and Judgment." Advances in
Consumer Research 13:111-115. Izard, Carroll E. 1972. Patterns of Emotions: A New Analysis of Anxiety
and Depression. New York: Academic Press.
Frijda, Nico H. 1986. The Emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press. .1991. The Psychology of Emotions. New York: Plenum.
-. 1993. "Moods, Emotion Episodes9 and Emotions9 In Handbook 9 1992. "Basic Emotions, Relations Among Emotions, and
of Emotions. Eds. M. Lewis and J. M. Haviland. New York: Guilford9 Emotion-Cognition Relations:' Psychological Review 99:561-565.
381-403. James, William. [1890] 1950. The Principles of Psychology. Vol 2. New
York: Holt.
-. 19949"Emotions Are Functional, Most of the Time." In The Na- Laird, James D., John J. Wagener, Mark Halal, and Martha Szcgda. 1982.
ture of Emotion. Eds. P. Ekman and R. J. Davidson. New York: Ox- "Remembering What You Feel: The Effects of Emotion on Memory."
ford University Press, 112-122.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 42 (4): 646-657.
, Peter Kuipers, and Elisabeth ter Schure. 1989. "Relations Larsen, Randy J. and Ed Diencr. 1992. "Promises and Problems With the
Among Emotion9 Appraisal, and Emotional Action Readiness." Circumplex Model of Emotion." In Review of Personality and Social
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57:212-228. Psychology: Vol.14. Emotional and SociaI Behavior. Ed. M. S. Clark.
Gardner, Meryl P. 1985. "Mood States and Consumer Behavior: A Criti- Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 25-59.
cal Review." Journal of ConxumerResearch 12 (December): 281-300. Lazarus, Richard S. 1982. "Thoughts on the Relations Between Emotion
- - a n d G. J. Simokos. 1986. "Toward a Methodology for Assessing and Cognition." American Psychologist 35:1019-1024.
Effects of Instore Atmospherics." Advances in Consumer Research 9 1991. Emotion and Adaptation. New York: Oxford University
13:27-31. Press.
Gleicber, Faith, David S. Boninger, Alan Strathman, David Armor, John LeDoux, Joseph. 1996. The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpin-
Hetts, and Mina Ahn. 1995. "With an Eye Toward the Future: The Im- nings of Emotional Life. New York: Simon & Schuster.
pact of Counterfactual Thinking on Affect, Attitudes, and Behavior?' Lewis, Michael 1993. "Self-Conscious Emotions: Embarrassment,
In What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Pride, Shame, and Guilt" In Handbook of Emotions. Eds. M. Lewis
Thinking. Eds. Neal J. Rocse and James M. Olson. Mahwah, NJ: and J. M. Haviland. New York: Guilford, 563-573.
Lawrence Erlbaum, 283-304. Locke, Karen. 1996. "A Funny Thing Happened! The Management of
Goldberg, Marvin E. and Gerald J. Gorn. 1987. "Happy and Sad TV Pro- ConsumerEmotions in Service Encounters" OrganizationaIScience
grams: How They Affect Reactions to Commercials." Journal of 7 (January-February): 40-59.
Consumer Research 14:387-403. MacKenzie, Scott B., Richard J. Lutz, and George Belch. 1986. "The
Gopinath, Mahesh and Richard P. Bagozzi. 1999. "Cognitive Appraisals Role of Attitude Toward the Ad as a Mediator of Advertising Effec-
Leading to Consumer Emotions and Action Tendencies:' Unpub- tiveness: A Test of Competing Explanations." Journal of Marketing
lished working paper, University of Michigan. Research 23 (May): 130-143.
Green, Donald P., Susan L. Goldman, and Peter Salovey. 1993. "Mea- Mackie, Diane M. and Leila T. Worth. 1989. "Processing Deficits and the
surement Error Masks Bipolarity in Affect Ratings." Journal of Per- Mediation of Positive Affect in Persuasion." Journal of Personality
sonality and Social Psychology 64:1029-1041. and Social Psychology 57 (1): 27-40.
