You are on page 1of 15

Journal of Youth and Adolescence

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-018-0822-9

EMPIRICAL RESEARCH

The Rise and Fall of Depressive Symptoms and Academic Stress in


Two Samples of University Students
Erin T. Barker1 Andrea L. Howard2 Rosanne Villemaire-Krajden1 Nancy L. Galambos3
● ● ●

Received: 19 October 2017 / Accepted: 1 February 2018


© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
Self-reported depressive experiences are common among university students. However, most studies assessing depression in
university students are cross-sectional, limiting our understanding of when in the academic year risk for depression is
greatest and when interventions may be most needed. We examined within-person change in depressive symptoms from
September to April. Study 1 (N = 198; 57% female; 72% white; Mage = 18.4): Depressive symptoms rose from September,
peaked in December, and fell across the second semester. The rise in depressive symptoms was associated with higher
perceived stress in December. Study 2 (N = 267; 78.7% female; 67.87% white; Mage = 21.25): Depressive symptoms
1234567890();,:

peaked in December and covaried within persons with perceived stress and academic demands. The results have implications
for understanding when and for whom there is increased risk for depressive experiences among university students.
Keywords Depressive symptoms Academic stress University students Longitudinal
● ● ●

Introduction college students rose substantially from the 1930s through


2010 (Twenge et al. 2010).
On average, psychological well-being improves across the With greater proportions of the young adult population
transition to adulthood (Schulenberg and Zarrett 2006) and pursuing post-secondary education (Bureau of Labor Sta-
depressive symptoms in particular decline from age 18 to 25 tistics U.S. Department of Labor 2014; Galarneau et al.
(Galambos et al. 2006). At the same time, prevalence rates 2013), high rates of depression in this population likely
for major depression peak during this transition (Pearson reflect, at least in part, the fact that more people vulnerable
et al. 2013; Rohde et al. 2012) and many university students to depression are attending university (e.g., Rohde et al.
in particular screen above clinical cut-off scores for major 2012). That said, experiences specific to the university
depression (Eisenberg et al. 2007). A recent review of context may also contribute to depressive experiences.
prevalence rates of depression among university students Given that depressive symptoms vary across the academic
from around the world showed that, on average, 30% of year (e.g., Cooke Bewick et al. 2006) and that both objec-
undergraduate students experience depression (Ibrahim tive and subjective experiences of academic stress are
et al. 2013). Moreover, a recent birth cohort analysis in the associated with depressive symptoms (e.g., Chambel and
United States showed that rates of major depression among Curral 2005), it is likely that stress inherent in the university
context contributes to variation in university students’
depressive experiences.
Although many studies have measured depressive
experiences in university students, most have done so cross-
* Erin T. Barker sectionally. Results from cross-sectional studies tell us only
erin.barker@concordia.ca
who is at greater risk—that some students are more or less
1
Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, at risk than others—but do not speak to when students are at
Canada greatest risk. In the current study, we examined within-
2
Department of Psychology, Carleton University, Ottawa, ON, person patterns of change in depressive symptoms in two
Canada samples of Canadian university students, each of which was
3
Department of Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AL, followed across one academic year. We aimed to determine
Canada when risk for depression may be highest by assessing
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

whether increases in depressive symptoms coincided with Depressive symptoms and perceived stress have been
expected periods of high academic stress (e.g., the end of shown to correlate with academic demands in several uni-
semester when final papers and exams are due) and whether versity samples from different parts of the world (e.g., in
within-person patterns of change in depressive symptoms Europe, Haldorsen et al. 2014; Mikolajczyk et al. 2008; and
were related to academic demands (i.e., workload) and in China, Sha and Xia 2004). In a recent cross-sectional
perceived stress. study of 900 Canadian university students (Newcomb-Anjo
et al. 2017), perceived stress associated with academic
Depression in University Students demands was related to elevated depressive symptoms after
controlling for the effects of 13 established risk factors for
University students around the world experience elevated depression (e.g., demographic characteristics, history of
depressive symptoms and many are at risk for clinical abuse, cognitive style and personality, recent stressful life
depression (for a review, see Ibrahim et al. 2013). In a study events, social support). Others have shown that students
of U.S. students, 53% indicated that they had experienced who report many demands and who also appraise them as
what they considered depression since starting college (Furr stressful report the most depressive symptoms (Chambel
et al. 2001), with similar percentages found in countries as and Curral 2005; Haldorsen et al. 2014).
diverse as France (Bouteyre et al. 2006), Malaysia (Sham- These associations could be explained by models of
suddin et al. 2013), and Kenya (Othieno et al. 2014). Of the stress and coping that emphasize the role of appraisal in the
few longitudinal studies that assessed change in depressive stress response (Lazarus and Folkman 1984), including
experiences in university samples, symptom levels tended Pancer and colleagues’ university adjustment model (Pancer
to increase over time. For example, depressive symptoms et al. 2004). Appraisal involves evaluating demands in the
rose between orientation and seven months later in first-year environment against the availability of resources, such as
students in the eastern U. S. (Alfred-Liro and Sigelman effective coping styles and social support. According to
1998). Similar increases were found in three longitudinal these models, if a demand is deemed threatening and
studies conducted in the United Kingdom. In the first, stu- resources considered insufficient, perceived stress and
dents assessed before entering and six weeks after starting associated negative affect will be elevated, possibly leading
university showed significant increases in depressive to adjustment difficulties such as mental health problems.
symptoms (Fisher and Hood 1987). In the second study, Drawing on general appraisal models, Pancer et al. (2004)
depressive symptoms were higher during students’ second developed their model to explain positive and negative
year of university compared to the month before starting adjustment outcomes for students coping with university
university (Andrews and Wilding 2004). In the third challenges. According to Pancer et al., students appraise the
study, incoming students were surveyed before starting stressors they face during their transition to university, and
university and three times across the first year; depressive deem them manageable if resources are adequate. Con-
symptoms were highest at the end of first semester (Cooke versely, if a mismatch between stressors and resources is
et al. 2006). Thus, it appears that the university context perceived, risk of experiencing negative outcomes such as
increases risk for depression, and that symptoms may depression increases. Stressors and resources in the aca-
fluctuate over time. demic domain pertain to workloads, aptitude, and study and
time management skills.
Stress in University

University students are confronted with multiple stressors, Current Study


including academic demands (Montgomery and Côté 2003),
and show high levels of subjective distress associated with Studies examining the association between depressive
these demands. Recent results from the American College symptoms and academic stress in university students at a
Health Association National College Health Assessment single point during the academic year do not capture within-
(American College Health Association 2016) survey person change in symptoms and stress. Cross-sectional
showed that 42.3% of Canadian university student partici- studies might provide information about which students are
pants reported overwhelming levels of anxiety during the more at risk, but cannot impart knowledge about when
previous year. More (58.1%) found their academic work students are at greater risk. Furthermore, results from cross-
very difficult to handle. In a large sample of first-year sectional studies cannot determine whether risk is occurring
university students in Germany, Poland, and Bulgaria, between individuals or within individuals as a function of
course work was rated as the greatest burden compared to the oscillating demands inherent in the university context.
relationships or concerns about the future (Mikolajczyk Separating between and within-person effects is important
et al. 2008). for understanding whether risk is a function of the
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

