You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Interprofessional Care

ISSN: 1356-1820 (Print) 1469-9567 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ijic20

Impact of peer pressure on accuracy of reporting


vital signs: An interprofessional comparison
between nursing and medical students

Alyshah Kaba & Tanya N. Beran

To cite this article: Alyshah Kaba & Tanya N. Beran (2016) Impact of peer pressure
on accuracy of reporting vital signs: An interprofessional comparison between
nursing and medical students, Journal of Interprofessional Care, 30:1, 116-122, DOI:
10.3109/13561820.2015.1075967

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/13561820.2015.1075967

Published online: 01 Feb 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 13

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ijic20

Download by: ["Queen's University Libraries, Kingston"] Date: 11 February 2016, At: 05:48
JOURNAL OF INTERPROFESSIONAL CARE
2016, VOL. 30, NO. 1, 116–122
http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/13561820.2015.1075967

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Impact of peer pressure on accuracy of reporting vital signs: An interprofessional


comparison between nursing and medical students
Alyshah Kaba and Tanya N. Beran
Medical Education and Research, Department of Community Health Sciences, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta,
Canada

ABSTRCT ARTICLE HISTORY


The hierarchical relationship between nursing and medicine has long been known, yet its direct Received 13 August 2014
influence on procedural tasks has yet to be considered. Drawing on the theory of conformity from Revised 14 June 2015
Downloaded by ["Queen's University Libraries, Kingston"] at 05:48 11 February 2016

social psychology, we suggest that nursing students are likely to report incorrect information in Accepted 21 July 2015
response to subtle social pressures imposed by medical students. Second-year medical and third-year KEYWORDS
nursing students took vital signs readings from a patient simulator. In a simulation exercise, three actors, Group conformity;
posing as medical students, and one nursing student participant all took a total of three rounds of vital interprofessional
signs on a high-fidelity patient simulator. In the first two rounds the three actors individually stated the collaboration;
same correct vital signs values, and on the third round the three actors individually stated the same interprofessional education;
incorrect vital sign values. This same procedure was repeated with actors posing as nursing students, and medical students; nursing
one medical student. A two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed that nursing student participants students; peer pressure;
simulation
(M = 2.84; SD = 1.24) reported a higher number of incorrect vital signs than did medical student
participants (M = 2.13; SD = 1.07), F (1,100) = 5.51, p = 0.021 (Cohen’s d = 0.61). The study indicated that
social pressure may prevent nursing students from questioning incorrect information within interprofes-
sional environments, potentially affecting quality of care.

Introduction programmes may be hindered by social factors. One such factor


is peer pressure that causes an individual to change his/her
Effective collaboration has been long recognized as a means to
behaviours to match those of the majority of group members –
achieve better quality of care and enhanced patient outcomes
even when the group members present incorrect information.
(Peterson et al., 2014; Reeves, Perrier, Goldman, Freeth, &
Known as conformity, this phenomenon has been studied in
Zwarenstein, 2013; Zwarenstein, Goldman, Reeves, 2009.). Yet,
social psychology over the last 60 years, but has yet to be
the way in which health professionals are socialized and trained
examined among interprofessional groups (Beran, Kaba, Caird,
continues to present significant barriers to collaboration (Hall,
& McLaughlin, 2014). Given that the social relationships
2005; Khalili, Hall, & DeLuca, 2014; Landman, Aannestad,
between medicine and nursing largely remain unexamined
Smoldt, & Cortese, 2014; Milne, Greenfield, & Braithwaite,
empirically, the overall objective of the present study is to deter-
2015). Globally, healthcare professionals are typically trained in
mine whether medical and nursing students enrolled in health
uniprofessional settings, independent of interprofessional edu-
professional programmes are likely to conform by giving incor-
cation (IPE) and collaboration, leading to challenges in practice
rect responses on a vital sign skills task upon hearing incorrect
(Hall, 2005; Khalili, Orchard, Laschinger, & Farah, 2013;
responses from group members of another profession.
Langendyk, Hegazi, Cowin, Johnson, & Wilson, 2015). A lack
of understanding and knowledge of others’ professional roles
and perspectives, along with “turf” wars and fear of “identity Conformity theory
loss”, have been cited as the main barriers to interprofessional
In a series of seminal studies, social psychologist Solomon
collaborative practice (Carpenter, Barnes, Dickinson & Woof,
Asch (1951) examined the phenomenon of group member-
2006; Chung et al., 2012; Khalili et al., 2014). The introduction of
ship on the behaviour of an individual member within the
IPE particularly at the pre-licensure level is a critical early inter-
group. When members of the group were instructed prior to
vention to enable students from two or more professions the
the experiment to provide the same incorrect response to a
opportunities to learn about, from, and with each other (CAIPE
line matching perception test, the individual who was unin-
(Centre for the Advancement of Interprofessional Education),
formed about the purpose of the study was more likely to
1997). Although reviews (Hammick, Freeth, Koppel, Reeves, &
provide the same incorrect response. These studies on group
Barr, 2007; Reeves et al., 2010; Reeves et al., 2013; Zwarenstein
conformity demonstrated that when an individual encounters
et al., 2009) have identified that IPE interventions can improve
information from the group that is contrary to his own under-
collaboration and healthcare outcomes, the effectiveness of IPE
standing, he is likely to revise his response to match that of

