You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Business Research 133 (2021) 231–241

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Business Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbusres

New product pricing in business markets: The role of psychological traits


Andreas Hinterhuber a, *, Mario Kienzler b, Stephan Liozu c
a
Department of Management, Ca’ Foscari University of Venice, Italy
b
Department of Management and Engineering, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
c
Weatherhead School of Management, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: How does the bounded rationality of managers affect pricing? We examine this under-researched question by
New products examining how different psychological traits of managers relate to new product pricing practices and how these
Behavioral pricing pricing practices, in turn, relate to new product performance. To do so, we survey 302 American marketing,
Value-based pricing
sales, and pricing managers responsible for new product pricing decisions in business markets. Among others, our
Performance
Business markets
study identifies conformity and intuition as distinct psychological traits associated with pricing practices that
have a positive relationship with new product performance. The main contribution of this study is the empirical
demonstration that psychological traits offer valuable insights into how managers determine prices for new
products in business markets.

1. Introduction psychological aspects of pricing in business markets. Nevertheless,


empirical research on behavioral and psychological aspects of pricing is
The mind is the battleground. A terrain that is tricky and difficult to important to better understand how managers’ cognition, affect, and
understand. […] Mapping the mental battleground can give you an motivation influence new product pricing.
enormous advantage. (Ries & Trout, 1986, pp. 44-45) We aim to fill this gap and advance the understanding of new product
pricing decisions in business markets by asking the following research
Pricing decisions for new products are challenging, given the question: How do psychological traits of managers relate to new product
complexity of demand, competition, and costs information (see Ingen­ pricing practices, and how do these practices, in turn, relate to new product
bleek, Debruyne, Frambach, & Verhallen, 2003; Monroe & Bitta, 1978). performance? To do so, we build on the literature on bounded rationality
Under such circumstances, managers’ subjective interpretation of in­ that suggests it is necessary to understand decision makers’ minds to
formation with pricing responsibility becomes a crucial factor in the predict their behavior (see Simon, 1959).
pricing process. In short, psychological aspects come into play (e.g., Indeed, the results of our study show that understanding the minds of
Hinterhuber, 2015). managers can give companies a competitive advantage. In particular,
In line with this argument, there has been a surge in interest in the surveying over 300 American marketing, sales, and pricing managers
behavioral and psychological aspects of pricing, that is, the link between responsible for new product pricing decisions, we find that psycholog­
managerial decision-making (e.g., individual differences, perceptions, ical traits play a role in new product pricing in business markets. For
preferences, beliefs, and biases) and organizational outcomes related to example, conformity, zero-sum beliefs, and intuition are related to how
pricing (Hinterhuber & Liozu, 2017). However, much of this research is managers emphasize different pricing practices. We also explore the
conceptual (e.g., Hinterhuber, 2015; Iyer, Hong Xiao, Sharma, & Nich­ psychological traits associated with increased new product perfor­
olson, 2015; Kienzler, 2018) or focuses on managerial pricing decisions mance: Conformity and intuition. Interestingly, tentative results from
in consumer markets (e.g., Benoit, Kienzler, & Kowalkowski, 2020; this exploratory analysis suggest that conformity and intuition may play
Bogomolova, Szabo, & Kennedy, 2017; Estelami & Nejad, 2017; Ruset­ a more complicated role in new product pricing; their relationship with
ski, 2014). Except for a few studies (e.g., Hallberg, 2017; Kienzler, 2017; new product performance seems to be mediated not only by cost-based
Töytäri, Keränen, & Rajala, 2017; Töytäri, Rajala, & Alejandro, 2015), but, surprisingly, also by value-based pricing. Finally, we find that cost-
there is surprisingly little empirical insight about many behavioral and based and value-based pricing have a positive relationship with new

* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: andreas.hinterhuber@unive.it (A. Hinterhuber).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2021.04.076
Received 17 May 2020; Received in revised form 26 April 2021; Accepted 29 April 2021
Available online 11 May 2021
0148-2963/© 2021 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
A. Hinterhuber et al. Journal of Business Research 133 (2021) 231–241

product performance. making through cognitive biases (Hinterhuber, 2015; Iyer et al., 2015;
In so doing, we contribute to the predominantly conceptual work on Kienzler, 2018), intuition (Benoit et al., 2020; Bogomolova et al., 2017;
behavioral and psychological aspects of pricing in business markets. In Estelami & Nejad, 2017; Rusetski, 2014), and individual differences
particular, showing that psychological traits predict new product pricing (Kienzler, 2017; Töytäri et al., 2017), for instance.
practices and have downstream consequences for product performance Moreover, this emerging stream of research provides insights into a
highlights the impact of individual managers in the pricing process. Our number of psychological traits that potentially influence the information
results illustrate how psychological traits are related to managers’ in­ processing of bounded rational managers when engaging in different
formation processing during the pricing process and offer new insights pricing practices to determine prices for the first time. These psycho­
into new product pricing practices. This insight marks a necessary logical traits concern implicit beliefs held by managers about their
extension of the prior pricing literature that has only begun to empiri­ ability to control external events (Hinterhuber, 2004; Kienzler, 2018),
cally investigate behavioral and psychological aspects in business the tendency to conform to the behavior of others (Hinterhuber, 2015;
markets. Kienzler, 2018), the aversion to ambiguity (Kienzler, 2018; Urbany,
2001), the tendency to view own interests as opposed to interests of
2. Theoretical background other parties (Hinterhuber, 2004; Kienzler, 2018), and the reliance on
intuition in the decision-making process (Estelami & Nejad, 2017;
The current study draws on bounded rationality (Simon, 1957), Rusetski, 2014).
which posits that environmental complexity and limitations in their
abilities hinder humans from making fully rational decisions (Simon, 3. Hypotheses development
1959). Instead of the normative view of rational choice theory, bounded
rationality makes more realistic assumptions about human decision- Psychological traits can profoundly impact how humans make de­
making. Humans do not consider all available information, have cisions, as prior research across various contexts shows. We discuss their
inconsistent preferences, and rather try to achieve satisfactory than potential relevance in and impact for the pricing practices bounded
maximum outcomes (Simon, 1959). Moreover, Simon (1967) has rational managers engage in when setting prices for new products in the
pointed out that motivation and affect are—besides cogni­ following sections. Fig. 1 shows our conceptual model and the
tion—important influences on people’s information processing. Indeed, hypotheses.
others have also discussed the impact of motivation and affect in
managerial decision-making (e.g., Bazerman & Moore, 2009). Given 3.1. Perceived lack of control and pricing practices
these and other assumptions, bounded rationality is useful in investi­
gating how managers actually make decisions (see March, 1978). Simon Perceived lack of control refers to the extent to which individuals
(1959) argues for this circumstance in the following way: perceive themselves to be unable to control their destiny (see also
Rotter, 1966). According to Rotter (1966), an external locus of control
It [is] hard to ignore the distinction between the objective environ­
indicates the perception that events are largely outside one’s control. An
ment in which the economic actor ‘really’ lives and the subjective
internal locus of control indicates the perception that one’s actions in­
environment that he perceives and to which he responds. When this
fluence external events. In the context of pricing, the perceived ability to
distinction is made, we can no longer predict his behavior—even if
control external events influences pricing decisions. For instance,
he behaves rationally—from the characteristics of the objective
research indicates that many managers do not believe in their ability to
environment; we also need to know something about his perceptual
influence industry price levels and thus focus on cost information when
and cognitive processes. (p. 256)
setting prices (Hinterhuber, 2004). Furthermore, research indicates that
Hence, the central feature of bounded rationality is to understand most managers base their pricing decisions on information about costs
how affective, motivational, and cognitive processes guide the inter­ or competition (Hinterhuber & Liozu, 2012; Liozu, 2017) due to a
pretation of the environment; in short, how humans actually make perceived lack of control (Dolan & Simon, 1996). An emphasis on value-
decisions. based pricing, by contrast, requires self-confidence and the ability to
Pricing has attracted substantial research interest (e.g., Kienzler & influence customer perceptions of value and price (Nagle et al., 2011).
Kowalkowski, 2017; Kuntner & Teichert, 2016; Leone, Robinson, This line of reasoning leads the current literature to propose, albeit
Bragge, & Somervuori, 2012). The existing pricing literature stresses the without empirical investigation, that perceived lack of control has a
complex nature of pricing decisions (Dutta, Zbaracki, & Bergen, 2003). negative relation with value-based pricing (Kienzler, 2018). Hence, we
Prior research indicates that there are three main approaches to deter­ hypothesize:
mine prices for new products: cost-based, competition-based, and value-
based pricing (Cannon & Morgan, 1990; Ingenbleek et al., 2003; H1: Perceived lack of control is positively related to cost-based pricing.
Ingenbleek, Frambach, & Verhallen, 2013; Nagle, Hogan, & Zale, 2011; H2: Perceived lack of control is positively related to competition-based
Shapiro & Jackson, 1978). To arrive at a price, managers engage in pricing.
pricing practices, that is, they collect, interpret, and integrate informa­ H3: Perceived lack of control is negatively related to value-based pricing.
tion on cost, competition, and customer value (Ingenbleek et al., 2003).
Yet, existing research on pricing practices has primarily examined the 3.2. Conformity and pricing practices
effectiveness of new product pricing decisions by analyzing internal and
external contingency factors, such as market or product characteristics Conformity is the tendency to agree with or to act in accordance with
on the relative emphasis managers should put on cost, competition, and others. Conformity is driven by social pressure and the need for
customer value information when setting prices (Ingenbleek et al., 2003; consensus (Asch, 1955). Thus, it can be seen as a type of social heuristic
Ingenbleek et al., 2013). (see Gigerenzer & Gaissmaier, 2011). In the context of new product
Nevertheless, in line with bounded rationality, prior new product pricing decisions, bounded rationality suggests that conformity leads
pricing research also highlights that managers do not consider all managers to either identify competitors in the external environment or a
available information (Ingenbleek et al., 2013) and interpret the infor­ specific market norm (e.g., cost-plus pricing) as a relevant reference
mation they use during the pricing process (Ingenbleek et al., 2003). point. Thus, managers who conform overemphasize a particular type of
Indeed, an emerging stream of research highlights the influence of information. Prior pricing research provides further insights into a
affect, motivation, and cognition on managers’ price-related decision- possible relationship between conformity and different pricing prac­
tices. For instance, research indicates that most companies focus on cost-