Bagozzi et al. / THE ROLE OF EMOTIONS 205

Mano, Haim and Richard L. Oliver. 1993. "Assessing the Dimensionality Parrott, Gerrod W. 1988. "The Role of Cognition in Emotional Experi-
and Structure of the Consumption Experience: Evaluation, Feeling, ence." In Recent Trends in TheoreticalPsychology. Eds. W. J. Baker,
and Satisfaction?' Journal of Consumer Research 20 (December): L. P. Mos, H. V. Rappard, and H. J. Stare. New York: Springer-Verlag,
451-466. 327-337.
Manstead, Antony S. R. and Hugh L. Wagner. 1981. "Arousal, Cognition, Perugini, Marco and Richard P. Bagozzi. 1998. "The Role of Desires and
and Emotion: An Appraisal of Two-Factor Theory?' CurrentPsycho- Anticipatory Emotions in Goal-Directed Behaviors: Extending the
logical Reviews 1: 35-54. Theory of Planned Behavior." Unpublished working paper, Univer-
Marcel, Anthony J. 1983. "Conscious and Unconscious Perception: An sity of Michigan Business School9
Approach to the Relations Between Phenomenal Experience and Per- Petty, Richard E. and John T. Cacioppo. 1986. CommunicationandPer-
ceptual Processes?' Cognitive Psychology 15:238-300. suasion. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Markus, Hazel R. and Shinobu Kitayama. 1994. "The Cultural Con- , David W. Schumann, Steven A. Richman, and Alan J. Strathman.
struction of Self and Emotion: Implications for Social Behavior." In 1993. "Positive Mood and Persuasion: Different Roles for Affect Un-
Emotion and Culture: Empirical Studies of Mutual Influence. Eds. der High and Low Elaboration Conditions9 Journal of Personality
S. Kitayama and H. R. Markus. Washington, DC: American Psycho- and Social Psychology64:5-20.
logical Association, 89-130. Pieters, Rik, Hans Baumgartner, and D. Allen. 1995. "A Means-End
Martin, Leonard L., David W. Ward, John W. Achee, and Robert S. Wyer Jr. Chain Approach to Consumer Goal Structures." InternationalJour-
1993. "Mood as Input: People Have to Interpret the Motivational Im- nal of Research in Marketing 12 (3): 227-244.
plications of Their Moods?' Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Plutchik, Robert 1980. Emotion: A PsychoevolutionarySynthesis. New
chology 64 (3): 317-326. York: Harper & Row.
Milliman, Ronald E. 1982. "Using Background Music to Affect the Be- Puto, Christopher P. and William D. Wells9 1984. "Informational and
havior of Supermarket Shoppers?' Journal of Marketing 46 (Sum- Transformational Advertising: The Differential Effects of Time." Ad-
mer): 86-91. vances in ConsumerResearch 11:638-643.
9. 1986. "The Influence of Background Music on the Behavior of Reisenzein, Rainer 1983. "The Schachter Theory of Emotion: Two Dec-
Restaurant Patrons?' Journal of Consumer Research 13:286-289. ades Later." PsychologicalBulletin 94:239-264.
Miniard, Paul W., Sunil Bhatla, and Randall L. Rose. 1990. "On the For- Richard, Ren6, Joop van der Pligt, and Nanne de Vries. 1995. "Antici-
mation and Relationship of Ad and Brand Attitudes: An Experimen- pated Affective Reactions and Prevention of AIDS?' British Journal
tal and Causal Analysis." Journal of Marketing Research 27 of Social Psychology 34:9-21.
(August): 290-303. Richins, Marsha L. 1997. "Measuring Emotions in the Consumption Ex-
Mitchell, Andrew A. and Jerry C. Olson. 1981. "Are Product Attribute perience?' Journal of Consumer Research 24:127-146.
Beliefs the Only Mediator of Advertising Effects on Brand Atti- Roese, Neal J. and James M. Olson, eds. 1995. WhatMight Have Been:
tude?" Journal of Marketing Research 18 (August): 318-332. The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking. Mahwah, NJ:
Murray, Noel, Harish Sujan, Edward R. Hirt, and Mita Sujan. 1990. "The Lawrence Erlbanm.
