You are on page 1of 11

Cognition 179 (2018) 202–212

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Cognition
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cognit

Original Articles

A new approach to differentiate states of mind wandering: Effects of T


working memory capacity

Matthew J. Voss, Meera Zukosky , Ranxiao Frances Wang
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, United States

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Although widely studied, the process of how mind wandering occurs and is subsequently sustained still remains
Mind wandering unclear. Moreover, the traditional concept of mind wandering tendency/frequency based on the self- or probe-
Meditation caught methods alone is incoherent and problematic. We developed a new approach to characterize the mind
Working memory wandering process by combining the self-caught and probe-caught methods to estimate the time of focus and
Memory and attention
time of mind wandering separately, and examined their relationship to working memory capacity. Participants
performed an OSPAN task and subsequently a basic Mindfulness Meditation Task (focus on breath). During the
meditation task, participants indicated when they became aware that they were mind wandering (self-caught
method), or were asked if they were mind wandering when probed (probe-caught method). Results showed that
time of focus but not time of mind wandering increased with greater working memory capacity. This suggests
that individuals with higher working memory capacity were able to focus on the current task longer, but had
little effect on the ability to monitor and terminate mind wandering once it occurred. The theoretical and
methodological implications of this new approach are discussed.

1. Introduction associated with reduced delay discounting, but were not in a more at-
tention demanding working memory task. Although these findings have
While performing a task, internal thoughts often grab our attention, given us insights into the nature of mind wandering, the current
preventing us from focusing on the current task at hand. These in- methodology of measuring the tendency to mind wander is ambiguous.
ternally driven thoughts are termed mind wandering, and encompass Here we explain the problems that arise with the current mind wan-
thoughts that are unrelated to the current task at hand (Christoff, dering methodology, and develop a novel, more informative way to
Irving, Fox, Spreng, & Andrews- Hanna, 2016; Randall, Oswald, & examine mind wandering.
Beier, 2014; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006, 2015). Although the specific Research on mind wandering has employed various methods to
terms used and the exact definition of mind wandering varies across the estimate people’s tendency to mind wander. One of the main ways to
literature, it is agreed upon that mind wandering can be described as a measure mind wandering is with thought sampling, i.e., asking parti-
shift of attention away from the primary task at hand to internal cipants directly through self report whether their mind was focused on
thoughts (Christoff et al., 2016; Randall et al., 2014; Smallwood & the task at hand, or whether they were mind wandering (Bastian &
Schooler, 2006, 2015). Sackur, 2013; Christoff, Gordon, Smallwood, Smith, & Schooler, 2009;
Various factors have been found to affect mind wandering. Task Hasenkamp, Wilson-Mendenhall, Duncan, & Barsalou, 2012; Seli,
difficulty, clinical symptomology of ADHD and Dysphoria, as well as an Carriere, Levene, & Smilek, 2013; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006, 2015).
individual’s mood have all been shown to affect the tendency to mind For example, participants may be interrupted while performing a task
wander (Feng, D’Mello, & Graesser, 2013; Phillips, Mills, D’Mello, & to check for mind wandering (dichotomous probe-caught methods,
Risko, 2016; Poerio, Totterdell, & Miles, 2013; Seli, Smallwood, more fine-tuned probe-caught methods such as utilizing likert scales or
Cheyne, & Smilek, 2015; Smallwood, O’Connor, Sudbery, & Obonsawin, confidence ratings, and self-caught methods) (Christoff et al., 2009;
2007). For example, in regards to task demands, Smallwood, Ruby, and Hasenkamp et al., 2012; Mrazek, Smallwood, & Schooler, 2012; Seli,
Singer (2013) examined task unrelated thoughts in relationship to task Jonker, Cheyne, Cortes, & Smilek, 2015; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006;
demands and found that task unrelated thoughts under a non-de- McVay, Kane, and Kwapil, 2009, etc.). Post-hoc self reports, where
manding condition of a simple choice reaction time task were individuals are not interrupted during the task, have also been utilized


Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 603 E. Daniel St., Champaign, IL 61820, United States.
E-mail address: zukosky1@illinois.edu (M. Zukosky).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.013
Received 17 May 2017; Received in revised form 18 May 2018; Accepted 18 May 2018
Available online 29 June 2018
0010-0277/ © 2018 Published by Elsevier B.V.
M.J. Voss et al. Cognition 179 (2018) 202–212