CONTACT Alyshah Kaba akaba@ucalgary.ca Medical Education and Research, Department of Community Health Sciences, Cumming School of Medicine,
University of Calgary, 3280 Hospital Dr. N.W., Calgary, AB, T2N 4Z6, Canada.
© 2016 Taylor & Francis
JOURNAL OF INTERPROFESSIONAL CARE 117

the group’s. This phenomenon has since been extended to sessions. When participants arrived at the simulation room
other approaches such as group think theory (Janis, 1972) they were greeted by the experimenter and directed to the
when examining decision-making among group members, simulation room, where two confederates were seated and
and also Crew Resource Management training (Helmreich, filling out forms. The participant was given some forms to
Merritt, & Wilhelm, 1999) for pilots to learn interpersonal complete. The experimenter then left the room to retrieve
communication skills in aviation. Peer pressure has only the last confederate. Once forms were completed, the
begun to receive attention in medicine whereby Beran, experimenter asked everyone to provide a brief introduc-
McLaughlin, Al Ansari, and Kassam (2012) determined that tion. They did so by identifying which university they were
medical clerks were more likely to perform a knee aspiration from, their programme (nursing or medicine), and their
incorrectly if they believed that their peer group had also current clinical rotation placement. Subsequently, the stu-
performed it incorrectly. This study provides emerging evi- dents watched a 5-minute instructional video developed by
dence that clerks will improperly perform a clinical skill dur- the experimenter on the procedural steps to manually take
ing simulation when encountering peer pressure; it remains to vital signs (available upon request to the first author). The
be determined whether conformity will occur within an inter- researcher consulted with both nursing and medicine
professional context. faculty to ensure these values and steps were consistent
Downloaded by ["Queen's University Libraries, Kingston"] at 05:48 11 February 2016

In summary, conformity may be one factor that hinders with information taught by each of the programmes.
the ability of nurses and physicians to collaborate and com- Finally, the participants were oriented to the mannequin
municate effectively. Considering that professional values and by the experimenter and given 10 minutes to practice taking
impressions emerge at the pre-licensure level (Hallin, the vital signs on the simulation mannequin to ensure they
Kiessling, Waldner, & Henriksson, 2009), the present study were familiar with the simulator’s function and were able to
is conducted with students. Specifically, we examine whether obtain accurate readings.
nursing students are more likely to repeat incorrect informa- The precise experimental procedures follow the original
tion from medical students, rather than the reverse. There are Asch experimental design (Asch, 1951). Consistent with the
three aims for this study. First, we will compare baseline vital Asch design, the participant was the second last individual to
signs knowledge, skills and perception of skills between med- take the vital signs and report them aloud. The first two
ical and nursing students. Second, we will examine the vital confederates took three vital signs: RR, RP, and BP, and
signs values they report. Third, given the socio-historical then stated their findings consecutively to the group. The
relationship between the two professions, we will determine participant, having heard the response of the two confederates
whether nursing students are more likely to report incorrect that preceded him/her, was then asked to state verbally the
values than are medical students. With the emphasis on IPE values for RR, RP, and BP. Finally, the last confederate stated
in health professional education, it is necessary to obtain the vital sign values. This process was repeated three times.
empirical evidence of how conformity might be implicated Prior to the experiment, researchers instructed the confed-
in communication among health professionals to justify erates to state pre-determined vital sign values for all three
future curriculum planning at the pre-licensure level. trials. For the first and second rounds the confederates all
gave the correct values for all three vital signs, but for the last
round, they all gave the same incorrect values for all three vital
Background
signs: RR of 18 breaths/minute, RP of 70 beats/minute, and
The interprofessional experience took place at an interprofes- BP of 115 mmHg/70 mmHg.
sional clinical simulation centre at a major university in These values are considered wrong because they did not
Canada. Upon arriving at the centre, students were told that match the readings that the mannequin had been prepro-
they would participate in a 45-minute simulation exercise in grammed to display. The correct response (as programed in
which they would be required to take vital signs (radial pulse the patient simulator) for the RR was 24 breaths/minute, RP
(RP), respiration rate (RR), and blood pressure (BP)) on a was 52 beats/minutes, and BP was 135 mmHg/90 mmHg. In
high-fidelity simulation mannequin (iStan©). While the facul- maintaining a research design that closely approximates the
ties of nursing and medicine both have a simulation area, the classic conformity studies, Asch (1951) established that parti-
advantage of selecting one closer to the hospital was to ensure cipants felt a sense of trust with the confederates during the
neither group of students was familiar with the setting. The first two rounds, thus increasing their likelihood to conform
high-fidelity simulator uses the electronic software Muse©, on the third round. Through simulation, we were able to
which has the capabilities of changing the systolic/diastolic create a similar presence of social trust and rapport amongst
BP, RR, and RP read. The room had four video cameras that participants that would be analogous to the trust established
recorded the students’ behaviours and interactions. amongst interprofessional teams in healthcare. At the end of
A standard protocol was developed to ensure the parti- the experiment, the participants were debriefed about the
cipants did not interact with the confederates prior to the purpose of the study and obtained consent for their
start of session. The confederates received instructions from participation.
the experimenter prior to the session to state specific
responses to these questions to standardize the process
Methods
across the four female/male nursing and medical groups.
The confederates used a separate entrance and were con- The present study examines peer pressure amongst medical
cealed in a separate room between each of the study and nursing students using an experimental design that was
118 A. KABA AND T. N. BERAN

