You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Occupational Health Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association

2014, Vol. 19, No. 2, 259 –267 1076-8998/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0036267

Healthy Eating at Different Risk Levels for Job Stress:


Testing a Moderated Mediation

Daniel P. Fodor and Conny H. Antoni Amelie U. Wiedemann


Universität Trier Freie Universität Berlin

Silke Burkert
Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Health behavior, like fruit and vegetable consumption (FVC), is affected by unfavorable job conditions.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

However, there is little research to date that combines job stress models and health-behavior change
models. This longitudinal study examined the contribution of risk factors associated with job stress to the
intention–planning–FVC relationship. In the context of the Health Action Process Approach, action
planning (when-where-how plans) and coping planning (plans to overcome anticipated barriers) have
been shown to be successful mediators in the translation of health-related intentions into action. Risk
factors for job stress are operationalized as the interaction of job demands and job resources in line with
the Job Demands–Resources (JD-R) model. Two hundred seventy-two employees (mean age 41.2 years,
73.9% female) from different jobs completed measures of intention at baseline (t1), action planning and
coping planning 2 weeks later (t2), and FVC another 2 weeks later (t3). Job demands and job resources
were assessed at t1 and t2. A moderated mediation analysis indicated that risk factors for job stress
moderate the translation of intention into action planning (B ⫽ ⫺0.23, p ⬍ .05) and coping planning
(B ⫽ ⫺0.14, p ⬍ .05). No moderation effect of the planning-FVC relationship by risk factors for job
stress was found. However, coping planning directly predicted FVC (B ⫽ 0.36, p ⬍ .001). Findings
suggest that employees intending to eat healthily use action planning and coping planning when job
demands exceed job resources. For increasing FVC, coping planning appears most beneficial.

Keywords: job demands-resources model, healthy diet, work strain, planning, health behavior change

In recent years, research on the relationship between conditions the demand– control model (DCM; Karasek, 1979) was used as a
of job stress and health behaviors, like fruit and vegetable con- framework model for job strain. With respect to the complexity of
sumption (FVC), has increased. However, results are inconsistent the HAPA, we think that the Job Demands–Resources (JD-R)
(e.g., Siegrist & Rödel, 2006). We bring forward two assumptions model (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007), which has not been linked to
that may add to the understanding of how risk factors for job stress health behavior so far, more appropriately specifies risk factors
are related to FVC. First, the majority of studies on that topic associated with job demands and job resources. By combining the
focused on health or risk behaviors simply as outcomes of job JD-R model and the HAPA, the present study aims at investigating
stress. We think that risk factors for job stress need to be linked to how risk factors for job stress relate to FVC and its antecedents. In
the volitional steps of the behavior change process according to the particular, we will consider action planning and coping planning as
Health Action Process Approach (HAPA) (Schwarzer, 2008). potential mediators of the relationship between intentions and
Here, the realization of an intention to eat healthy can be supported FVC. Additionally, the interaction of job demands and job re-
by action planning and coping planning as personal resources. sources with these two planning types will be assessed as addi-
Whereas action planning is a goal-oriented strategy to achieve an tional moderators of this mediation.
intended behavior (Gollwitzer, 1999), coping planning is a barrier-
focused strategy to deal with obstacles that may arise while acting Predicting Healthy Eating
upon an intention (Sniehotta, 2009). Second, in almost all studies,
Consuming a sufficient amount of fruit and vegetables is a key
factor for a healthy diet and minimizes the risk of developing
cardiovascular disease and other health problems (World Health
Daniel P. Fodor, Universität Trier, Trier, Germany; Conny H. Antoni, Organization [WHO], 2003). The WHO recommends consuming
Work and Organizational Psychology, Universität Trier; Amelie U. Wie- at least 400 g or approximately 5 servings of fruit and vegetables
demann, Division of Health Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin,
each day. However, most people do not meet the recommended
Germany; Silke Burkert, Institute of Medical Psychology, Charité - Uni-
versitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany.
levels (e.g., Blanck, Gillespie, Kimmons, Seymour, & Serdula,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Daniel P. 2008). To explain why some people successfully adopt a healthy
Fodor, Insitute of Medical Psychology, Charité – Universitätsmedizin diet and others fail to do so, the HAPA (Schwarzer, 2008) differ-
Berlin, Luisenstr. 57, 10117 Berlin, Germany. E-mail: daniel.fodor@ entiates between a motivational and a volitional phase. The moti-
charite.de vational phase results in the formation of an intention which can be