to estimate the occurrence of mind wandering (Helton & Warm, 2008; report to another self report of an individual catching him/herself mind
Smallwood et al., 2012; Smallwood, Fitzgerald, Miles, & Phillips, 2009). wandering. From the self-caught method alone, one can get a mea-
Among them, the self-caught and dichotomous probe-caught methods surement of the entire focus-mind wandering episode duration (denoted
administered during a task are two of the most common thought sam- as Tself), as illustrated in Fig. 1a. However, this measurement includes
pling methods used to measure mind wandering, and are the primary both the duration of time when an individual is focusing on the current
focus of the present study. task at hand, which can be defined as the duration from the start of the
In the self-caught method, participants are asked to report whenever task to the moment one begins mind wandering (Tfocus), and the
they realize they are mind wandering (Braboszcz & Delorme, 2010; duration of time an individual is actually mind wandering, which can
Bastian & Sackur, 2013; Hasenkamp et al., 2012; Smallwood & be defined as the duration from the start of mind wandering to the
Schooler, 2006, 2015). For example, participants are asked to perform a moment that one catches him/herself and terminates the mind wan-
task and to report, usually with a button press, whenever they notice dering process (TMW). Since it is unknown when the time of focus stops
that their focus is off the current task. Here the number of times an and when the time of mind wandering begins with this method, one
individual catches her/himself mind wandering can be measured, as cannot distinguish the focus state duration (Tfocus) from the mind
well as the duration of time between reports. The frequency an in- wandering state duration (TMW) using the self-caught method alone. As
dividual catches her/himself mind wandering or the duration of time a result, people with the same self-caught mind wandering duration (or
between reports 1is interpreted as an indication of the likelihood or same self reported mind wandering frequency) can have drastically
tendency for an individual to mind wander. That is, a longer duration different durations that they actually spend mind wandering. For ex-
between reports, or consequently a lower number of mind wandering ample, as illustrated in Fig. 1b, one participant could have a short
instances reported (lower frequency of self-caught reports of mind duration of being in the focus state and a longer duration of being in the
wandering), is usually interpreted as a lower tendency or likelihood to mind wandering state, while another participant might have a long
mind wander. In contrast, a higher number of mind wandering in- duration of focusing on the task at hand and a short duration of being in
stances reported (higher frequency of self-caught reports of mind the mind wandering state, even though both participants catch them-
wandering), or a shorter duration between reports, is usually inter- selves mind wandering at the same frequency (and thus have the same
preted as a higher tendency or likelihood to mind wander (Braboszcz & focus-mind wandering episode duration). This difference is indis-
Delorme, 2010; Bastian & Sackur, 2013; Hasenkamp et al., 2012; tinguishable using only the self-caught method, and thus the actual
Smallwood & Schooler, 2006, 2015). duration of time an individual spent in the mind wandering state re-
In the probe-caught method, participants are interrupted (usually mains unclear. Thus, the self-caught frequency of mind wandering is
with a tone or a screen display) at different time intervals while per- not a satisfactory measure of how much people mind wander.
forming a task (Bastian & Sackur, 2013; Christoff et al., 2009; Seli et al., The probe-caught method measures the relative proportion of time
2013; Smallwood & Schooler, 2015). At the time of the probe, parti- people are in a focus state and a mind wandering state. That is, if the
cipants are asked to report whether their focus was on the task at hand, probes are sampled randomly or uniformly across the focus-mind
or if they were mind wandering. The proportion of time an individual wandering episode, then the chance a probe happens to land at a time
was mind wandering can be calculated by dividing the probes labeled while the participant is mind wandering is determined by the overall
as being in the mind wandering state by the total number of probes proportion of time s/he spends in mind wandering (p(MW)). If an in-
given. The probe-caught method thus can provide a measure of the dividual spends 60% of the time in an actual mind wandering state,
overall proportion of time an individual was in a mind wandering state then the chance a random probe occurs at a time s/he is mind wan-
based on the mind wandering probe rate (Bastian & Sackur, 2013; Seli dering would be 0.6. Although this method can estimate the proportion
et al., 2013; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). With the probe-caught of time an individual is in a focus state and a mind wandering state, it
method, the proportion or percentage of time reported to be in a mind cannot determine the duration an individual spends in a focus state and
wandering state is usually interpreted as an indication of the likelihood in a mind wandering state in individual episodes. As illustrated in
or tendency for an individual to mind wander. That is, a higher pro- Fig. 1c, individuals with the same probe-caught mind wandering rate
portion or percentage of probes caught in the mind wandering state is can have drastically different durations of focus and mind wandering in
thought to indicate a higher tendency or likelihood to mind wander. In individual episodes, which reflects their different ability to maintain
contrast, a lower proportion or percentage of mind wandering probes is attention on the current task and their ability to detect mind wandering.
thought to indicate a lower tendency or likelihood to mind wander. Participant 1 (P1) is able to focus on the current task for 10 s before
Although intuitively sound, a closer examination of the self- and mind wandering is initiated, and remains in a mind wandering state for
probe-caught method reveals issues with the traditional interpretations 30 s before catching mind wandering and returning to the task, while
of mind wandering, and suggests that the concept of mind wandering P2 is able to stay on focus for 5 s before entering a mind wandering state
frequency/tendency itself is incoherent. As discussed by Smallwood which lasts for 15 s. If we randomly probe these two participants, they
(2013), the inability to covertly detect the onset of mind wandering will both be caught mind wandering 75% of the time, however both the
poses difficulties in studying mind wandering. For example, as illu- duration of focus (Tfocus) and the duration of mind wandering (TMW) are
strated in Fig. 1a, a focus-mind wandering episode can be defined as the longer for P1 compared to P2. Thus, the probe-caught rate of mind
time when an individual starts focusing (or refocusing) on a task, to wandering is also not a satisfactory measure of the underlying me-
when an individual realizes s/he is mind wandering and terminates the chanism of mind wandering.
mind wandering. A focus-mind wandering episode therefore consists of These methodological limitations can lead to confusion and mis-
a period when an individual is focused on the current task s/he is interpretations in evaluating people’s tendencies to mind wander. For
performing, and a period when s/he is actually mind wandering. This example, Fig. 1d shows two individuals and their mind wandering time
time course within a focus-mind wandering episode illuminates po- course. The first participant (P1) has an average of 15 s in the focus
tential problems with the current use of the self- and probe-caught state, and 5 s in the mind wandering state, while the second participant
methods to measure the tendency of mind wandering. (P2) has 10 s in the focus state, and 30 s in the mind wandering state.
For example, the self-caught method provides a measure of the Therefore P1 has an overall focus-mind wandering episode duration of
frequency of self-caught reports or the duration of time from one self 20 s (or will self-catch her/himself mind wandering 3 times per
minute), while P2 has an overall focus-mind wandering episode dura-
tion of 40 s (or self-caught mind wandering rate of 1.5 times per
1
Frequency of reports and the duration between reports are inversely related; there- minute). Thus, according to the self-caught method, P1 would be con-
fore they are essentially the same measurement in different forms. ventionally interpreted as having a higher tendency to mind wander

203
M.J. Voss et al. Cognition 179 (2018) 202–212

Fig. 1. A theoretical analysis of the traditional


interpretations of mind wandering tendency
based on the self- and probe-caught methods.
(a) The different states of a focus-mind wan-
dering episode. (b) Problems of the self-caught
method as a measure of mind wandering ten-
dency. Participant 1 (P1) and participant 2
(P2), both have focus-mind wandering epi-
sodes of the same duration, but the duration in
the mind wandering state of P1 is longer and
the duration in the focus state is shorter, than
for P2. These two scenarios cannot be dis-
tinguished using only the self-caught method.
(c) Problems of the probe-caught method as a
measure of mind wandering tendency. The
ratio between the duration in a focus state and
the duration in a mind wandering state is the
same for both participants, but the actual
durations of being in the focus state and mind
wandering state in a given episode are dif-
ferent. These two scenarios cannot be dis-
tinguished using only the probe-caught
method alone. (d) Conflicts between the self-
caught method and the probe-caught method.
Here the probe-caught and the self-caught
method used alone lead to contradictory in-
terpretations when evaluating an individual’s
tendency to mind wander.

compared to P2, as P1′s self-reported mind wandering frequency is The present study took a new approach to characterize the mind
higher than P2′s. wandering process by estimating the time course of the two main states
In contrast, using only the probe-caught method, one would reach of the focus-mind wandering episode separately, following the princi-
the opposite conclusion. When probed randomly, P1 would be caught in ples of the Process-Occurrence Framework by Smallwood (2013). This
the focus state 75% of the time and in the mind wandering state 25% of theory emphasizes the importance of distinguishing between processes
the time, while P2 would be caught in the focus state 25% of the time that lead to the occurrence of an experience from those supporting the
and in a mind wandering state 75% of the time. According to the probe- continuation of the experience once it is initiated. According to this
caught method, P1 would be traditionally interpreted as having a lower framework, the first state Tfocus can be taken as a measure of one’s
tendency to mind wander compared to P2, as P1 is in a focus state ability to maintain attention on the current task and resist the occur-
proportionally more and a mind wandering state proportionally less rence/initiation of mind wandering, while the second state TMW can be
than P2. Therefore for the same participants, the self-caught method taken as a measure of processes that keep people in the mind wandering
and probe-caught method give exactly the opposite conclusion on who state after it occurs. We used this new approach to examine one of the
mind wanders more. This contradiction of the interpretations of more main factors that has been shown to affect mind wandering, namely
vs less tendency to mind wander based on the two methods suggests an working memory capacity.
intrinsic ambiguity of the traditional concept of mind wandering ten- The effect of working memory capacity on mind wandering has
dency itself. been an important issue on theories of mind wandering. There is debate