consistent with the original experimental procedures (Asch, that participant gender is related to conformity (Eagly & Carli,
1951). 1981), random assignment was conducted according to gen-
der to ensure an equal number of men and women per group.
Groups consisted of one medical student participant and three
Sample actors posing as nursing students, or one nursing student
A total of 104 second-year medical (n = 60) and third-year participant and three actors posing as medical students. In
nursing (n = 44) students participated in the study. This each session there was one participant who was in the pre-
sample size is adequate to detect a significant difference sence of three hired actors from the other profession. The
between the two groups, based on an effect size of 0.70 confederates were previously hired actors by the experimenter
(Cohen’s d), with a one-tailed probability criterion of 0.05, who pretended to play the roles of these student group parti-
and power at 0.90 (Cohen, 1988). This effect size was selected cipants (see Figure 1). Actors were recruited from outside of
because Bond and Smith (1996) reported a large effect size in the medical programme and received a total of six hours of
their meta-analysis. The sample of medical students (n = 30 training to prepare them to accurately take vital signs. These
male; n = 30 female) represents 35.29% of the total number of actors had not previously encountered either the nursing or
medical students in the second-year class (n = 170). Also, the medical students and received a standardized script and vital
Downloaded by ["Queen's University Libraries, Kingston"] at 05:48 11 February 2016

number of nursing students (n = 11 male; n = 30 female) sign values to memorize. Although the use of confederates has
represents 15.60% of the total number of nursing students in been described as a limitation in the literature as they may not
the third-year class (n = 282). We were unable to recruit a represent their peers (Crutchfield, 1955), recruiting nursing
larger sample of male nursing students, as there is a limited and medical students to misinform their peers would have
population of men enrolled (n = 15). Second-year medical and been unethical and would make the experience unrealistic.
third-year nursing students were selected to ensure similar Having the students act in the role of confederates and know-
exposure to the amount of clinical practice (approximately ingly “deceive” their own peer group would have potentially
two semesters) and number of simulated learning experiences put them in an uncomfortable situation and perhaps forced in
in their respective programmes. nature.

Recruitment and randomization Measures


Students from both faculties of nursing and medicine were Demographic form
recruited through a multistage process (Kaba & Beran, 2014). The demographic form included questions about gender, year
Students were not fully informed of the purpose of the study, in the programme, and a question about confidence in taking
as this information would likely have impacted their beha- RR, RP, and BP on a 5-point scale where 1 = “not confident”,
viour during the session. Rather, during the recruitment phase 2 = “somewhat confident”, 3 = “neither confident/or not
they were told that they would practice medical skills in a confident”, 4 = “confident”, 5 = “very confident”. Other
simulation setting, while working with health professional questions included the approximate number of times they
students from other professions. had used a high-fidelity simulation mannequin, the number
Students were randomly assigned into four homogenous of semesters of clinical experience they had completed, and
experimental groups using a randomized block design. Given current clinical placement or pre-clerkship rotation.

Figure 1. Experimental groups.