individual or the context, or both (Curran et al. 2012; described the study to students who were present on the day
Hoffman and Stawski 2009). If depressive symptoms rise of their recruitment visit, and students interested in parti-
and fall in conjunction with fluctuating demands, the aca- cipating provided contact information. Students were then
demic context is likely partly accountable for changes in contacted by email and invited to complete an initial paper-
students’ mood. Determining whether depressive symptoms and-pencil questionnaire in groups at the beginning of the
ebb and flow with academic demands will clarify when and semester. A total of 198 students attended initial sessions in
for whom risk for depression is highest. September or October (baseline), where they completed
In the current study, depressive symptoms were assessed demographic questions and measures of depressive symp-
multiple times across one academic year in two separate toms. Participants were then invited to complete web-based
samples of Canadian university students. In both samples, questionnaires each month across their first year (through
waves of measurement coincided with periods of relatively April). Depressive symptoms measures were assessed at the
low (start-of-semester) and relatively high (end-of-semester) baseline, December and April waves of measurement.
academic workloads. In Study 1, global academic workload Measures of academic demands and perceived stress were
and global perceived stress were assessed at the end of the included in the November and December assessments. At
first semester. In Study 2, global academic workload and all waves of measurement, informed consent was obtained
perceived stress specific to academic demands were mea- from participants who completed that wave. The study was
sured multiple times across the academic year during both approved by the university research ethics review commit-
low and high stress periods. tee in accordance with the Government of Canada’s Tri-
Given that depressive symptoms in university students Council Policy Statement: Ethical Conduct for Research
may represent, at least in part, a response to stress asso- Involving Humans.
ciated with academic demands, we hypothesized that All 198 participants had complete data on the outcome
depressive symptoms would rise toward the end of the measure (depressive symptoms) at baseline; four partici-
semester, and that their peak would be associated with pants were missing demographic variables used as covari-
periods of increased academic workload as measured by the ates in the analysis, reducing the sample to 194. Of these
number of recent academic demands. Given the role of participants, 171 (88%) completed end-of-semester assess-
appraisal in the stress process, we further hypothesized that ments in November or December and 152 (78%) completed
depressive symptoms would be higher for students who the end-of-semester assessment in April. Participants who
appraise their academic demands as unmanageable. completed all waves (n = 144, 74%) were compared on all
study variables to participants who only completed baseline.
The groups differed on one variable: participants who only
Study 1: Method completed baseline had higher baseline depressive symp-
toms scores than participants who completed all three waves
Participants (t = 2.40, p < .05).

Participants were 198 full-time first-year students at a large Measures


Canadian university taking part in Making the Transition II,
a web-based study of health-related behaviors, well-being, Depressive symptoms
and academic performance. Sixty percent of students iden-
tified as female (n = 113), and students’ ages ranged from The 10-item version (CESD-10; Andresen et al. 1994) of
17.5 to 19.8 years (M = 18.4, SD = .44). Self-reports the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale
showed that the racial/ethnic distribution was 72% white, (CES-D; Radloff 1991) measured depressive symptoms.
16% Asian or South Asian, 5% mixed ethnicity, and 5% Participants completed the scale in September or early
another visible minority (two students declined to report). October (baseline/start-of-semester 1), in December (end-
About half of students lived at home with parents (53%). of-semester 1), and in April (end-of-semester 2) of their first
Most students’ mothers (73%) and fathers (75%) had year of university. Participants were asked how often in the
completed two-year college or four-year university degrees. past week they had experienced each of 10 depressive
Given these characteristics, this sample is similar to student symptoms (e.g., felt depressed, felt fearful, felt lonely).
populations at other large 4-year Canadian universities. Responses ranged from 0 [rarely or none of the time (less
than 1 day)] to 3 [most or all of the time (5-7 days)]. The
Procedures CESD-10 was developed and found to be reliable and valid
in samples of older adults, and it compared well with the
In Fall 2005, participants were recruited from compulsory full 20-item version (Andresen et al. 1994). Total scores
first-year classes across campus. Research assistants were calculated for descriptive purposes to assess clinically
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

significant levels of symptomatology (scores of 10 or person model included a random intercept to estimate
greater; Andresen et al. 1994). Mean scores were used in baseline levels of depressive symptoms, and linear and
our main analyses, with higher scores indicating more quadratic effects of time to assess the pattern of change in
symptoms in the past week. Cronbach’s alphas for the three depressive symptoms across the academic year. Eq. 1 shows
waves were .80, .82, and .83, respectively. the Level 1 model predicting depressive symptoms
(DEPRESS) at each wave t for each person i, from the linear
End-of-semester academic workload combination of a random intercept (β0i), effects of linear
time (β1iLINEAR) and quadratic time trends (β2iQUAD), and
In November and December students were asked to indicate residual deviations of depressive symptom scores at each
the number of grades that had been returned to them (e.g., wave t for each person i from the average trajectory (rti):
on tests, assignments) in the past 14 days. Some students
were missing values for either November (n = 23) or DEPRESSti ¼β0i þβ1i LINEARþβ2i QUAD þ rti ð1Þ
December (n = 30). To maximize the available n, reports
for these two months were averaged and used as an indi- The Level 2 between-persons model included the effects of
cator of end-of-semester workload or academic demands. demographic covariates, perceived stress and academic
workload on the intercept and the effects of perceived
End-of-semester perceived stress stress, academic workload, and their interaction on the
linear and quadratic effects of time. Eq. 2 shows a
The 4-item version of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; simplified Level 2 model containing stress and academic
Cohen et al. 1983) was administered in November and workload covariates:
December. Participants were asked to indicate how often
over the past 2 weeks they felt 1) unable to control the
β0i ¼ γ 00 þ γ 01 STRESSi þ γ 02 WORKLOADi þ u0i
important things in their lives; 2) confident about their
ability to handle personal problems (reverse scored); 3) that β1i ¼ γ 10 þ γ 11 STRESSi þ γ 12 WORKLOADi ð2Þ
things were going their way (reverse scored); and 4) that β2i ¼ γ 20 þ γ 21 STRESSi þ γ 22 WORKLOADi
difficulties were piling up so high that they could not
overcome them. Items were rated on a 5-point scale ranging Estimates include average levels of depressive symptoms
from 0 (never) to 4 (very often). Reports for both months at baseline (γ00), the instantaneous rate of change in
were averaged and used as a measure of perceived stress at symptoms (γ10; “linear” growth), average acceleration/
the end of the semester. Higher scores indicated greater deceleration in the rate of change in symptoms (γ20;
perceived stress. Cronbach’s alpha for the combined “quadratic” growth), associations between perceived stress
November and December scale was .85. and academic workload at the end of semester 1 and
baseline depressive symptoms (γ01 and γ02, respectively),
Control variables and estimates of stress and workload differences in the
rates of change in depressive symptoms (γ11 and γ21 for
Main analyses controlled for demographic variables that perceived stress; γ12 and γ22 for academic workload).
could be related to depressive symptoms. These included Deviations of individual mean scores from the conditional
mother’s education (a proxy for family socioeconomic mean level of depressive symptoms are captured in
status/SES), race/ethnicity, and sex. Living situation (whe- the error term u0i. The linear and quadratic time trends were
ther the student lived with parents or not) was also treated as fixed (nonrandomly varying), because pre-
controlled. liminary model testing showed that these effects did not
vary significantly across participants. Thus, any deviations
Data Analysis Plan of individual trajectories from the mean rates of change,
conditional on stress and workload, are captured by residual
Multilevel linear models were estimated using HLM version error at Level 1. Additional effects not shown in Eq. 2
7.01 (Raudenbush et al. 2011), using restricted maximum (intercept covariates; interaction terms for stress and
likelihood estimation to account for missing data. This workload) are entered in Eq. 2, as we have shown for stress
procedure computes an individual likelihood function for and workload.
each participant based on available data, provided that Models were tested in two steps, beginning with
complete predictor data are present for each wave of unconditional growth models to establish an optimal func-
available outcome data. Importantly, cases contributing tional form of growth in depressive symptoms. Perceived
partial outcome data are retained and leveraged to improve stress, academic workload, their interaction, and all cov-
the accuracy of the model estimates. The Level 1 within- ariates were added simultaneously in a second step.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Study 1: Results