204
M.J. Voss et al. Cognition 179 (2018) 202–212

in the literature on whether mind wandering consumes executive con- between different states of mind wandering and the corresponding
trol resources or whether mind wandering rather results from a failure underlying processes, both at the theoretical and at the experimental
of executive control (Levinson, Smallwood, & Davidson, 2012; McVay & level. The failure to separately measure how working memory capacity
Kane, 2010; Randall et al., 2014; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006, 2015). affects different states of mind wandering can cause confusion in the
The Resource Hypothesis states that mind wandering instances take up interpretation of the data. The goal of this study was to develop a new
executive control resources (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). In this view, approach to estimating time of focus (Tfocus) and time of mind wan-
both the current task at hand and mind wandering instances are com- dering (TMW) within a self-caught focus-mind wandering episode se-
peting for the same executive control resources. Here the same execu- parately in relationship to working memory capacity, to examine the
tive control resources intended to keep an individual focused on the role of executive control on the ability to focus on the task at hand, and
current task at hand, are diverted from the current task and used to that on the ability to maintain and/or detect mind wandering sepa-
entertain internal mind wandering instances (Smallwood & Schooler, rately.
2006). Thus, the resource hypothesis emphasizes that maintaining a Experiment 1 is a pilot study following the traditional methods to
mind wandering instance requires executive resources. As such, when estimate some basic parameters and provide an initial measurement of
more executive control resources are available, more mind wandering the relationship between working memory capacity and the raw self-
should be present (Antrobus, 1968; Levinson et al., 2012; Rummel & caught or probe-caught mind wandering rate in a mindfulness of breath
Boywitt, 2014; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006; Teasdale et al., 1995; meditation task with our experimental setup and procedure. This served
Thomson, Besner, & Smilek, 2013). as the basis for Experiment 2, where the two methods were combined
In contrast to the Resource Hypothesis, McVay and Kane (2010) within the same participants to estimate the two states of the focus-
proposed the Executive Control Hypothesis, stating that instead of mind wandering episode.
consuming the executive control resources needed to focus on the
current task at hand, mind wandering is actually a result of a failure in 2. Experiment 1a
our executive control (McVay & Kane, 2010). Thus mind wandering is a
result from a failure in executive control to block thoughts about cur- Experiment 1 was conducted to estimate the temporal parameters of
rent concerns (the default mode) during task performance (McVay & self-caught mind wandering and examine the relationship between
Kane, 2010). As such, individuals with lower executive control re- working memory capacity and mind wandering using the self-caught
sources will be worse at blocking out this default mode, which will lead method alone within a non-demanding task, i.e., a Mindfulness Breath
to more mind wandering during task performance (Buckner, Andrews- Meditation Task.
Hanna, & Schacter, 2008; Christoff et al., 2016; Christoff et al., 2009;
Hasenkamp et al., 2012; Mason et al., 2007; McVay & Kane, 2009, 2.1. Methods
2010, 2012).
In summary, these two theories predict different behaviors in the 2.1.1. Participants
relationship between executive control, as measured by working Twenty undergraduate students aged 18–22 from the University of
memory capacity, and mind wandering. If mind wandering instances do Illinois participated in experiment 1a. All participants had less than a
in fact consume executive control resources (Resource Hypothesis), total of 10 h of meditation practice experience during their lives and
individuals who have higher working memory capacity should have were novice meditators, to avoid practice effects of meditation, as it has
more available executive control resources to divert to mind wandering. been shown that meditation practice can improve focused attention and
Thus it is thought that individuals with higher working memory capa- reduce mind wandering (Braboszcz, Hahusseau, & Delorme, 2010;
city should report being in a mind wandering state more compared to Brewer et al., 2011; Hasenkamp et al., 2012; Lutz, Slagter, Dunne, &
individuals who have lower working memory capacity (Levinson et al., Davidson, 2008).
2012; Rummel & Boywitt, 2014; Teasdale et al., 1995; Thomson et al.,
2013; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). If, on the other hand, mind 2.1.2. Working memory task
wandering instances are a result of a failure in executive control (Ex- Individual’s working memory score was measured using the
ecutive Control Hypothesis), individuals who have higher working Automated OSPAN task. This OSPAN task is a measure of working
memory capacity should have better executive control capacities to memory capacity (Unsworth, Heitz, Schrock, & Engle, 2005). During
block out mind wandering instances. Thus it is thought that individuals this OSPAN task, participants first mentally solved a basic math pro-
should report being in a mind wandering state less compared to in- blem (eg. (2 ∗ 4) + 2 = ?). Once participants finished solving the math
dividuals who have lower working memory capacity (McVay & Kane, problem, a number would be presented on the screen. The participant
2010, 2012). would then respond with a mouse click, whether the number presented
The Process-Occurrence Framework by Smallwood (2013), on the on the screen was true or false to the solution the participant mentally
other hand, sought to reconcile this debate between the Executive calculated. After they made their selection, participants would see a
Control Hypothesis and the Resource Hypothesis. The Process Occur- letter of the alphabet flash on the screen. After 3–7 trials, participants
rence Framework stresses the importance of separating different ele- were prompted to report what letters they saw in the order they saw
ments of mind wandering, such as mind wandering’s initiation and the them. The entire OSPAN task took approximately 20 min where parti-
maintenance of mind wandering (Smallwood, 2013). For example, as cipants saw a total of 75 math problems and letters. The absolute
Smallwood (2013) explains, executive control could influence mind OSPAN score is calculated as the total number of letters correctly re-
wandering directly (inhibiting mind wandering from being initiated) or called (for more information on the OSPAN task please see Unsworth
indirectly (maintenance and continuation of mind wandering). The et al., 2005). The OSPAN test used for the current study was created by
Executive Control Hypothesis and the Resource Hypothesis thus are not Millisecond Software from the following website (1http://www.
in direct conflict, as these different hypotheses can illuminate the role millisecond.com/download/library/OSPAN/).
of executive control in different aspects of mind wandering.
However, the lack of empirical methods to indicate when mind 2.1.3. Mindfulness breath meditation task
wandering begins, makes it difficult to separate the initiation of mind The main task for the current study was a Mindfulness Breath
wandering (for example when a mind wandering state begins) and the Meditation Task. During the Mindfulness Breath Meditation Task, par-
maintenance of mind wandering once it has been initiated (for example ticipants were instructed to breathe normally and to focus on their
maintaining mind wandering until it is recognized and terminated) breath (participants were instructed to focus on the sensation of their in
(Smallwood, 2013). As a result, most previous work did not distinguish breath and out breath from their nostrils). The Mindfulness Breath

205
M.J. Voss et al. Cognition 179 (2018) 202–212

Meditation Task has been chosen as the task for this current study as it again). The Tself ranged from 13.33 s to 132.54 s (M = 58.12 s,
is thought to be a task with a high propensity for mind wandering and SD = 34.56 s). Fig. 2a shows the linear regression on the mean Tself as a
thus ideal to study the natural fluctuation between states of focus and function of the participants’ OSPAN score. The results did not show a
states of mind wandering (Braboszcz & Delorme, 2010; Hasenkamp significant correlation between working memory capacity score and the
et al., 2012; Levinson et al., 2012). average focus-mind wandering episode duration (r = −.15, t
(18) = −0.66, p = .52), suggesting that the mean focus-mind wan-
2.2. Procedure dering episode duration (Tfocus + TMW) was not affected by the OSPAN
score.
Participants were first given the OSPAN working memory test on a These results replicated those of previous studies that show that self-
computer. After completion of the OSPAN working memory test, par- caught task unrelated thoughts did not correlate with working memory
ticipants were instructed on how to practice mindfulness meditation. capacity in a breath counting task (Levinson et al., 2012). As stated in
Participants sat in a fully lit room to avoid drowsiness, but were in- the introduction, due to the self-caught method’s inclusion of different
structed to at least partially close their eyes to avoid distractions. components within a focus-mind wandering episode (Tself and TMW), it
Participants were told to focus only on the sensation of breath and not is unclear what these results alone mean in relationship to working
to count breaths in their head. During this task, participants were in- memory and mind wandering. To develop a new measurement to assess
structed to press a spacebar when they noticed their focus was not on different states of the focus-mind wandering episode, the probe method
their breath and their attention was instead focused on other thoughts is also needed. As such, Experiment 1b was conducted to measure the
(any thought that is not focused on the breath, for example a past event, basic parameters of probe-caught mind wandering and to examine the
a future plan, etc.). Anytime participants noticed a task- unrelated relationship between working memory capacity and mind wandering
thought, they were instructed to recognize the task unrelated-thought, using a modified probe-caught method.
push it out of their mind, and continue to focus on their breath.
Once participants felt comfortable in doing this, they were in-
structed to press the space bar on the computer when they recognized a 3. Experiment 1b
thought, then return to their breath. The computer ran a program on E-
prime Psychology Software 2.0, which tracked and recorded every This experiment followed the traditional probe-caught method to
button press during the meditation session. The participants completed examine whether working memory capacity affects the rate of mind
two 10-minute meditation sessions with a 5 min break in between wandering reports, with a methodological modification on the dis-
where they were allowed to use the restroom and have a drink of water. tribution of probes that addressed an important issue in the traditional
The total experiment took no longer than 50 min. methods.