259
260 FODOR, ANTONI, WIEDEMANN, AND BURKERT

defined as an individual’s motivation to show a specific behavior of high job demands and low job resources increases the likelihood of
to reach a desired goal. Meta-analyses have identified fundamental job stress. The JD-R model also proposes that high job resources can
correlations between intentions and different health behaviors, buffer the negative impact of high job demands on the development
including a healthy diet (e.g., Armitage & Conner, 2001). Never- of job stress (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007). Evidence for the buffering
theless, intentions still appear insufficient to change behavior in effects of several job resources (e.g., “autonomy,” “social support”)
experimental studies (Webb & Sheeran, 2006). According to the on the impact of various job demands (e.g., “emotional demands,”
HAPA, an individual who has formed an intention steps into the “work-home conflict”) on symptoms of burnout, for example, support
volitional phase, which can be further subdivided into a preactional this buffering hypothesis (Bakker, Demerouti, & Euwema, 2005). The
phase and an action phase. In the preactional phase, individuals, second hypothesis of the JD-R model, the coping hypothesis, which
who are intending to perform but have not (yet) performed a health states that job demands moderate the motivating effect of job re-
behavior, prepare for the implementation of their healthy actions. sources (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007), was confirmed by the finding
In this phase, forming action plans and coping plans is acknowl- that job resources mainly foster employees’ motivation when job
edged as an effective self-regulatory strategy to overcome voli- resources and job demands are high (e.g., Hakanen, Bakker, & De-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tional barriers to action. In creating action plans, individuals spec- merouti, 2005). Previous studies on the JD-R model investigated the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ify when, where, and what action they will carry out to achieve a interaction effects of job demands and job resources on outcomes
certain goal (Sniehotta, Scholz, & Schwarzer, 2005). By mentally such as burnout, organizational commitment, or work engagement. To
linking environmental cues to a behavioral response, goal- our knowledge, there are only a few studies combining assumptions
fulfilling behavior becomes more likely (Gollwitzer, 1999). In the from job stress models and health-behavior theory so far.
work context, action planning is assumed to be particularly nec- Payne, Jones, and Harris (2002) showed detrimental effects of job
essary. Highly demanding jobs are characterized by time pressure, conditions with high job demands and low job control on health
goal conflicts, and instable intentions. Under such conditions, behavior change (i.e., exercise). However, they found no influence of
individuals may favor time-effective and convenient fast-food job conditions on FVC (Payne et al., 2005). In an experimental study
solutions over healthy meals rich in fruits and vegetables. To adapt with a planning intervention they investigated direct effects of job
a healthier diet anyway, an individual may specify an action demands on exercise behavior and their interaction with psychosocial
plan—“If I am checking my e-mails in the morning (when) at my variables (Payne, Jones, & Harris, 2010). Unexpectedly, the planning
workplace (where), then I will eat an apple (what).” However, intervention did not help to overcome work related barriers. However,
despite realistic and meaningful action planning, the implementa- job demands moderated the relationship between intention and exer-
tion of health behavior often requires dealing effectively with cise. Job resources, which might have buffered the job demands,
barriers for action. In the context of the present study, barriers may however, were not investigated.
arise from negative occupational conditions (e.g., time pressure) Referring to the JD-R model, the present study aims at exam-
and a lack of resources for counterregulation (e.g., time control). ining whether self-administered planning helps to enact intentions,
Coping planning helps to deal with such barriers by mentally and takes unfavorable job conditions, that is, the imbalance of
linking a suitable coping response to an anticipated risk situation diverse job demands and job resources, into account.
(Sniehotta, 2009). For instance, an employee may anticipate over-
time hours and form a coping plan to still consume fruits or
The Present Study
vegetables—“If overtime hours prevent me from eating fruit salad
at lunch-time, then I will buy and consume fruit salad take-out on The purpose of this study is to investigate how challenging job
my way back home.” Several studies demonstrate that action conditions relate to the health-behavior change process. In line
planning and coping planning explain the intention-behavior rela- with the HAPA, we assume that intention is the prerequisite of any
tionship, and thus are effective personal resources (e.g., Scholz, action like FVC. Furthermore, we acknowledge action planning
Schüz, Ziegelmann, Lippke, & Schwarzer, 2008; Schwarzer et al., and coping planning as powerful strategies to concretize the in-
2007; Sniehotta et al., 2005). tention, which is assumed to be especially helpful if intention
Nevertheless, forming good intentions, action plans, and coping implementation is impeded. With the buffering hypothesis, the
plans still do not guarantee the adoption and maintenance of a JD-R model provides a rationale for such impeding conditions in
healthy diet. Because the majority of employed adults in Germany the work context. When job resources are insufficient to buffer
spend most of their waking hours at work (Federal Statistical high job demands, unfavorable job conditions may arise, which
Office of Germany, 2006), the occupational situation, especially potentially lead to job stress and which also impede the achieve-
resources and barriers at work, should play a crucial role in both ment of health goals. Accordingly, we combine the buffering
translating an intention into planning strategies and translating hypothesis of the JD-R model and the HAPA in our moderated
these strategies into actual behavior. mediation model (see Figure 1). In that, we test whether action
planning and coping planning are successfully used to implement
an intention for a proper FVC. “Used” refers to the transfer of an
Healthy Eating at Challenging Job Conditions
intention into action planning or coping planning (a-path), and
The JD-R model (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007) acknowledges that “successfully used” refers to the transfer of these plans into actual
every occupational setting has specific physical, psychological, social, FVC (b-path). Additionally, we consider a moderating effect of high
or organizational factors associated with job stress, which can be and low risk factors for job stress on both paths (see Figure 1).
either classified as “job demands” or “job resources.” Thus, job In the work context, planning strategies can be considered as
demands and job resources may interact in predicting job stress, additional, job-independent resources. Their application might
depending on the specific occupational circumstances. An imbalance equilibrate the ratio of job demands and job resources if healthy
HEALTHY EATING AND JOB STRESS 261

Job resources t1 Job resources t2


Job demands t1 Job demands t2

Planning t2

a b
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Intention t1 FVC t3
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 1. Moderated mediation model with job demands and job resources moderating the a-path and the
b-path according to moderated mediation Model 48 (Hayes, 2013). Planning t2 ⫽ action planning or coping
planning at t2, respectively; FVC t3 ⫽ fruits and vegetable consumption at t3.