Influence of Mood on Categorization: A Cognitive Flexibilty Inter- Roseman, Ira J. 1991. "Appraisal Determinants of Discrete Emotions?'
pretation?' Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59 (3): Cognition and Emotion 5:161-200.
411-425. Rozin, Paul, Jonathan Haidt, and Clark R. McCauley. 1993. "DisgustY' In
Nasby, William and Regina Yando. 1982. "Selective Encoding and Re- Handbook of Emotions. Eds. M. Lewis and J. M. Haviland. New
trieval of Affectively Valent Information?' Journalof Personalityand York: Guilford, 575-594.
Social Psychology43 (6): 1244-1253. Russell, James A. 1980. "A Circumplex Model of Affect?' JournalofPer-
Nyer, Prashanth U. 1997a. "Modeling the Cognitive Antecedents of sonality and Social Psychology39 (December): 1161-1178.
Post-Consumption Emotions?' Journal of Consumer Satisfaction,
~ . 1997. "How Shall an Emotion Be Called?' In CircumplexMod-
Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior 10:80-90.
els of Personality and Emotions. Eds. R. Plutchick and H. R. Conte.
9 1997b. "A Study of the Relationships Between Cognitive Ap-
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 205-220.
praisals and Consumption Emotions." Journal t~f the Academy of
Marketing Science 25 (4): 296-304. and Albert Mehrabian. 1977. "Evidence for a Three-Factor The-
Oatley, Keith 1992. Best Laid Schemes: The Psychology of Emotions. ory of Emotions?' Journal of Research in Personality 11:273-294.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. , Anna Weiss, and Gerald A. Mendelsohn. 1989. "Affect Grid: A
~and Philip N. Johnson-Laird. 1987. "Towards a Cognitive Theory Single-ltem Scale of Pleasure and Arousal?' Journal of Personality
of Emotions?' Cognition andEmotion 1:29-50. and Social Psychology 57:493-502.
Obermiller, Carl. 1985. "Varieties of Mere Exposure: The Effects of Ruth, Julie A., Cele C. Omes, and Fr6d~ric F. Brunel. 1998. "Bonds and
Processing Style and Repetition on Affective Response?' Journal of Burdens: Gift Receipt and Relationship Reformulation?' Unpub-
Consumer Research 12:17-30. lished working paper, University of Washington.
Oliver, Richard L. 1980. "A Cognitive Model of the Antecedents and Salovey, Peter and Deborah Birnbaum. 1989. "Influence of Mood on
Consequences of Satisfaction Decisions?' Journal of Marketing Re- Health-Related Cognitions?' Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
search 17 (November): 460-469. chology 57:539-551.
9 1993. "Cognitive, Affective, and Attribute Bases of the Satisfac- Sanbonmatsu, David M. and Frank R. Kardes. 1988. "The Effect of
tion Response" Journal of Consumer Research 20 (December): Physiological Arousal on Information Processing and Persuasion?'
418-430. Journal of Consumer Research 15:379-395.
- - . 1994. "Conceptual Issues in the Structural Analysis of Con- Schachter, Stanley and Jerome E. Singer. 1962. "Cognitive, Social, and
sumption Emotion, Satisfaction, and Quality?' In Advancesin Consumer Physiological Determinants of Emotional State?' PsychologicalRe-
Research, Vol. 21. Eds. Chris T. Allen and Deborah Roedder John. view 69:379-399.
Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 16-22. Schaller, Mark and Robert B. Cialdini. 1990. "Happiness, Sadness, and
Oiney, Thomas J., Morris B. Holbrook, and Rajeev Batra. 1991. "Con- Helping: A Motivational Integration." In Handbook of Motiva-
sumer Responses to Advertising: The Effects of Ad Content, Emo- tion and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior, Vol. 2. Eds.
tions, and Attitude Toward the Ad on Viewing Time?' Journal of R. M. Sorrentino and E. T. Higgins. New York: Guilford, 265-296.
Consumer Research 17 (March): 440-453. Schwarz, Norbert. 1990. "Feelings as Information: Informational and
Ortony, Andrew, Gerald L. Clore, and Allan Collins. 1988. The Cognitive Motivational Functions of Affective States." In Handbook of Motiva-
Structure of Emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. tion and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior, Vol. 2. Eds.