2.3. Results and discussion


3.1. Methods
The OSPAN working memory scores ranged from 23 to 69
(M = 48.85, SD = 13.50). The self-caught mind wandering time (Tself) 3.1.1. Participants
was calculated as the mean interval between the start of a meditation Thirty-four undergraduate students aged 18–22 from the University
session to the first key press indicating the end of one focus-mind of Illinois participated in Experiment 1b. Similar to Experiment 1a, in
wandering episode, or between one key press and the next (i.e., from Experiment 1b all participants had less than a total of 10 h of medita-
the end of the previous mind wandering event after which participants tion practice experience during their lives and were novice meditators.
returned to meditation, until they caught themselves mind wandering

Fig. 2. Results of Experiments 1a and 1b. (a) The scatter plot of Tself as a function of the OSPAN scores in Experiment 1a showing the linear regression line. (b) The
scatter plot of p(MW) as a function of the OSPAN scores in Experiment 1b showing the linear regression line.

206
M.J. Voss et al. Cognition 179 (2018) 202–212

3.2. Procedure reported they were mind wandering decreased.


Experiments 1a and 1b measured some basic parameters of the self-
The design and procedure was the same as Experiment 1a except the and probe-caught mind wandering using our modified procedure, and
following. After completion of the OSPAN working memory test and the provided an initial test of the effect of working memory capacity on the
instruction on how to practice mindfulness meditation, a series of two two traditional measures of mind wandering tendency (Tself and p
meditation sessions were performed with a break in between. During (MW)). However, to better characterize the mind wandering process,
each session, participants were instructed to practice the mindfulness we need to estimate the two main states of mind wandering (Tfocus and
breath meditation, but this time when they heard a tone they were to TMW) separately. Experiment 2 developed a new method to calculate
respond with where their focus was at the time of the tone. If their focus the mean duration in the mind wandering state and duration in the
was solely on the sensation of their breath, they were instructed to focus state for each participant by combining the self-caught and probe-
respond with a V on the keyboard for yes they were focused on their caught method within the same participant.
breath. If they found themselves mind wandering at the time of the
tone, they were instructed to respond with an N on the keyboard for no 4. Experiment 2
they were not focused on their breath and were having other thoughts.
Once the participants began their mindfulness meditation, a com- In this experiment, each participant performed both the self-caught
puter program was run using E-prime Psychology Software 2.0. This and the probe-caught mind wandering sessions. The duration of focus
program was set to run three consecutive cycles of 8 different trials each (Tfocus) and duration of mind wandering (TMW) were calculated for each
(with no division or gap between cycles). Each trial was set to play a participant, and the relationship between their OSPAN scores and these
tone at one of these time intervals from the previous response: 5 s, 10 s, two duration estimates were tested separately to examine whether
15 s, 20 s, 25 s, 30 s, 35 s, and 40 s. All 8 trials were randomized within working memory capacity affects the two aspects of mind wandering,
each of the three cycles. Once the participants heard the tone, they had i.e., people’s ability to remain on task and resist the occurrence of mind
ten seconds to respond if their focus was on or off their breath. If the wandering (indicated by Tfocus), and the continuation/detection of
participants did not respond, the program would move to the next trial mind wandering once it occurs (indicated by TMW).
and the trial was marked as a “miss.” Once the participants responded,
the next time interval would begin until the participants were given all 4.1. Methods
three cycles of eight trials each for a total of 24 probes during the entire
meditation session. Once the session was over, the participant was The methods were the same as in Experiments 1a and 1b except the
given a five minute break. Upon conclusion of the break, the second following. 111 participants participated in the current study.
session of mindfulness breath meditation began. Each session lasted Participants ranged in age from 18 years of age to 22 years of age and
around 10.5 min. were recruited from the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
A key modification of the current procedure from previous studies is Like Experiment 1, only participants who have had less than a total of
the timing of the probe. Based on the theoretical discussion in the 10 h of meditation experience were included in the data analysis, to
Introduction, the probes need to be distributed randomly or uniformly avoid practice effects of meditation, as it has been shown that medi-
across the focus-mind wandering episode for a probe-caught measure to tation practice can improve focused attention and reduce mind wan-
accurately reflect the true proportion of time one is in a focus state dering (Braboszcz et al., 2010; Brewer et al., 2011; Hasenkamp et al.,
versus in a mind wandering state. That is, if more probes are distributed 2012; Lutz et al., 2008).
in the earlier portion of the focus-mind wandering episode, then the Each participant completed the OSPAN task and four Mindfulness
probe-caught mind wandering rate can potentially be under-estimated. Breath Mediation Sessions in the following order.
In contrast, if more probes are distributed in the later portion of the
focus-mind wandering episode, then the measured proportion of mind 1. Self-caught session 1: The participant performed the Mindfulness
wandering responses would be an over-estimation of the time people Breath Meditation Task and focus-mind wandering episodes were
spend mind wandering. Since the all-trial mean Tself measured in measured using the self-caught method. This session allowed us to
Experiment 1a was about 40 s, the probes in Experiment 1b were set to calculate the average focus-mind wandering episode duration.
the eight different time intervals evenly distributed across an average 2. Probe-caught session 1: The participant performed the Mindfulness
focus-mind wandering episode to provide a uniform sample of the mind Breath Meditation Task and mind wandering instances were mea-
wandering event and minimize any potential bias created by probing sured using the probe-caught method, where the probe intervals
shorter or longer intervals. were based on the average focus-mind wandering episode duration
calculated from the first self-caught session (further explained
below).
3.3. Results and discussion
3. Probe-caught session 2: If participants’ self-caught average time was
less than 80 s, participants performed a second probe-caught ses-
Five participants failed to respond within the 10-second response
sion. This session was exactly the same as probe-caught session 1. If
period in some of the trials and were not included in the data analysis.2
participants’ self-caught average time was longer than 80 s, parti-
For the remaining 29 participants, the OSPAN working memory scores
cipants only performed one probe-caught session, due to experi-
ranged from 4 to 75 (M = 51.10, SD = 18.28).
mental time limitations (further explained below).
Fig. 2b showed the linear regression on the proportion of mind
4. Self-caught session 2: The participant performed the Mindfulness
wandering responses (p(MW)) as a function of participants’ OSPAN
Breath Meditation Task and mind wandering episodes were mea-
scores to determine if there was any relation between their working
sured using the self-caught method. This session allowed us to cal-
memory capacity and the proportion of times they were mind wan-
culate the average focus-mind wandering episode duration, and was
dering. The p(MW) ranged from 0 to .77 (M = .39, SD = .18). The
exactly the same as the self-caught session 1. The session was con-
OSPAN working memory score was able to predict p(MW), (r = −.41, t
ducted to ensure that the timing measure between a participant’s
(27) = −2.37, p = .03). These data showed that as the participants'
first self-caught session and the last self-caught session remained
working memory capacity increased, the proportion of times they
consistent over time (further explained below).

2
The results remained the same when these participants were included in the data Overall, the entire experiment lasted approximately 2 h, where
(r = −.38, t(32) = −2.35, p = .02). participants were allowed to take a break in between sessions.