eating should be implemented at work. Therefore, in our first employees were invited to participate in a questionnaire study by
hypothesis, we predict that action planning and coping planning emails that detailed study’s aims and procedures and contained an
are applied to support FVC-intentions especially under unfavor- Internet link to the first online questionnaire (t1). Individuals
able job conditions, that is, high job demands and low job re- providing their e-mail address at the end of the t1 questionnaire
sources. This hypothesis is tested by a three-way interaction be- received a second (t2) and third (t3) questionnaire in two-week
tween a) intention, b) job demands, and c) job resources predicting intervals via e-mail. The study was conducted in line with the
action planning and coping planning, respectively, which refers to National Psychological Society’s ethical guidelines.
the a-path. A total of n ⫽ 668 employees participated in the baseline
However, even if employees have generated plans to support questionnaire, and 272 of them completed the longitudinal survey
their FVC-intention, the successful translation of these plans into until t3 (40.7% of baseline). This met the required sample size,
FVC may still be challenged. For example, in the presence of high calculated in an a priori power analysis with an assumed power of
job demands the implementation of the planned behavior could be .80 and p value of .05 (n ⫽ 227), which was based on similar
impeded if job resources (e.g., time control) are low. Therefore, in studies (Payne et al., 2010; Sniehotta et al., 2005); 73.9% of the
our second hypothesis, we assume that employees with high job participants were female, and the average age was 41.2 years
demands are more likely to translate their plans into FVC when (SD ⫽ 12.5; range 20 to 87 years). The average body mass index
they experience high job resources (e.g., high time control) than (BMI) of female participants was 23.3 (SD ⫽ 4.2) and of male
employees with an insufficient amount of job resources (e.g., low participants 25.6 (SD ⫽ 4.2), which are both slightly below the
time control). Again, this hypothesis is tested by a three-way average BMI of employed adults in Germany (female ⫽ 24.1 and
interaction between a) action planning and coping planning, re- male ⫽ 26.1; Federal Statistical Office of Germany, 2011). Work-
spectively, b) job demands, and c) job resources predicting FVC,
ing hours per week ranged from 5 to 80 with an average of 38.2
which refers to the b-path.
hours per week (SD ⫽ 12.4). Most participants were employed in
Both hypotheses constitute a moderated mediation model
the research sector (44.5%), followed by administration/public
with action planning or coping planning as mediators between
services (18.4%), IT/marketing service providers (16.2%), and
intention and FVC and job stress conditions as moderators,
other sectors (19.9%).
depicted in Figure 1.
Drop-out analyses using analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
showed that those participants who completed the survey had
Method significantly lower working hours per week (p ⬍ .01), lower job
demands (p ⬍ .05), and a lower BMI (p ⫽ .05) than dropouts.
Participants and Procedure They also reported higher baseline levels of action planning (p ⬍
To cover a broad variety of job characteristics, participants were .001).
recruited from three different sectors, namely the IT and marketing
sector (service providers: atypical working hours, high time pres- Measures
sure), the research sector (researchers: variation in time pressure,
higher degree of time and decision latitudes), and the public sector If not otherwise stated, items were answered on 5-point Likert
(administrative employees: fixed working hours, narrow time and scales either ranging from 1 ⫽ very rarely to 5 ⫽ very often or
decision latitudes). E-mail addresses were acquired through elec- from 1 ⫽ completely disagree to 5 ⫽ completely agree and were
tronic trade and research directories and business contacts; 4,177 framed consider the participant’s previous week.
262 FODOR, ANTONI, WIEDEMANN, AND BURKERT

Job demands and job resources. According to the JD-R fruit or vegetable (to eat), with Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ .84 at t2.
model (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007), an imbalance between job Coping planning was measured by two items such as Last week I
demands and job resources can lead to job stress. Therefore, job made specific plans regarding how to consume 5 servings of fruit
demands were assessed with a five-item scale “time pressure” and vegetable in the face of difficult situations, with Cronbach’s
(e.g., How often did you work under time pressure?) from the alpha ⫽ .95 at t2. Means, standard errors, and correlations of all
validated Instrument for Stress-oriented Job Analysis (Instrument variables are provided in Table 1.
zur Stressbezogenen Tätigkeitsanalyse; ISTA; Semmer, Zapf, &
Dunckel, 1999) and three additional items (e.g., In the last week,
time pressure at work was so high that work quality was suffering Analytical Procedure
once in a while; Semmer, Kälin, Elfering, & Tschan, 2008). To
Hypotheses were tested by means of regression analyses. In
consider specific nutrition-related job demands, two self-
accordance with the HAPA, two regression analyses represent the
constructed items were added similarly to Semmer et al. (2008). A
health-behavior change process from intention through planning
sample item is In the last week, time pressure at work was so high
strategies to behavior. Additionally, according to the buffering
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

that I hardly had time to eat. Cronbach’s alpha was .92 at t1 and
assumption of the JD-R model, risk factors for job stress are
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