JOURNAL OF INTERPROFESSIONAL CARE 119

Knowledge questions Table 1. Means and SDs of demographic and knowledge questions (n = 104).
Participants were asked to complete nine knowledge questions Medical Nursing
to ensure there were no differences in knowledge of vital signs Baseline characteristics students students
between medical and nursing students. The questions asked Age 27.43 (3.78) 26.48 (5.80)
Number of times practicing with a 2.72 (1.82) 2.75 (3.06)
participants to identify normal ranges for RR, RP, and BP simulator
readings. A sample item includes, “which of the following Number of semesters of clinical 2.48 (1.27) 2.39 (1.04)
experience
ranges is most likely considered normal resting radial Perceived confidence in taking RR 4.17 (0.67) 4.18 (0.58)
pulse?” Responses were: (a) greater than 100 beats per minute; Perceived confidence in taking RP 4.50 (0.54) 4.36 (0.53)
(c) 60–90 beats per minute; and (c) less than 60 beats per Perceived confidence in taking BP 4.08 (0.67) 4.00 (0.43)
Knowledge questions (total correct) 8.80 (0.32) 8.50 (0.79)
minute. The minimum score for the nine knowledge ques-
tions is 0 and the maximum is 9.

Vital signs session; thus, there were no differences in their skill level.
Conformity was determined according to whether the parti- There was no significant difference in the participants’ per-
cipant reported a vital sign the same as or close to the ceived confidence in taking RR, RP, and BP (p = 0.079).
Downloaded by ["Queen's University Libraries, Kingston"] at 05:48 11 February 2016

incorrect vital sign reported by the confederates. Finally we determined that there was no significant difference
Consultation took place with both nursing and medicine in baseline knowledge of vital signs between medical and
faculty experts to confirm that the vital sign cut-off ranges nursing students (p = 0.898). Thus, baseline demographic
for conforming/not conforming were within educational stan- and practice characteristics between the medical and nursing
dards (Simel, 2011). Responses were coded as correct (not students were similar.
conforming) if they were within ±2 breaths/minute of the Table 2 presents the values and frequency of each vital sign
correct value of 24 breaths/minutes for RR. The RP values (RR, RP, systolic, and diastolic BP) reported by all students.
were marked accurate (not conforming) if the answer was About a quarter (27.0%) of the participants reported a RR
within ±4 beats/minute of the correct value of 52 beats/min- value that fell within the incorrect range of 16–20, as shown in
ute. The systolic BP values were correct (not conforming) if italics. All other participants reported an RR in the correct
they were within ±4 mmHg of the correct value of 135 mmHg range of 22–26. The most frequently identified value was 24,
and within ±4 mmHg of the correct diastolic BP of 90 mmHg. which is the correct value, as shown in bold, followed by the
Values outside of these ranges were coded as incorrect (con- value of 18, which is the incorrect value reported by the
forming to confederates). confederates, and is indicated in the table by the underline.
A similar pattern was found with RP with more than half
(63.6%) the total number of participants reporting the correct
Data analysis response in the range 48–56. The most frequently endorsed
Descriptive statistics and frequencies were reported for all response was the incorrect value of 70 they heard reported by
variables, and skewness and kurtosis were used to examine the confederates.
the shape and the distribution of the data. A multivariate A different pattern was observed for systolic and diastolic
analysis of variance (ANOVA) was employed to determine BP. The large majority (87.5%) of participants reported a
whether baseline knowledge, demographic, and practice char- systolic BP value in the incorrect range, with most responses
acteristics differed between medical and nursing students. The at the upper end of this range. For the diastolic BP, the
total number of correct responses for the four vital signs was majority (72.1%) of participants were also likely to endorse
compared between nursing and medical students using a two- values within the incorrect range. Most students reported a
way ANOVA. A Chi-square analysis was used to determine value of 80, which is halfway between the correct and incor-
which of the four vital signs were the most frequently rect values.
reported incorrectly. Probability values, p < 0.05, were con-
sidered statistically significant. Differences between medical and nursing students
Overall, 95.2% of all students reported a value within the
Ethical considerations incorrect range for at least one of the four vital signs.
The study followed the guidelines for behavioural research of Table 3 presents the mean/SD for each of the four vital
the American Psychological Association and was approved by signs values reported by the medical and nursing students.
the University of Calgary Health Research Ethics Board. Since most of the variables are skewed (Tabachnick & Fidell,
1996), the Mann-Whitney U test was used. A Bonferroni
correction was applied to account for the multiple compar-
Results isons, with the new alpha set to 0.0125. Furthermore, a two-
way ANOVA revealed that nursing students (M = 2.84; SD =
Demographic characteristics
1.24) reported a higher number of incorrect vital signs than
A multivariate ANOVA determined that none of the demo- did medical students (M = 2.13; SD = 1.07), F (1,100) = 5.51, p
graphic and practice characteristics differed between medical = 0.021, Cohen’s d = 0.61. No significant difference or inter-
and nursing students (p = 0.68) (Table 1). All students in both action effect between male and female students (p = 0.571)
groups took accurate vital sign readings during the practice was found.
120 A. KABA AND T. N. BERAN

Table 2. Frequency of vital signs values reported by students (n = 104).