207
M.J. Voss et al. Cognition 179 (2018) 202–212

The self-caught session was the same as Experiment 1a except that it Tfocus = Tself ∗ (1−p (MW)) (1)
lasted 20 min (similar to Hasenkamp et al., 2012). The probe-caught
TMW = Tself ∗p (MW) (2)
session was the same as Experiment 1b, except that the timing of the
probes were based on the average self-caught time duration of the Tself is the average time of a focus-mind wandering episode calculated
participant’s own individual focus-mind wandering episode, calculated from the first and second self-caught sessions and ranged from 10.27 s
from the first self-caught session. Upon hearing a probe, participants to 124.90 s (M = 43.08 s, SD = 27.36 s). p(MW) is the proportion of
were asked to press the keyboard button “m” if their focus was on their time a participant said that s/he was mind wandering based on the
breath, and press a keyboard button “z” if their focus was off their probe caught session alone, and ranged from .23 to .83 (M = .47,
breath. These button presses were counterbalanced between partici- SD = .13). Both Tfocus and TMW were calculated separately for each
pants to account for possible response biases, where half of the parti- participant individually.
cipants were instructed to press “m” if their focus was on their breath The logic for Eqs. (1) and (2) is straightforward. As illustrated above
and “z” if there focus was off their breath, and half of the participants in Fig. 1a, Tself is a measure of the average duration of an individual’s
were instructed to press “m” if their focus was off their breath, and “z” focus-mind wandering episodes in seconds (when they start focusing on
if their focus was on their breath. Focus off their breath was defined as the task to when they catch themselves mind wandering), which is the
any thought that is not on the breath or breath sensation, such as future sum of the time of focus and the time of mind wandering. That is,
events, past events, personal events, and events that are happening
Tself = Tfocus + TMW (3)
around the world. The probe-caught session contained 4 cycles (each
cycle containing 8 probes), and was run using a program created in C+ Moreover, when sampled randomly or uniformly, the chance a
+. This session time varied depending on the average timing from the probe falls in the period of focus state (p(focus)) relative to the chance
self-caught session. of the probe falling in the period of mind wandering (p(MW) = 1 − p
The placement timing of the probes for the probe-caught session (focus)) is determined by the relative duration of time of focus and time
was based on the average duration of the self-caught session. After the of mind wandering in a focus-mind wandering episode. That is,
20 min self-caught session, the average duration of a focus-mind wan-
p (focus): p (MW) = Tfocus: TMW (4)
dering episode was calculated by finding the average duration in sec-
onds between button presses during the self-caught session (measuring Solving Eqs. (3) and (4), we will get Eqs. (1) and (2).
the start of focusing on breath, to when a participant realized their To test whether a participant’s first self-caught session and the last
mind had wandered). We then used this average to calculate the pre- self-caught session were generally consistent over time, the first self-
sentation timing of the probes for the following probe-caught sessions. caught session and the last self-caught session for each participant were
To determine when to present the probes for each participant, we di- compared. Of the 98 participants who had data from the first self-
vided the mean duration from the self-caught session by 8, and added caught session (ranging from 6.53 s to 353.94 s (M = 50.12 s,
three seconds to each time to ensure a minimal time that would be SD = 45.50 s)) and the second self-caught session (ranging from 6.91 s
meaningful to probe and allow time for participants to enter the med- to 304.46 s (M = 55.27 s, SD = 51.20 s)), there was a strong significant
itation state. For example, if a participant has an average of 40 s for the correlation between the average duration of the self-caught session one
self-caught session, the 8 probe delays for a cycle would be 8 s, 13 s, and the self-caught session two, r(96) = .78, p < .001.
18 s, 23 s, 28 s, 33 s, 38 s, and 43 s. These delays were measured from Because there were a relatively large number of participants ex-
participants’ response to the previous probe, e.g., an 8 s probe would be cluded from analysis, mostly due to incomplete data, the average
presented 8 s after the participant responded to the previous probe. The OSPAN score of included participants was compared with the average
order of the probes was randomized for each cycle. OSPAN score of excluded participants to determine whether we have
differentially excluded certain participants. A two-sample t-test showed
that there was not a significant difference in OSPAN scores for included
4.2. Results and discussion participants (M = 49.66, SD = 14.20) and OSPAN scores for excluded
participants (M = 49.33, SD = 15.16), t(1 0 1) = .11, p = .91,
Overall forty-seven participants were excluded from data analysis. d = 0.02, 95% CI [−5.55, 6.19].
Twelve participants (10.8%) were excluded from data analysis because
they had meditated for more than 10 h, eight participants (7.2%) were 4.2.2. Linear regression analysis
excluded from data analysis because the OSPAN score was not properly As illustrated in Fig. 3a, there was not a significant relationship
collected, twenty-two participants (19.8%) were excluded because of between the average duration of self-caught focus-mind wandering
incomplete/inaccurate data3 (none or less than 32 data points for the episodes calculated from reports using only the self-caught sessions
probe session, only one self-caught session, etc.), and five participants (Tself) and OSPAN score, r = .16, t(62) = 1.26, p = 0.21 with an R2 of
(4.5%) were excluded because there was a large discrepancy between .03, F(1, 62) = 1.60. This result replicated that of Experiment 1a. As
their self-caught 1 and self-caught 2 data (more than one minute).4 illustrated in Fig. 3b, there was a marginally significant relationship
Overall, data from sixty-four participants were analyzed for the current between proportion of mind wandering probes and OSPAN score,
study. r = −.24, t(62) = −1.96, p = .054 with an R2 of .06, F(1, 62) = 3.86.
This result was similar to that of Experiment 1b.
4.2.1. Calculating time of focus and time of mind wandering Of most importance to the current study, to analyze the relationship
The duration for the time of focusing on a task (Tfocus) and the time between working memory capacity and different states within a focus-
of mind wandering (TMW) were calculated by combining both the self- mind wandering episode, two linear regressions were performed sepa-
caught sessions and the probe caught sessions using the following for- rately: time of focus (Tfocus) as a function of OSPAN score, and time of
mulas: mind wandering (TMW) as a function of OSPAN score. As illustrated in
Fig. 3c ((Tfocus) as a function of OSPAN score), there was a significant
positive relationship between mean time within the focus state (fo-
3
The regression results remained the same when these participants were included (for cusing on their breath) and OSPAN score, r = .27, t(62) = 2.20, p = .03
Tself and OSPAN: r = .17, t(80) = 1.52, p = .13; for p(MW) and OSPAN: r = −.20, t
with an R2 of .07, F(1, 62) = 4.83. As OSPAN scores increased, so did
(80) = −1.85, p = .07; for Tfocus and OSPAN: r = .25, t(80) = 2.27, p = .03; for TMW and
OSPAN: r = .07, t(80) = .66, p = .51). the time within the focus state.
4
Discrepancies larger than 1 min were more than 2 standard deviations from the mean As illustrated in Fig. 3d (time in a mind wandering state (TMW) as a
and were in the long tail of the distribution. function of OSPAN score), there was not a significant relationship

208
M.J. Voss et al. Cognition 179 (2018) 202–212

Fig. 3. Results of Experiment 2. (a) The scatter plot along with the linear regression line of duration of a full focus-mind wandering episode (including both Tfocus and
TMW), calculated from the self-caught sessions alone (Tself), as a function of OSPAN score. (b) Scatter plot along with the linear regression line of the proportion of
time in the mind wandering state (mind wandering proportion or p(MW)) overall, calculated from the probe-caught session alone, as a function of OSPAN score. (c)
The scatter plot along with the linear regression line, of the mean time in the focus state (Tfocus) during a focus-mind wandering episode, calculated from both the self-
caught and probe-caught sessions, as a function of OSPAN score. (d) The scatter plot along with the linear regression line, of the mean time in the mind wandering
state (TMW) during a focus-mind wandering episode, calculated from both the self-caught and probe-caught sessions, as a function of OSPAN score.