Academic Workload
End of Semester 1
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations are presented in
Table 1. A score of 10 or above on the 10-item version of

4.41 (2.40)
the CES-D represents clinically significant symptom levels
and is comparable to a score of 16 or above on the 20-item

.05
.00
−.03
.09
version of the CES-D (Andresen et al. 1994). Total scores
for depressive symptoms at all three waves were around the

Perceived Stress End


clinically significant level, on average. Furthermore, 38.8%,
50.3%, and 41.8% of students had depressive symptoms

of Semester 1
scores equal to or greater than 10 at baseline, December,

1.77 (.72)
and April, respectively. The average perceived stress score

.59***
.76***
.57***
was low, falling between the Likert-scale anchors of
“almost never” and “sometimes.” Students reported having
had on average between 4 and 5 academic demands in the
past 14 days in November/December. Scores on the CES-D

Depressive Symptoms
were highly intercorrelated across waves and with end-of-

End of Semester 2
semester perceived stress. End-of-semester 1 academic

10.37 (5.84)
demands was not correlated with CES-D scores from any
wave nor with end-of-semester 1 perceived stress.

.50***
.56***
Multilevel models of within-person change in CES-D
scores (Table 2) showed significant linear and quadratic
effects of time for depressive symptoms (controlling for the
effects of covariates on initial status), with symptoms
Depressive Symptoms

peaking in December (Fig. 1). In these models, linear time


End of Semester 1

was coded 0, 1, 2, corresponding to each wave of mea-

11.01 (5.74)
surement. Symptoms initially increased at a steeper rate for
students with higher end-of-semester levels of perceived
.60***

stress, but switched direction and were declining by the end


Table 1 Sample 1 Descriptive Statistics and Intercorrelations among Study Variables

of the academic year. At the December peak, estimated


depressive symptoms for students with higher stress were
Depressive Symptoms

82% of a standard deviation above average, falling slightly


Start of Semester 1

to 72% of a standard deviation above average by the end of


April. End-of-semester 1 academic demands and the inter-
9.88 (5.29)

action between perceived stress and academic demands


were not related to depressive symptom trajectories. None
of the control variables were significant predictors of
depressive symptom levels.

Study 2: Method
Depressive Symptoms Start of Semester 1
Depressive Symptoms End of Semester 1
Depressive Symptoms End of Semester 2

Study 2 replicates and extends Study 1 with a different


Perceived Stress End of Semester 1

sample of Canadian university students. Study 2 collected


*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001

four waves of data from students enrolled in any year of


study (across the 2013–2014 academic year). Depressive
symptoms, perceived stress, and academic workload were
measured at all waves. This afforded the opportunity to 1)
test the hypothesis that academic workload and stress
associated with those demands would rise toward the end of
Mean (SD)

the semester; 2) examine within-person associations of


perceived stress and academic workload with depressive
symptoms; and 3) to test whether the pattern of change in
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Table 2 Sample 1 Multilevel Model of Between-Person Effects of 2.4


End-of-Semester 1 Perceived Stress and Academic Workload on Level
(intercept) and Change in Depressive Symptoms Across One Year
2.0
Fixed Effects Coefficient SE

Depressive Symptoms
Intercept (random), π0 1.6

Intercept (initial status September) 1.081*** .105


Mother’s Education −.042 .030 1.2
Race/Ethnicity (white = 0; another = 1) −.073 .056
Sex (male = 0; female = 1) .026 .044
0.8
Living Situation (with parents = 1; other = 0) .076 .046
Perceived Stress (end-of-semester 1 average) .435*** .044
0.4
Academic Workload (end-of-semester 1 average) −.003 .012
Linear time slope (fixed), π1
0.0
Intercept .253*** .066
September December April
Perceived Stress (end-of-semester 1 average) .339*** .088
Wave of Assessment
Academic Workload (end-of-semester 1 average) −.019 .022
Perceived Stress X Academic Workload −.026 .032 High Stress Low Stress