Respiration rate Radial pulse Systolic BP Diastolic BP
Value N(%) Value N(%) Value N(%) Value N(%)
16.00 6(5.8) 48.00 2(1.9) 100.00 2(1.9) 70.00 15(14.4)
18.00 22(21.2) 50.00 16(15.4) 110.00 1(1.0) 72.00 4(3.8)
20.00 18(17.3) 52.00 14(13.5) 115.00 13(12.5) 74.00 2 (1.9)
22.00 10(9.6) 53.00 1(1.0) 116.00 1 (1.0) 75.00 2(1.9)
24.00 37(35.6) 54.00 18(17.3) 118.00 2 (1.9) 76.00 3(2.9)
25.00 1(1.0) 55.00 2(1.9) 119.00 1(1.0) 78.00 2(1.9)
26.00 10(9.6) 56.00 10(9.6) 120.00 10(9.6) 80.00 25(24.0)
66.00 6(5.8) 121.00 1(1.0) 82.00 8(7.7)
68.00 2(1.9) 122.00 3(2.9) 84.00 7 (6.7)
70.00 19(18.3) 124.00 1(1.0) 85.00 7(6.7)
72.00 12(11.95) 125.00 2 (1.9) 86.00 4(3.8)
74.00 2(1.9) 126.00 4(3.8) 88.00 7(6.7)
128.00 6 (5.8) 90.00 17(16.3)
130.00 44(42.3) 92.00 1(1.0)
132.00 7(6.7)
134.00 1(1.0)
135.00 4(3.8)
Downloaded by ["Queen's University Libraries, Kingston"] at 05:48 11 February 2016

138.00 1(1.0)
Notes. Incorrect value reported by confederates is underlined. Correct value programmed in the simulator is in bold. Incorrect range of responses is in italics. Correct
range of responses is in regular font.

Table 3. Mean (SD) of reported vital signs reported by medical and nursing conformity. In addition, we found that nursing students were
students.
more likely to conform than did medical students, in parti-
Medical students Nursing students U statistics (df = 1) cular when reporting RR and RP. For systolic and diastolic
RR 22.23 (2.69)a 20.84 (3.63)a 1,031.50* BP, medical students were as likely to conform.
RP 56.33 (6.73) 59.45 (8.19)a 1,037.00*
Systolic BP 128.35 (9.45) 123.77 (6.91)a 877.00* Our empirical evidence supports other work that have
Diastolic BP 84.13 (7.44) 79.71 (7.96) 830.00* identified the entrenched hierarchies between nursing and
a
Skewed variables. medicine (Hall, 2005; Langendyk et al., 2015; McInnes,
*Asterisk indicates significant at less than .0125.
Peters, Bonney, & Halcomb, 2015; MacMillan, 2012; Price,
Doucet, & Hall, 2014; Stein, Watts, & Howell, 1990; Stein,
Rate of conformity for each vital sign 1967). The nature of this dominant, subordinate relationship
was effectively coined by Stein’s (1967) seminal paper on the
To further understand which of the four vital signs the nur- interprofessional dynamics as the “doctor–nurse game”.
sing students most frequently reported incorrectly, four Chi- Although the relationship between the two professions has
square analyses were conducted. A Bonferroni correction was become more collegial over time, the educational system in
applied to account for the multiple comparisons. Accordingly, which physicians and nurses are trained may instil through
the new alpha was set to 0.0125. For RR, nursing students (n = the informal curriculum omnipotence in medical students and
59.1%) were significantly more likely to provide incorrect subservience in nursing students (Burford et al., 2013;
responses than were medical students (n = 33.3%), X2(1) = Gilligan, Outram, & Levett-Jones, 2014; MacMillan, 2012;
6.83, p = 0.01 (Cramer’s phi = 0.26). Similarly for RP, nursing Price et al., 2014). Furthermore, medical students are also
students (n = 52.3%) were significantly more likely to provide socialized within environments that perpetuate an under-
incorrect responses than were medical students (n = 30%), X2 standing that physicians play a superior role in patient care
(1) = 5.27, p = 0.001 (Cramer’s phi = 0.23). However, a decision-making, which can impede interprofessional respect
different pattern was observed for systolic BP, in which nur- and collaboration (Price et al., 2014; Stein et al., 1990).
sing students (n = 90.9%) were not significantly more likely to Our findings suggest that group conformity behaviours dur-
provide incorrect responses than were medical students (n = ing interprofessional interactions may have critical implications
85.0%), X2(1) = 0.81, p = 0.368. The same was observed for for these existing hierarchical relationships between the two
diastolic BP, in which both nursing students (n = 81.8%) and professions. This differential may contribute to moral distress
medical students (n = 65.0%) were likely to provide incorrect – a phenomenon describing the experience of cognitive-emo-
values X2(1) = 3.57, p = 0.059. Thus, it appears that the tional dissonance that arises when an individual feels compelled
inaccuracy was more likely to be reported by nursing students to act contrary to one’s moral requirements (Berger, 2014;
for respiration and RP, but not for BP. There was no signifi- Papathanassoglou et al., 2012). Nurses may feel reluctant to
cant difference for gender of participant. speak up about physicians’ opinions, believing that their opi-
nions would not be accepted (Papathanassoglou et al., 2012).
Thus, the nursing students in our study may have been experi-
Discussion
encing feelings of moral distress when taking vital signs with
Although all participants reported correct vital signs in the medical students who reported values that did not match the
practice round, the vast majority of participants reported at vital signs they took from the mannequin. This may explain
least one incorrect value upon hearing group members report why they were more likely to conform to a higher number of
incorrect values. This demonstrates a surprisingly high rate of vital signs than did the medical students.
JOURNAL OF INTERPROFESSIONAL CARE 121