232
A. Hinterhuber et al. Journal of Business Research 133 (2021) 231–241

Fig. 1. Conceptual model and hypotheses.

based and competition-based pricing (Hinterhuber & Liozu, 2012; Liozu, 3.4. Zero-sum belief and pricing
2017), likely due to managers’ need to conform (Hinterhuber, 2015).
Indeed, in its most extreme form, conformity manifests itself in simply A zero-sum belief is a view that one party’s win needs to results in the
copying the behavior of major competitors (Dolan & Simon, 1996). Prior other party’s loss. Hinterhuber (2004) points out that many managers
research also indicates that value-based pricing is not the norm. In this perceive pricing as a means to divide gains among themselves and the
regard, Hinterhuber (2015) suggests the low adoption of value-based customer, making it a zero-sum game. Thus, managers may focus on
pricing might be due to the need to conform. Put differently, confor­ costs that have to be covered rather than customers that need to be
mity decreases the focus on unfamiliar pricing practices that emphasize understood. Likewise, an overemphasis of competition-related measures
customers’ perceived value. Thus, we hypothesize: such as market share indicates a narrow focus. In fact, ample evidence
suggests that managers are obsessed with beating the competition
H4: Conformity is positively related to cost-based pricing. (Armstrong & Collopy, 1996; Arnett & Hunt, 2002). Simon (1959)
H5: Conformity is positively related to competition-based pricing. stresses that bounded rationality entails that humans engage in “an
H6: Conformity is negatively related to value-based pricing. active process involving attention to a very small part of the whole and
exclusion […] of almost all that is not within the scope of attention” (p.
3.3. Ambiguity aversion and pricing practices 273). Thus, managers with a zero-sum belief may focus their attention
on the ostensible win-lose structure of pricing. However, research sug­
Ambiguity aversion is the tendency to dislike uncertain situations. gests that customer value increases customer satisfaction and company
The literature on bounded rationality indicates that managers tend to profits jointly (Hinterhuber & Liozu, 2014). By focusing on customer-
avoid uncertain and vague situations (Cyert & March 1963). Further­ end benefits, value-based pricing allows increasing the overall pie size
more, Simon (1959) highlights the importance of considering decision (Anderson, Kumar, & Narus, 2007; Sawhney, 2004) and enables
makers’ perceptions of uncertainty to predict their behavior. In the win–win arrangements between suppliers and customers. Thus, we
context of new product pricing practices, the information that managers hypothesize:
encounter exhibits a varying degree of uncertainty. For instance, infor­
mation about costs and competitors is more certain and objective than H10: Zero-sum belief is positively related to cost-based pricing.
information about customers’ perceived value (see Kienzler, 2017). H11: Zero-sum belief is positively related to competition-based pricing.
Hence, prior research has suggested a positive relation between ambi­ H12: Zero-sum belief is negatively related to value-based pricing.
guity aversion and preferences for cost-based pricing and competition-
based pricing (Urbany, 2001). In stark contrast, value is by definition
always uncertain and subjective (Hinterhuber, 2017; Nagle et al., 2011) 3.5. Intuition and pricing practices
and requires managers to deal with ill-structured and uncertain infor­
mation. Hence, we hypothesize: Intuition refers to thought processes that are automatic, unconscious,
or emotional (Hodgkinson, Sadler-Smith, Burke, Claxton, & Sparrow,
H7: Ambiguity aversion is positively related to cost-based pricing. 2009). Intuition can rely on heuristics (Rusetski, 2014), for instance, in
H8: Ambiguity aversion is positively related to competition-based pricing. the form of simple rules of thumb regarding cost and competitor infor­
H9: Ambiguity aversion is negatively related to value-based pricing. mation, such as “‘The lower the cost, the higher the markup,’ [and]
‘Price slightly below the market share leader,’” respectively (Hinter­
huber, 2015, p. 70). The use of simplified rules is in line with bounded
rationality, which suggests that managers use heuristics to achieve good
enough results (see Simon, 1959). Thus, it is not surprising that the cost-