Quadratic time slope (fixed), π2


Fig. 1 Study 1: Trajectories of depressive symptoms across the aca-
Intercept −.106** .033 demic year for students with high perceived stress levels at the end of
Perceived Stress (end-of-semester 1 average) −.158*** .043 semester 1 (+1 SD above the mean) and low perceived stress levels at
Academic Workload (end-of-semester 1 average) .003 .012 the end of semester 1 (−1 SD below the mean). Dashed lines represent
95% confidence intervals
Perceived Stress X Academic Workload .012 .019
Random Effects Estimate SE
Intercept .047*** .216 Procedures
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < = .001
Potential participants were recruited at student events held
at the start of an academic year and by flyers posted on
depressive symptoms and associations with perceived stress campus. Those who agreed to participate were sent an email
and academic workload were the same across years of that described the study and provided a link to an online
study. survey. Similar to Study 1, the survey assessed many con-
structs relevant to the well-being of university students.
Participants These procedures likely contributed to the greater propor-
tion of female participants in Study 2. It has been shown
Participants in Study 2 were 267 (78.7% identified as that more women than men participate in this type of stu-
female) full-time undergraduate students between the ages dent survey research (Gosling et al. 2004).
of 18 and 25 years (M = 21.25, SD = 1.67) enrolled in their Data collection for the study proceeded over four waves
first university degree at a large urban university located in a spanning one academic year: Wave 1 = start-of-semester 1
different Canadian province than that of the Study 1 sample. (September/early October); Wave 2 = end-of-semester 1
Participants in Study 2 were relatively evenly spread over (late November/early December); Wave 3 = start-of-seme-
the first (26.0%), second (32.0%), third (29.0%), and fourth ster 2 (late January); Wave 4 = end-of-semester 2 (late
(13.0%) years of university study or beyond (maximum 6th March/early April). At each wave, participants who had at
year). Over two-thirds of participants (67.8%) self-identified least partially completed one or more previous online sur-
as white and not belonging to a visible racial or ethnic vey(s) were contacted via email and invited to complete a
minority group, 7.8% identified as Black, 10.5% as Asian, new survey. For each survey, participants were required to
3.3% as Native North American, 6.7% as Hispanic, and give their free and informed consent, and indicate that they
4.5% as Arab. A further 13.9% identified as both white and were between the ages of 18 and 25 years and enrolled in
belonging to a visible racial or ethnic minority group. their first university degree program. Participants who
Similar to the Study 1 sample, about half (51.3%) of Study completed surveys at each wave were sent an electronic gift
2 participants reported living at home with their parents and card of their choice. The study was approved by the uni-
75.1% percent of participants’ mothers and 76.1% of par- versity research ethics review committee in accordance with
ticipants’ fathers had completed two-year (college), four- the Government of Canada’s Tri-Council Policy Statement:
year (university), or higher education degrees. Ethical Conduct for Research Involving Humans.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Of the 267 full-time students who completed the Wave semester 1, 42% of students had a quiz in the past two
1 survey, 256 (96%) had complete data on the outcome weeks and 61% expected another quiz in the next 2 weeks.
measure (depressive symptoms) at Wave 1. Across the At the end of semester 1, 63% had a quiz in the past
study variables, 23 participants were missing information 2 weeks and 24% expected a quiz in the next two weeks. In
on demographic variables used as covariates or predictor contrast, at the start of semester 1, 16% of students had an
variables in the main analysis. Of the 244 participants with exam in the previous 2 weeks and 56% reported an
complete Wave 1 data, 198 (74.2%) completed Wave 2 upcoming exam in the next two weeks. At the end of
assessments, 165 (61.8%) completed Wave 3 assessments, semester 1, 66% of students had an exam in the previous
and 146 (54.7%) completed Wave 4 assessments. Partici- 2 weeks and 89% expected an exam within two weeks.
pants who completed all waves (n = 127; 47.6%) were
compared on all study variables to participants who com- Perceived stress associated with current academic
pleted only Wave 1. The groups differed on only two workload
variables. First, men were more likely to drop out of the
study. Of participants who did not complete all waves, 30% As in Study 1, the 4-item version of the Perceived Stress
were men, and only 13.4% of students who completed all Scale (PSS; Cohen et al. 1983) measured perceived stress.
waves were men (χ2 = 9.92, p < .05). Second, students who The items were the same as the PSS 4-item scale described
completed all waves reported having more academic in Study 1, but were posed after participants completed their
demands (6.75 tests or assignments) at Wave 1 compared to reports of recent academic workload and were framed
students who did not complete all waves (6.0 tests or around these demands rather than life in general (e.g., “How
assignments; t = 2.31, p < .05). The same patterns emerged often have you felt that difficulties in your academic work
when an ANOVA was conducted to compare participants were piling up so high that you could not overcome them”;
based on the total number of waves completed. In total 57 “How often did you feel confident about your ability to
participants (21%) completed only Wave 1, 39 (15%) handle your academic work”). Mean scores were calculated
completed Wave 1 plus one other wave, and 44 (16%) with higher scores indicating higher perceived academic
completed Wave 1 plus two other waves. stress. Cronbach’s alphas ranged from .76 to .81 across
waves.
Measures
Control variables
Depressive symptoms
Main analyses controlled for demographic variables:
In Study 2, the complete 20-item Center for Epidemiologic mother’s education (to assess SES), race/ethnicity, and sex.
Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) was administered at all Living situation (whether the student lived with parents or
four waves (Radloff 1977). Total scores were calculated for not) was also controlled, as was year of university study.
descriptive purposes to assess clinically significant levels of
symptomatology (scores of 16 or greater). Mean scores Data Analysis Plan
were calculated for main analyses. Higher scores reflected
elevated frequency of depressive symptoms experienced in Multilevel linear models were estimated using Mplus soft-
the previous week. Cronbach’s alphas ranged from .88 to ware (Version 7.0). Multiple imputation for clustered data
.92 across waves. was used to retain all 267 participants in our analyses (joint
modeling using an unstructured within-cluster covariance
Current academic workload matrix; Asparouhov and Muthén 2010; Enders et al. 2016).
Results pool over estimates from 50 imputed datasets.
At all four waves, participants reported the number of Within-person (time-varying) estimates of academic
quizzes, exams, papers, presentations, labs, readings, pro- demands and perceived stress were respectively calculated
blem sets, and other course work they completed in the past by subtracting, for each person, the mean of his/her own set
two weeks and how many of these assessments they would of repeated measures from their score at each wave of
have to complete in the upcoming two weeks. Reports for assessment (person mean centering, e.g., Curran and Bauer
both two-week periods were totaled to create an index of 2011; Howard 2015). Between-person (average) estimates
academic workload. Frequencies for each type of academic were the means for each person across their own set of
demand at each wave of measurement showed that more repeated measures of academic demands and perceived
students had labs, readings, problem sets, and quizzes ear- stress, respectively. These averages were included in the
lier in the semester and more exams, papers, and pre- model to examine individual differences in depressive
sentations later in the semester. For example, at the start of symptoms explained by students carrying higher average
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Table 3 Sample 2 Descriptive


Start of Semester End of Semester Start of Semester End of Semester
Statistics, by Wave of
1 1 2 2
Measurement
Variable M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

Depressive Symptoms 14.90 (9.39) 17.72 (11.64) 15.51 (10.48) 15.78 (9.55)
(Total Scores)
Perceived Stress 1.62 (1.68) 1.81 (.78) 1.51 (.76) 1.65 (.77)
Academic Workload 6.40 (2.49) 6.67 (2.49) 6.03 (2.63) 6.33 (2.84)

academic loads and students reporting higher levels of stress each wave of measurement, average total scores for
on average. Separate estimates of the time-varying and depressive symptoms were around the clinically significant
average components allow us to test unique hypotheses score of 16 (Radloff 1977). Across the academic year, at
about effects of within-person variation and between-person Waves 1 through 4 respectively, 38.2%, 46.0%, 39.3%, and
differences in academic demands and stress on depression 41.2% of students scored equal to or greater than 16. Means
over time. Tests of within-person effects are orthogonal to for perceived stress were low, falling between the Likert
their between-person counterparts, allowing us to triangu- scale anchors of “almost never” to “sometimes” across the
late on specific sources of influence on students’ depressive year. Students reported having between six and seven aca-
symptoms. demic demands, on average, at each wave.
As in Study 1, our Level 1 model included linear and Intercorrelations between depressive symptoms with
quadratic time trends, with the addition of terms capturing perceived stress and counts of academic workload for each
time-varying effects of perceived stress and academic wave of measurement are presented in Table 4. Within and
workload, as follows: across waves, depressive symptoms and perceived stress
DEPRESSti ¼ β0i þ β1i LINEAR þ β2i QUAD were consistently moderately correlated. Depressive symp-
ð3Þ toms and counts of academic workload were uncorrelated at
þβ3i STRESSti þ β4i WORKLOADti þ rti
most waves. Additionally, three correlations between per-
Our Level 2 equations were similar to those shown in Study ceived stress scores and academic workload were sig-
1, except that we had sufficient variability to estimate nificant: Wave 2 workload (end-of-semester 1) with both
random effects for each of the time trends (u1i and u2i added Wave 1 perceived stress (r = .16, p < .05) and Wave 2
to Eq. 2 for the linear and quadratic time trends, perceived stress (r = .20, p < .05); Wave 3 perceived stress
respectively), capturing systematic individual differences with Wave 3 workload (r = .17, p < .05). Autocorrelations
in rates of change in depressive symptoms. We treated our for CES-D scores across waves were all significant (p < .05)
time-varying estimates of stress and academic workload as and ranged from r = .43 to r = .70, as were autocorrelations
fixed effects at Level 2, as follows: for perceived stress (r = .48 to r = .71) and academic
β3i ¼ γ 30 workload (r = .34 to r = .52).
ð4Þ To test the hypothesis that academic workload and per-
β4i ¼ γ 40
ceived stress associated with those demands rise toward the
Model testing proceeded as in Study 1, beginning with end of the semester, we ran unconditional growth models
unconditional growth models to establish an optimal for both academic workload and perceived stress. Linear
functional form of growth in depressive symptoms. Perceived time was coded 0, 1, 2, 3, corresponding to each wave of
stress, academic workload, and all covariates were included measurement. Results showed non-significant linear change
simultaneously. We also included several exploratory inter- (coefficient = .086, SE = .056, p = .12), but a significant
actions between stress (time-varying and person mean), quadratic effect for perceived stress (coefficient = -.036, SE
workload (time-varying and person mean), and linear time. = .018, p = .04), with stress levels rising from September
None were found to be statistically significant and we and peaking in December. For academic workload, the
trimmed these terms from our model one at a time, from linear (coefficient = -.090, SE = .188, p = .63) and quad-
largest to smallest p-values (Aiken and West 1991). ratic rates of change (coefficient = .005, SE = .062, p = .94)
were not significantly different from zero.
Next, our multilevel model tested the associations of
Study 2: Results academic workload and perceived stress with depressive
symptoms. Similar to results for Study 1, the multilevel
Descriptive statistics for depressive symptoms, perceived model for Study 2 (Table 5) showed significant linear and
stress, and academic workload are reported in Table 3. At quadratic rates of change in depressive symptoms
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Table 4 Sample 2 Correlations