between time of mind wandering and OSPAN score, r = .01, t memory capacity.
(62) = 0.05, p = .96 with an R2 of .00, F(1, 62) = 0.003. Two traditional measures of mind wandering tendency (mean
Overall, the results of mean time of focus (Tfocus) and mean time of duration/frequency of self-caught mind wandering and the proportion
mind wandering (TMW) were of most importance for the second ex- of mind wandering responses when probed) were estimated as a func-
periment. Results showed that Tfocus increased with greater working tion of working memory capacity (OSPAN score). In both experiments,
memory capacity, suggesting that individuals with higher working there was no significant correlation between working memory capacity
memory capacity were able to focus on the current task longer. Results and the mean duration of the self-caught focus-mind wandering epi-
did not show a significant relationship between TMW and working sode. This result replicated that of previous research (Levinson et al.,
memory capacity, suggesting that the ability to sustain the mind wan- 2012). However, there was a negative correlation between working
dering state and/or detect the mind wandering state was not affected by memory capacity and proportion of mind wandering responses in a
working memory capacity. probe-caught method (significant for Experiment 1b, marginally sig-
nificant for Experiment 2). This finding was in contradiction to that of
Levinson et al., 2012, but broadly consistent with other work in the
5. General discussion
literature (McVay & Kane, 2009; Randall et al., 2014).
More importantly, Experiment 2 was conducted to estimate the
The aim of the current experiments was to develop a more in-
mean time of focus (Tfocus) and mean time of mind wandering (TMW)
formative method to measure mind wandering, by calculating the mean
separately using the raw self-caught duration and the proportion of
time focused on the current task at hand (measuring the ability to stay
mind wandering measured in the same participants. The results showed
on task and resist mind wandering (Tfocus)) and mean time in a mind
a significant positive relationship between working memory capacity
wandering state (measuring processes for the continuation of mind
and time of focus (Tfocus). These results suggest that individuals with
wandering and/or ability to catch mind wandering (TMW)), and how
higher working memory capacities were better able to block out mind
these new measures relate to executive control as measured by working

209
M.J. Voss et al. Cognition 179 (2018) 202–212

wandering to focus on the current task at hand longer. Moreover, et al., 2012; Randall et al., 2014; Smallwood & Andrews-Hanna, 2013;
working memory capacity did not have a significant effect on the time Smallwood et al., 2013) could be partly due to issues related to the
of mind wandering (TMW) in a focus-mind wandering episode, sug- inappropriate probe sampling method, in addition to other moderating
gesting that once mind wandering occurs, it will last a comparable factors such as task demand. Allowing some variations of the delay to
amount of time regardless of the working memory capacity. make “random” or “unpredictable” probes (e.g., instead of probing
exactly every 3 min, one may probe every 2–4 min) may alleviate the
5.1. Implications on the methodology of mind wandering research issue to some degree, however it’s still an inappropriate sample unless
the variation window matches the focus-mind wandering episode
These findings have important implications both on theories of duration, which becomes equivalent to our method. Increasing the in-
mind wandering and on the methodologies used to study mind wan- terval between two probes may also alleviate the issue. Due to varia-
dering. There are three main implications regarding measurements of tions in Tself, when two consecutive probes are separated by more
mind wandering. First, a careful examination of the time course of a episodes in between, they become more independent of each other and
focus-mind wandering episode revealed that the traditional concept of the sampling may approximate a random sample. However, length-
“tendency to mind wander” is not self-coherent. That is, measurements ening the interval between probes may substantially increase the ex-
of how often people mind wander based on the self-caught method and perimental time/cost, and the traditional method is still theoretically
the probe-caught method can be intrinsically contradictory and do not inappropriate.
have a consistent interpretation. Therefore despite the intuitive appeal, Thus, although our sampling method is still not ideal (see more
researchers studying mind wandering should restrain from character- discussion on the limitations below), it’s theoretically more justified
izing mind wandering by the vague notion of “how often people mind and improved than the typical method used in previous research, and
wander”. Instead, characteristics of mind wandering may be better should be adopted in mind wandering research using on-line probe-
described by measuring the durations of its sub-components separately, caught methods instead of the traditional one.
such as Tfocus and TMW. Although the current study divided a focus-mind wandering episode
Second, we developed a new method to estimate the duration of the into only two main states, the same approach can be used to study mind
two states of a focus-mind wandering episode, which was a key dis- wandering events in a more fine grain scale by using more fine-tuned
tinction proposed in the Process-Occurrence Framework (Smallwood, questions in the probe tests. For example, instead of asking participants
2013). However, there has been no empirical method to measure them to make a two-alternative forced choice between “on-task” vs “mind
separately to date. Our method relies on the combination of the tradi- wandering,” one can include intermediate states such as partially on-
tional self-caught and the probe-caught methods, where the self-caught task/partially mind wandering (Mrazek et al., 2012). Using the finer
method provides a measure of the length of the entire episode, while scale probes, one may divide the states of a focus-mind wandering
the probe-caught method provides a measure of the relative proportion episode into more detailed sub-components, such as including a sub-
of each of the components. The duration of each component can thus be state of gradually entering the focus state at the beginning, a sub-state
calculated, providing the first empirical methodology to examine the of already being aware of mind wandering but having not terminated
time course of the sub-components of the focus-mind wandering epi- the task-unrelated thoughts yet, and so on. Our method can in principle
sode separately. be adopted to estimate the durations of these finer grain sub-
Third, we developed a new probe sampling scheme in the timing of components of the focus-mind wandering episode separately. In addi-
probe placement. The probe placement in our probe caught session was tion, our method may also be used to examine the effects of other
distributed uniformly within one focus-mind wandering episode cycle. factors, such as to differentiate spontaneous mind wandering vs. in-
This method of probe distribution was adopted because it was a ne- tentional mind wandering (Seli, Smallwood et al., 2015; Seli, Risko, &
cessary requirement for our experimental logic, i.e., for Equation #4 to Smilek, 2016; etc.), since the time course of the focus-mind wandering
hold. This new method corrected a common error in the probe place- episode is likely to be different for spontaneous and intentional mind
ments in previous research using the probe-caught method. The most wandering.
common probing scheme used in the literature was to distribute the
probes uniformly across an experimental session. For example, the 5.2. Implications on theories of mind wandering
participants may be probed every certain number of trials, or every
certain amount of time during an experiment. Although this method The potentially different effects of working memory capacity on
appears intuitive and reasonable, it is actually not appropriate and most Tfocus versus TMW in a focus-mind wandering episode suggest that the-
likely biased in general. For example, if during an experimental session, ories of mind wandering need to address these different states sepa-
a probe is presented every ∼3 min, this is not a uniform distribution of rately. As outlined in the Introduction, there is debate as to whether
probes across the focus-mind wandering episode, but a fixed delay mind wandering instances consume executive control resources
probe. Because the task (and the focus-mind wandering episode) re- (Resource Hypothesis), or if mind wandering is a result of a failure in
starts after each probe, probing the participants every 3 min means the executive control to block out mind wandering instances (Executive
probe always occurs 3 min after the start of each episode. If a partici- Control Hypothesis). Each theory predicts different patterns of how
pant tends to remain on task for ∼2 min, followed by ∼3 min of mind mind wandering should behave in relationship to executive control, as
wandering before she catches herself, then the probe will almost always measured by working memory capacity. For example, the Resource
catch her during the mind wandering state, and overestimate the true Hypothesis predicts that individuals with higher working memory ca-
proportion of time she’s in a mind wandering state. Similarly, if a pacity should report being in a mind wandering state more compared to
participant tends to have ∼4 min of task focus followed by ∼1 min of individuals who have lower working memory capacity (Levinson et al.,
mind wandering, then the probe is most likely going to catch him 2012; Rummel & Boywitt, 2014; Teasdale et al., 1995; Thomson et al.,
during the focus state and again mis-represent the likelihood he’s in a 2013; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006), while the Executive Control Hy-
mind wandering state. pothesis predicts that individuals with higher working memory capacity
As a result, probes evenly distributed across an experimental session should report being in a mind wandering state less compared to in-
generally will not provide an appropriate assessment of the proportion dividuals who have lower working memory capacity (McVay & Kane,
of time a given participant is in the mind wandering state, and conse- 2010, 2012).
quently could affect the estimation of its relation to other factors. For Both theories rely heavily on the idea of how much mind wandering
example, the apparent contradictory findings on the relationship be- one should have. However, neither theory in its current form is based
tween WMC and mind wandering in previous research (e.g., Levinson on a clear definition of what “more” mind wandering means. Our