.93 at t2. Job resources were measured with two five-item scales
considered as moderators (i.e., the interaction of job demands and
from the ISTA, “time-control” (e.g., How much were you able to
job resources). Hypotheses were tested separately for action plan-
determine your pace of work on your own?) and “control at work”
ning and coping planning as a mediator, respectively. For this
(e.g., How much influence did you have on the type of work that
purpose, the PROCESS macro for SPSS (Hayes, 2013) was used,
was assigned to you?). The response format of two items was a
which allows testing moderation and mediation models as well as
6-point Likert scale (from 1 ⫽ not at all to 6 ⫽ more than one hour
and from 1 ⫽ less than five minutes a day to 6 ⫽ more than two indirect effects on different levels of continuous moderators.
hours a day, respectively). These items were transformed into Model 48 in this macro refers to a mediation model with both
5-point scales (see Semmer et al., 1999). The response format of mediation-paths being moderated (see Figure 1). Hypothesis 1
one item was a 5-point Likert scale (1 ⫽ not at all to 5 ⫽ all refers to a moderated a-path; an interaction of intention (t1) with
results of work). Cronbach’s alpha was .87 at both t1 and t2. job demands and job resources (t1) predicting planning strategies
Fruit and vegetable consumption (FVC). FVC was assessed (t2). Hypothesis 2 refers to a moderated b-path; an interaction of
with an open response item (e.g., Wiedemann, Lippke, & Schwar- the planning strategies (t2) with job demands and job resources
zer, 2012): How many servings of fruit and vegetables did you eat (t2) predicting FVC (t3). Each regression analysis was controlled
on a typical day of the last week? A serving was defined as one for age, sex, and baseline FVC. Predictors that built the interaction
handful; for example, one handful of salad. A statement was also terms were mean centered. Given a significant three-way interac-
provided that rice and products made of potatoes should not be tion, the size of the conditional effect of intention on planning
regarded as fruit or vegetables. strategies, and the conditional effect of planning strategies on
Social– cognitive variables. Social– cognitive variables were FVC, respectively, will be analyzed at combinations of high and
assessed in accordance with the 5-a-day recommendation for fruit low values of job demands and job resources (i.e., at the 10th, 50th
and vegetables consumption and had been used previously (e.g., and 90th percentiles). Conditional effects based on percentiles are
Wiedemann et al., 2012). Response formats were 6-point Likert in line with simple slope analyses (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken,
scales (1 ⫽ completely disagree to 6 ⫽ completely agree). Behav- 2003), but this procedure guarantees that all quantile values are
ioral intention was measured at baseline by the item, Last week I within the range of the observed data, even if the distribution is
intended to eat at least 5 servings of fruit and vegetable each day. skewed (Hayes, 2013). Moreover, it allows observing extreme
Action planning was assessed by three items: Last week I made values of job stress condition (the 10th percentile stands for very
specific plans regarding [when to eat, where to eat, what kind of] low; the 90th for very high).

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations for All Study Variables (n ⫽ 272)

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

1. JD t1 2.55 .90
2. JD t2 2.47 .87 .76ⴱⴱ
3. JR t1 3.88 .62 ⫺.37ⴱⴱ ⫺.28ⴱⴱ
4. JR t2 3.82 .63 ⫺.31ⴱⴱ ⫺.36ⴱⴱ .73ⴱⴱ
5. INT t1 2.07 1.38 .02 .07 ⫺.12 ⫺.11
6. AP t2 3.16 1.28 .11 .08 ⫺.09 ⫺.13ⴱ .12
7. CP t2 1.91 .99 .17ⴱ .13ⴱ ⫺.16ⴱ ⫺.11 .46ⴱⴱ .41ⴱⴱ
9. FVC t1 2.55 1.52 ⫺.11 ⫺.04 .07 ⫺.01 .46ⴱⴱ .07 .16ⴱ
8. FVC t3 2.79 1.43 .01 ⫺.02 .01 .01 .38ⴱⴱ .10 .35ⴱⴱ .51ⴱⴱ
10. Sexa 1.25 .43 ⫺.05 .01 .17ⴱⴱ .16ⴱⴱ ⫺.18ⴱⴱ ⫺.22ⴱⴱ ⫺.23ⴱⴱ ⫺.16ⴱⴱ ⫺.16ⴱⴱ
11. Age 41.20 12.45 .04 .05 .15ⴱ .14ⴱ .03 .01 .09 .13ⴱ .09 .13ⴱ
Note. JD ⫽ Job demands; JR ⫽ Job resources; INT ⫽ Intention; AP ⫽ Action planning; CP ⫽ Coping planning; FVC ⫽ Fruit and vegetable consumption.
a
1 ⫽ male, 2 ⫽ female.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.
HEALTHY EATING AND JOB STRESS 263

Taking hypothesis 1 and 2 together, they depict the volitional upper part of Table 2. Three-way interactions of intention, job
phase in the HAPA from intention to FVC through the mediator demands, and job resources were significantly associated both with
action planning or coping planning, respectively, and risk factors action planning and coping planning.
for job stress as additional moderators in line with the JD-R-model. The a-path three-way interaction is exemplarily displayed for
Therefore, we finally test this moderated mediation model. If the the prediction of coping planning in Figure 2. Conditional effects
strength of the indirect effect of intention on FVC via action of intention on coping planning for all combinations of low,
planning or coping planning, respectively, changes as a result of average, and high values of job demands and job resources were
one of the two assumed moderation effects, this indicates moder- tested and support hypothesis 1. Under all combinations of job
ated mediation. If a moderated mediation is indicated, the condi- demands and job resources intention was significantly associated
tional indirect effect of the mediator on the intention-FVC asso- with coping planning, with the exception of the high job demands-
ciation at combinations of different levels (at the mean and ⫾ one high job resources combination (B ⫽ 0.15, ns) and the low job
standard deviation) of job demands and job resources at t1 and/or demands-low job resources combination (B ⫽ 0.18, ns). In line
t2 will be tested with bias-corrected bootstrap analysis. These with hypothesis 1, the highest effects were found for the high job
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

asymmetric bootstrap intervals are robust against irregularly demands–low job resources condition (B ⫽ 0.42, p ⬍ .001) and the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

shaped sampling distributions (Hayes, 2013). low job demands– high job resources condition (B ⫽ 0.43, p ⬍
All analyses were run with the 272 completed surveys after item .001). Similar patterns were found for the three-way interaction
and unit missings (t2) were imputed using the Expectation Maxi- predicting action planning.
mization algorithm in SPSS 20 (Enders, 2001).
Moderation of the B-Path Between Planning Strategies
Results and FVC