Depressive Symptoms
Between Depressive Symptoms
with Perceived Stress and Start of Semester 1 End of Semester 1 Start of Semester 2 End of Semester 2
Academic Workload by Wave of
Measurement Perceived Stress
Start of Semester 1 .52** .46** .42** .33**
End of Semester 1 .46** .60** .55** .42**
Start of Semester 2 .42** .45** .46** .40**
End of Semester 2 .49** .48** .54** .51**
Academic Workload
Start of Semester 1 .08 .12 .01 −.08
End of Semester 1 .07 .16* .08 .10
Start of Semester 2 .21** .12 .14 .09
End of Semester 2 −.01 −.02 .09 .09
*p < .05. **p < .01

Table 5 Sample 2 Multilevel


Coefficient SE
Model of Between-Person
Effects of Average Perceived Fixed Effects
Stress and Academic Workload
on Level (intercept) and Change Intercept (initial status September) 0.719*** .068
in Depressive Symptoms and Mother’s Education .026 .017
Within-Person Effects of Race/Ethnicity (white = 0; visible minority = 1) −.008 .043
Perceived Stress and Academic
Sex (male = 0; female = 1) .019 .048
Workload on Change in
Depressive Symptoms Across Living Situation (with parents = 1; other = 0) −.041 .041
One Year Year of Study (1, 2, 3, 4) −.009 .021
Average Perceived Stress (mean across waves) .450*** .042
Average Academic Workload (mean across waves) .011 .017
Linear time slope
Rate of change in depressive symptoms .124*** .036
Average Perceived Stress (mean across waves) .102 .057
Average Academic Workload (mean across waves) .001 .023
Quadratic time slope
Deceleration in rate of change in depressive symptoms −.039*** .012
Average Perceived Stress (mean across waves) −.041* .019
Average Academic Workload (mean across waves) −.001 .007
Time-varying effect of perceived stress (person-centered) .151*** .031
Time-varying effect of academic workload (person-centered) .019* .007
Random effects
Residual (σ2) .097*** .012
Covariance matrix of L2 random effects u0 u1 u2
Random intercept (u0) .057** (.018) . .
Linear time slope (u1) .017 (.020) .021 (.035) .
Quadratic time slope (u2) −.007 (.006) −.006 (.011) .002 (.003)
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001

(controlling for the effects of covariates on initial status), was a deceleration in depressive symptoms among students
with symptoms peaking in December. An effect of average with higher average levels of perceived stress (shown by the
perceived stress on the intercept showed that students with significant negative effect for average perceived stress on
higher average levels of perceived stress started semester 1 the quadratic time slope). As shown in Fig. 2, at the end-of-
with higher depressive symptoms, a difference in level that semester 1 peak, predicted depressive symptoms for stu-
was maintained until the December peak. After that, there dents with higher average perceived stress were nearly two
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

2.4
fluctuations in depressive symptoms and how they relate to
students’ challenges is essential for designing strategies
2.0 aimed at promoting mental health and academic success.
Surprisingly, few studies of university students have mon-
Depressive Symptoms

1.6 itored change in depressive symptoms. To address this gap,


we examined within-person patterns of change in depressive
symptoms across one academic year in two samples of
1.2
Canadian university students. We hypothesized that
depressive symptoms would rise toward the end of the
0.8
semester, peaking when academic workloads and perceived
stress are likely highest (i.e., final exam period and major
0.4 project deadlines). Multilevel models for both samples
showed significant increases in depressive symptoms over
0.0
time, with symptoms peaking in December, at the end of
Start S1 End S1 Start S2 End S2 semester 1.
Wave of Assessment After determining peak timing of students’ depressive
symptoms in the academic cycle, we examined whether that
High Stress Low Stress
pattern was related to students’ objective and subjective
Fig. 2 Study 2: Trajectories of depressive symptoms across the aca- experiences of academic stress. We examined both
demic year for students with high average perceived stress levels (+1 between-person and within-person associations of academic
SD above the mean) and low average perceived stress levels (−1 SD workload (i.e., counts of academic demands) and ratings of
below the mean). Dashed lines represent 95% confidence intervals
perceived stress with depressive symptoms. In Study 1, we
found that students with higher levels of global perceived
thirds of a standard deviation (62%) above average, falling stress at the end of semester 1, when depressive symptoms
to just under half a standard deviation (47%) above average peaked, experienced more depressive symptoms at that time
by the end of semester 2. Average levels of current aca- compared to students whose perceived stress was lower.
demic workload were not related to level or rate of change Number of end-of-semester academic demands, however,
in depressive symptoms across the academic year. Within was not related to depressive symptoms in Study 1.
individuals, depressive symptoms were highest in months In Study 2, we improved upon Study 1 by assessing
when students reported greater within-person perceived academic workload, level of perceived stress associated
stress and more academic demands than usual (time-varying with these academic demands, and depressive symptoms at
effects in Table 5). However, these effects were small: when four times across the academic year. Study 2 assessed both
students felt higher-than-usual stress (+1 SD above their between- and within-person associations of depressive
own mean stress level), depressive symptoms were 11% of symptoms with counts of academic demands and ratings of
a standard deviation higher than usual. At times when stu- perceived stress. Between-persons results showed that stu-
dents reported higher-than-usual academic workload (+1 dents whose levels of perceived stress were higher on
SD above their own mean number of demands), depressive average across the academic year started off with higher
symptoms were 4.5% of a standard deviation higher than depressive symptoms, which increased at a steeper rate
usual. None of the control variables was a significant pre- toward peak symptoms around the end of semester 1, but
dictor of the intercept. The interaction between time-varying moved slightly toward average symptom levels by the end
perceived stress and academic workload was not significant of semester 2. Within persons, depressive symptoms rose to
and thus was not presented in the final model. greater levels on occasions when perceived stress was
higher than usual for a given person. Importantly, this
association is over and above the effect of average per-
Discussion ceived stress shown in Fig. 2. Even among students
reporting high average perceived stress, occasions marked
Given concerns about the prevalence of poor mental health by unusually high levels of perceived stress were associated
on university campuses (e.g., Canadian Association of with additional increases in depressive symptoms.
College and University Student Services and the Canadian In addition to assessing the number of academic demands
Mental Health Association 2013), it is critical to examine along with depressive symptoms at each wave of the study,
patterns of change in depressive symptoms across the aca- Study 2 also improved the measurement of academic
demic year to identify when, in the typical academic cycle, workload relative to Study 1. In Study 2 participants were
risk for depression may be highest. Knowledge about asked to indicate the number of assessments they had to
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