210
M.J. Voss et al. Cognition 179 (2018) 202–212

analyses of the focus-mind wandering time course suggest that the 5.3. Limitations and future directions
concept of how much one mind wanders is intrinsically ambiguous. In
fact, the intuitive idea of the “tendency to mind wander” can have at Despite the improvements in our methodology, there are still some
least three totally different meanings. First, the tendency to mind limitations in the method we adopted in this study. The current study
wander can be defined as the likelihood that mind wandering will occur probed individuals in a uniform manner based on a fixed number de-
after one has started focusing on a task for a certain amount of time. rived from the average of the first self-caught session. However, the
The mean duration one can stay on the current task before switching to actual duration of the focus-mind wandering episode varied from one
mind wandering (i.e., Tfocus) provides an index for this likelihood. That episode to the next, therefore the uniform probe distribution is only an
is, the shorter the Tfocus, the higher one’s tendency/likelihood to mind approximation and may not fully reflect the true proportion of Tfocus
wander. Second, the tendency to mind wander can be defined as the and TMW in the focus-mind wandering episode. Ideally, the probe dis-
likelihood one will stay in a mind wandering state once it occurs. The tribution should match the individual episode durations. However, due
mean duration of mind wandering in each episode (i.e., TMW) measures to the unpredictability of the individual episode durations, it’s not
this type of tendency. That means the longer the TMW, the higher one’s possible to match all of them in practice. Whatever size of the window
tendency to mind wander. Third, the tendency to mind wander can also within which we choose to distribute the probes, we can only correctly
be defined as the total amount of time one ends up spending in mind estimate the proportion of mind wandering for focus-mind wandering
wandering. The proportion of mind wandering in a probe-caught episodes of that specific length, and estimation of all other events will
method provides a measure of this tendency. have some errors. For example, if we distribute probes within a range
Among these three different definitions of mind wandering ten- longer than the mean episode duration, we can match focus-mind
dency, the proportion of mind wandering is the only type that has been wandering episodes that happen to be at that particular length, how-
empirically examined in the literature. However, the theoretical ever as a result it will not properly estimate those episodes equal to or
meaning of this type of tendency is the most vague and most difficult to shorter than the average duration. Therefore as a compromise we tai-
interpret. As discussed in the Introduction, the proportion of time one lored the probe sampling distribution to the mean episode duration to
spends on mind wandering is determined jointly by two independent maximize the number of episodes we can match properly. Whether
quantities Tfocus and TMW, and does not have a clear correspondence to there are alternative ways to determine the probe distribution to further
the underlying mechanism or processes. Therefore this measure is improve the sampling accuracy remains a challenge for future research,
theoretically vague unless such a correspondence can be established. for example, by varying the sampling window location (e.g., instead of
Because these theories of mind wandering and existing experimental sampling from a fixed range within [3 s, Tself + 3 s], one may sample
work are all based on the ambiguous concept of “mind wandering from [x, Tself + x] with varying x), or by varying the sampling window
tendency,” the experimental findings are difficult to interpret with re- size according to the variance of Tself, instead of using a fixed value of
gards to whether they support or reject these theories. As such, these mean Tself. Whether these alternative methods improve the overall ac-
theories themselves are difficult to test empirically until the conceptual curacy of the estimation awaits further investigation.
vagueness of the above mentioned theories are clarified. Another consequence of our method is that by uniformly dis-
The Process-Occurrence Framework, on the other hand, provides a tributing the probes within a focus-mind wandering cycle, some of the
theoretical account for the two states of the focus-mind wandering probe delays were quite short. The frequent probe presentation could
episode separately. According to the Process-Occurrence Framework potentially interfere with typical task focus, especially for individuals
(Smallwood, 2013), executive control could influence mind wandering who have a short focus-mind wandering episode. One way to amelio-
directly by increasing focus on the task and thus inhibiting the initiation rate this limitation in future research is to move the time window for
of mind wandering from occurring. Smallwood (2013) argued that at- the uniform distribution of the probes past the average focus-mind
tentional control would help participants stay on task, so that “under wandering episode to increase the delay between each probing, espe-
tasks demanding continual external focus attentional control can limit cially for people with short Tself. In addition, the probe presentation was
the occurrence of self-generated thought by ensuring the continuity of a based on the average duration of the self-caught mind wandering
task-relevant train of thought” (p. 529). Our finding that time of focus measure for each individual. This means that each individual was
(Tfocus) positively correlates with working memory capacity is con- probed at different times depending on their self-caught mind wan-
sistent with this prediction. dering measure. Whether the different sampling times can change in-
Although working memory capacity affects one’s ability to maintain dividuals’ behavior is a topic for future research.
task focus, it does not show a reciprocal relationship to the duration of It is also important to stress that the relationship between working
the mind-wandering state. There are at least two possible explanations memory capacity and the durations of the two states of a focus-mind
for the independence of TMW from the working memory capacity, de- wandering episode found here is specific to the mindfulness of breath
pending on what determines the termination of the mind wandering task implemented. It has been well established that people’s mind
state. If the termination of a mind wandering state is due to the failure wandering can be affected by various factors, including the demand of
to sustain the internal thought processes (e.g., due to lack of resources), the current task (Levinson et al., 2012; Randall et al., 2014; Smallwood
then these results suggest that processes that sustain one on the current & Andrews-Hanna, 2013; Smallwood et al., 2013). Because the main
task are different from those that buffer the internal train of thought. task for the current study was a mindfulness breath meditation task,
Alternatively, it is also possible that the termination of a mind wan- which is a low demanding task, the relationships found here may only
dering state is primarily due to a separate, self-monitoring system that apply to low demanding tasks such as mindfulness meditation, and may
determines one’s meta-awareness of the mind wandering event. Thus, a not generalize to other tasks, specifically higher demand tasks that re-
mind wandering event may be terminated even if there are sufficient quire more cognitive control, or tasks where motivation to perform the
resources to maintain its continuation. Under this scenario, our findings task is higher. In those cases the relationship might even show the re-
would suggest that the self-monitoring/meta-awareness system is not serve pattern. For example, the moderating effect of task demand has
dependent on working memory capacity. Which of these hypotheses is been proposed to account for different results in the effects of working
true awaits future research. Regardless of the cause of the termination memory capacity on mind wandering in previous research. It was
of a mind wandering event, our findings provide empirical evidence suggested that in lower demanding tasks, individuals who have higher
that theories of mind-wandering need to account for multiple compo- working memory capacity might mind wander because of an excess of
nent processes, as emphasized in the Process-Occurrence Framework executive resources, but in tasks that require more attentional demands
(Smallwood, 2013). to perform, mind wandering can be a result of a failure of executive
control, showing different patterns of results in regards to task difficulty