Although the nursing students were more likely to con- other sites. It only included students who volunteered to parti-
form overall and for RR and RP in comparison to medical cipate. It is also possible that the participants, who value IPE,
students, this difference was not found for BP. This can be may have been more likely to sign up. We were unable to
explained by the fact that so many of the medical students recruit a larger sample of male nursing students, as there are
also conformed on BP – with over half and three quarters a limited number of men enrolled in the programme (n = 15).
reporting incorrect values for systolic and diastolic values, However, because the participants represented the population
respectively. This may be explained by the cognitive load of students, the findings may be applicable locally. In addition,
theory (Paas & Van Merriënboer, 1994). Accordingly, infor- participants may have acted differently in a simulation envir-
mation received into the brain must be processed in working onment than within a clinical setting; thus, further studies
memory (Kavic, 2013), which is limited in the amount of should be conducted in a clinical context. Finally the students
discrete information – “chunks” of information that can be may have informed other students about the study, which
retained at once. All information in working memory must be could have affected the later participants’ performance. All
encoded and transferred to long-term memory, before mean- students, however, were asked if they were aware of the study,
ingful learning can continue (Kavic, 2013). BP involves more and no one reported having been informed.
complex measuring techniques compared to other vital signs
Downloaded by ["Queen's University Libraries, Kingston"] at 05:48 11 February 2016

(Armstrong, 2002), as the participant has to first place the Concluding comments
diaphragm of the stethoscope over the brachial pulse, close
the valve on the air pump, and inflate the cuff until the gauge Given the hierarchical relationships between the two profes-
reaches 200 mmHg. Then the cuff has to be slowly deflated at sions, IPE programmes need to ensure that all students are
a constant rate (2 mmHg per second) while also watching the encouraged to question, and politely challenge what seems to
sphygmomanometer and listening with the stethoscope to be incorrect information. To improve teamwork and colla-
record the first and last heartbeat sounds. The first sound of boration between medicine and nursing, interprofessional
a heartbeat is recorded on the sphygmomanometer as the curricula need to include the promotion of shared interpro-
systolic pressure reading (e.g.135 mmHg) and the last sound fessional responsibility, open dialogue, and joint decision-
of the heartbeat is recorded on the sphygmomanometer as the making to improve the quality of patient care (Pian-Smith
diastolic pressure reading (e.g. 90 mmHg). All these steps et al., 2009). Given the initial evidence that medical and
require reading and remembering many numbers. This com- nursing students are likely to conform on vital skill task and
plexity could have created uncertainty about the correct the reproducibility of similar results across different samples
answer; thus, both medical and nursing students may have and settings from recent conformity studies (Beran, Drefs,
relied on the group’s responses for this particular vital sign. Kaba, Al Baz, & Al Harbi, 2015; Beran et al., 2012; Kaba, &
Perhaps the most surprising result from our study was that Beran, White, in press), replication with clinicians, different
the large majority of participants reported at least one value in clinical skills, and complex team decision-making are called
the incorrect range. That is, one quarter of the participants for, to explore the consistency of this phenomenon across
selected a diastolic BP value of about 80 and between the different study designs and contexts.
correct value of 90 and the incorrect value of 70, which was
reported by the confederates. One possible reason is that the
Acknowledgements
confederates’ value provided an anchor that pulled the partici-
pants’ score in that direction, resulting in a “hovering effect” We would like to acknowledge the following sources of support: Ward of
midway between the correct and incorrect values. This middle the 21st Century (W2IC); Undergraduate Medical Education
Standardized Patient Program; Faculty of Nursing University of
area may have seemed socially safe to report. This could be
Calgary and Undergraduate and Medical Education Department,
explained by social referencing, whereby individuals utilize University of Calgary, Canada.
others’ appraisals of events to formulate their own interpreta-
tions of those events (Feinman, 1982). Social referencing is
heightened when there is ambiguity or uncertainty which Declaration of interest
others’ evaluations help to reduce (Zarbatany & Lamb, 1985). The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are
In our study, perhaps the reason the participants reported a responsible for the writing and content of this article.
value that was halfway between the correct and incorrect value
for the diastolic BP was that the value of 80 represented an
invisible border or “reference” of what the participant per- Funding
ceived as socially safe to report. As the participants gained This study was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities
information from their social environment, they may have Research Council grant # 430-2011-0032.
looked to each other as familiar and trusted figures for gui-
dance in their actions (Zarbatany & Lamb, 1985). Since indivi- References
duals learn their behaviour through observation and imitation
of peers, the participants in our study may have felt a sense of Armstrong, R. S. (2002). Nurses’ knowledge of error in blood pressure
trust with the confederates during the first two rounds, thus measurement technique. International Journal of Nursing Practice, 8
(3), 118–126. doi:10.1046/j.1440-172X.2002.00348.x
increasing their likelihood to conform on the third round. Asch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure on the modification and
In relation to study limitations, this work was restricted to distortion of judgments. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, leadership and
one university; thus, the results may not be generalizable to men (pp. 177–190). Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Press.
122 A. KABA AND T. N. BERAN