Parker, Dianne, Antony S. R. Manstead, and Stephen G. Stradling. 1995. R. M. Sorrentino and E. T. Higgins. New York: Guilford, 527-561.
"Extending the Theory of Planned Behaviour: The Role of Personal - - and Herbert Bless. 1991. "Happy and Mindless, But Sad and
Norm?' British Journal of Social Psychology 34:127-137. Smart? The Impact of Affective States on Analytic Reasoning?' In
Parkinson, Brian. 1995. Ideas and Realities of Emotion. London: Emotionand SocialJudgments. Ed. Joseph P. Forgas. London: Perga-
Routledge. mon, 55-71.
206 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE SPRING 1999

and Gerald L. Clore. 1983. "Mood, Misattribution, and Judg- , --, and David J. Klein. 1994. "Effects of Mood on High
ments of Well-Being: Informative and Directive Functions of Affec- Elaboration Attitude Change: The Mediating Role of Likelihood
tive States." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45 (3): Judgments?' European Journal of Social Psychology 24:25-439
513-523. --, ---, and Stephen M. Smith. 1995. "Positive Mood Can In-
~ , Fritz Strack, Detlev Kommer, and Dirk Wagner. 1987. "Soccer, crease or Decrease Message Scrutiny: The Hedonic Contingency
Rooms and the Quality of Your Life: Mood Effects on Judgments of View of Mood and Message Processing:' Journal of Personality and
Satisfaction With Life in General and with Specific Life Domains:' Social Psychology69 (1): 5-15.
European Journal of Social Psychology 17:69-79. Westbrook, Robert A. 1987. "Product/Consumption-Based Affective
Shaver, Phillip, Judith Schwartz, Donald Kirson, and Cary O'Connor9 Responses and Postpurchase Processes:' Journal of Marketing Re-
1987. "Emotion Knowledge: Further Exploration of a Prototype Ap- search 24 (August): 258-270.
proach?' Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52:1061- - - and Richard L. Oliver. 1991. "The Dimensionality of Consump-
1086. tion Emotion Patterns and Consumer Satisfaction." Journal of Con-
Shimp, Terence A. 1981. "Attitude Toward the Ad as a Mediator of Con- sumer Research 18 (June): 84-919
sumer Brand Choice," Journal of Advertising 10:9-15. Wiles, Judith A. and T. Bettina Cornwell. 1990. "A Review of Methods
9 1991. "Neo-Pavlovian Conditioning and Its Implications for Utilized in Measuring Affect, Feelings, and Emotion in Advertising."
Consumer Theory and Research." In Handbookof ConsumerBehav- Current Issues and Research in Advertising 13:241-275.
ior Eds. Thomas S. Robertson and Harold H. Kassarjian. Englewood Worth, Leila T. and Diane M. Mackie. 1987. "Cognitive Mediation of
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 162-187. Positive Affect in Persuasion." Social Cognition 5:76-94.
Smith, Craig A. and Phoebe C. Ellsworth. t985. "Patterns of Cognitive Yi, Youjae. 1990. "A Critical Review of Consumer Satisfaction?' In Re-
Appraisals in Emotion?' Journal of Personalityand Social Psychol- view of Marketing 1990. Ed. V. A. Zeithaml. Chicago: American
ogy 48:813-838. Marketing Association, 68-123.
Srull, Thomas K. 1983. "Affect and Memory: The Impact of Affective Zajonc, Robert B. 1968. "Attitudinal Effects of Mere Exposure?' Journal
Reactions in Advertising on the Representation of Product Informa- of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph Supplement
tion in Memory." In Advances in ConsumerResearch, Vol. 109 Eds. 9:1-27.
Richard P. Bagozzi and Alice Tybout. Ann Arbor, MI: Association -91980. "Feeling and Thinking: Preferences Need No Inferences?'
for Consumer Research, 520-5259 American Psychologist35:151 - 175.
.1984. "The Effects of Subjective Affective States on Memory 9 1984. "On the Primacy of Affect?' American Psychologist
and Judgment?' In Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 11. Ed. 39:117-123.