211
M.J. Voss et al. Cognition 179 (2018) 202–212

(Levinson et al., 2012; Randall et al., 2014; Smallwood & Andrews- Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews- Hanna, J. R. (2016).
Hanna, 2013; Smallwood et al., 2013). Further research should address Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 17, 718–731.
the differences between these two states of mind wandering in relation Feng, S., D’Mello, S., & Graesser, A. C. (2013). Mind wandering while reading easy and
to task difficulty. difficult texts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 586–592.
Our method relies on an implicit assumption that the only way to Hasenkamp, W., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Duncan, E., & Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Mind
wandering and attention during focused meditation: A fine-grained temporal analysis
return to focus is through the self-awareness mechanism (self mon- of fluctuating cognitive states. Neuroimage, 59(1), 750–760.
itoring/meta-awareness system), where the participants realize that Helton, W. S., & Warm, J. S. (2008). Signal salience and the mindlessness theory of
they are mind wandering, then voluntarily terminate the mind wan- vigilance. Acta Psychologica, 129, 18–25.
Levinson, D. B., Smallwood, J., & Davidson, R. J. (2012). The persistence of thought:
dering process and return to the basic task, unless they are interrupted Evidence for a role of working memory in the maintenance of task-unrelated
by an external probe. This is an assumption implied (but not required) thinking. Psychological Science, 23, 375–380.
in previous theories of mind wandering (e.g., Schooler et al., 2011; Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and
monitoring in meditation. Trends Cognitive Science, 12(4), 163–169.
Smallwood, 2013). If individuals can return to focus without self
Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C.
awareness, and have multiple switches between the focus and mind N. (2007). Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought.
wandering states during a single self-caught focus-mind wandering Science, 315, 393–395.
episode, then the current method may not measure these switches, and McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought: Working memory
capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. Journal of
the estimations we obtained (Tfocus and TMW) do not indicate the mean Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(1), 196–204.
duration of each individual focus (or mind wandering) state, but in- McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect executive function or
stead indicate the total duration of the multiple focus (or mind wan- executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008).
Psychological Bulletin, 136, 188–197.
dering) states within a self-caught focus-mind wandering episode. McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2012). Drifting from Slow to “d’oh!” Working memory ca-
Whether there can be multiple switches without self-awareness be- pacity and mind wandering predict extreme reaction times and executive-control
tween focus and mind wandering states within a single self-caught errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(3),
525–549.
focus-mind wandering episode awaits future research. McVay, J. C., Kane, M. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2009). Tracking the train of thought from the
In summary, the current study developed a new methodology to laboratory into everyday life: An experience-sampling study of mind wandering
estimate the timing of different states within a focus-mind wandering across controlled and ecological contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(5),
857–863.
episode and measured how they are affected by working memory ca- Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Mindfulness and mind-wan-
pacity. The results showed that in a low demanding task, people with dering: Finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion, 12, 442–448.
higher working memory capacity tend to remain on the current task Phillips, N., Mills, C., D’Mello, S. K., & Risko, E. (2016). On the influence of re-reading on
mind wandering. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(12),
longer. However, once mind wandering occurred, it lasted for a com-
2338–2357.
parable duration regardless of the working memory capacity. These Poerio, G. L., Totterdell, P., & Miles, E. (2013). Mind-wandering and negative mood: Does
findings suggest that existing theories of mind wandering and experi- one thing really lead to another? Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 1412–1421.
mental work based on a vague, ambiguous concept of mind wandering Randall, J. G., Oswald, F. L., & Beier, M. E. (2014). Mind-wandering, cognition, and
performance: A theory-driven meta-analysis of attention regulation. Psychological
tendency need revision to address different states and processes in- Bulletin, 140, 1411–1431.
volved in mind wandering. Rummel, J., & Boywitt, C. D. (2014). Controlling the stream of thought: Working memory
capacity predicts adjustment of mind-wandering to situational demands. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 21, 1309–1315.
Acknowledgement Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, T. C., Reichle, E. D., & Sayette, M. A.
(2011). Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in
We would like to especially thank Mike Kane, Paul Seli, Jonathan Cognitive Sciences, 15, 319–326.
Seli, P., Carriere, J. S., Levene, M., & Smilek, D. (2013). How few and far between?
Smallwood, Dan Simons, and David Irwin for discussion of the findings, Examining the effects of probe rate on self-reported mind wandering. Frontiers in
and Sophia Dumlao, Dashiell Koester, Jackin Feng Sheh, Heidi Soon, Psychology, 4, 430.
and Faith Tan with help with data collection. This project was funded in Seli, P., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2016). On the necessity of distinguishing between
unintentional and intentional mind wandering. Psychological Science, 27, 685–691.
part by the National Science Foundation grant #1519407. Comments
Seli, P., Jonker, T. R., Cheyne, J. A., Cortes, K., & Smilek, D. (2015). Can research par-
should be sent to Meera Zukosky (email: zukosky1@illinois.edu). ticipants comment authoritatively on the validity of their self-reports of mind wan-
dering and task engagement? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 703–709.
Seli, P., Smallwood, J., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2015). On the relation of mind
Appendix A. Supplementary material
wandering and ADHD symptomatology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22, 629–636.
Smallwood, J. (2013). Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: A process-oc-
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the currence framework for self generated thought. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 519–535.
online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.013. Smallwood, J., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: The
importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Frontiers in
Psychology, 4, 441.
References Smallwood, J., Brown, K. S., Baird, B., Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., & Schooler, J. W.
(2012). Insulation for daydreams: A role for tonic norepinephrine in the facilitation of
internally guided thought. PLoS ONE, 7, e33706.
Antrobus, J. S. (1968). Information theory and stimulus-independent thought. British Smallwood, J. M., Fitzgerald, A., Miles, L. K., & Phillips, L. H. (2009). Shifting moods,
Journal of Psychology, 59, 423–430. wandering minds: Negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion, 9, 271–276.
Bastian, M., & Sackur, J. (2013). Mind wandering at the fingertips: Automatic parsing of Smallwood, J., O’Connor, R. C., Sudbery, M. V., & Obonsawin, M. (2007). Mind-wan-
subjective states based on response time variability. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 573. dering and dysphoria. Cognition Emotion, 21, 816–842.
Braboszcz, C., & Delorme, A. (2010). Lost in thoughts: Neural markers of low alertness Smallwood, J., Ruby, F. J., & Singer, T. (2013). Letting go of the present: Mind-wandering
during mind wandering. Neuroimage, 54, 3040–3047. is associated with reduced delay discounting. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(1), 1–7.
Braboszcz, C., Hahusseau, S., & Delorme, A. (2010). Meditation and neuroscience: From Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6),
basic research to clinical practice. In R. Carlstedt (Ed.), Integrative clinical psychology, 946–958.
psychiatry and behavioral medicine: Perspectives, practices and research (pp. Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically
1910–1929). New York, NY: Springer Publishing. navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1).
Brewer, J., Worhunsky, P., Gray, J., Tang, Y., Weber, J., & Kober, H. (2011). Meditation Teasdale, J. D., Dritschell, B. H., Taylor, M. J., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., Nimmo-Smith, I.,
experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and con- et al. (1995). Stimulus-independent-thought depends upon central executive re-
nectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 20254–20259. sources. Memory and Cognition, 28, 551–559.
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default net- Thomson, D. R., Besner, D., & Smilek, D. (2013). In pursuit of off-task thought: Mind
work: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy wandering-performance trade-offs while reading aloud and color naming. Frontiers in
of Sciences, 1124, 1–38. Psychology, 4, 360.
Christoff, K., Gordon, A., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. (2009). Experience Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of
sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to the operation span task. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 498–505.
mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 8719–8724.

212

You might also like