Beran, T., Drefs, M., Kaba, A., Al Baz, N., & Al Harbi, N. (2015). education and collaborative practice. Journal of Interprofessional
Conformity of responses among graduate students in an online envir- Care, 28, 92–97. doi:10.3109/13561820.2013.869197
onment. The Internet and Higher Education, 25, 63–69. Khalili, H., Orchard, C., Laschinger, H. K. S., & Farah, R. (2013). An
Beran, T., Kaba, A., Caird, J. K., & McLaughlin, K. (2014). The good and bad interprofessional socialization framework for developing an interpro-
of group conformity: A call for a new programme of research in medical fessional identity among health professions students. Journal of
education. Medical Education, 48, 851–859. doi:10.1111/medu.12510 Interprofessional Care, 27(6), 448–453.
Beran, T., McLaughlin, K., Al Ansari, A., & Kassam, A. (2012). Landman, N., Aannestad, L. K., Smoldt, R. K., & Cortese, D. A. (2014).
Conformity of behaviors among medical students: Impact on perfor- Teamwork in Health Care. Nursing Administration Quarterly, 38(3),
mance of knee arthrocentesis in simulation. Advances in Health 198–205.
Sciences Education, 18(4), 589–596. Langendyk, V., Hegazi, I., Cowin, L., Johnson, M., & Wilson, I. (2015).
Berger, J. T. (2014). Moral distress in medical education and training. Imagining alternative professional identities: Reconfiguring profes-
Journal of General Internal Medicine, 29(2), 395–398. doi:10.1007/ sional boundaries between nursing students and medical students.
s11606-013-2665-0 Academic Medicine, 90(6), 732–737.
Bond, R., Smith, P. B. (1996). Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis of MacMillan, K. M. (2012). The challenge of achieving interprofessional
studies using Asch’s (1952b, 1956) line judgment task. Psychological collaboration: Should we blame Nightingale? Journal of
bulletin, 119, 111–137. Interprofessional Care, 26(5), 410–415.
Burford, B., Morrow, G., Morrison, J., Baldauf, B., Spencer, J., Johnson, McInnes, S., Peters, K., Bonney, A., & Halcomb, E. (2015). An integrative
N., . . . & Illing, J. (2013). Newly qualified doctors’ perceptions of review of facilitators and barriers influencing collaboration and team-
Downloaded by ["Queen's University Libraries, Kingston"] at 05:48 11 February 2016