Thomas C. Kinnear. Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research,
-. 1998. "Emotions?' In The Handbook of Social Psychology. Eds.
530-5339
D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, and G. Lindzey. Boston: McGraw-Hill,
Stayman, Douglas M. and David A. Aaker. 1988. "Are All the Effects of
591-632.
Ad-Induced Feelings Mediated by Aad?" Journal of ConsumerRe-
search 15:368-373. and Hazel Markus. 1982. "Affective and Cognitive Factors in
Preferences?' Journal of Consumer Research 9 (September): 123-
and Rajeev Batra. 1991. "Encoding and Retrieval of Ad Affect in
Memory." Journal of Marketing Research 28 (May): 232-239. 131.
Stein, Nancy L., Mafia D. Liwag, and Elizabeth Wade. 1996. "A Goal- Zevon, Michael A. and Auke Tellegen. 1982. "The Structure of Mood
Based Approach to Memory for Emotional Events: Implementations Change: An Idiographic/Nomothetic Analysis:' JournalofPersonal-
for Theories of Understanding and Socialization." In Emotion: Inter- ity and Social Psychology43:111-1229
disciplinary Perspectives9Eds. R. D. Kavanangh, B. Zimmerberg, Zillman, Dolf. 1971. "Excitation Transfer in Communication-Mediated
and S. Fein. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 91-118. Aggressive Behavior?' Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
Taylor, Shirley. 1994. "Waiting for Service: The Relationship Between 7:419-434.
Delays and Evaluations of Services?' Journal of Marketing 58 91983. "Transfer of Excitation in Emotional Behavior." In Hand-
(April): 56-699 book of Social Psychology. Eds. John T. Cacioppo and Richard E.
Teasdale, John D. and Sarah J. Fogarty. 1979. "Differential Effects of In- Petty. New York: Guilford, 215-230.
duced Mood on Retrieval of Pleasant and Unpleasant Events From
Episodic Memory?' JournalofAbnormalPsychology88 (3): 248-257.
and Louise M. Russell. 1983. "Differential Aspects of Induced ABOUTTHEAUTHORS
Mood on the Recall of Positive, Negative and Neutral Words:' British
Journal of Clinical Psychology 22: 163-171.
Tulving, Endel and Zena Peafistone, 1966. "Availability Versus Accessi- Richard E Bagozzi is the Dwight F. Benton Professor of Behav-
bility of Information in Memory for Words." Journal of Verbal ioral Science in Management at the University of Michigan Busi-
Learning and VerbalBehavior 5:381-391. ness School. He is a graduate of Northwestern University and has
Verbeke, Willem and Richard P. Bagozzi. 1998. "Self-Conscious Emo- been a faculty member at the University of California-Berkeley,
tions in the Context of Personal Selling: Performance Is aFunction of
the Ability to Cope With Shame and Embarrassment:' Unpublished the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, and Stanford Univer-
working paper, University of Michigan. sity. He is currently doing research in emotions, goal-directed be-
and .1999. "Coping With Anxiety in Personal Selling: A havior, and social identity theory.
Study of Financial Service Salespeople?' Unpublished working pa-
per, University of Michigan. Mahesh Gopinath is an assistant professor of marketing in the
Watson, David and Auke Tellegen. 1985. "Toward a Consensual Struc-
ture of Mood?' PsychologicalBulletin 98 (September): 219-2359 A. B. Freeman School of Business at Tulane University. He is a
Wegener, Duane T. and Richard E. Petty. 1994. "Mood-Management graduate of the University of Michigan Business School and is
Across Affective States: The Hedonic Contingency Hypothesis?' doing research in emotions and customer satisfaction.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology66 (6): 1034-1048.
- - and . 1996. "Effects of Mood on Persuasion Processes: Prashanth U. Nyer is an assistant professor of marketing in the
Enhancing, Reducing, and Biasing Scrutiny of Attitude-Relevant In-
formation." In Striving and Feeling:InteractionsBetween Goalsand School of Business and Economics at Chapman University. He is
Affect. Eds. L. L. Martin and A. Tesser. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erl- a graduate of the University of Michigan Business School and is
baum, 329-362. doing research in emotions and customer satisfaction9

You might also like