233
A. Hinterhuber et al. Journal of Business Research 133 (2021) 231–241

plus heuristic (Urbany, 2001)—an emphasis on cost information—is Table 1


widespread because it feels right. Furthermore, more intuitive managers Sample characteristics.
react stronger to competitors’ price cuts; they reduce their prices more Aspects n %
than less intuitive managers (Estelami & Nejad, 2017). Findings by
Company type
Rusetski (2014) also imply that intuition leads many managers to match Manufacturing firm 60 20
competitors’ prices. Taken together, this indicates a link between intu­ Service organization 130 43
ition and a focus on cost and competitor information. In contrast, value- Distribution/retail company 112 37
based pricing relies heavily on rigorous, evidence-based analysis to Company size (no. employees)
Less than 250 159 53
determine customers’ willingness to pay (Nagle et al., 2011). In this 251 to 500 37 12
regard, an emphasis on customer value requires managers to establish 501 to 1000 32 11
the right information sources (Ingenbleek, 2007), not only focusing on 1,001 to 10,000 48 16
their intuition regarding customer value. Hence, we hypothesize: Over 10,000 26 9
Company age
Less than 5 years old 46 15
H13: Intuition is positively related to cost-based pricing. 5 to less than 10 years old 65 22
H14: Intuition is positively related to competition-based pricing. 10 to less than 25 years old 82 27
H15: Intuition is negatively related to value-based pricing. 25 to less than 50 years old 68 23
50 years old or greater 41 14
Company headquarter
In the Americas 293 97
3.6. Pricing practices and new product performance Other 9 3
Organizational function
The relationship between different pricing practices and product and Marketing 105 35
company performance has been the subject of several empirical studies Sales 150 50
Pricing 47 16
(e.g., Ingenbleek et al., 2003; Ingenbleek et al., 2013; Liozu & Hinter­
huber, 2013b) that are, broadly, in agreement with the following: Value- N = 302; percentage might not sum to 100 percent due to rounding.
based pricing has a positive relationship with product and company
performance, while other pricing practices can be beneficial in some 4.2. Measures
circumstances. For instance, an understanding of costs and customers’
perceived value gives managers an understanding of the minimum and All measures are based on established scales but were adapted to fit
maximum price they can charge, allowing them to calibrate new product the context of the current study. Importantly, we asked the respondents
prices against these two boundaries (see Ingenbleek et al., 2003). Cost to think about one specific new product or one specific new service they
information is also essential to understand minimum price levels for new set the price for within the last one or two years when answering the
products and has thus profit implications (Ingenbleek et al., 2003). pricing practices and new product performance questions.
While emphasizing competition-based pricing may be beneficial under
some specific circumstances for new product performance (Ingenbleek 4.2.1. Perceived lack of control
et al., 2003; Ingenbleek et al., 2013), overall, it seems to have a negative We measure perceived lack of control with a five-item scale (1 =
relation with company performance (Liozu & Hinterhuber, 2013b). strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree) adapted from Hodgkinson (1992).
Hence, it has been argued that an overemphasis on competitive infor­ Respondents indicated the extent to which they agree or disagree with
mation is dangerous (Hinterhuber & Liozu, 2012). We hypothesize: each item.

H16: Cost-based pricing is positively related to new product performance. 4.2.2. Conformity
H17: Competition-based pricing is negatively related to new product We use a five-item scale adapted from Goldsmith, Clark, and Lafferty
performance. (2005) to measure conformity. The six-point scale is characterized by
H18: Value-based pricing is positively related to new product one adjective reflecting conformity and one reflecting disconformity on
performance. each respective end of the scale. We asked respondents to consider how
they behave in a typical business situation and to rate themselves
4. Method accordingly on the scale.

4.1. Data collection and sampling description 4.2.3. Ambiguity aversion


We measure ambiguity aversion with a five-item scale (1 = strongly
We commissioned Qualtrics, a leading global provider of online disagree; 7 = strongly agree) adapted from McLain (2009). Respondents
panel data, to recruit suitable respondents for this survey from April 9 to indicated the extent to which they agree or disagree with each item.
April 27, 2018. Prior to data collection, we decided on a target sample
size of 300. We obtained responses from 302 managers working for 4.2.4. Zero-sum belief
business-to-business companies that operate in the United States (see We measure zero-sum beliefs with a five-item scale (1 = strongly
Table 1 for more information). All respondents have been involved in disagree; 7 = strongly agree) adapted from Różycka-Tran, Boski, and
pricing decisions for a new product or a new service in the last two years. Wojciszke (2015). Respondents indicated the extent to which they agree
Hence, respondents are qualified. or disagree with each item.
To ensure data quality, we included two attention check questions
within the online survey. Specifically, we used two items embedded in 4.2.5. Intuition
two different survey blocks that were exactly formatted as preceding and We measure intuition with a five-item scale (1 = strongly disagree; 7
succeeding items and asked respondents to give a particular response to = strongly agree) adapted from Scott and Bruce (1995). Respondents
indicate that they were paying attention. Before data analysis, re­ indicated the extent to which they agree or disagree with each item.
spondents who did not pass these attention checks were automatically
excluded and replaced. Similarly, we asked the panel provider to replace 4.2.6. Cost-based pricing
inattentive respondents (e.g., straight liners, speeders) before data We measure cost-based pricing with a four-item scale (1 = very weak
analysis. influence; 7 = very strong influence) developed Ingenbleek et al. (2003).