informal learning from nurses: Implications for interprofessional edu- work between general practitioners and nurses working in general
cation and practice. Journal of interprofessional care, 27(5), 394–400. practice. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 71, 1973–1985.
CAIPE (Centre for the Advancement of Interprofessional Education). Milne, J., Greenfield, D., & Braithwaite, J. (2015). An ethnographic investiga-
(1997). Interprofessional education—A definition. Retrieved from tion of junior doctors’ capacities to practice interprofessionally in three
http://www.caipeorg.uk/about-us/defining-ipe teaching hospitals. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 29, 347–353.
Carpenter, J., Barnes, D., Dickinson, C., & Woof, D. (2006). Outcomes of Paas, F. G., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. (1994). Variability of worked
interprofessional education for Community Mental Health Services in examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cogni-
England. The longitudinal evaluation of a postgraduate programme. tive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 122.
Journal of Interprofessional Care, 20, 145–161. Papathanassoglou, E. D. E., Karanikola, M. N. K., Kalafati, M.,
Chung, C. L., Manga, J., McGregor, M., Michailidis, C., Stavros, D., & Giannakopoulou, M., Lemonidou, C., & Albarran, J. W. (2012).
Woodhouse, L. J. (2012). Interprofessional collaboration and turf wars Professional autonomy, collaboration with physicians, and moral dis-
how prevalent are hidden attitudes? Journal of Chiropractic Education, tress among European intensive care nurses. American Journal of
26, 32–39. Critical Care, 21(2), e41–52. doi:10.4037/ajcc2012205
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences Peterson, E. D., Heidarian, S., Effinger, S., Gunther, C., Diltz, M.,
(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Saunders, R., & Dombrowski, P. A. (2014). Outcomes of an interpro-
Crutchfield, R. S. (1955). Conformity and character. American fessional team learning and improvement project aimed at reducing
Psychologist, 10, 191–198. post-surgical delirium in elderly patients admitted with hip fracture.
Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. L. (1981). Sex of researchers and sex-typed com- CE Measure, 8(1), 2–7.
munications as determinants of sex differences in influenceability: A Pian-Smith, M., Simon, R., Minehart, R., Podraza, M., Rudolf, J., Walzer,
meta-analysis of social influence studies. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 1–20. T., & Raemer, D. (2009). Teaching residents the two-challenge rule: A
Feinman, S. (1982). Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-Palmer simulation-based approach to improve education and patient safety.
Quarterly, 28, 445–470. Simulation Healthcare, 4(2), 84–91.
Gilligan, C., Outram, S., & Levett-Jones, T. (2014). Recommendations Price, S., Doucet, S., & Hall, L. M. (2014). The historical social positioning of
from recent graduates in medicine, nursing and pharmacy on improv- nursing and medicine: Implications for career choice, early socialization
ing interprofessional education in university programs: A qualitative and interprofessional collaboration. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 28,
study. BMC medical education, 14(1), 52. 103–109. doi:10.3109/13561820.2013.867839
Hall, P. (2005). Interprofessional teamwork: Professional cultures as Reeves, S., Perrier, L., Goldman, J., Freeth, D., & Zwarenstein, M. (2013).
barriers. Journal of Interprofessional Care, 19(S1), 188–196. Interprofessional education: Effects on professional practice and
Hallin, K., Kiessling, A., Waldner, A., & Henriksson, P. (2009). Active healthcare outcomes (update). Cochrane Database of Systematic
interprofessional education in a patient based setting increases per- Reviews, 3, 1–47.
ceived collaborative and professional competence. Medical Teacher, 31 Reeves, S., Zwarenstein, M., Goldman, J., Barr, H., Freeth, D., Koppel, I.,
(2), 151–157. & Hammick, M. (2010). The effectiveness of interprofessional educa-
Hammick, M., Freeth, D., Koppel, I., Reeves, S., & Barr, H. (2007). A best tion: Key findings from a new systematic review. Journal of
evidence systematic review of interprofessional education: BEME Interprofessional Care, 24(3), 230–241.
Guide no. 9. Medical teacher, 29(8), 735–751. Simel, D. (2011). Approach to the patient: History and physical exam-
Helmreich, R. L., Merritt, A. C., & Wilhelm, J. A. (1999). The evolution ination (Chapter 6). In L. Goldman & A. I. Schafer (Eds.), Goldman’s
of crew resource management training in commercial aviation. Cecil Medicine (24th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Saunders Elsevier.
International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 9(1), 19. Stein, L. (1967). The doctor-nurse game. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Janis, I. (1972). Victims of groupthink. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 16, 699–703.
Kaba, A., & Beran, T. (2014). Twelve tips to guide effective participant Stein, L., Watts, D. T., & Howell, T. (1990). The doctor-nurse game
recruitment for interprofessional education research. Medical Teacher, revisited. The New England Journal of Medicine, 322, 546–549.
36(7), 578–584. doi:10.3109/0142159X.2014.907489 Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.).
Kaba, A., Beran, T., & White, D. (in press). Accuracy of interpreting vital New York: HarperCollins.
signs in simulation: An empirical study of conformity between medical Zarbatany, L., & Lamb, M. E. (1985). Social referencing as a function of
and nursing students. Journal of Interprofessional Education and Practice. information source: Mothers versus strangers. Infant Behavior and
Kavic, M. S. (2013). Cognitive load theory and learning medicine. Development, 8(1), 25–33.
Photomedicine and Laser Surgery, 31(8), 357–359. doi:10.1089/ Zwarenstein, M., Goldman, J., & Reeves, S. (2009). Interprofessional
pho.2013.9874 collaboration: Effects of practice-based interventions on professional
Khalili, H., Hall, J., & DeLuca, S. (2014). Historical analysis of profes- practice and healthcare outcomes. Cochrane Database of Systematic
sionalism in western societies: Implications for interprofessional Reviews, 3(3), 1–29.

You might also like