Moderation of the A-Path Between Intention and With regard to the b-path, results did not support hypothesis 2.
Planning Strategies Risk factors for job stress had no effect on the b-path, the three-
way interaction of job demands, job resources, and neither action
To test hypothesis 1, regression analyses both for action plan- planning nor coping planning reached significance (see lower part
ning and coping planning were conducted and are depicted in the of Table 2).

Table 2
Moderated Mediation Model With A-Path and B-Path Moderation for Each Mediator Action
Planning and Coping Planning (PROCESS Model 48; Hayes, 2013)

Me: Action Me: Coping


planning t2 planning t2
Moderation B SE B SE

A-path moderation: planning t2 (Me) as outcome


Sex ⫺.56ⴱⴱ .19 ⫺.34ⴱⴱ .13
Age .00 .01 .01ⴱ .00
Past behavior t1 .00 .06 ⫺.06 .04
Intention t1 (a-path) .01 .07 .29ⴱⴱⴱ .05
Job demands t1 .07 .09 .10 .06
Job resources t1 ⫺.13 .14 ⫺.12 .10
Intention ⫻ job demands t1 ⫺.01 .07 ⫺.01 .05
Intention ⫻ job resources t1 .00 .10 .00 .07
Job demands ⫻ job resources t1 .21 .13 ⫺.13 .09
Intention ⫻ job demands ⫻ job resources t1
(moderated a-path) ⫺.23ⴱ .09 ⫺.14ⴱ .06
R2 .09ⴱⴱ .29ⴱⴱⴱ
B-path moderation: behavior t3 (DV) as outcome
Sex ⫺.21 .19 ⫺.07 .18
Age .00 .01 .00 .01
Past behavior t1 .40ⴱⴱⴱ .06 .42ⴱⴱⴱ .05
Intention t1 .18ⴱⴱ .06 .07 .07
Planning t2 (b-path) .03 .06 .36ⴱⴱⴱ .09
Job demands t2 ⫺.02 .09 ⫺.05 .09
Job resources t2 .08 .14 .09 .13
Planning ⫻ job demands t2 ⫺.06 .07 ⫺.09 .09
Planning ⫻ job resources t2 .10 .10 .07 .14
Job demands ⫻ job resources t2 .00 .13 .01 .12
Planning ⫻ job demands ⫻ job resources t2
(moderated b-path) .03 .08 ⫺.02 .11
R2 .30ⴱⴱⴱ .35ⴱⴱⴱ
Note. Me ⫽ mediator.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .001.
264 FODOR, ANTONI, WIEDEMANN, AND BURKERT
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 2. Moderation effect of low, average, and high job resources on the transfer of intention (t1) into coping
planning (t2) under high, average, and low job demands (t1). ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.

Conditional Indirect Effect of Intentions on FVC resources (⫹1 SD). Only if employees experienced high job de-
Through Planning Strategies mands (⫹1 SD), which were buffered by high job resources (⫹1
SD), the indirect effect of intention on FVC through coping plan-
Finally, both paths were tested within a moderated mediation ning was not significant.
model. In our second regression analysis action planning was not
related to FVC, neither directly nor in interaction with risk factors
for job stress. Therefore, action planning is no mediator of the Discussion
intention–FVC association. In contrast, coping planning mediated
The present study tested the effects of risk factors for job stress
the intention–FVC association. Thus, the conditional indirect ef-
on health-behavior change. New insights into the role of stress-
fect of intention on FVC through coping planning at interactions of
related job conditions on health-behavior change were obtained by
different levels of job demands and job resources was tested by
including job stress antecedents as a moderator of the intention–
means of bootstrap analyses. As Table 3 shows, coping planning
planning–FVC association.
mediated the intention–FVC association at almost all levels of job
A significant moderation of the intention–planning association
demands and job resources with the strongest indirect effects under
by risk factors for job stress (a-path) was found, both for action
conditions of high job demands (⫹1 SD) and low job resources
planning and coping planning. This conditional effect sheds further
(⫺1 SD) as well as of low job demands (⫺1 SD) and high job
light on health-behavior change under different job stress condi-
tions. Supporting our first hypothesis, particularly those employees
with the intention to eat recommended amounts of fruits and
Table 3
vegetables and who are working under conditions associated with
Bootstrapped Indirect Effects of Intention (t1) on Change in
job stress, engaged in action planning and coping planning. In line
FVC (t1–t3) Through Coping Planning at Interactions of Job
with the buffer hypothesis of the JD-R model, employees also
Demands and Job Resources t1 at Specific Values (With Means
abstained from forming action plans or coping plans for intention
for Job Demands and Job Resources t2)
implementation when they perceived to have sufficient levels of
Job Job job resources to buffer their job demands. For instance, when
demands resources B SE LL CI UL CI employees perceived enough time control for FVC even in highly
demanding situations, they refrained from furnishing their inten-
⫹1 SD ⫺1 SD .13 .05 .06 .24
⫹1 SD Mean .10 .05 .04 .23 tions with action plans or coping plans. Moreover, especially
⫹1 SD ⫹1 SD .07 .07 ⫺.01 .26 coping plans helped to enact intentions for FVC at all combina-
Mean ⫺1 SD .11 .04 .05 .19 tions of job demands and job resources (b-path) in our study. The
Mean Mean .11 .04 .05 .20 mediation of coping planning itself is in line with previous re-
Mean ⫹1 SD .11 .05 .04 .23
⫺1 SD ⫺1 SD .08 .04 .02 .19
search (e.g., Scholz et al., 2008). Results suggest that coping
⫺1 SD Mean .11 .04 .05 .20 planning is a personal work-independent resource that compen-
⫺1 SD ⫹1 SD .14 .05 .06 .26 sates missing work-related resources in highly demanding jobs.
Note. n ⫽ 5000 bootstrapping resamples; LL CI ⫽ Lower level and UL
Findings also tentatively support the coping hypothesis of the
CI ⫽ upper level of bias corrected bootstrapped confidence intervals for JD-R model (Bakker & Demerouti, 2007): Employees make am-
␣ ⬍ .05. plified use of their self-regulatory resources when experiencing
HEALTHY EATING AND JOB STRESS 265