234
A. Hinterhuber et al. Journal of Business Research 133 (2021) 231–241

Respondents indicated the extent to which different information related Table 2


to costs influenced the price of the new product/service. Overview of constructs.
Construct FL
4.2.7. Competition-based pricing
New product performance (CR ¼ 0.86; Alpha ¼ 0.80; AVE ¼ 0.56)
We measure competition-based pricing with a six-item scale (1 = 1. The degree to which the product/service satisfies a customer want or need 0.65
very weak influence; 7 = very strong influence) developed by Ingenbleek as compared with the objective or expectation
et al. (2003). Respondents indicated the extent to which different in­ 2. Sales to current customers as compared with the objective or expectation 0.80
formation related to competition influenced the price of the new prod­ 3. Sales to new customers as compared with the objective or expectation 0.78
4. Market share as compared with the objective or expectation 0.79
uct/service. 5. Degree to which the product/service offers a competitive advantage as 0.72
compared with the objective or expectation
4.2.8. Value-based pricing Cost-based pricing (CR ¼ 0.85; Alpha ¼ 0.77; AVE ¼ 0.59)
We measure value-based pricing with a five-item scale (1 = very weak 1. The variable costs of the product/service 0.82
2. The price necessary to break-even 0.67
influence; 7 = very strong influence) developed by Ingenbleek et al.
3. The investments in the new product/service 0.81
(2003). Respondents indicated the extent to which different information 4. The share of fixed costs in the cost price 0.76
related to customer value influenced the price of the new product/ Competition-based pricing (CR ¼ 0.91; Alpha ¼ 0.88; AVE ¼ 0.63)
service. 1. The price of competitors’ products/services 0.78
2. The competitors’ current pricing strategy 0.81
3. The estimation of competitors’ strength to react 0.75
4.2.9. New product performance 4. The market structure (number and strength of competitors) 0.78
We measure new product performance with a five-item scale (1 = 5. The degree of competition in the market 0.80
much worse/lower than competitors; 7 = much better/higher than competi­ 6. The competitive advantage of competitors in the market 0.83
tors) adapted from (Ingenbleek et al., 2013). Respondents indicated the Value-based pricing (CR ¼ 0.83; Alpha ¼ 0.73; AVE ¼ 0.55)
1. The advantages the new product/service offers to customers 0.76
extent to which the new product/service achieved a number of financial
2. The advantages of the product/service compared to substitutes 0.73
and non-financial outcomes during the first 12 months after its launch. 3. The advantages of the product/service compared to competitors’ products/ 0.73
services
4.2.10. Control variables 4. The customer’s perceived value of the product/service D
We also control for a number of other variables, namely, company 5. The balance between advantages of the product/service and its price 0.74
Perceived lack of control (CR ¼ 0.81; Alpha ¼ 0.77; AVE ¼ 0.52)
type, size and age, location of company headquarters, and organiza­
1. There is very little my company can do in order to change the rules of 0.45
tional function. All these control variables are categorical in nature (see competition in our industry
Table 1). Thus, after data collection, we created dummy variables that 2. To a great extent, the competitive environment in which my company 0.93
we include in the model. operates is shaped by forces beyond its control
3. There is little point in engaging in detailed strategic analyses and planning 0.65
because often events occur that my company cannot control
5. Analyses and results 4. My company is able to influence the basis upon which it competes with D
other firms (R)
Considering the exploratory nature of this study and our goal to build 5. Market opportunities in my industry are largely predetermined by factors 0.77
theory, partial least square structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) is beyond my company’s control
Conformity (CR ¼ 0.86; Alpha ¼ 0.79; AVE ¼ 0.61)
an appropriate analytical technique (Hair, Hult, Ringle, & Sarstedt,
1. Compliant– Defiant (R) 0.78
2014). In particular, Hair et al. (2014) state: “In situations where theory 2. Agreeing– Disagreeing (R) 0.75
is less developed, researchers should consider the use of PLS-SEM” (p. 3. Cooperative – Uncooperative (R) 0.83
14). In fact, in contexts where the focus is more on exploration than 4. Opposing – Accommodating D
5. Adapting – Inflexible (R) 0.74
confirmation, PLS-SEM is preferable to co-variance-based structural
Ambiguity aversion (CR ¼ 0.80; Alpha ¼ 0.75; AVE ¼ 0.52)
equation modeling (Hair, Howard, & Nitzl, 2020). Given the nascent 1. I try to avoid business situations that are ambiguous 0.75
empirical research of behavioral and psychological aspects of pricing in 2. I avoid business situations that are too complicated for me to easily 0.41
business markets (e.g., Hinterhuber, 2015), PLS-SEM is the appropriate understand
choice. Additionally, PLS-SEM is preferable with complex models (see 3. I try to avoid business problems that don’t seem to have only one “best” D
solution
Hair et al., 2020; Hair et al., 2014). Considering that our model contains
4. I dislike ambiguous business situations 0.75
nine main variables and several control variables, it is complex. We use 5. I prefer a business situation in which there is some ambiguity (R) 0.88
SmartPLS 3 (Ringle, Wende, & Becker, 2015) to estimate the conceptual Zero-sum belief (CR ¼ 0.89; Alpha ¼ 0.85; AVE ¼ 0.63)
model. In the following, we assess the measurement model, discuss 1. Successes of some companies are usually failures of others 0.75
2. If someone gets richer, it means that somebody else gets poorer 0.77
common method bias and endogeneity, estimate the structural model,
3. Business is so devised that when somebody gains, others have to lose 0.87
and then evaluate the overall model. Since we have very few missing 4. Business is like tennis – a person wins only when others lose 0.77
values, we use SmartPLS 3’s mean replacement function. 5. When some companies are making losses, it means that other companies 0.79
are making gains
5.1. Measurement model Intuition (CR ¼ 0.89; Alpha ¼ 0.85; AVE ¼ 0.63)
1. When making business decisions, I rely upon my instincts 0.81
2. When I make business decisions, I tend to rely on my intuition 0.86
We start by assessing the constructs’ reliability (i.e., Cronbach’s 3. I generally make business decisions that feel right to me 0.81
alpha and composite reliability) and validity (i.e., average variance 4. When I make a business decision, it is more important for me to feel that the 0.56
extracted [AVE]). Initially, some of the constructs fell below the com­ decision is right than to have a rational reason for it
5. When I make a business decision, I trust my inner feelings and reactions 0.89
mon cut-off values of 0.70 and 0.50, respectively. Following Hair et al.
(2014) recommendation, we deleted indicators below 0.40 and low- Notes: FL = Factor Loading; CR = Composite Reliability; AVE = Average
loading indicators until the constructs’ reliability and validity excee­ Variance Extracted; D = Deleted; (R) = reverse coded.
ded the cut-off values. In total, we deleted four indicators, one each from
the value-based pricing, perceived lack of control, conformity, and Next, we check for discriminant validity with several commonly
ambiguity aversion scale. After this procedure, all constructs have employed methods. First, none of the indicators’ cross-loadings exceeds
Cronbach’s alpha values and composite reliabilities above 0.70 and AVE their outer loadings. Second, the Fornell–Larcker criterion (Fornell &
values above 0.50 (see Table 2). Hence, the constructs display satisfac­ Larcker, 1981) is met since none of the constructs’ correlation with
tory reliability and validity according to standard assessment criteria.

235
A. Hinterhuber et al. Journal of Business Research 133 (2021) 231–241

another construct exceeds the square root of its AVE (see Table 3). based pricing (H6, ß = 0.25, p < .001). Thus, the findings support the
Finally, since cross-loadings and the Fornell–Larcker criterion do not hypotheses that managers’ tendency to conform is positively related to
always accurately assess discriminant validity, we follow the recent cost- and competition-based pricing. Contrary to our hypothesis, con­
literature and also employ the heterotrait-monotrait ratio (HTMT) of formity has a positive relation with value-based pricing.
correlations (Henseler, Ringle, & Sarstedt, 2015). The HTMT evaluates Ambiguity aversion is negatively and significantly related to cost-
“the average of the heterotrait-heteromethod correlations (i.e., the based pricing (H7, ß = − 0.13, p = .02), to competition-based pricing
correlations of indicators across constructs measuring different phe­ (H8, ß = − 0.15, p = .02), and not significantly related to value-based
nomena), relative to the average of the monotrait-heteromethod corre­ pricing (H9, ß = − 0.05, p = .24). Surprisingly, none of our hypotheses
lations (i.e., the correlations of indicators within the same construct)” for ambiguity aversion are supported. Nevertheless, we find directional
and is used to examine whether it is below a specific threshold (Henseler support for the circumstance that the more ambiguity averse managers
et al., 2015, p. 121). The HTMT of the constructs shown in Table 3 are, the less they emphasize value-based pricing in their company’s
ranges from 0.08 to 0.69 and is thus below the HTMT 0.90 threshold. pricing practices.
Taken together, these results indicate discriminant validity (Hair et al., Zero-sum beliefs are not significantly related to cost-based pricing
2014). (H10, ß = 0.03, p = .36), are positively and significantly related to
competition-based pricing (H11, ß = 0.14, p = .03), and are positively
and significantly related to value-based pricing (H12, ß = 0.11, p = .04).
5.2. Common method bias and endogeneity
Thus, the findings support the predicted relationship between zero-sum
beliefs and competition-based pricing. We also find directional support
Common method bias might be a concern in cross-sectional data
for zero-sum beliefs and cost-based pricing. While contrary to our hy­
collected from a single informant (see Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, &
pothesis, zero-sum beliefs have a positive relation with value-based
Podsakoff, 2003). First, we use Harman’s single-factor test to assess
pricing. Yet, the results do not hold when adjusting for multiple com­
common method bias. We subject all indicators to exploratory factor
parisons (see Table 4).
analysis (EFA) and inspect the unrotated factor solution; no single factor
Intuition is positively and significantly related to cost-based (H13, ß =
appears, and the first factor does not explain a majority of the variance
0.15, p = .002), competition-based (H14, ß = 0.17, p = .001), and value-
(Podsakoff et al., 2003). Second, we follow Lindell and Whitney (2001)
based pricing (H15, ß = 0.14, p = .007). Thus, the findings support the
approach and use the smallest positive correlation (0.001; between
predicted relationships between managers’ intuition and cost-based and
perceived lack of control and new product performance) to assess the
competition-based pricing. Contrary to our hypothesis, intuition has a
extent of common method variance and adjust for it. The results of the
positive relation with value-based pricing.
adjusted correlations (see Table 3, above the diagonal) indicate that
Finally, we examine the relations between pricing practices and new
common method bias does not seem to be a major issue in our sample.
product performance. Cost-based pricing is positively and significantly
Finally, following recent suggestions (Hult et al., 2018), we address
related to new product performance (H16, ß = 0.17, p = .013),
endogeneity by including suitable control variables in our model,
competition-based pricing is not significantly related to new product
capturing the endogenous influence of respondent, firm, and industry
performance (H17, ß = .03, p = .37), and value-based pricing is positively
differences on our results.
and significantly related to new product performance (H18, ß = 0.21, p
= .005). Thus, the findings support the predicted relationships between
5.3. Structural model cost-based and value-based pricing and new product performance. It
appears that emphasizing competition-based pricing does not have any
We use 5,000 bootstrapping re-samples and one-tailed tests to esti­ significant relation with new product performance.
mate the significance of the structural model’s standardized path co­
efficients (ß) (see Fig. 2). Since PLS-SEM does not make distributional
assumptions, bootstrapping is used to allow for significance testing of 5.4. Model evaluation
these path coefficients (Hair et al., 2014). In case of results in opposition
to our predictions, we assess them in an exploratory manner (see notes The model explains a fair amount of variance: 19% in cost-based,
under Table 4). Perceived lack of control is not significantly related to 19% in competition-based, and 14% in value-based pricing (see
cost-based (H1, ß = 0.05, p = .27), competition-based (H2, ß = 0.09, p = Table 4). Moreover, the model explains 16% of the variance in new
.13), and value-based pricing (H3, ß = − 0.03, p = .35). Hence, none of product performance. Collinearity appears to not influence our results as
our hypotheses for perceived lack of control are significant, yet all hy­ the variance inflation factor (VIF) is below 3 (Hair et al., 2020); it ranges
potheses receive directional support. between 1.05 and 2.53. Finally, to assess the model’s predictive rele­
Conformity is positively and significantly related to cost-based (H4, ß vance, we estimate Stone-Geisser’s Q2 (Geisser, 1974; Stone, 1974). The
= 0.16, p = .001), competition-based (H5, ß = 0.08, p = .07), and value- structural model displays predictive relevance (see Table 4) for the