complete in the previous and upcoming weeks, rather than The second contribution of this research comes from
the number of grades returned to them. Results showed that Study 2, wherein we showed that on occasions when stu-
average academic workload levels (the mean number of dents experienced greater academic workloads and more
academic demands across the year) was not related to perceived stress than their own average levels, they also
depressive symptoms, but depressive symptoms were reported increases in depressive symptoms. Although the
higher on occasions when counts of academic demands sizes of these effects were small, these findings are impor-
were also slightly higher than one’s typical levels (the time- tant: they suggest that risk for depression increases when
varying effect). That is, when students reported having more students are challenged to manage more academic demands
academic work to complete than usual, they reported more than is typical for them. These results correspond with
depressive symptoms. The within-person effect of academic models of stress and coping suggesting that perceived stress
workload in the absence of a between-person effect rules will increase when demands in the environment are per-
out the possibility that students who were enrolled in more ceived as exceeding one’s individual resources (Lazarus and
demanding programs (e.g., disciplines with more frequent Folkman 1984; Pancer et al. 2004). Importantly, we were
assignments and supplemental lab courses) experienced able to rule out the possibility that students with heavier
more depressive symptoms in general. Instead, it only academic demands in general were also the most depressed.
appears to matter whether academic workload at a particular Average workload was unrelated to depressive symptoms.
time exceeded what is typical for a given person. In general models of depression, stress is a central con-
Both Study 1 and Study 2 results replicate the robust tributing component to the onset of depression (e.g.,
finding that students who experience greater perceived diathesis-stress models, Hammen 2005; allostatic load
stress associated with academic demands also report more models, McEwen 2003; developmental models, Compas
depressive symptoms (e.g., Newcomb-Anjo et al. 2017). 2004). And, in models of student burnout (Salmela-Aro
Additionally, the current study contributes two novel find- et al. 2008), burnout has been observed in students who
ings to the literature on depressive experiences in university were initially immersed in their studies, but later came to
students, both of which arise from the repeated measure- develop a chronic stress response after exposure to repeated
ment of depressive symptoms across the academic year. achievement pressures (Maroco and Campos 2012). Thus, if
First, the results of Studies 1 and 2 revealed when in the depressive symptoms arise from students appraising their
academic year risk for depression may be highest. Depres- academic demands as exceeding what they typically can
sive symptoms fluctuated within-individuals across the manage, risk for major depression and burnout increase.
academic year as expected, rising from the beginning of the
first semester and peaking at the end of the first semester,
when final assessments typically occur. These results have Limitations and Future Research
implications for the accurate assessment of depression in
university students. It has been suggested that two of the Several limitations of the current studies point to further
most commonly used screening measures for depression, directions for research on the role of academic demands in
the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II; Beck et al. 1996) university students’ depressive experiences. First, future
and the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale studies should explore the temporal ordering of demands,
(CES-D; Radloff 1991) overestimate rates of depression in perceived stress, and depressive symptoms to better
university samples (Santor et al. 1995). Our results showed understand the progression from stress response to depres-
that average levels of depressive symptoms exceeded what sion in university students. In the university context where
is considered clinically significant symptomatology in demands, perceived stress, and depressive symptoms rise
December, at the end of semester 1, and that more students and fall together, pathways to depression may not be linear.
fell into the “at-risk” group at that time relative to other Thus, it would be important to conduct path models to map
points in the academic year. Thus, at least a portion of the out the temporal ordering of risk for depression in this
discrepancy between rates of clinically significant levels of context.
depressive symptoms obtained through self-report screening Future research should also identify the factors that dis-
measures in university students (around 50%) and tinguish students for whom elevated depressive symptoms
population-level prevalence rates obtained via diagnostic represent compromised mental health from those for whom
interviews (10% to 25%; Pearson et al. 2013; Rohde et al. short-lived increases in depressive symptoms associated
2012) may be accounted for by the timing of assessment. with academic stress serve an adaptive motivational func-
Given that most research on depression in university sam- tion. Although negative affect reflected in clinical depres-
ples to date has been cross-sectional, levels of clinically sion and student burnout is considered maladaptive,
significant symptoms may be somewhat inflated or deflated negative affect in other contexts can serve adaptive self-
if time of year is not taken into account. regulatory functions by motivating cognitive and behavioral
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

responses to challenges, including academic ones (Harmon- be assessed in future research. On the one hand, it is pos-
Jones et al. 2013; for an example with adolescents see sible that December is particularly challenging due to other
Wrosch and Miller 2009). For example, daily diary results factors associated with time of year, such as seasonal
showed that university students who experienced occasional effects, financial burdens associated with holiday travel, and
periods of negative affect across one semester achieved the a briefer break before resuming studies. On the other hand,
greatest academic success (Oishi et al. 2007). In another resources such as coping behaviors, personality traits and
study spanning four years of university, students who were social support may, for some students, buffer the effects of
generally happy across all years achieved the greatest levels stress on depressive experiences across the academic year.
of academic success in semesters during which they More generally, future research aimed at replicating and
experienced increases in negative affect. In fact, these stu- extending these findings may benefit from the use of
dents benefited only when within-person negative affect was recruitment strategies other than convenience sampling.
high (Barker et al. 2016). Thus, for some students, increases This could improve representativeness in terms of demo-
in depressive symptoms may reflect productive investment graphic variables, possible range in depressive experiences,
in academic pursuits motivated by negative affect. In the and factors other than academics that may contribute to or
current studies, emotion and self-regulatory strategies stu- protect against these experiences for university students.
dents may employ to adaptively manage demands and
perceived stress and direct them toward academic success
were not assessed, however. Conclusion
A third limitation of the current research was the
assessment of academic workload. In Studies 1 and 2, In recent years, media outlets have drawn attention to a
counts were not weighted by the effort students invested in possible mental health crisis on Canadian university cam-
the workload, or by the type of work or assignment they puses (e.g., Goffin 2017; Lunau 2012) and campus com-
completed and which might have differed in its impact on munities have responded by paying closer attention to the
final grades (e.g., quiz vs. assignment vs. final exam). emotional needs of students. For example, in 2013, the
Moreover, perceived stress associated with demands was Canadian Association of College and University Student
not assessed at the exact time they were experienced. Daily Services and the Canadian Mental Health Association
diary, experience sampling, and stress interviews are jointly published Post-Secondary Student Mental Health:
methods that would more precisely identify which academic Guide to a Systemic Approach in which they outline dif-
demands are particularly stressful for students and better ferent ways in which universities can organize to support
capture the temporal relationship between type of demand student well-being. Our results showed that symptom levels
and associated perceived stress. peak (i.e., that risk is highest) at the end of the first seme-
Finally, future research should examine factors that may ster, constituting a first important step toward clarifying
exacerbate or ameliorate perceived stress associated with when the risk for depression may be highest in university
academic demands and its association with depressive samples. The second important finding from the current
experiences. Although academic stress has been shown to research is that symptoms were higher when students
be independently associated with depressive symptoms, appraised their academic demands as exceeding what they
over and above the effects of established risk and protective typically could manage. This implies that enhancing indi-
factors (Newcomb-Anjo et al. 2017), moderators of the vidual psychological resources that involve stress appraisal
effects of perceived stress on trajectories of depressive may be a prime target for programs aimed at supporting
symptoms should be examined. In the Pancer et al. model student well-being and mental health. Together, the key
(2004), stressors and resources are not only described in the findings from the current research have implications for the
academic domain, but in the personal and social domains as assessment, prevention, and treatment of depression in the
well. These additional domains include, for example, university student population. Effective intervention at this
finances, individual personality characteristics, expectations stage of development is important, given that experiencing
for university life, and social environments and levels of one major depressive episode increases the likelihood of
social support. Certainly, the university experience can be experiencing another episode in the future (Kendler et al.
characterized as a context in which socialization occurs 2001) and of experiencing poor educational and future
broadly and wherein many developmental challenges are economic outcomes (Fergusson et al. 2007). More gen-
encountered en route to adulthood (Montgomery and Côté erally, well-being and success across the transition to
2003). In the current study, the fact that depressive symp- adulthood accumulate and predict future well-being in adult
toms peaked in December and did not rise to the same levels occupational and social roles (e.g., Howard et al. 2010).
in April, when, theoretically, demands should be similarly Considering that promoting university students’ self-
high, suggests that additional stressors and resources should management and coping skills is one avenue of support
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