risk of job strain to prevent a loss of resources. Under unfavorable study, the dropout rate also points to the possibility of response
job conditions, they may apply coping planning as a resource to biases.
follow a healthy diet which in turn is a key condition for successful Moreover, a single-item measure was used for FVC instead of
stress management at work. objective measures of actual fruit consumption because of practi-
However, contrary to our second hypothesis, risk factors for job cal reasons in this online-study. However, several aspects point to
stress did not moderate the coping planning-FVC relationship the validity of this self-reported single-item measure. First, recent
(b-path), that is, they did not impede the beneficial effects of studies have shown high correlations of very similar single-items
coping plans for FVC: Employees’ coping plans appear to be and fruit and vegetable measures (Chapman, Armitage, & Norman,
helpful for the adoption or maintenance of the intended FVC, 2009). Second, a similar single-item measure of FVC has been
irrespective of current job stress conditions. This perfectly sup- validated against dietary biomarkers (Steptoe et al., 2003). Third,
ports the purpose of coping planning: Anticipated barriers (e.g., because of anonymity, these self-reports are less prone to be biased
unforeseen working hours over lunch time) can be overcome by (Armitage, 2007). Nevertheless, distortions in measuring FVC
the use of ex-situ planning strategies (e.g., consumption of pre- cannot be ruled out, which may possibly explain why action
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

produced fruit salad). Furthermore, the lack of direct effects of risk planning did not mediate the intention–FVC relationship. Future
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

factors for job stress on FVC indicates that job demands are not per studies on this topic should consider more reliable measures of
se a threat to FVC at work. FVC as provided in the Compendium of Surveys for F&V con-
Our findings confirm a moderated mediation model for coping sumptions (California Department of Public Health, 2012).
planning, combining HAPA and JD-R model assumptions, in that Generalizability was also limited with respect to job types.
the buffering assumption of the JD-R model adds to the under- Future research on this topic should therefore include samples
standing of the volitional phase of the HAPA. Nevertheless, in from other branches (e.g., handcraft or social services). Within the
contrast to previous studies (e.g., Schwarzer et al., 2007; Wiede- limits of a quantitative online study, our operationalization of risk
mann, Schüz, Sniehotta, Scholz, & Schwarzer, 2009), action plan- factors for job stress does not sufficiently comply with the theo-
ning did not mediate the relationship between intentions and retical approach of the JD-R model. According to Bakker and
behavior. Neither a mean association between intentions and ac- Demerouti (2007) each job type has its specific job demands and
tion planning (a-path) nor an association between action planning job resources. Our sample, which included a broad range of
and FVC (b-path) were identified. The nonsignificant mean a-path different job types, “time pressure” as job demand and “time-
may be attributable to the conditionality of the intention–action control” and “control of work” as job resource, respectively, may
planning relationship under certain conditions of job stress (three- not have represented all characteristics for each job type under
way interaction). Intentions further had a significant direct effect investigation. Consequently, risk factors for job stress may have
on FVC. Also, the nonsignificant association between action plan- been underestimated in the present sample. Future research would
ning and FVC is explicable in context. Action plans may not have benefit from investigating job-specific demands and resources.
been flexible enough to prevail under challenging job conditions. Complementarily, expert ratings (e.g., see Demerouti, Bakker,
Previous research points to further factors like self-efficacy beliefs, Nachreiner, & Schaufeli, 2001) or external assessments (e.g., see
which may also play a crucial role in this context (Lippke, Wie- Bakker, Demerouti, & Verbeke, 2004; Salanova, Agut, & Peiró,
demann, Ziegelmann, Reuter, & Schwarzer, 2009). 2005) could be used. Subsequent studies on the role of risk factors
for job stress on the volitional processes of health-behavior change
might also consider health behaviors other than healthy eating.
Limitations and Future Directions
Limitations of the present study and subsequent implications for
Practical Implications and Conclusion
future research should be mentioned. This longitudinal study cov-
ers only a short period of health-behavior change. Two-week The present findings suggest that considering risk factors for job
intervals allow to document changes in frequent health behavior stress further qualifies under which conditions intentions lead to
like FVC. Nevertheless, the causality of the association between behavior change through planning strategies. Taking work-related
risk factors for job stress and health-behavior change remains conditions into account when investigating health behavior is
unclear. Therefore, future research should investigate long-term crucial given the large amount of time individuals spend working.
influences of risk factors for job stress on health-behavior change Further research should investigate at which levels of job stress
via randomized controlled trials, as well as daily fluctuations in the action planning and coping planning are beneficial or whether they
association via diary studies. In contrast to online-surveys, with an can be complemented by other strategies. To preserve health and
excessive reduction of the baseline number of participants during obviate sickness, practical implications of our research primarily
the survey period attributable to the nonbinding and anonymous refer to coping planning. Employees working under unfavorable
nature of this type of study (Galesic, 2006), surveys with face-to- job conditions should anticipate individual obstacles that hamper
face contacts will likely lead to higher commitment. their FVC at work and equip themselves with detailed plans how
Dropout analysis points to a selective sample. Active partici- to overcome these obstacles. Furthermore, health promotion pro-
pants reported more action planning, a lower BMI, and less job grams in the work context (e.g., stress management) could be
demands as well as less working hours than those who dropped supplemented with coping planning strategies for successful be-
out. These factors limit generalizability of our findings. Besides havior change. From an organizational point of view, managers
the effectiveness of coping planning, this may partly explain why should provide their employees with appropriate decision latitudes
risk factors for job stress were not associated with the planning- that enable them dealing with job demands but also to decide when
FVC-association. As questionnaire measures were used in this it is time to interrupt the work process, for example, to act upon the
266 FODOR, ANTONI, WIEDEMANN, AND BURKERT