Table 3
Correlation matrix.
Constructs M SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9.

1. New product performance 5.34 0.91 0.75 0.29 0.26 0.31 0.00 0.23 − 0.11 0.09 0.20
2. Cost-based pricing 5.43 1.13 0.29 0.77 0.53 0.47 0.05 0.18 − 0.18 0.11 0.16
3. Competition-based pricing 5.17 1.18 0.26 0.53 0.79 0.56 0.13 0.08 − 0.18 0.23 0.20
4. Value-based pricing 5.52 0.99 0.31 0.47 0.56 0.74 0.01 0.27 − 0.07 0.15 0.15
5. Perceived lack of control 4.01 1.40 0.00 0.05 0.13 0.01 0.72 − 0.06 0.05 0.19 0.10
6. Conformity 4.74 0.89 0.23 0.18 0.08 0.27 − 0.06 0.78 − 0.04 0.02 0.02
7. Ambiguity aversion 4.06 1.21 − 0.11 − 0.17 − 0.18 − 0.07 0.06 − 0.04 0.72 0.00 − 0.09
8. Zero-sum belief 3.82 1.31 0.09 0.11 0.23 0.15 0.20 0.02 0.00 0.79 0.07
9. Intuition 5.22 1.02 0.21 0.16 0.20 0.15 0.10 0.02 − 0.09 0.07 0.79

Note: N = 302; M = mean; SD = standard deviation. The square root of AVE is reported in bold on the diagonal; unadjusted construct correlations are reported below
and adjusted ones above the diagonal (see Lindell & Whitney, 2001). The adjusted correlation coefficients show the extent of common method bias in the data. The
adjusted correlation coefficients that differ from their unadjusted counterparts are underlined. The table omits correlation for the control variables for clarity. Cor­
relations ≥±0.10 are significant at 0.10 (two-tailed).

236
A. Hinterhuber et al. Journal of Business Research 133 (2021) 231–241

Fig. 2. Structural model with path coefficients of non-rejected predictions.

endogenous constructs since their Q2 values are all above 0 (Hair et al., through which they actually make decisions (Simon, 1959). More spe­
2014). cifically, Foss (2003) has argued that research ought to add content to
the “thin” notion of bounded rationality: to provide richer illustrations
of bounded rational decision-making. We do so by demonstrating how
5.5. Post-hoc analysis: Mediation of pricing practices1 managers’ psychological traits allow us to predict their focus on some
information (i.e., on cost, competition, or customer value). For example,
In an additional model, we include paths from the predictors to new managers with a stronger zero-sum belief seem to simplify complex
product performance to investigate these direct effects and potential market dynamics and buyer–supplier relationships by focusing on in­
mediation via the three pricing practices. Since this analysis does not formation about rivals when setting prices.
test a priori predictions, we use two-tailed tests. The results in Table 5 Yet, we also found a number of unexpected relationships between
show that only conformity (ß = 0.17, p = .003) and intuition (ß = 0.16, p psychological traits and pricing practices. First, we found a positive
= .006) have a significant direct effect on new product performance. relationship between conformity and value-based pricing. A tentative
These direct effects are partially mediated by cost-based and value- explanation is that conformity groups together qualities mirroring a
based pricing. Additionally, the analysis shows an indirect only media­ customer service orientation, which is characterized by being responsive
tion of ambiguity aversion on new product performance via cost-based and adaptive to customers’ needs. Service orientation is an essential part
pricing and an indirect only mediation of zero-sum beliefs on new of a value-based approach to business as offerings are chosen or adapted
product performance via value-based pricing (see bold confidence in­ to customers’ perceived value. Second, ambiguity aversion had a
tervals in Table 5 for mediation). negative relation with cost-based and competition-based pricing. Under
certain circumstances, ambiguity aversion may plausibly reduce the
6. Discussion focus on cost and competitive information. For instance, in the context
of radically new products, cost information may be highly ambiguous or
Building on the theory of bounded rationality (e.g., Simon, 1957, simply unavailable (see Christen, 2005). Thus, bounded rational man­
1959, 1967) and prior research on behavioral and psychological aspects agers’ ambiguity aversion could, under such circumstances, lead to
in pricing (e.g., Hinterhuber, 2015; Iyer et al., 2015; Kienzler, 2018), we negative relations with cost-based and competition-based pricing.
find evidence that managers’ psychological traits are related to their Finally, managers’ intuition had a positive relation with value-based
emphasis on different pricing practices. While the theory of bounded pricing. While unexpected, the result is compatible with prior research
rationality has been used in prior research on behavioral and psycho­ finding no relationship between managers’ rationality and firm perfor­
logical aspects of pricing (e.g., Hallberg, 2017; Iyer et al., 2015; Kien­ mance (Liozu & Hinterhuber, 2013a), thus allowing for the possibility
zler, 2018), we are among the first to empirically show its impact on for a positive but untested relationship between intuition and perfor­
pricing decisions. In particular, we find support for our predictions that mance. As already noted, we find a positive relationship between intu­
both conformity and intuition are positively related to cost-based and ition and cost-based and competition-based pricing. Intuition involves
competition-based pricing. Similarly, zero-sum beliefs have a positive “affectively charged judgments that arise through rapid, nonconscious,
relation with competition-based pricing. and holistic associations” (Dane & Pratt, 2007, p. 40), but also “a
These findings are in line with the premise of bounded rationality cognitive conclusion based on a decision maker’s previous experiences”
that humans do not consider all available information (Simon, 1957) (Burke & Miller, 1999, p. 92). Taken together, our findings point to the
and the notion that researchers instead need to understand the processes possibility that intuition allows managers to quickly process cost and
competitive information and—at the same time—understand complex
customer value information based on experience and insight. Again,
1
We thank the editor for suggesting this exploratory analysis.