identified by the Canadian Association of College and Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2010). Weighted least squares esti-
University Student Services and the Canadian Mental mation with missing data. Mplus Technical Appendix, 2010,
1–10.
Health Association, evidence from the current study may be
Barker, E. T., Howard, A. L., Galambos, N. L., & Wrosch, C. (2016).
used in efforts to reduce the incidence of depression and to Tracking affect and academic success across university: Happy
enhance the concurrent and future well-being of university students benefit from bouts of negative mood. Developmental
students. Psychology, 52, 2022–2030. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000231.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck
Authors’ Contributions All authors contributed to the development of Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
the study concept. Study 1 design and data collection were the Corporation.
responsibility of N.L.G., A.L.H., and E.T.B. Study 2 design and data Bouteyre, E., Maurel, M., & Bernaud, J.-L. (2006). Daily hassles and
collection were the responsibility of E.T.B., A.L.H., and R.V.K. E.T. depressive symptoms among first year psychology students in
B. and A.L.H. performed the data analyses and drafted the Methods France: The role of coping and social support. Stress and Health,
and Results sections. E.T.B. wrote the Introduction and Discussion 23, 93–99. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.1125.
sections. All co-authors provided critical revisions and approved the Bureau of Labor Statistics U.S. Department of Labor (2014). College
final version of the manuscript. Enrollment and Work Activity of2013 High School Graduates.
Economic News Release. http://www.bls.gov/news.release/hsgec.
Funding Study 1 was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities nr0.htm.
Research Council of Canada operating grant awarded to N.L.G. and J. Canadian Association of College & University Student Services and
L.M. Study 2 was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities the Canadian Mental Health Association. (2013). Post-Secondary
Research Council of Canada Insight Development Grant awarded to E. Student Mental Health: Guide to a Systemic Approach. Van-
T.B. and A.L.H. couver, BC: Author.
Chambel, M. J., & Curral, L. (2005). Stress in academic life: Work
Data Sharing Declaration The datasets generated and/or analyzed characteristics as predictors of student well-being and perfor-
during the current study are not publicly available but are available mance. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 54,
from the corresponding author on reasonable request. 135–147. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2005.00200.x.
Cohen, S., Kamark, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure
Compliance with Ethical Standards These studies were conducted in of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24,
compliance with ethical standards outlined by the Government of 385–396. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404.
Canada’s Tri-Council Policy Statement: Ethical Conduct for Research Compas, B. E. (2004). Processes of risk and resilience during ado-
Involving Humans. lescence: Linking context and individuals. In R. M. Lerner, & L.
Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescence (2nd ed., pp.
Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of 263–296). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
interest. Cooke, R., Bewick, B. M., Barkham, M., Bradley, M., & Audin, K.
(2006). Measuring, monitoring and managing the psychological
Ethical Approval Both studies were approved by their respective well-being of first year university students. British Journal of
university research ethics review committees in accordance with the Guidance and Counselling, 34, 505–517. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Government of Canada’s Tri-Council Policy Statement: Ethical Con- 03069880600942624.
duct for Research Involving Humans. Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2011). The disaggregation of within-
person and between-person effects in longitudinal models of
Informed Consent Informed consent was received from all partici- change. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 583–619.
pants who participated at each wave of measurement as per the Curran, P. J., Lee, T., Howard, A. L., Lane, S., & MacCallum, R.
Government of Canada’s Tri-Council Policy Statement: Ethical Con- (2012). Disaggregating within-person and between-person effects
duct for Research Involving Humans. in multilevel and structural equation growth models. In J. R.
Harring, & G. R. Hancock (Eds.), CILVR series on latent variable
methodology. Advances in longitudinal methods in the social and
References behavioral sciences (pp. 217–253). Charlotte, NC: IAP Infor-
mation Age Publishing.
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and Eisenberg, D., Gollust, S. E., Golberstein, E., & Hefner, J. L. (2007).
interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Prevalence and correlates of depression, anxiety, and suicidality
Alfred-Liro, C., & Sigelman, C. K. (1998). Sex differences in self- among university students. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
concept and symptoms of depression during the transition to 77, 534–542. https://doi.org/10.1037/0002-9432.77.4.534.
college. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 27, 219–244. https:// Enders, C. K., Mistler, S. A., & Keller, B. T. (2016). Multilevel
doi.org/10.1023/A:1021667813858. multiple imputation: A review and evaluation of joint modeling
American College Health Association. (2016). American College and chained equations imputation. Psychological Methods, 21,
Health Association-National College Health Assessment II: 222–240. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000063.
Canadian Reference Group Executive Summary. Hanover, MD: Fisher, S., & Hood, B. (1987). The stress of the transition to university:
American College Health Association. A longitudinal study of psychological disturbance and vulner-
Andresen, E. M., Malmgren, J. A., Carter, W. B., & Patrick, D. L. ability to homesickness. British Journal of Psychology, 78,
(1994). Screening for depression in well older adults: Evaluation 425–441. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1987.tb02260.x.
of a short form of the CES-D. American Journal of Preventive Fergusson, D. M., Boden, J. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2007). Recurrence
Medicine, 10, 77–84. of major depression in adolescence and early adulthood, and later
Andrews, B., & Wilding, J. M. (2004). The relation of depression and mental health, educational and economic outcomes. British
anxiety to life-stress and achievement in students. British Journal Journal of Psychiatry, 191, 335–342. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.
of Psychology, 95, 509–521. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712604 bp.107.036079.
2369802.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Furr, S. R., Westefeld, J. S., McConnell, G. N., & Jenkins, J. M. Mikolajczyk, R.T., Maxwell, A.E., Naydenova, V., Meier, S., & El
(2001). Suicide and depression among college students: A decade Ansari, W. (2008). Depressive symptoms and perceived burdens
later. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 32, related to being a student: Survey in three European countries.
97–100. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.32.1.97. Clinical Practice and Epidemiology in Mental Health, 4. https://
Galambos, N. L., Barker, E. T., & Krahn, H. J. (2006). Depression, doi.org/10.1186/1745-0179-4-19.
self-esteem, and anger in emerging adulthood: Seven-year tra- Montgomery, M. J., & Côté, J. E. (2003). College as a transition to
jectories. Developmental Psychology, 42, 350–365. https://doi. adulthood. In G. R. Adams & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell
org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.350. handbook of adolescence (pp. 149–172). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Galarneau, D., Marissette, R., & Usalcas, J. (2013). What has changed Newcomb-Anjo, S., Villemaire-Krajden, R., Takefman, K., & Barker,