intention to consume an apple in that very moment. Combining job Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and condi-
stress models such as the JD-R model and health-behavior change tional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY:
models such as the HAPA may be a fruitful avenue for future Guilford Press.
research. Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental
strain: Implications for job redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly,
24, 285–308. doi:10.2307/2392498
References Lippke, S., Wiedemann, A. U., Ziegelmann, J. P., Reuter, T., & Schwarzer,
R. (2009). Self-efficacy moderates the mediation of intentions into
Armitage, C. J. (2007). Effects of an implementation intention-based behavior via plans. American Journal of Health Behavior, 33, 521–529.
intervention on fruit consumption. Psychology & Health, 22, 917–928. doi:10.5993/AJHB.33.5.5
doi:10.1080/14768320601070662 Payne, N., Jones, F., & Harris, P. (2002). The impact of working life on
Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned health behavior: The effect of job strain on the cognitive predictors of
behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychol- exercise. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7, 342–353. doi:
ogy, 40, 471– 499. doi:10.1348/014466601164939 10.1037/1076-8998.7.4.342
Bakker, A. B., & Demerouti, E. (2007). The job demands-resources model:
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Payne, N., Jones, F., & Harris, P. R. (2005). The impact of job strain on the
State of the art. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 22, 309 –328.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