237
A. Hinterhuber et al. Journal of Business Research 133 (2021) 231–241

Table 4 Table 4 (continued )


Results of hypothesis testing. No. Path Direction β t- p- Support
No. Path Direction β t- p- Support values values
values values
R2/Q2 for 0.14/
H1 Perceived lack + 0.05 0.62 0.27 Value-based 0.05
of control → pricing
Cost-based R2/Q2 for New 0.16/
pricing product 0.07
H2 Perceived lack + 0.09 1.11 0.13 performance
of control →
Controls: Included (company type, size, and age, location of company head­
Competition-
based pricing quarter, and organizational function).
H3 Perceived lack – − 0.03 0.38 0.35 Notes: The t-values are estimated using the bootstrap resampling procedure in
of control → SmartPLS (302 cases. 5,000 runs). * p < .10; ** p < .05; *** p < .01; **** p <
Value-based .001 (one-tailed). ✔ indicates significant path in predicted direction ↪indicates
pricing significant path in opposite direction (note, p-values of these paths are taken
H4 Conformity → + 0.16*** 2.99 0.001 ✔ directly from SmartPLS and are not corrected for the fact that the path is in
Cost-based opposition to our prediction. However, the significance holds for all these paths
pricing
[at or below 0.10, two-tailed test, and adjusting for multiple comparisons, that
H5 Conformity → + 0.08* 1.46 0.07 ✔
Competition-
is, two], except zero-sum belief → value-based pricing). Q2 are calculated with
based pricing an omission distance of 7.
H6 Conformity → – 0.25**** 4.55 <0.001 ↪
Value-based
pricing Table 5
H7 Ambiguity + − 0.13** 2.04 0.02 ↪ Mediation analyses.
aversion →
Cost-based Indirect effect on new product performance
pricing via…
H8 Ambiguity + − 0.15** 1.98 0.02 ↪ Cost-based Competition- Value- Direct effect on
aversion → pricing based pricing based new product
Competition- pricing performance
based pricing
H9 Ambiguity – − 0.05 0.07 0.24 Predictor [LB, UB] [LB, UB] [LB, UB] Path (sig.)
aversion → Perceived [− 0.007, [− 0.009, [− 0.033, − 0.02 (0.784)
Value-based lack of 0.031] 0.028] 0.011]
pricing control
H10 Zero-sum + 0.03 0.37 0.36 Conformity [0.002, [− 0.009, [0.008, 0.17 (0.003)
belief → Cost- 0.049] 0.020] 0.084]
based pricing Ambiguity [¡0.048, [− 0.029, [− 0.035, − 0.05 (0.450)
H11 Zero-sum + 0.14** 1.96 0.03 ✔ aversion ¡0.002] 0.017] 0.007]
belief → Zero-sum [− 0.008, [− 0.017, [0.002, − 0.01 (0.832)
Competition- belief 0.025] 0.031] 0.053]
based pricing Intuition [0.002, [− 0.022, [0.003, 0.16 (0.006)
H12 Zero-sum – 0.11** 1.75 0.04 ↪ 0.046] 0.032] 0.061]
belief →
Note: Lower bound (LB) and upper bound (UB) of the 90% confidence interval
Value-based
(CI) of the path coefficients are shown.
pricing
H13 Intuition → + 0.15*** 2.84 0.002 ✔ We use the bias-corrected and accelerated (BCa) bootstrap procedure and two-
Cost-based tailed tests. The 90% CI’s marked in bold do not contain zero. 5,000 bootstrap
pricing re-samples were used to calculate the values shown. The table omits control
H14 Intuition → + 0.17*** 3.18 0.001 ✔ variables for clarity.
Competition-
based pricing
H15 Intuition → – 0.14*** 2.44 0.007 ↪
these findings contribute to a richer picture of bounded rational de­
Value-based cisions by managers (see also Foss, 2003).
pricing Finally, we provide tentative insights into mediation effects. How­
H16 Cost-based + 0.17** 2.24 0.013 ✔ ever, it is vital to highlight that these implications must be preliminarily
pricing → New
considered due to the post-hoc nature of this analysis. Both cost-based
product
performance and value-based pricing partially mediate the positive effect of confor­
H17 Competition- – 0.03 0.34 0.37 mity on new product performance. This suggests that more conforming
based pricing managers can make more accurate product performance forecasts and
→ New
that this ability can be partially attributed to their engagement in cost-
product
performance
based and value-based pricing. Similarly, both cost-based and value-
H18 Value-based + 0.21*** 2.56 0.005 ✔ based pricing partially mediate the positive effect of intuition on new
pricing → New product performance. A plausible explanation is that both pricing
product practices help to integrate the information managers’ instincts and
performance
feelings provide to them (e.g., about costs and customers) into a more
R2/Q2 for 0.19/
Cost-based 0.09 precise product performance estimate. Again, this seems to be in line
pricing with prior research suggesting intuition may be helpful in pricing (Liozu
R2/Q2 for 0.19/ & Hinterhuber, 2013a). The results also indicate an indirect only
Competition- 0.10 mediation of ambiguity aversion on new product performance via cost-
based pricing
based pricing and zero-sum beliefs on new product performance via
value-based pricing. These results suggest that ambiguity aversion and
zero-sum beliefs, combined with particular pricing practices, may also

238
A. Hinterhuber et al. Journal of Business Research 133 (2021) 231–241

play a role in explaining new product performance. limited information for profitable pricing. Considering information
related to competitors’ prices does not help managers forecast how well
6.1. Theoretical implications new products will actually do because customer perception is not in
focus. In other words, competition-based pricing helps managers to
The present study makes three broad theoretical contributions to the understand what competitors charge for existing products but not to
pricing literature. First, this is the first quantitative study investigating accurately predict market acceptance of new products. Managers need
the relationship between psychological traits of individual managers, to reinforce the point that pricing practices that emphasize competition-
pricing practices, and new product performance. Hence, we built based pricing are problematic since they seem not to contribute to a
directly on prior conceptual research on the behavioral and psycho­ company’s goal of reaching its new product performance targets.
logical aspects of pricing (e.g., Hinterhuber, 2015; Kienzler, 2018) and
empirical research on companies’ pricing practices (e.g., Ingenbleek 6.3. Limitations and suggestions for future research
et al., 2003). In so doing, we advance the current state of the pricing
literature by linking both research streams and demonstrating that this Like all research, this study has its limitations that offer fruitful av­
link helps advance marketing theory. enues for future research. First, while we investigate the effects of a
Second, our study indicates that the psychological traits of managers broad set of psychological traits, the present study is clearly not
are clearly related to new product pricing practices, which in turn have a comprehensive. For instance, prior conceptual work on behavioral and
direct effect on new product performance. Pricing psychology is psychological aspects of pricing has argued that managers employ
generally well accepted in consumer markets. Yet, it has only recently simple heuristics in their pricing practices (Hinterhuber, 2015). How­
received increased attention in research on business markets; so far, ever, we are currently missing systematic research that goes beyond
primarily in conceptual work (e.g., Hinterhuber, 2015; Iyer et al., 2015; anecdotal evidence. Hence, pricing heuristics make for an intriguing
Kienzler, 2018). Thus, our study highlights the value in exploring the topic for behavioral pricing research investigating bounded rational
minds of bounded rational managers to better understand the behavioral managers. In particular, further research should investigate how simple
and psychological aspects of pricing in business markets. heuristics relate to new product performance. A similar argument can be
Third, the literature on behavioral and psychological aspects of made for other behavioral and psychological aspects (for examples of
pricing has diverse theoretical foundations (for instance, see Estelami & these aspects, see Hinterhuber, 2015; Iyer et al., 2015; Kienzler, 2018).
Nejad, 2017; Kienzler, 2017; Töytäri et al., 2017; Woodside, 2015). Second, our findings highlight that it is challenging to accurately
However, an overall theoretical framework that guides this emerging predict managers’ emphasis on value-based pricing from their psycho­
stream of research is currently missing. We illustrate that bounded ra­ logical traits. We surmise that challenges related to cost-based and
tionality can function as such a theoretical framework and guide further competition-based pricing are general, while those related to value-
investigations into managerial pricing decisions. In particular, bounded based pricing may be specific to a particular pricing situation. We
rationality is suitable because it allows integrating research on diverse measure managers’ general psychological disposition (e.g., their general
behavioral and psychological aspects, such as individual differences, perceived lack of control in business situations) instead of their
heuristics, and biases, under one common framework. In turn, this perceived lack of control during a particular pricing decision. Hence,
receptiveness allows for “thicker” descriptions of bounded rationality further research should investigate psychological states instead of traits.
(see Foss, 2003). Moreover, bounded rationality lends itself naturally to Again, this seems to be particularly important for investigating man­
function as such a framework as prior research on behavioral and psy­ agers’ relative emphasis on value-based pricing.
chological aspects of pricing has already used it (e.g., Hallberg, 2017; Third, we employ a comparatively large and diverse sample of
Iyer et al., 2015; Kienzler, 2018). American managers involved in pricing decisions. Despite this advan­
tage, our sample does not allow us to draw conclusions about managers
6.2. Managerial implications in the rest of the world. Future research should investigate our hy­
potheses with a sample from another country. For instance, perceived
The findings have implications for managers directly or indirectly lack of control may have a different relationship with pricing practices
involved in new product pricing decisions. Generally, our study high­ due to a more or less competitive business culture.
lights the need to manage the influence individuals have on pricing Fourth, while companies need to consider how individual managers
decisions. More specifically, the study has the following managerial influence the pricing process, pricing is not always the responsibility of a
implications. single person. Indeed, pricing decisions are to an increasing extent made
First, our study shows that psychological traits can affect new by pricing teams (e.g., Feurer, Schuhmacher, & Kuester, 2019). Thus, a
product performance via three pricing practices. The traits associated potentially fruitful direction for further research is to investigate how
with pricing practices that have a positive relationship with new product behavioral and psychological aspects of pricing teams impact pricing
performance are conformity and intuition. Hence, our findings highlight decisions.
that new product performance is also a function of the psychological Fifth and finally, we are convinced that original studies are impor­
traits of managers with pricing responsibility. Senior executives inter­ tant to develop a field further. However, single studies do not offer
ested in increasing new product sales could include intuition and con­ robust evidence about whether an effect replicates across different
formity in a list of criteria and competencies for selecting and samples, time frames, and operationalizations of measures. Thus, we
developing marketing, sales, or pricing managers with new product encourage other scholars interested in behavioral and psychological
pricing responsibility. aspects of pricing to conceptually replicate and extend our findings.
Second, our results highlight the positive and significant relation
between value-based pricing and new product performance. This finding Declaration of Competing Interest
indicates that investing in value-based pricing is a profitable decision.
While we also find a positive and significant relation between cost-based The authors declare that they have no known competing financial
pricing and new product performance, a sole focus on cost-based pricing interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence
is not recommended because it neglects market information (e.g., cus­ the work reported in this paper.
tomers’ willingness-to-pay). Moreover, we find no relation between
competition-based pricing and new product performance. While this Acknowledgements
finding can seem surprising, it is in line with the extended literature on
pricing, highlighting that competition-based pricing often offers only We want to thank the Editors, Prof. Kunal Swani, Ph.D, Prof.