for young people in Canada? In Statistics Canada. http://www.sta E. T. (2017). The unique associations of university experiences
tcan.gc.ca/pub/75-006-x/2013001/article/11847-eng.htm. with depressive symptoms in emerging adulthood. Emerging
Goffin, P. (2017). ‘We’re not a treatment facility’: The struggle for Adulthood, 5, 75–80. https://doi.org/10.1177/216769681665
campuses to provide students mental health care. The Toronto 7233.
Star. https://www.thestar.com/news/gta/2017/08/13/were-not-a- Oishi, S., Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (2007). The optimum level of
treatment-facility-the-struggle-for-campuses-to-provide-students- well-being: Can people be too happy. Perspectives on Psycho-
mental-health-care.html. logical Science, 2, 346–360. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.
Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004). 2007.00048.x.
Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six Othieno, C. J., Okoth, R. O., Peltzer, K., Pengpid, S., & Malla, L. O.
pre-conceptions about internet questionnaires. American Psy- (2014). Depression among university students in Kenya: Pre-
chologist, 59, 93–104. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X. valence and sociodemographic correlates. Journal of Affective
59.2.93. Disorders, 165, 120–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.
Haldorsen, H., Bak, N. H., Dissing, A., & Petersson, B. (2014). Stress 070.
and symptoms of depression among medical students at the Pancer, S. M., Pratt, M., Hunsberger, B., & Alisat, S. (2004). Bridging
University of Copenhagen. Scandinavian Journal of Public troubled waters: Helping students make the transition from high
Health, 42, 89–95. https://doi.org/10.1177/1403494813503055. school to university. Guidance and Counselling, 9, 184–190.
Hammen, C. (2005). Stress and depression. Annual Review of Clinical Pearson, C., Janz, T., & Ali, J. (2013). Mental and substance use
Psychology, 1, 293–319. disorders in Canada. In Health at a Glance, Statistics Canada.
Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Price, T. F. (2013). Does negative http://www.statcan.gc.ca/pub/82-624-x/2013001/article/11855-
affect always narrow and positive affect always broaden the eng.htm.
mind? Considering the influence of motivational intensity on Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale
cognitive scope. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, or research in the general population. Applied Psychological
301–307. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721413481353. Measurement, 1, 385–401.
Hoffman, L., & Stawski, R. S. (2009). Persons as contexts: Evaluating Radloff, L. S. (1991). The use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies
between-person and within-person effects in longitudinal analy- Depression Scale in adolescents and young adults. Journal of
sis. Research in Human Development, 6, 97–120. https://doi.org/ Youth and Adolescence, 20, 149–166. https://doi.org/10.1007/
10.1080/15427600902911189. BF01537606.
Howard, A. L. (2015). Leveraging time-varying covariates to test Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., Cheong, Y. F., Congdon, R. T., & Du
within- and between-person effects and interactions in the mul- Toit, M. (2011). HLM 7. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software
tilevel linear model. Emerging Adulthood, 3, 400–412. https:// International Inc.
doi.org/10.1177/2167696815592726. Rohde, P., Lewinsohn, P. M., Klein, D. N., Seeley, J. R., & Gau, J. M.
Howard, A. L., Galambos, N. L., & Krahn, H. J. (2010). Paths to (2012). Key characteristics of major depressive disorder occur-
success in young adulthood from mental health and life transi- ring in childhood, adolescence, emerging adulthood, and adult-
tions in emerging adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral hood. Clinical Psychological Science, 1, 41–53. https://doi.org/
Development, 34, 538–546. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 10.1177/2167702612457599.
0165025410365803. Salmela-Aro, K., Kiuru, N., Pietikäinen, M., & Jokela, J. (2008). Does
Ibrahim, A. K., Kelly, S. J., Adams, C. E., & Glazebrook, C. (2013). A school matter? The role of school context in adolescents’ school-
systematic review of studies of depression prevalence in uni- related burnout. European Psychologist, 13, 12–23. https://doi.
versity students. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 391–400. org/10.1027/1016-9040.13.1.12.
https://doi.org/10.1037/t06165-000. Santor, D. A., Zuroff, D. C., Ramsay, J. O., Cervantes, P., & Palacios,
Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., & Gardner, C. O. (2001). Genetic J. (1995). Examining scale discriminability in the BDI and CES-
risk, number of previous depressive episodes, and stressful life D as a function of depressive severity. Psychological Assessment,
events in predicting onset of major depression. American Journal 7, 131–139. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.2.131.
of Psychiatry, 158, 582–586. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp. Sha, T., & Xia, L. (2004). Mediation effect of cognitive appraisal
158.4.582. orientation on the relationship between stress and negative feel-
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. ings in university students. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 18,
New York, NY: Springer. 107–110.
Lunau, K. (2012). Campus crisis: the broken generation. Why so many Shamsuddin, K., Fadzil, F., Wan Ismail, W. S., Shah, S. A., Omar, K.,
of our best and brightest students report feeling hopeless, Muhammad, N. A., Jaffar, A., Ismail, A., & Mahadevan, R.
depressed, even suicidal. McLean’s Magazine. http://www.ma (2013). Correlates of depression, anxiety and stress among
cleans.ca/news/canada/the-broken-generation/. Malaysian university students. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 6,
Maroco, J., & Campos, J. A. D. B. (2012). Defining the student 318–323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2013.01.014.
burnout construct: A structural analysis from three burnout Schulenberg, J. E., & Zarrett, N. R. (2006). Mental health during
inventories. Psychological Reports, 111, 814–830. https://doi. emerging adulthood: Continuity and discontinuity in courses,
org/10.2466/14.10.20.PR0.111.6.814-830. causes, and functions. In J. J. Arnett & J. Tanner (Eds.), Emer-
McEwen, B. S. (2003). Mood disorders and allostatic load. Biological ging Adults in America: Coming of Age in the 21st Century (pp.
Psychiatry, 54, 200–207. 135–72). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Twenge, J. M., Gentile, B., DeWall, C. N., Ma, D., Lacefield, K., & the University of Alberta. Her research focuses on mental health and
Schurtz, D. R. (2010). Birth cohort increases in psychopathology substance use in adolescence and the transition to adulthood, with an
among young Americans, 1938–2007: A cross-temporal meta- emphasis on quantitative methods for developmental data analysis.
analysis of the MMPI. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 145–154.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.10.005. Rosanne Villemaire-Krajden received her BA Honours degree in
Wrosch, C., & Miller, G. E. (2009). Depressive symptoms can be Psychology from Concordia University and MSc in Psychiatry from
useful: Self-regulatory and emotional benefits of dysphoric mood McGill University. Her research focuses on depression and cognitive
in adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, behaviour therapy.
96, 1181–1190. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015172.

Nancy L. Galambos is Professor in the Department of Psychology at


the University of Alberta. She received her doctorate from the
Erin T. Barker is Associate Professor in the Department of Pennsylvania State University. Her research examines psychosocial
Psychology at Concordia University. She received her doctorate development and well-being across adolescence, the transition to
from the University of Alberta. Her major research interests include adulthood, and into midlife.
stress, coping, mental health and wellbeing during the transition to
adulthood.

Andrea L. Howard is Assistant Professor in the Department of


Psychology at Carleton University. She received her doctorate from

You might also like