predictive validity of the theory of planned behavior: An investigation of


doi:10.1108/02683940710733115 exercise and healthy eating. British Journal of Health Psychology, 10,
Bakker, A. B., Demerouti, E., & Euwema, M. C. (2005). Job resources 115–131. doi:10.1348/135910704X14636
buffer the impact of job demands on burnout. Journal of Occupational Payne, N., Jones, F., & Harris, P. R. (2010). A daily diary investigation of
Health Psychology, 10, 170 –180. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.10.2.170
the impact of work stress on exercise intention realisation: Can planning
Bakker, A. B., Demerouti, E., & Verbeke, W. (2004). Using the job
overcome the disruptive influence of work? Psychology & Health, 25,
demands-resources model to predict burnout and performance. Human
111–129. doi:10.1080/08870440903337622
Resource Management, 43, 83–104. doi:10.1002/hrm.20004
Salanova, M., Agut, S., & Peiró, J. M. (2005). Linking organizational
Blanck, H. M., Gillespie, C., Kimmons, J. E., Seymour, J. D., & Serdula,
resources and work engagement to employee performance and customer
M. K. (2008). Trends in fruit and vegetable consumption among U.S.
loyalty: The mediation of service climate. Journal of Applied Psychol-
men and women, 1994 –2005. Preventing Chronic Diseases, 5, A35.
ogy, 90, 1217–1227. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.6.1217
California Department of Public Health. (2012, October 1). Compendium
Scholz, U., Schüz, B., Ziegelmann, J. P., Lippke, S., & Schwarzer, R.
of surveys for nutrition education and obesity prevention. Retrieved
(2008). Beyond behavioral intentions: Planning mediates between inten-
from http://www.cdph.ca.gov/programs/cpns/Documents/Compendium%
tions and physical activity. British Journal of Health Psychology, 13,
20of%20Surveys.pdf
479 – 494. doi:10.1348/135910707X216062
Chapman, J., Armitage, C. J., & Norman, P. (2009). Comparing imple-
Schwarzer, R. (2008). Modeling health behavior change: How to predict
mentation intention interventions in relation to young adults’ intake of
and modify the adoption and maintenance of health behaviors. Applied
fruit and vegetables. Psychology & Health, 24, 317–332. doi:10.1080/
Psychology, 57, 1–29. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00325.x
08870440701864538
Schwarzer, R., Schüz, B., Ziegelmann, J. P., Lippke, S., Luszczynska, A.,
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple
regression/correlation analyses for the behavioural sciences (3rd ed.). & Scholz, U. (2007). Adoption and maintenance of four health behav-
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. iors: Theory-guided longitudinal studies on dental flossing, seat belt use,
Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., Nachreiner, F., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2001). dietary behavior, and physical activity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine,
The job demands-resources model of burnout. Journal of Applied Psy- 33, 156 –166. doi:10.1007/BF02879897
chology, 86, 499 –512. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.499 Semmer, N. K., Kälin, W., Elfering, A., & Tschan, F. (2008, November).
Enders, C. K. (2001). A primer on maximum likelihood algorithms avail- When time pressure really hurts: The case of performance impairment.
able for use with missing data. Structural Equation Modeling: A Mul- Paper presented at the 6th Conference of the European Academy of
tidisciplinary Journal, 8, 128 –141. doi:10.1207/S15328007SEM0801_7 Occupational Health Psychology, Valencia, Spain.
Federal Statistical Office of Germany. (2006, December 7). Zeitbudgets – Semmer, N. K., Zapf, D., & Dunckel, H. (1999). Instrument zur streßb-
Tabellenband I–2001/2002 [time budgets–list of tables I–2001/2002]. ezogenen Tätigkeitsanalyse [Instrument for stress-related job analysis].
Wiesbaden, Germany: Federal Statistical Office of Germany. Retrieved In H. Dunckel (Ed.), Handbuch psychologischer Arbeitsanalysever-
from https://www.destatis.de/DE/Publikationen/Thematisch/ fahren [Analytical Instrumentation Handbook] (pp. 179 –204). Zürich,
EinkommenKonsumLebensbedingungen/Zeitbudgeterhebung/ Switzerland: VdF Hochschulverlag.
ZeitbudgetsTabellenband1.html Siegrist, J., & Rödel, A. (2006). Work stress and health risk behavior.
Federal Statistical Office of Germany. (2011, January 24). Mikrozensus - Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment & Health, 32, 473– 481.
Körperma␤e der Bevölkerung 2009 [micro census – body measurements doi:10.5271/sjweh.1052
of the population 2009]. Wiesbaden, Germany: Federal Statistical Office Sniehotta, F. F. (2009). Towards a theory of intentional behaviour change:
of Germany. Retrieved from https://www.destatis.de/DE/Publikationen/ Plans, planning, and self-regulation. British Journal of Health Psychol-
Thematisch/Gesundheit/Gesundheitszustand/Koerpermasse5239003099004 ogy, 14, 261–273. doi:10.1348/135910708X389042
.pdf?__blob⫽publicationFile Sniehotta, F. F., Scholz, U., & Schwarzer, R. (2005). Bridging the
Galesic, M. (2006). Dropouts on the web: Effects of interest and burden intention-behavior gap: Planning, self-efficacy and action control in the
experienced during an online survey. Journal of Official Statistics, 22, adoption and maintenance of physical exercise. Psychology & Health,
313–328. 20, 143–160. doi:10.1080/08870440512331317670
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of Steptoe, A., Perkins-Porras, L., McKay, C., Rink, E., Hilton, S., & Cap-
simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503. doi:10.1037/0003- puccio, F. P. (2003). Psychological factors associated with fruit and
066X.54.7.493 vegetable intake and with biomarkers in adults from a low-income
Hakanen, J. J., Bakker, A. B., & Demerouti, E. (2005). How dentists cope neighborhood. Health Psychology, 22, 148 –155. doi:10.1037/0278-
with their job demands and stay engaged: The moderating role of job 6133.22.2.148
resources. European Journal of Oral Sciences, 113, 479 – 487. doi: Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions
10.1111/j.1600-0722.2005.00250.x engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evi-
HEALTHY EATING AND JOB STRESS 267

dence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249 – 68. doi:10.1037/0033-2909 World Health Organization. (2003). Fruit, vegetables and NCD disease
.132.2.249 prevention. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO. Retrieved from http://
Wiedemann, A. U., Lippke, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2012). Multiple plans and whqlibdoc.who.int/trs/WHO_trs_916.pdf
memory performance: Results of a randomized controlled trial targeting
fruit and vegetable intake. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 35, 387–392.
doi:10.1007/s10865-011-9364-2
Wiedemann, A. U., Schüz, B., Sniehotta, F., Scholz, U., & Schwarzer, R.
(2009). Disentangling the relation between intentions, planning, and Received June 25, 2013
behavior: A moderated mediation analysis. Psychology & Health, 24, Revision received January 21, 2014
67–79. doi:10.1080/08870440801958214 Accepted February 12, 2014 䡲
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Showcase your work in APA’s newest database.

Make your tests available to other researchers and students; get wider recognition for your work.
“PsycTESTS is going to be an outstanding resource for psychology,” said Ronald F. Levant,
PhD. “I was among the first to provide some of my tests and was happy to do so. They will be
available for others to use—and will relieve me of the administrative tasks of providing them to
individuals.”
Visit http://www.apa.org/pubs/databases/psyctests/call-for-tests.aspx
to learn more about PsycTESTS and how you can participate.
Questions? Call 1-800-374-2722 or write to tests@apa.org.
Not since PsycARTICLES has a database been so eagerly anticipated!

You might also like