239
A. Hinterhuber et al. Journal of Business Research 133 (2021) 231–241

Domingo Ribeiro-Soriano and the two anonymous referees for their Hinterhuber, A. (2015). Violations of rational choice principles in pricing decisions.
Industrial Marketing Management, 47, 65–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
contributions. We are grateful beneficiaries of intellectually stimulating,
indmarman.2015.02.006.
curious and developmental comments to earlier versions of this paper. Hinterhuber, A. (2017). Value quantification capabilities in industrial markets. Journal of
Business Research, 76, 163–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2016.11.019.
Hinterhuber, A., & Liozu, S. (2012). Is it time to rethink your pricing strategy? MIT Sloan
Funding information Management Review, 53(4), 69–77.
Hinterhuber, A., & Liozu, S. M. (2014). Is innovation in pricing your next source of
Jan Wallanders och Tom Hedelius Stiftelse samt Tore Browaldhs competitive advantage? Business Horizons, 57(3), 413–423. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.bushor.2014.01.002.
Stiftelse, Grant: W19-0018, recipient: Mario Kienzler. Hinterhuber, A., & Liozu, S. M. (2017). The micro-foundations of pricing. Journal of
Business Research, 76, 159–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2016.11.018.
References Hodgkinson, G. P. (1992). Research notes and communications development and
validation of the strategic locus of control scale. Strategic Management Journal, 13(4),
311–317. https://doi.org/10.1002/smj.4250130405.
Anderson, J. C., Kumar, N., & Narus, J. (2007). Value merchants: Demonstrating and
Hodgkinson, G. P., Sadler-Smith, E., Burke, L. A., Claxton, G., & Sparrow, P. R. (2009).
documenting superior value in business markets. Boston, MA: Harvard Business School
Intuition in organizations: Implications for strategic management. Long Range
Press.
Planning, 42(3), 277–297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lrp.2009.05.003.
Armstrong, J. S., & Collopy, F. (1996). Competitor orientation: Effects of objectives and
Hult, G. T. M., Hair, J. F., Proksch, D., Sarstedt, M., Pinkwart, A., & Ringle, C. M. (2018).
information on managerial decisions and profitability. Journal of Marketing Research,
Addressing endogeneity in international marketing applications of partial least
33(2), 188–199. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224379603300206.
squares structural equation modeling. Journal of International Marketing, 26(3), 1–21.
Arnett, D. B., & Hunt, S. D. (2002). Competitive irrationality: The influence of moral
https://doi.org/10.1509/jim.17.0151.
philosophy. Business Ethics Quarterly, 12(3), 279–303. https://doi.org/10.2307/
Ingenbleek, P. (2007). Value-informed pricing in its organizational context: Literature
3858018.
review, conceptual framework, and directions for future research. Journal of Product
Asch, S. E. (1955). Opinions and social pressure. Scientific American, 193(5), 31–35.
& Brand Management, 16(7), 441–458. https://doi.org/10.1108/
Bazerman, M. H., & Moore, D. A. (2009). Judgment in managerial decision making. New
10610420710834904.
Caledonia: John Wiley & Sons.
Ingenbleek, P., Debruyne, M., Frambach, R. T., & Verhallen, T. M. M. (2003). Successful
Benoit, S., Kienzler, M., & Kowalkowski, C. (2020). Intuitive pricing by independent store
new product pricing practices: A contingency approach. Marketing Letters, 14(4),
managers: Challenging beliefs and practices. Journal of Business Research, 115,
289–305. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:MARK.0000012473.92160.3d.
70–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.04.027.
Ingenbleek, P., Frambach, R. T., & Verhallen, T. M. M. (2013). Best practices for new
Bogomolova, S., Szabo, M., & Kennedy, R. (2017). Retailers’ and manufacturers’ price-
product pricing: Impact on market performance and price level under different
promotion decisions: Intuitive or evidence-based? Journal of Business Research, 76,
conditions. Journal of Product Innovation Management, 30(3), 560–573. https://doi.
189–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2016.05.020.
org/10.1111/jpim.12008.
Burke, L. A., & Miller, M. K. (1999). Taking the mystery out of intuitive decision making.
Iyer, G. R., Hong Xiao, S., Sharma, A., & Nicholson, M. (2015). Behavioral issues in price
Academy of Management Perspectives, 13(4), 91–99. https://doi.org/10.5465/
setting in business-to-business marketing: A framework for analysis. Industrial
ame.1999.2570557.
Marketing Management, 47, 6–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Cannon, H. M., & Morgan, F. W. (1990). A strategic pricing framework. Journal of Services
indmarman.2015.02.001.
Marketing, 4(2), 19–30. https://doi.org/10.1108/EUM0000000002508.
Kienzler, M. (2017). Does managerial personality influence pricing practices under
Christen, M. (2005). Research note—cost uncertainty is bliss: The effect of competition
uncertainty? Journal of Product & Brand Management, 26(7), 771–784. https://doi.
on the acquisition of cost information for pricing new products. Management Science,
org/10.1108/JPBM-11-2016-1352.
51(4), 668–676. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.1040.0320.
Kienzler, M. (2018). Value-based pricing and cognitive biases: An overview for business
Cyert, R. M., & March, J. G. (1963). A behavioral theory of the firm. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
markets. Industrial Marketing Management, 68, 86–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Prentice Hall.
indmarman.2017.09.028.
Dane, E., & Pratt, M. G. (2007). Exploring intuition and its role in managerial decision
Kienzler, M., & Kowalkowski, C. (2017). Pricing strategy: A review of 22 years of
making. Academy of Management Review, 32(1), 33–54. https://doi.org/10.5465/
marketing research. Journal of Business Research, 78, 101–110. https://doi.org/
amr.2007.23463682.
10.1016/j.jbusres.2017.05.005.
Dolan, R., & Simon, H. (1996). Power: How managing price transforms the bottom line. New
Kuntner, T., & Teichert, T. (2016). The scope of price promotion research: An informetric
York, NY: The Free Press.
study. Journal of Business Research, 69(8), 2687–2696. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Dutta, S., Zbaracki, M. J., & Bergen, M. (2003). Pricing process as a capability: A
jbusres.2015.11.004.
resource-based perspective. Strategic Management Journal, 24(7), 615–630. https://
Leone, R. P., Robinson, L. M., Bragge, J., & Somervuori, O. (2012). A citation and
doi.org/10.1002/smj.323.
profiling analysis of pricing research from 1980 to 2010. Journal of Business Research,
Estelami, H., & Nejad, M. G. (2017). The impact of cognitive style, entrepreneurial
65(7), 1010–1024. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2011.04.007.
attitudes and gender on competitive price responses. Journal of Product & Brand
Lindell, M. K., & Whitney, D. J. (2001). Accounting for common method variance in
Management, 26(7), 759–770. https://doi.org/10.1108/JPBM-05-2016-1189.
cross-sectional research designs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(1), 114–121.
Feurer, S., Schuhmacher, M. C., & Kuester, S. (2019). How pricing teams develop
https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.1.114.
effective pricing strategies for new products. Journal of Product Innovation
Liozu, S., & Hinterhuber, A. (2013a). CEO championing of pricing, pricing capabilities
Management, 36(1), 66–86. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpim.12444.
and firm performance in industrial firms. Industrial Marketing Management, 42(4),
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with
633–643. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.indmarman.2012.09.003.
unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1),
Liozu, S., & Hinterhuber, A. (2013b). Pricing orientation, pricing capabilities, and firm
39–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224378101800104.
performance. Management Decision, 51(3), 594–614. https://doi.org/10.1108/
Foss, N. J. (2003). Bounded rationality in the economics of organization: “Much cited
00251741311309670.
and little used”. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24(2), 245–264. https://doi.org/
Liozu, S. M. (2017). State of value-based-pricing survey: Perceptions, challenges, and
10.1016/S0167-4870(02)00206-4.
impact. Journal of Revenue and Pricing Management, 16(1), 18–29. https://doi.org/
Geisser, S. (1974). A predictive approach to the random effect model. Biometrika, 61(1),
10.1057/s41272-016-0059-8.
101–107. https://doi.org/10.1093/biomet/61.1.101.
March, J. G. (1978). Bounded rationality, ambiguity, and the engineering of choice. The
Gigerenzer, G., & Gaissmaier, W. (2011). Heuristic decision making. Annual Review of
Bell Journal of Economics, 9(2), 587–608. https://doi.org/10.2307/3003600.
Psychology, 62(1), 451–482. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-
McLain, D. L. (2009). Evidence of the properties of an ambiguity tolerance measure: The
145346.
multiple stimulus types ambiguity tolerance scale–II (MSTAT–II). Psychological
Goldsmith, R. E., Clark, R. A., & Lafferty, B. A. (2005). Tendency to conform: A new
Reports, 105(3), 975–988. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.105.3.975-988.
measure and its relationship to psychological reactance. Psychological Reports, 96(3),
Monroe, K. B., & Bitta, A. J. D. (1978). Models for pricing decisions. Journal of Marketing
591–594. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.96.3.591-594.
Research, 15(3), 413–428. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224377801500312.
Hair, J. F. J., Howard, M. C., & Nitzl, C. (2020). Assessing measurement model quality in
Nagle, T. T., Hogan, J. E., & Zale, J. (2011). The strategy and tactics of pricing: A guide to
PLS-SEM using confirmatory composite analysis. Journal of Business Research, 109,
growing more profitably (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
101–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2019.11.069.
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method
Hair, J. F. J., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C., & Sarstedt, M. (2014). A primer on partial least
biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended
squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) (1st ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE
remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879–903. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Publications.
0021-9010.88.5.879.
Hallberg, N. L. (2017). The micro-foundations of pricing strategy in industrial markets: A
Ries, A., & Trout, J. (1986). Marketing Warfare. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
case study in the European packaging industry. Journal of Business Research, 76,
Ringle, C. M., Wende, S., & Becker, J.-M. (2015). SmartPLS 3. Bönningstedt: SmartPLS
179–188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2017.01.001.
GmbH. http://www.smartpls.com.
Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M., & Sarstedt, M. (2015). A new criterion for assessing
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of
discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. Journal of the
reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80(1), 1–28. https://
Academy of Marketing Science, 43(1), 115–135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-
doi.org/10.1037/h0092976.
014-0403-8.
Różycka-Tran, J., Boski, P., & Wojciszke, B. (2015). Belief in a zero-sum game as a social
Hinterhuber, A. (2004). Towards value-based pricing—An integrative framework for
axiom: A 37-nation study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46(4), 525–548.
decision making. Industrial Marketing Management, 33(8), 765–778. https://doi.org/
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022115572226.
10.1016/j.indmarman.2003.10.006.

240
A. Hinterhuber et al. Journal of Business Research 133 (2021) 231–241

Rusetski, A. (2014). Pricing by intuition: Managerial choices with limited information. Töytäri, P., Rajala, R., & Alejandro, T. B. (2015). Organizational and institutional barriers
Journal of Business Research, 67(8), 1733–1743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. to value-based pricing in industrial relationships. Industrial Marketing Management,
jbusres.2014.02.020. 47, 53–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.indmarman.2015.02.005.
Sawhney, M. (2004). Trilogy corporation: Customer value-based pricing. Evanston, IL: Urbany, J. E. (2001). Justifying profitable pricing. Journal of Product & Brand
Kellogg School of Management Cases, KEL 106, Northwestern University. https:// Management, 10(3), 141–159. https://doi.org/10.1108/10610420110395386.
doi.org/10.1108/case.kellogg.2016.000379. Woodside, A. G. (2015). The general theory of behavioral pricing: Applying complexity
Scott, S. G., & Bruce, R. A. (1995). Decision-making style: The development and theory to explicate heterogeneity and achieve high-predictive validity. Industrial
assessment of a new measure. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55(5), Marketing Management, 47, 39–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
818–831. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164495055005017. indmarman.2015.02.004.
Shapiro, B. P., & Jackson, B. B. (1978). Industrial pricing to meet customer needs.
Harvard Business Review, 56(6), 119–127.
Andreas Hinterhuber is Associate Professor at the Department of Management, Ca’
Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man: Social and rational. Oxford, England: Wiley.
Foscari University of Venice, Italy.
Simon, H. A. (1959). Theories of decision-making in economics and behavioral science.
The American Economic Review, 49(3), 253–283.
Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychological Mario Kienzler is Assistant Professor at the Department of Management and Engineering,
Review, 74(1), 29–39. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024127. Linköping University, Sweden.
Stone, M. (1974). Cross-validatory choice and assessment of statistical predictions.
Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 36(2), 111–133.
Stephan Liozu is Adjunct professor and research fellow at Case Western Reserve Uni­
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1974.tb00994.x.
versity, Weatherhead School of Management, USA.
Töytäri, P., Keränen, J., & Rajala, R. (2017). Barriers to implementing value-based
pricing in industrial markets: A micro-foundations perspective. Journal of Business
Research, 76, 237–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2016.04.183.

241

You might also like